fbpx
Wikipedia

Anti-predator adaptation

Anti-predator adaptations are mechanisms developed through evolution that assist prey organisms in their constant struggle against predators. Throughout the animal kingdom, adaptations have evolved for every stage of this struggle, namely by avoiding detection, warding off attack, fighting back, or escaping when caught.

Anti-predator adaptation in action: the seal shark Dalatias licha (a–c) and the wreckfish Polyprion americanus (d–f) attempt to prey on hagfishes. First, the predators approach their potential prey. Predators bite or try to swallow the hagfishes, but the hagfishes have already projected jets of slime (arrows) into the predators' mouths. Choking, the predators release the hagfishes and gag in an attempt to remove slime from their mouths and gill chambers.[1]

The first line of defence consists in avoiding detection, through mechanisms such as camouflage, masquerade, apostatic selection, living underground, or nocturnality.

Alternatively, prey animals may ward off attack, whether by advertising the presence of strong defences in aposematism, by mimicking animals which do possess such defences, by startling the attacker, by signalling to the predator that pursuit is not worthwhile, by distraction, by using defensive structures such as spines, and by living in a group. Members of groups are at reduced risk of predation, despite the increased conspicuousness of a group, through improved vigilance, predator confusion, and the likelihood that the predator will attack some other individual.

Avoiding detection edit

Staying out of sight edit

 
Fruit bats forage by night to avoid predators.

Animals may avoid becoming prey by living out of sight of predators, whether in caves, burrows, or by being nocturnal.[2][3][4][5] Nocturnality is an animal behavior characterized by activity during the night and sleeping during the day. This is a behavioral form of detection avoidance called crypsis used by animals to either avoid predation or to enhance prey hunting. Predation risk has long been recognized as critical in shaping behavioral decisions. For example, this predation risk is of prime importance in determining the time of evening emergence in echolocating bats. Although early access during brighter times permits easier foraging, it also leads to a higher predation risk from bat hawks and bat falcons. This results in an optimum evening emergence time that is a compromise between the conflicting demands.[4] Another nocturnal adaptation can be seen in kangaroo rats. They forage in relatively open habitats, and reduce their activity outside their nest burrows in response to moonlight. During a full moon, they shift their activity towards areas of relatively dense cover to compensate for the extra brightness.[5]

 
Camouflage illustrated by the flat-tail horned lizard, its flattened, fringed and disruptively patterned body eliminating shadow

Camouflage edit

Camouflage uses any combination of materials, coloration, or illumination for concealment to make the organism hard to detect by sight. It is common in both terrestrial and marine animals. Camouflage can be achieved in many different ways, such as through resemblance to surroundings, disruptive coloration, shadow elimination by countershading or counter-illumination, self-decoration, cryptic behavior, or changeable skin patterns and colour.[6][7] Animals such as the flat-tail horned lizard of North America have evolved to eliminate their shadow and blend in with the ground. The bodies of these lizards are flattened, and their sides thin towards the edge. This body form, along with the white scales fringed along their sides, allows the lizards to effectively hide their shadows. In addition, these lizards hide any remaining shadows by pressing their bodies to the ground.[2]

Masquerade edit

 
Kallima inachus masquerading as a dead leaf

Animals can hide in plain sight by masquerading as inedible objects. For example, the potoo, a South American bird, habitually perches on a tree, convincingly resembling a broken stump of a branch,[8] while a butterfly, Kallima, looks just like a dead leaf.[9]

Apostatic selection edit

Another way to remain unattacked in plain sight is to look different from other members of the same species. Predators such as tits selectively hunt for abundant types of insect, ignoring less common types that were present, forming search images of the desired prey. This creates a mechanism for negative frequency-dependent selection, apostatic selection.[10]

Warding off attack edit

 
A Mediterranean mantis, Iris oratoria, attempting to startle a predator with deimatic behaviour

Many species make use of behavioral strategies to deter predators.[11]

Startling the predator edit

Many weakly-defended animals, including moths, butterflies, mantises, phasmids, and cephalopods such as octopuses, make use of patterns of threatening or startling behaviour, such as suddenly displaying conspicuous eyespots, so as to scare off or momentarily distract a predator, thus giving the prey animal an opportunity to escape. In the absence of toxins or other defences, this is essentially bluffing, in contrast to aposematism which involves honest signals.[12][13][14]

Pursuit-deterrent signals edit

 
An impala stotting, signalling honestly to the predator that the chase will be unprofitable

Pursuit-deterrent signals are behavioral signals used by prey to convince predators not to pursue them. For example, gazelles stot, jumping high with stiff legs and an arched back. This is thought to signal to predators that they have a high level of fitness and can outrun the predator. As a result, predators may choose to pursue a different prey that is less likely to outrun them.[15]White-tailed deer and other prey mammals flag with conspicuous (often black and white) tail markings when alarmed, informing the predator that it has been detected.[16] Warning calls given by birds such as the Eurasian jay are similarly honest signals, benefiting both predator and prey: the predator is informed that it has been detected and might as well save time and energy by giving up the chase, while the prey is protected from attack.[17][18]

Playing dead edit

 
Eastern hog-nosed snake playing dead

Another pursuit-deterrent signal is thanatosis or playing dead. Thanatosis is a form of bluff in which an animal mimics its own dead body, feigning death to avoid being attacked by predators seeking live prey. Thanatosis can also be used by the predator in order to lure prey into approaching.[19]

An example of this is seen in white-tailed deer fawns, which experience a drop in heart rate in response to approaching predators. This response, referred to as "alarm bradycardia", causes the fawn's heart rate to drop from 155 to 38 beats per minute within one beat of the heart. This drop in heart rate can last up to two minutes, causing the fawn to experience a depressed breathing rate and decrease in movement, called tonic immobility. Tonic immobility is a reflex response that causes the fawn to enter a low body position that simulates the position of a corpse. Upon discovery of the fawn, the predator loses interest in the "dead" prey. Other symptoms of alarm bradycardia, such as salivation, urination, and defecation, can also cause the predator to lose interest.[20]

Distraction edit

 
A killdeer plover, distracting a predator from its nest by feigning a broken wing

Marine molluscs such as sea hares, cuttlefish, squid and octopuses give themselves a last chance to escape by distracting their attackers. To do this, they eject a mixture of chemicals, which may mimic food or otherwise confuse predators.[21][22] In response to a predator, animals in these groups release ink, creating a cloud, and opaline, affecting the predator's feeding senses, causing it to attack the cloud.[21][23]

Distraction displays attract the attention of predators away from an object, typically the nest or young, that is being protected,[24] as when some birds feign a broken wing while hopping about on the ground.[25]

Mimicry and aposematism edit

 
Viceroy and monarch are Müllerian mimics, similar in appearance, unpalatable to predators.

Mimicry occurs when an organism (the mimic) simulates signal properties of another organism (the model) to confuse a third organism. This results in the mimic gaining protection, food, and mating advantages.[26] There are two classical types of defensive mimicry: Batesian and Müllerian. Both involve aposematic coloration, or warning signals, to avoid being attacked by a predator.[27][28]

In Batesian mimicry, a palatable, harmless prey species mimics the appearance of another species that is noxious to predators, thus reducing the mimic's risk of attack.[27] This form of mimicry is seen in many insects. The idea behind Batesian mimicry is that predators that have tried to eat the unpalatable species learn to associate its colors and markings with an unpleasant taste. This results in the predator learning to avoid species displaying similar colours and markings, including Batesian mimics, which are in effect parasitic on the chemical or other defences of the unprofitable models.[29][30] Some species of octopus can mimic a selection of other animals by changing their skin color, skin pattern and body motion. When a damselfish attacks an octopus, the octopus mimics a banded sea-snake.[31] The model chosen varies with the octopus's predator and habitat.[32] Most of these octopuses use Batesian mimicry, selecting an organism repulsive to predators as a model.[33][34]

In Müllerian mimicry, two or more aposematic forms share the same warning signals,[27][35] as in viceroy and monarch butterflies. Birds avoid eating both species because their wing patterns honestly signal their unpleasant taste.[28]

 
The porcupine Erethizon dorsatum combines sharp spines with warning coloration

Defensive structures edit

Many animals are protected against predators with armour in the form of hard shells (such as most molluscs and turtles), leathery or scaly skin (as in reptiles), or tough chitinous exoskeletons (as in arthropods).[25]

A spine is a sharp, needle-like structure used to inflict pain on predators. An example of this seen in nature is in the sohal surgeonfish. These fish have a sharp scalpel-like spine on the front of each of their tail fins, able to inflict deep wounds. The area around the spines is often brightly colored to advertise the defensive capability;[36] predators often avoid the Sohal surgeonfish.[37] Defensive spines may be detachable, barbed or poisonous. Porcupine spines are long, stiff, break at the tip, and in some species are barbed to stick into a would-be predator. In contrast, the hedgehog's short spines, which are modified hairs,[38] readily bend, and are barbed into the body, so they are not easily lost; they may be jabbed at an attacker.[37]

 
Stinging Limacodidae slug moth caterpillars

Many species of slug caterpillar, Limacodidae, have numerous protuberances and stinging spines along their dorsal surfaces. Species that possess these stinging spines suffer less predation than larvae that lack them, and a predator, the paper wasp, chooses larvae without spines when given a choice.[39]

Safety in numbers edit

Group living can decrease the risk of predation to the individual in a variety of ways,[40] as described below.

Dilution effect edit

A dilution effect is seen when animals living in a group "dilute" their risk of attack, each individual being just one of many in the group. George C. Williams and W.D. Hamilton proposed that group living evolved because it provides benefits to the individual rather than to the group as a whole, which becomes more conspicuous as it becomes larger. One common example is the shoaling of fish. Experiments provide direct evidence for the decrease in individual attack rate seen with group living, for example in Camargue horses in Southern France. The horse-fly often attacks these horses, sucking blood and carrying diseases. When the flies are most numerous, the horses gather in large groups, and individuals are indeed attacked less frequently.[41] Water striders are insects that live on the surface of fresh water, and are attacked from beneath by predatory fish. Experiments varying the group size of the water striders showed that the attack rate per individual water strider decreases as group size increases.[42]

 
In a group, prey seek central positions in order to reduce their domain of danger. Individuals along the outer edges of the group are more at risk of being targeted by the predator.

Selfish herd edit

The selfish herd theory was proposed by W.D. Hamilton to explain why animals seek central positions in a group.[43] The theory's central idea is to reduce the individual's domain of danger. A domain of danger is the area within the group in which the individual is more likely to be attacked by a predator. The center of the group has the lowest domain of danger, so animals are predicted to strive constantly to gain this position. Testing Hamilton's selfish herd effect, Alta De Vos and Justin O'Rainn (2010) studied brown fur seal predation from great white sharks. Using decoy seals, the researchers varied the distance between the decoys to produce different domains of danger. The seals with a greater domain of danger had an increased risk of shark attack.[44]

Predator satiation edit

 
A newly emerged periodical cicada: millions emerge at once, at long intervals, likely to satiate predators.

A radical strategy for avoiding predators which may otherwise kill a large majority of the emerging stage of a population is to emerge very rarely, at irregular intervals. Predators with a life-cycle of one or a few years are unable to reproduce rapidly enough in response to such an emergence. Predators may feast on the emerging population, but are unable to consume more than a fraction of the brief surfeit of prey. Periodical cicadas, which emerge at intervals of 13 or 17 years, are often used as an example of this predator satiation, though other explanations of their unusual life-cycle have been proposed.[45]

 
Vervet monkeys have different alarm signals that warn of attacks by eagles, leopards and snakes.

Alarm calls edit

Animals that live in groups often give alarm calls that give warning of an attack. For example, vervet monkeys give different calls depending on the nature of the attack: for an eagle, a disyllabic cough; for a leopard or other cat, a loud bark; for a python or other snake, a "chutter". The monkeys hearing these calls respond defensively, but differently in each case: to the eagle call, they look up and run into cover; to the leopard call, they run up into the trees; to the snake call, they stand on two legs and look around for snakes, and on seeing the snake, they sometimes mob it. Similar calls are found in other species of monkey, while birds also give different calls that elicit different responses.[46]

Improved vigilance edit

 
A raptor, a northern harrier, chases up an alert flock of American avocets.

In the improved vigilance effect, groups are able to detect predators sooner than solitary individuals.[47] For many predators, success depends on surprise. If the prey is alerted early in an attack, they have an improved chance of escape. For example, wood pigeon flocks are preyed upon by goshawks. Goshawks are less successful when attacking larger flocks of wood pigeons than they are when attacking smaller flocks. This is because the larger the flock size, the more likely it is that one bird will notice the hawk sooner and fly away. Once one pigeon flies off in alarm, the rest of the pigeons follow.[48] Wild ostriches in Tsavo National Park in Kenya feed either alone or in groups of up to four birds. They are subject to predation by lions. As the ostrich group size increases, the frequency at which each individual raises its head to look for predators decreases. Because ostriches are able to run at speeds that exceed those of lions for great distances, lions try to attack an ostrich when its head is down. By grouping, the ostriches present the lions with greater difficulty in determining how long the ostriches' heads stay down. Thus, although individual vigilance decreases, the overall vigilance of the group increases.[49]

 
A single zebra is hard to catch amongst a herd.

Predator confusion edit

Individuals living in large groups may be safer from attack because the predator may be confused by the large group size. As the group moves, the predator has greater difficulty targeting an individual prey animal. The zebra has been suggested by the zoologist Martin Stevens and his colleagues as an example of this. When stationary, a single zebra stands out because of its large size. To reduce the risk of attack, zebras often travel in herds. The striped patterns of all the zebras in the herd may confuse the predator, making it harder for the predator to focus in on an individual zebra. Furthermore, when moving rapidly, the zebra stripes create a confusing, flickering motion dazzle effect in the eye of the predator.[50]

Fighting back edit

Defensive structures such as spines may be used both to ward off attack as already mentioned, and if need be to fight back against a predator.[37] Methods of fighting back include chemical defences,[51] mobbing,[52] defensive regurgitation,[53] and suicidal altruism.[54]

Chemical defences edit

 
The bloody-nose beetle, Timarcha tenebricosa, exuding a drop of noxious red liquid (upper right)

Many prey animals, and to defend against seed predation also seeds of plants,[55] make use of poisonous chemicals for self-defence.[51][56] These may be concentrated in surface structures such as spines or glands, giving an attacker a taste of the chemicals before it actually bites or swallows the prey animal: many toxins are bitter-tasting.[51] A last-ditch defence is for the animal's flesh itself to be toxic, as in the puffer fish, danaid butterflies and burnet moths. Many insects acquire toxins from their food plants; Danaus caterpillars accumulate toxic cardenolides from milkweeds (Asclepiadaceae).[56]

Some prey animals are able to eject noxious materials to deter predators actively. The bombardier beetle has specialized glands on the tip of its abdomen that allows it to direct a toxic spray towards predators. The spray is generated explosively through oxidation of hydroquinones and is sprayed at a temperature of 100 °C.[57] Armoured crickets similarly release blood at their joints when threatened (autohaemorrhaging).[58] Several species of grasshopper including Poecilocerus pictus,[59] Parasanaa donovani,[59] Aularches miliaris,[59] and Tegra novaehollandiae secrete noxious liquids when threatened, sometimes ejecting these forcefully.[59] Spitting cobras accurately squirt venom from their fangs at the eyes of potential predators,[60] striking their target eight times out of ten, and causing severe pain.[61] Termite soldiers in the Nasutitermitinae have a fontanellar gun, a gland on the front of their head which can secrete and shoot an accurate jet of resinous terpenes "many centimeters". The material is sticky and toxic to other insects. One of the terpenes in the secretion, pinene, functions as an alarm pheromone.[62] Seeds deter predation with combinations of toxic non-protein amino acids, cyanogenic glycosides, protease and amylase inhibitors, and phytohemagglutinins.[55]

A few vertebrate species such as the Texas horned lizard are able to shoot squirts of blood from their eyes, by rapidly increasing the blood pressure within the eye sockets, if threatened. Because an individual may lose up to 53% of blood in a single squirt,[63] this is only used against persistent predators like foxes, wolves and coyotes (Canidae), as a last defence.[64] Canids often drop horned lizards after being squirted, and attempt to wipe or shake the blood out of their mouths, suggesting that the fluid has a foul taste;[65] they choose other lizards if given the choice,[66] suggesting a learned aversion towards horned lizards as prey.[66]

The slime glands along the body of the hagfish secrete enormous amounts of mucus when it is provoked or stressed. The gelatinous slime has dramatic effects on the flow and viscosity of water, rapidly clogging the gills of any fish that attempt to capture hagfish; predators typically release the hagfish within seconds. Common predators of hagfish include seabirds, pinnipeds and cetaceans, but few fish, suggesting that predatory fish avoid hagfish as prey.[67]

Communal defence edit

 
Group of muskoxen in defensive formation, horns ready, and highly alert

In communal defence, prey groups actively defend themselves by grouping together, and sometimes by attacking or mobbing a predator, rather than allowing themselves to be passive victims of predation. Mobbing is the harassing of a predator by many prey animals. Mobbing is usually done to protect the young in social colonies. For example, red colobus monkeys exhibit mobbing when threatened by chimpanzees, a common predator. The male red colobus monkeys group together and place themselves between predators and the group's females and juveniles. The males jump together and actively bite the chimpanzees.[52] Fieldfares are birds which may nest either solitarily or in colonies. Within colonies, fieldfares mob and defecate on approaching predators, shown experimentally to reduce predation levels.[68]

Defensive regurgitation edit

 
A northern fulmar chick protects itself with a jet of stomach oil.

Some birds and insects use defensive regurgitation to ward off predators. The northern fulmar vomits a bright orange, oily substance called stomach oil when threatened.[53] The stomach oil is made from their aquatic diets. It causes the predator's feathers to mat, leading to the loss of flying ability and the loss of water repellency.[53] This is especially dangerous for aquatic birds because their water repellent feathers protect them from hypothermia when diving for food.[53]

European roller chicks vomit a bright orange, foul smelling liquid when they sense danger. This repels prospective predators and may alert their parents to danger: they respond by delaying their return.[69]

Numerous insects utilize defensive regurgitation. The eastern tent caterpillar regurgitates a droplet of digestive fluid to repel attacking ants.[70] Similarly, larvae of the noctuid moth regurgitate when disturbed by ants. The vomit of noctuid moths has repellent and irritant properties that help to deter predator attacks.[71]

Suicidal altruism edit

An unusual type of predator deterrence is observed in the Malaysian exploding ant. Social hymenoptera rely on altruism to protect the entire colony, so the self-destructive acts benefit all individuals in the colony.[54] When a worker ant's leg is grasped, it suicidally expels the contents of its hypertrophied submandibular glands,[54] expelling corrosive irritant compounds and adhesives onto the predator. These prevent predation and serve as a signal to other enemy ants to stop predation of the rest of the colony.[72]

Escaping edit

 
Startled pheasants and partridges fly from possible danger.

Flight edit

The normal reaction of a prey animal to an attacking predator is to flee by any available means, whether flying, gliding,[73] falling, swimming, running, jumping, burrowing[74] or rolling,[75] according to the animal's capabilities.[76] Escape paths are often erratic, making it difficult for the predator to predict which way the prey will go next: for example, birds such as snipe, ptarmigan and black-headed gulls evade fast raptors such as peregrine falcons with zigzagging or jinking flight.[76] In the tropical rain forests of Southeast Asia in particular, many vertebrates escape predators by falling and gliding.[73] Among the insects, many moths turn sharply, fall, or perform a powered dive in response to the sonar clicks of bats.[76] Among fish, the stickleback follows a zigzagging path, often doubling back erratically, when chased by a fish-eating merganser duck.[76]

Autotomy edit

 
Lizard tail autotomy can distract predators, continuing to writhe while the lizard makes its escape.

Some animals are capable of autotomy (self-amputation), shedding one of their own appendages in a last-ditch attempt to elude a predator's grasp or to distract the predator and thereby allow escape. The lost body part may be regenerated later. Certain sea slugs discard stinging papillae; arthropods such as crabs can sacrifice a claw, which can be regrown over several successive moults; among vertebrates, many geckos and other lizards shed their tails when attacked: the tail goes on writhing for a while, distracting the predator, and giving the lizard time to escape; a smaller tail slowly regrows.[77]

History of observations edit

Aristotle recorded observations (around 350 BC) of the antipredator behaviour of cephalopods in his History of Animals, including the use of ink as a distraction, camouflage, and signalling.[78]

In 1940, Hugh Cott wrote a compendious study of camouflage, mimicry, and aposematism, Adaptive Coloration in Animals.[6]

By the 21st century, adaptation to life in cities had markedly reduced the antipredator responses of animals such as rats and pigeons; similar changes are observed in captive and domesticated animals.[79]

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ Zintzen, Vincent; Roberts, Clive D.; Anderson, Marti J.; Stewart, Andrew L.; Struthers, Carl D.; Harvey, Euan S. (2011). "Hagfish Slime as a Defense Mechanism against Gill-breathing Predators". Scientific Reports. 1: 2011. Bibcode:2011NatSR...1E.131Z. doi:10.1038/srep00131. PMC 3216612. PMID 22355648.
  2. ^ a b Sherbrooke, W. C. (2003). Introduction to horned lizards of North America. University of California Press. pp. 117–118.
  3. ^ Cott, H.B. (1940). Adaptive Coloration in Animals. London: Methuen. pp. 330–335.
  4. ^ a b Duverge, P.L.; Jones, G; Rydell, J.; Ransome, R. (2000). "Functional significance of emergence timing in bats". Ecography. 23 (1): 32–40. Bibcode:2000Ecogr..23...32D. doi:10.1111/j.1600-0587.2000.tb00258.x.
  5. ^ a b Daly, M.; Behrends, P.R.; Wilson, M.; Jacobs, L. (1992). "Behavioural modulation of predation risk: moonlight avoidance and crepuscular compensation in a nocturnal desert rodent, Dipodomys merriami". Animal Behaviour. 44: 1–9. doi:10.1016/s0003-3472(05)80748-1. S2CID 4077513.
  6. ^ a b Cott 1940.
  7. ^ Caro 2005, pp. 35–60.
  8. ^ Caro 2005, pp. 53–55.
  9. ^ Cott 1940, pp. 318–320.
  10. ^ Caro 2005, pp. 61–65.
  11. ^ Cooper, William E. . IDEA. University of California, Riverside. Archived from the original on 18 May 2018. Retrieved 23 October 2014.
  12. ^ Stevens, Martin (2005). "The role of eyespots as anti-predator mechanisms, principally demonstrated in the Lepidoptera". Biological Reviews. 80 (4): 573–588. doi:10.1017/S1464793105006810. PMID 16221330. S2CID 24868603.
  13. ^ Edmunds, Malcolm (2012). "Deimatic Behavior". Springer. Retrieved 31 December 2012.
  14. ^ Smith, Ian (3 December 2012). "Octopus vulgaris. Dymantic display". The Conchological Society of Great Britain and Ireland. Retrieved 1 January 2013.
  15. ^ Caro, T. M. (1986). "The functions of stotting in Thomson's gazelles: Some tests of the predictions". Animal Behaviour. 34 (3): 663–684. doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(86)80052-5. S2CID 53155678.
  16. ^ Bildstein, Keith L. (May 1983). "Why White-Tailed Deer Flag Their Tails". The American Naturalist. 121 (5): 709–715. doi:10.1086/284096. JSTOR 2460873. S2CID 83504795.
  17. ^ Bergstrom, C. T.; Lachmann, M. (2001). "Alarm calls as costly signals of antipredator vigilance: the watchful babbler game". Animal Behaviour. 61 (3): 535–543. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.28.773. doi:10.1006/anbe.2000.1636. S2CID 2295026.
  18. ^ Getty, T. (2002). "The discriminating babbler meets the optimal diet hawk". Anim. Behav. 63 (2): 397–402. doi:10.1006/anbe.2001.1890. S2CID 53164940.
  19. ^ Pasteur, Georges (1982). "A classificatory review of mimicry systems". Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics. 13: 169–199. doi:10.1146/annurev.es.13.110182.001125.
  20. ^ Alboni, Paolo; Alboni, Marco; Bertorelle, Giorgio (2008). "The origin of vasovagal syncope: to protect the heart or to escape predation?". Clinical Autonomic Research. 18 (4): 170–8. doi:10.1007/s10286-008-0479-7. PMID 18592129. S2CID 7739227.
  21. ^ a b Inman, Mason (29 March 2005). "Sea Hares Lose Their Lunch". Sciencemag.org. from the original on 2021-09-24. Retrieved 10 May 2015.
  22. ^ Derby, Charles D. (December 2007). "Escape by Inking and Secreting: Marine Molluscs Avoid Predators Through a Rich Array of Chemicals and Mechanisms". The Biological Bulletin. 213 (3): 274–289. doi:10.2307/25066645. JSTOR 25066645. PMID 18083967. S2CID 9539618.
  23. ^ Derby, Charles D.; Kicklighter, Cynthia E.; Johnson, P. M. & Xu Zhang (29 March 2007). (PDF). Journal of Chemical Ecology. 2007 (33): 1105–1113. Bibcode:2007JCEco..33.1105D. doi:10.1007/s10886-007-9279-0. PMID 17393278. S2CID 92064. Archived from the original (PDF) on 15 November 2009. Retrieved 9 May 2015.
  24. ^ Barrows, Edward M. (2001). Animal behavior (2nd ed.). CRC Press. p. 177. ISBN 978-0-8493-2005-7.
  25. ^ a b Ruxton, Sherratt & Speed 2004, p. 198.
  26. ^ Endler, J. A. (1981). "An overview of the relationships between mimicry and crypsis". Biological Journal of the Linnean Society. 16: 25–31. doi:10.1111/j.1095-8312.1981.tb01840.x.
  27. ^ a b c Holmgren, H.; Enquist, M. (1999). "Dynamics of mimicry evolution". Biological Journal of the Linnean Society. 66 (2): 145–158. Bibcode:1999BJLS...66..145H. doi:10.1006/bijl.1998.0269.
  28. ^ a b Ritland, D. B. (1995). "Comparative unpalatability of mimetic viceroy butterflies (Limenitis archippus) from four south-eastern United States populations". Oecologia. 103 (3): 327–336. Bibcode:1995Oecol.103..327R. doi:10.1007/BF00328621. PMID 28306826. S2CID 13436225.
  29. ^ Bates, H. W. (1961). "Contributions to an insect fauna of the Amazon valley. Lepidoptera: Heliconidae". Transactions of the Linnean Society. 23 (3): 495–566. doi:10.1111/j.1096-3642.1860.tb00146.x.
  30. ^ Stearns, Stephen; Hoekstra, Rolf (2005). Evolution: An Introduction. Oxford University Press. p. 464.
  31. ^ Norman, Mark; Finn, Julian; Tregenza, Tom (September 7, 2001). "Dynamic mimicry in an Indo-Malayan octopus". Proceedings: Biological Sciences. 268 (1478): 1755–1758. doi:10.1098/rspb.2001.1708. PMC 1088805. PMID 11522192.
  32. ^ Hanlon, R.T.; Forsythe, J.W.; Joneschild, D.E. (1999). "Crypsis, conspicuousness, mimicry and polyphenism as antipredator defences of foraging octopuses on Indo-Pacific coral reefs, with a method of quantifying crypsis form video tapes". Biological Journal of the Linnean Society. 66 (1): 1–22. Bibcode:1999BJLS...66....1H. doi:10.1006/bijl.1998.0264.
  33. ^ Holen, O.H.; Johnstone, R. A. (2004). "The Evolution of Mimicry under Constraints". The American Naturalist. 164 (5): 598–613. doi:10.1086/424972. PMID 15540150. S2CID 8153271.
  34. ^ Norman, M.D.; Finn, J.; Tregenza, T. (2001). "Dynamic Mimicry in an Indo-Malayan Octopus". Proceedings: Biological Sciences. 268 (1478): 1755–1758. doi:10.1098/rspb.2001.1708. PMC 1088805. PMID 11522192.
  35. ^ Müller, Fritz (1879). "Ituna and Thyridia; a remarkable case of mimicry in butterflies. (R. Meldola translation.)". Proclamations of the Entomological Society of London. 1879: 20–29.
  36. ^ Thomas, Craig. Scott, Susan. (1997). All Stings Considered. University of Hawaii Press. pp. 96–97.
  37. ^ a b c Vincent, J. F. V.; Owers, P. (1986). "Mechanical design of hedgehog spines and porcupine quills". Journal of Zoology. 210: 55–75. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7998.1986.tb03620.x.
  38. ^ Warwick, Hugh (15 June 2014). Hedgehog. Reaktion Books. p. 10. ISBN 978-1-78023-315-4.
  39. ^ Murphy, Shannon M.; Leahy, Susannah M.; Williams, Laila S.; Lill, John T. (2010). "Stinging spines protect slug caterpillars (Limacodidae) from multiple generalist predators". Behavioral Ecology. 21 (1): 153–160. doi:10.1093/beheco/arp166. hdl:10.1093/beheco/arp166.
  40. ^ Edmunds 1974, pp. 202–207.
  41. ^ Duncan, P.; Vigne, N. (1979). "The effect of group size in horses on the rate of attacks by blood-sucking flies". Animal Behaviour. 27: 623–625. doi:10.1016/0003-3472(79)90201-x. S2CID 53154054.
  42. ^ Foster, W.A.; Treherne, J.E. (1981). "Evidence for the dilution effect in the selfish herd from fish predation on a marine insect". Nature. 295 (5832): 466–467. Bibcode:1981Natur.293..466F. doi:10.1038/293466a0. S2CID 4365789.
  43. ^ Hamilton, W. (1971). "Geometry for the selfish herd". Journal of Theoretical Biology. 31 (2): 295–311. Bibcode:1971JThBi..31..295H. doi:10.1016/0022-5193(71)90189-5. PMID 5104951.
  44. ^ De Vos, A.; O'Riain, M. J. (September 2009). "Sharks shape the geometry of a selfish seal herd: experimental evidence from seal decoys". Biology Letters. 6 (1): 48–50. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2009.0628. PMC 2817263. PMID 19793737.
  45. ^ Williams, K.S. & C. Simon (1995). "The ecology, behavior, and evolution of periodical cicadas" (PDF). Annual Review of Entomology. 40: 269–295. doi:10.1146/annurev.en.40.010195.001413.
  46. ^ Maynard Smith, John; Harper, David (2003). Animal Signals. Oxford University Press. pp. 113–121. ISBN 978-0-19852-685-8.
  47. ^ Caro 2005, pp. 115–149.
  48. ^ Pulliam, H. R. (1973). "On the advantages of flocking". J. Theor. Biol. 38 (2): 419–22. Bibcode:1973JThBi..38..419P. doi:10.1016/0022-5193(73)90184-7. PMID 4734745.
  49. ^ Bertram, Brian C. (1980). "Vigilance and group size in ostriches". Animal Behaviour. 28 (1): 278–286. doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(80)80030-3. S2CID 53144763.
  50. ^ Stevens, M; Searle, WT; Seymour, JE; Marshall, KL; Ruxton, GD (2011). "Motion dazzle and camouflage as distinct anti-predator defenses". BMC Biol. 9: 81. doi:10.1186/1741-7007-9-81. PMC 3257203. PMID 22117898.
  51. ^ a b c Ruxton, Sherratt & Speed 2004, pp. 64–69.
  52. ^ a b Stanford, Craig B (1995). "The influence of chimpanzee predation on group size and anti-predator behavior in red colobus monkeys". Animal Behaviour. 49 (3): 577–587. doi:10.1016/0003-3472(95)90033-0.
  53. ^ a b c d Warham, John (1977). "The Incidence, Functions and Ecological Significance of Petrel Stomach Oils" (PDF). New Zealand Ecological Society. 24: 84–93.
  54. ^ a b c Davidson, D.W.; Salim, K.A.; Billen, J. (2011). "A review on self-destructive defense behaviors in social insects" (PDF). Insectes Sociaux. 59: 1–10. doi:10.1007/s00040-011-0210-x. S2CID 13257903.
  55. ^ a b Hulme, P. E.; Benkman, C. W. (2002). Herrera, C. M.; Pellmyr, O. (eds.). Granivory. Blackwell. pp. 132–154. ISBN 978-0-632-05267-7. {{cite book}}: |work= ignored (help)
  56. ^ a b Edmunds 1974, pp. 189–201.
  57. ^ Eisner, Thomas; Jones, Tappey H.; Aneshansley, Daniel J.; Tschinkel, Walter R.; Silberglied, Robert E.; Meinwald, Jerrold (1977). "Chemistry of defensive secretions of bombardier beetles (Brachinini, Metriini, Ozaenini, Paussini)". Journal of Insect Physiology. 23 (11–12): 1383–1386. doi:10.1016/0022-1910(77)90162-7.
  58. ^ "See It to Believe It: Animals Vomit, Spurt Blood to Thwart Predators" 2017-09-14 at the Wayback Machine, Allison Bond, Discover Magazine blog, 28 July 2009, retrieved 17 March 2010
  59. ^ a b c d Hingston, R. W. G. (1927). "The liquid-squirting habit of oriental grasshoppers". Transactions of the Entomological Society of London. 75: 65–69. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2311.1927.tb00060.x.
  60. ^ Young, B. A.; Dunlap, K.; Koenig, K.; Singer, M. (September 2004). "The buccal buckle: The functional morphology of venom spitting in cobras". Journal of Experimental Biology. 207 (20): 3483–3494. doi:10.1242/jeb.01170. PMID 15339944.
  61. ^ Mayell, Hillary (February 10, 2005). . National Geographic. Archived from the original on November 10, 2005.
  62. ^ O. Wilson, Edward (2000). Sociobiology: the New Synthesis. Harvard University Press. pp. 302–305. ISBN 9780674000896.
  63. ^ Sherbrooke, W.C. (2001). "Do vertebral-line patterns in two horned lizards (Phrynosoma spp.) mimic plant-stem shadows and stem litter?". J Arid Environ. 50 (1): 109–120. Bibcode:2002JArEn..50..109S. doi:10.1006/jare.2001.0852.
  64. ^ Middendorf, George A.; Sherbrooke, Wade C. (1992). "Canid Elicitation of Blood-Squirting in a Horned Lizard (Phrynosoma Cornutum)". Copeia. 1992 (2): 519–27. doi:10.2307/1446212. JSTOR 1446212.
  65. ^ Pianka, Erika R. & Wendy L. Hodges. . University of Texas. Archived from the original on 29 April 2011. Retrieved 18 November 2013.
  66. ^ a b Sherbrooke, Wade C.; George, A. Middendorf III; Douglas, M. E. (2004). "Responses of Kit Foxes (Vulpes macrotis) to Antipredator Blood-Squirting and Blood of Texas Horned Lizards (Phrynosoma cornutum)". Copeia. 2004 (3): 652–658. doi:10.1643/ch-03-157r1. JSTOR 1448486. S2CID 55365586.
  67. ^ Lim, Jeanette; Fudge, Douglas F.; Levy, Nimrod; Gosline, John M. (2006). "Hagfish Slime Ecomechanics: testing the gill-clogging mechanics". The Journal of Experimental Biology. 209 (Pt 4): 702–10. doi:10.1242/jeb.02067. PMID 16449564.
  68. ^ Andersson, Malte; Wiklund, Christer G. (1978). "Clumping versus spacing out: Experiments on nest predation in fieldfares (Turdus pilaris)". Animal Behaviour. 26 (4): 1207–1212. doi:10.1016/0003-3472(78)90110-0. S2CID 53195968.
  69. ^ Parejo, D; Avilés, JM; Peña, A; Sánchez, L; Ruano, F; et al. (2013). "Armed Rollers: Does Nestling's Vomit Function as a Defence against Predators?". PLOS ONE. 8 (7): e68862. Bibcode:2013PLoSO...868862P. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0068862. PMC 3707886. PMID 23874791.
  70. ^ Peterson, Steven C., Nelson D. Johnson, and John L. LeGuyader (1987). "Defensive Regurgitation of Allelochemicals Derived From Host Cyanogenesis By Eastern Tent Caterpillars". Ecology. 68 (5): 1268–272. Bibcode:1987Ecol...68.1268P. doi:10.2307/1939211. JSTOR 1939211.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  71. ^ Smedley, Scott R., Elizabeth Ehrhardt, and Thomas Eisner (1993). "Defensive Regurgitation by a Noctuid Moth Larva (Litoprosopus futilis)". Psyche: A Journal of Entomology. 100 (3–4): 209–21. doi:10.1155/1993/67950.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  72. ^ Jones, T.H.; Clark, D.A.; Edwards, A.; Davidson, D.W.; Spande, T.F.; Snelling, R.R. (2004). "The chemistry of exploding ants, Camponotus spp. (Cylindricus complex)". Journal of Chemical Ecology. 30 (8): 1479–1492. Bibcode:2004JCEco..30.1479J. doi:10.1023/b:joec.0000042063.01424.28. PMID 15537154. S2CID 23756265.
  73. ^ a b Corlett, Richard T.; Primack, Richard B. (2011). Tropical rain forests : an ecological and biogeographical comparison (2nd ed.). Wiley-Blackwell. pp. 197, 200. ISBN 978-1444332551.
  74. ^ Bromley, Richard G. (2012). Trace Fossils: Biology, Taxonomy and Applications. Routledge. pp. 69–72. ISBN 978-1-135-07607-8.
  75. ^ Kruszelnicki, Karl S. (August 9, 1999). "Real Wheel Animals—Part Two". Great Moments in Science. ABC Science. from the original on October 1, 2016.
  76. ^ a b c d Edmunds 1974, pp. 145–149.
  77. ^ Edmunds 1974, pp. 179–181.
  78. ^ Aristotle (1910) [350 BC]. The History of Animals. Vol. IX. pp. 621b–622a.
  79. ^ Geffroy, Benjamin; Sadoul, Bastien; Putman, Breanna J.; Berger-Tal, Oded; Garamszegi, László Zsolt; Møller, Anders Pape; Blumstein, Daniel T. (22 September 2020). "Evolutionary dynamics in the Anthropocene: Life history and intensity of human contact shape antipredator responses". PLOS Biology. 18 (9): e3000818. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.3000818. ISSN 1545-7885. PMC 7508406. PMID 32960897. S2CID 221864354.

Sources edit

External links edit

    anti, predator, adaptation, mechanisms, developed, through, evolution, that, assist, prey, organisms, their, constant, struggle, against, predators, throughout, animal, kingdom, adaptations, have, evolved, every, stage, this, struggle, namely, avoiding, detect. Anti predator adaptations are mechanisms developed through evolution that assist prey organisms in their constant struggle against predators Throughout the animal kingdom adaptations have evolved for every stage of this struggle namely by avoiding detection warding off attack fighting back or escaping when caught Anti predator adaptation in action the seal shark Dalatias licha a c and the wreckfish Polyprion americanus d f attempt to prey on hagfishes First the predators approach their potential prey Predators bite or try to swallow the hagfishes but the hagfishes have already projected jets of slime arrows into the predators mouths Choking the predators release the hagfishes and gag in an attempt to remove slime from their mouths and gill chambers 1 The first line of defence consists in avoiding detection through mechanisms such as camouflage masquerade apostatic selection living underground or nocturnality Alternatively prey animals may ward off attack whether by advertising the presence of strong defences in aposematism by mimicking animals which do possess such defences by startling the attacker by signalling to the predator that pursuit is not worthwhile by distraction by using defensive structures such as spines and by living in a group Members of groups are at reduced risk of predation despite the increased conspicuousness of a group through improved vigilance predator confusion and the likelihood that the predator will attack some other individual Contents 1 Avoiding detection 1 1 Staying out of sight 1 2 Camouflage 1 3 Masquerade 1 4 Apostatic selection 2 Warding off attack 2 1 Startling the predator 2 2 Pursuit deterrent signals 2 3 Playing dead 2 4 Distraction 2 5 Mimicry and aposematism 2 6 Defensive structures 3 Safety in numbers 3 1 Dilution effect 3 2 Selfish herd 3 3 Predator satiation 3 4 Alarm calls 3 5 Improved vigilance 3 6 Predator confusion 4 Fighting back 4 1 Chemical defences 4 2 Communal defence 4 3 Defensive regurgitation 4 4 Suicidal altruism 5 Escaping 5 1 Flight 5 2 Autotomy 6 History of observations 7 See also 8 References 9 Sources 10 External linksAvoiding detection editFurther information Prey detection Staying out of sight edit Main article Nocturnality nbsp Fruit bats forage by night to avoid predators Animals may avoid becoming prey by living out of sight of predators whether in caves burrows or by being nocturnal 2 3 4 5 Nocturnality is an animal behavior characterized by activity during the night and sleeping during the day This is a behavioral form of detection avoidance called crypsis used by animals to either avoid predation or to enhance prey hunting Predation risk has long been recognized as critical in shaping behavioral decisions For example this predation risk is of prime importance in determining the time of evening emergence in echolocating bats Although early access during brighter times permits easier foraging it also leads to a higher predation risk from bat hawks and bat falcons This results in an optimum evening emergence time that is a compromise between the conflicting demands 4 Another nocturnal adaptation can be seen in kangaroo rats They forage in relatively open habitats and reduce their activity outside their nest burrows in response to moonlight During a full moon they shift their activity towards areas of relatively dense cover to compensate for the extra brightness 5 nbsp Camouflage illustrated by the flat tail horned lizard its flattened fringed and disruptively patterned body eliminating shadowCamouflage edit Main article Camouflage Camouflage uses any combination of materials coloration or illumination for concealment to make the organism hard to detect by sight It is common in both terrestrial and marine animals Camouflage can be achieved in many different ways such as through resemblance to surroundings disruptive coloration shadow elimination by countershading or counter illumination self decoration cryptic behavior or changeable skin patterns and colour 6 7 Animals such as the flat tail horned lizard of North America have evolved to eliminate their shadow and blend in with the ground The bodies of these lizards are flattened and their sides thin towards the edge This body form along with the white scales fringed along their sides allows the lizards to effectively hide their shadows In addition these lizards hide any remaining shadows by pressing their bodies to the ground 2 Masquerade edit nbsp Kallima inachus masquerading as a dead leafAnimals can hide in plain sight by masquerading as inedible objects For example the potoo a South American bird habitually perches on a tree convincingly resembling a broken stump of a branch 8 while a butterfly Kallima looks just like a dead leaf 9 Apostatic selection edit Another way to remain unattacked in plain sight is to look different from other members of the same species Predators such as tits selectively hunt for abundant types of insect ignoring less common types that were present forming search images of the desired prey This creates a mechanism for negative frequency dependent selection apostatic selection 10 Warding off attack edit nbsp A Mediterranean mantis Iris oratoria attempting to startle a predator with deimatic behaviourMany species make use of behavioral strategies to deter predators 11 Startling the predator edit Main article Deimatic behaviour Many weakly defended animals including moths butterflies mantises phasmids and cephalopods such as octopuses make use of patterns of threatening or startling behaviour such as suddenly displaying conspicuous eyespots so as to scare off or momentarily distract a predator thus giving the prey animal an opportunity to escape In the absence of toxins or other defences this is essentially bluffing in contrast to aposematism which involves honest signals 12 13 14 Pursuit deterrent signals edit Further information Signalling theory nbsp An impala stotting signalling honestly to the predator that the chase will be unprofitablePursuit deterrent signals are behavioral signals used by prey to convince predators not to pursue them For example gazelles stot jumping high with stiff legs and an arched back This is thought to signal to predators that they have a high level of fitness and can outrun the predator As a result predators may choose to pursue a different prey that is less likely to outrun them 15 White tailed deer and other prey mammals flag with conspicuous often black and white tail markings when alarmed informing the predator that it has been detected 16 Warning calls given by birds such as the Eurasian jay are similarly honest signals benefiting both predator and prey the predator is informed that it has been detected and might as well save time and energy by giving up the chase while the prey is protected from attack 17 18 Playing dead edit Main article apparent death nbsp Eastern hog nosed snake playing deadAnother pursuit deterrent signal is thanatosis or playing dead Thanatosis is a form of bluff in which an animal mimics its own dead body feigning death to avoid being attacked by predators seeking live prey Thanatosis can also be used by the predator in order to lure prey into approaching 19 An example of this is seen in white tailed deer fawns which experience a drop in heart rate in response to approaching predators This response referred to as alarm bradycardia causes the fawn s heart rate to drop from 155 to 38 beats per minute within one beat of the heart This drop in heart rate can last up to two minutes causing the fawn to experience a depressed breathing rate and decrease in movement called tonic immobility Tonic immobility is a reflex response that causes the fawn to enter a low body position that simulates the position of a corpse Upon discovery of the fawn the predator loses interest in the dead prey Other symptoms of alarm bradycardia such as salivation urination and defecation can also cause the predator to lose interest 20 Distraction edit nbsp A killdeer plover distracting a predator from its nest by feigning a broken wingMain articles phagomimicry and distraction display Marine molluscs such as sea hares cuttlefish squid and octopuses give themselves a last chance to escape by distracting their attackers To do this they eject a mixture of chemicals which may mimic food or otherwise confuse predators 21 22 In response to a predator animals in these groups release ink creating a cloud and opaline affecting the predator s feeding senses causing it to attack the cloud 21 23 Distraction displays attract the attention of predators away from an object typically the nest or young that is being protected 24 as when some birds feign a broken wing while hopping about on the ground 25 Mimicry and aposematism edit Main articles mimicry and aposematism nbsp Viceroy and monarch are Mullerian mimics similar in appearance unpalatable to predators Mimicry occurs when an organism the mimic simulates signal properties of another organism the model to confuse a third organism This results in the mimic gaining protection food and mating advantages 26 There are two classical types of defensive mimicry Batesian and Mullerian Both involve aposematic coloration or warning signals to avoid being attacked by a predator 27 28 In Batesian mimicry a palatable harmless prey species mimics the appearance of another species that is noxious to predators thus reducing the mimic s risk of attack 27 This form of mimicry is seen in many insects The idea behind Batesian mimicry is that predators that have tried to eat the unpalatable species learn to associate its colors and markings with an unpleasant taste This results in the predator learning to avoid species displaying similar colours and markings including Batesian mimics which are in effect parasitic on the chemical or other defences of the unprofitable models 29 30 Some species of octopus can mimic a selection of other animals by changing their skin color skin pattern and body motion When a damselfish attacks an octopus the octopus mimics a banded sea snake 31 The model chosen varies with the octopus s predator and habitat 32 Most of these octopuses use Batesian mimicry selecting an organism repulsive to predators as a model 33 34 In Mullerian mimicry two or more aposematic forms share the same warning signals 27 35 as in viceroy and monarch butterflies Birds avoid eating both species because their wing patterns honestly signal their unpleasant taste 28 nbsp The porcupine Erethizon dorsatum combines sharp spines with warning colorationDefensive structures edit Many animals are protected against predators with armour in the form of hard shells such as most molluscs and turtles leathery or scaly skin as in reptiles or tough chitinous exoskeletons as in arthropods 25 A spine is a sharp needle like structure used to inflict pain on predators An example of this seen in nature is in the sohal surgeonfish These fish have a sharp scalpel like spine on the front of each of their tail fins able to inflict deep wounds The area around the spines is often brightly colored to advertise the defensive capability 36 predators often avoid the Sohal surgeonfish 37 Defensive spines may be detachable barbed or poisonous Porcupine spines are long stiff break at the tip and in some species are barbed to stick into a would be predator In contrast the hedgehog s short spines which are modified hairs 38 readily bend and are barbed into the body so they are not easily lost they may be jabbed at an attacker 37 nbsp Stinging Limacodidae slug moth caterpillarsMany species of slug caterpillar Limacodidae have numerous protuberances and stinging spines along their dorsal surfaces Species that possess these stinging spines suffer less predation than larvae that lack them and a predator the paper wasp chooses larvae without spines when given a choice 39 Safety in numbers editFurther information Social animal Group living can decrease the risk of predation to the individual in a variety of ways 40 as described below Dilution effect edit A dilution effect is seen when animals living in a group dilute their risk of attack each individual being just one of many in the group George C Williams and W D Hamilton proposed that group living evolved because it provides benefits to the individual rather than to the group as a whole which becomes more conspicuous as it becomes larger One common example is the shoaling of fish Experiments provide direct evidence for the decrease in individual attack rate seen with group living for example in Camargue horses in Southern France The horse fly often attacks these horses sucking blood and carrying diseases When the flies are most numerous the horses gather in large groups and individuals are indeed attacked less frequently 41 Water striders are insects that live on the surface of fresh water and are attacked from beneath by predatory fish Experiments varying the group size of the water striders showed that the attack rate per individual water strider decreases as group size increases 42 nbsp In a group prey seek central positions in order to reduce their domain of danger Individuals along the outer edges of the group are more at risk of being targeted by the predator Selfish herd edit Main article Selfish herd theory The selfish herd theory was proposed by W D Hamilton to explain why animals seek central positions in a group 43 The theory s central idea is to reduce the individual s domain of danger A domain of danger is the area within the group in which the individual is more likely to be attacked by a predator The center of the group has the lowest domain of danger so animals are predicted to strive constantly to gain this position Testing Hamilton s selfish herd effect Alta De Vos and Justin O Rainn 2010 studied brown fur seal predation from great white sharks Using decoy seals the researchers varied the distance between the decoys to produce different domains of danger The seals with a greater domain of danger had an increased risk of shark attack 44 Predator satiation edit nbsp A newly emerged periodical cicada millions emerge at once at long intervals likely to satiate predators Main articles Predator satiation and Periodical cicada A radical strategy for avoiding predators which may otherwise kill a large majority of the emerging stage of a population is to emerge very rarely at irregular intervals Predators with a life cycle of one or a few years are unable to reproduce rapidly enough in response to such an emergence Predators may feast on the emerging population but are unable to consume more than a fraction of the brief surfeit of prey Periodical cicadas which emerge at intervals of 13 or 17 years are often used as an example of this predator satiation though other explanations of their unusual life cycle have been proposed 45 nbsp Vervet monkeys have different alarm signals that warn of attacks by eagles leopards and snakes Alarm calls edit Main article Alarm signal Animals that live in groups often give alarm calls that give warning of an attack For example vervet monkeys give different calls depending on the nature of the attack for an eagle a disyllabic cough for a leopard or other cat a loud bark for a python or other snake a chutter The monkeys hearing these calls respond defensively but differently in each case to the eagle call they look up and run into cover to the leopard call they run up into the trees to the snake call they stand on two legs and look around for snakes and on seeing the snake they sometimes mob it Similar calls are found in other species of monkey while birds also give different calls that elicit different responses 46 Improved vigilance edit nbsp A raptor a northern harrier chases up an alert flock of American avocets In the improved vigilance effect groups are able to detect predators sooner than solitary individuals 47 For many predators success depends on surprise If the prey is alerted early in an attack they have an improved chance of escape For example wood pigeon flocks are preyed upon by goshawks Goshawks are less successful when attacking larger flocks of wood pigeons than they are when attacking smaller flocks This is because the larger the flock size the more likely it is that one bird will notice the hawk sooner and fly away Once one pigeon flies off in alarm the rest of the pigeons follow 48 Wild ostriches in Tsavo National Park in Kenya feed either alone or in groups of up to four birds They are subject to predation by lions As the ostrich group size increases the frequency at which each individual raises its head to look for predators decreases Because ostriches are able to run at speeds that exceed those of lions for great distances lions try to attack an ostrich when its head is down By grouping the ostriches present the lions with greater difficulty in determining how long the ostriches heads stay down Thus although individual vigilance decreases the overall vigilance of the group increases 49 nbsp A single zebra is hard to catch amongst a herd Predator confusion edit Individuals living in large groups may be safer from attack because the predator may be confused by the large group size As the group moves the predator has greater difficulty targeting an individual prey animal The zebra has been suggested by the zoologist Martin Stevens and his colleagues as an example of this When stationary a single zebra stands out because of its large size To reduce the risk of attack zebras often travel in herds The striped patterns of all the zebras in the herd may confuse the predator making it harder for the predator to focus in on an individual zebra Furthermore when moving rapidly the zebra stripes create a confusing flickering motion dazzle effect in the eye of the predator 50 Fighting back editDefensive structures such as spines may be used both to ward off attack as already mentioned and if need be to fight back against a predator 37 Methods of fighting back include chemical defences 51 mobbing 52 defensive regurgitation 53 and suicidal altruism 54 Chemical defences edit nbsp The bloody nose beetle Timarcha tenebricosa exuding a drop of noxious red liquid upper right Further information autohaemorrhaging and bombardier beetle Many prey animals and to defend against seed predation also seeds of plants 55 make use of poisonous chemicals for self defence 51 56 These may be concentrated in surface structures such as spines or glands giving an attacker a taste of the chemicals before it actually bites or swallows the prey animal many toxins are bitter tasting 51 A last ditch defence is for the animal s flesh itself to be toxic as in the puffer fish danaid butterflies and burnet moths Many insects acquire toxins from their food plants Danaus caterpillars accumulate toxic cardenolides from milkweeds Asclepiadaceae 56 Some prey animals are able to eject noxious materials to deter predators actively The bombardier beetle has specialized glands on the tip of its abdomen that allows it to direct a toxic spray towards predators The spray is generated explosively through oxidation of hydroquinones and is sprayed at a temperature of 100 C 57 Armoured crickets similarly release blood at their joints when threatened autohaemorrhaging 58 Several species of grasshopper including Poecilocerus pictus 59 Parasanaa donovani 59 Aularches miliaris 59 and Tegra novaehollandiae secrete noxious liquids when threatened sometimes ejecting these forcefully 59 Spitting cobras accurately squirt venom from their fangs at the eyes of potential predators 60 striking their target eight times out of ten and causing severe pain 61 Termite soldiers in the Nasutitermitinae have a fontanellar gun a gland on the front of their head which can secrete and shoot an accurate jet of resinous terpenes many centimeters The material is sticky and toxic to other insects One of the terpenes in the secretion pinene functions as an alarm pheromone 62 Seeds deter predation with combinations of toxic non protein amino acids cyanogenic glycosides protease and amylase inhibitors and phytohemagglutinins 55 A few vertebrate species such as the Texas horned lizard are able to shoot squirts of blood from their eyes by rapidly increasing the blood pressure within the eye sockets if threatened Because an individual may lose up to 53 of blood in a single squirt 63 this is only used against persistent predators like foxes wolves and coyotes Canidae as a last defence 64 Canids often drop horned lizards after being squirted and attempt to wipe or shake the blood out of their mouths suggesting that the fluid has a foul taste 65 they choose other lizards if given the choice 66 suggesting a learned aversion towards horned lizards as prey 66 The slime glands along the body of the hagfish secrete enormous amounts of mucus when it is provoked or stressed The gelatinous slime has dramatic effects on the flow and viscosity of water rapidly clogging the gills of any fish that attempt to capture hagfish predators typically release the hagfish within seconds Common predators of hagfish include seabirds pinnipeds and cetaceans but few fish suggesting that predatory fish avoid hagfish as prey 67 Communal defence edit Further information Mobbing animal behavior nbsp Group of muskoxen in defensive formation horns ready and highly alertIn communal defence prey groups actively defend themselves by grouping together and sometimes by attacking or mobbing a predator rather than allowing themselves to be passive victims of predation Mobbing is the harassing of a predator by many prey animals Mobbing is usually done to protect the young in social colonies For example red colobus monkeys exhibit mobbing when threatened by chimpanzees a common predator The male red colobus monkeys group together and place themselves between predators and the group s females and juveniles The males jump together and actively bite the chimpanzees 52 Fieldfares are birds which may nest either solitarily or in colonies Within colonies fieldfares mob and defecate on approaching predators shown experimentally to reduce predation levels 68 Defensive regurgitation edit nbsp A northern fulmar chick protects itself with a jet of stomach oil Some birds and insects use defensive regurgitation to ward off predators The northern fulmar vomits a bright orange oily substance called stomach oil when threatened 53 The stomach oil is made from their aquatic diets It causes the predator s feathers to mat leading to the loss of flying ability and the loss of water repellency 53 This is especially dangerous for aquatic birds because their water repellent feathers protect them from hypothermia when diving for food 53 European roller chicks vomit a bright orange foul smelling liquid when they sense danger This repels prospective predators and may alert their parents to danger they respond by delaying their return 69 Numerous insects utilize defensive regurgitation The eastern tent caterpillar regurgitates a droplet of digestive fluid to repel attacking ants 70 Similarly larvae of the noctuid moth regurgitate when disturbed by ants The vomit of noctuid moths has repellent and irritant properties that help to deter predator attacks 71 Suicidal altruism edit Further information Autothysis An unusual type of predator deterrence is observed in the Malaysian exploding ant Social hymenoptera rely on altruism to protect the entire colony so the self destructive acts benefit all individuals in the colony 54 When a worker ant s leg is grasped it suicidally expels the contents of its hypertrophied submandibular glands 54 expelling corrosive irritant compounds and adhesives onto the predator These prevent predation and serve as a signal to other enemy ants to stop predation of the rest of the colony 72 Escaping edit nbsp Startled pheasants and partridges fly from possible danger Flight edit The normal reaction of a prey animal to an attacking predator is to flee by any available means whether flying gliding 73 falling swimming running jumping burrowing 74 or rolling 75 according to the animal s capabilities 76 Escape paths are often erratic making it difficult for the predator to predict which way the prey will go next for example birds such as snipe ptarmigan and black headed gulls evade fast raptors such as peregrine falcons with zigzagging or jinking flight 76 In the tropical rain forests of Southeast Asia in particular many vertebrates escape predators by falling and gliding 73 Among the insects many moths turn sharply fall or perform a powered dive in response to the sonar clicks of bats 76 Among fish the stickleback follows a zigzagging path often doubling back erratically when chased by a fish eating merganser duck 76 Autotomy edit Main article Autotomy nbsp Lizard tail autotomy can distract predators continuing to writhe while the lizard makes its escape Some animals are capable of autotomy self amputation shedding one of their own appendages in a last ditch attempt to elude a predator s grasp or to distract the predator and thereby allow escape The lost body part may be regenerated later Certain sea slugs discard stinging papillae arthropods such as crabs can sacrifice a claw which can be regrown over several successive moults among vertebrates many geckos and other lizards shed their tails when attacked the tail goes on writhing for a while distracting the predator and giving the lizard time to escape a smaller tail slowly regrows 77 History of observations editAristotle recorded observations around 350 BC of the antipredator behaviour of cephalopods in his History of Animals including the use of ink as a distraction camouflage and signalling 78 In 1940 Hugh Cott wrote a compendious study of camouflage mimicry and aposematism Adaptive Coloration in Animals 6 By the 21st century adaptation to life in cities had markedly reduced the antipredator responses of animals such as rats and pigeons similar changes are observed in captive and domesticated animals 79 See also editEcology of fear concept Psychological impact induced by predatorsPages displaying short descriptions of redirect targets Plant defense against herbivory Plants defenses against being eatenReferences edit Zintzen Vincent Roberts Clive D Anderson Marti J Stewart Andrew L Struthers Carl D Harvey Euan S 2011 Hagfish Slime as a Defense Mechanism against Gill breathing Predators Scientific Reports 1 2011 Bibcode 2011NatSR 1E 131Z doi 10 1038 srep00131 PMC 3216612 PMID 22355648 a b Sherbrooke W C 2003 Introduction to horned lizards of North America University of California Press pp 117 118 Cott H B 1940 Adaptive Coloration in Animals London Methuen pp 330 335 a b Duverge P L Jones G Rydell J Ransome R 2000 Functional significance of emergence timing in bats Ecography 23 1 32 40 Bibcode 2000Ecogr 23 32D doi 10 1111 j 1600 0587 2000 tb00258 x a b Daly M Behrends P R Wilson M Jacobs L 1992 Behavioural modulation of predation risk moonlight avoidance and crepuscular compensation in a nocturnal desert rodent Dipodomys merriami Animal Behaviour 44 1 9 doi 10 1016 s0003 3472 05 80748 1 S2CID 4077513 a b Cott 1940 Caro 2005 pp 35 60 Caro 2005 pp 53 55 Cott 1940 pp 318 320 Caro 2005 pp 61 65 Cooper William E Antipredatory Behavior IDEA University of California Riverside Archived from the original on 18 May 2018 Retrieved 23 October 2014 Stevens Martin 2005 The role of eyespots as anti predator mechanisms principally demonstrated in the Lepidoptera Biological Reviews 80 4 573 588 doi 10 1017 S1464793105006810 PMID 16221330 S2CID 24868603 Edmunds Malcolm 2012 Deimatic Behavior Springer Retrieved 31 December 2012 Smith Ian 3 December 2012 Octopus vulgaris Dymantic display The Conchological Society of Great Britain and Ireland Retrieved 1 January 2013 Caro T M 1986 The functions of stotting in Thomson s gazelles Some tests of the predictions Animal Behaviour 34 3 663 684 doi 10 1016 S0003 3472 86 80052 5 S2CID 53155678 Bildstein Keith L May 1983 Why White Tailed Deer Flag Their Tails The American Naturalist 121 5 709 715 doi 10 1086 284096 JSTOR 2460873 S2CID 83504795 Bergstrom C T Lachmann M 2001 Alarm calls as costly signals of antipredator vigilance the watchful babbler game Animal Behaviour 61 3 535 543 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 28 773 doi 10 1006 anbe 2000 1636 S2CID 2295026 Getty T 2002 The discriminating babbler meets the optimal diet hawk Anim Behav 63 2 397 402 doi 10 1006 anbe 2001 1890 S2CID 53164940 Pasteur Georges 1982 A classificatory review of mimicry systems Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 13 169 199 doi 10 1146 annurev es 13 110182 001125 Alboni Paolo Alboni Marco Bertorelle Giorgio 2008 The origin of vasovagal syncope to protect the heart or to escape predation Clinical Autonomic Research 18 4 170 8 doi 10 1007 s10286 008 0479 7 PMID 18592129 S2CID 7739227 a b Inman Mason 29 March 2005 Sea Hares Lose Their Lunch Sciencemag org Archived from the original on 2021 09 24 Retrieved 10 May 2015 Derby Charles D December 2007 Escape by Inking and Secreting Marine Molluscs Avoid Predators Through a Rich Array of Chemicals and Mechanisms The Biological Bulletin 213 3 274 289 doi 10 2307 25066645 JSTOR 25066645 PMID 18083967 S2CID 9539618 Derby Charles D Kicklighter Cynthia E Johnson P M amp Xu Zhang 29 March 2007 Chemical Composition of Inks of Diverse Marine Molluscs Suggests Convergent Chemical Defenses PDF Journal of Chemical Ecology 2007 33 1105 1113 Bibcode 2007JCEco 33 1105D doi 10 1007 s10886 007 9279 0 PMID 17393278 S2CID 92064 Archived from the original PDF on 15 November 2009 Retrieved 9 May 2015 Barrows Edward M 2001 Animal behavior 2nd ed CRC Press p 177 ISBN 978 0 8493 2005 7 a b Ruxton Sherratt amp Speed 2004 p 198 Endler J A 1981 An overview of the relationships between mimicry and crypsis Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 16 25 31 doi 10 1111 j 1095 8312 1981 tb01840 x a b c Holmgren H Enquist M 1999 Dynamics of mimicry evolution Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 66 2 145 158 Bibcode 1999BJLS 66 145H doi 10 1006 bijl 1998 0269 a b Ritland D B 1995 Comparative unpalatability of mimetic viceroy butterflies Limenitis archippus from four south eastern United States populations Oecologia 103 3 327 336 Bibcode 1995Oecol 103 327R doi 10 1007 BF00328621 PMID 28306826 S2CID 13436225 Bates H W 1961 Contributions to an insect fauna of the Amazon valley Lepidoptera Heliconidae Transactions of the Linnean Society 23 3 495 566 doi 10 1111 j 1096 3642 1860 tb00146 x Stearns Stephen Hoekstra Rolf 2005 Evolution An Introduction Oxford University Press p 464 Norman Mark Finn Julian Tregenza Tom September 7 2001 Dynamic mimicry in an Indo Malayan octopus Proceedings Biological Sciences 268 1478 1755 1758 doi 10 1098 rspb 2001 1708 PMC 1088805 PMID 11522192 Hanlon R T Forsythe J W Joneschild D E 1999 Crypsis conspicuousness mimicry and polyphenism as antipredator defences of foraging octopuses on Indo Pacific coral reefs with a method of quantifying crypsis form video tapes Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 66 1 1 22 Bibcode 1999BJLS 66 1H doi 10 1006 bijl 1998 0264 Holen O H Johnstone R A 2004 The Evolution of Mimicry under Constraints The American Naturalist 164 5 598 613 doi 10 1086 424972 PMID 15540150 S2CID 8153271 Norman M D Finn J Tregenza T 2001 Dynamic Mimicry in an Indo Malayan Octopus Proceedings Biological Sciences 268 1478 1755 1758 doi 10 1098 rspb 2001 1708 PMC 1088805 PMID 11522192 Muller Fritz 1879 Ituna and Thyridia a remarkable case of mimicry in butterflies R Meldola translation Proclamations of the Entomological Society of London 1879 20 29 Thomas Craig Scott Susan 1997 All Stings Considered University of Hawaii Press pp 96 97 a b c Vincent J F V Owers P 1986 Mechanical design of hedgehog spines and porcupine quills Journal of Zoology 210 55 75 doi 10 1111 j 1469 7998 1986 tb03620 x Warwick Hugh 15 June 2014 Hedgehog Reaktion Books p 10 ISBN 978 1 78023 315 4 Murphy Shannon M Leahy Susannah M Williams Laila S Lill John T 2010 Stinging spines protect slug caterpillars Limacodidae from multiple generalist predators Behavioral Ecology 21 1 153 160 doi 10 1093 beheco arp166 hdl 10 1093 beheco arp166 Edmunds 1974 pp 202 207 Duncan P Vigne N 1979 The effect of group size in horses on the rate of attacks by blood sucking flies Animal Behaviour 27 623 625 doi 10 1016 0003 3472 79 90201 x S2CID 53154054 Foster W A Treherne J E 1981 Evidence for the dilution effect in the selfish herd from fish predation on a marine insect Nature 295 5832 466 467 Bibcode 1981Natur 293 466F doi 10 1038 293466a0 S2CID 4365789 Hamilton W 1971 Geometry for the selfish herd Journal of Theoretical Biology 31 2 295 311 Bibcode 1971JThBi 31 295H doi 10 1016 0022 5193 71 90189 5 PMID 5104951 De Vos A O Riain M J September 2009 Sharks shape the geometry of a selfish seal herd experimental evidence from seal decoys Biology Letters 6 1 48 50 doi 10 1098 rsbl 2009 0628 PMC 2817263 PMID 19793737 Williams K S amp C Simon 1995 The ecology behavior and evolution of periodical cicadas PDF Annual Review of Entomology 40 269 295 doi 10 1146 annurev en 40 010195 001413 Maynard Smith John Harper David 2003 Animal Signals Oxford University Press pp 113 121 ISBN 978 0 19852 685 8 Caro 2005 pp 115 149 Pulliam H R 1973 On the advantages of flocking J Theor Biol 38 2 419 22 Bibcode 1973JThBi 38 419P doi 10 1016 0022 5193 73 90184 7 PMID 4734745 Bertram Brian C 1980 Vigilance and group size in ostriches Animal Behaviour 28 1 278 286 doi 10 1016 S0003 3472 80 80030 3 S2CID 53144763 Stevens M Searle WT Seymour JE Marshall KL Ruxton GD 2011 Motion dazzle and camouflage as distinct anti predator defenses BMC Biol 9 81 doi 10 1186 1741 7007 9 81 PMC 3257203 PMID 22117898 a b c Ruxton Sherratt amp Speed 2004 pp 64 69 a b Stanford Craig B 1995 The influence of chimpanzee predation on group size and anti predator behavior in red colobus monkeys Animal Behaviour 49 3 577 587 doi 10 1016 0003 3472 95 90033 0 a b c d Warham John 1977 The Incidence Functions and Ecological Significance of Petrel Stomach Oils PDF New Zealand Ecological Society 24 84 93 a b c Davidson D W Salim K A Billen J 2011 A review on self destructive defense behaviors in social insects PDF Insectes Sociaux 59 1 10 doi 10 1007 s00040 011 0210 x S2CID 13257903 a b Hulme P E Benkman C W 2002 Herrera C M Pellmyr O eds Granivory Blackwell pp 132 154 ISBN 978 0 632 05267 7 a href Template Cite book html title Template Cite book cite book a work ignored help a b Edmunds 1974 pp 189 201 Eisner Thomas Jones Tappey H Aneshansley Daniel J Tschinkel Walter R Silberglied Robert E Meinwald Jerrold 1977 Chemistry of defensive secretions of bombardier beetles Brachinini Metriini Ozaenini Paussini Journal of Insect Physiology 23 11 12 1383 1386 doi 10 1016 0022 1910 77 90162 7 See It to Believe It Animals Vomit Spurt Blood to Thwart Predators Archived 2017 09 14 at the Wayback Machine Allison Bond Discover Magazine blog 28 July 2009 retrieved 17 March 2010 a b c d Hingston R W G 1927 The liquid squirting habit of oriental grasshoppers Transactions of the Entomological Society of London 75 65 69 doi 10 1111 j 1365 2311 1927 tb00060 x Young B A Dunlap K Koenig K Singer M September 2004 The buccal buckle The functional morphology of venom spitting in cobras Journal of Experimental Biology 207 20 3483 3494 doi 10 1242 jeb 01170 PMID 15339944 Mayell Hillary February 10 2005 Cobras Spit Venom at Eyes With Nearly Perfect Aim National Geographic Archived from the original on November 10 2005 O Wilson Edward 2000 Sociobiology the New Synthesis Harvard University Press pp 302 305 ISBN 9780674000896 Sherbrooke W C 2001 Do vertebral line patterns in two horned lizards Phrynosoma spp mimic plant stem shadows and stem litter J Arid Environ 50 1 109 120 Bibcode 2002JArEn 50 109S doi 10 1006 jare 2001 0852 Middendorf George A Sherbrooke Wade C 1992 Canid Elicitation of Blood Squirting in a Horned Lizard Phrynosoma Cornutum Copeia 1992 2 519 27 doi 10 2307 1446212 JSTOR 1446212 Pianka Erika R amp Wendy L Hodges Horned Lizards University of Texas Archived from the original on 29 April 2011 Retrieved 18 November 2013 a b Sherbrooke Wade C George A Middendorf III Douglas M E 2004 Responses of Kit Foxes Vulpes macrotis to Antipredator Blood Squirting and Blood of Texas Horned Lizards Phrynosoma cornutum Copeia 2004 3 652 658 doi 10 1643 ch 03 157r1 JSTOR 1448486 S2CID 55365586 Lim Jeanette Fudge Douglas F Levy Nimrod Gosline John M 2006 Hagfish Slime Ecomechanics testing the gill clogging mechanics The Journal of Experimental Biology 209 Pt 4 702 10 doi 10 1242 jeb 02067 PMID 16449564 Andersson Malte Wiklund Christer G 1978 Clumping versus spacing out Experiments on nest predation in fieldfares Turdus pilaris Animal Behaviour 26 4 1207 1212 doi 10 1016 0003 3472 78 90110 0 S2CID 53195968 Parejo D Aviles JM Pena A Sanchez L Ruano F et al 2013 Armed Rollers Does Nestling s Vomit Function as a Defence against Predators PLOS ONE 8 7 e68862 Bibcode 2013PLoSO 868862P doi 10 1371 journal pone 0068862 PMC 3707886 PMID 23874791 Peterson Steven C Nelson D Johnson and John L LeGuyader 1987 Defensive Regurgitation of Allelochemicals Derived From Host Cyanogenesis By Eastern Tent Caterpillars Ecology 68 5 1268 272 Bibcode 1987Ecol 68 1268P doi 10 2307 1939211 JSTOR 1939211 a href Template Cite journal html title Template Cite journal cite journal a CS1 maint multiple names authors list link Smedley Scott R Elizabeth Ehrhardt and Thomas Eisner 1993 Defensive Regurgitation by a Noctuid Moth Larva Litoprosopus futilis Psyche A Journal of Entomology 100 3 4 209 21 doi 10 1155 1993 67950 a href Template Cite journal html title Template Cite journal cite journal a CS1 maint multiple names authors list link Jones T H Clark D A Edwards A Davidson D W Spande T F Snelling R R 2004 The chemistry of exploding ants Camponotus spp Cylindricus complex Journal of Chemical Ecology 30 8 1479 1492 Bibcode 2004JCEco 30 1479J doi 10 1023 b joec 0000042063 01424 28 PMID 15537154 S2CID 23756265 a b Corlett Richard T Primack Richard B 2011 Tropical rain forests an ecological and biogeographical comparison 2nd ed Wiley Blackwell pp 197 200 ISBN 978 1444332551 Bromley Richard G 2012 Trace Fossils Biology Taxonomy and Applications Routledge pp 69 72 ISBN 978 1 135 07607 8 Kruszelnicki Karl S August 9 1999 Real Wheel Animals Part Two Great Moments in Science ABC Science Archived from the original on October 1 2016 a b c d Edmunds 1974 pp 145 149 Edmunds 1974 pp 179 181 Aristotle 1910 350 BC The History of Animals Vol IX pp 621b 622a Geffroy Benjamin Sadoul Bastien Putman Breanna J Berger Tal Oded Garamszegi Laszlo Zsolt Moller Anders Pape Blumstein Daniel T 22 September 2020 Evolutionary dynamics in the Anthropocene Life history and intensity of human contact shape antipredator responses PLOS Biology 18 9 e3000818 doi 10 1371 journal pbio 3000818 ISSN 1545 7885 PMC 7508406 PMID 32960897 S2CID 221864354 Sources editCaro Tim 2005 Antipredator Defenses in Birds and Mammals University of Chicago Press Cott Hugh 1940 Adaptive Coloration in Animals Oxford University Press Edmunds Malcolm 1974 Defence in Animals Longman Ruxton Graeme D Sherratt Thomas N Speed Michael P 2004 Avoiding Attack The Evolutionary Ecology of Crypsis Warning Signals and Mimicry Oxford External links editAntipredatory Behavior lecture by William E Cooper Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Anti predator adaptation amp oldid 1207317571, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

    article

    , read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.