fbpx
Wikipedia

Visible spectrum

The visible spectrum is the band of the electromagnetic spectrum that is visible to the human eye. Electromagnetic radiation in this range of wavelengths is called visible light (or simply light). The optical spectrum is sometimes considered to be the same as the visible spectrum, but some authors define the term more broadly, to include the ultraviolet and infrared parts of the electromagnetic spectrum as well.[1]

White light is dispersed by a prism into the colors of the visible spectrum.

A typical human eye will respond to wavelengths from about 380 to about 750 nanometers.[2] In terms of frequency, this corresponds to a band in the vicinity of 400–790 terahertz. These boundaries are not sharply defined and may vary per individual.[3] Under optimal conditions these limits of human perception can extend to 310 nm (ultraviolet) and 1100 nm (near infrared).[4][5][6]

The spectrum does not contain all the colors that the human visual system can distinguish. Unsaturated colors such as pink, or purple variations like magenta, for example, are absent because they can only be made from a mix of multiple wavelengths. Colors containing only one wavelength are also called pure colors or spectral colors.[7]

Visible wavelengths pass largely unattenuated through the Earth's atmosphere via the "optical window" region of the electromagnetic spectrum. An example of this phenomenon is when clean air scatters blue light more than red light, and so the midday sky appears blue (apart from the area around the Sun which appears white because the light is not scattered as much). The optical window is also referred to as the "visible window" because it overlaps the human visible response spectrum. The near infrared (NIR) window lies just out of the human vision, as well as the medium wavelength infrared (MWIR) window, and the long-wavelength or far-infrared (LWIR or FIR) window, although other animals may perceive them.

Spectral colors edit

 
sRGB rendering of the spectrum of visible light
Color Wavelength
(nm)
Frequency
(THz)
Photon energy
(eV)
  violet
380–450 670–790 2.75–3.26
  blue
450–485 620–670 2.56–2.75
  cyan
485–500 600–620 2.48–2.56
  green
500–565 530–600 2.19–2.48
  yellow
565–590 510–530 2.10–2.19
  orange
590–625 480–510 1.98–2.10
  red
625–750 400–480 1.65–1.98

Colors that can be produced by visible light of a narrow band of wavelengths (monochromatic light) are called pure spectral colors. The various color ranges indicated in the illustration are an approximation: The spectrum is continuous, with no clear boundaries between one color and the next.[8]

History edit

 
Newton's color circle, from Opticks of 1704, showing the colors he associated with musical notes. The spectral colors from red to violet are divided by the notes of the musical scale, starting at D. The circle completes a full octave, from D to D. Newton's circle places red, at one end of the spectrum, next to violet, at the other. This reflects the fact that non-spectral purple colors are observed when red and violet light are mixed.

In the 13th century, Roger Bacon theorized that rainbows were produced by a similar process to the passage of light through glass or crystal.[9]

In the 17th century, Isaac Newton discovered that prisms could disassemble and reassemble white light, and described the phenomenon in his book Opticks. He was the first to use the word spectrum (Latin for "appearance" or "apparition") in this sense in print in 1671 in describing his experiments in optics. Newton observed that, when a narrow beam of sunlight strikes the face of a glass prism at an angle, some is reflected and some of the beam passes into and through the glass, emerging as different-colored bands. Newton hypothesized light to be made up of "corpuscles" (particles) of different colors, with the different colors of light moving at different speeds in transparent matter, red light moving more quickly than violet in glass. The result is that red light is bent (refracted) less sharply than violet as it passes through the prism, creating a spectrum of colors.

 
Newton's observation of prismatic colors (David Brewster 1855)

Newton originally divided the spectrum into six named colors: red, orange, yellow, green, blue, and violet. He later added indigo as the seventh color since he believed that seven was a perfect number as derived from the ancient Greek sophists, of there being a connection between the colors, the musical notes, the known objects in the Solar System, and the days of the week.[10] The human eye is relatively insensitive to indigo's frequencies, and some people who have otherwise-good vision cannot distinguish indigo from blue and violet. For this reason, some later commentators, including Isaac Asimov,[11] have suggested that indigo should not be regarded as a color in its own right but merely as a shade of blue or violet. Evidence indicates that what Newton meant by "indigo" and "blue" does not correspond to the modern meanings of those color words. Comparing Newton's observation of prismatic colors with a color image of the visible light spectrum shows that "indigo" corresponds to what is today called blue, whereas his "blue" corresponds to cyan.[12][13][14]

In the 18th century, Johann Wolfgang von Goethe wrote about optical spectra in his Theory of Colours. Goethe used the word spectrum (Spektrum) to designate a ghostly optical afterimage, as did Schopenhauer in On Vision and Colors. Goethe argued that the continuous spectrum was a compound phenomenon. Where Newton narrowed the beam of light to isolate the phenomenon, Goethe observed that a wider aperture produces not a spectrum but rather reddish-yellow and blue-cyan edges with white between them. The spectrum appears only when these edges are close enough to overlap.

In the early 19th century, the concept of the visible spectrum became more definite, as light outside the visible range was discovered and characterized by William Herschel (infrared) and Johann Wilhelm Ritter (ultraviolet), Thomas Young, Thomas Johann Seebeck, and others.[15] Young was the first to measure the wavelengths of different colors of light, in 1802.[16]

The connection between the visible spectrum and color vision was explored by Thomas Young and Hermann von Helmholtz in the early 19th century. Their theory of color vision correctly proposed that the eye uses three distinct receptors to perceive color.

Limits to visible range edit

 
Photopic (black) and scotopic (green) luminous efficiency functions. The horizontal axis is wavelength in nm. See luminous efficiency function for more info.

The visible spectrum is limited to wavelengths that can both reach the retina and trigger visual phototransduction (excite a visual opsin). Insensitivity to UV light is generally limited by transmission through the lens. Insensitivity to IR light is limited by the spectral sensitivity functions of the visual opsins. The range is defined psychometrically by the luminous efficiency function, which accounts for all of these factors. In humans, there is a separate function for each of two visual systems, one for photopic vision, used in daylight, which is mediated by cone cells, and one for scotopic vision, used in dim light, which is mediated by rod cells. Each of these functions have different visible ranges. However, discussion on the visible range generally assumes photopic vision.

Atmospheric transmission edit

The visible range of most animals evolved to match the optical window, which is the range of light that can pass through the atmosphere. The ozone layer absorbs almost all UVA light (below 315 nm).[17] However, this only affects cosmic light (e.g. sunlight), not terrestrial light (e.g. Bioluminescence).

Ocular transmission edit

 
Cumulative transmission spectra of light as it passes through the ocular media, namely after the cornea (blue), before the lens (red), after the lens (gray) and before the retina (orange). The solid lines are for a 4.5 year old eye. The dashed orange line is for a 53 year old eye, and dotted for a 75 year old eye, indicating the effect of lens yellowing.)

Before reaching the retina, light must first transmit through the cornea and lens. UVB light (< 315 nm) is filtered mostly by the cornea, and UVA light (315–400 nm) is filtered mostly by the lens.[18] The lens also yellows with age, attenuating transmission most strongly at the blue part of the spectrum.[18] This can cause xanthopsia as well as a slight truncation of the short-wave (blue) limit of the visible spectrum. Subjects with aphakia are missing a lens, so UVA light can reach the retina and excite the visual opsins; this expands the visible range and may also lead to cyanopsia.

Opsin absorption edit

Each opsin has a spectral sensitivity function, which defines how likely it is to absorb a photon of each wavelength. The luminous efficiency function is approximately the superposition of the contributing visual opsins. Variance in the position of the individual opsin spectral sensitivity functions therefore affects the luminous efficiency function and the visible range. For example, the long-wave (red) limit changes proportionally to the position of the L-opsin. The positions are defined by the peak wavelength (wavelength of highest sensitivity), so as the L-opsin peak wavelength blue shifts by 10 nm, the long-wave limit of the visible spectrum also shifts 10 nm. Large deviations of the L-opsin peak wavelength lead to a form of color blindness called protanomaly and a missing L-opsin (protanopia) shortens the visible spectrum by about 30 nm at the long-wave limit. Forms of color blindness affecting the M-opsin and S-opsin do not significantly affect the luminous efficiency function nor the limits of the visible spectrum.

Different definitions edit

Regardless of actual physical and biological variance, the definition of the limits is not standard and will change depending on the industry. For example, some industries may be concerned with practical limits, so would conservatively report 420–680 nm,[19][20] while others may be concerned with psychometrics and achieving the broadest spectrum would liberally report 380–750, or even 380–800 nm.[21][22] The luminous efficiency function in the NIR does not have a hard cutoff, but rather an exponential decay, such that the function's value (or vision sensitivity) at 1,050 nm is about 109 times weaker than at 700 nm; much higher intensity is therefore required to perceive 1,050 nm light than 700 nm light.[23]

Vision outside the visible spectrum edit

Under ideal laboratory conditions, subjects may perceive infrared light up to at least 1,064 nm.[23] While 1,050 nm NIR light can evoke red, suggesting direct absorption by the L-opsin, there are also reports that pulsed NIR lasers can evoke green, which suggests two-photon absorption may be enabling extended NIR sensitivity.[23]

Similarly, young subjects may perceive ultraviolet wavelengths down to about 310–313 nm,[24][25][26] but detection of light below 380 nm may be due to fluorescence of the ocular media, rather than direct absorption of UV light by the opsins. As UVA light is absorbed by the ocular media (lens and cornea), it may fluoresce and be released at a lower energy (longer wavelength) that can then be absorbed by the opsins. For example, when the lens absorbs 350 nm light, the fluorescence emission spectrum is centered on 440 nm.[27]

Non-visual light detection edit

In addition to the photopic and scotopic systems, humans have other systems for detecting light that do not contribute to the primary visual system. For example, melanopsin has an absorption range of 420–540 nm and regulates circadian rhythm and other reflexive processes.[28] Since the melanopsin system does not form images, it is not strictly considered vision and does not contribute to the visible range.

In non-humans edit

The visible spectrum is defined as that visible to humans, but the variance between species is large. Not only can cone opsins be spectrally shifted to alter the visible range, but vertebrates with 4 cones (tetrachromatic) or 2 cones (dichromatic) relative to humans' 3 (trichromatic) will also tend to have a wider or narrower visible spectrum than humans, respectively.

Vertebrates tend to have 1-4 different opsin classes:[17]

  • longwave sensitive (LWS) with peak sensitivity between 500–570 nm,
  • middlewave sensitive (MWS) with peak sensitivity between 480–520 nm,
  • shortwave sensitive (SWS) with peak sensitivity between 415–470 nm, and
  • violet/ultraviolet sensitive (VS/UVS) with peak sensitivity between 355–435 nm.

Testing the visual systems of animals behaviorally is difficult, so the visible range of animals is usually estimated by comparing the peak wavelengths of opsins with those of typical humans (S-opsin at 420 nm and L-opsin at 560 nm).

Mammals edit

Most mammals have retained only two opsin classes (LWS and VS), due likely to the nocturnal bottleneck. However, old world primates (including humans) have since evolved two versions in the LWS class to regain trichromacy.[17] Unlike most mammals, rodents' UVS opsins have remained at shorter wavelengths. Along with their lack of UV filters in the lens, mice have a UVS opsin that can detect down to 340 nm. While allowing UV light to reach the retina can lead to retinal damage, the short lifespan of mice compared with other mammals may minimize this disadvantage relative to the advantage of UV vision.[29] Dogs have two cone opsins at 429 nm and 555 nm, so see almost the entire visible spectrum of humans, despite being dichromatic.[30] Horses have two cone opsins at 428 nm and 539 nm, yielding a slightly more truncated red vision.[31]

Birds edit

Most other vertebrates (birds, lizards, fish, etc.) have retained their tetrachromacy, including UVS opsins that extend further into the ultraviolet than humans' VS opsin.[17] The sensitivity of avian UVS opsins vary greatly, from 355–425 nm, and LWS opsins from 560–570 nm.[32] This translates to some birds with a visible spectrum on par with humans, and other birds with greatly expanded sensitivity to UV light. The LWS opsin of birds is sometimes reported to have a peak wavelength above 600 nm, but this is an effective peak wavelength that incorporates the filter of avian oil droplets.[32] The peak wavelength of the LWS opsin alone is the better predictor of the long-wave limit. A possible benefit of avian UV vision involves sex-dependent markings on their plumage that are visible only in the ultraviolet range.[33][34]

Fish edit

Teleosts (bony fish) are generally tetrachromatic. The sensitivity of fish UVS opsins vary from 347-383 nm, and LWS opsins from 500-570 nm.[35] However, some fish that use alternative chromophores can extend their LWS opsin sensitivity to 625 nm.[35] The popular belief that the common goldfish is the only animal that can see both infrared and ultraviolet light[36] is incorrect, because goldfish cannot see infrared light.[37]

Invertebrates edit

The visual systems of invertebrates deviate greatly from vertebrates, so direct comparisons are difficult. However, UV sensitivity has been reported in most insect species.[38]Bees and many other insects can detect ultraviolet light, which helps them find nectar in flowers. Plant species that depend on insect pollination may owe reproductive success to their appearance in ultraviolet light rather than how colorful they appear to humans. Bees' long-wave limit is at about 590 nm.[39] Mantis shrimp exhibit up to 14 opsins, enabling a visible range of less than 300 nm to above 700 nm.[17]

Thermal vision edit

Some snakes can "see"[40] radiant heat at wavelengths between 5 and 30 μm to a degree of accuracy such that a blind rattlesnake can target vulnerable body parts of the prey at which it strikes,[41] and other snakes with the organ may detect warm bodies from a meter away.[42] It may also be used in thermoregulation and predator detection.[43][44]

Spectroscopy edit

 
Earth's atmosphere partially or totally blocks some wavelengths of electromagnetic radiation, but in visible light it is mostly transparent

Spectroscopy is the study of objects based on the spectrum of color they emit, absorb or reflect. Visible-light spectroscopy is an important tool in astronomy (as is spectroscopy at other wavelengths), where scientists use it to analyze the properties of distant objects. Chemical elements and small molecules can be detected in astronomical objects by observing emission lines and absorption lines. For example, Helium was first detected by analysis of the spectrum of the Sun. The shift in frequency of spectral lines is used to measure the Doppler shift (redshift or blueshift) of distant objects to determine their velocities towards or away from the observer. Astronomical spectroscopy uses high-dispersion diffraction gratings to observe spectra at very high spectral resolutions.

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ Pedrotti, Frank L.; Pedrotti, Leno M.; Pedrotti, Leno S. (December 21, 2017). Introduction to Optics. Cambridge University Press. pp. 7–8. ISBN 9781108428262.
  2. ^ Starr, Cecie (2005). Biology: Concepts and Applications. Thomson Brooks/Cole. p. 94. ISBN 978-0-534-46226-0.
  3. ^ "The visible spectrum". Britannica.
  4. ^ D. H. Sliney (February 2016). "What is light? The visible spectrum and beyond". Eye. 30 (2): 222–229. doi:10.1038/eye.2015.252. ISSN 1476-5454. PMC 4763133. PMID 26768917.
  5. ^ W. C. Livingston (2001). Color and light in nature (2nd ed.). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 0-521-77284-2.
  6. ^ Grazyna Palczewska; et al. (December 2014). "Human infrared vision is triggered by two-photon chromophore isomerization". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 111 (50): E5445–E5454. Bibcode:2014PNAS..111E5445P. doi:10.1073/pnas.1410162111. PMC 4273384. PMID 25453064.
  7. ^ Nave, R. "Spectral Colors". Hyperphysics. Retrieved 2022-05-11.
  8. ^ Bruno, Thomas J. and Svoronos, Paris D. N. (2005). CRC Handbook of Fundamental Spectroscopic Correlation Charts. CRC Press. ISBN 9781420037685
  9. ^ Coffey, Peter (1912). The Science of Logic: An Inquiry Into the Principles of Accurate Thought. Longmans. p. 185. roger bacon prism.
  10. ^ Isacoff, Stuart (16 January 2009). Temperament: How Music Became a Battleground for the Great Minds of Western Civilization. Knopf Doubleday Publishing Group. pp. 12–13. ISBN 978-0-307-56051-3. Retrieved 18 March 2014.
  11. ^ Asimov, Isaac (1975). Eyes on the universe : a history of the telescope. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. p. 59. ISBN 978-0-395-20716-1.
  12. ^ Evans, Ralph M. (1974). The perception of color (null ed.). New York: Wiley-Interscience. ISBN 978-0-471-24785-2.
  13. ^ McLaren, K. (March 2007). "Newton's indigo". Color Research & Application. 10 (4): 225–229. doi:10.1002/col.5080100411.
  14. ^ Waldman, Gary (2002). Introduction to light : the physics of light, vision, and color (Dover ed.). Mineola: Dover Publications. p. 193. ISBN 978-0-486-42118-6.
  15. ^ Mary Jo Nye, ed. (2003). The Cambridge History of Science: The Modern Physical and Mathematical Sciences. Vol. 5. Cambridge University Press. p. 278. ISBN 978-0-521-57199-9.
  16. ^ John C. D. Brand (1995). Lines of light: the sources of dispersive spectroscopy, 1800–1930. CRC Press. pp. 30–32. ISBN 978-2-88449-163-1.
  17. ^ a b c d e Hunt, D.M.; Wilkie, S.E.; Bowmaker, J.K.; Poopalasundaram, S. (October 2001). "Vision in the ultraviolet". Cellular and Molecular Life Sciences. 58 (11): 1583–1598. doi:10.1007/PL00000798. PMID 11706986. S2CID 22938704. Radiation below 320 nm [ultraviolet (UV)A] is largely screened out by the ozone layer in the Earth's upper atmosphere and is therefore unavailable to the visual system,
  18. ^ a b Boettner, Edward A.; Wolter, J. Reimer (December 1962). "Transmission of Ocular Media". Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science. 1: 776-783.
  19. ^ Laufer, Gabriel (1996). "Geometrical Optics". Introduction to Optics and Lasers in Engineering. p. 11. Bibcode:1996iole.book.....L. doi:10.1017/CBO9781139174190.004. ISBN 978-0-521-45233-5. Retrieved 20 October 2013.
  20. ^ Bradt, Hale (2004). Astronomy Methods: A Physical Approach to Astronomical Observations. Cambridge University Press. p. 26. ISBN 978-0-521-53551-9. Retrieved 20 October 2013.
  21. ^ Ohannesian, Lena; Streeter, Anthony (2001). Handbook of Pharmaceutical Analysis. CRC Press. p. 187. ISBN 978-0-8247-4194-5. Retrieved 20 October 2013.
  22. ^ Ahluwalia, V.K.; Goyal, Madhuri (2000). A Textbook of Organic Chemistry. Narosa. p. 110. ISBN 978-81-7319-159-6. Retrieved 20 October 2013.
  23. ^ a b c Sliney, David H.; Wangemann, Robert T.; Franks, James K.; Wolbarsht, Myron L. (1976). "Visual sensitivity of the eye to infrared laser radiation". Journal of the Optical Society of America. 66 (4): 339–341. Bibcode:1976JOSA...66..339S. doi:10.1364/JOSA.66.000339. PMID 1262982. The foveal sensitivity to several near-infrared laser wavelengths was measured. It was found that the eye could respond to radiation at wavelengths at least as far as 1,064 nm. A continuous 1,064 nm laser source appeared red, but a 1,060 nm pulsed laser source appeared green, which suggests the presence of second harmonic generation in the retina.
  24. ^ Lynch, David K.; Livingston, William Charles (2001). Color and Light in Nature (2nd ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. p. 231. ISBN 978-0-521-77504-5. from the original on 8 October 2022. Retrieved 12 October 2013. Limits of the eye's overall range of sensitivity extends from about 310 to 1,050 nanometers
  25. ^ Dash, Madhab Chandra; Dash, Satya Prakash (2009). Fundamentals of Ecology 3E. Tata McGraw-Hill Education. p. 213. ISBN 978-1-259-08109-5. from the original on 8 October 2022. Retrieved 18 October 2013. Normally the human eye responds to light rays from 390 to 760 nm. This can be extended to a range of 310 to 1,050 nm under artificial conditions.
  26. ^ Saidman, Jean (15 May 1933). "Sur la visibilité de l'ultraviolet jusqu'à la longueur d'onde 3130" [The visibility of the ultraviolet to the wave length of 3130]. Comptes rendus de l'Académie des sciences (in French). 196: 1537–9. from the original on 24 October 2013. Retrieved 21 October 2013.
  27. ^ Kurzel, Richard B.; Wolbarsht, Myron L.; Yamanashi, Bill S. (1977). "Ultraviolet Radiation Effects on the Human Eye". Photochemical and Photobiological Reviews: 133–167. doi:10.1007/978-1-4684-2577-2_3. ISBN 978-1-4684-2579-6.
  28. ^ Enezi J, Revell V, Brown T, Wynne J, Schlangen L, Lucas R (August 2011). "A "melanopic" spectral efficiency function predicts the sensitivity of melanopsin photoreceptors to polychromatic lights". Journal of Biological Rhythms. 26 (4): 314–323. doi:10.1177/0748730411409719. PMID 21775290. S2CID 22369861.
  29. ^ Gouras, Peter; Ekesten, Bjorn (December 2004). "Why do mice have ultra-violet vision?". Experimental Eye Research. 79 (6): 887–892. doi:10.1016/j.exer.2004.06.031. PMID 15642326.
  30. ^ Neitz, Jay; Geist, Timothy; Jacobs, Gerald H. (August 1989). "Color vision in the dog". Visual Neuroscience. 3 (2): 119–125. doi:10.1017/S0952523800004430. PMID 2487095. S2CID 23509491.
  31. ^ Carroll, Joseph; Murphy, Christopher J.; Neitz, Maureen; Ver Hoeve, James N.; Neitz, Jay (3 October 2001). "Photopigment basis for dichromatic color vision in the horse". Journal of Vision. 1 (2): 80–87. doi:10.1167/1.2.2. PMID 12678603.
  32. ^ a b Hart, Nathan S.; Hunt, David M. (January 2007). "Avian Visual Pigments: Characteristics, Spectral Tuning, and Evolution". The American Naturalist. 169 (S1): S7–S26. doi:10.1086/510141. PMID 19426092. S2CID 25779190.
  33. ^ Cuthill, Innes C (1997). "Ultraviolet vision in birds". In Peter J.B. Slater (ed.). Advances in the Study of Behavior. Vol. 29. Oxford, England: Academic Press. p. 161. ISBN 978-0-12-004529-7.
  34. ^ Jamieson, Barrie G. M. (2007). Reproductive Biology and Phylogeny of Birds. Charlottesville VA: University of Virginia. p. 128. ISBN 978-1-57808-386-2.
  35. ^ a b Carleton, Karen L.; Escobar-Camacho, Daniel; Stieb, Sara M.; Cortesi, Fabio; Marshall, N. Justin (15 April 2020). "Seeing the rainbow: mechanisms underlying spectral sensitivity in teleost fishes". Journal of Experimental Biology. 223 (8). doi:10.1242/jeb.193334. PMC 7188444. PMID 32327561.
  36. ^ . Skeptive. 2013. Archived from the original on December 24, 2013. Retrieved September 28, 2013.
  37. ^ Neumeyer, Christa (2012). "Chapter 2: Color Vision in Goldfish and Other Vertebrates". In Lazareva, Olga; Shimizu, Toru; Wasserman, Edward (eds.). How Animals See the World: Comparative Behavior, Biology, and Evolution of Vision. Oxford Scholarship Online. ISBN 978-0-19-533465-4.
  38. ^ Briscoe, Adriana D.; Chittka, Lars (January 2001). "T<SCP>HE</SCP>E<SCP>VOLUTION OF</SCP>C<SCP>OLOR</SCP>V<SCP>ISION IN</SCP>I<SCP>NSECTS</SCP>". Annual Review of Entomology. 46 (1): 471–510. doi:10.1146/annurev.ento.46.1.471. PMID 11112177.
  39. ^ Skorupski, Peter; Chittka, Lars (10 August 2010). "Photoreceptor Spectral Sensitivity in the Bumblebee, Bombus impatiens (Hymenoptera: Apidae)". PLOS ONE. 5 (8): e12049. Bibcode:2010PLoSO...512049S. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0012049. PMC 2919406. PMID 20711523.
  40. ^ Newman, EA; Hartline, PH (1981). "Integration of visual and infrared information in bimodal neurons in the rattlesnake optic tectum". Science. 213 (4509): 789–91. Bibcode:1981Sci...213..789N. doi:10.1126/science.7256281. PMC 2693128. PMID 7256281.
  41. ^ Kardong, KV; Mackessy, SP (1991). "The strike behavior of a congenitally blind rattlesnake". Journal of Herpetology. 25 (2): 208–211. doi:10.2307/1564650. JSTOR 1564650.
  42. ^ Fang, Janet (14 March 2010). "Snake infrared detection unravelled". Nature News. doi:10.1038/news.2010.122.
  43. ^ Krochmal, Aaron R.; George S. Bakken; Travis J. LaDuc (15 November 2004). "Heat in evolution's kitchen: evolutionary perspectives on the functions and origin of the facial pit of pitvipers (Viperidae: Crotalinae)". Journal of Experimental Biology. 207 (Pt 24): 4231–4238. doi:10.1242/jeb.01278. PMID 15531644.
  44. ^ Greene HW. (1992). "The ecological and behavioral context for pitviper evolution", in Campbell JA, Brodie ED Jr. Biology of the Pitvipers. Texas: Selva. ISBN 0-9630537-0-1.

visible, spectrum, color, spectrum, redirects, here, music, album, color, spectrum, visible, spectrum, band, electromagnetic, spectrum, that, visible, human, electromagnetic, radiation, this, range, wavelengths, called, visible, light, simply, light, optical, . Color spectrum redirects here For the music album see The Color Spectrum The visible spectrum is the band of the electromagnetic spectrum that is visible to the human eye Electromagnetic radiation in this range of wavelengths is called visible light or simply light The optical spectrum is sometimes considered to be the same as the visible spectrum but some authors define the term more broadly to include the ultraviolet and infrared parts of the electromagnetic spectrum as well 1 White light is dispersed by a prism into the colors of the visible spectrum A typical human eye will respond to wavelengths from about 380 to about 750 nanometers 2 In terms of frequency this corresponds to a band in the vicinity of 400 790 terahertz These boundaries are not sharply defined and may vary per individual 3 Under optimal conditions these limits of human perception can extend to 310 nm ultraviolet and 1100 nm near infrared 4 5 6 The spectrum does not contain all the colors that the human visual system can distinguish Unsaturated colors such as pink or purple variations like magenta for example are absent because they can only be made from a mix of multiple wavelengths Colors containing only one wavelength are also called pure colors or spectral colors 7 Visible wavelengths pass largely unattenuated through the Earth s atmosphere via the optical window region of the electromagnetic spectrum An example of this phenomenon is when clean air scatters blue light more than red light and so the midday sky appears blue apart from the area around the Sun which appears white because the light is not scattered as much The optical window is also referred to as the visible window because it overlaps the human visible response spectrum The near infrared NIR window lies just out of the human vision as well as the medium wavelength infrared MWIR window and the long wavelength or far infrared LWIR or FIR window although other animals may perceive them Contents 1 Spectral colors 2 History 3 Limits to visible range 3 1 Atmospheric transmission 3 2 Ocular transmission 3 3 Opsin absorption 3 4 Different definitions 3 5 Vision outside the visible spectrum 3 6 Non visual light detection 4 In non humans 4 1 Mammals 4 2 Birds 4 3 Fish 4 4 Invertebrates 4 5 Thermal vision 5 Spectroscopy 6 See also 7 ReferencesSpectral colors edit nbsp sRGB rendering of the spectrum of visible lightColor Wavelength nm Frequency THz Photon energy eV violet 380 450 670 790 2 75 3 26 blue 450 485 620 670 2 56 2 75 cyan 485 500 600 620 2 48 2 56 green 500 565 530 600 2 19 2 48 yellow 565 590 510 530 2 10 2 19 orange 590 625 480 510 1 98 2 10 red 625 750 400 480 1 65 1 98Main article Spectral color Colors that can be produced by visible light of a narrow band of wavelengths monochromatic light are called pure spectral colors The various color ranges indicated in the illustration are an approximation The spectrum is continuous with no clear boundaries between one color and the next 8 History edit nbsp Newton s color circle from Opticks of 1704 showing the colors he associated with musical notes The spectral colors from red to violet are divided by the notes of the musical scale starting at D The circle completes a full octave from D to D Newton s circle places red at one end of the spectrum next to violet at the other This reflects the fact that non spectral purple colors are observed when red and violet light are mixed In the 13th century Roger Bacon theorized that rainbows were produced by a similar process to the passage of light through glass or crystal 9 In the 17th century Isaac Newton discovered that prisms could disassemble and reassemble white light and described the phenomenon in his book Opticks He was the first to use the word spectrum Latin for appearance or apparition in this sense in print in 1671 in describing his experiments in optics Newton observed that when a narrow beam of sunlight strikes the face of a glass prism at an angle some is reflected and some of the beam passes into and through the glass emerging as different colored bands Newton hypothesized light to be made up of corpuscles particles of different colors with the different colors of light moving at different speeds in transparent matter red light moving more quickly than violet in glass The result is that red light is bent refracted less sharply than violet as it passes through the prism creating a spectrum of colors nbsp Newton s observation of prismatic colors David Brewster 1855 Newton originally divided the spectrum into six named colors red orange yellow green blue and violet He later added indigo as the seventh color since he believed that seven was a perfect number as derived from the ancient Greek sophists of there being a connection between the colors the musical notes the known objects in the Solar System and the days of the week 10 The human eye is relatively insensitive to indigo s frequencies and some people who have otherwise good vision cannot distinguish indigo from blue and violet For this reason some later commentators including Isaac Asimov 11 have suggested that indigo should not be regarded as a color in its own right but merely as a shade of blue or violet Evidence indicates that what Newton meant by indigo and blue does not correspond to the modern meanings of those color words Comparing Newton s observation of prismatic colors with a color image of the visible light spectrum shows that indigo corresponds to what is today called blue whereas his blue corresponds to cyan 12 13 14 In the 18th century Johann Wolfgang von Goethe wrote about optical spectra in his Theory of Colours Goethe used the word spectrum Spektrum to designate a ghostly optical afterimage as did Schopenhauer in On Vision and Colors Goethe argued that the continuous spectrum was a compound phenomenon Where Newton narrowed the beam of light to isolate the phenomenon Goethe observed that a wider aperture produces not a spectrum but rather reddish yellow and blue cyan edges with white between them The spectrum appears only when these edges are close enough to overlap In the early 19th century the concept of the visible spectrum became more definite as light outside the visible range was discovered and characterized by William Herschel infrared and Johann Wilhelm Ritter ultraviolet Thomas Young Thomas Johann Seebeck and others 15 Young was the first to measure the wavelengths of different colors of light in 1802 16 The connection between the visible spectrum and color vision was explored by Thomas Young and Hermann von Helmholtz in the early 19th century Their theory of color vision correctly proposed that the eye uses three distinct receptors to perceive color Limits to visible range editSee also Color vision Physiology of color perception nbsp Photopic black and scotopic green luminous efficiency functions The horizontal axis is wavelength in nm See luminous efficiency function for more info The visible spectrum is limited to wavelengths that can both reach the retina and trigger visual phototransduction excite a visual opsin Insensitivity to UV light is generally limited by transmission through the lens Insensitivity to IR light is limited by the spectral sensitivity functions of the visual opsins The range is defined psychometrically by the luminous efficiency function which accounts for all of these factors In humans there is a separate function for each of two visual systems one for photopic vision used in daylight which is mediated by cone cells and one for scotopic vision used in dim light which is mediated by rod cells Each of these functions have different visible ranges However discussion on the visible range generally assumes photopic vision Atmospheric transmission edit The visible range of most animals evolved to match the optical window which is the range of light that can pass through the atmosphere The ozone layer absorbs almost all UVA light below 315 nm 17 However this only affects cosmic light e g sunlight not terrestrial light e g Bioluminescence Ocular transmission edit nbsp Cumulative transmission spectra of light as it passes through the ocular media namely after the cornea blue before the lens red after the lens gray and before the retina orange The solid lines are for a 4 5 year old eye The dashed orange line is for a 53 year old eye and dotted for a 75 year old eye indicating the effect of lens yellowing Before reaching the retina light must first transmit through the cornea and lens UVB light lt 315 nm is filtered mostly by the cornea and UVA light 315 400 nm is filtered mostly by the lens 18 The lens also yellows with age attenuating transmission most strongly at the blue part of the spectrum 18 This can cause xanthopsia as well as a slight truncation of the short wave blue limit of the visible spectrum Subjects with aphakia are missing a lens so UVA light can reach the retina and excite the visual opsins this expands the visible range and may also lead to cyanopsia Opsin absorption edit Each opsin has a spectral sensitivity function which defines how likely it is to absorb a photon of each wavelength The luminous efficiency function is approximately the superposition of the contributing visual opsins Variance in the position of the individual opsin spectral sensitivity functions therefore affects the luminous efficiency function and the visible range For example the long wave red limit changes proportionally to the position of the L opsin The positions are defined by the peak wavelength wavelength of highest sensitivity so as the L opsin peak wavelength blue shifts by 10 nm the long wave limit of the visible spectrum also shifts 10 nm Large deviations of the L opsin peak wavelength lead to a form of color blindness called protanomaly and a missing L opsin protanopia shortens the visible spectrum by about 30 nm at the long wave limit Forms of color blindness affecting the M opsin and S opsin do not significantly affect the luminous efficiency function nor the limits of the visible spectrum Different definitions edit Regardless of actual physical and biological variance the definition of the limits is not standard and will change depending on the industry For example some industries may be concerned with practical limits so would conservatively report 420 680 nm 19 20 while others may be concerned with psychometrics and achieving the broadest spectrum would liberally report 380 750 or even 380 800 nm 21 22 The luminous efficiency function in the NIR does not have a hard cutoff but rather an exponential decay such that the function s value or vision sensitivity at 1 050 nm is about 109 times weaker than at 700 nm much higher intensity is therefore required to perceive 1 050 nm light than 700 nm light 23 Vision outside the visible spectrum edit Under ideal laboratory conditions subjects may perceive infrared light up to at least 1 064 nm 23 While 1 050 nm NIR light can evoke red suggesting direct absorption by the L opsin there are also reports that pulsed NIR lasers can evoke green which suggests two photon absorption may be enabling extended NIR sensitivity 23 Similarly young subjects may perceive ultraviolet wavelengths down to about 310 313 nm 24 25 26 but detection of light below 380 nm may be due to fluorescence of the ocular media rather than direct absorption of UV light by the opsins As UVA light is absorbed by the ocular media lens and cornea it may fluoresce and be released at a lower energy longer wavelength that can then be absorbed by the opsins For example when the lens absorbs 350 nm light the fluorescence emission spectrum is centered on 440 nm 27 Non visual light detection edit In addition to the photopic and scotopic systems humans have other systems for detecting light that do not contribute to the primary visual system For example melanopsin has an absorption range of 420 540 nm and regulates circadian rhythm and other reflexive processes 28 Since the melanopsin system does not form images it is not strictly considered vision and does not contribute to the visible range In non humans editSee also Evolution of color vision The visible spectrum is defined as that visible to humans but the variance between species is large Not only can cone opsins be spectrally shifted to alter the visible range but vertebrates with 4 cones tetrachromatic or 2 cones dichromatic relative to humans 3 trichromatic will also tend to have a wider or narrower visible spectrum than humans respectively Vertebrates tend to have 1 4 different opsin classes 17 longwave sensitive LWS with peak sensitivity between 500 570 nm middlewave sensitive MWS with peak sensitivity between 480 520 nm shortwave sensitive SWS with peak sensitivity between 415 470 nm and violet ultraviolet sensitive VS UVS with peak sensitivity between 355 435 nm Testing the visual systems of animals behaviorally is difficult so the visible range of animals is usually estimated by comparing the peak wavelengths of opsins with those of typical humans S opsin at 420 nm and L opsin at 560 nm Mammals edit Most mammals have retained only two opsin classes LWS and VS due likely to the nocturnal bottleneck However old world primates including humans have since evolved two versions in the LWS class to regain trichromacy 17 Unlike most mammals rodents UVS opsins have remained at shorter wavelengths Along with their lack of UV filters in the lens mice have a UVS opsin that can detect down to 340 nm While allowing UV light to reach the retina can lead to retinal damage the short lifespan of mice compared with other mammals may minimize this disadvantage relative to the advantage of UV vision 29 Dogs have two cone opsins at 429 nm and 555 nm so see almost the entire visible spectrum of humans despite being dichromatic 30 Horses have two cone opsins at 428 nm and 539 nm yielding a slightly more truncated red vision 31 Birds edit See also Bird vision Ultraviolet sensitivity Most other vertebrates birds lizards fish etc have retained their tetrachromacy including UVS opsins that extend further into the ultraviolet than humans VS opsin 17 The sensitivity of avian UVS opsins vary greatly from 355 425 nm and LWS opsins from 560 570 nm 32 This translates to some birds with a visible spectrum on par with humans and other birds with greatly expanded sensitivity to UV light The LWS opsin of birds is sometimes reported to have a peak wavelength above 600 nm but this is an effective peak wavelength that incorporates the filter of avian oil droplets 32 The peak wavelength of the LWS opsin alone is the better predictor of the long wave limit A possible benefit of avian UV vision involves sex dependent markings on their plumage that are visible only in the ultraviolet range 33 34 Fish edit See also Vision in fish Ultraviolet Teleosts bony fish are generally tetrachromatic The sensitivity of fish UVS opsins vary from 347 383 nm and LWS opsins from 500 570 nm 35 However some fish that use alternative chromophores can extend their LWS opsin sensitivity to 625 nm 35 The popular belief that the common goldfish is the only animal that can see both infrared and ultraviolet light 36 is incorrect because goldfish cannot see infrared light 37 Invertebrates edit The visual systems of invertebrates deviate greatly from vertebrates so direct comparisons are difficult However UV sensitivity has been reported in most insect species 38 Bees and many other insects can detect ultraviolet light which helps them find nectar in flowers Plant species that depend on insect pollination may owe reproductive success to their appearance in ultraviolet light rather than how colorful they appear to humans Bees long wave limit is at about 590 nm 39 Mantis shrimp exhibit up to 14 opsins enabling a visible range of less than 300 nm to above 700 nm 17 Thermal vision edit Main article Infrared sensing in snakes Some snakes can see 40 radiant heat at wavelengths between 5 and 30 mm to a degree of accuracy such that a blind rattlesnake can target vulnerable body parts of the prey at which it strikes 41 and other snakes with the organ may detect warm bodies from a meter away 42 It may also be used in thermoregulation and predator detection 43 44 Spectroscopy edit nbsp Earth s atmosphere partially or totally blocks some wavelengths of electromagnetic radiation but in visible light it is mostly transparentMain article Spectroscopy Spectroscopy is the study of objects based on the spectrum of color they emit absorb or reflect Visible light spectroscopy is an important tool in astronomy as is spectroscopy at other wavelengths where scientists use it to analyze the properties of distant objects Chemical elements and small molecules can be detected in astronomical objects by observing emission lines and absorption lines For example Helium was first detected by analysis of the spectrum of the Sun The shift in frequency of spectral lines is used to measure the Doppler shift redshift or blueshift of distant objects to determine their velocities towards or away from the observer Astronomical spectroscopy uses high dispersion diffraction gratings to observe spectra at very high spectral resolutions See also edit nbsp Wikisource has original text related to this article Definition of the Color Indigo nbsp Wikimedia Commons has media related to Visible spectrum High energy visible light Cosmic ray visual phenomena Electromagnetic absorption by water Two photon absorption A method for seeing outside the visible spectrumReferences edit Pedrotti Frank L Pedrotti Leno M Pedrotti Leno S December 21 2017 Introduction to Optics Cambridge University Press pp 7 8 ISBN 9781108428262 Starr Cecie 2005 Biology Concepts and Applications Thomson Brooks Cole p 94 ISBN 978 0 534 46226 0 The visible spectrum Britannica D H Sliney February 2016 What is light The visible spectrum and beyond Eye 30 2 222 229 doi 10 1038 eye 2015 252 ISSN 1476 5454 PMC 4763133 PMID 26768917 W C Livingston 2001 Color and light in nature 2nd ed Cambridge UK Cambridge University Press ISBN 0 521 77284 2 Grazyna Palczewska et al December 2014 Human infrared vision is triggered by two photon chromophore isomerization Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 111 50 E5445 E5454 Bibcode 2014PNAS 111E5445P doi 10 1073 pnas 1410162111 PMC 4273384 PMID 25453064 Nave R Spectral Colors Hyperphysics Retrieved 2022 05 11 Bruno Thomas J and Svoronos Paris D N 2005 CRC Handbook of Fundamental Spectroscopic Correlation Charts CRC Press ISBN 9781420037685 Coffey Peter 1912 The Science of Logic An Inquiry Into the Principles of Accurate Thought Longmans p 185 roger bacon prism Isacoff Stuart 16 January 2009 Temperament How Music Became a Battleground for the Great Minds of Western Civilization Knopf Doubleday Publishing Group pp 12 13 ISBN 978 0 307 56051 3 Retrieved 18 March 2014 Asimov Isaac 1975 Eyes on the universe a history of the telescope Boston Houghton Mifflin p 59 ISBN 978 0 395 20716 1 Evans Ralph M 1974 The perception of color null ed New York Wiley Interscience ISBN 978 0 471 24785 2 McLaren K March 2007 Newton s indigo Color Research amp Application 10 4 225 229 doi 10 1002 col 5080100411 Waldman Gary 2002 Introduction to light the physics of light vision and color Dover ed Mineola Dover Publications p 193 ISBN 978 0 486 42118 6 Mary Jo Nye ed 2003 The Cambridge History of Science The Modern Physical and Mathematical Sciences Vol 5 Cambridge University Press p 278 ISBN 978 0 521 57199 9 John C D Brand 1995 Lines of light the sources of dispersive spectroscopy 1800 1930 CRC Press pp 30 32 ISBN 978 2 88449 163 1 a b c d e Hunt D M Wilkie S E Bowmaker J K Poopalasundaram S October 2001 Vision in the ultraviolet Cellular and Molecular Life Sciences 58 11 1583 1598 doi 10 1007 PL00000798 PMID 11706986 S2CID 22938704 Radiation below 320 nm ultraviolet UV A is largely screened out by the ozone layer in the Earth s upper atmosphere and is therefore unavailable to the visual system a b Boettner Edward A Wolter J Reimer December 1962 Transmission of Ocular Media Investigative Ophthalmology amp Visual Science 1 776 783 Laufer Gabriel 1996 Geometrical Optics Introduction to Optics and Lasers in Engineering p 11 Bibcode 1996iole book L doi 10 1017 CBO9781139174190 004 ISBN 978 0 521 45233 5 Retrieved 20 October 2013 Bradt Hale 2004 Astronomy Methods A Physical Approach to Astronomical Observations Cambridge University Press p 26 ISBN 978 0 521 53551 9 Retrieved 20 October 2013 Ohannesian Lena Streeter Anthony 2001 Handbook of Pharmaceutical Analysis CRC Press p 187 ISBN 978 0 8247 4194 5 Retrieved 20 October 2013 Ahluwalia V K Goyal Madhuri 2000 A Textbook of Organic Chemistry Narosa p 110 ISBN 978 81 7319 159 6 Retrieved 20 October 2013 a b c Sliney David H Wangemann Robert T Franks James K Wolbarsht Myron L 1976 Visual sensitivity of the eye to infrared laser radiation Journal of the Optical Society of America 66 4 339 341 Bibcode 1976JOSA 66 339S doi 10 1364 JOSA 66 000339 PMID 1262982 The foveal sensitivity to several near infrared laser wavelengths was measured It was found that the eye could respond to radiation at wavelengths at least as far as 1 064 nm A continuous 1 064 nm laser source appeared red but a 1 060 nm pulsed laser source appeared green which suggests the presence of second harmonic generation in the retina Lynch David K Livingston William Charles 2001 Color and Light in Nature 2nd ed Cambridge Cambridge University Press p 231 ISBN 978 0 521 77504 5 Archived from the original on 8 October 2022 Retrieved 12 October 2013 Limits of the eye s overall range of sensitivity extends from about 310 to 1 050 nanometers Dash Madhab Chandra Dash Satya Prakash 2009 Fundamentals of Ecology 3E Tata McGraw Hill Education p 213 ISBN 978 1 259 08109 5 Archived from the original on 8 October 2022 Retrieved 18 October 2013 Normally the human eye responds to light rays from 390 to 760 nm This can be extended to a range of 310 to 1 050 nm under artificial conditions Saidman Jean 15 May 1933 Sur la visibilite de l ultraviolet jusqu a la longueur d onde 3130 The visibility of the ultraviolet to the wave length of 3130 Comptes rendus de l Academie des sciences in French 196 1537 9 Archived from the original on 24 October 2013 Retrieved 21 October 2013 Kurzel Richard B Wolbarsht Myron L Yamanashi Bill S 1977 Ultraviolet Radiation Effects on the Human Eye Photochemical and Photobiological Reviews 133 167 doi 10 1007 978 1 4684 2577 2 3 ISBN 978 1 4684 2579 6 Enezi J Revell V Brown T Wynne J Schlangen L Lucas R August 2011 A melanopic spectral efficiency function predicts the sensitivity of melanopsin photoreceptors to polychromatic lights Journal of Biological Rhythms 26 4 314 323 doi 10 1177 0748730411409719 PMID 21775290 S2CID 22369861 Gouras Peter Ekesten Bjorn December 2004 Why do mice have ultra violet vision Experimental Eye Research 79 6 887 892 doi 10 1016 j exer 2004 06 031 PMID 15642326 Neitz Jay Geist Timothy Jacobs Gerald H August 1989 Color vision in the dog Visual Neuroscience 3 2 119 125 doi 10 1017 S0952523800004430 PMID 2487095 S2CID 23509491 Carroll Joseph Murphy Christopher J Neitz Maureen Ver Hoeve James N Neitz Jay 3 October 2001 Photopigment basis for dichromatic color vision in the horse Journal of Vision 1 2 80 87 doi 10 1167 1 2 2 PMID 12678603 a b Hart Nathan S Hunt David M January 2007 Avian Visual Pigments Characteristics Spectral Tuning and Evolution The American Naturalist 169 S1 S7 S26 doi 10 1086 510141 PMID 19426092 S2CID 25779190 Cuthill Innes C 1997 Ultraviolet vision in birds In Peter J B Slater ed Advances in the Study of Behavior Vol 29 Oxford England Academic Press p 161 ISBN 978 0 12 004529 7 Jamieson Barrie G M 2007 Reproductive Biology and Phylogeny of Birds Charlottesville VA University of Virginia p 128 ISBN 978 1 57808 386 2 a b Carleton Karen L Escobar Camacho Daniel Stieb Sara M Cortesi Fabio Marshall N Justin 15 April 2020 Seeing the rainbow mechanisms underlying spectral sensitivity in teleost fishes Journal of Experimental Biology 223 8 doi 10 1242 jeb 193334 PMC 7188444 PMID 32327561 True or False The common goldfish is the only animal that can see both infra red and ultra violet light Skeptive 2013 Archived from the original on December 24 2013 Retrieved September 28 2013 Neumeyer Christa 2012 Chapter 2 Color Vision in Goldfish and Other Vertebrates In Lazareva Olga Shimizu Toru Wasserman Edward eds How Animals See the World Comparative Behavior Biology and Evolution of Vision Oxford Scholarship Online ISBN 978 0 19 533465 4 Briscoe Adriana D Chittka Lars January 2001 T lt SCP gt HE lt SCP gt E lt SCP gt VOLUTION OF lt SCP gt C lt SCP gt OLOR lt SCP gt V lt SCP gt ISION IN lt SCP gt I lt SCP gt NSECTS lt SCP gt Annual Review of Entomology 46 1 471 510 doi 10 1146 annurev ento 46 1 471 PMID 11112177 Skorupski Peter Chittka Lars 10 August 2010 Photoreceptor Spectral Sensitivity in the Bumblebee Bombus impatiens Hymenoptera Apidae PLOS ONE 5 8 e12049 Bibcode 2010PLoSO 512049S doi 10 1371 journal pone 0012049 PMC 2919406 PMID 20711523 Newman EA Hartline PH 1981 Integration of visual and infrared information in bimodal neurons in the rattlesnake optic tectum Science 213 4509 789 91 Bibcode 1981Sci 213 789N doi 10 1126 science 7256281 PMC 2693128 PMID 7256281 Kardong KV Mackessy SP 1991 The strike behavior of a congenitally blind rattlesnake Journal of Herpetology 25 2 208 211 doi 10 2307 1564650 JSTOR 1564650 Fang Janet 14 March 2010 Snake infrared detection unravelled Nature News doi 10 1038 news 2010 122 Krochmal Aaron R George S Bakken Travis J LaDuc 15 November 2004 Heat in evolution s kitchen evolutionary perspectives on the functions and origin of the facial pit of pitvipers Viperidae Crotalinae Journal of Experimental Biology 207 Pt 24 4231 4238 doi 10 1242 jeb 01278 PMID 15531644 Greene HW 1992 The ecological and behavioral context for pitviper evolution in Campbell JA Brodie ED Jr Biology of the Pitvipers Texas Selva ISBN 0 9630537 0 1 Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Visible spectrum amp oldid 1188198342, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.