fbpx
Wikipedia

Color blindness

Color blindness or color vision deficiency (CVD) is the decreased ability to see color or differences in color.[2] It can impair tasks such as selecting ripe fruit, choosing clothing, and reading traffic lights.[2] Color blindness may make some academic activities more difficult.[2] However, issues are generally minor, and people with colorblindness automatically develop adaptations and coping mechanisms.[2] People with total color blindness (achromatopsia) may also be uncomfortable in bright environments[2] and have decreased visual acuity.

Color blindness
Other namesColor vision deficiency, impaired color vision[1]
Example of an Ishihara color test plate. Viewers with normal color vision should clearly see the number "74".
SpecialtyOphthalmology
SymptomsDecreased ability to see colors[2]
DurationLong term[2]
CausesGenetic (inherited usually X-linked)[2]
Diagnostic methodIshihara color test[2]
TreatmentAdjustments to teaching methods, mobile apps[1][2]
FrequencyRed–green: 8% males, 0.5% females (Northern European descent)[2]

The most common cause of color blindness is an inherited problem or variation in the functionality of one or more of the three classes of cone cells in the retina, which mediate color vision.[2] The most common form is caused by a genetic disorder called congenital red–green color blindness. Males are more likely to be color blind than females, because the genes responsible for the most common forms of color blindness are on the X chromosome.[2] Females who are not color-blind can carry genes for color blindness and pass them on to their children.[2] Color blindness can also result from physical or chemical damage to the eye, the optic nerve, or parts of the brain.[2] Screening for color blindness is typically done with the Ishihara color test.[2]

There is no cure for color blindness.[2] Diagnosis may allow an individual, or their parents/teachers to actively accommodate the condition.[1] Special lenses such as EnChroma glasses or X-chrom contact lenses may help people with red–green color blindness at some color tasks,[2] but they do not grant the wearer "normal color vision".[3] Mobile apps can help people identify colors.[2]

Red–green color blindness is the most common form, followed by blue–yellow color blindness and total color blindness.[2] Red–green color blindness affects up to 1 in 12 males (8%) and 1 in 200 females (0.5%).[2][4] The ability to see color also decreases in old age.[2] In certain countries, color blindness may make people ineligible for certain jobs,[1] such as those of aircraft pilots, train drivers, crane operators, and people in the armed forces.[1][5] The effect of color blindness on artistic ability is controversial,[1][6] but a number of famous artists are believed to have been color blind.[1][7]

This article is about color blindness in humans, but other organisms also have color blindness. Many species have color vision that is different from human vision, with either a limited or extended range of visible colors as compared with humans.

Effects

A person who is colorblind will have decreased (or no) color discrimination along the red–green axis, blue–yellow axis, or both. However, the vast majority of people with colorblindness are only affected on their red–green axis.

The first indication of colorblindness generally consists of a person using the wrong color for an object, such as when painting, or calling a color by the wrong name. The colors that are confused are very consistent among people with the same type of color blindness.

Confusion colors

 
Confusion Lines for the three types of Dichromacy superimposed on CIEXYZ color space.

Confusion colors are pairs or groups of colors that will often be mistaken by people with colorblindness. Confusion colors for red–green color blindness include:

  • cyan and grey
  • rose-pink and grey
  • blue and purple
  • yellow and neon green
  • red, green, orange, brown

Confusion colors for tritan include:

  • yellow and grey
  • blue and green
  • dark blue/violet and black
  • violet and yellow-green
  • red and rose-pink

These colors of confusion are defined quantitatively by straight confusion lines plotted in CIEXYZ, usually plotted on the corresponding chromaticity diagram. The lines all intersect at a copunctal point, which varies with the type of color blindness.[8] Chromaticities along a confusion line will appear metameric to people with dichromacy of that type. People with trichromacy of that type will see the chromaticities as metameric if they are close enough, depending on the strength of their CVD. For two colors on a confusion line to be metameric, the chromaticities first have to be made isoluminant, meaning equal in lightness. Also, colors that may be isoluminant to the standard observer may not be isoluminant to a person with dichromacy.

Color tasks

Cole describes four color tasks, all of which are impeded to some degree by color blindness:[9]

  • Comparative – When multiple colors must be compared, such as with mixing paint
  • Connotative – When colors are given an implicit meaning, such as red = stop
  • Denotative – When identifying colors, for example by name, such as "where is the yellow ball?"
  • Aesthetic – When colors look nice – or convey an emotional response – but don't carry explicit meaning

The following sections describe specific color tasks with which people with colorblindness typically have difficulty.

Food

 
Simulation of the normal (above) and dichromatic (below) perception of red and green apples

Colorblindness causes difficulty with the connotative color tasks associated with selecting or preparing food. Selecting food for ripeness can be difficult. The green–yellow transition of bananas is particularly hard to identify. It can also be difficult to detect bruises, mold or rot on some foods, to determine when meat is done by color, to distinguish some varietals, such as a Braeburn vs. a Granny Smith apple, or to distinguish colors associated with artificial flavors (e.g. jelly beans, sports drinks).

Skin color

Changes in skin color due to bruising, sunburn, rashes or even blushing are easily missed by people with red–green colorblindness.

Traffic lights

 
The lack of standard positional clues makes this light difficult to interpret.

The colors of traffic lights can be difficult for people with red–green colorblindness. This difficulty includes distinguishing red/amber lights from sodium street lamps, distinguishing green lights (closer to cyan) from normal white lights, and distinguishing red from amber lights, especially when there are no positional clues available (see image).

 
The infamous inverted traffic light in Syracuse, NY

The main coping mechanism to overcome these challenges is to memorize the position of lights. The order of the common triplet traffic light is standardized as red–amber–green from top to bottom or left to right. Cases that deviate from this standard are rare. One such case is a traffic light in Tipperary Hill in Syracuse, New York, which is upside-down (green–amber–red top to bottom) due to the sentiments of its Irish American community.[10] However, the light has been criticized due to the potential hazard it poses for color-blind drivers.[11]

 
Horizontal traffic light in Halifax, Nova Scotia, Canada

There are other several features of traffic lights available that help accommodate people with colorblindness. British Rail signals use more easily identifiable colors: The red is blood red, the amber is yellow and the green is a bluish color.[citation needed] Most British road traffic lights are mounted vertically on a black rectangle with a white border (forming a "sighting board"), so that drivers can more easily look for the position of the light. In the eastern provinces of Canada, traffic lights are sometimes differentiated by shape in addition to color: square for red, diamond for yellow, and circle for green (see image).

Signal lights

Navigation lights in marine and aviation settings employ red and green lights to signal the relative position of other ships or aircraft. Railway signal lights also rely heavily on red–green–yellow colors. In both cases, these color combinations can be difficult for people with red–green colorblindness. Lantern Tests are a common means of simulating these light sources to determine not necessarily whether someone is colorblind, but whether they can functionally distinguish these specific signal colors. Those who cannot pass this test are generally completely restricted from working on aircraft, ships or rail.

Fashion

Color analysis is the analysis of color in its use in fashion, to determine personal color combinations that are most aesthetically pleasing.[citation needed] Colors to combine can include clothing, accessories, makeup, hair color, skin color, eye color, etc. Color analysis involves many aesthetic and comparative color task that can be difficult for people with color blindness.

Art

Inability to distinguish color does not necessarily preclude the ability to become a celebrated artist. The 20th century expressionist painter Clifton Pugh, three-time winner of Australia's Archibald Prize, on biographical, gene inheritance and other grounds has been identified as a person with protanopy.[12] 19th century French artist Charles Méryon became successful by concentrating on etching rather than painting after he was diagnosed as having a red–green deficiency.[13] Jin Kim's red–green color blindness did not stop him from becoming first an animator and later a character designer with Walt Disney Animation Studios.[14]

Advantages

People with deuteranomaly are better at distinguishing shades of khaki,[15] which may be advantageous when looking for predators, food, or camouflaged objects hidden among foliage.[16] People with dichromacy tend to learn to use texture and shape clues and so may be able to penetrate camouflage that has been designed to deceive individuals with normal color vision.[17][18]

Some tentative evidence finds that people with color blindness are better at penetrating certain color camouflages. Such findings may give an evolutionary reason for the high rate of red–green color blindness.[17] There is also a study suggesting that people with some types of color blindness can distinguish colors that people with normal color vision are not able to distinguish.[19] In World War II, color blind observers were used to penetrate camouflage.[20]

In the presence of chromatic noise, people with colorblindness are more capable of seeing a luminous signal, as long as the chromatic noise appears metameric to them.[21] This is the effect behind most "reverse" Pseudoisochromatic plates (e.g. "hidden digit" Ishihara plates) that are discernible to people with colorblindness but unreadable to people with typical color perception.[citation needed]

Digital design

 
Testing the colors of a web chart, (center), to ensure that no information is lost to the various forms of color blindness.

Color codes are useful tools for designers to convey information. The interpretation of this information requires users to perform a variety of Color Tasks, usually comparative but also sometimes connotative or denotative. However, these tasks are often problematic for people with colorblindness when design of the color code has not followed best practices for accessibility.[22] For example, one of the most ubiquitous connotative color codes is the "red means bad and green means good" or similar systems, based on the classic signal light colors. However, this color coding will almost always be undifferentiable to people with either deutan or protan CVD, and therefore should be avoided or supplemented with a parallel connotative system (symbols, smileys, etc.).

Good practices to ensure design is accessible to people with colorblindness include:

  • When possible (e.g. in simple video games or apps), allowing the user to choose their own colors is the most inclusive design practice.
  • Using other signals that are parallel to the color coding, such as patterns, shapes, size or order.[23] This not only helps people with color blindness, but also aids understanding by normally sighted people by providing them with multiple reinforcing cues.
  • Using brightness contrast (different shades) in addition to color contrast (different hues)
  • To achieve good contrast, conventional wisdom suggests converting a (digital) design to grayscale to ensure there is sufficient brightness contrast between colors. However, this does not account for the different perceptions of brightness to different varieties of colorblindness, especially protan CVD, tritan CVD and monochromacy.
  • Viewing the design through a CVD Simulator to ensure the information carried by color is still sufficiently conveyed. At a minimum, the design should be tested for deutan CVD, the most common kind of colorblindness.
  • Maximizing the area of colors (e.g. increase size, thickness or boldness of colored element) makes the color easier to identify. Color contrast improves as the angle the color subtends on the retina increases. This applies to all types of color vision.
  • Maximizing brightness (value) and saturation (chroma) of the colors to maximize color contrast.
  • Converting connotative tasks to comparative tasks by including a legend, even when the meaning is considered obvious (e.g. red means danger).
  • Avoiding denotative color tasks (color naming) when possible. Some denotative tasks can be converted to comparative tasks by depicting the actual color whenever the color name is mentioned; for example, colored typography in "purple",  purple  or "purple ()".
  • For denotative tasks (color naming), using the most common shades of colors. For example, green and yellow are colors of confusion in red–green CVD, but it is very common to mix forest green () with bright yellow (). Mistakes by people with colorblindness increase drastically when uncommon shades are used, e.g. neon green () with dark yellow ().
  • For denotative tasks, using colors that are classically associated with a color name. For example, use "firetruck" red () instead of burgundy () to represent the word "red".

Unordered Information

 
Colors of Boardgame pieces must be carefully chosen to be accessible to people with colorblindness

A common task for designers is to select a subset of colors (qualitative colormap) that are as mutually differentiable as possible (salient). For example, player pieces in a board game should be as different as possible.

Classic advice suggests using Brewer palettes, but several of these are not actually accessible to people with colorblindness.

Unfortunately, the colors with the greatest contrast to the red–green colorblind tend to be colors of confusion to the blue–yellow colorblind, and vice versa. However, since red–green is much more prevalent than blue–yellow CVD, design should[according to whom?] generally prioritize those users (people with deutan CVD then protan CVD).

Ordered Information

 
Three sequential colormaps that have been designed to be accessible to people with color blindness.

A common task for data visualization is to represent a color scale, or sequential colormap, often in the form of a heat map or choropleth. Several scales are designed with special consideration for people with colorblindness and are widespread in academia, including Cividis,[24] Viridis[24] and Parula. These comprise a light-to-dark scale superimposed on a yellow-to-blue scale, making them monotonic and perceptually uniform to all forms of color vision.

Classification

 
These color charts show how different colorblind people see compared to a person with normal color vision.[dubious ]

Much terminology has existed and does exist for the classification of color blindness, but the typical classification for color blindness follows the von Kries classifications,[25] which uses severity and affected cone for naming.

Based on severity

Based on clinical appearance, color blindness may be described as total or partial. Total color blindness (monochromacy) is much less common than partial color blindness.[26] Partial colorblindness includes dichromacy and anomalous trichromacy, but is often clinically defined as mild, moderate or strong.

Monochromacy

Monochromacy is often called total color blindness since there is no ability to see color. Although the term may refer to acquired disorders such as cerebral achromatopsia, it typically refers to congenital color vision disorders, namely rod monochromacy and blue cone monochromacy).[27][28]

In cerebral achromatopsia, a person cannot perceive colors even though the eyes are capable of distinguishing them. Some sources do not consider these to be true color blindness, because the failure is of perception, not of vision. They are forms of visual agnosia.[28]

Monochromacy is the condition of possessing only a single channel for conveying information about color. Monochromats are unable to distinguish any colors and perceive only variations in brightness. Congenital monochromacy occurs in two primary forms:

  1. Rod monochromacy, frequently called complete achromatopsia, where the retina contains no cone cells, so that in addition to the absence of color discrimination, vision in lights of normal intensity is difficult.
  2. Cone monochromacy is the condition of having only a single class of cone. A cone monochromat can have good pattern vision at normal daylight levels, but will not be able to distinguish hues. Cone monochromacy is divided into classes defined by the single remaining cone class. However, red and green cone monochromats have not been definitively described in the literature. Blue cone monochromacy is caused by lack of functionality of L (red) and M (green) cones, and is therefore mediated by the same genes as red–green color blindness (on the X chromosome). Peak spectral sensitivities are in the blue region of the visible spectrum (near 440 nm). People with this condition generally show nystagmus ("jiggling eyes"), photophobia (light sensitivity), reduced visual acuity, and myopia (nearsightedness).[29] Visual acuity usually falls to the 20/50 to 20/400 range.

Dichromacy

Dichromats can match any color they see with some mixture of just two primary colors (in contrast to those with normal sight (trichromats) who can distinguish three primary colors). Dichromats usually know they have a color vision problem, and it can affect their daily lives. Dichromacy in humans includes protanopia, deuteranopia, and tritanopia. Out of the male population, 2% have severe difficulties distinguishing between red, orange, yellow, and green. (Orange and yellow are different combinations of red and green light.) Colors in this range, which appear very different to a normal viewer, appear to a dichromat to be the same or a similar color. The terms protanopia, deuteranopia, and tritanopia come from Greek, and respectively mean "inability to see (anopia) with the first (prot-), second (deuter-), or third (trit-) [cone]".

Anomalous trichromacy

Anomalous trichromacy is the mildest type of color deficiency, but the severity ranges from almost dichromacy (strong) to almost normal trichromacy (mild).[30] In fact, many mild anomalous trichromats have very little difficulty carrying out tasks that require normal color vision and some may not even be aware that they have a color vision deficiency. The types of anomalous trichromacy include protanomaly, deuteranomaly and tritanomaly. It is approximately three times more common than dichromacy.[31] Anomalous trichromats exhibit trichromacy, but the color matches they make differ from normal trichromats. In order to match a given spectral yellow light, protanomalous observers need more red light in a red/green mixture than a normal observer, and deuteranomalous observers need more green. This difference can be measured by an instrument called an Anomaloscope, where red and green lights are mixed by a subject to match a yellow light.[32]

Based on affected cone

There are two major types of color blindness: difficulty distinguishing between red and green, and difficulty distinguishing between blue and yellow.[33][34][dubious ] These definitions are based on the phenotype of the partial colorblindness. Clinically, it is more common to use a genotypical definition, which describes which cone/opsin is affected.

Red–green color blindness

Red–green color blindness includes protan and deutan CVD. Protan CVD is related to the L-cone and includes protanomaly (anomalous trichromacy) and protanopia (dichromacy). Deutan CVD is related to the M-cone and includes deuteranomaly (anomalous trichromacy) and deuteranopia (dichromacy).[35][36] The phenotype (visual experience) of deutans and protans is quite similar. Common colors of confusion include red/brown/green/yellow as well as blue/purple. Both forms are almost always symptomatic of congenital red–green color blindness, so affects males disproportionately more than females.[37] This form of colorblindness is sometimes referred to as daltonism after John Dalton, who had red–green dichromacy. In some languages, daltonism is still used to describe red–green color blindness.

 
Illustration of the distribution of cone cells in the fovea of an individual with normal color vision (left), and a color blind (protanopic) retina. The center of the fovea holds very few blue-sensitive cones.

  • Protan (2% of males): Lacking, or possessing anomalous L-opsins for long-wavelength sensitive cone cells. Protans have a neutral point at a cyan-like wavelength around 492 nm (see spectral color for comparison)—that is, they cannot discriminate light of this wavelength from white. For a protanope, the brightness of red, is much reduced compared to normal.[38] This dimming can be so pronounced that reds may be confused with black or dark gray, and red traffic lights may appear to be extinguished. They may learn to distinguish reds from yellows primarily on the basis of their apparent brightness or lightness, not on any perceptible hue difference. Violet, lavender, and purple are indistinguishable from various shades of blue. A very few people have been found who have one normal eye and one protanopic eye. These unilateral dichromats report that with only their protanopic eye open, they see wavelengths shorter than neutral point as blue and those longer than it as yellow.

  • Deutan (6% of males): Lacking, or possessing anomalous M-opsins for medium-wavelength sensitive cone cells. Their neutral point is at a slightly longer wavelength, 498 nm, a more greenish hue of cyan. Deutans have the same hue discrimination problems as protans, but without the dimming of long wavelengths. Deuteranopic unilateral dichromats report that with only their deuteranopic eye open, they see wavelengths shorter than neutral point as blue and longer than it as yellow.[39]

Blue–yellow color blindness

Blue–yellow color blindness includes tritan CVD. Tritan CVD is related to the S-cone and includes tritanomaly (anomalous trichromacy) and tritanopia (dichromacy). Blue–yellow color blindness is much less common than red–green color blindness, and more often has acquired causes than genetic. Tritans have difficulty discerning between bluish and greenish hues.[40] Tritans have a neutral point at 571 nm (yellowish).[citation needed]

  • Tritan (<0.01% of individuals): Lacking, or possessing anomalous S-opsins or short-wavelength sensitive cone cells. Tritans see short-wavelength colors (blue, indigo and spectral violet) as greenish and drastically dimmed, some of these colors even as black. Yellow and orange are indistinguishable from white and pink respectively, and purple colors are perceived as various shades of red. Unlike protans and deutans, the mutation for this color blindness is carried on chromosome 7. Therefore, it is not sex-linked (equally prevalent in both males and females). The OMIM gene code for this mutation is 304000 "Colorblindness, Partial Tritanomaly".[41]

  • Tetartan is the "fourth type" of colorblindness, and a type of blue–yellow color blindness. However, its existence is hypothetical and given the molecular basis of human color vision, it is unlikely this type could exist.[citation needed]

Summary of cone complements

The below table shows the cone complements for different types of human color vision, including those considered color blindness, normal color vision and 'superior' color vision. The cone complement contains the types of cones (or their opsins) expressed by an individual.

Cone system Red Green Blue N=normal
A=anomalous
N A N A N A
1 Normal vision Trichromacy Normal
2 Protanomaly Anomalous trichromacy Partial
color
blindness
Red–
green
3 Protanopia Dichromacy
4 Deuteranomaly Anomalous trichromacy
5 Deuteranopia Dichromacy
6 Tritanomaly Anomalous trichromacy Blue–
yellow
7 Tritanopia Dichromacy
8 Blue Cone Monochromacy Monochromacy Total color blindness
9 Achromatopsia
10 Tetrachromacy
(carrier theory)
Tetrachromacy 'Superior'
11

Causes

Color blindness is any deviation of color vision from normal trichromatic color vision (often as defined by the standard observer) that produces a reduced gamut. Mechanisms for color blindness are related to the functionality of cone cells, and often to the expression of photopsins, the photopigments that 'catch' photons and thereby convert light into chemical signals.

Color vision deficiencies can be classified as inherited or acquired.

  • Inherited: inherited or congenital/genetic color vision deficiencies are most commonly caused by mutations of the genes encoding opsin proteins. However, several other genes can also lead to less common and/or more severe forms of color blindness.
  • Acquired: color blindness that is not present at birth, may be caused by chronic illness, accidents, medication, chemical exposure or simply normal aging processes.[42]

Genetics

Color blindness is typically an inherited genetic disorder. The most common forms of colorblindness are associated with the Photopsin genes, but the mapping of the human genome has shown there are many causative mutations that don't directly affect the opsins. Mutations capable of causing color blindness originate from at least 19 different chromosomes and 56 different genes (as shown online at the Online Mendelian Inheritance in Man [OMIM]).

Genetics of red–green color blindness

 
Punnett squares for each combination of parents' color vision status giving probabilities of their offsprings' status; A superscript 'c' denotes a chromosome with an affected gene

By far the most common form of colorblindness is congenital red–green color blindness (Daltonism), which includes protanopia/protanomaly and deuteranopia/deuteranomaly. These conditions are mediated by the OPN1LW and OPN1MW genes, respectively, both on the X chromosome. An 'affected' gene is either missing (as in Protanopia and Deuteranopia - Dichromacy) or is a chimeric gene (as in Protanomaly and Deuteranomaly).

Since the OPN1LW and OPN1MW genes are on the X chromosome, they are sex-linked, and therefore affect males and females disproportionately. Because the colorblind 'affected' alleles are recessive, color blindness specifically follows X-linked recessive inheritance. Males have only one X chromosome (XY), and females have two (XX); Because the male only has one of each gene, if it is affected, the male will be colorblind. Because a female has two alleles of each gene (one on each chromosome), if only one gene is affected, the dominant normal alleles will "override" the affected, recessive allele and the female will have normal color vision. However, if the female has two mutated alleles, she will still be colorblind. This is why there is a disproportionate prevalence of colorblindness, with ~8% of males exhibiting colorblindness and ~0.5% of females.

Genetics of blue–yellow color blindness

Blue–yellow color blindness is a rarer form of colorblindness including tritanopia/tritanomaly. These conditions are mediated by the OPN1SW gene on Chromosome 7.

Other genetic causes

Several inherited diseases are known to cause color blindness:

They can be congenital (from birth) or can commence in childhood or adulthood. They can be stationary, that is, remain the same throughout a person's lifetime, or progressive. As progressive phenotypes involve deterioration of the retina and other parts of the eye, many of the above forms of color blindness can progress to legal blindness, i.e. an acuity of 6/60 (20/200) or worse, and often leave a person with complete blindness.

Non-genetic causes

Physical trauma can cause color blindness, either neurologically – brain trauma which produces swelling of the brain in the occipital lobe – or retinally, either acute (e.g. from laser exposure) or chronic (e.g. from ultraviolet light exposure).

Color blindness may also present itself as a symptom of degenerative diseases of the eye, such as cataract and age-related macular degeneration, and as part of the retinal damage caused by diabetes. Vitamin A deficiency may also cause color blindness.[43]

Color blindness may be a side effect of prescription drug use. For example, red–green color blindness can be caused by ethambutol, a drug used in the treatment of tuberculosis.[44] Blue–yellow color blindness can be caused by sildenafil, an active component of Viagra.[45] Hydroxychloroquine can also lead to hydroxychloroquine retinopathy, which includes various color defects.[46] Exposure to chemicals such as styrene[47] or organic solvents[48][49] can also lead to color vision defects.

Simple colored filters can also create mild color vision deficiencies. John Dalton's original hypothesis for his deuteranopia was actually that the vitreous humor of his eye was discolored:

I was led to conjecture that one of the humours of my eye must be a transparent, but coloured, medium, so constituted as to absorb red and green rays principally... I suppose it must be the vitreous humor.

— John Dalton, Extraordinary facts relating to the vision of colours: with observations (1798)

An autopsy of his eye after his death in 1844 showed this to be definitively untrue,[50] though other filters are possible. Actual physiological examples usually affect the blue–yellow opponent channel and are named Cyanopsia and Xanthopsia, and are most typically an effect of yellowing or removal of the lens.

The opponent channels can also be affected by the prevalence of certain cones in the retinal mosaic. The cones are not equally prevalent and not evenly distributed in the retina. When the number of one of these cone types is significantly reduced, this can also lead to or contribute to a color vision deficiency. This is one of the causes of tritanomaly.

Diagnosis

Color vision test

 
An Ishihara test image as seen by subjects with normal color vision and by those with a variety of color deficiencies

The main method for diagnosing a color vision deficiency is in testing the color vision directly. The Ishihara color test is the test most often used to detect red–green deficiencies and most often recognized by the public.[1] Some tests are clinical in nature, designed to be fast, simple, and effective at identifying broad categories of color blindness. Others focus on precision and are generally available only in academic settings.[51]

  • pseudoisochromatic plates, a classification which includes the Ishihara color test and HRR test, embed a figure in the plate as a number of spots surrounded by spots of a slightly different color. These colors must appear identical (metameric) to the colorblind but distinguishable to color normals. Pseudoisochromatic plates are used as screening tools because they are cheap, fast, and simple, but they do not provide precise diagnosis of CVD.
  • Lanterns, such as the Farnsworth Lantern Test, project small colored lights to a subject, who is required to identify the color of the lights. The colors are those of typical signal lights, i.e. red, green, and yellow, which also happen to be colors of confusion of red–green CVD. Lanterns do not diagnose colorblindness, but they are occupational screening tests to ensure an applicant has sufficient color discrimination to be able to perform a job.
 
A Farnsworth D-15 test
  • Arrangement tests can be used as screening or diagnostic tools. The Farnsworth–Munsell 100 hue test is very sensitive, but the Farnsworth D-15 is a simplified version used specifically for screening for CVD. In either case, the subject is asked to arrange a set of colored caps or chips to form a gradual transition of color between two anchor caps.[52]
  • Anomaloscopes are typically designed to detect red–green deficiencies and are based on the Rayleigh match, which compares a mixture of red and green light in variable proportions to a fixed spectral yellow of variable luminosity. The subject must change the two variables until the colors appear to match. They are expensive and require expertise to administer, so they are generally only used in academic settings.

Genetic testing

While genetic testing cannot directly evaluate a subject's color vision (phenotype), most congenital color vision deficiencies are well-correlated with genotype. Therefore, the genotype can be directly evaluated and used to predict the phenotype. This is especially useful for progressive forms that do not have a strongly color deficient phenotype at a young age. However, it can also be used to sequence the L- and M-Opsins on the X-Chromosome, since the most common alleles of these two genes are known and have even been related to exact spectral sensitivities and peak wavelengths. A subject's color vision can therefore be classified through genetic testing,[53] but this is just a prediction of the phenotype, since color vision can be affected by countless non-genetic factors such as your cone mosaic.

Management

Despite much recent improvement in gene therapy for color blindness, there is currently no FDA approved treatment for any form of CVD, and otherwise no cure for CVD currently exists. Management of the condition by using lenses to alleviate symptoms or smartphone apps to aid with daily tasks is possible.

Lenses

There are three kinds of lenses that an individual can wear that can increase their accuracy in some color related tasks (although none of these will "fix" color blindness or grant the wearer normal color vision):

  • A red-tint contact lens worn over the non-dominant eye, will leverage binocular disparity to improve discrimination of some colors. However, it can make other colors more difficult to distinguish. A 1981 review of various studies to evaluate the effect of the X-chrom (one brand) contact lens concluded that, while the lens may allow the wearer to achieve a better score on certain color vision tests, it did not correct color vision in the natural environment.[54] A case history using the X-Chrom lens for a rod monochromat is reported[55] and an X-Chrom manual is online.[56]
  • Tinted glasses (e.g. Pilestone/Colorlite glasses) apply a tint (e.g. magenta) to incoming light that can distort colors in a way that makes some color tasks easier to complete. These glasses can circumvent many colorblind tests, though this is typically not allowed.[57]
  • Glasses with a notch filter (e.g. EnChroma glasses) filter a narrow band of light that excites both the L and M cones (yellow–green wavelengths).[58] When combined with an additional stopband in the short wavelength (blue) region, these lenses may constitute a neutral-density filter (have no color tint). They improve on the other lens types by causing less distortion of colors and will essentially increase the saturation of some colors. They will only work on trichromats (anomalous or normal), and unlike the other types, do not have a significant effect on Dichromats. The glasses do not significantly increase one's ability on colorblind tests.[59]

Aids

Many mobile and computer applications have been developed to aid color blind individuals in completing color tasks:

  • Some applications can identify a color—by name or RGB code—of a color on screen or the color of an object by using the device's camera.
  • Some applications will make images easier to interpret by the colorblind by enhancing color contrast in natural images and/or information graphics. These methods are generally called daltonization algorithms.[60]
  • Some applications can simulate color blindness by applying a filter to an image or screen that reduces the gamut of an image to that of a specific type of color blindness. While they don't directly help colorblind people, they allow those with normal color vision to understand how people with color blindness see the world. Their use can help improve inclusive design by allowing designers to simulate their own images to ensure they are accessible to the colorblind.[61]

In 2003, a cybernetic device called eyeborg was developed to allow the wearer to hear sounds representing different colors.[62] Achromatopsic artist Neil Harbisson was the first to use such a device in early 2004; the eyeborg allowed him to start painting in color by memorizing the sound corresponding to each color. In 2012, at a TED Conference, Harbisson explained how he could now perceive colors outside the ability of human vision.[63]

Epidemiology

Rates of color blindness[clarification needed][citation needed]
Males Females
Dichromacy 2.4% 0.03%
Protanopia 1.3% 0.02%
Deuteranopia 1.2% 0.01%
Tritanopia 0.008% 0.008%
Anomalous trichromacy 6.3% 0.37%
Protanomaly 1.3% 0.02%
Deuteranomaly 5.0% 0.35%
Tritanomaly 0.0001% 0.0001%

Color blindness affects a large number of individuals, with protans and deutans being the most common types.[35] In individuals with Northern European ancestry, as many as 8 percent of men and 0.4 percent of women experience congenital color deficiency.[64] Interestingly, even Dalton's very first paper already arrived upon this 8% number:[65]

...it is remarkable that, out of 25 pupils I once had, to whom I explained this subject, 2 were found to agree with me...

— John Dalton, Extraordinary facts relating to the vision of colours: with observations (1798)

History

 
An 1895 illustration of normal vision and various kinds of color blindness.

During the 17th and 18th century, several philosophers hypothesized that not all individuals perceived colors in the same way:[66]

...there is no reason to suppose a perfect resemblance in the disposition of the Optic Nerve in all Men, since there is an infinite variety in every thing in Nature, and chiefly in those that are Material, 'tis therefore very probable that all Men see not the same Colours in the same Objects.

— Nicolas Malebranche, The search after truth (1674) [67]

In the power of conceiving colors, too, there are striking differences among individuals: and, indeed, I am inclined to suspect, that, in the greater number of instances, the supposed defects of sight in this respect ought to be ascribed rather to a defect in the power of conception.

— Dugald Stewart, Elements of the philosophy of the human mind (1792) [68]

The phenomenon only came to be scientifically studied in 1794, when English chemist John Dalton gave the first account of colour blindness in a paper to the Manchester Literary and Philosophical Society, which was published in 1798 as Extraordinary Facts relating to the Vision of Colours: With Observations.[69][65] Genetic analysis of Dalton's preserved eyeball confirmed him as having deuteranopia in 1995, some 150 years after his death.[70]

Influenced by Dalton, German writer J. W. von Goethe studied color vision abnormalities in 1798 by asking two young subjects to match pairs of colors.[71]

In 1875, the Lagerlunda train crash in Sweden brought color blindness to the forefront. Following the crash, Professor Alarik Frithiof Holmgren, a physiologist, investigated and concluded that the color blindness of the engineer (who had died) had caused the crash. Professor Holmgren then created the first test for color vision using multicolored skeins of wool to detect color blindness and thereby exclude the colorblind from jobs in the transportation industry requiring color vision to interpret safety signals.[72] However, there is a claim that there is no firm evidence that color deficiency did cause the collision, or that it might have not been the sole cause.[73]

In 1920, Frederick William Edridge-Green devised an alternative theory of color vision and color blindness based on Newton's classification of 7 fundamental colors (ROYGBIV). Edridge-Green classified color vision based on how many distinct colors a subject could see in the spectrum. Normal subjects were termed hexachromic as they could not discern Indigo. Subjects with superior color vision, who could discern indigo, where heptachromic. The colorblind were therefore dichromic (equivalent to dichromacy) or tri-, tetra- or pentachromic (anomalous trichromacy).[74][75]

Rights

In the United States, under federal anti-discrimination laws such as the Americans with Disabilities Act, color vision deficiencies have not been found to constitute a disability that triggers protection from workplace discrimination.

A Brazilian court ruled that people with color blindness are protected by the Inter-American Convention on the Elimination of All Forms of Discrimination against Person with Disabilities.[76][77][78] At trial, it was decided that the carriers of color blindness have a right of access to wider knowledge, or the full enjoyment of their human condition.[citation needed]

Occupations

Color blindness may make it difficult or impossible for a person to engage in certain activities. Persons with color blindness may be legally or practically barred from occupations in which color perception is an essential part of the job (e.g., mixing paint colors), or in which color perception is important for safety (e.g., operating vehicles in response to color-coded signals). This occupational safety principle originates from the aftermath of the 1875 Lagerlunda train crash, which Alarik Frithiof Holmgren blamed on the color blindness of the engineer and created the first occupational screening test (Holmgren's wool test) against the colorblind.[72]

...I consider that to [Holmgren] above all others do we owe the present and future control of color-blindness on land and sea, by which life and property are safer, and the risks of travelling less.

— Benjamin Joy Jeffries, Color-blindness: Its Danger & Its Detection (1879)

Color vision is important for occupations using telephone or computer networking cabling, as the individual wires inside the cables are color-coded using green, orange, brown, blue and white colors.[79] Electronic wiring, transformers, resistors, and capacitors are color-coded as well, using black, brown, red, orange, yellow, green, blue, violet, gray, white, silver, gold.[80]

Participation, officiating and viewing sporting events can be impacted by color blindness. Professional football players Thomas Delaney and Fabio Carvalho have discussed the difficulties when colour clashes occur, and research undertaken by FIFA has shown that enjoyment and player progression can be hampered by issues distinguishing the difference between the pitch and training objects or field markings. [81]

Driving

Red–green colorblindness can make it difficult to drive, primarily due to the inability to differentiate red–amber–green traffic lights. Protans are further disadvantaged due to the darkened perception of reds, which can make it more difficult to quickly recognize brake lights.[82] In response, some countries have refused to grant driver's licenses to individuals with color blindness:

  • In April 2003, Romania removed color blindness from its list of disqualifying conditions for learner driver's licenses.[83][84] It is now qualified as a condition that could potentially compromise driver safety, therefore a driver may have to be evaluated by an authorized ophthalmologist to determine if they can drive safely. As of May 2008, there is an ongoing campaign to remove the legal restrictions that prohibit colorblind citizens from getting driver's licenses.[85]
  • In June 2020, India relaxed its ban on driver's licenses for the colorblind to now only apply to those with strong CVD. While previously restricted, those who test as mild or moderate can now pass the medical requirements.[86]
  • Australia instituted a tiered ban on the colorblind from obtaining commercial driver's licenses in 1994. This included a ban for all protans, and a stipulation that deutans must pass the Farnsworth Lantern. The stipulation on deutans was revoked in 1997 citing a lack of available test facilities, and the ban on protans was revoked in 2003.[82]
  • All colorblind individuals are banned from obtaining a driver's license in China[87] and since 2016 in Russia (2012 for dichromats).[88]

Piloting aircraft

Although many aspects of aviation depend on color coding, only a few of them are critical enough to be interfered with by some milder types of color blindness. Some examples include color-gun signaling of aircraft that have lost radio communication, color-coded glide-path indications on runways, and the like. Some jurisdictions restrict the issuance of pilot credentials to persons with color blindness for this reason. Restrictions may be partial, allowing color-blind persons to obtain certification but with restrictions, or total, in which case color-blind persons are not permitted to obtain piloting credentials at all.[89]

In the United States, the Federal Aviation Administration requires that pilots be tested for normal color vision as part of their medical clearance in order to obtain the required medical certificate, a prerequisite to obtaining a pilot's certification. If testing reveals color blindness, the applicant may be issued a license with restrictions, such as no night flying and no flying by color signals—such a restriction effectively prevents a pilot from holding certain flying occupations, such as that of an airline pilot, although commercial pilot certification is still possible, and there are a few flying occupations that do not require night flight and thus are still available to those with restrictions due to color blindness (e.g., agricultural aviation). The government allows several types of tests, including medical standard tests (e.g., the Ishihara, Dvorine, and others) and specialized tests oriented specifically to the needs of aviation. If an applicant fails the standard tests, they will receive a restriction on their medical certificate that states: "Not valid for night flying or by color signal control". They may apply to the FAA to take a specialized test, administered by the FAA. Typically, this test is the "color vision light gun test". For this test an FAA inspector will meet the pilot at an airport with an operating control tower. The color signal light gun will be shone at the pilot from the tower, and they must identify the color. If they pass they may be issued a waiver, which states that the color vision test is no longer required during medical examinations. They will then receive a new medical certificate with the restriction removed. This was once a Statement of Demonstrated Ability (SODA), but the SODA was dropped, and converted to a simple waiver (letter) early in the 2000s.[90]

Research published in 2009 carried out by the City University of London's Applied Vision Research Centre, sponsored by the UK's Civil Aviation Authority and the U.S. Federal Aviation Administration, has established a more accurate assessment of color deficiencies in pilot applicants' red/green and yellow–blue color range which could lead to a 35% reduction in the number of prospective pilots who fail to meet the minimum medical threshold.[91]

See also

References

  1. ^ a b c d e f g h Gordon N (March 1998). "Colour blindness". Public Health. 112 (2): 81–4. doi:10.1038/sj.ph.1900446. PMID 9581449.
  2. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o p q r s t u v "Facts About Color Blindness". NEI. February 2015. from the original on 28 July 2016. Retrieved 29 July 2016.
  3. ^ Gómez-Robledo, L (2018). "Do EnChroma glasses improve color vision for colorblind subjects?". Optics Express. 26 (22): 28693–28703. Bibcode:2018OExpr..2628693G. doi:10.1364/OE.26.028693. PMID 30470042. S2CID 53721875.
  4. ^ "Colour vision deficiency (colour blindness)". nhs.uk. 18 October 2017. Retrieved 17 March 2022.
  5. ^ "OSHA does not have requirements for normal color vision. | Occupational Safety and Health Administration". www.osha.gov. Retrieved 6 May 2019.
  6. ^ Marmor MF, Lanthony P (March 2001). "The dilemma of color deficiency and art". Survey of Ophthalmology. 45 (5): 407–15. doi:10.1016/S0039-6257(00)00192-2. PMID 11274694.
  7. ^ Marmor MF (February 2016). "Vision, eye disease, and art: 2015 Keeler Lecture". Eye. 30 (2): 287–303. doi:10.1038/eye.2015.197. PMC 4763116. PMID 26563659.
  8. ^ Fomins, S (2011). "Multispectral analysis of color vision deficiency tests". Materials Science. 17 (1): 104–108. doi:10.5755/j01.ms.17.1.259.
  9. ^ Cole, Barry L (1972). "The handicap of abnormal colour vision". Clinical and Experimental Optometry. 55 (8): 304–310. doi:10.1111/j.1444-0938.1972.tb06271.x.
  10. ^ "New documentary uncovers the Irish links to America's Tipperary Hill". TheJournal.ie. 6 November 2016. from the original on 15 August 2017. Retrieved 15 August 2017.
  11. ^ Zhang, Sarah (17 March 2014). "The Story Behind Syracuse's Upside-Down Traffic Light". Gizmodo. from the original on 16 September 2014.
  12. ^ Cole BL, Harris RW (September 2009). "Colour blindness does not preclude fame as an artist: celebrated Australian artist Clifton Pugh was a protanope". Clinical & Experimental Optometry. 92 (5): 421–8. doi:10.1111/j.1444-0938.2009.00384.x. PMID 19515095. S2CID 21676461.
  13. ^ Anon. "Charles Meryon". Art Encyclopedia. The Concise Grove Dictionary of Art. Oxford University Press. from the original on 25 November 2010. Retrieved 7 January 2010.
  14. ^ Lee, Hyo-won (15 May 2011). "Dreams come true, Disney style". The Korea Times. Retrieved 25 November 2019.
  15. ^ Simonite, Tom (5 December 2005). "Colour blindness may have hidden advantages". Nature. doi:10.1038/news051205-1.
  16. ^ Bosten, J.M.; Robinson, J.D.; Jordan, G.; Mollon, J.D. (December 2005). "Multidimensional scaling reveals a color dimension unique to 'color-deficient' observers". Current Biology. 15 (23): R950–R952. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2005.11.031. PMID 16332521. S2CID 6966946.
  17. ^ a b Morgan MJ, Adam A, Mollon JD (June 1992). "Dichromats detect colour-camouflaged objects that are not detected by trichromats". Proceedings. Biological Sciences. 248 (1323): 291–5. Bibcode:1992RSPSB.248..291M. doi:10.1098/rspb.1992.0074. PMID 1354367. S2CID 35694740.
  18. ^ "Colour-Blindness and Camouflage". Nature. 146 (3694): 226. 1940. Bibcode:1940Natur.146Q.226.. doi:10.1038/146226a0. S2CID 4071103.
  19. ^ Bosten JM, Robinson JD, Jordan G, Mollon JD (December 2005). "Multidimensional scaling reveals a color dimension unique to 'color-deficient' observers" (PDF). Current Biology. 15 (23): R950-2. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2005.11.031. PMID 16332521. S2CID 6966946.
  20. ^ "Colour blindness not all it seems". BBC News. 6 December 2015. from the original on 23 June 2016. Retrieved 21 June 2016.
  21. ^ Sousa, Bruna Rafaela Silva; Loureiro, Terezinha Medeiros Gonçalves; Goulart, Paulo Roney Kilpp; Cortes, Maria Izabel Tentes; Costa, Marcelo Fernandes; Bonci, Daniela Maria Oliveira; Baran, Luiz Claudio Portnoi; Hauzman, Einat; Ventura, Dora Fix; Miquilini, Leticia; Souza, Givago Silva (21 October 2020). "Specificity of the chromatic noise influence on the luminance contrast discrimination to the color vision phenotype". Scientific Reports. 10 (1): 17897. Bibcode:2020NatSR..1017897S. doi:10.1038/s41598-020-74875-3. PMC 7578001. PMID 33087826.
  22. ^ Hovis, Jeffery K. (July 2002). "Diagnosis of Defective Colour Vision, 2nd Ed". Optometry and Vision Science. 79 (7): 406. doi:10.1097/00006324-200207000-00005. ISSN 1538-9235.
  23. ^ Caprette, Heather. "14 Avoiding the Use of Color Alone to Convey Meaning and Algorithms That Help". Best Practices in Accessible Online Design. Pressbooks @ MSL.
  24. ^ a b Nuñez, JR (2018). "Optimizing colormaps with consideration for color vision deficiency to enable accurate interpretation of scientific data". PLOS ONE. 13 (7): e0199239. arXiv:1712.01662. Bibcode:2018PLoSO..1399239N. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0199239. PMC 6070163. PMID 30067751.
  25. ^ von Kries, J. (1897). "Ueber Farbensysteme". Zeitschrift für Psychologie Physiologie Sinnesorg. 13: 241–324.
  26. ^ Spring KR, Parry-Hill MJ, Fellers TJ, Davidson MW. "Human Vision and Color Perception". Florida State University. from the original on 27 August 2007. Retrieved 5 April 2007.
  27. ^ "Types of Colour Blindness". Colour Blind Awareness. from the original on 29 May 2014.
  28. ^ a b Blom, Jan Dirk (2009). A Dictionary of Hallucinations. Springer. p. 4. ISBN 978-1-4419-1222-0. from the original on 27 December 2016.
  29. ^ Weiss AH, Biersdorf WR (1989). "Blue cone monochromatism". Journal of Pediatric Ophthalmology and Strabismus. 26 (5): 218–23. doi:10.3928/0191-3913-19890901-04. PMID 2795409. S2CID 23037026.
  30. ^ Simunovic MP (May 2010). "Colour vision deficiency". Eye. 24 (5): 747–55. doi:10.1038/eye.2009.251. PMID 19927164.
  31. ^ Deeb, Samir S. (2006). "Genetics of variation in human color vision and the retinal cone mosaic". Current Opinion in Genetics & Development. 16 (3): 301–307. doi:10.1016/j.gde.2006.04.002. PMID 16647849.
  32. ^ Moreland, J.D.; Kerr, J. (1979). "Optimization of a Rayleigh-type equation for the detection of tritanomaly". Vision Research. 19 (12): 1369–1375. doi:10.1016/0042-6989(79)90209-8. PMID 316945. S2CID 29379397.
  33. ^ Hoffman, Paul S. (PDF). Archived from the original (PDF) on 15 May 2008. Retrieved 1 July 2009.
  34. ^ Neitz, Maureen E. . Medical College of Wisconsin. Archived from the original on 5 February 2007. Retrieved 5 April 2007.
  35. ^ a b Wong, B. (June 2011). "Color blindness". Nature Methods. 8 (6): 441. doi:10.1038/nmeth.1618. PMID 21774112. S2CID 36690778.
  36. ^ Neitz J, Neitz M (April 2011). "The genetics of normal and defective color vision". Vision Research. 51 (7): 633–51. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2010.12.002. PMC 3075382. PMID 21167193.
  37. ^ Harrison G, Tanner J, Pilbeam D, Baker P (1988). Human Biology. Oxford: Oxford University Press. pp. 183–187, 287–290. ISBN 978-0-19-854144-8.
  38. ^ Neitz, Jay; Neitz, Maureen (2011). "The genetics of normal and defective color vision". Vision Research. 51 (7): 633–651. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2010.12.002. PMC 3075382. PMID 21167193.
  39. ^ MacAdam, David L.; Judd, Deane B., eds. (1979). Contributions to color science. NBS. p. 584.
  40. ^ Steefel, Lorraine T., and Timothy E. Moore, PhD. "Color Blindness." The Gale Encyclopedia of Nursing and Allied Health, edited by Jacqueline L. Longe, 4th ed., vol. 2, Gale, 2018, pp. 890–892. Gale eBooks, Accessed 29 Dec. 2021.
  41. ^ . UCL Biochemistry BSM Group. Archived from the original on 1 May 2005. Retrieved 2 April 2007.
  42. ^ . colourblindawareness.org. Archived from the original on 16 December 2014.
  43. ^ Leikin, Jerrold B.; Lipsky, Martin S., eds. (2003). Complete Medical Encyclopedia. Random House Reference (First ed.). New York, NY: Random House, for the American Medical Association. p. 388. ISBN 978-0-8129-9100-0. Retrieved 1 December 2011 – via archive.org.
  44. ^ "Myambutol (Ethambutol)". RxList.com. drug information. from the original on 8 July 2014. Retrieved 24 May 2014. Description, user reviews, drug side effects, interactions–prescribing information
  45. ^ "VIAGRA (SILDENAFIL CITRATE) DRUG". RxList.com. drug information. Retrieved 3 June 2022. Description, user reviews, drug side effects, interactions–prescribing information
  46. ^ Fraunfelder, Frederick T.; Fraunfelder, Frederick W.; Chambers, Wiley A. (2014). Drug-Induced Ocular Side Effects: Clinical ocular toxicology e‑book. Elsevier Health Sciences. p. 79. ISBN 978-0-323-31985-0.
  47. ^ Choi AR, Braun JM, Papandonatos GD, Greenberg PB (November 2017). "Occupational styrene exposure and acquired dyschromatopsia: A systematic review and meta-analysis". American Journal of Industrial Medicine. 60 (11): 930–946. doi:10.1002/ajim.22766. PMC 5652067. PMID 28836685.
  48. ^ Betancur-Sánchez AM, Vásquez-Trespalacios EM, Sardi-Correa C (January 2017). "Impaired colour vision in workers exposed to organic solvents: A systematic review". Archivos de la Sociedad Espanola de Oftalmologia. 92 (1): 12–18. doi:10.1016/j.oftal.2016.05.008. PMID 27422480.
  49. ^ Dick, F.D. (March 2006). "Solvent neurotoxicity". Occupational and Environmental Medicine. 63 (3): 221–6, 179. doi:10.1136/oem.2005.022400. PMC 2078137. PMID 16497867.
  50. ^ Hunt, D.; Dulai, K.; Bowmaker, J.; Mollon, J. (17 February 1995). "The Chemistry of John Dalton's Color Blindness". Science. 267 (5200): 984–988. Bibcode:1995Sci...267..984H. doi:10.1126/science.7863342. PMID 7863342. S2CID 6764146.
  51. ^ Toufeeq A (October 2004). "Specifying colours for colour vision testing using computer graphics". Eye. 18 (10): 1001–5. doi:10.1038/sj.eye.6701378. PMID 15192692.
  52. ^ Kinnear PR, Sahraie A (December 2002). "New Farnsworth–Munsell 100 hue test norms of normal observers for each year of age 5–22 and for age decades 30–70". The British Journal of Ophthalmology. 86 (12): 1408–11. doi:10.1136/bjo.86.12.1408. PMC 1771429. PMID 12446376.
  53. ^ Reference, Genetics Home. "Color vision deficiency". Genetics Home Reference. Retrieved 6 May 2019.
  54. ^ Siegel IM (1981). "The X-Chrom lens. On seeing red". Survey of Ophthalmology. 25 (5): 312–24. doi:10.1016/S0039-6257(81)80001-X. PMID 6971497.
  55. ^ Zeltzer HI (July 1979). "Use of modified X-Chrom for relief of light dazzlement and color blindness of a rod monochromat". Journal of the American Optometric Association. 50 (7): 813–8. PMID 315420.
  56. ^ An X-Chrom manual 2015-04-12 at the Wayback Machine. Artoptical.com. Retrieved on 2016-12-10.
  57. ^ Welsh, Kenneth W (April 1978). "Aeromedical implications of the X-chrom lens for improving color vision deficiencies". Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine. Oklahoma City: Federal Aviation Administration. 50 (3): 249–255. PMID 313209. Retrieved 30 September 2022.
  58. ^ Zhou L. "A Scientist Accidentally Developed Sunglasses That Could Correct Color Blindness". Smithsonian. Retrieved 6 January 2018.
  59. ^ Gómez-Robledo, L.; Valero, E. M.; Huertas, R.; Martínez-Domingo, M. A.; Hernández-Andrés, J. (29 October 2018). "Do EnChroma glasses improve color vision for colorblind subjects?". Optics Express. 26 (22): 28693–28703. Bibcode:2018OExpr..2628693G. doi:10.1364/OE.26.028693. PMID 30470042. S2CID 53721875.
  60. ^ Simon-Liedtke, Joschua Thomas; Farup, Ivar (February 2016). "Evaluating color vision deficiency daltonization methods using a behavioral visual-search method". Journal of Visual Communication and Image Representation. 35: 236–247. doi:10.1016/j.jvcir.2015.12.014. hdl:11250/2461824.
  61. ^ "Colour Blindness: Experience it". Colour Blind Awareness. Retrieved 11 December 2019.
  62. ^ Alfredo M. Ronchi: Eculture: Cultural Content in the Digital Age. Springer (New York, 2009). p. 319 ISBN 978-3-540-75273-8
  63. ^ "I listen to color" 2012-08-12 at the Wayback Machine, Neil Harbisson at TED Global, 27 June 2012.
  64. ^ Chan X, Goh S, Tan N (2014). "Subjects with colour vision deficiency in the community: what do primary care physicians need to know?". Asia Pacific Family Medicine. 13 (1): 10. doi:10.1186/s12930-014-0010-3.
  65. ^ a b Dalton, John (1798). "Extraordinary Facts relating to the Vision of Colours: With Observations". Manchester Literary and Philosophical Society. Memoirs. England, Manchester. 5 (1): 28–45.
  66. ^ Lanthony, Philippe (2018). The History of Color Blindness. Wayenborgh Publishing. p. 3. ISBN 978-9062999033. Retrieved 14 April 2022.
  67. ^ Malebranche, Nicolas (1712) [1674]. Malebranch's search after truth, or, A treatise of the nature of the humane mind and of its management for avoiding error in the sciences : vol I: done out of French from the last edition. p. 88. Retrieved 14 April 2022.
  68. ^ Stewart, Dugald (1792). Elements of the philosophy of the human mind (1 ed.). p. 80. Retrieved 14 April 2022.
  69. ^ Lanthony 2018, p. 14.
  70. ^ Hunt, D. M.; Dulai, K. S.; Bowmaker, J. K.; Mollon, J. D. (17 February 1995). "The chemistry of John Dalton's color blindness". Science. 267 (5200): 984–988. Bibcode:1995Sci...267..984H. doi:10.1126/science.7863342. PMID 7863342. S2CID 6764146.
  71. ^ Lanthony 2018, pp. 25–26.
  72. ^ a b Vingrys AJ, Cole BL (1986). "Origins of colour vision standards within the transport industry". Ophthalmic & Physiological Optics. 6 (4): 369–75. doi:10.1111/j.1475-1313.1986.tb01155.x. PMID 3306566. S2CID 41486427.
  73. ^ Mollon JD, Cavonius LR (2012). "The Lagerlunda collision and the introduction of color vision testing". Survey of Ophthalmology. 57 (2): 178–94. doi:10.1016/j.survophthal.2011.10.003. PMID 22301271.
  74. ^ McLaren, K. (1985). "Newton's indigo". Color Research & Application. 10 (4): 225–229. doi:10.1002/col.5080100411.
  75. ^ Edridge-Green, F. W. (1913). "Trichromic Vision and Anomalous Trichromatism". Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Containing Papers of a Biological Character. 86 (586): 164–170. doi:10.1098/rspb.1913.0010. ISSN 0950-1193. JSTOR 80517. S2CID 129045064.
  76. ^ "Full text of the decision of the court – in Portuguese language". Archived from the original on 14 July 2012. Retrieved 9 March 2012.
  77. ^ "Decree issued by president of a republic ratifying Legislative Decree No. 198, of june 13, which approved the Inter-American Convention AG/RES. 1608 – in Portuguese language". from the original on 25 March 2012. Retrieved 9 March 2012.
  78. ^ "Inter-American Convention on the Elimination of All Forms of Discrimination against Person with Disabilities". from the original on 16 April 2013. Retrieved 9 March 2012.
  79. ^ Meyers, Michael (2002). All in One A+ Certification Exam Guide (4th ed.). Berkeley, California: McGraw-Hill/Osborne. ISBN 978-0-07-222274-6.[page needed]
  80. ^ Grob, Bernard (2001). Basic Electronics. Columbus, Ohio: Glencoe/McGraw-Hill. ISBN 978-0-02-802253-6.[page needed]
  81. ^ "Football players shocked when experiencing Colour Blindness". Tacbis.
  82. ^ a b Cole, Barry (September 2016). "Colour Blindness and Driving". Clinical and Experimental Optometry. 99 (5): 484–487. doi:10.1111/cxo.12396. PMID 27470192. S2CID 26368283.
  83. ^ "ORDIN 87 03/02/2003 – Portal Legislativ". PORTAL LEGISLATIV (in Romanian). Ministerul Justiției. Retrieved 31 December 2021.
  84. ^ "ORDIN 87 03/02/2003 – Portal Legislativ". PORTAL LEGISLATIV (in Romanian). Ministerul Justiției. Retrieved 31 December 2021.
  85. ^ Corlăţean, Titus. "Discrimination against Romanians with genetic chromatic deficiencies". Retrieved 31 December 2021.
  86. ^ "Mild to medium colour blind people can now obtain driver's license". Times of India. Press Trust of India. 26 June 2020. Retrieved 1 June 2022.
  87. ^ Lu, Feiran. "Some of us see the world in a different light". Shine. Retrieved 1 June 2022.
  88. ^ "Do color blindmen a driver's license. Color blindmen allowed to drive cars". Rozavet. Retrieved 1 June 2022.
  89. ^ "Answers for Pilots: Color vision". January 2012.
  90. ^ "Aerospace Medical Dispositions – Color vision". from the original on 12 May 2009. Retrieved 11 April 2009.
  91. ^ Warburton, Simon (29 May 2009). "Colour-blindness research could clear more pilots to fly: UK CAA". Air transport. Reed Business Information. from the original on 2 June 2009. Retrieved 29 October 2009.

Further reading

  • Kaiser PK, Boynton RM (1996). Human color vision. Washington, DC: Optical Society of America. ISBN 978-1-55752-461-4. OCLC 472932250.
  • McIntyre D (2002). Colour blindness: causes and effects. Chester: Dalton Publishing. ISBN 978-0-9541886-0-3. OCLC 49204679.
  • Rubin ML, Cassin B, Solomon S (1984). Dictionary of eye terminology. Gainesville, Fla: Triad Pub. Co. ISBN 978-0-937404-07-2. OCLC 10375427.
  • Shevell SK (2003). The science of color. Amsterdam: Elsevier. ISBN 978-0-444-51251-2. OCLC 52271315.
  • Hilbert D, Byrne A (1997). Readings on color. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press. ISBN 978-0-262-52231-1. OCLC 35762680.
  • Stiles WS, Wyszecki G (2000). Color science: concepts and methods, quantitative data and formulae. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons. ISBN 978-0-471-39918-6. OCLC 799532137.
  • Kuchenbecker J, Broschmann D (2014). Plates for color vision testing. New York: Thieme. ISBN 978-3-13-175481-3.
  • Dalton J (1798). "Extraordinary facts relating to the vision of colours: with observations". Memoirs of the Literary and Philosophical Society of Manchester. 5: 28–45. OCLC 9879327.

External links

  • Color blindness at Curlie
  • "A Glossary of Color Science."

color, blindness, colorblind, redirects, here, disorder, that, causes, most, forms, color, blindness, congenital, green, color, blindness, other, uses, color, blind, disambiguation, color, vision, deficiency, decreased, ability, color, differences, color, impa. Colorblind redirects here For the disorder that causes most forms of color blindness see Congenital red green color blindness For other uses see Color blind disambiguation Color blindness or color vision deficiency CVD is the decreased ability to see color or differences in color 2 It can impair tasks such as selecting ripe fruit choosing clothing and reading traffic lights 2 Color blindness may make some academic activities more difficult 2 However issues are generally minor and people with colorblindness automatically develop adaptations and coping mechanisms 2 People with total color blindness achromatopsia may also be uncomfortable in bright environments 2 and have decreased visual acuity Color blindnessOther namesColor vision deficiency impaired color vision 1 Example of an Ishihara color test plate Viewers with normal color vision should clearly see the number 74 SpecialtyOphthalmologySymptomsDecreased ability to see colors 2 DurationLong term 2 CausesGenetic inherited usually X linked 2 Diagnostic methodIshihara color test 2 TreatmentAdjustments to teaching methods mobile apps 1 2 FrequencyRed green 8 males 0 5 females Northern European descent 2 The most common cause of color blindness is an inherited problem or variation in the functionality of one or more of the three classes of cone cells in the retina which mediate color vision 2 The most common form is caused by a genetic disorder called congenital red green color blindness Males are more likely to be color blind than females because the genes responsible for the most common forms of color blindness are on the X chromosome 2 Females who are not color blind can carry genes for color blindness and pass them on to their children 2 Color blindness can also result from physical or chemical damage to the eye the optic nerve or parts of the brain 2 Screening for color blindness is typically done with the Ishihara color test 2 There is no cure for color blindness 2 Diagnosis may allow an individual or their parents teachers to actively accommodate the condition 1 Special lenses such as EnChroma glasses or X chrom contact lenses may help people with red green color blindness at some color tasks 2 but they do not grant the wearer normal color vision 3 Mobile apps can help people identify colors 2 Red green color blindness is the most common form followed by blue yellow color blindness and total color blindness 2 Red green color blindness affects up to 1 in 12 males 8 and 1 in 200 females 0 5 2 4 The ability to see color also decreases in old age 2 In certain countries color blindness may make people ineligible for certain jobs 1 such as those of aircraft pilots train drivers crane operators and people in the armed forces 1 5 The effect of color blindness on artistic ability is controversial 1 6 but a number of famous artists are believed to have been color blind 1 7 Contents 1 Effects 1 1 Confusion colors 1 2 Color tasks 1 3 Food 1 4 Skin color 1 5 Traffic lights 1 6 Signal lights 1 7 Fashion 1 8 Art 1 9 Advantages 1 10 Digital design 1 10 1 Unordered Information 1 10 2 Ordered Information 2 Classification 2 1 Based on severity 2 1 1 Monochromacy 2 1 2 Dichromacy 2 1 3 Anomalous trichromacy 2 2 Based on affected cone 2 2 1 Red green color blindness 2 2 2 Blue yellow color blindness 2 3 Summary of cone complements 3 Causes 3 1 Genetics 3 1 1 Genetics of red green color blindness 3 1 2 Genetics of blue yellow color blindness 3 1 3 Other genetic causes 3 2 Non genetic causes 4 Diagnosis 4 1 Color vision test 4 2 Genetic testing 5 Management 5 1 Lenses 5 2 Aids 6 Epidemiology 7 History 8 Rights 8 1 Occupations 8 2 Driving 8 3 Piloting aircraft 9 See also 10 References 11 Further reading 12 External links This article is about color blindness in humans but other organisms also have color blindness Many species have color vision that is different from human vision with either a limited or extended range of visible colors as compared with humans Effects EditA person who is colorblind will have decreased or no color discrimination along the red green axis blue yellow axis or both However the vast majority of people with colorblindness are only affected on their red green axis The first indication of colorblindness generally consists of a person using the wrong color for an object such as when painting or calling a color by the wrong name The colors that are confused are very consistent among people with the same type of color blindness Normal sight Deuteranopia sight Protanopia sight Tritanopia sight Monochromacy sightConfusion colors Edit Confusion Lines for the three types of Dichromacy superimposed on CIEXYZ color space Confusion colors are pairs or groups of colors that will often be mistaken by people with colorblindness Confusion colors for red green color blindness include cyan and grey rose pink and grey blue and purple yellow and neon green red green orange brownConfusion colors for tritan include yellow and grey blue and green dark blue violet and black violet and yellow green red and rose pinkThese colors of confusion are defined quantitatively by straight confusion lines plotted in CIEXYZ usually plotted on the corresponding chromaticity diagram The lines all intersect at a copunctal point which varies with the type of color blindness 8 Chromaticities along a confusion line will appear metameric to people with dichromacy of that type People with trichromacy of that type will see the chromaticities as metameric if they are close enough depending on the strength of their CVD For two colors on a confusion line to be metameric the chromaticities first have to be made isoluminant meaning equal in lightness Also colors that may be isoluminant to the standard observer may not be isoluminant to a person with dichromacy Color tasks Edit Main article Color task Cole describes four color tasks all of which are impeded to some degree by color blindness 9 Comparative When multiple colors must be compared such as with mixing paint Connotative When colors are given an implicit meaning such as red stop Denotative When identifying colors for example by name such as where is the yellow ball Aesthetic When colors look nice or convey an emotional response but don t carry explicit meaningThe following sections describe specific color tasks with which people with colorblindness typically have difficulty Food Edit Simulation of the normal above and dichromatic below perception of red and green applesColorblindness causes difficulty with the connotative color tasks associated with selecting or preparing food Selecting food for ripeness can be difficult The green yellow transition of bananas is particularly hard to identify It can also be difficult to detect bruises mold or rot on some foods to determine when meat is done by color to distinguish some varietals such as a Braeburn vs a Granny Smith apple or to distinguish colors associated with artificial flavors e g jelly beans sports drinks Skin color Edit Main article Evolution of color vision in primates Skin Tone Changes in skin color due to bruising sunburn rashes or even blushing are easily missed by people with red green colorblindness Traffic lights Edit See also Driving The lack of standard positional clues makes this light difficult to interpret The colors of traffic lights can be difficult for people with red green colorblindness This difficulty includes distinguishing red amber lights from sodium street lamps distinguishing green lights closer to cyan from normal white lights and distinguishing red from amber lights especially when there are no positional clues available see image The infamous inverted traffic light in Syracuse NYThe main coping mechanism to overcome these challenges is to memorize the position of lights The order of the common triplet traffic light is standardized as red amber green from top to bottom or left to right Cases that deviate from this standard are rare One such case is a traffic light in Tipperary Hill in Syracuse New York which is upside down green amber red top to bottom due to the sentiments of its Irish American community 10 However the light has been criticized due to the potential hazard it poses for color blind drivers 11 Horizontal traffic light in Halifax Nova Scotia CanadaThere are other several features of traffic lights available that help accommodate people with colorblindness British Rail signals use more easily identifiable colors The red is blood red the amber is yellow and the green is a bluish color citation needed Most British road traffic lights are mounted vertically on a black rectangle with a white border forming a sighting board so that drivers can more easily look for the position of the light In the eastern provinces of Canada traffic lights are sometimes differentiated by shape in addition to color square for red diamond for yellow and circle for green see image Signal lights Edit See also Occupations Navigation lights in marine and aviation settings employ red and green lights to signal the relative position of other ships or aircraft Railway signal lights also rely heavily on red green yellow colors In both cases these color combinations can be difficult for people with red green colorblindness Lantern Tests are a common means of simulating these light sources to determine not necessarily whether someone is colorblind but whether they can functionally distinguish these specific signal colors Those who cannot pass this test are generally completely restricted from working on aircraft ships or rail Fashion Edit See also Color of clothing Color analysis is the analysis of color in its use in fashion to determine personal color combinations that are most aesthetically pleasing citation needed Colors to combine can include clothing accessories makeup hair color skin color eye color etc Color analysis involves many aesthetic and comparative color task that can be difficult for people with color blindness Art Edit Inability to distinguish color does not necessarily preclude the ability to become a celebrated artist The 20th century expressionist painter Clifton Pugh three time winner of Australia s Archibald Prize on biographical gene inheritance and other grounds has been identified as a person with protanopy 12 19th century French artist Charles Meryon became successful by concentrating on etching rather than painting after he was diagnosed as having a red green deficiency 13 Jin Kim s red green color blindness did not stop him from becoming first an animator and later a character designer with Walt Disney Animation Studios 14 Advantages Edit People with deuteranomaly are better at distinguishing shades of khaki 15 which may be advantageous when looking for predators food or camouflaged objects hidden among foliage 16 People with dichromacy tend to learn to use texture and shape clues and so may be able to penetrate camouflage that has been designed to deceive individuals with normal color vision 17 18 Some tentative evidence finds that people with color blindness are better at penetrating certain color camouflages Such findings may give an evolutionary reason for the high rate of red green color blindness 17 There is also a study suggesting that people with some types of color blindness can distinguish colors that people with normal color vision are not able to distinguish 19 In World War II color blind observers were used to penetrate camouflage 20 In the presence of chromatic noise people with colorblindness are more capable of seeing a luminous signal as long as the chromatic noise appears metameric to them 21 This is the effect behind most reverse Pseudoisochromatic plates e g hidden digit Ishihara plates that are discernible to people with colorblindness but unreadable to people with typical color perception citation needed Digital design Edit See also Color coding in data visualization Testing the colors of a web chart center to ensure that no information is lost to the various forms of color blindness Color codes are useful tools for designers to convey information The interpretation of this information requires users to perform a variety of Color Tasks usually comparative but also sometimes connotative or denotative However these tasks are often problematic for people with colorblindness when design of the color code has not followed best practices for accessibility 22 For example one of the most ubiquitous connotative color codes is the red means bad and green means good or similar systems based on the classic signal light colors However this color coding will almost always be undifferentiable to people with either deutan or protan CVD and therefore should be avoided or supplemented with a parallel connotative system symbols smileys etc Good practices to ensure design is accessible to people with colorblindness include When possible e g in simple video games or apps allowing the user to choose their own colors is the most inclusive design practice Using other signals that are parallel to the color coding such as patterns shapes size or order 23 This not only helps people with color blindness but also aids understanding by normally sighted people by providing them with multiple reinforcing cues Using brightness contrast different shades in addition to color contrast different hues To achieve good contrast conventional wisdom suggests converting a digital design to grayscale to ensure there is sufficient brightness contrast between colors However this does not account for the different perceptions of brightness to different varieties of colorblindness especially protan CVD tritan CVD and monochromacy Viewing the design through a CVD Simulator to ensure the information carried by color is still sufficiently conveyed At a minimum the design should be tested for deutan CVD the most common kind of colorblindness Maximizing the area of colors e g increase size thickness or boldness of colored element makes the color easier to identify Color contrast improves as the angle the color subtends on the retina increases This applies to all types of color vision Maximizing brightness value and saturation chroma of the colors to maximize color contrast Converting connotative tasks to comparative tasks by including a legend even when the meaning is considered obvious e g red means danger Avoiding denotative color tasks color naming when possible Some denotative tasks can be converted to comparative tasks by depicting the actual color whenever the color name is mentioned for example colored typography in purple purple or purple For denotative tasks color naming using the most common shades of colors For example green and yellow are colors of confusion in red green CVD but it is very common to mix forest green with bright yellow Mistakes by people with colorblindness increase drastically when uncommon shades are used e g neon green with dark yellow For denotative tasks using colors that are classically associated with a color name For example use firetruck red instead of burgundy to represent the word red Unordered Information Edit Colors of Boardgame pieces must be carefully chosen to be accessible to people with colorblindnessA common task for designers is to select a subset of colors qualitative colormap that are as mutually differentiable as possible salient For example player pieces in a board game should be as different as possible Classic advice suggests using Brewer palettes but several of these are not actually accessible to people with colorblindness Unfortunately the colors with the greatest contrast to the red green colorblind tend to be colors of confusion to the blue yellow colorblind and vice versa However since red green is much more prevalent than blue yellow CVD design should according to whom generally prioritize those users people with deutan CVD then protan CVD Ordered Information Edit Three sequential colormaps that have been designed to be accessible to people with color blindness A common task for data visualization is to represent a color scale or sequential colormap often in the form of a heat map or choropleth Several scales are designed with special consideration for people with colorblindness and are widespread in academia including Cividis 24 Viridis 24 and Parula These comprise a light to dark scale superimposed on a yellow to blue scale making them monotonic and perceptually uniform to all forms of color vision Classification Edit These color charts show how different colorblind people see compared to a person with normal color vision dubious discuss Much terminology has existed and does exist for the classification of color blindness but the typical classification for color blindness follows the von Kries classifications 25 which uses severity and affected cone for naming Based on severity Edit Based on clinical appearance color blindness may be described as total or partial Total color blindness monochromacy is much less common than partial color blindness 26 Partial colorblindness includes dichromacy and anomalous trichromacy but is often clinically defined as mild moderate or strong Monochromacy Edit Main article Monochromacy Monochromacy is often called total color blindness since there is no ability to see color Although the term may refer to acquired disorders such as cerebral achromatopsia it typically refers to congenital color vision disorders namely rod monochromacy and blue cone monochromacy 27 28 In cerebral achromatopsia a person cannot perceive colors even though the eyes are capable of distinguishing them Some sources do not consider these to be true color blindness because the failure is of perception not of vision They are forms of visual agnosia 28 Monochromacy is the condition of possessing only a single channel for conveying information about color Monochromats are unable to distinguish any colors and perceive only variations in brightness Congenital monochromacy occurs in two primary forms Rod monochromacy frequently called complete achromatopsia where the retina contains no cone cells so that in addition to the absence of color discrimination vision in lights of normal intensity is difficult Cone monochromacy is the condition of having only a single class of cone A cone monochromat can have good pattern vision at normal daylight levels but will not be able to distinguish hues Cone monochromacy is divided into classes defined by the single remaining cone class However red and green cone monochromats have not been definitively described in the literature Blue cone monochromacy is caused by lack of functionality of L red and M green cones and is therefore mediated by the same genes as red green color blindness on the X chromosome Peak spectral sensitivities are in the blue region of the visible spectrum near 440 nm People with this condition generally show nystagmus jiggling eyes photophobia light sensitivity reduced visual acuity and myopia nearsightedness 29 Visual acuity usually falls to the 20 50 to 20 400 range Dichromacy Edit Main article Dichromacy Dichromats can match any color they see with some mixture of just two primary colors in contrast to those with normal sight trichromats who can distinguish three primary colors Dichromats usually know they have a color vision problem and it can affect their daily lives Dichromacy in humans includes protanopia deuteranopia and tritanopia Out of the male population 2 have severe difficulties distinguishing between red orange yellow and green Orange and yellow are different combinations of red and green light Colors in this range which appear very different to a normal viewer appear to a dichromat to be the same or a similar color The terms protanopia deuteranopia and tritanopia come from Greek and respectively mean inability to see anopia with the first prot second deuter or third trit cone Anomalous trichromacy Edit Anomalous trichromacy is the mildest type of color deficiency but the severity ranges from almost dichromacy strong to almost normal trichromacy mild 30 In fact many mild anomalous trichromats have very little difficulty carrying out tasks that require normal color vision and some may not even be aware that they have a color vision deficiency The types of anomalous trichromacy include protanomaly deuteranomaly and tritanomaly It is approximately three times more common than dichromacy 31 Anomalous trichromats exhibit trichromacy but the color matches they make differ from normal trichromats In order to match a given spectral yellow light protanomalous observers need more red light in a red green mixture than a normal observer and deuteranomalous observers need more green This difference can be measured by an instrument called an Anomaloscope where red and green lights are mixed by a subject to match a yellow light 32 Based on affected cone Edit There are two major types of color blindness difficulty distinguishing between red and green and difficulty distinguishing between blue and yellow 33 34 dubious discuss These definitions are based on the phenotype of the partial colorblindness Clinically it is more common to use a genotypical definition which describes which cone opsin is affected Red green color blindness Edit Red green color blindness includes protan and deutan CVD Protan CVD is related to the L cone and includes protanomaly anomalous trichromacy and protanopia dichromacy Deutan CVD is related to the M cone and includes deuteranomaly anomalous trichromacy and deuteranopia dichromacy 35 36 The phenotype visual experience of deutans and protans is quite similar Common colors of confusion include red brown green yellow as well as blue purple Both forms are almost always symptomatic of congenital red green color blindness so affects males disproportionately more than females 37 This form of colorblindness is sometimes referred to as daltonism after John Dalton who had red green dichromacy In some languages daltonism is still used to describe red green color blindness Illustration of the distribution of cone cells in the fovea of an individual with normal color vision left and a color blind protanopic retina The center of the fovea holds very few blue sensitive cones Protan 2 of males Lacking or possessing anomalous L opsins for long wavelength sensitive cone cells Protans have a neutral point at a cyan like wavelength around 492 nm see spectral color for comparison that is they cannot discriminate light of this wavelength from white For a protanope the brightness of red is much reduced compared to normal 38 This dimming can be so pronounced that reds may be confused with black or dark gray and red traffic lights may appear to be extinguished They may learn to distinguish reds from yellows primarily on the basis of their apparent brightness or lightness not on any perceptible hue difference Violet lavender and purple are indistinguishable from various shades of blue A very few people have been found who have one normal eye and one protanopic eye These unilateral dichromats report that with only their protanopic eye open they see wavelengths shorter than neutral point as blue and those longer than it as yellow Deutan 6 of males Lacking or possessing anomalous M opsins for medium wavelength sensitive cone cells Their neutral point is at a slightly longer wavelength 498 nm a more greenish hue of cyan Deutans have the same hue discrimination problems as protans but without the dimming of long wavelengths Deuteranopic unilateral dichromats report that with only their deuteranopic eye open they see wavelengths shorter than neutral point as blue and longer than it as yellow 39 Blue yellow color blindness Edit Blue yellow color blindness includes tritan CVD Tritan CVD is related to the S cone and includes tritanomaly anomalous trichromacy and tritanopia dichromacy Blue yellow color blindness is much less common than red green color blindness and more often has acquired causes than genetic Tritans have difficulty discerning between bluish and greenish hues 40 Tritans have a neutral point at 571 nm yellowish citation needed Tritan lt 0 01 of individuals Lacking or possessing anomalous S opsins or short wavelength sensitive cone cells Tritans see short wavelength colors blue indigo and spectral violet as greenish and drastically dimmed some of these colors even as black Yellow and orange are indistinguishable from white and pink respectively and purple colors are perceived as various shades of red Unlike protans and deutans the mutation for this color blindness is carried on chromosome 7 Therefore it is not sex linked equally prevalent in both males and females The OMIM gene code for this mutation is 304000 Colorblindness Partial Tritanomaly 41 Tetartan is the fourth type of colorblindness and a type of blue yellow color blindness However its existence is hypothetical and given the molecular basis of human color vision it is unlikely this type could exist citation needed Summary of cone complements Edit The below table shows the cone complements for different types of human color vision including those considered color blindness normal color vision and superior color vision The cone complement contains the types of cones or their opsins expressed by an individual Cone system Red Green Blue N normalA anomalousN A N A N A1 Normal vision Trichromacy Normal2 Protanomaly Anomalous trichromacy Partialcolorblindness Red green3 Protanopia Dichromacy4 Deuteranomaly Anomalous trichromacy5 Deuteranopia Dichromacy6 Tritanomaly Anomalous trichromacy Blue yellow7 Tritanopia Dichromacy8 Blue Cone Monochromacy Monochromacy Total color blindness9 Achromatopsia10 Tetrachromacy carrier theory Tetrachromacy Superior 11Causes EditSee also Trichromatic color vision and Congenital red green color blindness Mechanism Color blindness is any deviation of color vision from normal trichromatic color vision often as defined by the standard observer that produces a reduced gamut Mechanisms for color blindness are related to the functionality of cone cells and often to the expression of photopsins the photopigments that catch photons and thereby convert light into chemical signals Color vision deficiencies can be classified as inherited or acquired Inherited inherited or congenital genetic color vision deficiencies are most commonly caused by mutations of the genes encoding opsin proteins However several other genes can also lead to less common and or more severe forms of color blindness Acquired color blindness that is not present at birth may be caused by chronic illness accidents medication chemical exposure or simply normal aging processes 42 Genetics Edit This section does not cite any sources Please help improve this section by adding citations to reliable sources Unsourced material may be challenged and removed May 2023 Learn how and when to remove this template message Color blindness is typically an inherited genetic disorder The most common forms of colorblindness are associated with the Photopsin genes but the mapping of the human genome has shown there are many causative mutations that don t directly affect the opsins Mutations capable of causing color blindness originate from at least 19 different chromosomes and 56 different genes as shown online at the Online Mendelian Inheritance in Man OMIM Genetics of red green color blindness Edit Main article Congenital red green color blindness Genetics Punnett squares for each combination of parents color vision status giving probabilities of their offsprings status A superscript c denotes a chromosome with an affected geneBy far the most common form of colorblindness is congenital red green color blindness Daltonism which includes protanopia protanomaly and deuteranopia deuteranomaly These conditions are mediated by the OPN1LW and OPN1MW genes respectively both on the X chromosome An affected gene is either missing as in Protanopia and Deuteranopia Dichromacy or is a chimeric gene as in Protanomaly and Deuteranomaly Since the OPN1LW and OPN1MW genes are on the X chromosome they are sex linked and therefore affect males and females disproportionately Because the colorblind affected alleles are recessive color blindness specifically follows X linked recessive inheritance Males have only one X chromosome XY and females have two XX Because the male only has one of each gene if it is affected the male will be colorblind Because a female has two alleles of each gene one on each chromosome if only one gene is affected the dominant normal alleles will override the affected recessive allele and the female will have normal color vision However if the female has two mutated alleles she will still be colorblind This is why there is a disproportionate prevalence of colorblindness with 8 of males exhibiting colorblindness and 0 5 of females Genetics of blue yellow color blindness Edit This section needs expansion You can help by adding to it June 2022 Blue yellow color blindness is a rarer form of colorblindness including tritanopia tritanomaly These conditions are mediated by the OPN1SW gene on Chromosome 7 Other genetic causes Edit Several inherited diseases are known to cause color blindness achromatopsia also called rod monochromatism stationary cone dystrophy or cone dysfunction syndrome cone dystrophy cone rod dystrophy Leber s congenital amaurosis retinitis pigmentosa initially affects rods but can later progress to cones and therefore color blindness They can be congenital from birth or can commence in childhood or adulthood They can be stationary that is remain the same throughout a person s lifetime or progressive As progressive phenotypes involve deterioration of the retina and other parts of the eye many of the above forms of color blindness can progress to legal blindness i e an acuity of 6 60 20 200 or worse and often leave a person with complete blindness Non genetic causes Edit Physical trauma can cause color blindness either neurologically brain trauma which produces swelling of the brain in the occipital lobe or retinally either acute e g from laser exposure or chronic e g from ultraviolet light exposure Color blindness may also present itself as a symptom of degenerative diseases of the eye such as cataract and age related macular degeneration and as part of the retinal damage caused by diabetes Vitamin A deficiency may also cause color blindness 43 Color blindness may be a side effect of prescription drug use For example red green color blindness can be caused by ethambutol a drug used in the treatment of tuberculosis 44 Blue yellow color blindness can be caused by sildenafil an active component of Viagra 45 Hydroxychloroquine can also lead to hydroxychloroquine retinopathy which includes various color defects 46 Exposure to chemicals such as styrene 47 or organic solvents 48 49 can also lead to color vision defects Simple colored filters can also create mild color vision deficiencies John Dalton s original hypothesis for his deuteranopia was actually that the vitreous humor of his eye was discolored I was led to conjecture that one of the humours of my eye must be a transparent but coloured medium so constituted as to absorb red and green rays principally I suppose it must be the vitreous humor John Dalton Extraordinary facts relating to the vision of colours with observations 1798 An autopsy of his eye after his death in 1844 showed this to be definitively untrue 50 though other filters are possible Actual physiological examples usually affect the blue yellow opponent channel and are named Cyanopsia and Xanthopsia and are most typically an effect of yellowing or removal of the lens The opponent channels can also be affected by the prevalence of certain cones in the retinal mosaic The cones are not equally prevalent and not evenly distributed in the retina When the number of one of these cone types is significantly reduced this can also lead to or contribute to a color vision deficiency This is one of the causes of tritanomaly Diagnosis EditColor vision test Edit Main article Color vision test An Ishihara test image as seen by subjects with normal color vision and by those with a variety of color deficienciesThe main method for diagnosing a color vision deficiency is in testing the color vision directly The Ishihara color test is the test most often used to detect red green deficiencies and most often recognized by the public 1 Some tests are clinical in nature designed to be fast simple and effective at identifying broad categories of color blindness Others focus on precision and are generally available only in academic settings 51 pseudoisochromatic plates a classification which includes the Ishihara color test and HRR test embed a figure in the plate as a number of spots surrounded by spots of a slightly different color These colors must appear identical metameric to the colorblind but distinguishable to color normals Pseudoisochromatic plates are used as screening tools because they are cheap fast and simple but they do not provide precise diagnosis of CVD Lanterns such as the Farnsworth Lantern Test project small colored lights to a subject who is required to identify the color of the lights The colors are those of typical signal lights i e red green and yellow which also happen to be colors of confusion of red green CVD Lanterns do not diagnose colorblindness but they are occupational screening tests to ensure an applicant has sufficient color discrimination to be able to perform a job A Farnsworth D 15 testArrangement tests can be used as screening or diagnostic tools The Farnsworth Munsell 100 hue test is very sensitive but the Farnsworth D 15 is a simplified version used specifically for screening for CVD In either case the subject is asked to arrange a set of colored caps or chips to form a gradual transition of color between two anchor caps 52 Anomaloscopes are typically designed to detect red green deficiencies and are based on the Rayleigh match which compares a mixture of red and green light in variable proportions to a fixed spectral yellow of variable luminosity The subject must change the two variables until the colors appear to match They are expensive and require expertise to administer so they are generally only used in academic settings Genetic testing Edit While genetic testing cannot directly evaluate a subject s color vision phenotype most congenital color vision deficiencies are well correlated with genotype Therefore the genotype can be directly evaluated and used to predict the phenotype This is especially useful for progressive forms that do not have a strongly color deficient phenotype at a young age However it can also be used to sequence the L and M Opsins on the X Chromosome since the most common alleles of these two genes are known and have even been related to exact spectral sensitivities and peak wavelengths A subject s color vision can therefore be classified through genetic testing 53 but this is just a prediction of the phenotype since color vision can be affected by countless non genetic factors such as your cone mosaic Management EditDespite much recent improvement in gene therapy for color blindness there is currently no FDA approved treatment for any form of CVD and otherwise no cure for CVD currently exists Management of the condition by using lenses to alleviate symptoms or smartphone apps to aid with daily tasks is possible Lenses Edit Main article Color blind glasses There are three kinds of lenses that an individual can wear that can increase their accuracy in some color related tasks although none of these will fix color blindness or grant the wearer normal color vision A red tint contact lens worn over the non dominant eye will leverage binocular disparity to improve discrimination of some colors However it can make other colors more difficult to distinguish A 1981 review of various studies to evaluate the effect of the X chrom one brand contact lens concluded that while the lens may allow the wearer to achieve a better score on certain color vision tests it did not correct color vision in the natural environment 54 A case history using the X Chrom lens for a rod monochromat is reported 55 and an X Chrom manual is online 56 Tinted glasses e g Pilestone Colorlite glasses apply a tint e g magenta to incoming light that can distort colors in a way that makes some color tasks easier to complete These glasses can circumvent many colorblind tests though this is typically not allowed 57 Glasses with a notch filter e g EnChroma glasses filter a narrow band of light that excites both the L and M cones yellow green wavelengths 58 When combined with an additional stopband in the short wavelength blue region these lenses may constitute a neutral density filter have no color tint They improve on the other lens types by causing less distortion of colors and will essentially increase the saturation of some colors They will only work on trichromats anomalous or normal and unlike the other types do not have a significant effect on Dichromats The glasses do not significantly increase one s ability on colorblind tests 59 Aids Edit Many mobile and computer applications have been developed to aid color blind individuals in completing color tasks Some applications can identify a color by name or RGB code of a color on screen or the color of an object by using the device s camera Some applications will make images easier to interpret by the colorblind by enhancing color contrast in natural images and or information graphics These methods are generally called daltonization algorithms 60 Some applications can simulate color blindness by applying a filter to an image or screen that reduces the gamut of an image to that of a specific type of color blindness While they don t directly help colorblind people they allow those with normal color vision to understand how people with color blindness see the world Their use can help improve inclusive design by allowing designers to simulate their own images to ensure they are accessible to the colorblind 61 In 2003 a cybernetic device called eyeborg was developed to allow the wearer to hear sounds representing different colors 62 Achromatopsic artist Neil Harbisson was the first to use such a device in early 2004 the eyeborg allowed him to start painting in color by memorizing the sound corresponding to each color In 2012 at a TED Conference Harbisson explained how he could now perceive colors outside the ability of human vision 63 Epidemiology EditRates of color blindness clarification needed citation needed Males FemalesDichromacy 2 4 0 03 Protanopia 1 3 0 02 Deuteranopia 1 2 0 01 Tritanopia 0 008 0 008 Anomalous trichromacy 6 3 0 37 Protanomaly 1 3 0 02 Deuteranomaly 5 0 0 35 Tritanomaly 0 0001 0 0001 Color blindness affects a large number of individuals with protans and deutans being the most common types 35 In individuals with Northern European ancestry as many as 8 percent of men and 0 4 percent of women experience congenital color deficiency 64 Interestingly even Dalton s very first paper already arrived upon this 8 number 65 it is remarkable that out of 25 pupils I once had to whom I explained this subject 2 were found to agree with me John Dalton Extraordinary facts relating to the vision of colours with observations 1798 History Edit An 1895 illustration of normal vision and various kinds of color blindness During the 17th and 18th century several philosophers hypothesized that not all individuals perceived colors in the same way 66 there is no reason to suppose a perfect resemblance in the disposition of the Optic Nerve in all Men since there is an infinite variety in every thing in Nature and chiefly in those that are Material tis therefore very probable that all Men see not the same Colours in the same Objects Nicolas Malebranche The search after truth 1674 67 In the power of conceiving colors too there are striking differences among individuals and indeed I am inclined to suspect that in the greater number of instances the supposed defects of sight in this respect ought to be ascribed rather to a defect in the power of conception Dugald Stewart Elements of the philosophy of the human mind 1792 68 The phenomenon only came to be scientifically studied in 1794 when English chemist John Dalton gave the first account of colour blindness in a paper to the Manchester Literary and Philosophical Society which was published in 1798 as Extraordinary Facts relating to the Vision of Colours With Observations 69 65 Genetic analysis of Dalton s preserved eyeball confirmed him as having deuteranopia in 1995 some 150 years after his death 70 Influenced by Dalton German writer J W von Goethe studied color vision abnormalities in 1798 by asking two young subjects to match pairs of colors 71 In 1875 the Lagerlunda train crash in Sweden brought color blindness to the forefront Following the crash Professor Alarik Frithiof Holmgren a physiologist investigated and concluded that the color blindness of the engineer who had died had caused the crash Professor Holmgren then created the first test for color vision using multicolored skeins of wool to detect color blindness and thereby exclude the colorblind from jobs in the transportation industry requiring color vision to interpret safety signals 72 However there is a claim that there is no firm evidence that color deficiency did cause the collision or that it might have not been the sole cause 73 In 1920 Frederick William Edridge Green devised an alternative theory of color vision and color blindness based on Newton s classification of 7 fundamental colors ROYGBIV Edridge Green classified color vision based on how many distinct colors a subject could see in the spectrum Normal subjects were termed hexachromic as they could not discern Indigo Subjects with superior color vision who could discern indigo where heptachromic The colorblind were therefore dichromic equivalent to dichromacy or tri tetra or pentachromic anomalous trichromacy 74 75 Rights EditIn the United States under federal anti discrimination laws such as the Americans with Disabilities Act color vision deficiencies have not been found to constitute a disability that triggers protection from workplace discrimination A Brazilian court ruled that people with color blindness are protected by the Inter American Convention on the Elimination of All Forms of Discrimination against Person with Disabilities 76 77 78 At trial it was decided that the carriers of color blindness have a right of access to wider knowledge or the full enjoyment of their human condition citation needed Occupations Edit Color blindness may make it difficult or impossible for a person to engage in certain activities Persons with color blindness may be legally or practically barred from occupations in which color perception is an essential part of the job e g mixing paint colors or in which color perception is important for safety e g operating vehicles in response to color coded signals This occupational safety principle originates from the aftermath of the 1875 Lagerlunda train crash which Alarik Frithiof Holmgren blamed on the color blindness of the engineer and created the first occupational screening test Holmgren s wool test against the colorblind 72 I consider that to Holmgren above all others do we owe the present and future control of color blindness on land and sea by which life and property are safer and the risks of travelling less Benjamin Joy Jeffries Color blindness Its Danger amp Its Detection 1879 Color vision is important for occupations using telephone or computer networking cabling as the individual wires inside the cables are color coded using green orange brown blue and white colors 79 Electronic wiring transformers resistors and capacitors are color coded as well using black brown red orange yellow green blue violet gray white silver gold 80 Participation officiating and viewing sporting events can be impacted by color blindness Professional football players Thomas Delaney and Fabio Carvalho have discussed the difficulties when colour clashes occur and research undertaken by FIFA has shown that enjoyment and player progression can be hampered by issues distinguishing the difference between the pitch and training objects or field markings 81 Driving Edit See also Traffic lights Red green colorblindness can make it difficult to drive primarily due to the inability to differentiate red amber green traffic lights Protans are further disadvantaged due to the darkened perception of reds which can make it more difficult to quickly recognize brake lights 82 In response some countries have refused to grant driver s licenses to individuals with color blindness In April 2003 Romania removed color blindness from its list of disqualifying conditions for learner driver s licenses 83 84 It is now qualified as a condition that could potentially compromise driver safety therefore a driver may have to be evaluated by an authorized ophthalmologist to determine if they can drive safely As of May 2008 there is an ongoing campaign to remove the legal restrictions that prohibit colorblind citizens from getting driver s licenses 85 In June 2020 India relaxed its ban on driver s licenses for the colorblind to now only apply to those with strong CVD While previously restricted those who test as mild or moderate can now pass the medical requirements 86 Australia instituted a tiered ban on the colorblind from obtaining commercial driver s licenses in 1994 This included a ban for all protans and a stipulation that deutans must pass the Farnsworth Lantern The stipulation on deutans was revoked in 1997 citing a lack of available test facilities and the ban on protans was revoked in 2003 82 All colorblind individuals are banned from obtaining a driver s license in China 87 and since 2016 in Russia 2012 for dichromats 88 Piloting aircraft Edit See also Signal lights Although many aspects of aviation depend on color coding only a few of them are critical enough to be interfered with by some milder types of color blindness Some examples include color gun signaling of aircraft that have lost radio communication color coded glide path indications on runways and the like Some jurisdictions restrict the issuance of pilot credentials to persons with color blindness for this reason Restrictions may be partial allowing color blind persons to obtain certification but with restrictions or total in which case color blind persons are not permitted to obtain piloting credentials at all 89 In the United States the Federal Aviation Administration requires that pilots be tested for normal color vision as part of their medical clearance in order to obtain the required medical certificate a prerequisite to obtaining a pilot s certification If testing reveals color blindness the applicant may be issued a license with restrictions such as no night flying and no flying by color signals such a restriction effectively prevents a pilot from holding certain flying occupations such as that of an airline pilot although commercial pilot certification is still possible and there are a few flying occupations that do not require night flight and thus are still available to those with restrictions due to color blindness e g agricultural aviation The government allows several types of tests including medical standard tests e g the Ishihara Dvorine and others and specialized tests oriented specifically to the needs of aviation If an applicant fails the standard tests they will receive a restriction on their medical certificate that states Not valid for night flying or by color signal control They may apply to the FAA to take a specialized test administered by the FAA Typically this test is the color vision light gun test For this test an FAA inspector will meet the pilot at an airport with an operating control tower The color signal light gun will be shone at the pilot from the tower and they must identify the color If they pass they may be issued a waiver which states that the color vision test is no longer required during medical examinations They will then receive a new medical certificate with the restriction removed This was once a Statement of Demonstrated Ability SODA but the SODA was dropped and converted to a simple waiver letter early in the 2000s 90 Research published in 2009 carried out by the City University of London s Applied Vision Research Centre sponsored by the UK s Civil Aviation Authority and the U S Federal Aviation Administration has established a more accurate assessment of color deficiencies in pilot applicants red green and yellow blue color range which could lead to a 35 reduction in the number of prospective pilots who fail to meet the minimum medical threshold 91 See also EditColor agnosia Ability to see colors but inability to recognize colors Color anomia Ability to see colors but inability to name colors List of people with color blindness Motion blindness TetrachromacyReferences Edit a b c d e f g h Gordon N March 1998 Colour blindness Public Health 112 2 81 4 doi 10 1038 sj ph 1900446 PMID 9581449 a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o p q r s t u v Facts About Color Blindness NEI February 2015 Archived from the original on 28 July 2016 Retrieved 29 July 2016 Gomez Robledo L 2018 Do EnChroma glasses improve color vision for colorblind subjects Optics Express 26 22 28693 28703 Bibcode 2018OExpr 2628693G doi 10 1364 OE 26 028693 PMID 30470042 S2CID 53721875 Colour vision deficiency colour blindness nhs uk 18 October 2017 Retrieved 17 March 2022 OSHA does not have requirements for normal color vision Occupational Safety and Health Administration www osha gov Retrieved 6 May 2019 Marmor MF Lanthony P March 2001 The dilemma of color deficiency and art Survey of Ophthalmology 45 5 407 15 doi 10 1016 S0039 6257 00 00192 2 PMID 11274694 Marmor MF February 2016 Vision eye disease and art 2015 Keeler Lecture Eye 30 2 287 303 doi 10 1038 eye 2015 197 PMC 4763116 PMID 26563659 Fomins S 2011 Multispectral analysis of color vision deficiency tests Materials Science 17 1 104 108 doi 10 5755 j01 ms 17 1 259 Cole Barry L 1972 The handicap of abnormal colour vision Clinical and Experimental Optometry 55 8 304 310 doi 10 1111 j 1444 0938 1972 tb06271 x New documentary uncovers the Irish links to America s Tipperary Hill TheJournal ie 6 November 2016 Archived from the original on 15 August 2017 Retrieved 15 August 2017 Zhang Sarah 17 March 2014 The Story Behind Syracuse s Upside Down Traffic Light Gizmodo Archived from the original on 16 September 2014 Cole BL Harris RW September 2009 Colour blindness does not preclude fame as an artist celebrated Australian artist Clifton Pugh was a protanope Clinical amp Experimental Optometry 92 5 421 8 doi 10 1111 j 1444 0938 2009 00384 x PMID 19515095 S2CID 21676461 Anon Charles Meryon Art Encyclopedia The Concise Grove Dictionary of Art Oxford University Press Archived from the original on 25 November 2010 Retrieved 7 January 2010 Lee Hyo won 15 May 2011 Dreams come true Disney style The Korea Times Retrieved 25 November 2019 Simonite Tom 5 December 2005 Colour blindness may have hidden advantages Nature doi 10 1038 news051205 1 Bosten J M Robinson J D Jordan G Mollon J D December 2005 Multidimensional scaling reveals a color dimension unique to color deficient observers Current Biology 15 23 R950 R952 doi 10 1016 j cub 2005 11 031 PMID 16332521 S2CID 6966946 a b Morgan MJ Adam A Mollon JD June 1992 Dichromats detect colour camouflaged objects that are not detected by trichromats Proceedings Biological Sciences 248 1323 291 5 Bibcode 1992RSPSB 248 291M doi 10 1098 rspb 1992 0074 PMID 1354367 S2CID 35694740 Colour Blindness and Camouflage Nature 146 3694 226 1940 Bibcode 1940Natur 146Q 226 doi 10 1038 146226a0 S2CID 4071103 Bosten JM Robinson JD Jordan G Mollon JD December 2005 Multidimensional scaling reveals a color dimension unique to color deficient observers PDF Current Biology 15 23 R950 2 doi 10 1016 j cub 2005 11 031 PMID 16332521 S2CID 6966946 Colour blindness not all it seems BBC News 6 December 2015 Archived from the original on 23 June 2016 Retrieved 21 June 2016 Sousa Bruna Rafaela Silva Loureiro Terezinha Medeiros Goncalves Goulart Paulo Roney Kilpp Cortes Maria Izabel Tentes Costa Marcelo Fernandes Bonci Daniela Maria Oliveira Baran Luiz Claudio Portnoi Hauzman Einat Ventura Dora Fix Miquilini Leticia Souza Givago Silva 21 October 2020 Specificity of the chromatic noise influence on the luminance contrast discrimination to the color vision phenotype Scientific Reports 10 1 17897 Bibcode 2020NatSR 1017897S doi 10 1038 s41598 020 74875 3 PMC 7578001 PMID 33087826 Hovis Jeffery K July 2002 Diagnosis of Defective Colour Vision 2nd Ed Optometry and Vision Science 79 7 406 doi 10 1097 00006324 200207000 00005 ISSN 1538 9235 Caprette Heather 14 Avoiding the Use of Color Alone to Convey Meaning and Algorithms That Help Best Practices in Accessible Online Design Pressbooks MSL a b Nunez JR 2018 Optimizing colormaps with consideration for color vision deficiency to enable accurate interpretation of scientific data PLOS ONE 13 7 e0199239 arXiv 1712 01662 Bibcode 2018PLoSO 1399239N doi 10 1371 journal pone 0199239 PMC 6070163 PMID 30067751 von Kries J 1897 Ueber Farbensysteme Zeitschrift fur Psychologie Physiologie Sinnesorg 13 241 324 Spring KR Parry Hill MJ Fellers TJ Davidson MW Human Vision and Color Perception Florida State University Archived from the original on 27 August 2007 Retrieved 5 April 2007 Types of Colour Blindness Colour Blind Awareness Archived from the original on 29 May 2014 a b Blom Jan Dirk 2009 A Dictionary of Hallucinations Springer p 4 ISBN 978 1 4419 1222 0 Archived from the original on 27 December 2016 Weiss AH Biersdorf WR 1989 Blue cone monochromatism Journal of Pediatric Ophthalmology and Strabismus 26 5 218 23 doi 10 3928 0191 3913 19890901 04 PMID 2795409 S2CID 23037026 Simunovic MP May 2010 Colour vision deficiency Eye 24 5 747 55 doi 10 1038 eye 2009 251 PMID 19927164 Deeb Samir S 2006 Genetics of variation in human color vision and the retinal cone mosaic Current Opinion in Genetics amp Development 16 3 301 307 doi 10 1016 j gde 2006 04 002 PMID 16647849 Moreland J D Kerr J 1979 Optimization of a Rayleigh type equation for the detection of tritanomaly Vision Research 19 12 1369 1375 doi 10 1016 0042 6989 79 90209 8 PMID 316945 S2CID 29379397 Hoffman Paul S Accommodating Color Blindness PDF Archived from the original PDF on 15 May 2008 Retrieved 1 July 2009 Neitz Maureen E Severity of Colorblindness Varies Medical College of Wisconsin Archived from the original on 5 February 2007 Retrieved 5 April 2007 a b Wong B June 2011 Color blindness Nature Methods 8 6 441 doi 10 1038 nmeth 1618 PMID 21774112 S2CID 36690778 Neitz J Neitz M April 2011 The genetics of normal and defective color vision Vision Research 51 7 633 51 doi 10 1016 j visres 2010 12 002 PMC 3075382 PMID 21167193 Harrison G Tanner J Pilbeam D Baker P 1988 Human Biology Oxford Oxford University Press pp 183 187 287 290 ISBN 978 0 19 854144 8 Neitz Jay Neitz Maureen 2011 The genetics of normal and defective color vision Vision Research 51 7 633 651 doi 10 1016 j visres 2010 12 002 PMC 3075382 PMID 21167193 MacAdam David L Judd Deane B eds 1979 Contributions to color science NBS p 584 Steefel Lorraine T and Timothy E Moore PhD Color Blindness The Gale Encyclopedia of Nursing and Allied Health edited by Jacqueline L Longe 4th ed vol 2 Gale 2018 pp 890 892 Gale eBooks Accessed 29 Dec 2021 Disease causing Mutations and protein structure UCL Biochemistry BSM Group Archived from the original on 1 May 2005 Retrieved 2 April 2007 Acquired colour vision defects colourblindawareness org Archived from the original on 16 December 2014 Leikin Jerrold B Lipsky Martin S eds 2003 Complete Medical Encyclopedia Random House Reference First ed New York NY Random House for the American Medical Association p 388 ISBN 978 0 8129 9100 0 Retrieved 1 December 2011 via archive org Myambutol Ethambutol RxList com drug information Archived from the original on 8 July 2014 Retrieved 24 May 2014 Description user reviews drug side effects interactions prescribing information VIAGRA SILDENAFIL CITRATE DRUG RxList com drug information Retrieved 3 June 2022 Description user reviews drug side effects interactions prescribing information Fraunfelder Frederick T Fraunfelder Frederick W Chambers Wiley A 2014 Drug Induced Ocular Side Effects Clinical ocular toxicology e book Elsevier Health Sciences p 79 ISBN 978 0 323 31985 0 Choi AR Braun JM Papandonatos GD Greenberg PB November 2017 Occupational styrene exposure and acquired dyschromatopsia A systematic review and meta analysis American Journal of Industrial Medicine 60 11 930 946 doi 10 1002 ajim 22766 PMC 5652067 PMID 28836685 Betancur Sanchez AM Vasquez Trespalacios EM Sardi Correa C January 2017 Impaired colour vision in workers exposed to organic solvents A systematic review Archivos de la Sociedad Espanola de Oftalmologia 92 1 12 18 doi 10 1016 j oftal 2016 05 008 PMID 27422480 Dick F D March 2006 Solvent neurotoxicity Occupational and Environmental Medicine 63 3 221 6 179 doi 10 1136 oem 2005 022400 PMC 2078137 PMID 16497867 Hunt D Dulai K Bowmaker J Mollon J 17 February 1995 The Chemistry of John Dalton s Color Blindness Science 267 5200 984 988 Bibcode 1995Sci 267 984H doi 10 1126 science 7863342 PMID 7863342 S2CID 6764146 Toufeeq A October 2004 Specifying colours for colour vision testing using computer graphics Eye 18 10 1001 5 doi 10 1038 sj eye 6701378 PMID 15192692 Kinnear PR Sahraie A December 2002 New Farnsworth Munsell 100 hue test norms of normal observers for each year of age 5 22 and for age decades 30 70 The British Journal of Ophthalmology 86 12 1408 11 doi 10 1136 bjo 86 12 1408 PMC 1771429 PMID 12446376 Reference Genetics Home Color vision deficiency Genetics Home Reference Retrieved 6 May 2019 Siegel IM 1981 The X Chrom lens On seeing red Survey of Ophthalmology 25 5 312 24 doi 10 1016 S0039 6257 81 80001 X PMID 6971497 Zeltzer HI July 1979 Use of modified X Chrom for relief of light dazzlement and color blindness of a rod monochromat Journal of the American Optometric Association 50 7 813 8 PMID 315420 An X Chrom manual Archived 2015 04 12 at the Wayback Machine Artoptical com Retrieved on 2016 12 10 Welsh Kenneth W April 1978 Aeromedical implications of the X chrom lens for improving color vision deficiencies Aviation Space and Environmental Medicine Oklahoma City Federal Aviation Administration 50 3 249 255 PMID 313209 Retrieved 30 September 2022 Zhou L A Scientist Accidentally Developed Sunglasses That Could Correct Color Blindness Smithsonian Retrieved 6 January 2018 Gomez Robledo L Valero E M Huertas R Martinez Domingo M A Hernandez Andres J 29 October 2018 Do EnChroma glasses improve color vision for colorblind subjects Optics Express 26 22 28693 28703 Bibcode 2018OExpr 2628693G doi 10 1364 OE 26 028693 PMID 30470042 S2CID 53721875 Simon Liedtke Joschua Thomas Farup Ivar February 2016 Evaluating color vision deficiency daltonization methods using a behavioral visual search method Journal of Visual Communication and Image Representation 35 236 247 doi 10 1016 j jvcir 2015 12 014 hdl 11250 2461824 Colour Blindness Experience it Colour Blind Awareness Retrieved 11 December 2019 Alfredo M Ronchi Eculture Cultural Content in the Digital Age Springer New York 2009 p 319 ISBN 978 3 540 75273 8 I listen to color Archived 2012 08 12 at the Wayback Machine Neil Harbisson at TED Global 27 June 2012 Chan X Goh S Tan N 2014 Subjects with colour vision deficiency in the community what do primary care physicians need to know Asia Pacific Family Medicine 13 1 10 doi 10 1186 s12930 014 0010 3 a b Dalton John 1798 Extraordinary Facts relating to the Vision of Colours With Observations Manchester Literary and Philosophical Society Memoirs England Manchester 5 1 28 45 Lanthony Philippe 2018 The History of Color Blindness Wayenborgh Publishing p 3 ISBN 978 9062999033 Retrieved 14 April 2022 Malebranche Nicolas 1712 1674 Malebranch s search after truth or A treatise of the nature of the humane mind and of its management for avoiding error in the sciences vol I done out of French from the last edition p 88 Retrieved 14 April 2022 Stewart Dugald 1792 Elements of the philosophy of the human mind 1 ed p 80 Retrieved 14 April 2022 Lanthony 2018 p 14 Hunt D M Dulai K S Bowmaker J K Mollon J D 17 February 1995 The chemistry of John Dalton s color blindness Science 267 5200 984 988 Bibcode 1995Sci 267 984H doi 10 1126 science 7863342 PMID 7863342 S2CID 6764146 Lanthony 2018 pp 25 26 a b Vingrys AJ Cole BL 1986 Origins of colour vision standards within the transport industry Ophthalmic amp Physiological Optics 6 4 369 75 doi 10 1111 j 1475 1313 1986 tb01155 x PMID 3306566 S2CID 41486427 Mollon JD Cavonius LR 2012 The Lagerlunda collision and the introduction of color vision testing Survey of Ophthalmology 57 2 178 94 doi 10 1016 j survophthal 2011 10 003 PMID 22301271 McLaren K 1985 Newton s indigo Color Research amp Application 10 4 225 229 doi 10 1002 col 5080100411 Edridge Green F W 1913 Trichromic Vision and Anomalous Trichromatism Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B Containing Papers of a Biological Character 86 586 164 170 doi 10 1098 rspb 1913 0010 ISSN 0950 1193 JSTOR 80517 S2CID 129045064 Full text of the decision of the court in Portuguese language Archived from the original on 14 July 2012 Retrieved 9 March 2012 Decree issued by president of a republic ratifying Legislative Decree No 198 of june 13 which approved the Inter American Convention AG RES 1608 in Portuguese language Archived from the original on 25 March 2012 Retrieved 9 March 2012 Inter American Convention on the Elimination of All Forms of Discrimination against Person with Disabilities Archived from the original on 16 April 2013 Retrieved 9 March 2012 Meyers Michael 2002 All in One A Certification Exam Guide 4th ed Berkeley California McGraw Hill Osborne ISBN 978 0 07 222274 6 page needed Grob Bernard 2001 Basic Electronics Columbus Ohio Glencoe McGraw Hill ISBN 978 0 02 802253 6 page needed Football players shocked when experiencing Colour Blindness Tacbis a b Cole Barry September 2016 Colour Blindness and Driving Clinical and Experimental Optometry 99 5 484 487 doi 10 1111 cxo 12396 PMID 27470192 S2CID 26368283 ORDIN 87 03 02 2003 Portal Legislativ PORTAL LEGISLATIV in Romanian Ministerul Justiției Retrieved 31 December 2021 ORDIN 87 03 02 2003 Portal Legislativ PORTAL LEGISLATIV in Romanian Ministerul Justiției Retrieved 31 December 2021 Corlăţean Titus Discrimination against Romanians with genetic chromatic deficiencies Retrieved 31 December 2021 Mild to medium colour blind people can now obtain driver s license Times of India Press Trust of India 26 June 2020 Retrieved 1 June 2022 Lu Feiran Some of us see the world in a different light Shine Retrieved 1 June 2022 Do color blindmen a driver s license Color blindmen allowed to drive cars Rozavet Retrieved 1 June 2022 Answers for Pilots Color vision January 2012 Aerospace Medical Dispositions Color vision Archived from the original on 12 May 2009 Retrieved 11 April 2009 Warburton Simon 29 May 2009 Colour blindness research could clear more pilots to fly UK CAA Air transport Reed Business Information Archived from the original on 2 June 2009 Retrieved 29 October 2009 Further reading EditKaiser PK Boynton RM 1996 Human color vision Washington DC Optical Society of America ISBN 978 1 55752 461 4 OCLC 472932250 McIntyre D 2002 Colour blindness causes and effects Chester Dalton Publishing ISBN 978 0 9541886 0 3 OCLC 49204679 Rubin ML Cassin B Solomon S 1984 Dictionary of eye terminology Gainesville Fla Triad Pub Co ISBN 978 0 937404 07 2 OCLC 10375427 Shevell SK 2003 The science of color Amsterdam Elsevier ISBN 978 0 444 51251 2 OCLC 52271315 Hilbert D Byrne A 1997 Readings on color Cambridge Mass MIT Press ISBN 978 0 262 52231 1 OCLC 35762680 Stiles WS Wyszecki G 2000 Color science concepts and methods quantitative data and formulae Chichester John Wiley amp Sons ISBN 978 0 471 39918 6 OCLC 799532137 Kuchenbecker J Broschmann D 2014 Plates for color vision testing New York Thieme ISBN 978 3 13 175481 3 Dalton J 1798 Extraordinary facts relating to the vision of colours with observations Memoirs of the Literary and Philosophical Society of Manchester 5 28 45 OCLC 9879327 External links Edit Wikimedia Commons has media related to Color blindness Wikisource has original text related to this article Color blindness Color blindness at Curlie A Glossary of Color Science Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Color blindness amp oldid 1170669047, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.