fbpx
Wikipedia

Color

Color (American English) or colour (Commonwealth English) is the visual perception based on the electromagnetic spectrum. Though color is not an inherent property of matter, color perception is related to an object's light absorption, reflection, emission spectra and interference. For most humans, color are perceived in the visible light spectrum with three types of cone cells (trichromacy). Other animals may have a different number of cone cell types or have eyes sensitive to different wavelength, such as bees that can distinguish ultraviolet, thus has a different color sensitivity range. Animal perception of color originates from different light wavelength or spectral sensitivity in cone cell types, which is then being processed by the brain.

Pencils shown in various colors

Colors have perceived properties such as hue, colorfulness (saturation) and luminance. Colors can also be additively mixed (commonly used for actual light) or subtractively mixed (commonly used for materials). If the colors are mixed in the right proportions, because of metamerism, they may look the same as a single-wavelength light. For convenience, colors can be organized in a color space, which when being abstracted as a mathematical color model can assign each region of color with a corresponding set of numbers. As such, color spaces are an essential tool for color reproduction in print, photography, computer monitors and television. The most well-known color models are RGB, CMYK, YUV, HSL and HSV.

Because the perception of color is an important aspect of human life, different colors have been associated with emotions, activity, and nationality. Names of color regions in different cultures can have different, sometimes overlapping areas. In visual arts, color theory is used to govern the use of colors in an asthetically pleasing and harmonious way. The theory of color includes the color complements; color balance; and classification of primary colors (traditionally red, yellow, blue), secondary colors (traditionally orange, green, purple) and tertiary colors. The study of colors in general is called color science.

Physical properties

 
The visible spectrum perceived from 390–710 nm wavelength

Electromagnetic radiation is characterized by its wavelength (or frequency) and its intensity. When the wavelength is within the visible spectrum (the range of wavelengths humans can perceive, approximately from 390 nm to 700 nm), it is known as "visible light".

Most light sources emit light at many different wavelengths; a source's spectrum is a distribution giving its intensity at each wavelength. Although the spectrum of light arriving at the eye from a given direction determines the color sensation in that direction, there are many more possible spectral combinations than color sensations. In fact, one may formally define a color as a class of spectra that give rise to the same color sensation, although such classes would vary widely among different species, and to a lesser extent among individuals within the same species. In each such class, the members are called metamers of the color in question. This effect can be visualized by comparing the light sources' spectral power distributions and the resulting colors.

Spectral colors

The familiar colors of the rainbow in the spectrum—named using the Latin word for appearance or apparition by Isaac Newton in 1671—include all those colors that can be produced by visible light of a single wavelength only, the pure spectral or monochromatic colors. The table at right shows approximate frequencies (in terahertz) and wavelengths (in nanometers) for spectral colors in the visible range. Spectral colors have 100% purity, and are fully saturated. A complex mixture of spectral colors can be used to describe any color, which is the definition of a light power spectrum.

The color table should not be interpreted as a definitive list; the spectral colors form a continuous spectrum, and how it is divided into distinct colors linguistically is a matter of culture and historical contingency.[1] Despite the ubiquitous ROYGBIV mnemonic used to remember the spectral colors in english, the inclusion or exclusion of colors in this table is contentious, with disagreement often focused on indigo and cyan.[2] Even if the subset of color terms is agreed, their wavelength ranges and borders between them may not be.

The intensity of a spectral color, relative to the context in which it is viewed, may alter its perception considerably according to the Bezold–Brücke shift; for example, a low-intensity orange-yellow is brown, and a low-intensity yellow-green is olive green.

Color of objects

 
Simple depiction of how objects gain color, showing selective absorption of light, which only describes the first factor that affects perception of an object's color

The color of an object as perceived by an observer is not an intrinsic quality of that object, but depends on several factors:

  1. the physics of the object (which wavelengths of light are selectively absorbed, reflected, transmitted, or emitted)
  2. the color of the light shining on the object (color cast of the illuminant)
  3. the angles between observer, object and illuminant (applicable to structural color)
  4. the physics of light in its environment (how the atmosphere may affect the light through Rayleigh scattering or dispersion, for example)
  5. relative velocity between object and observer (red shift; mostly applicable to astronomy)
  6. the characteristics of the perceiving eye (the number and spectral sensitivity of cone classes and dimensionality of color vision)
  7. higher order processes in the brain that affect the color, such as color constancy

Some generalizations of the physics can be drawn, neglecting perceptual effects for now:

  • Light arriving at an opaque surface is either reflected "specularly" (that is, in the manner of a mirror), scattered (that is, reflected with diffuse scattering), or absorbed—or some combination of these.
  • Opaque objects that do not reflect specularly (which tend to have rough surfaces) have their color determined by which wavelengths of light they scatter strongly (with the light that is not scattered being absorbed). If objects scatter all wavelengths with roughly equal strength, they appear white. If they absorb all wavelengths, they appear black.[3]
  • Opaque objects that specularly reflect the light of different wavelengths with different efficiencies look like mirrors tinted with colors determined by those differences. An object that reflects some fraction of impinging light and absorbs the rest may look black but also be faintly reflective; examples are black objects coated with layers of enamel or lacquer.
  • Objects that transmit light are either translucent (scattering the transmitted light) or transparent (not scattering the transmitted light). If they also absorb (or reflect) light of various wavelengths differentially, they appear tinted with a color determined by the nature of that absorption (or that reflectance).
  • Objects may emit light that they generate from having excited electrons, rather than merely reflecting or transmitting light. The electrons may be excited due to elevated temperature (incandescence), as a result of chemical reactions (chemiluminescence), after absorbing light of other frequencies ("fluorescence" or "phosphorescence") or from electrical contacts as in light-emitting diodes, or other light sources.

To summarize, the color of an object is a complex result of its surface properties, its transmission properties, and its emission properties, all of which contribute to the mix of wavelengths in the light leaving the surface of the object. The perceived color is then further conditioned by the nature of the ambient illumination, and by the color properties of other objects nearby, and via other characteristics of the perceiving eye and brain.

Color vision

Development of theories of color vision

 
The upper disk and the lower disk have exactly the same objective color, and are in identical gray surroundings; based on context differences, humans perceive the squares as having different reflectances, and may interpret the colors as different color categories; see checker shadow illusion.

Although Aristotle and other ancient scientists had already written on the nature of light and color vision, it was not until Newton that light was identified as the source of the color sensation. In 1810, Goethe published his comprehensive Theory of Colors in which he provided a rational description of colour experience, which 'tells us how it originates, not what it is'. (Schopenhauer)

In 1801 Thomas Young proposed his trichromatic theory, based on the observation that any color could be matched with a combination of three lights. This theory was later refined by James Clerk Maxwell and Hermann von Helmholtz. As Helmholtz puts it, "the principles of Newton's law of mixture were experimentally confirmed by Maxwell in 1856. Young's theory of color sensations, like so much else that this marvelous investigator achieved in advance of his time, remained unnoticed until Maxwell directed attention to it."[4]

At the same time as Helmholtz, Ewald Hering developed the opponent process theory of color, noting that color blindness and afterimages typically come in opponent pairs (red-green, blue-orange, yellow-violet, and black-white). Ultimately these two theories were synthesized in 1957 by Hurvich and Jameson, who showed that retinal processing corresponds to the trichromatic theory, while processing at the level of the lateral geniculate nucleus corresponds to the opponent theory.[5]

In 1931, an international group of experts known as the Commission internationale de l'éclairage (CIE) developed a mathematical color model, which mapped out the space of observable colors and assigned a set of three numbers to each.

Color in the eye

 
Normalized typical human cone cell responses (S, M, and L types) to monochromatic spectral stimuli

The ability of the human eye to distinguish colors is based upon the varying sensitivity of different cells in the retina to light of different wavelengths. Humans are trichromatic—the retina contains three types of color receptor cells, or cones. One type, relatively distinct from the other two, is most responsive to light that is perceived as blue or blue-violet, with wavelengths around 450 nm; cones of this type are sometimes called short-wavelength cones or S cones (or misleadingly, blue cones). The other two types are closely related genetically and chemically: middle-wavelength cones, M cones, or green cones are most sensitive to light perceived as green, with wavelengths around 540 nm, while the long-wavelength cones, L cones, or red cones, are most sensitive to light that is perceived as greenish yellow, with wavelengths around 570 nm.

Light, no matter how complex its composition of wavelengths, is reduced to three color components by the eye. Each cone type adheres to the principle of univariance, which is that each cone's output is determined by the amount of light that falls on it over all wavelengths. For each location in the visual field, the three types of cones yield three signals based on the extent to which each is stimulated. These amounts of stimulation are sometimes called tristimulus values.[6]

The response curve as a function of wavelength varies for each type of cone. Because the curves overlap, some tristimulus values do not occur for any incoming light combination. For example, it is not possible to stimulate only the mid-wavelength (so-called "green") cones; the other cones will inevitably be stimulated to some degree at the same time. The set of all possible tristimulus values determines the human color space. It has been estimated that humans can distinguish roughly 10 million different colors.[7]

The other type of light-sensitive cell in the eye, the rod, has a different response curve. In normal situations, when light is bright enough to strongly stimulate the cones, rods play virtually no role in vision at all.[8] On the other hand, in dim light, the cones are understimulated leaving only the signal from the rods, resulting in a colorless response. (Furthermore, the rods are barely sensitive to light in the "red" range.) In certain conditions of intermediate illumination, the rod response and a weak cone response can together result in color discriminations not accounted for by cone responses alone. These effects, combined, are summarized also in the Kruithof curve, which describes the change of color perception and pleasingness of light as a function of temperature and intensity.

Color in the brain

 
The visual dorsal stream (green) and ventral stream (purple) are shown. The ventral stream is responsible for color perception.

While the mechanisms of color vision at the level of the retina are well-described in terms of tristimulus values, color processing after that point is organized differently. A dominant theory of color vision proposes that color information is transmitted out of the eye by three opponent processes, or opponent channels, each constructed from the raw output of the cones: a red–green channel, a blue–yellow channel, and a black–white "luminance" channel. This theory has been supported by neurobiology, and accounts for the structure of our subjective color experience. Specifically, it explains why humans cannot perceive a "reddish green" or "yellowish blue", and it predicts the color wheel: it is the collection of colors for which at least one of the two color channels measures a value at one of its extremes.

The exact nature of color perception beyond the processing already described, and indeed the status of color as a feature of the perceived world or rather as a feature of our perception of the world—a type of qualia—is a matter of complex and continuing philosophical dispute.[citation needed]

Nonstandard color perception

Color vision deficiency

A color vision deficiency causes an individual to perceive a smaller gamut of colors than the standard observer with normal color vision. The effect can be mild, having lower "color resolution" (i.e. anomalous trichromacy), moderate, lacking an entire dimension or channel of color (e.g. dichromacy), or complete, lacking all color perception (i.e. monochromacy). Most forms of color blindness derive from one or many of the three classes of cone cells either being missing, having a shifted spectral sensitivity or having lower responsiveness to incoming light. In addition, cerebral achromatopsia is caused by neural anomalies in those parts of the brain where visual processing takes place.

Some colors that appear distinct to an individual with normal color vision will appear metameric to the color blind. The most common form of color blindness is congenital red-green color blindness, affecting ~8% of males. Individuals with the strongest form of this condition (dichromacy) will experience blue and purple, green and yellow, teal and gray as colors of confusion, i.e. metamers.[9]

Tetrachromacy

Outside of humans, which are mostly trichromatic (having three types of cones), most mammals are dichromatic, possessing only two cones. However, outside of mammals, most vertebrate are tetrachromatic, having four types of cones, and includes most, birds, reptiles, amphibians and bony fish. An extra dimension of color vision means these vertebrates can see two distinct colors that a normal human would view as metamers. Some invertebrates, such as the mantis shrimp, have an even higher number of cones (12) that could lead to a richer color gamut than even imaginable by humans.

The existence of human tetrachromats is a contentious notion. As many as half of all human females have 4 distinct cone classes, which could enable tetrachromacy.[10]: p.256  However, a distinction must be made between retinal (or weak) tetrachromats, which express four cone classes in the retina, and functional (or strong) tetrachromats, which are able to make the enhanced color discriminations expected of tetrachromats. In fact, there is only one peer-reviewed report of a functional tetrachromat.[11] It is estimated that while the average person is able to see one million colors, someone with functional tetrachromacy could see a hundred million colors. [12]

Synesthesia

In certain forms of synesthesia, perceiving letters and numbers (grapheme–color synesthesia) or hearing sounds (chromesthesia) will evoke a perception of color. Behavioral and functional neuroimaging experiments have demonstrated that these color experiences lead to changes in behavioral tasks and lead to increased activation of brain regions involved in color perception, thus demonstrating their reality, and similarity to real color percepts, albeit evoked through a non-standard route. Synesthesia can occur genetically, with 4% of the population having variants associated with the condition. Synesthesia has also been known to occur with brain damage, drugs, and sensory deprivation. [13]

The philosopher Pythagoras experienced synesthesia and provided one of the first written accounts of the condition in approximately 550 BCE. He created mathematical equations for musical notes that could form part of a scale, such as an octave. [14]

Afterimages

After exposure to strong light in their sensitivity range, photoreceptors of a given type become desensitized. For a few seconds after the light ceases, they will continue to signal less strongly than they otherwise would. Colors observed during that period will appear to lack the color component detected by the desensitized photoreceptors. This effect is responsible for the phenomenon of afterimages, in which the eye may continue to see a bright figure after looking away from it, but in a complementary color.

Afterimage effects have also been used by artists, including Vincent van Gogh.

Color constancy

When an artist uses a limited color palette, the human eye tends to compensate by seeing any gray or neutral color as the color which is missing from the color wheel. For example, in a limited palette consisting of red, yellow, black, and white, a mixture of yellow and black will appear as a variety of green, a mixture of red and black will appear as a variety of purple, and pure gray will appear bluish.[15]

The trichromatic theory is strictly true when the visual system is in a fixed state of adaptation. In reality, the visual system is constantly adapting to changes in the environment and compares the various colors in a scene to reduce the effects of the illumination. If a scene is illuminated with one light, and then with another, as long as the difference between the light sources stays within a reasonable range, the colors in the scene appear relatively constant to us. This was studied by Edwin H. Land in the 1970s and led to his retinex theory of color constancy.

Both phenomena are readily explained and mathematically modeled with modern theories of chromatic adaptation and color appearance (e.g. CIECAM02, iCAM).[16] There is no need to dismiss the trichromatic theory of vision, but rather it can be enhanced with an understanding of how the visual system adapts to changes in the viewing environment.

Reproduction

 
The CIE 1931 color space xy chromaticity diagram with the visual locus plotted using the CIE (2006) physiologically-relevant LMS fundamental color matching functions transformed into the CIE 1931 xy color space and converted into Adobe RGB. The triangle shows the gamut of Adobe RGB. The Planckian locus is shown with color temperatures labeled in Kelvins. The outer curved boundary is the spectral (or monochromatic) locus, with wavelengths shown in nanometers. Note that the colors in this file are being specified using Adobe RGB. Areas outside the triangle cannot be accurately rendered since they are outside the gamut of Adobe RGB, therefore they have been interpreted. Note that the colors depicted depend on the gamut and color accuracy of your display.

Color reproduction is the science of creating colors for the human eye that faithfully represent the desired color. It focuses on how to construct a spectrum of wavelengths that will best evoke a certain color in an observer. Most colors are not spectral colors, meaning they are mixtures of various wavelengths of light. However, these non-spectral colors are often described by their dominant wavelength, which identifies the single wavelength of light that produces a sensation most similar to the non-spectral color. Dominant wavelength is roughly akin to hue.

There are many color perceptions that by definition cannot be pure spectral colors due to desaturation or because they are purples (mixtures of red and violet light, from opposite ends of the spectrum). Some examples of necessarily non-spectral colors are the achromatic colors (black, gray, and white) and colors such as pink, tan, and magenta.

Two different light spectra that have the same effect on the three color receptors in the human eye will be perceived as the same color. They are metamers of that color. This is exemplified by the white light emitted by fluorescent lamps, which typically has a spectrum of a few narrow bands, while daylight has a continuous spectrum. The human eye cannot tell the difference between such light spectra just by looking into the light source, although the color rendering index of each light source may affect the color of objects illuminated by these metameric light sources.

Similarly, most human color perceptions can be generated by a mixture of three colors called primaries. This is used to reproduce color scenes in photography, printing, television, and other media. There are a number of methods or color spaces for specifying a color in terms of three particular primary colors. Each method has its advantages and disadvantages depending on the particular application.

No mixture of colors, however, can produce a response truly identical to that of a spectral color, although one can get close, especially for the longer wavelengths, where the CIE 1931 color space chromaticity diagram has a nearly straight edge. For example, mixing green light (530 nm) and blue light (460 nm) produces cyan light that is slightly desaturated, because response of the red color receptor would be greater to the green and blue light in the mixture than it would be to a pure cyan light at 485 nm that has the same intensity as the mixture of blue and green.

Because of this, and because the primaries in color printing systems generally are not pure themselves, the colors reproduced are never perfectly saturated spectral colors, and so spectral colors cannot be matched exactly. However, natural scenes rarely contain fully saturated colors, thus such scenes can usually be approximated well by these systems. The range of colors that can be reproduced with a given color reproduction system is called the gamut. The CIE chromaticity diagram can be used to describe the gamut.

Another problem with color reproduction systems is connected with the initial measurement of color, or colorimetry. The characteristics of the color sensors in measurement devices (e.g. cameras, scanners) are often very far from the characteristics of the receptors in the human eye.

A color reproduction system "tuned" to a human with normal color vision may give very inaccurate results for other observers, according to color vision deviations to the standard observer.

The different color response of different devices can be problematic if not properly managed. For color information stored and transferred in digital form, color management techniques, such as those based on ICC profiles, can help to avoid distortions of the reproduced colors. Color management does not circumvent the gamut limitations of particular output devices, but can assist in finding good mapping of input colors into the gamut that can be reproduced.

Additive coloring

 
Additive color mixing: combining red and green yields yellow; combining all three primary colors together yields white.

Additive color is light created by mixing together light of two or more different colors. Red, green, and blue are the additive primary colors normally used in additive color systems such as projectors and computer terminals.

Subtractive coloring

 
Subtractive color mixing: combining yellow and magenta yields red; combining all three primary colors together yields black.
 
Twelve main pigment colors

Subtractive coloring uses dyes, inks, pigments, or filters to absorb some wavelengths of light and not others. The color that a surface displays comes from the parts of the visible spectrum that are not absorbed and therefore remain visible. Without pigments or dye, fabric fibers, paint base and paper are usually made of particles that scatter white light (all colors) well in all directions. When a pigment or ink is added, wavelengths are absorbed or "subtracted" from white light, so light of another color reaches the eye.

If the light is not a pure white source (the case of nearly all forms of artificial lighting), the resulting spectrum will appear a slightly different color. Red paint, viewed under blue light, may appear black. Red paint is red because it scatters only the red components of the spectrum. If red paint is illuminated by blue light, it will be absorbed by the red paint, creating the appearance of a black object.

Structural color

Structural colors are colors caused by interference effects rather than by pigments. Color effects are produced when a material is scored with fine parallel lines, formed of one or more parallel thin layers, or otherwise composed of microstructures on the scale of the color's wavelength. If the microstructures are spaced randomly, light of shorter wavelengths will be scattered preferentially to produce Tyndall effect colors: the blue of the sky (Rayleigh scattering, caused by structures much smaller than the wavelength of light, in this case, air molecules), the luster of opals, and the blue of human irises. If the microstructures are aligned in arrays, for example, the array of pits in a CD, they behave as a diffraction grating: the grating reflects different wavelengths in different directions due to interference phenomena, separating mixed "white" light into light of different wavelengths. If the structure is one or more thin layers then it will reflect some wavelengths and transmit others, depending on the layers' thickness.

Structural color is studied in the field of thin-film optics. The most ordered or the most changeable structural colors are iridescent. Structural color is responsible for the blues and greens of the feathers of many birds (the blue jay, for example), as well as certain butterfly wings and beetle shells. Variations in the pattern's spacing often give rise to an iridescent effect, as seen in peacock feathers, soap bubbles, films of oil, and mother of pearl, because the reflected color depends upon the viewing angle. Numerous scientists have carried out research in butterfly wings and beetle shells, including Isaac Newton and Robert Hooke. Since 1942, electron micrography has been used, advancing the development of products that exploit structural color, such as "photonic" cosmetics.[17]

Cultural perspective

Colors, their meanings and associations can play a major role in works of art, including literature.[18]

Associations

Individual colors have a variety of cultural associations such as national colors (in general described in individual color articles and color symbolism). The field of color psychology attempts to identify the effects of color on human emotion and activity. Chromotherapy is a form of alternative medicine attributed to various Eastern traditions. Colors have different associations in different countries and cultures.[19]

Different colors have been demonstrated to have effects on cognition. For example, researchers at the University of Linz in Austria demonstrated that the color red significantly decreases cognitive functioning in men.[20] The combination of the colors red and yellow together can induce hunger, which has been capitalized on by a number of chain restaurants. [21]

Color plays a role in memory development too. A photograph that is in black and white is slightly less memorable than one in color. [22] Studies also show that wearing bright colors makes you more memorable to people you meet.

Terminology

Colors vary in several different ways, including hue (shades of red, orange, yellow, green, blue, and violet), saturation, brightness, and gloss. Some color words are derived from the name of an object of that color, such as "orange" or "salmon", while others are abstract, like "red".

In the 1969 study Basic Color Terms: Their Universality and Evolution, Brent Berlin and Paul Kay describe a pattern in naming "basic" colors (like "red" but not "red-orange" or "dark red" or "blood red", which are "shades" of red). All languages that have two "basic" color names distinguish dark/cool colors from bright/warm colors. The next colors to be distinguished are usually red and then yellow or green. All languages with six "basic" colors include black, white, red, green, blue, and yellow. The pattern holds up to a set of twelve: black, gray, white, pink, red, orange, yellow, green, blue, purple, brown, and azure (distinct from blue in Russian and Italian, but not English).

See also

References

  1. ^ Berlin, B. and Kay, P., Basic Color Terms: Their Universality and Evolution, Berkeley: University of California Press, 1969.
  2. ^ Waldman, Gary (2002). Introduction to light: the physics of light, vision, and color. Mineola: Dover Publications. p. 193. ISBN 978-0-486-42118-6.
  3. ^ Pastoureau, Michael (2008). Black: The History of a Color. Princeton University Press. p. 216. ISBN 978-0691139302.
  4. ^ Hermann von Helmholtz, Physiological Optics: The Sensations of Vision, 1866, as translated in Sources of Color Science, David L. MacAdam, ed., Cambridge: MIT Press, 1970.
  5. ^ Palmer, S.E. (1999). Vision Science: Photons to Phenomenology, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. ISBN 0-262-16183-4.
  6. ^ Magazine, Nicola Jones,Knowable. "Color Is in the Eye, and Brain, of the Beholder". Scientific American. Retrieved 2022-11-08.
  7. ^ Judd, Deane B.; Wyszecki, Günter (1975). Color in Business, Science and Industry. Wiley Series in Pure and Applied Optics (third ed.). New York: Wiley-Interscience. p. 388. ISBN 978-0-471-45212-6.
  8. ^ "Under well-lit viewing conditions (photopic vision), cones  ...are highly active and rods are inactive."Hirakawa, K.; Parks, T.W. (2005). (PDF). IEEE International Conference on Image Processing 2005. IEEE ICIP. pp. III-984. doi:10.1109/ICIP.2005.1530559. ISBN 0-7803-9134-9. Archived from the original (PDF) on November 28, 2006.
  9. ^ Flück, Daniel. "Colorblind colors of confusion". Colblindor. Retrieved 14 November 2022.
  10. ^ Jameson, K.A.; Highnote, S.M.; Wasserman, L.M. (2001). "Richer color experience in observers with multiple photopigment opsin genes" (PDF). Psychonomic Bulletin and Review. 8 (2): 244–61. doi:10.3758/BF03196159. PMID 11495112. S2CID 2389566. (PDF) from the original on 2013-10-04.
  11. ^ Jordan, G.; Deeb, S.S.; Bosten, J.M.; Mollon, J.D. (20 July 2010). "The dimensionality of color vision in carriers of anomalous trichromacy". Journal of Vision. 10 (8): 12. doi:10.1167/10.8.12. PMID 20884587.
  12. ^ Kershner, Kate (26 July 2016). "Lucky Tetrachromats See World With Up to 100 Million Colors". Retrieved 9 February 2022.
  13. ^ Brang, David (22 November 2011). "Survival of the Synesthesia Gene: Why Do People Hear Colors and Taste Words?". PLOS Biology. 9 (11): e1001205. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.1001205. PMC 3222625. PMID 22131906.
  14. ^ "A Brief History of Synesthesia in the Arts". Retrieved 9 February 2022.
  15. ^ Depauw, Robert C. . Archived from the original on 6 January 2012. Retrieved 20 March 2011.
  16. ^ M.D. Fairchild, Color Appearance Models May 5, 2011, at the Wayback Machine, 2nd Ed., Wiley, Chichester (2005).
  17. ^ . Archived from the original on November 2, 2007. Retrieved 2007-10-07.
  18. ^ Gary Westfahl (2005). The Greenwood Encyclopedia of Science Fiction and Fantasy: Themes, Works, and Wonders. Greenwood Publishing Group. pp. 142–143. ISBN 978-0-313-32951-7.
  19. ^ "Chart: Color Meanings by Culture". Archived from the original on 2010-10-12. Retrieved 2010-06-29.
  20. ^ Dzulkifli, Mariam; Mustafar, Muhammad (2013). "The Influence of Colour on Memory Performance: A Review". The Malaysian Journal of Medical Sciences. 20 (2): 3–9. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2010.06.010.
  21. ^ "There's a sneaky reason why you always see red and yellow on fast food logos". Business Insider. Retrieved 2022-02-09.
  22. ^ Gnambs, Timo; Appel, Markus; Batinic, Bernad (2010). "Color red in web-based knowledge testing". Computers in Human Behavior. 26 (6): 1625–31. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2010.06.010.

External links

color, other, uses, disambiguation, disambiguation, this, article, needs, additional, citations, verification, please, help, improve, this, article, adding, citations, reliable, sources, unsourced, material, challenged, removed, find, sources, news, newspapers. For other uses see Color disambiguation and Colorful disambiguation This article needs additional citations for verification Please help improve this article by adding citations to reliable sources Unsourced material may be challenged and removed Find sources Color news newspapers books scholar JSTOR September 2017 Learn how and when to remove this template message Color American English or colour Commonwealth English is the visual perception based on the electromagnetic spectrum Though color is not an inherent property of matter color perception is related to an object s light absorption reflection emission spectra and interference For most humans color are perceived in the visible light spectrum with three types of cone cells trichromacy Other animals may have a different number of cone cell types or have eyes sensitive to different wavelength such as bees that can distinguish ultraviolet thus has a different color sensitivity range Animal perception of color originates from different light wavelength or spectral sensitivity in cone cell types which is then being processed by the brain Pencils shown in various colors Colors have perceived properties such as hue colorfulness saturation and luminance Colors can also be additively mixed commonly used for actual light or subtractively mixed commonly used for materials If the colors are mixed in the right proportions because of metamerism they may look the same as a single wavelength light For convenience colors can be organized in a color space which when being abstracted as a mathematical color model can assign each region of color with a corresponding set of numbers As such color spaces are an essential tool for color reproduction in print photography computer monitors and television The most well known color models are RGB CMYK YUV HSL and HSV Because the perception of color is an important aspect of human life different colors have been associated with emotions activity and nationality Names of color regions in different cultures can have different sometimes overlapping areas In visual arts color theory is used to govern the use of colors in an asthetically pleasing and harmonious way The theory of color includes the color complements color balance and classification of primary colors traditionally red yellow blue secondary colors traditionally orange green purple and tertiary colors The study of colors in general is called color science Contents 1 Physical properties 1 1 Spectral colors 1 2 Color of objects 2 Color vision 2 1 Development of theories of color vision 2 2 Color in the eye 2 3 Color in the brain 2 4 Nonstandard color perception 2 4 1 Color vision deficiency 2 4 2 Tetrachromacy 2 4 3 Synesthesia 2 5 Afterimages 2 6 Color constancy 3 Reproduction 3 1 Additive coloring 3 2 Subtractive coloring 3 3 Structural color 4 Cultural perspective 4 1 Associations 4 2 Terminology 5 See also 6 References 7 External linksPhysical properties The visible spectrum perceived from 390 710 nm wavelength Electromagnetic radiation is characterized by its wavelength or frequency and its intensity When the wavelength is within the visible spectrum the range of wavelengths humans can perceive approximately from 390 nm to 700 nm it is known as visible light Most light sources emit light at many different wavelengths a source s spectrum is a distribution giving its intensity at each wavelength Although the spectrum of light arriving at the eye from a given direction determines the color sensation in that direction there are many more possible spectral combinations than color sensations In fact one may formally define a color as a class of spectra that give rise to the same color sensation although such classes would vary widely among different species and to a lesser extent among individuals within the same species In each such class the members are called metamers of the color in question This effect can be visualized by comparing the light sources spectral power distributions and the resulting colors Spectral colors Main article Spectral color The familiar colors of the rainbow in the spectrum named using the Latin word for appearance or apparition by Isaac Newton in 1671 include all those colors that can be produced by visible light of a single wavelength only the pure spectral or monochromatic colors The table at right shows approximate frequencies in terahertz and wavelengths in nanometers for spectral colors in the visible range Spectral colors have 100 purity and are fully saturated A complex mixture of spectral colors can be used to describe any color which is the definition of a light power spectrum The color table should not be interpreted as a definitive list the spectral colors form a continuous spectrum and how it is divided into distinct colors linguistically is a matter of culture and historical contingency 1 Despite the ubiquitous ROYGBIV mnemonic used to remember the spectral colors in english the inclusion or exclusion of colors in this table is contentious with disagreement often focused on indigo and cyan 2 Even if the subset of color terms is agreed their wavelength ranges and borders between them may not be The intensity of a spectral color relative to the context in which it is viewed may alter its perception considerably according to the Bezold Brucke shift for example a low intensity orange yellow is brown and a low intensity yellow green is olive green Color of objects Simple depiction of how objects gain color showing selective absorption of light which only describes the first factor that affects perception of an object s color The color of an object as perceived by an observer is not an intrinsic quality of that object but depends on several factors the physics of the object which wavelengths of light are selectively absorbed reflected transmitted or emitted the color of the light shining on the object color cast of the illuminant the angles between observer object and illuminant applicable to structural color the physics of light in its environment how the atmosphere may affect the light through Rayleigh scattering or dispersion for example relative velocity between object and observer red shift mostly applicable to astronomy the characteristics of the perceiving eye the number and spectral sensitivity of cone classes and dimensionality of color vision higher order processes in the brain that affect the color such as color constancySome generalizations of the physics can be drawn neglecting perceptual effects for now Light arriving at an opaque surface is either reflected specularly that is in the manner of a mirror scattered that is reflected with diffuse scattering or absorbed or some combination of these Opaque objects that do not reflect specularly which tend to have rough surfaces have their color determined by which wavelengths of light they scatter strongly with the light that is not scattered being absorbed If objects scatter all wavelengths with roughly equal strength they appear white If they absorb all wavelengths they appear black 3 Opaque objects that specularly reflect the light of different wavelengths with different efficiencies look like mirrors tinted with colors determined by those differences An object that reflects some fraction of impinging light and absorbs the rest may look black but also be faintly reflective examples are black objects coated with layers of enamel or lacquer Objects that transmit light are either translucent scattering the transmitted light or transparent not scattering the transmitted light If they also absorb or reflect light of various wavelengths differentially they appear tinted with a color determined by the nature of that absorption or that reflectance Objects may emit light that they generate from having excited electrons rather than merely reflecting or transmitting light The electrons may be excited due to elevated temperature incandescence as a result of chemical reactions chemiluminescence after absorbing light of other frequencies fluorescence or phosphorescence or from electrical contacts as in light emitting diodes or other light sources To summarize the color of an object is a complex result of its surface properties its transmission properties and its emission properties all of which contribute to the mix of wavelengths in the light leaving the surface of the object The perceived color is then further conditioned by the nature of the ambient illumination and by the color properties of other objects nearby and via other characteristics of the perceiving eye and brain Color visionMain article Color vision Development of theories of color vision Main article Color theory The upper disk and the lower disk have exactly the same objective color and are in identical gray surroundings based on context differences humans perceive the squares as having different reflectances and may interpret the colors as different color categories see checker shadow illusion Although Aristotle and other ancient scientists had already written on the nature of light and color vision it was not until Newton that light was identified as the source of the color sensation In 1810 Goethe published his comprehensive Theory of Colors in which he provided a rational description of colour experience which tells us how it originates not what it is Schopenhauer In 1801 Thomas Young proposed his trichromatic theory based on the observation that any color could be matched with a combination of three lights This theory was later refined by James Clerk Maxwell and Hermann von Helmholtz As Helmholtz puts it the principles of Newton s law of mixture were experimentally confirmed by Maxwell in 1856 Young s theory of color sensations like so much else that this marvelous investigator achieved in advance of his time remained unnoticed until Maxwell directed attention to it 4 At the same time as Helmholtz Ewald Hering developed the opponent process theory of color noting that color blindness and afterimages typically come in opponent pairs red green blue orange yellow violet and black white Ultimately these two theories were synthesized in 1957 by Hurvich and Jameson who showed that retinal processing corresponds to the trichromatic theory while processing at the level of the lateral geniculate nucleus corresponds to the opponent theory 5 In 1931 an international group of experts known as the Commission internationale de l eclairage CIE developed a mathematical color model which mapped out the space of observable colors and assigned a set of three numbers to each Color in the eye Normalized typical human cone cell responses S M and L types to monochromatic spectral stimuli The ability of the human eye to distinguish colors is based upon the varying sensitivity of different cells in the retina to light of different wavelengths Humans are trichromatic the retina contains three types of color receptor cells or cones One type relatively distinct from the other two is most responsive to light that is perceived as blue or blue violet with wavelengths around 450 nm cones of this type are sometimes called short wavelength cones or S cones or misleadingly blue cones The other two types are closely related genetically and chemically middle wavelength cones M cones or green cones are most sensitive to light perceived as green with wavelengths around 540 nm while the long wavelength cones L cones or red cones are most sensitive to light that is perceived as greenish yellow with wavelengths around 570 nm Light no matter how complex its composition of wavelengths is reduced to three color components by the eye Each cone type adheres to the principle of univariance which is that each cone s output is determined by the amount of light that falls on it over all wavelengths For each location in the visual field the three types of cones yield three signals based on the extent to which each is stimulated These amounts of stimulation are sometimes called tristimulus values 6 The response curve as a function of wavelength varies for each type of cone Because the curves overlap some tristimulus values do not occur for any incoming light combination For example it is not possible to stimulate only the mid wavelength so called green cones the other cones will inevitably be stimulated to some degree at the same time The set of all possible tristimulus values determines the human color space It has been estimated that humans can distinguish roughly 10 million different colors 7 The other type of light sensitive cell in the eye the rod has a different response curve In normal situations when light is bright enough to strongly stimulate the cones rods play virtually no role in vision at all 8 On the other hand in dim light the cones are understimulated leaving only the signal from the rods resulting in a colorless response Furthermore the rods are barely sensitive to light in the red range In certain conditions of intermediate illumination the rod response and a weak cone response can together result in color discriminations not accounted for by cone responses alone These effects combined are summarized also in the Kruithof curve which describes the change of color perception and pleasingness of light as a function of temperature and intensity Color in the brain Main article Color vision Color in the primate brain The visual dorsal stream green and ventral stream purple are shown The ventral stream is responsible for color perception While the mechanisms of color vision at the level of the retina are well described in terms of tristimulus values color processing after that point is organized differently A dominant theory of color vision proposes that color information is transmitted out of the eye by three opponent processes or opponent channels each constructed from the raw output of the cones a red green channel a blue yellow channel and a black white luminance channel This theory has been supported by neurobiology and accounts for the structure of our subjective color experience Specifically it explains why humans cannot perceive a reddish green or yellowish blue and it predicts the color wheel it is the collection of colors for which at least one of the two color channels measures a value at one of its extremes The exact nature of color perception beyond the processing already described and indeed the status of color as a feature of the perceived world or rather as a feature of our perception of the world a type of qualia is a matter of complex and continuing philosophical dispute citation needed Nonstandard color perception Color vision deficiency Main article Color blindness A color vision deficiency causes an individual to perceive a smaller gamut of colors than the standard observer with normal color vision The effect can be mild having lower color resolution i e anomalous trichromacy moderate lacking an entire dimension or channel of color e g dichromacy or complete lacking all color perception i e monochromacy Most forms of color blindness derive from one or many of the three classes of cone cells either being missing having a shifted spectral sensitivity or having lower responsiveness to incoming light In addition cerebral achromatopsia is caused by neural anomalies in those parts of the brain where visual processing takes place Some colors that appear distinct to an individual with normal color vision will appear metameric to the color blind The most common form of color blindness is congenital red green color blindness affecting 8 of males Individuals with the strongest form of this condition dichromacy will experience blue and purple green and yellow teal and gray as colors of confusion i e metamers 9 Tetrachromacy Main article Tetrachromacy Outside of humans which are mostly trichromatic having three types of cones most mammals are dichromatic possessing only two cones However outside of mammals most vertebrate are tetrachromatic having four types of cones and includes most birds reptiles amphibians and bony fish An extra dimension of color vision means these vertebrates can see two distinct colors that a normal human would view as metamers Some invertebrates such as the mantis shrimp have an even higher number of cones 12 that could lead to a richer color gamut than even imaginable by humans The existence of human tetrachromats is a contentious notion As many as half of all human females have 4 distinct cone classes which could enable tetrachromacy 10 p 256 However a distinction must be made between retinal or weak tetrachromats which express four cone classes in the retina and functional or strong tetrachromats which are able to make the enhanced color discriminations expected of tetrachromats In fact there is only one peer reviewed report of a functional tetrachromat 11 It is estimated that while the average person is able to see one million colors someone with functional tetrachromacy could see a hundred million colors 12 Synesthesia In certain forms of synesthesia perceiving letters and numbers grapheme color synesthesia or hearing sounds chromesthesia will evoke a perception of color Behavioral and functional neuroimaging experiments have demonstrated that these color experiences lead to changes in behavioral tasks and lead to increased activation of brain regions involved in color perception thus demonstrating their reality and similarity to real color percepts albeit evoked through a non standard route Synesthesia can occur genetically with 4 of the population having variants associated with the condition Synesthesia has also been known to occur with brain damage drugs and sensory deprivation 13 The philosopher Pythagoras experienced synesthesia and provided one of the first written accounts of the condition in approximately 550 BCE He created mathematical equations for musical notes that could form part of a scale such as an octave 14 Afterimages After exposure to strong light in their sensitivity range photoreceptors of a given type become desensitized For a few seconds after the light ceases they will continue to signal less strongly than they otherwise would Colors observed during that period will appear to lack the color component detected by the desensitized photoreceptors This effect is responsible for the phenomenon of afterimages in which the eye may continue to see a bright figure after looking away from it but in a complementary color Afterimage effects have also been used by artists including Vincent van Gogh Color constancy Main article Color constancy When an artist uses a limited color palette the human eye tends to compensate by seeing any gray or neutral color as the color which is missing from the color wheel For example in a limited palette consisting of red yellow black and white a mixture of yellow and black will appear as a variety of green a mixture of red and black will appear as a variety of purple and pure gray will appear bluish 15 The trichromatic theory is strictly true when the visual system is in a fixed state of adaptation In reality the visual system is constantly adapting to changes in the environment and compares the various colors in a scene to reduce the effects of the illumination If a scene is illuminated with one light and then with another as long as the difference between the light sources stays within a reasonable range the colors in the scene appear relatively constant to us This was studied by Edwin H Land in the 1970s and led to his retinex theory of color constancy Both phenomena are readily explained and mathematically modeled with modern theories of chromatic adaptation and color appearance e g CIECAM02 iCAM 16 There is no need to dismiss the trichromatic theory of vision but rather it can be enhanced with an understanding of how the visual system adapts to changes in the viewing environment ReproductionMain article Color reproduction The CIE 1931 color space xy chromaticity diagram with the visual locus plotted using the CIE 2006 physiologically relevant LMS fundamental color matching functions transformed into the CIE 1931 xy color space and converted into Adobe RGB The triangle shows the gamut of Adobe RGB The Planckian locus is shown with color temperatures labeled in Kelvins The outer curved boundary is the spectral or monochromatic locus with wavelengths shown in nanometers Note that the colors in this file are being specified using Adobe RGB Areas outside the triangle cannot be accurately rendered since they are outside the gamut of Adobe RGB therefore they have been interpreted Note that the colors depicted depend on the gamut and color accuracy of your display Color reproduction is the science of creating colors for the human eye that faithfully represent the desired color It focuses on how to construct a spectrum of wavelengths that will best evoke a certain color in an observer Most colors are not spectral colors meaning they are mixtures of various wavelengths of light However these non spectral colors are often described by their dominant wavelength which identifies the single wavelength of light that produces a sensation most similar to the non spectral color Dominant wavelength is roughly akin to hue There are many color perceptions that by definition cannot be pure spectral colors due to desaturation or because they are purples mixtures of red and violet light from opposite ends of the spectrum Some examples of necessarily non spectral colors are the achromatic colors black gray and white and colors such as pink tan and magenta Two different light spectra that have the same effect on the three color receptors in the human eye will be perceived as the same color They are metamers of that color This is exemplified by the white light emitted by fluorescent lamps which typically has a spectrum of a few narrow bands while daylight has a continuous spectrum The human eye cannot tell the difference between such light spectra just by looking into the light source although the color rendering index of each light source may affect the color of objects illuminated by these metameric light sources Similarly most human color perceptions can be generated by a mixture of three colors called primaries This is used to reproduce color scenes in photography printing television and other media There are a number of methods or color spaces for specifying a color in terms of three particular primary colors Each method has its advantages and disadvantages depending on the particular application No mixture of colors however can produce a response truly identical to that of a spectral color although one can get close especially for the longer wavelengths where the CIE 1931 color space chromaticity diagram has a nearly straight edge For example mixing green light 530 nm and blue light 460 nm produces cyan light that is slightly desaturated because response of the red color receptor would be greater to the green and blue light in the mixture than it would be to a pure cyan light at 485 nm that has the same intensity as the mixture of blue and green Because of this and because the primaries in color printing systems generally are not pure themselves the colors reproduced are never perfectly saturated spectral colors and so spectral colors cannot be matched exactly However natural scenes rarely contain fully saturated colors thus such scenes can usually be approximated well by these systems The range of colors that can be reproduced with a given color reproduction system is called the gamut The CIE chromaticity diagram can be used to describe the gamut Another problem with color reproduction systems is connected with the initial measurement of color or colorimetry The characteristics of the color sensors in measurement devices e g cameras scanners are often very far from the characteristics of the receptors in the human eye A color reproduction system tuned to a human with normal color vision may give very inaccurate results for other observers according to color vision deviations to the standard observer The different color response of different devices can be problematic if not properly managed For color information stored and transferred in digital form color management techniques such as those based on ICC profiles can help to avoid distortions of the reproduced colors Color management does not circumvent the gamut limitations of particular output devices but can assist in finding good mapping of input colors into the gamut that can be reproduced Additive coloring Additive color mixing combining red and green yields yellow combining all three primary colors together yields white Additive color is light created by mixing together light of two or more different colors Red green and blue are the additive primary colors normally used in additive color systems such as projectors and computer terminals Subtractive coloring Subtractive color mixing combining yellow and magenta yields red combining all three primary colors together yields black Twelve main pigment colors Subtractive coloring uses dyes inks pigments or filters to absorb some wavelengths of light and not others The color that a surface displays comes from the parts of the visible spectrum that are not absorbed and therefore remain visible Without pigments or dye fabric fibers paint base and paper are usually made of particles that scatter white light all colors well in all directions When a pigment or ink is added wavelengths are absorbed or subtracted from white light so light of another color reaches the eye If the light is not a pure white source the case of nearly all forms of artificial lighting the resulting spectrum will appear a slightly different color Red paint viewed under blue light may appear black Red paint is red because it scatters only the red components of the spectrum If red paint is illuminated by blue light it will be absorbed by the red paint creating the appearance of a black object Structural color Further information Structural coloration and Animal coloration Structural colors are colors caused by interference effects rather than by pigments Color effects are produced when a material is scored with fine parallel lines formed of one or more parallel thin layers or otherwise composed of microstructures on the scale of the color s wavelength If the microstructures are spaced randomly light of shorter wavelengths will be scattered preferentially to produce Tyndall effect colors the blue of the sky Rayleigh scattering caused by structures much smaller than the wavelength of light in this case air molecules the luster of opals and the blue of human irises If the microstructures are aligned in arrays for example the array of pits in a CD they behave as a diffraction grating the grating reflects different wavelengths in different directions due to interference phenomena separating mixed white light into light of different wavelengths If the structure is one or more thin layers then it will reflect some wavelengths and transmit others depending on the layers thickness Structural color is studied in the field of thin film optics The most ordered or the most changeable structural colors are iridescent Structural color is responsible for the blues and greens of the feathers of many birds the blue jay for example as well as certain butterfly wings and beetle shells Variations in the pattern s spacing often give rise to an iridescent effect as seen in peacock feathers soap bubbles films of oil and mother of pearl because the reflected color depends upon the viewing angle Numerous scientists have carried out research in butterfly wings and beetle shells including Isaac Newton and Robert Hooke Since 1942 electron micrography has been used advancing the development of products that exploit structural color such as photonic cosmetics 17 Cultural perspectiveColors their meanings and associations can play a major role in works of art including literature 18 Associations Individual colors have a variety of cultural associations such as national colors in general described in individual color articles and color symbolism The field of color psychology attempts to identify the effects of color on human emotion and activity Chromotherapy is a form of alternative medicine attributed to various Eastern traditions Colors have different associations in different countries and cultures 19 Different colors have been demonstrated to have effects on cognition For example researchers at the University of Linz in Austria demonstrated that the color red significantly decreases cognitive functioning in men 20 The combination of the colors red and yellow together can induce hunger which has been capitalized on by a number of chain restaurants 21 Color plays a role in memory development too A photograph that is in black and white is slightly less memorable than one in color 22 Studies also show that wearing bright colors makes you more memorable to people you meet Terminology Main article Color term See also Lists of colors and Web colors Colors vary in several different ways including hue shades of red orange yellow green blue and violet saturation brightness and gloss Some color words are derived from the name of an object of that color such as orange or salmon while others are abstract like red In the 1969 study Basic Color Terms Their Universality and Evolution Brent Berlin and Paul Kay describe a pattern in naming basic colors like red but not red orange or dark red or blood red which are shades of red All languages that have two basic color names distinguish dark cool colors from bright warm colors The next colors to be distinguished are usually red and then yellow or green All languages with six basic colors include black white red green blue and yellow The pattern holds up to a set of twelve black gray white pink red orange yellow green blue purple brown and azure distinct from blue in Russian and Italian but not English See alsoChromophore Color analysis art Color in Chinese culture Color mapping Complementary color Impossible color International Color Consortium International Commission on Illumination Lists of colors compact version Neutral color Pearlescent coating including Metal effect pigments Pseudocolor Primary secondary and tertiary colorsReferences Berlin B and Kay P Basic Color Terms Their Universality and Evolution Berkeley University of California Press 1969 Waldman Gary 2002 Introduction to light the physics of light vision and color Mineola Dover Publications p 193 ISBN 978 0 486 42118 6 Pastoureau Michael 2008 Black The History of a Color Princeton University Press p 216 ISBN 978 0691139302 Hermann von Helmholtz Physiological Optics The Sensations of Vision 1866 as translated in Sources of Color Science David L MacAdam ed Cambridge MIT Press 1970 Palmer S E 1999 Vision Science Photons to Phenomenology Cambridge MA MIT Press ISBN 0 262 16183 4 Magazine Nicola Jones Knowable Color Is in the Eye and Brain of the Beholder Scientific American Retrieved 2022 11 08 Judd Deane B Wyszecki Gunter 1975 Color in Business Science and Industry Wiley Series in Pure and Applied Optics third ed New York Wiley Interscience p 388 ISBN 978 0 471 45212 6 Under well lit viewing conditions photopic vision cones are highly active and rods are inactive Hirakawa K Parks T W 2005 Chromatic Adaptation and White Balance Problem PDF IEEE International Conference on Image Processing 2005 IEEE ICIP pp III 984 doi 10 1109 ICIP 2005 1530559 ISBN 0 7803 9134 9 Archived from the original PDF on November 28 2006 Fluck Daniel Colorblind colors of confusion Colblindor Retrieved 14 November 2022 Jameson K A Highnote S M Wasserman L M 2001 Richer color experience in observers with multiple photopigment opsin genes PDF Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 8 2 244 61 doi 10 3758 BF03196159 PMID 11495112 S2CID 2389566 Archived PDF from the original on 2013 10 04 Jordan G Deeb S S Bosten J M Mollon J D 20 July 2010 The dimensionality of color vision in carriers of anomalous trichromacy Journal of Vision 10 8 12 doi 10 1167 10 8 12 PMID 20884587 Kershner Kate 26 July 2016 Lucky Tetrachromats See World With Up to 100 Million Colors Retrieved 9 February 2022 Brang David 22 November 2011 Survival of the Synesthesia Gene Why Do People Hear Colors and Taste Words PLOS Biology 9 11 e1001205 doi 10 1371 journal pbio 1001205 PMC 3222625 PMID 22131906 A Brief History of Synesthesia in the Arts Retrieved 9 February 2022 Depauw Robert C United States Patent Archived from the original on 6 January 2012 Retrieved 20 March 2011 M D Fairchild Color Appearance Models Archived May 5 2011 at the Wayback Machine 2nd Ed Wiley Chichester 2005 Economic and Social Research Council Science in the Dock Art in the Stocks Archived from the original on November 2 2007 Retrieved 2007 10 07 Gary Westfahl 2005 The Greenwood Encyclopedia of Science Fiction and Fantasy Themes Works and Wonders Greenwood Publishing Group pp 142 143 ISBN 978 0 313 32951 7 Chart Color Meanings by Culture Archived from the original on 2010 10 12 Retrieved 2010 06 29 Dzulkifli Mariam Mustafar Muhammad 2013 The Influence of Colour on Memory Performance A Review The Malaysian Journal of Medical Sciences 20 2 3 9 doi 10 1016 j chb 2010 06 010 There s a sneaky reason why you always see red and yellow on fast food logos Business Insider Retrieved 2022 02 09 Gnambs Timo Appel Markus Batinic Bernad 2010 Color red in web based knowledge testing Computers in Human Behavior 26 6 1625 31 doi 10 1016 j chb 2010 06 010 External linksColor at the Encyclopaedia Britannica Maund Barry Color In Zalta Edward N ed Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy Color Internet Encyclopedia of Philosophy Portals Technology Books Electronics Physics PaintingColor at Wikipedia s sister projects Media from Commons Quotations from Wikiquote Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Color amp oldid 1132854291, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.