fbpx
Wikipedia

Nominative–accusative alignment

In linguistic typology, nominative–accusative alignment is a type of morphosyntactic alignment in which subjects of intransitive verbs are treated like subjects of transitive verbs, and are distinguished from objects of transitive verbs in basic clause constructions. Nominative–accusative alignment can be coded by case-marking, verb agreement and/or word order. It has a wide global distribution and is the most common alignment system among the world's languages (including English). Languages with nominative–accusative alignment are commonly called nominative–accusative languages.

Nominative–accusative alignment

Comparison with other alignment types edit

A transitive verb is associated with two noun phrases (or arguments): a subject and a direct object. An intransitive verb is associated with only one argument, a subject. The different kinds of arguments are usually represented as S, A, and O. S is the sole argument of an intransitive verb, A is the subject (or most agent-like) argument of a transitive verb, and O is the direct object (or most patient-like) argument of a transitive verb. English has nominative–accusative alignment in its case marking of personal pronouns:[1] the single argument (S) of an intransitive verb ("I" in the sentence "I walked.") behaves grammatically like the agent (A) of a transitive verb ("I" in the sentence "I saw them.") but differently from the object (O) of a transitive verb ("me" in the sentence “they saw me.").

  Nominative–accusative Ergative–absolutive Tripartite
A same different different
S same different
O different different

This is in contrast with ergative–absolutive alignment, where S is coded in the same way as O, while A receives distinct marking, or tripartite alignment, where A, S and O all are coded in a different manner.

Split ergativity edit

It is common for languages (such as Georgian and Hindustani) to have overlapping alignment systems, which exhibit both nominative–accusative and ergative–absolutive coding, a phenomenon called split ergativity. In fact, there are relatively few languages that exhibit only ergative–absolutive alignment (called pure ergativity) and tend to be isolated in certain regions of the world, such as the Caucasus, parts of North America and Mesoamerica, the Tibetan Plateau, and Australia. Such languages include Sumerian, Standard Tibetan, and Mayan.[2]

Coding properties of nominative–accusative alignment edit

Nominative–accusative alignment can manifest itself in visible ways, called coding properties. Often, these visible properties are morphological and the distinction will appear as a difference in the actual morphological form and spelling of the word, or as case particles (pieces of morphology) which will appear before or after the word.

Case marking edit

If a language exhibits morphological case marking, arguments S and A will appear in the nominative case and argument O will appear in the accusative case, or in a similar case such as the oblique. There may be more than one case fulfilling the accusative role; for instance, Finnish marks objects with the partitive or the accusative to contrast telicity. It is highly common for only accusative arguments to exhibit overt case marking while nominative arguments exhibit null (or absent) case markings. In Modern English, case marking is only found with first and (non-neuter) third person pronouns, which have distinct subject and object forms.

English

I

1SG:SBJ

walked.

walk:PAST

I walked.

1SG:SBJ walk:PAST

I

1SG:SBJ

saw

see:PAST

them.

3PL:OBJ

I saw them.

1SG:SBJ see:PAST 3PL:OBJ

Japanese

花瓶が

Kabin-ga(S)

vase-NOM

壊れた

kowareta

broke

[3]

 

 

花瓶が 壊れた

Kabin-ga(S) kowareta

vase-NOM broke

‘A vase broke’

私は

Watashi-wa(S)

I-NOM

花瓶を

kabin-wo(O)

vase-ACC

壊した

kowashita

broke

私は 花瓶を 壊した

Watashi-wa(S) kabin-wo(O) kowashita

I-NOM vase-ACC broke

‘I broke the vase’

Russian

Девушка-Ø

Dyevushka-Ø

(adolescent-/youth-)girl-NOM

работа-ет

rabota-yet

work

Девушка-Ø работа-ет

Dyevushka-Ø rabota-yet

(adolescent-/youth-)girl-NOM work

‘A/The (adolescent/youth) girl/young lady/young woman works/is working’

Студент-Ø

Studyent-Ø

student-NOM

читает

chitayet

read-3.SG.PRES

книг-у

knig-u

book-ACC

Студент-Ø читает книг-у

Studyent-Ø chitayet knig-u

student-NOM read-3.SG.PRES book-ACC

‘A/The student read/is reading a/the book’

Sanskrit

Áśva-ḥ(S)

horse-NOM

aghnata

slain

Áśva-ḥ(S) aghnata

horse-NOM slain

‘A horse was slain’

Vīrá-ḥ(S)

man-NOM

áśva-m(O)

horse-ACC

ahan

slew

Vīrá-ḥ(S) áśva-m(O) ahan

man-NOM horse-ACC slew

‘The man slew a horse’

Differential object marking (DOM) edit

Not all arguments are equally likely to exhibit overt case marking. In languages with nominative–accusative alignment, it is common to divide direct objects into two classes (with respect to overt case marking), a phenomenon called ‘differential object marking’ by Bossong (1985).

Word order edit

Some languages code very little through morphology and are more dependent on syntax to encode meaning and grammatical relationships. If a language relies less on overt case marking, alignment may be coded through word order, as in this example from Indonesian.

Indonesian

sayai

1SG

mei-mandi-kan

AT-wash-APPL

pria

man

itu

that

[4]

 

sayai mei-mandi-kan pria itu

1SG AT-wash-APPL man that

‘I bathe that man’

In the following example from French, all subjects, both S and A, appear before the verb while O appears after the verb. Arguments occurring before the verb are coded as nominative, while arguments occurring directly after the verb are coded as accusative.

French

Je(S)

I-NOM

travaille

work

Je(S) travaille

I-NOM work

‘I work’

Je(A)

I-NOM

jette

throw

un

a

ballon(O)

ball-ACC

Je(A) jette un ballon(O)

I-NOM throw a ball-ACC

‘I throw a ball’

Verb agreement edit

Alternatively, alignment can also manifest visibly through agreement on the verb. In the following example from Amharic, the verb can be head-marked for S, A, and O. Both S in the intransitive clause and A in the transitive clause are marked by the same affix ( ‘3SG.M’), while O in the transitive clause is marked by a different affix (-w ‘3SG.M.O’).[4]

Amharic
intransitive

Ləmma

Lemma

hed-ə

go.PFV-3SG.M

Ləmma hed-ə

Lemma go.PFV-3SG.M

‘Lemma came’

transitive

Ləmma

Lemma

t’ərmus-u-n

bottle-DEF-ACC

səbbər-ə-w

break.PFV-3SG.M-3SG.M.O

Ləmma t’ərmus-u-n səbbər-ə-w

Lemma bottle-DEF-ACC break.PFV-3SG.M-3SG.M.O

‘Lemma breaks the bottle’

English has residual verb agreement with nominative–accusative alignment, which is only manifest with third person singular S and A in present tense.[5]

Behavioral properties of accusativity edit

Nominative–accusative alignment can also be distinguished through behavioral properties, in the way a nominative or accusative argument will behave when placed in particular syntactic constructions. This has to do with the impact of alignment on the level of the whole sentence rather than the individual word. Morphosyntactic alignment determines which arguments can be omitted in a coordinate structure during the process of conjunction reduction (deleting arguments from the ends of joined clauses). In nominative–accusative, only arguments S and A can be omitted and not argument O.

English

a. Sue-NOMi saw Judy-ACCj , and shei/j ran.
b. Suei saw Judyj and ___i/*j ran.
c. Suei saw Judyj, and shei/j was frightened.
d. Suei saw Judyj and ___i/*j was frightened.

The omitted subject argument of the embedded clause must correspond to the subject (nominative) of the matrix-clause. If it corresponds to the object (accusative), the sentence is ungrammatical.

If English were an ergative–absolutive language, one would expect to see:

b’. Suei saw Judyj, and ___*i/j ran.
c’. Suei saw Judyj, and ___*i/j was frightened.

Here the omitted argument of the embedded clause corresponds to the direct object (absolutive) of the matrix-clause. If it corresponds to the subject (ergative), the sentence is ungrammatical.

The alignment system also impacts the triggering and realization of other such syntactic processes as raising constructions, subject-controlled subject deletion and object-controlled subject deletion.

Distribution edit

 
Distribution of languages by alignment type

Languages exhibiting accusative alignment are the most widespread of all of the alignment types. These languages can be found on every continent, in comparison to languages with ergative alignment that are restricted to certain areas of the world, namely the Caucasus, parts of North American and Mesoamerica, the Tibetan plateau, and Australia. The map shows the distribution of languages with the various alignment types, and the following list gives a short sampling of accusative languages and their distribution across the globe:[6]

North America:

Australasia:

South America:

Europe:

Africa:

Asia:

Relevant theory edit

Optimality theory edit

One of the ways in which the production of a nominative–accusative case marking system can be explained is from an Optimality Theoretic perspective. Case marking is said to fulfill two functions, or constraints: an identifying function and a distinguishing function.[7] The identifying function is exemplified when case morphology encodes (identifies) specific semantic, thematic, or pragmatic properties or information about the nominal argument. Accusative case in the position of the direct object, for example, can be a strong identifier of patienthood. The distinguishing function is used to distinguish between the core arguments, the subject and the object, of a transitive clause. Helen de Hoop and Andrej Malchukov explain the motivation and need for the distinguishing function in "Case marking strategies":

When a two-place predicate R(x,y) is used to describe an event involving two participants, usually an agent and a patient, it is of utmost importance to avoid ambiguity as to which noun phrase corresponds to the first argument x (the agent) and which to the second argument y (the patient). For this purpose, case can be used to mark one of the arguments. If one argument is case marked, this already suffices for the purpose of disambiguation. Thus, from the distinguishing perspective, there is no need to case mark both arguments. Neither would it be necessary to case mark the one and only argument of a one-place (intransitive) predicate. Indeed, it has been argued that in many nominative–accusative case systems only the y is case marked (with accusative case) while the x remains morphologically unmarked.[7]

It is rare for case to serve only the distinguishing function, which overlaps greatly with the ‘identify’ function. Other ways of disambiguating the arguments of a transitive predicate (subject agreement, word order restriction, context, intonation, etc.) may explain this cross-linguistic observation. De Hoop and Malchukov argue that case systems that are completely based on the identification function must be richer in case morphology compared to languages based mainly on the distinguishing function.

Functional pressure edit

One theory that has been posited to account for the occurrence of accusative systems is that of functional pressure. When applied to languages, this theory operates around the various needs and pressures on a speech community. It has been suggested that languages have evolved to suit the needs of their users.These communities will develop some functional system to meet the needs that they have. So, it has been proposed that the accusative system arose from a functional pressure to avoid ambiguity and make communication a simpler process.[8][9]

It is useful for languages to have a means of distinguishing between subjects and objects, and between arguments A, S, and O. This is helpful so that sentences like "Tom hit Fred" cannot be interpreted as "Fred hit Tom." Tripartite alignment systems accomplish this differentiation by coding S, A and O all differently. However, this is not structurally economical, and tripartite systems are comparatively rare, but to have all arguments marked the same makes the arguments too ambiguous. Alongside the principle of distinguishability seems to operate a principle of economy. It is more efficient to have as few cases as possible without compromising intelligibility. In this way the dual pressures of efficiency and economy have produced a system which patterns two kinds of arguments together a third separately. Both accusative and ergative systems use this kind of grouping to make meaning clearer.


See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ Donohue, Mark; Wichmann, Søren, eds. (2005), The Typology of Semantic Alignment, Oxford: Oxford University Press, p. 25, ISBN 9780199238385
  2. ^ Van Valin, Robert D. (2001). An Introduction to Syntax. Cambridge University Press. ISBN 9780521635660.
  3. ^ Tsujimura, Natsuko (2007). An introduction to Japanese linguistics. Wiley-Blackwell. p. 382. ISBN 978-1-4051-1065-5.
  4. ^ a b van de Visser, Mario. (2006) "The Marked Status of Ergativity". PhD. Dissertation.
  5. ^ Bickel, Balthasar; Iemmolo, Giorgio; Zakharko, Taras; Witzlack-Makarevich, Alena (2013). "Patterns of alignment in verb agreement". In: Bakker, Dik; Haspelmath, Martin. Languages across boundaries: Studies in memory of Anna Siewierska. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton, 15-36.
  6. ^ Dryer, Matthew S. & Haspelmath, Martin (eds.). (2011) "The World Atlas of Language Structures Online". Munich: Max Planck Digital Library. Available online at WALS
  7. ^ a b de Hoop, Helen and Malchukov, Andrej L. (2008) "Case-marking strategies". Linguistic Inquiry.
  8. ^ Bates, E., & MacWhinney, B. (1982) Functionalist approaches to grammar. In E.Wanner, & L. Gleitman (Ed.), Language acquisition: The state of the art. New York: Cambridge University Press.
  9. ^ Fedzechkina, Maryia & Jaeger, T. Florian & Newport, Elissa L. (2011) "Functional Biases in Language Learning: Evidence from Word Order and Case-Marking Interaction". Cognitive Science.

nominative, accusative, alignment, this, article, technical, most, readers, understand, please, help, improve, make, understandable, experts, without, removing, technical, details, february, 2016, learn, when, remove, this, template, message, linguistic, typol. This article may be too technical for most readers to understand Please help improve it to make it understandable to non experts without removing the technical details February 2016 Learn how and when to remove this template message In linguistic typology nominative accusative alignment is a type of morphosyntactic alignment in which subjects of intransitive verbs are treated like subjects of transitive verbs and are distinguished from objects of transitive verbs in basic clause constructions Nominative accusative alignment can be coded by case marking verb agreement and or word order It has a wide global distribution and is the most common alignment system among the world s languages including English Languages with nominative accusative alignment are commonly called nominative accusative languages Nominative accusative alignment Contents 1 Comparison with other alignment types 1 1 Split ergativity 2 Coding properties of nominative accusative alignment 2 1 Case marking 2 1 1 Differential object marking DOM 2 2 Word order 2 3 Verb agreement 3 Behavioral properties of accusativity 4 Distribution 5 Relevant theory 5 1 Optimality theory 5 2 Functional pressure 6 See also 7 ReferencesComparison with other alignment types editMain article Morphosyntactic alignment A transitive verb is associated with two noun phrases or arguments a subject and a direct object An intransitive verb is associated with only one argument a subject The different kinds of arguments are usually represented as S A and O S is the sole argument of an intransitive verb A is the subject or most agent like argument of a transitive verb and O is the direct object or most patient like argument of a transitive verb English has nominative accusative alignment in its case marking of personal pronouns 1 the single argument S of an intransitive verb I in the sentence I walked behaves grammatically like the agent A of a transitive verb I in the sentence I saw them but differently from the object O of a transitive verb me in the sentence they saw me Nominative accusative Ergative absolutive TripartiteA same different differentS same differentO different differentThis is in contrast with ergative absolutive alignment where S is coded in the same way as O while A receives distinct marking or tripartite alignment where A S and O all are coded in a different manner Split ergativity edit It is common for languages such as Georgian and Hindustani to have overlapping alignment systems which exhibit both nominative accusative and ergative absolutive coding a phenomenon called split ergativity In fact there are relatively few languages that exhibit only ergative absolutive alignment called pure ergativity and tend to be isolated in certain regions of the world such as the Caucasus parts of North America and Mesoamerica the Tibetan Plateau and Australia Such languages include Sumerian Standard Tibetan and Mayan 2 Coding properties of nominative accusative alignment editNominative accusative alignment can manifest itself in visible ways called coding properties Often these visible properties are morphological and the distinction will appear as a difference in the actual morphological form and spelling of the word or as case particles pieces of morphology which will appear before or after the word Case marking edit If a language exhibits morphological case marking arguments S and A will appear in the nominative case and argument O will appear in the accusative case or in a similar case such as the oblique There may be more than one case fulfilling the accusative role for instance Finnish marks objects with the partitive or the accusative to contrast telicity It is highly common for only accusative arguments to exhibit overt case marking while nominative arguments exhibit null or absent case markings In Modern English case marking is only found with first and non neuter third person pronouns which have distinct subject and object forms English I1SG SBJwalked walk PASTI walked 1SG SBJ walk PAST I1SG SBJsawsee PASTthem 3PL OBJI saw them 1SG SBJ see PAST 3PL OBJ Japanese 花瓶がKabin ga S vase NOM壊れたkowaretabroke 3 花瓶が 壊れたKabin ga S kowaretavase NOM broke A vase broke 私はWatashi wa S I NOM花瓶をkabin wo O vase ACC壊したkowashitabroke私は 花瓶を 壊したWatashi wa S kabin wo O kowashitaI NOM vase ACC broke I broke the vase Russian Devushka ODyevushka O adolescent youth girl NOMrabota etrabota yetworkDevushka O rabota etDyevushka O rabota yet adolescent youth girl NOM work A The adolescent youth girl young lady young woman works is working Student OStudyent Ostudent NOMchitaetchitayetread 3 SG PRESknig uknig ubook ACCStudent O chitaet knig uStudyent O chitayet knig ustudent NOM read 3 SG PRES book ACC A The student read is reading a the book Sanskrit Asva ḥ S horse NOMaghnataslainAsva ḥ S aghnatahorse NOM slain A horse was slain Vira ḥ S man NOMasva m O horse ACCahanslewVira ḥ S asva m O ahanman NOM horse ACC slew The man slew a horse Differential object marking DOM edit Main article Differential object marking Not all arguments are equally likely to exhibit overt case marking In languages with nominative accusative alignment it is common to divide direct objects into two classes with respect to overt case marking a phenomenon called differential object marking by Bossong 1985 Word order edit Some languages code very little through morphology and are more dependent on syntax to encode meaning and grammatical relationships If a language relies less on overt case marking alignment may be coded through word order as in this example from Indonesian Indonesian sayai1SGmei mandi kanAT wash APPLpriamanituthat 4 sayai mei mandi kan pria itu1SG AT wash APPL man that I bathe that man In the following example from French all subjects both S and A appear before the verb while O appears after the verb Arguments occurring before the verb are coded as nominative while arguments occurring directly after the verb are coded as accusative French Je S I NOMtravailleworkJe S travailleI NOM work I work Je A I NOMjettethrowunaballon O ball ACCJe A jette un ballon O I NOM throw a ball ACC I throw a ball Verb agreement edit Alternatively alignment can also manifest visibly through agreement on the verb In the following example from Amharic the verb can be head marked for S A and O Both S in the intransitive clause and A in the transitive clause are marked by the same affix e 3SG M while O in the transitive clause is marked by a different affix w 3SG M O 4 AmharicintransitiveLemmaLemmahed ego PFV 3SG MLemma hed eLemma go PFV 3SG M Lemma came transitiveLemmaLemmat ermus u nbottle DEF ACCsebber e wbreak PFV 3SG M 3SG M OLemma t ermus u n sebber e wLemma bottle DEF ACC break PFV 3SG M 3SG M O Lemma breaks the bottle English has residual verb agreement with nominative accusative alignment which is only manifest with third person singular S and A in present tense 5 Behavioral properties of accusativity editNominative accusative alignment can also be distinguished through behavioral properties in the way a nominative or accusative argument will behave when placed in particular syntactic constructions This has to do with the impact of alignment on the level of the whole sentence rather than the individual word Morphosyntactic alignment determines which arguments can be omitted in a coordinate structure during the process of conjunction reduction deleting arguments from the ends of joined clauses In nominative accusative only arguments S and A can be omitted and not argument O English a Sue NOMi saw Judy ACCj and shei j ran b Suei saw Judyj and i j ran c Suei saw Judyj and shei j was frightened d Suei saw Judyj and i j was frightened The omitted subject argument of the embedded clause must correspond to the subject nominative of the matrix clause If it corresponds to the object accusative the sentence is ungrammatical If English were an ergative absolutive language one would expect to see b Suei saw Judyj and i j ran c Suei saw Judyj and i j was frightened Here the omitted argument of the embedded clause corresponds to the direct object absolutive of the matrix clause If it corresponds to the subject ergative the sentence is ungrammatical The alignment system also impacts the triggering and realization of other such syntactic processes as raising constructions subject controlled subject deletion and object controlled subject deletion Distribution edit nbsp Distribution of languages by alignment typeLanguages exhibiting accusative alignment are the most widespread of all of the alignment types These languages can be found on every continent in comparison to languages with ergative alignment that are restricted to certain areas of the world namely the Caucasus parts of North American and Mesoamerica the Tibetan plateau and Australia The map shows the distribution of languages with the various alignment types and the following list gives a short sampling of accusative languages and their distribution across the globe 6 North America Cahuilla U S Koasati U S Miwok U S Australasia Kayardild Australia Mangarayi Australia Maori New Zealand South America Aymara Bolivia Chile Peru Barasana Colombia Quechua Ecuador Xavante BrazilEurope Armenian Armenia Finnish Finland German Germany Greek Greece Hungarian Hungary Turkish Turkey Russian Russia Spanish Spain Africa Berber Morocco Igbo Nigeria Iraqw Tanzania Kanuri Chad Khoekhoe Namibia Malagasy Madagascar Nubian Sudan Oromo Ethiopia Asia Brahui Pakistan Burmese Myanmar Garo India Hebrew Israel Japanese Japan Mongolian Mongolia Korean KoreaRelevant theory editOptimality theory edit One of the ways in which the production of a nominative accusative case marking system can be explained is from an Optimality Theoretic perspective Case marking is said to fulfill two functions or constraints an identifying function and a distinguishing function 7 The identifying function is exemplified when case morphology encodes identifies specific semantic thematic or pragmatic properties or information about the nominal argument Accusative case in the position of the direct object for example can be a strong identifier of patienthood The distinguishing function is used to distinguish between the core arguments the subject and the object of a transitive clause Helen de Hoop and Andrej Malchukov explain the motivation and need for the distinguishing function in Case marking strategies When a two place predicate R x y is used to describe an event involving two participants usually an agent and a patient it is of utmost importance to avoid ambiguity as to which noun phrase corresponds to the first argument x the agent and which to the second argument y the patient For this purpose case can be used to mark one of the arguments If one argument is case marked this already suffices for the purpose of disambiguation Thus from the distinguishing perspective there is no need to case mark both arguments Neither would it be necessary to case mark the one and only argument of a one place intransitive predicate Indeed it has been argued that in many nominative accusative case systems only the y is case marked with accusative case while the x remains morphologically unmarked 7 It is rare for case to serve only the distinguishing function which overlaps greatly with the identify function Other ways of disambiguating the arguments of a transitive predicate subject agreement word order restriction context intonation etc may explain this cross linguistic observation De Hoop and Malchukov argue that case systems that are completely based on the identification function must be richer in case morphology compared to languages based mainly on the distinguishing function Functional pressure edit One theory that has been posited to account for the occurrence of accusative systems is that of functional pressure When applied to languages this theory operates around the various needs and pressures on a speech community It has been suggested that languages have evolved to suit the needs of their users These communities will develop some functional system to meet the needs that they have So it has been proposed that the accusative system arose from a functional pressure to avoid ambiguity and make communication a simpler process 8 9 It is useful for languages to have a means of distinguishing between subjects and objects and between arguments A S and O This is helpful so that sentences like Tom hit Fred cannot be interpreted as Fred hit Tom Tripartite alignment systems accomplish this differentiation by coding S A and O all differently However this is not structurally economical and tripartite systems are comparatively rare but to have all arguments marked the same makes the arguments too ambiguous Alongside the principle of distinguishability seems to operate a principle of economy It is more efficient to have as few cases as possible without compromising intelligibility In this way the dual pressures of efficiency and economy have produced a system which patterns two kinds of arguments together a third separately Both accusative and ergative systems use this kind of grouping to make meaning clearer See also editAccusative case Case grammar Ergative absolutive language Morphosyntactic alignment Nominative caseReferences edit Donohue Mark Wichmann Soren eds 2005 The Typology of Semantic Alignment Oxford Oxford University Press p 25 ISBN 9780199238385 Van Valin Robert D 2001 An Introduction to Syntax Cambridge University Press ISBN 9780521635660 Tsujimura Natsuko 2007 An introduction to Japanese linguistics Wiley Blackwell p 382 ISBN 978 1 4051 1065 5 a b van de Visser Mario 2006 The Marked Status of Ergativity PhD Dissertation Bickel Balthasar Iemmolo Giorgio Zakharko Taras Witzlack Makarevich Alena 2013 Patterns of alignment in verb agreement In Bakker Dik Haspelmath Martin Languages across boundaries Studies in memory of Anna Siewierska Berlin De Gruyter Mouton 15 36 Dryer Matthew S amp Haspelmath Martin eds 2011 The World Atlas of Language Structures Online Munich Max Planck Digital Library Available online at WALS a b de Hoop Helen and Malchukov Andrej L 2008 Case marking strategies Linguistic Inquiry Bates E amp MacWhinney B 1982 Functionalist approaches to grammar In E Wanner amp L Gleitman Ed Language acquisition The state of the art New York Cambridge University Press Fedzechkina Maryia amp Jaeger T Florian amp Newport Elissa L 2011 Functional Biases in Language Learning Evidence from Word Order and Case Marking Interaction Cognitive Science Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Nominative accusative alignment amp oldid 1185132164, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.