fbpx
Wikipedia

American Sign Language grammar

The grammar of American Sign Language (ASL) is the best studied of any sign language, though research is still in its infancy, dating back only to William Stokoe in the 1960s.[citation needed]

Morphology

ASL morphology is to a large extent iconic.[dubious ][citation needed] This shows up especially well in reduplication and indexicality.

Derivation

Compounding is used to derive new words in ASL, which often differ in meaning from their constituent signs.[1] For example, the signs FACE and STRONG compound to create a new sign FACE^STRONG, meaning 'to resemble'.[1] Compounds undergo the phonetic process of "hold deletion", whereby the holds at the end of the first constituent and the beginning of the second are elided:[1]

ASL compounding[2]
Individual signs Compound sign
FACE STRONG FACE^STRONG
MH HMH MMH

Many ASL nouns are derived from verbs.[2] This may be done either by reduplicating the movement of the verb if the verb has a single movement, or by restraining (making smaller and faster) the movement of the verb if it already has repeated movement.[3] For example, the noun CHAIR is derived from the verb SIT through reduplication.[3] Another productive method is available for deriving nouns from non-stative verbs.[4] This form of derivation modifies the verb's movement, reduplicating it in a "trilled" manner ("small, quick, stiff movements").[4] For example, this method is used to derive the noun ACTION from the verb ACT.[4]

Characteristic adjectives, which refer to inherent states, may be derived from adjectives which refer to "incidental or temporary states".[5] Characteristic adjectives always use both hands, even if the source adjective only uses one, and they always have repeated, circular movement.[5] Additionally, if the source adjective was one-handed, the derived adjective has alternating movement.[5] "Trilling" may also be used productively to derive adjectives with an "ish" meaning, e.g. BLUE becomes BLUISH.[6]

ASL occasionally uses suffixation in derivation, but less often than in English.[6] Agent nouns may be derived from verbs by adding the suffix AGENT and deleting the final hold of the verb, e.g. TEACH+AGENT 'teacher'.[6] Superlatives are also formed by suffixation, e.g. SMART+MOST 'smartest'.[7]

Certain types of signs, for example those relating to time and age, may incorporate numbers by assimilating their handshape.[7] For example, the word WEEK has handshape /B/ with the weak hand and /1/ with the active hand; the active hand's handshape may be changed to the handshape of any number up to 9 to indicate that many weeks.[7]

There are about 20 non-manual modifiers in ASL, which are either adjectival or adverbial.[8] For example, the adverb 'th', realized as the tongue being placed between the teeth, means 'carelessly / lazily' when combined with a verb:[9]

JOHN

WRITE

LETTER

JOHN WRITE LETTER

'John writes a letter.'

JOHN

 

WRITE

          th

LETTER

 

JOHN WRITE LETTER

{}           th {}

'John writes a letter carelessly.'

Degree

Mouthing is when an individual appears to be making speech sounds, and this is very important for fluent signing. It also has specific morphological uses. For example, one may sign 'man tall' to indicate the man is tall, but by mouthing the syllable cha while signing 'tall', the phrase becomes that man is enormous!

There are other ways of modifying a verb or adjective to make it more intense. These are all more or less equivalent to adding the word "very" in English; which morphology is used depends on the word being modified. Certain words which are short in English, such as 'sad' and 'mad', are sometimes fingerspelled rather than signed to mean 'very sad' and 'very mad'. However, the concept of 'very sad' or 'very mad' can be portrayed with the use of exaggerated body movements and facial expressions. Reduplication of the signs may also occur to emphasize the degree of the statement. Some signs are produced with an exaggeratedly large motion, so that they take up more sign space than normal. This may involve a back-and-forth scissoring motion of the arms to indicate that the sign ought to be yet larger, but that one is physically incapable of making it big enough. Many other signs are given a slow, tense production. The fact that this modulation is morphological rather than merely mimetic can be seen in the sign for 'fast': both 'very slow' and 'very fast' are signed by making the motion either unusually slowly or unusually quickly than it is in the citation forms of 'slow' and 'fast'—not exclusively by making it slower for 'very slow' and faster for 'very fast'.

Reduplication

Reduplication is morphological repetition, and this is extremely common in ASL. Generally the motion of the sign is shortened as well as repeated. Nouns may be derived from verbs through reduplication. For example, the noun chair is formed from the verb to sit by repeating it with a reduced degree of motion. Similar relationships exist between acquisition and to get, airplane and to fly (on an airplane), also window and to open/close a window. Reduplication is commonly used to express intensity as well as several verbal aspects (see below). It is also used to derive signs such as 'every two weeks' from 'two weeks', and is used for verbal number (see below), where the reduplication is iconic for the repetitive meaning of the sign.

Compounds

Many ASL words are historically compounds. However, the two elements of these signs have fused, with features being lost from one or both, to create what might be better called a blend than a compound. Typically only the final hold (see above) remains from the first element, and any reduplication is lost from the second.

An example is the verb AGREE, which derives from the two signs THINK and ALIKE. The verb THINK is signed by bringing a 1 hand inward and touching the forehead (a move and a hold). ALIKE is signed by holding two 1 hands parallel, pointing outward, and bringing them together two or three times. The compound/blend AGREE starts as THINK ends: with the index finger touching the forehead (the final hold of that sign). In addition, the weak hand is already in place, in anticipation of the next part of the sign. Then the hand at the forehead is brought down parallel to the weak hand; it approaches but does not make actual contact, and there is no repetition.

Affixes

ASL, like other mature signed languages, makes extensive use of morphology.[10] Many of ASL's affixes are combined simultaneously rather than sequentially. For example, Ted Supalla's seminal work on ASL verbs of motion revealed that these signs consist of many different affixes, articulated simultaneously according to complex grammatical constraints.[11] This differs from the concatenative morphology of many spoken languages, which except for suprasegmental features such as tone are tightly constrained by the sequential nature of voice sounds.

ASL does have a limited number of concatenative affixes. For example, the agentive suffix (similar to the English '-er') is made by placing two B or 5 hands in front of the torso, palms facing each other, and lowering them. On its own this sign means 'person'; in a compound sign following a verb, it is a suffix for the performer of the action, as in 'drive-er' and 'teach-er'. However, it cannot generally be used to translate English '-er', as it is used with a much more limited set of verbs. It is very similar to the '-ulo' suffix in Esperanto, meaning 'person' by itself and '-related person' when combined with other words.

An ASL prefix, (touching the chin), is used with number signs to indicate 'years old'. The prefix completely assimilates with the initial handshape of the number. For instance, 'fourteen' is signed with a B hand that bends several times at the knuckles. The chin-touch prefix in 'fourteen years old' is thus also made with a B hand. For 'three years old', however, the prefix is made with a 3 hand.

Numeral incorporation and classifiers

Rather than relying on sequential affixes, ASL makes heavy use of simultaneous modification of signs. One example of this is found in the aspectual system (see below); another is numeral incorporation: There are several families of two-handed signs which require one of the hands to take the handshape of a numeral. Many of these deal with time. For example, drawing the dominant hand lengthwise across the palm and fingers of a flat B hand indicates a number of weeks; the dominant hand takes the form of a numeral from one to nine to specify how many weeks. There are analogous signs for 'weeks ago' and 'weeks from now', etc., though in practice several of these signs are only found with the lower numerals.

ASL also has a system of classifiers which may be incorporated into signs.[12] A fist may represent an inactive object such as a rock (this is the default or neutral classifier), a horizontal ILY hand may represent an aircraft, a horizontal 3 hand (thumb pointing up and slightly forward) a motor vehicle, an upright G hand a person on foot, an upright V hand a pair of people on foot, and so on through higher numbers of people. These classifiers are moved through sign space to iconically represent the actions of their referents. For example, an ILY hand may 'lift off' or 'land on' a horizontal B hand to sign an aircraft taking off or landing; a 3 hand may be brought down on a B hand to sign parking a car; and a G hand may be brought toward a V hand to represent one person approaching two.

The frequency of classifier use depends greatly on genre, occurring at a rate of 17.7% in narratives but only 1.1% in casual speech and 0.9% in formal speech.[13]

Frames

Frames are a morphological device that may be unique to sign languages (Liddell 2004). They are incomplete sets of the features which make up signs, and they combine with existing signs, absorbing features from them to form a derived sign. It is the frame which specifies the number and nature of segments in the resulting sign, while the basic signs it combines with lose all but one or two of their original features.

One, the WEEKLY frame, consists of a simple downward movement. It combines with the signs for the days of the week, which then lose their inherent movement. For example, 'Monday' consists of an M/O hand made with a circling movement. 'MondayWEEKLY' (that is, 'on Mondays') is therefore signed as an M/O hand that drops downward, but without the circling movement. A similar ALL DAY frame (a sideward pan) combines with times of the day, such as 'morning' and 'afternoon', which likewise keep their handshape and location but lose their original movement. Numeral incorporation (see above) also uses frames. However, in ASL frames are most productively utilized for verbal aspect.

Verbal aspect

While there is no grammatical tense in ASL, there are numerous verbal aspects. These are produced by modulating the verb: Through reduplication, by placing the verb in an aspectual frame (see above), or with a combination of these means.

An example of an aspectual frame is the unrealized inceptive aspect ('just about to X'), illustrated here with the verb 'to tell'. 'To tell' is an indexical (directional) verb, where the index finger (a G hand) begins with a touch to the chin and then moves outward to point out the recipient of the telling. 'To be just about to tell' retains just the locus and the initial chin touch, which now becomes the final hold of the sign; all other features from the basic verb (in this case, the outward motion and pointing) are dropped and replaced by features from the frame (which are shared with the unrealized inceptive aspects of other verbs such as 'look at', 'wash the dishes', 'yell', 'flirt', etc.). These frame features are: Eye gaze toward the locus (which is no longer pointed at with the hand), an open jaw, and a hand (or hands, in the case of two-hand verbs) in front of the trunk which moves in an arc to the onset location of the basic verb (in this case, touching the chin), while the trunk rotates and the signer inhales, catching her breath during the final hold. The hand shape throughout the sign is whichever is required by the final hold, in this case a G hand.

The variety of aspects in ASL can be illustrated by the verb 'to be sick', which involves the middle finger of the Y/8 hand touching the forehead, and which can be modified by a large number of frames. Several of these involve reduplication, which may but need not be analyzed as part of the frame. (The appropriate non-manual features are not described here.)

  • stative "to be sick" is made with simple iterated contact, typically with around four iterations. This is the basic, citation form of the verb.
  • inchoative "to get sick, to take sick" is made with a single straight movement to contact and a hold of the finger on the forehead.
  • predisposional "to be sickly, to be prone to get sick" is made with incomplete motion: three even circular cycles without contact. This aspect adds reduplication to verbs such as 'to look at' which do not already contain repetition.
  • susceptative "to get sick easily" is made with a thrusting motion: The onset is held; then there is a brief, tense thrust that is checked before actual contact can be made.
  • frequentative "to be often sick" is given a marcato articulation: A regular beat, with 4–6 iterations, and marked onsets and holds.
  • susceptive and frequentative may be combined to mean "to get sick easily and often": Four brief thrusts on a marked, steady beat, without contact with the forehead.
  • protractive "to be continuously sick" is made with a long, tense hold and no movement at all.
  • incessant "to get sick incessantly" has a reduplicated tremolo articulation: A dozen tiny, tense, uneven iterations, as rapid as possible and without contact.
  • durative "to be sick for a long time" is made with a reduplicated elliptical motion: Three slow, uneven cycles, with a heavy downward brush of the forehead and an arching return.
  • iterative "to get sick over and over again" is made with three tense movements and slow returns to the onset position.
  • intensive "to be very sick" is given a single tense articulation: A tense onset hold followed by a single very rapid motion to a long final hold.
  • resultative "to become fully sick" (that is, a complete change of health) is made with an accelerando articulation: A single elongated tense movement which starts slowly and heavily, accelerating to a long final hold.
  • approximative "to be sort of sick, to be a little sick" is made with a reduplicated lax articulation: A spacially extremely reduced, minimal movement, involving a dozen iterations without contact.
  • semblitive "to appear to be sick" [no description]
  • increasing "to get more and more sick" is made with the movements becoming more and more intense.

These modulations readily combine with each other to create yet finer distinctions. Not all verbs take all aspects, and the forms they do take will not necessarily be completely analogous to the verb illustrated here. Conversely, not all aspects are possible with this one verb.

Aspect is unusual in ASL in that transitive verbs derived for aspect lose their transitivity. That is, while you can sign 'dog chew bone' for the dog chewed on a bone, or 'she look-at me' for she looked at me, you cannot do the same in the durative to mean the dog gnawed on the bone or she stared at me. Instead, you must use other strategies, such as a topic construction (see below) to avoid having an object for the verb.

Verbal number

Reduplication is also used for expressing verbal number. Verbal number indicates that the action of the verb is repeated; in the case of ASL it is apparently limited to transitive verbs, where the motion of the verb is either extended or repeated to cover multiple object or recipient loci. (Simple plurality of action can also be conveyed with reduplication, but without indexing any object loci; in fact, such aspectual forms do not allow objects, as noted above.) There are specific dual forms (and for some signers trial forms), as well as plurals. With dual objects, the motion of the verb may be made twice with one hand, or simultaneously with both; while with plurals the object loci may be taken as a group by using a single sweep of the signing hand while the verbal motion is being performed, or individuated by iterating the move across the sweep. For example, 'to ask someone a question' is signed by flexing the index finger of an upright G hand in the direction of that person; the dual involves flexing it at both object loci (sequentially with one hand or simultaneously with both), the simple plural involves a single flexing which spans the object group while the hand arcs across it, and the individuated plural involves multiple rapid flexings while the hand arcs. If the singular verb uses reduplication, that is lost in the dual and plural forms.

Name signs

There are three types of personal name signs in ASL: fingerspelled, arbitrary, and descriptive. Fingerspelled names are simply spelled out letter-by-letter. Arbitrary name signs only refer to a person's name, while descriptive name signs refer to a person's personality or physical characteristics.[14] Once given, names are for life, apart from changing from one of the latter types to an arbitrary sign in childhood.[citation needed][15] Name signs are usually assigned by another member of the Deaf community, and signal inclusion in that community. Name signs are not used to address people, as names are in English, but are used only for third-person reference, and usually only when the person is absent.[16]

The majority of people, probably well in excess of 90%, have arbitrary name signs. These are initialized signs: The hand shape is the initial of one of the English names of the person, usually the first.[17] The sign may occur in neutral space, with a tremble; with a double-tap (as a noun) at one of a limited number of specific locations, such as the side of the chin, the temple, or the elbow;[18] or moving across a location or between two locations, with a single tap at each.[19] Single-location signs are simpler in connotation, like English "Vee"; double-location signs are fancier, like English "Veronica". Sam Supalla (1992) collected 525 simple arbitrary name signs like these.

There are two constraints on arbitrary signs. First, it should not mean anything. That is, it should not duplicate an existing ASL word.[20] Second, there should not be more than one person with the name sign in the local community. If a person moves to a new community where someone already has their name sign, then the newcomer is obligated to modify theirs[dubious ]. This is usually accomplished by compounding the hand shape, so that the first tap of the sign takes the initial of the person's first English name, and the second tap takes the initial of their last name. There are potentially thousands of such compound-initial signs.

Descriptive name signs are not initialized, but rather use non-core ASL signs. They tend to be assigned and used by children, rather like "Blinky" in English. Parents do not give such names to their children, but most Deaf people do not have deaf parents and are assigned their name sign by classmates in their first school for the deaf. At most 10% of Deaf people retain such name signs into adulthood.[citation needed]. Arbitrary name signs became established very early in the history of ASL. Descriptive name signs refer to a person's appearance or personality.[citation needed]

The two systems, arbitrary and descriptive, are sometimes combined, usually for humorous purposes. Hearing people learning ASL are also often assigned combined name signs. This is not traditional for Deaf people. Sometimes people with very short English names, such as "Ann" or "Lee", or ones that flow easily, such as "Larry", may never acquire a name sign, but may instead be referred to with finger-spelling.

Word order

Default word order is SVO

ASL is a subject-verb-object (SVO) language.[21]

Topic-comment order

The default SVO word order is sometimes altered by processes including topicalization and null elements;[22] this is marked either with non-manual signals like eyebrow or body position, or with prosodic marking such as pausing.[21] These non-manual grammatical markings (such as eyebrow movement or head-shaking) may optionally spread over the c-command domain of the node which it is attached to.[23] However, ASL is a pro-drop language, and when the manual sign that a non-manual grammatical marking is attached to is omitted, the non-manual marking obligatorily spreads over the c-command domain.[24]

The full sentence structure in ASL is [topic] [subject] verb [object] [subject-pronoun-tag]. Topics and tags are both indicated with non-manual features, and both give a great deal of flexibility to ASL word order.[25] Within a noun phrase, the word order is noun-number and noun-adjective.

ASL does not have a copula (linking 'to be' verb).[26] For example:

MY

HAIR

WET

MY HAIR WET

my hair is wet

[name

my]TOPIC

P-E-T-E

[name my]TOPIC P-E-T-E

my name is Pete

Noun-adjective order

In addition to its basic topic–comment structure, ASL typically places an adjective after a noun, though it may occur before the noun for stylistic purposes. Numerals also occur after the noun, a very rare pattern among oral languages.

DOG

BROWN

I

HAVE

DOG BROWN I HAVE

I have a brown dog.

Adverbs, however, occur before the verbs. Most of the time adverbs are simply the same sign as an adjective, distinguished by the context of the sentence.

HOUSE

I

QUIET

ENTER

HOUSE I QUIET ENTER

I enter the house quietly.

When the scope of the adverb is the entire clause, as in the case of time, it comes before the topic. This is the only thing which can appear before the topic in ASL: time–topic–comment.

9-HOUR

MORNING

STORE

I

GO

9-HOUR MORNING STORE I GO

I'm going to the store at 9:00AM.

Modal verbs come after the main verb of the clause:

FOR

YOU,

STORE

I

GO

CAN

FOR YOU, STORE I GO CAN

I can go to the store for you.

Time-sequenced clause ordering

ASL makes heavy use of time-sequenced ordering, meaning that events are signed in the order in which they occur. For example, for I was late to class last night because my boss handed me a huge stack of work after lunch yesterday, one would sign 'YESTERDAY LUNCH FINISH, BOSS GIVE-me WORK BIG-STACK, NIGHT CLASS LATE-me'. In stories, however, ordering is malleable, since one can choose to sequence the events either in the order in which they occurred or in the order in which one found out about them.

Tense and aspect

It has been claimed that tense in ASL is marked adverbially, and that ASL lacks a separate category of tense markers.[27] However, Aarons et al. (1992, 1995) argue that "Tense" (T) is indeed a distinct category of syntactic head, and that the T node can be occupied either by a modal (e.g. SHOULD) or a lexical tense marker (e.g. FUTURE-TENSE).[27] They support this claim by noting that only one such item can occupy the T slot:[28]

REUBEN

CAN

RENT

VIDEO-TAPE

REUBEN CAN RENT VIDEO-TAPE

'Reuben can rent a video tape.'

REUBEN

WILL

RENT

VIDEO-TAPE

REUBEN WILL RENT VIDEO-TAPE

'Reuben will rent a video tape.'

*

REUBEN

CAN

WILL

RENT

VIDEO-TAPE

* REUBEN CAN WILL RENT VIDEO-TAPE

* 'Reuben can will rent a video tape.'

Aspect may be marked either by verbal inflection or by separate lexical items.[29]

These are ordered: Tense – Negation – Aspect – Verb:[30]

                   neg (non-manual negation marker)
GINGER SHOULD NOT EAT BEEF
'Ginger should not eat beef.'
                                              neg
DAVE NOT FINISH SEE MOVIE
'Dave did not see (to completion) the movie.'

Aspect, topics, and transitivity

As noted above, in ASL aspectually marked verbs cannot take objects. To deal with this, the object must be known from context so that it does not need to be further specified. This is accomplished in two ways:

  1. The object may be made prominent in a prior clause, or
  2. It may be used as the topic of the utterance at hand.

Of these two strategies, the first is the more common. For my friend was typing her term paper all night to be used with a durative aspect, this would result in

my friend type T-E-R-M paper. typeDURATIVE all-night

The less colloquial topic construction may come out as,

[my friend]TOPIC, [T-E-R-M paper]TOPIC, typeDURATIVE all-night

CP Syntax

Topics and main clauses

A topic sets off background information that will be discussed in the following main clause. Topic constructions are not often used in standard English, but they are common in some dialects, as in,

That dog, I never could hunt him.

Topicalization is used productively in ASL and often results in surface forms that do not follow the basic SVO word order.[31] In order to non-manually mark topics, the eyebrows are raised and the head is tilted back during the production of a topic. The head is often lowered toward the end of the sign, and sometimes the sign is followed rapidly nodding the head. A slight pause follows the topic, setting it off from the rest of the sentence:[32]

[MEAT]tm,

I

LIKE

LAMB

[MEAT]tm, I LIKE LAMB

As for meat, I prefer lamb.

Another way topics may be signed is by shifting the body. The signer may use the space on one side of his/her body to sign the topic, and then shifts to the other side for the rest of the sentence.[32]

ASL utterances do not require topics, but their use is extremely common. They are used for purposes of information flow, to set up referent loci (see above), and to supply objects for verbs which are grammatically prevented from taking objects themselves (see below).

Without a topic, the dog chased my cat is signed:

DOG

CHASE

MY

CAT

DOG CHASE MY CAT

The dog chased my cat

However, people tend to want to set up the object of their concern first and then discuss what happened to it. English does this with passive clauses: my cat was chased by the dog. In ASL, topics are used with similar effect:

[MY

CAT]tm

DOG

CHASE

[MY CAT]tm DOG CHASE

lit. 'my cat, the dog chased it.'

If the word order of the main clause is changed, the meaning of the utterance also changes:

[MY

CAT]tm

CHASE

DOG

[MY CAT]tm CHASE DOG

'my cat chased the dog,'
lit. 'my cat, it chased the dog.'

There are three types of non-manual topic markers, all of which involve raised eyebrows.[33] The three types of non-manual topic markers are used with different types of topics and in different contexts, and the topic markings cannot spread over other elements in the utterance. Topics can be moved from and remain null in the main clause of an utterance, or topics can be base-generated and either be co-referential to either the subject or object in the main clause or be related to the subject of object by a semantic property.[34]

The first type of non-manual marking, topic marking 1 (tm1), is only used with a moved topic.[35] Tm1 is characterized by raised eyebrows, widened eyes, and head tilted backwards. At the end of the sign the head moves down and there is a pause, often with an eye blink, before the sentence is continued.[36] The following is an example of a context in which the tm1 marking is used:

[MARY]tm1

JOHN

LOVE

[MARY]tm1 JOHN LOVE

'Mary, John loves,' or 'John loves Mary'[37]

Topic marking 2 (tm2) and topic marking 3 (tm3) are both used with base-generated topics. Tm2 is characterized by raised eyebrows, widened eyes, and the head tilted backwards and to the side. Toward the end of the sign the head moves forward and to the opposite side, and there is a pause and often an eye blink before continuing.[38] For tm3 the eyebrows are raised and the eyes are opened wide, the head starts tilted down and jerks up and down, the lips are opened and raised, and the head is nodded rapidly a few times before pausing and continuing the sentence. Although both tm2 and tm3 accompany base-generated topics, they are used in different contexts. Tm2 is used to introduce new information and change the topic of a conversation to something that the signer is going to subsequently characterize, while tm3 is used to introduce new information that the signer believes is already known by his/her interlocutor.[39] Tm2 may be used with any base-generated topic, whereas only topics that are co-referential with an argument in the sentence may be marked with tm3.[40]

An example of a tm2 marking used with a topic related to the object of the main clause is:

[VEGETABLE]tm2,

JOHN

LIKE

CORN

[VEGETABLE]tm2, JOHN LIKE CORN

'As for vegetables, John likes corn.'[38]

An example of a tm2 marking used with a co-referential topic is:

[FRESH

VEGETABLE]tm3,

JOHN

LIKE

IX-3rd

[FRESH VEGETABLE]tm3, JOHN LIKE IX-3rd

'As for fresh vegetables, John likes them.'[41]

IX-3rd represents a 3rd person index.

Another example of a tm2 marking with a co-referential topic is:

[JOHNi]tm2,

IX-3rdi

LOVE

MARY

[JOHNi]tm2, IX-3rdi LOVE MARY

'as for John, he loves Mary'[41]

An example of a tm3 topic marking is:

[JOHNi]tm3,

IX-3rdi

LOVE

MARY

[JOHNi]tm3, IX-3rdi LOVE MARY

'(you know) John, he loves Mary'[42]

ASL sentences may have up to two marked topics.[33] Possible combinations of topic types are two tm2 topics, two tm3 topics, tm2 preceding tm1, tm3 preceding tm1, and tm2 preceding tm3. Sentences with these topic combinations in the opposite orders or with two tm1 topics are considered ungrammatical by native signers.[43]

Relative clauses

Relative clauses are signaled by tilting back the head and raising the eyebrows and upper lip. This is done during the performance of the entire clause. There is no change in word order. For example:

[recently

dog

chase

cat]RELATIVE

come

home

[recently dog chase cat]RELATIVE come home

The dog which recently chased the cat came home

where the brackets here indicate the duration of the non-manual features. If the sign 'recently' were made without these features, it would lie outside the relative clause, and the meaning would change to "the dog which chased the cat recently came home".

Negated clauses

Negated clauses may be signaled by shaking the head during the entire clause. A topic, however, cannot be so negated; the headshake can only be produced during the production of the main clause. (A second type of negation starts with the verb and continues to the end of the clause.)

In addition, in many communities, negation is put at the end of the clause, unless there is a wh- question word. For example, the sentence, "I thought the movie was not good," could be signed as, "BEFORE MOVIE ME SEE, THINK WHAT? IT GOOD NOT."

There are two manual signs that negate a sentence, NOT and NONE, which are accompanied by a shake of the head. NONE is typically used when talking about possession:

DOG

I

HAVE

NONE

DOG I HAVE NONE

I don't have any dogs.

NOT negates a verb:

TENNIS

I

LIKE

PLAY

NOT

TENNIS I LIKE PLAY NOT

I don't like to play tennis.

Interrogative clauses

There are three types of questions with different constructions in ASL: wh- questions, yes/no questions, and rhetorical questions.[44]

Non-manual grammatical markings

Non-manual grammatical markings are grammatical and semantic features that do not include the use of hands.  They can include mouth shape, eye gazes, facial expressions, body shifting, head tilting, and eyebrow raising.  Non -manual grammatical markings can also aid in identifying sentence type, which is especially relevant to our discussion of different types of interrogatives.[45]

Wh-questions

Wh-questions can be formed in a variety of ways in ASL. The wh-word can appear solely at the end of the sentence, solely at the beginning of the sentence, at both the beginning and end of the sentence (see section 4.4.2.1 on 'double-occurring wh-words', or in situ (i.e. where the wh-word is in the sentence structure before movement occurs)).[46] Manual wh-signs are also accompanied by a non-manual grammatical marking (see section 4.4.1), which can include a variety of features.[47] This non-manual grammatical marking can spread optionally over the entire wh-phrase or just a small part.

Some languages have very few wh-words, where context and discourse are sufficient to elicit the information that one needs. ASL has many different wh-words, with certain wh-words having multiple variations. A list of the wh-words of ASL can be found below. WHAT, WHAT-DO, WHAT-FOR, WHAT-PU, WHAT- FS, WHEN, WHERE, WHICH, WHO (several variations), WHY, HOW, HOW-MANY[48]

Double-occurring wh-words

As mentioned above, ASL possesses wh-questions with word initial placement, word final placement, in situ structure, but the most unique style of wh-word occurrence in ASL is where the wh-word occurs twice, copied in final position.[49] This doubling can be seen in the table below.

WHAT JOHN BUY WHAT
'what did John buy'

This doubling provides a useful template to analyze 2 separate analyses about whether wh-words move rightward or leftward in ASL. While some researchers argue for rightward movement in wh- questions such as Aarons and Neidle,[50] others, including Petronio and Lillo-Martin, have argued that ASL has leftward movement and wh- words that appear to the right of the clause move by other processes.[51] Both analyses agree upon the fact that there is wh-movement present in these interrogative phrases, but it is a matter of what direction the wh-movement is moving in that causes controversy. No matter what direction the wh-movement is analyzed to go in, it is crucial to the analysis that the movement of the wh-element is to the position of SPEC CP[52]

 
Leftward Wh-movement Analysis in American Sign Language
Lillo-Martin & Fischer's, and Petronio's leftward wh-movement analysis

Summary of the leftward wh-movement analysis in American Sign Language:

The leftward movement analysis is congruent with cross linguistic data that wh-movement is always leftward.  It can be seen as the less controversial of the 2 proposals.  The main arguments presented by the Leftward Wh-movement analysis are: That the spec-CP is on the left, that the wh-movement is leftward, and that the final wh-word in a sentence is a base-generated double.  This is illustrated in the syntax tree located to the right of this paragraph.[53]  Arguments for leftward movement are based on the facts that if wh-movement in ASL were rightward, ASL would be an exception to cross-linguistic generalizations that wh-movement is leftward.[51]

It has also been hypothesized that wh-elements cannot be topicalized, as topicalized elements must be presupposed and interrogatives are not.[54]  This would be detrimental to the rightward analysis, as they are analyzing the doubled wh-word as a 'base generated topic'.  

Aarons et al.'s rightward wh-movement analysis

Summary of the rightward wh-movement analysis in American Sign Language

 
Rightward Wh-movement Analysis in American Sign Language

The rightward movement analysis is a newer, more abstract argument of how wh-movement occurs in ASL.  The main arguments for rightward movement begin by analyzing spec-CP as being on the right, the wh-movement as being rightward, and as the initial wh-word as a base-generated topic.[55] This can be seen in the syntax tree on the right.  

One of the rightward movement analysis' main arguments is in regards to the non-manual grammatical markings, and their optional spreading over the sentence.  In ASL the use of non-manual grammatical markings is optional depending on the type of wh-question being asked.  In the rightward analysis both partial and full spreading of non-manual grammatical markers can be accounted for due to the association with the +WH feature over its c-command domain.[56] In the leftward analysis, the partial or full spreading of non manual grammatical markings cannot be accounted for in this same way.  The leftward movement analysis requires wh-marking to extend over the entirety of the question, regardless (which is not what is attested in ASL).  

Yes/no questions

In spoken language Yes/no questions will oftentimes differ in their word order from the statement form. For example, in English:

English Statement:

HE WILL BUY THE SHIRT.

English Yes/no Q:

WILL HE BUY THE SHIRT?[57]

In ASL, yes/no questions are marked by the non-manual grammatical markings (as discussed in section 4.4.1).  This eyebrow raise, slight tilt of the head and lean forward are what indicate that a yes/no question is being asked, without any change in word order from the statement form. There is speculation amongst linguists that these non-manual grammatical markings that indicate a yes/no questions are similar to the question intonation of spoken languages.[58]

Yes/no questions differ from wh-questions as they do not differ in word order from the original statement form of the sentence, whereas wh-questions do.  As well, in yes/no questions, the non-manual marking must be used over the whole utterance in order for it to be judged as a statement opposed to a question.[59] The yes/no question is the same word order as the statement form of the sentence, with the addition of non-manual grammatical markings. This can be seen in the examples below.

ASL Statement:

JUAN WILL BUY SHOES TODAY "Juan will buy shoes today"

ASL Yes/no Question:

_____________________brow raise

JUAN WILL BUY SHOES TODAY

"Will Juan buy shoes today?"[60]

Rhetorical questions

Non-manual grammatical markings are also used for rhetorical questions, which are questions that do not intend to elicit an answer. To distinguish the non-manual marking for rhetorical questions from that of yes/no questions, the body is in a neutral position opposed to tilted forward, and the head is tilted in a different way than in yes/no questions.[61] Rhetorical questions are much more common in ASL than in English. For example, in ASL:

[I

LIKE]NEGATIVE

[WHAT?]RHETORICAL,

GARLIC.

[I LIKE]NEGATIVE [WHAT?]RHETORICAL, GARLIC.

"I don't like garlic"

This strategy is commonly used instead of signing the word 'because' for clarity or emphasis. For instance:

PASTA

I

EAT

ENJOY

TRUE

[WHY?]RHETORICAL,

ITALIAN

I.

PASTA I EAT ENJOY TRUE [WHY?]RHETORICAL, ITALIAN I.

"I love to eat pasta because I am Italian"

DP syntax

Subject pronoun tags

Information may also be added after the main clause as a kind of 'afterthought'. In ASL this is commonly seen with subject pronouns. These are accompanied by a nod of the head, and make a statement more emphatic:

boy

fall

boy fall

"The boy fell down."

versus

boy

fall

[he]TAG

boy fall [he]TAG

"The boy fell down, he did."

The subject need not be mentioned, as in

fall

fall

"He fell down."

versus

fall

[he]TAG

fall [he]TAG

"He fell down, he did."

Deixis

In ASL signers set up regions of space (loci) for specific referents (see above); these can then be referred to indexically by pointing at those locations with pronouns and indexical verbs.

Pronouns

Personal pronouns in ASL are indexic. That is, they point to their referent, or to a locus representing their referent. When the referent is physically present, pronouns involve simply pointing at the referent, with different handshapes for different pronominal uses: A 'G' handshape is a personal pronoun, an extended 'B' handshape with an outward palm orientation is a possessive pronoun, and an extended-thumb 'A' handshape is a reflexive pronoun; these may be combined with numeral signs to sign 'you two', 'us three', 'all of them', etc.

If the referent is not physically present, the speaker identifies the referent and then points to a location (the locus) in the sign space near their body. This locus can then be pointed at to refer to the referent. Theoretically, any number of loci may be set up, as long as the signer and recipient remember them all, but in practice, no more than eight loci are used.

Meier 1990 demonstrates that only two grammatical persons are distinguished in ASL: First person and non-first person, as in Damin. Both persons come in several numbers as well as with signs such as 'my' and 'by myself'.

Meier provides several arguments for believing that ASL does not formally distinguish second from third person. For example, when pointing to a person that is physically present, a pronoun is equivalent to either 'you' or '(s)he' depending on the discourse. There is nothing in the sign itself, nor in the direction of eye gaze or body posture, that can be relied on to make this distinction. That is, the same formal sign can refer to any of several second or third persons, which the indexic nature of the pronoun makes clear. In English, indexic uses also occur, as in 'I need you to go to the store and you to stay here', but not so ubiquitously. In contrast, several first-person ASL pronouns, such as the plural possessive ('our'), look different from their non-first-person equivalents, and a couple of pronouns do not occur in the first person at all, so first and non-first persons are formally distinct.

Personal pronouns have separate forms for singular ('I' and 'you/(s)he') and plural ('we' and 'you/they'). These have possessive counterparts: 'my', 'our', 'your/his/her', 'your/their'. In addition, there are pronoun forms which incorporate numerals from two to five ('the three of us', 'the four of you/them', etc.), though the dual pronouns are slightly idiosyncratic in form (i.e., they have a K rather than 2 handshape, and the wrist nods rather than circles). These numeral-incorporated pronouns have no possessive equivalents.

Also among the personal pronouns are the 'self' forms ('by myself', 'by your/themselves', etc.). These only occur in the singular and plural (there is no numeral incorporation), and are only found as subjects. They have derived emphatic and 'characterizing' forms, with modifications used for derivation rather like those for verbal aspect. The 'characterizing' pronoun is used when describing someone who has just been mentioned. It only occurs as a non-first-person singular form.

Finally, there are formal pronouns used for honored guests. These occur as singular and plural in the non-first person, but only as singular in the first person.

ASL is a pro-drop language, which means that pronouns are not used when the referent is obvious from context and is not being emphasized.

Indexical verbs

Within ASL there is a class of indexical (often called 'directional') verbs. These include the signs for 'see', 'pay', 'give', 'show', 'invite', 'help', 'send', 'bite', etc. These verbs include an element of motion that indexes one or more referents, either physically present or set up through the referent locus system. If there are two loci, the first indicates the subject and the second the object, direct or indirect depending on the verb, reflecting the basic word order of ASL. For example, 'give' is a bi-indexical verb based on a flattened M/O handshape. For 'I give you', the hand moves from myself toward you; for 'you give me', it moves from you to me. 'See' is indicated with a V handshape. Two loci for a dog and a cat can be set up, with the sign moving between them to indicate 'the dog sees the cat' (if it starts at the locus for dog and moves toward the locus for cat) or 'the cat sees the dog' (with the motion in the opposite direction), or the V hand can circulate between both loci and myself to mean 'we (the dog, the cat, and myself) see each other'. The verb 'to be in pain' (index fingers pointed at each other and alternately approaching and separating) is signed at the location of the pain (head for headache, cheek for toothache, abdomen for stomachache, etc.). This is normally done in relation to the signer's own body, regardless of the person feeling the pain, but may take also use the locus system, especially for body parts which are not normally part of the sign space, such as the leg. There are also spatial verbs such as put-up and put-below, which allow signers to specify where things are or how they moved them around.

Conjunctions

There is no separate sign in ASL for the conjunction and. Instead, multiple sentences or phrases are combined with a short pause between. Often, lists are specified with a listing and ordering technique, a simple version of which is to show the length of the list first with the nondominant hand, then to describe each element after pointing to the nondominant finger that represents it.

English: I have three cats and they are named Billy, Bob, and Buddy.
ASL: CAT I HAVE THREE-LIST. NAME, FIRST-OF-THREE-LIST B-I-L-L-Y, SECOND-OF-THREE-LIST B-O-B, THIRD-OF-THREE-LIST B-U-D-D-Y.

There is a manual sign for the conjunction or, but the concept is usually signed nonmanually with a slight shoulder twist.

English: I'll leave at 5 or 6 o'clock.
ASL: I LEAVE TIME 5 [shoulder shift] TIME 6.

The manual sign for the conjunction but is similar to the sign for different. It is more likely to be used in Pidgin Signed English than in ASL. Instead, shoulder shifts can be used, similar to "or" with appropriate facial expression.

English: I like to swim, but I don't like to run.
ASL/PSE: SWIM I LIKE, BUT RUN I LIKE-NOT
ASL: SWIM I LIKE, [shoulder shift] RUN I LIKE-NOT

Notes

  1. ^ a b c Bahan (1996:20)
  2. ^ a b Bahan (1996:21)
  3. ^ a b Bahan (1996:21–22)
  4. ^ a b c Bahan (1996:22–23)
  5. ^ a b c Bahan (1996:23)
  6. ^ a b c Bahan (1996:24)
  7. ^ a b c Bahan (1996:25)
  8. ^ Bahan (1996:50)
  9. ^ Bahan (1996:50–51)
  10. ^ Aronoff, M., Meir, I., & Sandler, W. (2005). The paradox of sign language morphology. Language, 81(2), 301.
  11. ^ Supalla, T. R. (1982). Structure and acquisition of verbs of motion and location in American Sign Language (Doctoral dissertation, ProQuest Information & Learning).
  12. ^ Supalla, T. (1986). The classifier system in American sign language. Noun classes and categorization, 181–214.
  13. ^ Morford, Jill; MacFarlane, James (2003). "Frequency Characteristics of American Sign Language". Sign Language Studies. 3 (2): 213. doi:10.1353/sls.2003.0003. S2CID 6031673.
  14. ^ Supalla, Samuel J. (1990). "The Arbitrary Name Sign System in American Sign Language". Sign Language Studies. 1067 (1): 99–126. doi:10.1353/sls.1990.0006. ISSN 1533-6263. S2CID 144191789.
  15. ^ "Types and trends of name signs in the Swedish Sign Language community" (PDF).
  16. ^ Samuel J. Supalla (1992) The Book of Name Signs: Naming in American Sign Language.
  17. ^ The J hand shape is articulated with a brush of the pinkie finger against the sign location. It cannot occur in neutral space. There is no provision for Z: that is, there are no Z-initial arbitrary name signs.
  18. ^ Contrastive locations are limited to the temple, forehead, side of chin, chin, shoulder, chest, outside of elbow, inside of elbow, palm of a vertical flat hand, back of a horizontal back hand. Some name signs are distinguished by orientation. For example, an I hand shape may make contact with either the tip of the pinkie finger or the side of the thumb; the M hand shape with either the tips of the three fingers or the side of the index finger, etc.
  19. ^ The two locations are usually close by, such as the hand moving across the chin or down the chest, but may occasionally be further apart, as from the shoulder to the back of the hand.
  20. ^ There are occasional exceptions to this constraint. For example, I. King Jordan's name sign is homophonous with "king" (K hand moving from the shoulder to the hip).
  21. ^ a b Bahan (1996:30)
  22. ^ Pichler, Deborah Chen (2001). Word order variation and acquisition in American Sign Language. pp. 14–15.
  23. ^ Bahan (1996:31)
  24. ^ Neidle, Carol (2002). "Language across modalities". Linguistic Variation Yearbook. 2 (1): 71–98. doi:10.1075/livy.2.05nei.
  25. ^ Aarons, DebraAspects of the syntax of American Sign Language (1994). Aspects of the syntax of American Sign Language. p. 55.
  26. ^ Pichler, Deborah Chen (2001). Word order variation and acquisition in American Sign Language. p. 23.
  27. ^ a b Bahan (1996:33)
  28. ^ Bahan (1996:33–34)
  29. ^ Bahan (1996:27)
  30. ^ Bahan (1996:34–37)
  31. ^ Pichler, Deborah Chen (2001). Word order variation and acquisition in American Sign Language. p. 15.
  32. ^ a b Aarons, Debra (1994). Aspects of the syntax of American Sign Language. p. 70.
  33. ^ a b Bahan (1996:41–42)
  34. ^ Aarons, Debra (1994). Aspects of the syntax of American Sign Language. pp. 151–153.
  35. ^ Aarons, Debra (1994). Aspects of the syntax of American Sign Language. p. 155.
  36. ^ Aarons, Debra (1994). Aspects of the syntax of American Sign Language. pp. 156–157.
  37. ^ Aarons, Debra (1994). Aspects of the syntax of American Sign Language. p. 157.
  38. ^ a b Aarons, Debra (1994). Aspects of the syntax of American Sign Language. p. 160.
  39. ^ Aarons, Debra (1994). Aspects of the syntax of American Sign Language. pp. 163–165.
  40. ^ Pichler, Deborah Chen (2001). Word order variation and acquisition in American Sign Language. pp. 28–29.
  41. ^ a b Aarons, Debra (1994). Aspects of the syntax of American Sign Language. p. 162.
  42. ^ Aarons, Debra (1994). Aspects of the Syntax of American Sign Language. p. 165.
  43. ^ Aarons, Debra (1994). Aspects of the syntax of American Sign Language. pp. 177–181.
  44. ^ Aarons, Debra (1994). Aspects of the syntax of American Sign Language. pp. 67–69.
  45. ^ "Non-manual signals in sign language". www.handspeak.com. Retrieved April 21, 2022.
  46. ^ Petronio, Karen; Diane Lillo-Martin (1997). "Wh-Movement and the Position of Spec-CP: Evidence from American Sign Language" (PDF). Linguistic Society of America. 73 (1): 18–57. doi:10.2307/416592. JSTOR 416592.
  47. ^ Aarons, Debra (1994). Aspects of the syntax of American Sign Language. p. 69.
  48. ^ Joseph Christopher Hill; Diane C. Lillo-Martin; Sandra K. Wood (2019). Sign languages: structures and contexts. London. p. 65. ISBN 978-0-429-02087-2. OCLC 1078875378.
  49. ^ Josep Quer; Roland Pfau; Annika Herrmann (2021). The Routledge handbook of theoretical and experimental sign language research. Abingdon, Oxon. p. 234. ISBN 978-1-317-62427-1. OCLC 1182020388.
  50. ^ Neidle, Carol (2002). "Language across modalities: ASL focus and question constructions". Linguistic Variation Yearbook. 2 (1): 71–98. doi:10.1075/livy.2.05nei.
  51. ^ a b Petronio, Karen; Lillo-Martin, Diane (1997). "WH-Movement and the Position of Spec-CP: Evidence from American Sign Language". Language. 73 (1): 18–57. doi:10.2307/416592. ISSN 0097-8507. JSTOR 416592.
  52. ^ Petronio, Karen; Lillo-Martin, Diane (March 1997). "WH-Movement and the Position of Spec-CP: Evidence from American Sign Language". Language. 73 (1): 21. doi:10.2307/416592. JSTOR 416592.
  53. ^ Josep Quer; Roland Pfau; Annika Herrmann (2021). The Routledge handbook of theoretical and experimental sign language research. Abingdon, Oxon. p. 235. ISBN 978-1-317-62427-1. OCLC 1182020388.
  54. ^ Petronio, Karen; Lillo-Martin, Diane (March 1997). "WH-Movement and the Position of Spec-CP: Evidence from American Sign Language". Language. 73 (1): 22. doi:10.2307/416592. JSTOR 416592.
  55. ^ Petronio, Karen; Lillo-Martin, Diane (March 1997). "WH-Movement and the Position of Spec-CP: Evidence from American Sign Language". Language. 73 (1): 18. doi:10.2307/416592. ISSN 0097-8507. JSTOR 416592.
  56. ^ Neidle, Carol; MacLaughlin, Dawn; Lee, Robert G.; Bahan, Benjamin; Kegl, Judy (1998). "The Rightward Analysis of wh-Movement in ASL: A Reply to Petronio and Lillo-Martin". Language. 74 (4): 823–825. doi:10.2307/417004. ISSN 0097-8507. JSTOR 417004.
  57. ^ Baker, Anne; et al. (2016). The Linguistics of Sign Languages: An Introduction. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company. p. 130. ISBN 9789027212306.
  58. ^ Baker, Anne (July 8, 2016). The linguistics of sign languages : an introduction. Amsterdam. p. 131. ISBN 978-90-272-6734-4. OCLC 936433607.
  59. ^ Aarons, Debra (1994). Aspects of the syntax of American Sign Language. p. 92.
  60. ^ Hill, Joseph C.; Lillo-Martin, Diane C.; Wood, Sandra K. (2018), "Syntax", Sign Languages, New York: Routledge, pp. 55–81, doi:10.4324/9780429020872-4, ISBN 978-0-429-02087-2, S2CID 239556102, retrieved April 18, 2022
  61. ^ Aarons, Debra (1994). Aspects of the syntax of American Sign Language. p. 68.

References

  • Aarons, Debra (1994). Aspects of the syntax of American Sign Language (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Boston University, Boston, MA.
  • Bahan, Benjamin (1996). Non-Manual Realization of Agreement in American Sign Language (PDF). Boston University. (PDF) from the original on October 11, 2017. Retrieved November 25, 2012.
  • Klima, Edward & Bellugi, Ursula (1979). The Signs of Language. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. ISBN 0-674-80795-2.
  • Liddell, Scott K. (2003). Grammar, Gesture, and Meaning in American Sign Language. Cambridge University Press.
  • Neidle, Carol (2002). Language across Modalities: ASL focus and question constructions. Linguistic Variation Yearbook, 2(1), 71–98.
  • Petronio, Karen, & Lillo-Martin, Diane (1997). WH-Movement and the Position of Spec-CP: Evidence from American Sign Language. Language, 73(1), 18–57.
  • Pichler, Debora Chen (2001). Word order variation and acquisition in American Sign Language (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Connecticut.
  • Stokoe, William C. (1976). Dictionary of American Sign Language on Linguistic Principles. Linstok Press. ISBN 0-932130-01-1.
  • Stokoe, William C. (1960). Sign language structure: An outline of the visual communication systems of the American deaf. Studies in linguistics: Occasional papers (No. 8). Buffalo: Dept. of Anthropology and Linguistics, University of Buffalo.

Further reading

  • Signing Naturally by Ken Mikos
  • The Syntax of American Sign Language: Functional Categories and Hierarchical Structure by Carol Jan Neidle
  • Grammar, Gesture, and Meaning in American Sign Language by Scott K. Liddell
  • Linguistics of American Sign Language: An Introduction, 4th Ed. by Clayton Valli

american, sign, language, grammar, this, article, multiple, issues, please, help, improve, discuss, these, issues, talk, page, learn, when, remove, these, template, messages, this, article, needs, additional, citations, verification, please, help, improve, thi. This article has multiple issues Please help improve it or discuss these issues on the talk page Learn how and when to remove these template messages This article needs additional citations for verification Please help improve this article by adding citations to reliable sources Unsourced material may be challenged and removed Find sources American Sign Language grammar news newspapers books scholar JSTOR August 2009 Learn how and when to remove this template message This article s lead section may be too short to adequately summarize the key points Please consider expanding the lead to provide an accessible overview of all important aspects of the article July 2019 Learn how and when to remove this template message The grammar of American Sign Language ASL is the best studied of any sign language though research is still in its infancy dating back only to William Stokoe in the 1960s citation needed Contents 1 Morphology 1 1 Derivation 1 2 Degree 1 3 Reduplication 1 4 Compounds 1 5 Affixes 1 6 Numeral incorporation and classifiers 1 7 Frames 1 8 Verbal aspect 1 9 Verbal number 1 10 Name signs 2 Word order 2 1 Default word order is SVO 2 2 Topic comment order 2 3 Noun adjective order 2 4 Time sequenced clause ordering 3 Tense and aspect 3 1 Aspect topics and transitivity 4 CP Syntax 4 1 Topics and main clauses 4 2 Relative clauses 4 3 Negated clauses 4 4 Interrogative clauses 4 4 1 Non manual grammatical markings 4 4 2 Wh questions 4 4 2 1 Double occurring wh words 4 4 2 2 Lillo Martin amp Fischer s and Petronio s leftward wh movement analysis 4 4 2 3 Aarons et al s rightward wh movement analysis 4 4 3 Yes no questions 4 4 4 Rhetorical questions 5 DP syntax 5 1 Subject pronoun tags 5 2 Deixis 5 2 1 Pronouns 5 2 2 Indexical verbs 6 Conjunctions 7 Notes 8 References 9 Further readingMorphology EditASL morphology is to a large extent iconic dubious discuss citation needed This shows up especially well in reduplication and indexicality Derivation Edit Compounding is used to derive new words in ASL which often differ in meaning from their constituent signs 1 For example the signs FACE and STRONG compound to create a new sign FACE STRONG meaning to resemble 1 Compounds undergo the phonetic process of hold deletion whereby the holds at the end of the first constituent and the beginning of the second are elided 1 ASL compounding 2 Individual signs Compound signFACE STRONG FACE STRONGMH HMH MMHMany ASL nouns are derived from verbs 2 This may be done either by reduplicating the movement of the verb if the verb has a single movement or by restraining making smaller and faster the movement of the verb if it already has repeated movement 3 For example the noun CHAIR is derived from the verb SIT through reduplication 3 Another productive method is available for deriving nouns from non stative verbs 4 This form of derivation modifies the verb s movement reduplicating it in a trilled manner small quick stiff movements 4 For example this method is used to derive the noun ACTION from the verb ACT 4 Characteristic adjectives which refer to inherent states may be derived from adjectives which refer to incidental or temporary states 5 Characteristic adjectives always use both hands even if the source adjective only uses one and they always have repeated circular movement 5 Additionally if the source adjective was one handed the derived adjective has alternating movement 5 Trilling may also be used productively to derive adjectives with an ish meaning e g BLUE becomes BLUISH 6 ASL occasionally uses suffixation in derivation but less often than in English 6 Agent nouns may be derived from verbs by adding the suffix AGENT and deleting the final hold of the verb e g TEACH AGENT teacher 6 Superlatives are also formed by suffixation e g SMART MOST smartest 7 Certain types of signs for example those relating to time and age may incorporate numbers by assimilating their handshape 7 For example the word WEEK has handshape B with the weak hand and 1 with the active hand the active hand s handshape may be changed to the handshape of any number up to 9 to indicate that many weeks 7 There are about 20 non manual modifiers in ASL which are either adjectival or adverbial 8 For example the adverb th realized as the tongue being placed between the teeth means carelessly lazily when combined with a verb 9 JOHNWRITELETTERJOHN WRITE LETTER John writes a letter JOHN WRITE thLETTER JOHN WRITE LETTER th John writes a letter carelessly Degree Edit Mouthing is when an individual appears to be making speech sounds and this is very important for fluent signing It also has specific morphological uses For example one may sign man tall to indicate the man is tall but by mouthing the syllable cha while signing tall the phrase becomes that man is enormous There are other ways of modifying a verb or adjective to make it more intense These are all more or less equivalent to adding the word very in English which morphology is used depends on the word being modified Certain words which are short in English such as sad and mad are sometimes fingerspelled rather than signed to mean very sad and very mad However the concept of very sad or very mad can be portrayed with the use of exaggerated body movements and facial expressions Reduplication of the signs may also occur to emphasize the degree of the statement Some signs are produced with an exaggeratedly large motion so that they take up more sign space than normal This may involve a back and forth scissoring motion of the arms to indicate that the sign ought to be yet larger but that one is physically incapable of making it big enough Many other signs are given a slow tense production The fact that this modulation is morphological rather than merely mimetic can be seen in the sign for fast both very slow and very fast are signed by making the motion either unusually slowly or unusually quickly than it is in the citation forms of slow and fast not exclusively by making it slower for very slow and faster for very fast Reduplication Edit Reduplication is morphological repetition and this is extremely common in ASL Generally the motion of the sign is shortened as well as repeated Nouns may be derived from verbs through reduplication For example the noun chair is formed from the verb to sit by repeating it with a reduced degree of motion Similar relationships exist between acquisition and to get airplane and to fly on an airplane also window and to open close a window Reduplication is commonly used to express intensity as well as several verbal aspects see below It is also used to derive signs such as every two weeks from two weeks and is used for verbal number see below where the reduplication is iconic for the repetitive meaning of the sign Compounds Edit Many ASL words are historically compounds However the two elements of these signs have fused with features being lost from one or both to create what might be better called a blend than a compound Typically only the final hold see above remains from the first element and any reduplication is lost from the second An example is the verb AGREE which derives from the two signs THINK and ALIKE The verb THINK is signed by bringing a 1 hand inward and touching the forehead a move and a hold ALIKE is signed by holding two 1 hands parallel pointing outward and bringing them together two or three times The compound blend AGREE starts as THINK ends with the index finger touching the forehead the final hold of that sign In addition the weak hand is already in place in anticipation of the next part of the sign Then the hand at the forehead is brought down parallel to the weak hand it approaches but does not make actual contact and there is no repetition Affixes Edit ASL like other mature signed languages makes extensive use of morphology 10 Many of ASL s affixes are combined simultaneously rather than sequentially For example Ted Supalla s seminal work on ASL verbs of motion revealed that these signs consist of many different affixes articulated simultaneously according to complex grammatical constraints 11 This differs from the concatenative morphology of many spoken languages which except for suprasegmental features such as tone are tightly constrained by the sequential nature of voice sounds ASL does have a limited number of concatenative affixes For example the agentive suffix similar to the English er is made by placing two B or 5 hands in front of the torso palms facing each other and lowering them On its own this sign means person in a compound sign following a verb it is a suffix for the performer of the action as in drive er and teach er However it cannot generally be used to translate English er as it is used with a much more limited set of verbs It is very similar to the ulo suffix in Esperanto meaning person by itself and related person when combined with other words An ASL prefix touching the chin is used with number signs to indicate years old The prefix completely assimilates with the initial handshape of the number For instance fourteen is signed with a B hand that bends several times at the knuckles The chin touch prefix in fourteen years old is thus also made with a B hand For three years old however the prefix is made with a 3 hand Numeral incorporation and classifiers Edit Rather than relying on sequential affixes ASL makes heavy use of simultaneous modification of signs One example of this is found in the aspectual system see below another is numeral incorporation There are several families of two handed signs which require one of the hands to take the handshape of a numeral Many of these deal with time For example drawing the dominant hand lengthwise across the palm and fingers of a flat B hand indicates a number of weeks the dominant hand takes the form of a numeral from one to nine to specify how many weeks There are analogous signs for weeks ago and weeks from now etc though in practice several of these signs are only found with the lower numerals ASL also has a system of classifiers which may be incorporated into signs 12 A fist may represent an inactive object such as a rock this is the default or neutral classifier a horizontal ILY hand may represent an aircraft a horizontal 3 hand thumb pointing up and slightly forward a motor vehicle an upright G hand a person on foot an upright V hand a pair of people on foot and so on through higher numbers of people These classifiers are moved through sign space to iconically represent the actions of their referents For example an ILY hand may lift off or land on a horizontal B hand to sign an aircraft taking off or landing a 3 hand may be brought down on a B hand to sign parking a car and a G hand may be brought toward a V hand to represent one person approaching two The frequency of classifier use depends greatly on genre occurring at a rate of 17 7 in narratives but only 1 1 in casual speech and 0 9 in formal speech 13 Frames Edit Frames are a morphological device that may be unique to sign languages Liddell 2004 They are incomplete sets of the features which make up signs and they combine with existing signs absorbing features from them to form a derived sign It is the frame which specifies the number and nature of segments in the resulting sign while the basic signs it combines with lose all but one or two of their original features One the WEEKLY frame consists of a simple downward movement It combines with the signs for the days of the week which then lose their inherent movement For example Monday consists of an M O hand made with a circling movement MondayWEEKLY that is on Mondays is therefore signed as an M O hand that drops downward but without the circling movement A similar ALL DAY frame a sideward pan combines with times of the day such as morning and afternoon which likewise keep their handshape and location but lose their original movement Numeral incorporation see above also uses frames However in ASL frames are most productively utilized for verbal aspect Verbal aspect Edit While there is no grammatical tense in ASL there are numerous verbal aspects These are produced by modulating the verb Through reduplication by placing the verb in an aspectual frame see above or with a combination of these means An example of an aspectual frame is the unrealized inceptive aspect just about to X illustrated here with the verb to tell To tell is an indexical directional verb where the index finger a G hand begins with a touch to the chin and then moves outward to point out the recipient of the telling To be just about to tell retains just the locus and the initial chin touch which now becomes the final hold of the sign all other features from the basic verb in this case the outward motion and pointing are dropped and replaced by features from the frame which are shared with the unrealized inceptive aspects of other verbs such as look at wash the dishes yell flirt etc These frame features are Eye gaze toward the locus which is no longer pointed at with the hand an open jaw and a hand or hands in the case of two hand verbs in front of the trunk which moves in an arc to the onset location of the basic verb in this case touching the chin while the trunk rotates and the signer inhales catching her breath during the final hold The hand shape throughout the sign is whichever is required by the final hold in this case a G hand The variety of aspects in ASL can be illustrated by the verb to be sick which involves the middle finger of the Y 8 hand touching the forehead and which can be modified by a large number of frames Several of these involve reduplication which may but need not be analyzed as part of the frame The appropriate non manual features are not described here stative to be sick is made with simple iterated contact typically with around four iterations This is the basic citation form of the verb inchoative to get sick to take sick is made with a single straight movement to contact and a hold of the finger on the forehead predisposional to be sickly to be prone to get sick is made with incomplete motion three even circular cycles without contact This aspect adds reduplication to verbs such as to look at which do not already contain repetition susceptative to get sick easily is made with a thrusting motion The onset is held then there is a brief tense thrust that is checked before actual contact can be made frequentative to be often sick is given a marcato articulation A regular beat with 4 6 iterations and marked onsets and holds susceptive and frequentative may be combined to mean to get sick easily and often Four brief thrusts on a marked steady beat without contact with the forehead protractive to be continuously sick is made with a long tense hold and no movement at all incessant to get sick incessantly has a reduplicated tremolo articulation A dozen tiny tense uneven iterations as rapid as possible and without contact durative to be sick for a long time is made with a reduplicated elliptical motion Three slow uneven cycles with a heavy downward brush of the forehead and an arching return iterative to get sick over and over again is made with three tense movements and slow returns to the onset position intensive to be very sick is given a single tense articulation A tense onset hold followed by a single very rapid motion to a long final hold resultative to become fully sick that is a complete change of health is made with an accelerando articulation A single elongated tense movement which starts slowly and heavily accelerating to a long final hold approximative to be sort of sick to be a little sick is made with a reduplicated lax articulation A spacially extremely reduced minimal movement involving a dozen iterations without contact semblitive to appear to be sick no description increasing to get more and more sick is made with the movements becoming more and more intense These modulations readily combine with each other to create yet finer distinctions Not all verbs take all aspects and the forms they do take will not necessarily be completely analogous to the verb illustrated here Conversely not all aspects are possible with this one verb Aspect is unusual in ASL in that transitive verbs derived for aspect lose their transitivity That is while you can sign dog chew bone for the dog chewed on a bone or she look at me for she looked at me you cannot do the same in the durative to mean the dog gnawed on the bone or she stared at me Instead you must use other strategies such as a topic construction see below to avoid having an object for the verb Verbal number Edit Reduplication is also used for expressing verbal number Verbal number indicates that the action of the verb is repeated in the case of ASL it is apparently limited to transitive verbs where the motion of the verb is either extended or repeated to cover multiple object or recipient loci Simple plurality of action can also be conveyed with reduplication but without indexing any object loci in fact such aspectual forms do not allow objects as noted above There are specific dual forms and for some signers trial forms as well as plurals With dual objects the motion of the verb may be made twice with one hand or simultaneously with both while with plurals the object loci may be taken as a group by using a single sweep of the signing hand while the verbal motion is being performed or individuated by iterating the move across the sweep For example to ask someone a question is signed by flexing the index finger of an upright G hand in the direction of that person the dual involves flexing it at both object loci sequentially with one hand or simultaneously with both the simple plural involves a single flexing which spans the object group while the hand arcs across it and the individuated plural involves multiple rapid flexings while the hand arcs If the singular verb uses reduplication that is lost in the dual and plural forms Name signs Edit There are three types of personal name signs in ASL fingerspelled arbitrary and descriptive Fingerspelled names are simply spelled out letter by letter Arbitrary name signs only refer to a person s name while descriptive name signs refer to a person s personality or physical characteristics 14 Once given names are for life apart from changing from one of the latter types to an arbitrary sign in childhood citation needed 15 Name signs are usually assigned by another member of the Deaf community and signal inclusion in that community Name signs are not used to address people as names are in English but are used only for third person reference and usually only when the person is absent 16 The majority of people probably well in excess of 90 have arbitrary name signs These are initialized signs The hand shape is the initial of one of the English names of the person usually the first 17 The sign may occur in neutral space with a tremble with a double tap as a noun at one of a limited number of specific locations such as the side of the chin the temple or the elbow 18 or moving across a location or between two locations with a single tap at each 19 Single location signs are simpler in connotation like English Vee double location signs are fancier like English Veronica Sam Supalla 1992 collected 525 simple arbitrary name signs like these There are two constraints on arbitrary signs First it should not mean anything That is it should not duplicate an existing ASL word 20 Second there should not be more than one person with the name sign in the local community If a person moves to a new community where someone already has their name sign then the newcomer is obligated to modify theirs dubious discuss This is usually accomplished by compounding the hand shape so that the first tap of the sign takes the initial of the person s first English name and the second tap takes the initial of their last name There are potentially thousands of such compound initial signs Descriptive name signs are not initialized but rather use non core ASL signs They tend to be assigned and used by children rather like Blinky in English Parents do not give such names to their children but most Deaf people do not have deaf parents and are assigned their name sign by classmates in their first school for the deaf At most 10 of Deaf people retain such name signs into adulthood citation needed Arbitrary name signs became established very early in the history of ASL Descriptive name signs refer to a person s appearance or personality citation needed The two systems arbitrary and descriptive are sometimes combined usually for humorous purposes Hearing people learning ASL are also often assigned combined name signs This is not traditional for Deaf people Sometimes people with very short English names such as Ann or Lee or ones that flow easily such as Larry may never acquire a name sign but may instead be referred to with finger spelling Word order EditDefault word order is SVO Edit ASL is a subject verb object SVO language 21 Topic comment order Edit The default SVO word order is sometimes altered by processes including topicalization and null elements 22 this is marked either with non manual signals like eyebrow or body position or with prosodic marking such as pausing 21 These non manual grammatical markings such as eyebrow movement or head shaking may optionally spread over the c command domain of the node which it is attached to 23 However ASL is a pro drop language and when the manual sign that a non manual grammatical marking is attached to is omitted the non manual marking obligatorily spreads over the c command domain 24 The full sentence structure in ASL is topic subject verb object subject pronoun tag Topics and tags are both indicated with non manual features and both give a great deal of flexibility to ASL word order 25 Within a noun phrase the word order is noun number and noun adjective ASL does not have a copula linking to be verb 26 For example MYHAIRWETMY HAIR WETmy hair is wet namemy TOPICP E T E name my TOPIC P E T Emy name is Pete Noun adjective order Edit In addition to its basic topic comment structure ASL typically places an adjective after a noun though it may occur before the noun for stylistic purposes Numerals also occur after the noun a very rare pattern among oral languages DOGBROWNIHAVEDOG BROWN I HAVEI have a brown dog Adverbs however occur before the verbs Most of the time adverbs are simply the same sign as an adjective distinguished by the context of the sentence HOUSEIQUIETENTERHOUSE I QUIET ENTERI enter the house quietly When the scope of the adverb is the entire clause as in the case of time it comes before the topic This is the only thing which can appear before the topic in ASL time topic comment 9 HOURMORNINGSTOREIGO9 HOUR MORNING STORE I GOI m going to the store at 9 00AM Modal verbs come after the main verb of the clause FORYOU STOREIGOCANFOR YOU STORE I GO CANI can go to the store for you Time sequenced clause ordering Edit ASL makes heavy use of time sequenced ordering meaning that events are signed in the order in which they occur For example for I was late to class last night because my boss handed me a huge stack of work after lunch yesterday one would sign YESTERDAY LUNCH FINISH BOSS GIVE me WORK BIG STACK NIGHT CLASS LATE me In stories however ordering is malleable since one can choose to sequence the events either in the order in which they occurred or in the order in which one found out about them Tense and aspect EditIt has been claimed that tense in ASL is marked adverbially and that ASL lacks a separate category of tense markers 27 However Aarons et al 1992 1995 argue that Tense T is indeed a distinct category of syntactic head and that the T node can be occupied either by a modal e g SHOULD or a lexical tense marker e g FUTURE TENSE 27 They support this claim by noting that only one such item can occupy the T slot 28 REUBENCANRENTVIDEO TAPEREUBEN CAN RENT VIDEO TAPE Reuben can rent a video tape REUBENWILLRENTVIDEO TAPEREUBEN WILL RENT VIDEO TAPE Reuben will rent a video tape REUBENCANWILLRENTVIDEO TAPE REUBEN CAN WILL RENT VIDEO TAPE Reuben can will rent a video tape Aspect may be marked either by verbal inflection or by separate lexical items 29 These are ordered Tense Negation Aspect Verb 30 neg non manual negation marker GINGER SHOULD NOT EAT BEEF Ginger should not eat beef negDAVE NOT FINISH SEE MOVIE Dave did not see to completion the movie Aspect topics and transitivity Edit As noted above in ASL aspectually marked verbs cannot take objects To deal with this the object must be known from context so that it does not need to be further specified This is accomplished in two ways The object may be made prominent in a prior clause or It may be used as the topic of the utterance at hand Of these two strategies the first is the more common For my friend was typing her term paper all night to be used with a durative aspect this would result in my friend type T E R M paper typeDURATIVE all night The less colloquial topic construction may come out as my friend TOPIC T E R M paper TOPIC typeDURATIVE all nightCP Syntax EditTopics and main clauses Edit A topic sets off background information that will be discussed in the following main clause Topic constructions are not often used in standard English but they are common in some dialects as in That dog I never could hunt him Topicalization is used productively in ASL and often results in surface forms that do not follow the basic SVO word order 31 In order to non manually mark topics the eyebrows are raised and the head is tilted back during the production of a topic The head is often lowered toward the end of the sign and sometimes the sign is followed rapidly nodding the head A slight pause follows the topic setting it off from the rest of the sentence 32 MEAT tm ILIKELAMB MEAT tm I LIKE LAMBAs for meat I prefer lamb Another way topics may be signed is by shifting the body The signer may use the space on one side of his her body to sign the topic and then shifts to the other side for the rest of the sentence 32 ASL utterances do not require topics but their use is extremely common They are used for purposes of information flow to set up referent loci see above and to supply objects for verbs which are grammatically prevented from taking objects themselves see below Without a topic the dog chased my cat is signed DOGCHASEMYCATDOG CHASE MY CATThe dog chased my cat However people tend to want to set up the object of their concern first and then discuss what happened to it English does this with passive clauses my cat was chased by the dog In ASL topics are used with similar effect MYCAT tmDOGCHASE MY CAT tm DOG CHASElit my cat the dog chased it If the word order of the main clause is changed the meaning of the utterance also changes MYCAT tmCHASEDOG MY CAT tm CHASE DOG my cat chased the dog lit my cat it chased the dog There are three types of non manual topic markers all of which involve raised eyebrows 33 The three types of non manual topic markers are used with different types of topics and in different contexts and the topic markings cannot spread over other elements in the utterance Topics can be moved from and remain null in the main clause of an utterance or topics can be base generated and either be co referential to either the subject or object in the main clause or be related to the subject of object by a semantic property 34 The first type of non manual marking topic marking 1 tm1 is only used with a moved topic 35 Tm1 is characterized by raised eyebrows widened eyes and head tilted backwards At the end of the sign the head moves down and there is a pause often with an eye blink before the sentence is continued 36 The following is an example of a context in which the tm1 marking is used MARY tm1JOHNLOVE MARY tm1 JOHN LOVE Mary John loves or John loves Mary 37 Topic marking 2 tm2 and topic marking 3 tm3 are both used with base generated topics Tm2 is characterized by raised eyebrows widened eyes and the head tilted backwards and to the side Toward the end of the sign the head moves forward and to the opposite side and there is a pause and often an eye blink before continuing 38 For tm3 the eyebrows are raised and the eyes are opened wide the head starts tilted down and jerks up and down the lips are opened and raised and the head is nodded rapidly a few times before pausing and continuing the sentence Although both tm2 and tm3 accompany base generated topics they are used in different contexts Tm2 is used to introduce new information and change the topic of a conversation to something that the signer is going to subsequently characterize while tm3 is used to introduce new information that the signer believes is already known by his her interlocutor 39 Tm2 may be used with any base generated topic whereas only topics that are co referential with an argument in the sentence may be marked with tm3 40 An example of a tm2 marking used with a topic related to the object of the main clause is VEGETABLE tm2 JOHNLIKECORN VEGETABLE tm2 JOHN LIKE CORN As for vegetables John likes corn 38 An example of a tm2 marking used with a co referential topic is FRESHVEGETABLE tm3 JOHNLIKEIX 3rd FRESH VEGETABLE tm3 JOHN LIKE IX 3rd As for fresh vegetables John likes them 41 IX 3rd represents a 3rd person index Another example of a tm2 marking with a co referential topic is JOHNi tm2 IX 3rdiLOVEMARY JOHNi tm2 IX 3rdi LOVE MARY as for John he loves Mary 41 An example of a tm3 topic marking is JOHNi tm3 IX 3rdiLOVEMARY JOHNi tm3 IX 3rdi LOVE MARY you know John he loves Mary 42 ASL sentences may have up to two marked topics 33 Possible combinations of topic types are two tm2 topics two tm3 topics tm2 preceding tm1 tm3 preceding tm1 and tm2 preceding tm3 Sentences with these topic combinations in the opposite orders or with two tm1 topics are considered ungrammatical by native signers 43 Relative clauses Edit Relative clauses are signaled by tilting back the head and raising the eyebrows and upper lip This is done during the performance of the entire clause There is no change in word order For example recentlydogchasecat RELATIVEcomehome recently dog chase cat RELATIVE come homeThe dog which recently chased the cat came home where the brackets here indicate the duration of the non manual features If the sign recently were made without these features it would lie outside the relative clause and the meaning would change to the dog which chased the cat recently came home Negated clauses Edit Negated clauses may be signaled by shaking the head during the entire clause A topic however cannot be so negated the headshake can only be produced during the production of the main clause A second type of negation starts with the verb and continues to the end of the clause In addition in many communities negation is put at the end of the clause unless there is a wh question word For example the sentence I thought the movie was not good could be signed as BEFORE MOVIE ME SEE THINK WHAT IT GOOD NOT There are two manual signs that negate a sentence NOT and NONE which are accompanied by a shake of the head NONE is typically used when talking about possession DOGIHAVENONEDOG I HAVE NONEI don t have any dogs NOT negates a verb TENNISILIKEPLAYNOTTENNIS I LIKE PLAY NOTI don t like to play tennis Interrogative clauses Edit There are three types of questions with different constructions in ASL wh questions yes no questions and rhetorical questions 44 Non manual grammatical markings Edit Non manual grammatical markings are grammatical and semantic features that do not include the use of hands They can include mouth shape eye gazes facial expressions body shifting head tilting and eyebrow raising Non manual grammatical markings can also aid in identifying sentence type which is especially relevant to our discussion of different types of interrogatives 45 Wh questions Edit Wh questions can be formed in a variety of ways in ASL The wh word can appear solely at the end of the sentence solely at the beginning of the sentence at both the beginning and end of the sentence see section 4 4 2 1 on double occurring wh words or in situ i e where the wh word is in the sentence structure before movement occurs 46 Manual wh signs are also accompanied by a non manual grammatical marking see section 4 4 1 which can include a variety of features 47 This non manual grammatical marking can spread optionally over the entire wh phrase or just a small part Some languages have very few wh words where context and discourse are sufficient to elicit the information that one needs ASL has many different wh words with certain wh words having multiple variations A list of the wh words of ASL can be found below WHAT WHAT DO WHAT FOR WHAT PU WHAT FS WHEN WHERE WHICH WHO several variations WHY HOW HOW MANY 48 Double occurring wh words Edit As mentioned above ASL possesses wh questions with word initial placement word final placement in situ structure but the most unique style of wh word occurrence in ASL is where the wh word occurs twice copied in final position 49 This doubling can be seen in the table below WHAT JOHN BUY WHAT what did John buy This doubling provides a useful template to analyze 2 separate analyses about whether wh words move rightward or leftward in ASL While some researchers argue for rightward movement in wh questions such as Aarons and Neidle 50 others including Petronio and Lillo Martin have argued that ASL has leftward movement and wh words that appear to the right of the clause move by other processes 51 Both analyses agree upon the fact that there is wh movement present in these interrogative phrases but it is a matter of what direction the wh movement is moving in that causes controversy No matter what direction the wh movement is analyzed to go in it is crucial to the analysis that the movement of the wh element is to the position of SPEC CP 52 Leftward Wh movement Analysis in American Sign Language Lillo Martin amp Fischer s and Petronio s leftward wh movement analysis Edit Summary of the leftward wh movement analysis in American Sign Language The leftward movement analysis is congruent with cross linguistic data that wh movement is always leftward It can be seen as the less controversial of the 2 proposals The main arguments presented by the Leftward Wh movement analysis are That the spec CP is on the left that the wh movement is leftward and that the final wh word in a sentence is a base generated double This is illustrated in the syntax tree located to the right of this paragraph 53 Arguments for leftward movement are based on the facts that if wh movement in ASL were rightward ASL would be an exception to cross linguistic generalizations that wh movement is leftward 51 It has also been hypothesized that wh elements cannot be topicalized as topicalized elements must be presupposed and interrogatives are not 54 This would be detrimental to the rightward analysis as they are analyzing the doubled wh word as a base generated topic Aarons et al s rightward wh movement analysis EditSummary of the rightward wh movement analysis in American Sign Language Rightward Wh movement Analysis in American Sign LanguageThe rightward movement analysis is a newer more abstract argument of how wh movement occurs in ASL The main arguments for rightward movement begin by analyzing spec CP as being on the right the wh movement as being rightward and as the initial wh word as a base generated topic 55 This can be seen in the syntax tree on the right One of the rightward movement analysis main arguments is in regards to the non manual grammatical markings and their optional spreading over the sentence In ASL the use of non manual grammatical markings is optional depending on the type of wh question being asked In the rightward analysis both partial and full spreading of non manual grammatical markers can be accounted for due to the association with the WH feature over its c command domain 56 In the leftward analysis the partial or full spreading of non manual grammatical markings cannot be accounted for in this same way The leftward movement analysis requires wh marking to extend over the entirety of the question regardless which is not what is attested in ASL Yes no questions Edit In spoken language Yes no questions will oftentimes differ in their word order from the statement form For example in English English Statement HE WILL BUY THE SHIRT English Yes no Q WILL HE BUY THE SHIRT 57 In ASL yes no questions are marked by the non manual grammatical markings as discussed in section 4 4 1 This eyebrow raise slight tilt of the head and lean forward are what indicate that a yes no question is being asked without any change in word order from the statement form There is speculation amongst linguists that these non manual grammatical markings that indicate a yes no questions are similar to the question intonation of spoken languages 58 Yes no questions differ from wh questions as they do not differ in word order from the original statement form of the sentence whereas wh questions do As well in yes no questions the non manual marking must be used over the whole utterance in order for it to be judged as a statement opposed to a question 59 The yes no question is the same word order as the statement form of the sentence with the addition of non manual grammatical markings This can be seen in the examples below ASL Statement JUAN WILL BUY SHOES TODAY Juan will buy shoes today ASL Yes no Question brow raiseJUAN WILL BUY SHOES TODAY Will Juan buy shoes today 60 Rhetorical questions Edit Non manual grammatical markings are also used for rhetorical questions which are questions that do not intend to elicit an answer To distinguish the non manual marking for rhetorical questions from that of yes no questions the body is in a neutral position opposed to tilted forward and the head is tilted in a different way than in yes no questions 61 Rhetorical questions are much more common in ASL than in English For example in ASL ILIKE NEGATIVE WHAT RHETORICAL GARLIC I LIKE NEGATIVE WHAT RHETORICAL GARLIC I don t like garlic This strategy is commonly used instead of signing the word because for clarity or emphasis For instance PASTAIEATENJOYTRUE WHY RHETORICAL ITALIANI PASTA I EAT ENJOY TRUE WHY RHETORICAL ITALIAN I I love to eat pasta because I am Italian DP syntax EditSubject pronoun tags Edit Information may also be added after the main clause as a kind of afterthought In ASL this is commonly seen with subject pronouns These are accompanied by a nod of the head and make a statement more emphatic boyfallboy fall The boy fell down versus boyfall he TAGboy fall he TAG The boy fell down he did The subject need not be mentioned as in fallfall He fell down versus fall he TAGfall he TAG He fell down he did Deixis Edit In ASL signers set up regions of space loci for specific referents see above these can then be referred to indexically by pointing at those locations with pronouns and indexical verbs Pronouns Edit Personal pronouns in ASL are indexic That is they point to their referent or to a locus representing their referent When the referent is physically present pronouns involve simply pointing at the referent with different handshapes for different pronominal uses A G handshape is a personal pronoun an extended B handshape with an outward palm orientation is a possessive pronoun and an extended thumb A handshape is a reflexive pronoun these may be combined with numeral signs to sign you two us three all of them etc If the referent is not physically present the speaker identifies the referent and then points to a location the locus in the sign space near their body This locus can then be pointed at to refer to the referent Theoretically any number of loci may be set up as long as the signer and recipient remember them all but in practice no more than eight loci are used Meier 1990 demonstrates that only two grammatical persons are distinguished in ASL First person and non first person as in Damin Both persons come in several numbers as well as with signs such as my and by myself Meier provides several arguments for believing that ASL does not formally distinguish second from third person For example when pointing to a person that is physically present a pronoun is equivalent to either you or s he depending on the discourse There is nothing in the sign itself nor in the direction of eye gaze or body posture that can be relied on to make this distinction That is the same formal sign can refer to any of several second or third persons which the indexic nature of the pronoun makes clear In English indexic uses also occur as in I need you to go to the store and you to stay here but not so ubiquitously In contrast several first person ASL pronouns such as the plural possessive our look different from their non first person equivalents and a couple of pronouns do not occur in the first person at all so first and non first persons are formally distinct Personal pronouns have separate forms for singular I and you s he and plural we and you they These have possessive counterparts my our your his her your their In addition there are pronoun forms which incorporate numerals from two to five the three of us the four of you them etc though the dual pronouns are slightly idiosyncratic in form i e they have a K rather than 2 handshape and the wrist nods rather than circles These numeral incorporated pronouns have no possessive equivalents Also among the personal pronouns are the self forms by myself by your themselves etc These only occur in the singular and plural there is no numeral incorporation and are only found as subjects They have derived emphatic and characterizing forms with modifications used for derivation rather like those for verbal aspect The characterizing pronoun is used when describing someone who has just been mentioned It only occurs as a non first person singular form Finally there are formal pronouns used for honored guests These occur as singular and plural in the non first person but only as singular in the first person ASL is a pro drop language which means that pronouns are not used when the referent is obvious from context and is not being emphasized Indexical verbs Edit Within ASL there is a class of indexical often called directional verbs These include the signs for see pay give show invite help send bite etc These verbs include an element of motion that indexes one or more referents either physically present or set up through the referent locus system If there are two loci the first indicates the subject and the second the object direct or indirect depending on the verb reflecting the basic word order of ASL For example give is a bi indexical verb based on a flattened M O handshape For I give you the hand moves from myself toward you for you give me it moves from you to me See is indicated with a V handshape Two loci for a dog and a cat can be set up with the sign moving between them to indicate the dog sees the cat if it starts at the locus for dog and moves toward the locus for cat or the cat sees the dog with the motion in the opposite direction or the V hand can circulate between both loci and myself to mean we the dog the cat and myself see each other The verb to be in pain index fingers pointed at each other and alternately approaching and separating is signed at the location of the pain head for headache cheek for toothache abdomen for stomachache etc This is normally done in relation to the signer s own body regardless of the person feeling the pain but may take also use the locus system especially for body parts which are not normally part of the sign space such as the leg There are also spatial verbs such as put up and put below which allow signers to specify where things are or how they moved them around Conjunctions EditThere is no separate sign in ASL for the conjunction and Instead multiple sentences or phrases are combined with a short pause between Often lists are specified with a listing and ordering technique a simple version of which is to show the length of the list first with the nondominant hand then to describe each element after pointing to the nondominant finger that represents it English I have three cats and they are named Billy Bob and Buddy ASL CAT I HAVE THREE LIST NAME FIRST OF THREE LIST B I L L Y SECOND OF THREE LIST B O B THIRD OF THREE LIST B U D D Y There is a manual sign for the conjunction or but the concept is usually signed nonmanually with a slight shoulder twist English I ll leave at 5 or 6 o clock ASL I LEAVE TIME 5 shoulder shift TIME 6 The manual sign for the conjunction but is similar to the sign for different It is more likely to be used in Pidgin Signed English than in ASL Instead shoulder shifts can be used similar to or with appropriate facial expression English I like to swim but I don t like to run ASL PSE SWIM I LIKE BUT RUN I LIKE NOT ASL SWIM I LIKE shoulder shift RUN I LIKE NOTNotes Edit a b c Bahan 1996 20 a b Bahan 1996 21 a b Bahan 1996 21 22 a b c Bahan 1996 22 23 a b c Bahan 1996 23 a b c Bahan 1996 24 a b c Bahan 1996 25 Bahan 1996 50 Bahan 1996 50 51 Aronoff M Meir I amp Sandler W 2005 The paradox of sign language morphology Language 81 2 301 Supalla T R 1982 Structure and acquisition of verbs of motion and location in American Sign Language Doctoral dissertation ProQuest Information amp Learning Supalla T 1986 The classifier system in American sign language Noun classes and categorization 181 214 Morford Jill MacFarlane James 2003 Frequency Characteristics of American Sign Language Sign Language Studies 3 2 213 doi 10 1353 sls 2003 0003 S2CID 6031673 Supalla Samuel J 1990 The Arbitrary Name Sign System in American Sign Language Sign Language Studies 1067 1 99 126 doi 10 1353 sls 1990 0006 ISSN 1533 6263 S2CID 144191789 Types and trends of name signs in the Swedish Sign Language community PDF Samuel J Supalla 1992 The Book of Name Signs Naming in American Sign Language The J hand shape is articulated with a brush of the pinkie finger against the sign location It cannot occur in neutral space There is no provision for Z that is there are no Z initial arbitrary name signs Contrastive locations are limited to the temple forehead side of chin chin shoulder chest outside of elbow inside of elbow palm of a vertical flat hand back of a horizontal back hand Some name signs are distinguished by orientation For example an I hand shape may make contact with either the tip of the pinkie finger or the side of the thumb the M hand shape with either the tips of the three fingers or the side of the index finger etc The two locations are usually close by such as the hand moving across the chin or down the chest but may occasionally be further apart as from the shoulder to the back of the hand There are occasional exceptions to this constraint For example I King Jordan s name sign is homophonous with king K hand moving from the shoulder to the hip a b Bahan 1996 30 Pichler Deborah Chen 2001 Word order variation and acquisition in American Sign Language pp 14 15 Bahan 1996 31 Neidle Carol 2002 Language across modalities Linguistic Variation Yearbook 2 1 71 98 doi 10 1075 livy 2 05nei Aarons DebraAspects of the syntax of American Sign Language 1994 Aspects of the syntax of American Sign Language p 55 Pichler Deborah Chen 2001 Word order variation and acquisition in American Sign Language p 23 a b Bahan 1996 33 Bahan 1996 33 34 Bahan 1996 27 Bahan 1996 34 37 Pichler Deborah Chen 2001 Word order variation and acquisition in American Sign Language p 15 a b Aarons Debra 1994 Aspects of the syntax of American Sign Language p 70 a b Bahan 1996 41 42 Aarons Debra 1994 Aspects of the syntax of American Sign Language pp 151 153 Aarons Debra 1994 Aspects of the syntax of American Sign Language p 155 Aarons Debra 1994 Aspects of the syntax of American Sign Language pp 156 157 Aarons Debra 1994 Aspects of the syntax of American Sign Language p 157 a b Aarons Debra 1994 Aspects of the syntax of American Sign Language p 160 Aarons Debra 1994 Aspects of the syntax of American Sign Language pp 163 165 Pichler Deborah Chen 2001 Word order variation and acquisition in American Sign Language pp 28 29 a b Aarons Debra 1994 Aspects of the syntax of American Sign Language p 162 Aarons Debra 1994 Aspects of the Syntax of American Sign Language p 165 Aarons Debra 1994 Aspects of the syntax of American Sign Language pp 177 181 Aarons Debra 1994 Aspects of the syntax of American Sign Language pp 67 69 Non manual signals in sign language www handspeak com Retrieved April 21 2022 Petronio Karen Diane Lillo Martin 1997 Wh Movement and the Position of Spec CP Evidence from American Sign Language PDF Linguistic Society of America 73 1 18 57 doi 10 2307 416592 JSTOR 416592 Aarons Debra 1994 Aspects of the syntax of American Sign Language p 69 Joseph Christopher Hill Diane C Lillo Martin Sandra K Wood 2019 Sign languages structures and contexts London p 65 ISBN 978 0 429 02087 2 OCLC 1078875378 Josep Quer Roland Pfau Annika Herrmann 2021 The Routledge handbook of theoretical and experimental sign language research Abingdon Oxon p 234 ISBN 978 1 317 62427 1 OCLC 1182020388 Neidle Carol 2002 Language across modalities ASL focus and question constructions Linguistic Variation Yearbook 2 1 71 98 doi 10 1075 livy 2 05nei a b Petronio Karen Lillo Martin Diane 1997 WH Movement and the Position of Spec CP Evidence from American Sign Language Language 73 1 18 57 doi 10 2307 416592 ISSN 0097 8507 JSTOR 416592 Petronio Karen Lillo Martin Diane March 1997 WH Movement and the Position of Spec CP Evidence from American Sign Language Language 73 1 21 doi 10 2307 416592 JSTOR 416592 Josep Quer Roland Pfau Annika Herrmann 2021 The Routledge handbook of theoretical and experimental sign language research Abingdon Oxon p 235 ISBN 978 1 317 62427 1 OCLC 1182020388 Petronio Karen Lillo Martin Diane March 1997 WH Movement and the Position of Spec CP Evidence from American Sign Language Language 73 1 22 doi 10 2307 416592 JSTOR 416592 Petronio Karen Lillo Martin Diane March 1997 WH Movement and the Position of Spec CP Evidence from American Sign Language Language 73 1 18 doi 10 2307 416592 ISSN 0097 8507 JSTOR 416592 Neidle Carol MacLaughlin Dawn Lee Robert G Bahan Benjamin Kegl Judy 1998 The Rightward Analysis of wh Movement in ASL A Reply to Petronio and Lillo Martin Language 74 4 823 825 doi 10 2307 417004 ISSN 0097 8507 JSTOR 417004 Baker Anne et al 2016 The Linguistics of Sign Languages An Introduction Amsterdam Philadelphia John Benjamins Publishing Company p 130 ISBN 9789027212306 Baker Anne July 8 2016 The linguistics of sign languages an introduction Amsterdam p 131 ISBN 978 90 272 6734 4 OCLC 936433607 Aarons Debra 1994 Aspects of the syntax of American Sign Language p 92 Hill Joseph C Lillo Martin Diane C Wood Sandra K 2018 Syntax Sign Languages New York Routledge pp 55 81 doi 10 4324 9780429020872 4 ISBN 978 0 429 02087 2 S2CID 239556102 retrieved April 18 2022 Aarons Debra 1994 Aspects of the syntax of American Sign Language p 68 References EditAarons Debra 1994 Aspects of the syntax of American Sign Language Unpublished doctoral dissertation Boston University Boston MA Bahan Benjamin 1996 Non Manual Realization of Agreement in American Sign Language PDF Boston University Archived PDF from the original on October 11 2017 Retrieved November 25 2012 Klima Edward amp Bellugi Ursula 1979 The Signs of Language Cambridge Harvard University Press ISBN 0 674 80795 2 Liddell Scott K 2003 Grammar Gesture and Meaning in American Sign Language Cambridge University Press Neidle Carol 2002 Language across Modalities ASL focus and question constructions Linguistic Variation Yearbook 2 1 71 98 Petronio Karen amp Lillo Martin Diane 1997 WH Movement and the Position of Spec CP Evidence from American Sign Language Language 73 1 18 57 Pichler Debora Chen 2001 Word order variation and acquisition in American Sign Language Unpublished doctoral dissertation University of Connecticut Stokoe William C 1976 Dictionary of American Sign Language on Linguistic Principles Linstok Press ISBN 0 932130 01 1 Stokoe William C 1960 Sign language structure An outline of the visual communication systems of the American deaf Studies in linguistics Occasional papers No 8 Buffalo Dept of Anthropology and Linguistics University of Buffalo Further reading EditSigning Naturally by Ken Mikos The Syntax of American Sign Language Functional Categories and Hierarchical Structure by Carol Jan Neidle Grammar Gesture and Meaning in American Sign Language by Scott K Liddell Linguistics of American Sign Language An Introduction 4th Ed by Clayton Valli Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title American Sign Language grammar amp oldid 1116231158 Name signs, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.