fbpx
Wikipedia

Little Albert experiment

The Little Albert experiment was a study that mid-20th century psychologists interpret as evidence of classical conditioning in humans. The study is also claimed to be an example of stimulus generalization although reading the research report shows that fear did not generalize by color or tactile qualities.[1] It was carried out by John B. Watson and his graduate student, Rosalie Rayner, at Johns Hopkins University. The results were first published in the February 1920 issue of the Journal of Experimental Psychology.

One of a series of published stills taken from film of the experiment
The film of the experiment

After observing children in the field, Watson hypothesized that the fearful response of children to loud noises is an innate unconditioned response. He wanted to test the notion that by following the principles of the procedure now known as "classical conditioning", he could use this unconditioned response to condition a child to fear a distinctive stimulus that normally would not be feared by a child (in this case, furry objects). However, he admitted in his research article that the fear he generated was neither strong or lasting.

Method

The aim of Watson and Rayner was to condition a phobia in an emotionally stable child.[2] For this study they chose a nine-month old infant from a hospital. The child was referred to as "Albert" for the experiment.[3] Watson followed the procedures which Ivan Pavlov had used in his experiments with dogs.[4]

Before the experiment, Albert was given a battery of baseline emotional tests: the infant was exposed, briefly and for the first time, to a white rat, a rabbit, a dog, a monkey, masks (with and without hair), cotton, wool, burning newspapers, and other stimuli. Albert showed no fear of any of these items during the baseline tests.

For the experiment proper, by which point Albert was 11 months old, he was put on a mattress on a table in the middle of a room. A white laboratory rat was placed near Albert and he was allowed to play with it. At this point, Watson and Rayner made a loud sound behind Albert's back by striking a suspended steel bar with a hammer each time the baby touched the rat. Albert responded to the noise by crying and showing fear. After several such pairings of the two stimuli, Albert was presented with only the rat. Upon seeing the rat, Albert became very distressed, crying and crawling away. Apparently, the infant associated the white rat with the noise. The rat, originally a neutral stimulus, had become a conditioned stimulus, and was eliciting an emotional response (conditioned response) similar to the distress (unconditioned response) originally given to the noise (unconditioned stimulus).[5]

In further experiments, Little Albert seemed to generalize his response to the white rat. He became distressed at the sight of several other furry objects, such as a rabbit, a furry dog, and a seal-skin coat, and even a Santa Claus mask with white cotton balls in the beard. However, this stimulus generalization did not extend to everything with hair.[5]

Watson's experiment had many failings by modern standards.[6] For example, it had only a single subject and no control subjects. Furthermore, such an experiment could be hard to conduct in compliance with current law and regulations, given the expected risks to the subject.

Subsequent events

Albert was about one year old at the end of the experiment, and he reportedly left the hospital shortly thereafter.[7] Though Watson had discussed what might be done to remove Albert's conditioned fears, he chose not to attempt such desensitization with Albert, and it is thought likely that the infant's fear of furry things continued post-experimentally.[8]

Watson later gave a series of weekend lectures describing the Little Albert study. One of these lectures was attended by Mary Cover Jones, which sparked her interest in pursuing graduate work in psychology. Jones conducted an experiment to figure out how to eliminate fear responses in children and studied a boy named Peter, who was two years old. Peter shared similar fears of white rabbits and furry objects as Little Albert. Jones was able to increase Peter's tolerance of white rabbits by exposing him to the animal, known as direct conditioning, and having Peter interact with children who were not afraid of the rabbit. Mary Cover Jones was the first psychologist to desensitize or uncondition a fear response and become known as the "Mother of Behavior Therapy".[9]

Identifying Little Albert

According to some textbooks[example needed], Albert's mother worked in the same building as Watson and did not know the tests were being conducted. When she found out, she took Albert and moved away, letting no one know where they were going. A 2009 report, however, disputes that.[10] The original report had stated that the baby's mother was a wet nurse at the hospital, who may have felt coerced and unable to turn down a request for her baby to be used in Watson's experiment. The claim of coercion was successfully challenged in an article published in the same journal as the coercion claim.

Douglas Merritte

In 2009, psychologists Hall P. Beck and Sharman Levinson published an article in which they claimed to have discovered the true identity of "Albert B."[11] After reviewing Watson's correspondence and publications, as well as research in public documents (such as the 1920 United States Census and state birth and death records), Beck argued that "Albert B." was a pseudonym for Douglas Merritte, the son of Arvilla Merritte, then a woman who appears to have been a wet nurse at the Harriet Lane Home.[11]

It was later found that Douglas Merritte had hydrocephalus, from which he died at the age of 6. With this condition, which is when there is an accumulation of cerebrospinal fluid on the brain, Merritte may have had severe trouble seeing at the time of the experiment, and this disputes the claim that the child in question was average and healthy.[10] According to researchers who looked at this case years later, if Douglas Merritte was, indeed, Little Albert, his actions during the conditioning sessions align with signs of neurological impairment.[12] This includes Little Albert's use of hand-scooping, rather than grasping gestures typical of this age, as well as poor eye-scanning abilities and his lack of facial expressions. Other research has argued, however, that Douglas Merritte may not have been "Little Albert",[13] who may in fact have been young William Albert Barger.

William Albert Barger

The identity claimed by Beck, Levinson and Irons has been contested by psychology researchers Russ Powell and Nancy Digdon, who offer an alternative identity based on available data.[13][14] William Barger had been born within a day of Merritte, was known by friends and family as "Albert", which was his middle name, and his mother had also worked at the hospital where the experiment was conducted. In addition, his size and developmental condition much more closely matched the experiment's documentation of the subject baby's condition.[15]

Through the use of a professional genealogist, the researchers learned Barger had died in 2007 at age 87 and identified one close living relative, a niece. In an interview, Barger's niece stated that she and her uncle had been quite close throughout his life, acknowledged Barger's antipathy toward dogs as a well-known fact that family members, particularly his wife, would tease him about (the researchers noted there was no way to determine whether or not this behavior was linked to Watson's experiment). She also informed researchers of her uncle's aversion to animals in general, not just dogs. Though it was not a particularly strong aversion, family members would often have to keep their dogs in a separate room when he visited.[16] Outside of this, Barger's niece stated that she did not recall any other phobias he may have had. The researchers concluded that Barger would have been unaware of his role as an infant test subject.[17]

Ethical considerations

The experiment today would be considered unethical according to the American Psychological Association's ethic code, and legislation has been passed to prevent such potentially harmful experiments.[18] In the early 1970s, following widely publicized cases of research abuse, the National Commission for the Protection of Human Subjects of Biomedical and Behavioral Research (NCPHS) was created to study issues surrounding the protection of humans in research.[18] In 1979, the Commission issued a report entitled Ethical Principles and Guidelines for the Protection of Human Subjects of Research (commonly called the Belmont Report), which provided the ethical framework on which current federal regulations for the protection of human participants in research are based.[19][20] Under the NCPHS standards set in the late 1970s, an experiment such as Watson's would not have been allowed.[21][22] Research with human participants is also regulated by the Health Insurance Portability and Accountability Act of 1996 and the Public Health Service Act. In addition, training of researchers in the use of human participants has been required by the National Institutes of Health since 2000.[18]

Criticisms

A detailed review of the original study and its subsequent interpretations by Ben Harris (1979)[23] stated:

Critical reading of Watson and Rayner's (1920) report reveals little evidence either that Albert developed a rat phobia or even that animals consistently evoked his fear (or anxiety) during Watson and Rayner's experiment. It may be useful for modern learning theorists to see how the Albert study prompted subsequent research ... but it seems time, finally, to place the Watson and Rayner data in the category of "interesting but uninterpretable" results.

It is difficult to be certain exactly what happened during the Little Albert experiment since concrete evidence and scientific records are lacking. Though a film was shot during the experiment, textbooks interpret the movie differently. Various sources give contradicting accounts of events that took place, and they raise questions about exactly what stimuli were used, which stimuli the baby came to fear, and what happened to the child after the experiment. It was said that most textbooks "suffer from inaccuracies of various degrees" while referring to Watson and Rayner's study. Texts often misrepresent, exaggerate, or minimize the range of Albert's post-conditioning fears.[24]

Other criticisms stem from the health of the child (cited as Douglas Merritte) who was not a "healthy", "normal" infant as claimed in the study, but one who was very ill and had exhibited symptoms of hydrocephalus since birth—according to relatives he never learned to walk or talk later in life. The child would die five years after the experiment due to complications from the congenital disease. It is stated that the study's authors were aware of the child's severe cognitive deficit, abnormal behavior, and unusually frequent crying, but continued to terrify the sick infant and generalize their findings to healthy infants, an act criticized as academic fraud.[25][26] These accusations were successfully challenged in the same journals that published the initial claims. The articles refuting the "sick baby" claim explain that the child was actually William (called Albert by his family) Barger, and that the child was, in fact, healthy.[14]

Notes

  1. ^ Harris, B. (1979). Whatever happened to little Albert. American Psychologist, 34(2), 151-160
  2. ^ Hill, G. (2009). AS & A Level Psychology Through Diagrams, pg 27.
  3. ^ Watson & Rayner, 1920, p. 1
  4. ^ Anyone—Regardless of Their Nature—Can Be Trained to Be Anything; Watson, John B.; "Big Ideas Simply Explained: The Psychology Book." London: Dorling Kindersley Publishing, Inc., 2012; Credo web reference-Subscription Required; accessed September 23, 2013.
  5. ^ a b Steven Schwartz; Classic Studies in Psychology; Palo Alto: Mayfield Publishing,1986; print.
  6. ^ Harris, B. (1979). Whatever happened to little Albert. American Psychologist, 34(2), 151-160. http://users.sussex.ac.uk/~grahamh/RM1web/Classic%20papers/Harris1979.pdf
  7. ^ Hill, pg 177.
  8. ^ (Harris, 1979).
  9. ^ Rutherford, Alexandra. "Mary Cover Jones". Psychology's Feminist Voices, www.feministvoices.com/mary-cover-jones/.
  10. ^ a b H. P.; Levinson, S., & Irons, G.; 2009; pp.605–614"
  11. ^ a b Beck, H. P., Levinson, S., & Irons, G. (2009). "Finding Little Albert: A journey to John B. Watson's infant laboratory" (PDF). American Psychologist. 64 (7): 605–614. doi:10.1037/a0017234. PMID 19824748.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  12. ^ DeAngelis, T. "Was 'Little Albert' ill during the famed conditioning study?". www.apa.org. American Psychological Association. Retrieved 3 December 2020.
  13. ^ a b Powell, Russell A.; Digdon, Nancy; Harris, Ben; Smithson, Christopher (2014). "Correcting the record on Watson, Rayner, and Little Albert: Albert Barger as "Psychology's lost boy"". American Psychologist. 69 (6): 600–611. doi:10.1037/a0036854. PMID 25197838.
  14. ^ a b Digdon, Nancy; Powell, Russell A.; Harris, Ben (2014). "Little Albert's Alleged Neurologicial Impairment: Watson, Rayner, and Historical Revision". History of Psychology. 17 (4): 312–324. doi:10.1037/a0037325. PMID 25068585.
  15. ^ Bartlett, T. (June 2, 2014). "The Search for Psychology's Lost Boy: In 2009 the Decades-old Mystery of 'Little Albert' was Finally Solved... or Was It?". The Chronicle of Higher Education.
  16. ^ Powell, Russell A.; Digdon, Nancy; Harris, Ben; Smithson, Christopher (2014). "Correcting the record on Watson, Rayner, and Little Albert: Albert Barger as "Psychology's lost boy"". American Psychologist. American Psychological Association. 69 (6): 600–611. doi:10.1037/a0036854. PMID 25197838.
  17. ^ "Whatever Happened to Little Albert?". Edmonton, Alberta: MacEwan University News. June 2, 2014. Retrieved August 30, 2014.
  18. ^ a b c The Belmont Report – Ethical Principles and Guidelines for the Protection of Human Subjects of Research — Regulations and Policy; Website (Download and Video available); HHS.gov; US Dept. of Health and Human Services; Office for Human Research Protections; accessed September 2020
  19. ^ American Psychological Association; 2013.
  20. ^ National Institute of Health
  21. ^ American Psychological Association; 2013
  22. ^ Note: It is now considered unethical to evoke reactions of fear in humans under laboratory circumstances, except when the participant has given informed consent to being purposely horrified as part of the experiment. The standards dictate that experiments should not cause the human participants to suffer unnecessary distress or to be in any way physically harmed. The welfare of the human participants must always be the paramount consideration in any form of research, and this is especially true with specially protected groups such as children.
  23. ^ Ben Harris. "Whatever Happened to Little Albert?". Archived from the original on 3 August 2012. Retrieved 30 August 2010.
  24. ^ Harris, 2011, 10
  25. ^ Bartlett, Tom. "A New Twist in the Sad Saga of Little Albert". chronicle.com. The Chronicle of Higher Education. Retrieved 8 June 2017.
  26. ^ Fridlund, Alan J.; Beck, Hall P.; Goldie, William D.; Irons, Gary (November 2012). "Little Albert: A neurologically impaired child". History of Psychology. 15 (4): 302–327. doi:10.1037/a0026720. PMID 23397921.

References

  • American Psychological Association (2010). Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code of Conduct.
  • Bartlett, T. (2014, June 2). [1] The Search for Psychology's Lost Boy: In 2009 the decades-old mystery of 'Little Albert' was finally solved. Or was it? Chronicle of Higher Education.
  • Bartlett, T. (2012). A New Twist in the Sad Saga of Little Albert.
  • Cover Jones, M (1924). "A Laboratory Study of Fear: The Case of Peter". Pedagogical Seminary. 31 (4): 308–315. doi:10.1080/08856559.1924.9944851.
  • DeAngelis, T. (2010). 'Little Albert' regains his identity. Monitor on Psychology, 41, 1. pp. 10.
  • Digdon, Nancy; Powell, Russell A.; Harris, Ben (November 2014). "Little Albert's alleged neurological impairment: Watson, Rayner, and Historical Revision". History of Psychology. 17 (4): 312–324. doi:10.1037/a0037325. PMID 25068585.
  • Fridlund, A. J.; Beck, H. P.; Goldie, W. D.; Irons, G. (2012). "Little Albert: A neurologically impaired child". History of Psychology. 15 (4): 302–327. doi:10.1037/a0026720. PMID 23397921.
  • Harris, B. (2014). Rosalie Rayner, feminist? Revista de Historia de la Psicología, 35, 61–69.
  • Harris, B (2011). "Letting go of Little Albert: Disciplinary memory, history, and the uses of myth". Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences. 47 (1): 1–17. doi:10.1002/jhbs.20470. PMID 21207487.
  • Harris, B (1979). (PDF). American Psychologist. 34 (2): 151–160. doi:10.1037/0003-066x.34.2.151. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2009-02-05.
  • Hill, G. (2009). AS & A Level Psychology Through Diagrams, Oxford University Press.
  • Hock, R. (2005). Forty Studies That Changed Psychology: Explorations into the History of Psychological Research. 5th ed. New Jersey: Prentice Hall.
  • Kasschau, R. (2001). Understanding psychology. Columbus, OH: Glenco/McGraw-Hill.
  • National Institute of Health (2000). Required Education in the Protection of Human Research Participants.
  • Powell. R. A., Digdon, N., Harris, B. & Smithson, C. Correcting the record on Watson, Rayner and Little Albert: Albert Barger as ‘Psychology’s lost boy.’ American Psychologist.
  • Reiss, B. K. (1990). A biography of Mary Cover Jones. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. The Wright Institute, Los Angeles, CA.
  • Watson, J.B.; Rayner, R. (1920). "Conditioned emotional reactions". Journal of Experimental Psychology. 3 (1): 1–14. doi:10.1037/h0069608. hdl:21.11116/0000-0001-9171-B.

Further reading

  • Weiten, Wayne (2001). Psychology: Themes & Variations. Belmont: Wadsworth Thomson Learning. p. 230. ISBN 978-0-534-36714-5.

little, albert, experiment, study, that, 20th, century, psychologists, interpret, evidence, classical, conditioning, humans, study, also, claimed, example, stimulus, generalization, although, reading, research, report, shows, that, fear, generalize, color, tac. The Little Albert experiment was a study that mid 20th century psychologists interpret as evidence of classical conditioning in humans The study is also claimed to be an example of stimulus generalization although reading the research report shows that fear did not generalize by color or tactile qualities 1 It was carried out by John B Watson and his graduate student Rosalie Rayner at Johns Hopkins University The results were first published in the February 1920 issue of the Journal of Experimental Psychology One of a series of published stills taken from film of the experiment source source source source source source track The film of the experiment After observing children in the field Watson hypothesized that the fearful response of children to loud noises is an innate unconditioned response He wanted to test the notion that by following the principles of the procedure now known as classical conditioning he could use this unconditioned response to condition a child to fear a distinctive stimulus that normally would not be feared by a child in this case furry objects However he admitted in his research article that the fear he generated was neither strong or lasting Contents 1 Method 2 Subsequent events 3 Identifying Little Albert 3 1 Douglas Merritte 3 2 William Albert Barger 4 Ethical considerations 5 Criticisms 6 Notes 7 References 8 Further readingMethod EditThe aim of Watson and Rayner was to condition a phobia in an emotionally stable child 2 For this study they chose a nine month old infant from a hospital The child was referred to as Albert for the experiment 3 Watson followed the procedures which Ivan Pavlov had used in his experiments with dogs 4 Before the experiment Albert was given a battery of baseline emotional tests the infant was exposed briefly and for the first time to a white rat a rabbit a dog a monkey masks with and without hair cotton wool burning newspapers and other stimuli Albert showed no fear of any of these items during the baseline tests For the experiment proper by which point Albert was 11 months old he was put on a mattress on a table in the middle of a room A white laboratory rat was placed near Albert and he was allowed to play with it At this point Watson and Rayner made a loud sound behind Albert s back by striking a suspended steel bar with a hammer each time the baby touched the rat Albert responded to the noise by crying and showing fear After several such pairings of the two stimuli Albert was presented with only the rat Upon seeing the rat Albert became very distressed crying and crawling away Apparently the infant associated the white rat with the noise The rat originally a neutral stimulus had become a conditioned stimulus and was eliciting an emotional response conditioned response similar to the distress unconditioned response originally given to the noise unconditioned stimulus 5 In further experiments Little Albert seemed to generalize his response to the white rat He became distressed at the sight of several other furry objects such as a rabbit a furry dog and a seal skin coat and even a Santa Claus mask with white cotton balls in the beard However this stimulus generalization did not extend to everything with hair 5 Watson s experiment had many failings by modern standards 6 For example it had only a single subject and no control subjects Furthermore such an experiment could be hard to conduct in compliance with current law and regulations given the expected risks to the subject Subsequent events EditAlbert was about one year old at the end of the experiment and he reportedly left the hospital shortly thereafter 7 Though Watson had discussed what might be done to remove Albert s conditioned fears he chose not to attempt such desensitization with Albert and it is thought likely that the infant s fear of furry things continued post experimentally 8 Watson later gave a series of weekend lectures describing the Little Albert study One of these lectures was attended by Mary Cover Jones which sparked her interest in pursuing graduate work in psychology Jones conducted an experiment to figure out how to eliminate fear responses in children and studied a boy named Peter who was two years old Peter shared similar fears of white rabbits and furry objects as Little Albert Jones was able to increase Peter s tolerance of white rabbits by exposing him to the animal known as direct conditioning and having Peter interact with children who were not afraid of the rabbit Mary Cover Jones was the first psychologist to desensitize or uncondition a fear response and become known as the Mother of Behavior Therapy 9 Identifying Little Albert EditAccording to some textbooks example needed Albert s mother worked in the same building as Watson and did not know the tests were being conducted When she found out she took Albert and moved away letting no one know where they were going A 2009 report however disputes that 10 The original report had stated that the baby s mother was a wet nurse at the hospital who may have felt coerced and unable to turn down a request for her baby to be used in Watson s experiment The claim of coercion was successfully challenged in an article published in the same journal as the coercion claim Douglas Merritte Edit In 2009 psychologists Hall P Beck and Sharman Levinson published an article in which they claimed to have discovered the true identity of Albert B 11 After reviewing Watson s correspondence and publications as well as research in public documents such as the 1920 United States Census and state birth and death records Beck argued that Albert B was a pseudonym for Douglas Merritte the son of Arvilla Merritte then a woman who appears to have been a wet nurse at the Harriet Lane Home 11 It was later found that Douglas Merritte had hydrocephalus from which he died at the age of 6 With this condition which is when there is an accumulation of cerebrospinal fluid on the brain Merritte may have had severe trouble seeing at the time of the experiment and this disputes the claim that the child in question was average and healthy 10 According to researchers who looked at this case years later if Douglas Merritte was indeed Little Albert his actions during the conditioning sessions align with signs of neurological impairment 12 This includes Little Albert s use of hand scooping rather than grasping gestures typical of this age as well as poor eye scanning abilities and his lack of facial expressions Other research has argued however that Douglas Merritte may not have been Little Albert 13 who may in fact have been young William Albert Barger William Albert Barger Edit The identity claimed by Beck Levinson and Irons has been contested by psychology researchers Russ Powell and Nancy Digdon who offer an alternative identity based on available data 13 14 William Barger had been born within a day of Merritte was known by friends and family as Albert which was his middle name and his mother had also worked at the hospital where the experiment was conducted In addition his size and developmental condition much more closely matched the experiment s documentation of the subject baby s condition 15 Through the use of a professional genealogist the researchers learned Barger had died in 2007 at age 87 and identified one close living relative a niece In an interview Barger s niece stated that she and her uncle had been quite close throughout his life acknowledged Barger s antipathy toward dogs as a well known fact that family members particularly his wife would tease him about the researchers noted there was no way to determine whether or not this behavior was linked to Watson s experiment She also informed researchers of her uncle s aversion to animals in general not just dogs Though it was not a particularly strong aversion family members would often have to keep their dogs in a separate room when he visited 16 Outside of this Barger s niece stated that she did not recall any other phobias he may have had The researchers concluded that Barger would have been unaware of his role as an infant test subject 17 Ethical considerations EditThe experiment today would be considered unethical according to the American Psychological Association s ethic code and legislation has been passed to prevent such potentially harmful experiments 18 In the early 1970s following widely publicized cases of research abuse the National Commission for the Protection of Human Subjects of Biomedical and Behavioral Research NCPHS was created to study issues surrounding the protection of humans in research 18 In 1979 the Commission issued a report entitled Ethical Principles and Guidelines for the Protection of Human Subjects of Research commonly called the Belmont Report which provided the ethical framework on which current federal regulations for the protection of human participants in research are based 19 20 Under the NCPHS standards set in the late 1970s an experiment such as Watson s would not have been allowed 21 22 Research with human participants is also regulated by the Health Insurance Portability and Accountability Act of 1996 and the Public Health Service Act In addition training of researchers in the use of human participants has been required by the National Institutes of Health since 2000 18 Criticisms EditA detailed review of the original study and its subsequent interpretations by Ben Harris 1979 23 stated Critical reading of Watson and Rayner s 1920 report reveals little evidence either that Albert developed a rat phobia or even that animals consistently evoked his fear or anxiety during Watson and Rayner s experiment It may be useful for modern learning theorists to see how the Albert study prompted subsequent research but it seems time finally to place the Watson and Rayner data in the category of interesting but uninterpretable results It is difficult to be certain exactly what happened during the Little Albert experiment since concrete evidence and scientific records are lacking Though a film was shot during the experiment textbooks interpret the movie differently Various sources give contradicting accounts of events that took place and they raise questions about exactly what stimuli were used which stimuli the baby came to fear and what happened to the child after the experiment It was said that most textbooks suffer from inaccuracies of various degrees while referring to Watson and Rayner s study Texts often misrepresent exaggerate or minimize the range of Albert s post conditioning fears 24 Other criticisms stem from the health of the child cited as Douglas Merritte who was not a healthy normal infant as claimed in the study but one who was very ill and had exhibited symptoms of hydrocephalus since birth according to relatives he never learned to walk or talk later in life The child would die five years after the experiment due to complications from the congenital disease It is stated that the study s authors were aware of the child s severe cognitive deficit abnormal behavior and unusually frequent crying but continued to terrify the sick infant and generalize their findings to healthy infants an act criticized as academic fraud 25 26 These accusations were successfully challenged in the same journals that published the initial claims The articles refuting the sick baby claim explain that the child was actually William called Albert by his family Barger and that the child was in fact healthy 14 Notes Edit Harris B 1979 Whatever happened to little Albert American Psychologist 34 2 151 160 Hill G 2009 AS amp A Level Psychology Through Diagrams pg 27 Watson amp Rayner 1920 p 1 Anyone Regardless of Their Nature Can Be Trained to Be Anything Watson John B Big Ideas Simply Explained The Psychology Book London Dorling Kindersley Publishing Inc 2012 Credo web reference Subscription Required accessed September 23 2013 a b Steven Schwartz Classic Studies in Psychology Palo Alto Mayfield Publishing 1986 print Harris B 1979 Whatever happened to little Albert American Psychologist 34 2 151 160 http users sussex ac uk grahamh RM1web Classic 20papers Harris1979 pdf Hill pg 177 Harris 1979 Rutherford Alexandra Mary Cover Jones Psychology s Feminist Voices www feministvoices com mary cover jones a b H P Levinson S amp Irons G 2009 pp 605 614 a b Beck H P Levinson S amp Irons G 2009 Finding Little Albert A journey to John B Watson s infant laboratory PDF American Psychologist 64 7 605 614 doi 10 1037 a0017234 PMID 19824748 a href Template Cite journal html title Template Cite journal cite journal a CS1 maint multiple names authors list link DeAngelis T Was Little Albert ill during the famed conditioning study www apa org American Psychological Association Retrieved 3 December 2020 a b Powell Russell A Digdon Nancy Harris Ben Smithson Christopher 2014 Correcting the record on Watson Rayner and Little Albert Albert Barger as Psychology s lost boy American Psychologist 69 6 600 611 doi 10 1037 a0036854 PMID 25197838 a b Digdon Nancy Powell Russell A Harris Ben 2014 Little Albert s Alleged Neurologicial Impairment Watson Rayner and Historical Revision History of Psychology 17 4 312 324 doi 10 1037 a0037325 PMID 25068585 Bartlett T June 2 2014 The Search for Psychology s Lost Boy In 2009 the Decades old Mystery of Little Albert was Finally Solved or Was It The Chronicle of Higher Education Powell Russell A Digdon Nancy Harris Ben Smithson Christopher 2014 Correcting the record on Watson Rayner and Little Albert Albert Barger as Psychology s lost boy American Psychologist American Psychological Association 69 6 600 611 doi 10 1037 a0036854 PMID 25197838 Whatever Happened to Little Albert Edmonton Alberta MacEwan University News June 2 2014 Retrieved August 30 2014 a b c The Belmont Report Ethical Principles and Guidelines for the Protection of Human Subjects of Research Regulations and Policy Website Download and Video available HHS gov US Dept of Health and Human Services Office for Human Research Protections accessed September 2020 American Psychological Association 2013 National Institute of Health American Psychological Association 2013 Note It is now considered unethical to evoke reactions of fear in humans under laboratory circumstances except when the participant has given informed consent to being purposely horrified as part of the experiment The standards dictate that experiments should not cause the human participants to suffer unnecessary distress or to be in any way physically harmed The welfare of the human participants must always be the paramount consideration in any form of research and this is especially true with specially protected groups such as children Ben Harris Whatever Happened to Little Albert Archived from the original on 3 August 2012 Retrieved 30 August 2010 Harris 2011 10 Bartlett Tom A New Twist in the Sad Saga of Little Albert chronicle com The Chronicle of Higher Education Retrieved 8 June 2017 Fridlund Alan J Beck Hall P Goldie William D Irons Gary November 2012 Little Albert A neurologically impaired child History of Psychology 15 4 302 327 doi 10 1037 a0026720 PMID 23397921 References EditAmerican Psychological Association 2010 Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code of Conduct Bartlett T 2014 June 2 1 The Search for Psychology s Lost Boy In 2009 the decades old mystery of Little Albert was finally solved Or was it Chronicle of Higher Education Bartlett T 2012 A New Twist in the Sad Saga of Little Albert Cover Jones M 1924 A Laboratory Study of Fear The Case of Peter Pedagogical Seminary 31 4 308 315 doi 10 1080 08856559 1924 9944851 DeAngelis T 2010 Little Albert regains his identity Monitor on Psychology 41 1 pp 10 Digdon Nancy Powell Russell A Harris Ben November 2014 Little Albert s alleged neurological impairment Watson Rayner and Historical Revision History of Psychology 17 4 312 324 doi 10 1037 a0037325 PMID 25068585 Fridlund A J Beck H P Goldie W D Irons G 2012 Little Albert A neurologically impaired child History of Psychology 15 4 302 327 doi 10 1037 a0026720 PMID 23397921 Harris B 2014 Rosalie Rayner feminist Revista de Historia de la Psicologia 35 61 69 Harris B 2011 Letting go of Little Albert Disciplinary memory history and the uses of myth Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences 47 1 1 17 doi 10 1002 jhbs 20470 PMID 21207487 Harris B 1979 Whatever Happened to Little Albert PDF American Psychologist 34 2 151 160 doi 10 1037 0003 066x 34 2 151 Archived from the original PDF on 2009 02 05 Hill G 2009 AS amp A Level Psychology Through Diagrams Oxford University Press Hock R 2005 Forty Studies That Changed Psychology Explorations into the History of Psychological Research 5th ed New Jersey Prentice Hall Kasschau R 2001 Understanding psychology Columbus OH Glenco McGraw Hill National Institute of Health 2000 Required Education in the Protection of Human Research Participants Powell R A Digdon N Harris B amp Smithson C Correcting the record on Watson Rayner and Little Albert Albert Barger as Psychology s lost boy American Psychologist Reiss B K 1990 A biography of Mary Cover Jones Unpublished doctoral dissertation The Wright Institute Los Angeles CA Watson J B Rayner R 1920 Conditioned emotional reactions Journal of Experimental Psychology 3 1 1 14 doi 10 1037 h0069608 hdl 21 11116 0000 0001 9171 B Further reading EditWeiten Wayne 2001 Psychology Themes amp Variations Belmont Wadsworth Thomson Learning p 230 ISBN 978 0 534 36714 5 Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Little Albert experiment amp oldid 1133914183, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.