fbpx
Wikipedia

Assimilation (phonology)

Assimilation is a sound change in which some phonemes (typically consonants or vowels) change to become more similar to other nearby sounds. A common type of phonological process across languages, assimilation can occur either within a word or between words.

It occurs in normal speech but becomes more common in more rapid speech. In some cases, assimilation causes the sound spoken to differ from the normal pronunciation in isolation, such as the prefix in- of English input pronounced with phonetic [m] rather than [n]. In other cases, the change is accepted as canonical for that word or phrase, especially if it is recognized in standard spelling: implant pronounced with [m], composed historically of in + plant.

English "handbag" (canonically /ˈhændbæɡ/) is often pronounced /ˈhæmbæɡ/ in rapid speech because the [m] and [b] sounds are both bilabial consonants, and their places of articulation are similar. However, the sequence [d]-[b] has different places but similar manner of articulation (voiced stop) and is sometimes elided, which sometimes causes the canonical [n] phoneme to assimilate to [m] before the [b]. The pronunciations /ˈhænbæɡ/ or /ˈhændbæɡ/ are, however, common in normal speech.

In contrast, the word "cupboard", although it is historically a compound of "cup" /kʌp/ and "board" /bɔːrd/, is always pronounced /ˈkʌbərd/, never */ˈkʌpbɔːrd/, even in slow, highly-articulated speech.

Like in those examples, sound segments typically assimilate to a following sound,[note 1] but they may also assimilate to a preceding one.[note 2] Assimilation most commonly occurs between immediately adjacent-sounds but may occur between sounds that are separated by others.[note 3]

Assimilation can be synchronic, an active process in a language at a given point in time, or diachronic, a historical sound change.

A related process is coarticulation in which one segment influences another to produce an allophonic variation, such as vowels becoming nasalized before nasal consonants (/n, m, ŋ/) when the soft palate (velum) opens prematurely or /b/ becoming labialized as in "boot" [bʷuːt̚] or "ball" [bʷɔːɫ] in some accents. This article describes both processes under the term assimilation.

Concept

The physiological or psychological mechanisms of coarticulation are unknown, and coarticulation is often loosely referred to as a segment being "triggered" by an assimilatory change in another segment. In assimilation, the phonological patterning of the language, discourse styles and accent are some of the factors contributing to changes observed.

There are four configurations found in assimilations:

  • Between adjacent segments.
  • Between segments separated by one or more intervening segments.
  • Changes made in reference to a preceding segment
  • Changes made in reference to a following segment

Although all four occur, changes in regard to a following adjacent segment account for virtually all assimilatory changes and most of the regular ones.[citation needed] Assimilations to an adjacent segment are vastly more frequent than assimilations to a nonadjacent one. Those radical asymmetries might contain hints about the mechanisms involved, but they are not obvious.

If a sound changes with reference to a following segment, it is traditionally called "regressive assimilation". Changes with reference to a preceding segment are traditionally called "progressive".[1] Many[2] find those terms confusing, as they seem to mean the opposite of the intended meaning. Accordingly, a variety of alternative terms have arisen, not all of which avoid the problem of the traditional terms. Regressive assimilation is also known as right-to-left, leading, or anticipatory assimilation. Progressive assimilation is also known as left-to-right, perseveratory, preservative, lagging, or lag assimilation. The terms anticipatory and lag are used here.

Occasionally, two sounds (invariably adjacent) may influence each other in reciprocal assimilation. When such a change results in a single segment with some of the features of both components, it is known as coalescence or fusion.

Assimilation occurs in two different types: complete assimilation, in which the sound affected by assimilation becomes exactly the same as the sound causing assimilation, and partial assimilation, in which the sound becomes the same in one or more features but remains different in other features.

Tonal languages may exhibit tone assimilation (in effect tonal umlaut), but sign languages also exhibit assimilation when the characteristics of neighbouring cheremes may be mixed.

Examples

Anticipatory assimilation to an adjacent segment

Anticipatory assimilation to an adjacent segment[3] is the most common type of assimilation by far, and typically has the character of a conditioned sound change, i.e., it applies to the whole lexicon or part of it. For example, in English, the place of articulation of nasals assimilates to that of a following stop (handkerchief is pronounced [hæŋkɚtʃif], handbag in rapid speech is pronounced [hæmbæɡ]).

In Italian, voiceless stops assimilated historically to a following /t/:

  • Latin octo "eight" > It. otto
  • Latin lectus "bed" > letto
  • Latin subtus – pronounced suptus "under" > sotto

Italian otto, letto and sotto are examples of historical restructuring: otto and letto no longer contain /kt/ pronounced [tt], and sotto is no longer the structure /bt/ subject to the partial assimilation of devoicing of /b/ and full assimilation to produce [tt]. Over time, phonetic [tt] as a frequent assimilation of /kt/ and /bt/ was rather reinterpreted as reflecting /tt/. The structural sequence /kt/ is now all but absent in Italian, since all items in popular speech underwent the same restructuring, /kt/ > /tt/. On the rare occasion that Italian /kt/ is encountered, however, the same assimilation that triggered the restructuring can occur at the phonetic level. For example, the medical term ictus 'stroke', a relatively recent direct borrowing from Latin, is usually pronounced [ˈiktus] in deliberate speech, but [ˈittus] is frequent in more casual registers.

  • Latin ictus > Italian ictus, pronounced either [ˈiktus] or [ˈittus]

Affrication in English

There has been a notable change recognized across a variety of English dialects regarding the pronunciation of the /tr/ and /dr/ consonant clusters. Starting around the mid-20th century,[4] the alveolar stop in /tr, dr/ has slowly been replaced by a post-alveolar affricate instead, resulting in the all-postalveolar consonant clusters [tʃɹ] and [dʒɹ].[5] This phenomenon also occurs in /str/, resulting in the all-postalveolar consonant cluster [​ʃtʃɹ]. The affrication of /tr, dɹ/ has been seen in American English, British English, Australian English, and New Zealand English.[5] It is suspected that this change has occurred due to assimilation.[5]

One of the first papers that discussed the affrication of /tɹ, dr/ is "Pre-School Children's Knowledge of English Phonology" by Charles Read, published in 1971.[6] The study discussed in this paper focuses on how children in pre-school analyze the phonetic aspect of language in order to determine the proper spelling of English words. Read noticed that many of the children involved in the study misspelled words that began with /tr, dr/, spelling words like troubles and dragon as "chribls" and "jragin", respectively.[6] In a different test, Read also found that many of the children believed that words like train and chicken both started with /tʃ/.[6]

Anticipatory assimilation at a distance

Anticipatory assimilation at a distance is rare and usually merely an accident in the history of a specific word.

However, the diverse and common assimilations known as umlaut in which the phonetics of a vowel are influenced by the phonetics of a vowel in a following syllable, are common and in the nature of sound laws. Such changes abound in the histories of Germanic languages, Romance, Insular Celtic, Albanian, and many others.

For example, in the history of English, a back vowel became front if a high front vowel or semivowel (*i, ī, j) was in the following syllable, and a front vowel became higher unless it was already high:

  • Proto-Germanic *mūsiz "mice" > Old English mýs /myːs/ > Modern English mice
  • PGmc *batizōn "better" > OE bettre
  • PGmc *fōtiz "feet" > OE fét > ME feet

On the other hand, Proto-Germanic *i and *u > e, o respectively before *a in the following syllable (Germanic a-mutation) although that had already happened significantly earlier:

  • PGmc *wurdą > OE word
  • PGmc *nestaz > OE nest

Another example of a regular change is the sibilant assimilation of Sanskrit in which if there were two different sibilants as the onset of successive syllables, a plain /s/ was always replaced by the palatal /ɕ/:

  • Proto-Indo-European *smeḱru- "beard" > Skt. śmaśru-
  • PIE *ḱoso- "gray" > Skt. śaśa- "rabbit"
  • PIE *sweḱru- "husband's mother' > Skt. śvaśrū-

Lag assimilation to an adjacent segment

Lag assimilation to an adjacent segment[3] is tolerably common and often has the nature of a sound law.

Proto-Indo-European *-ln- becomes -ll- in both Germanic and Italic: *ḱl̥nis "hill" > PreLat. *kolnis > Lat. collis; > PGmc *hulliz > OE hyll /hyll/ > hill. The enclitic form of English is, eliding the vowel, becomes voiceless when adjacent to a word-final voiceless nonsibilant: it is [ɪtɪz], that is [ðætɪz] > it's [ɪts], that's [ðæts].

In Polish, /v/ regularly becomes /f/ after a voiceless obstruent:

  • kwiat 'flower', pronounced [kfjat], instead of [kvjat]
  • twarz 'face', pronounced [tfaʂ], instead of [tvaʂ]

Because of a similar process, Proto-Indo-Iranian *ćw became sp in Avestan: Old Avestan aspa 'horse' corresponds to Sanskrit aśva

Lag assimilation at a distance

Lag assimilation at a distance is rare and usually sporadic (except when part of something broader, as for the Sanskrit śaśa- example, above): Greek leirion > Lat. līlium "lily".

In vowel harmony, a vowel's phonetics is often influenced by that of a preceding vowel. Thus, for example, most Finnish case markers come in two flavors, with /ɑ/ (written a) and /æ/ (written ä), depending on whether the preceding vowel is back or front. However, it is difficult to know where and how in the history of Finnish an actual assimilatory change took place. The distribution of pairs of endings in Finnish is just that and is not in any sense the operation of an assimilatory innovation, but it was probably the outbirth of such an innovation long ago.

In the opposite direction, in umlaut, a vowel is modified to conform more closely to the vowel in the next syllable.

Coalescence (fusion)

Proto-Italic *dw > Latin b, as in *dwís "twice" > Lat. bis. Also, Old Latin duellum > Latin bellum "war".

Proto-Celtic *sw shows up in Old Irish in initial position as s, thus *swesōr "sister" > OIr siur */ʃuɾ/, *spenyo- > *swinea- > *swine "nipple" > sine. However, when preceded by a vowel, the *sw sequence becomes /f/: má fiur "my sister", bó tri-fne "a cow with three teats". There is also the famous change in P-Celtic of * -> p. Proto-Celtic also underwent the change * -> b.

See also

Notes

  1. ^ Assimilation to a following sound is called regressive or anticipatory assimilation.
  2. ^ Assimilation to a preceding sound is called progressive assimilation.
  3. ^ This is called assimilation at a distance.

References

Citations

  1. ^ Meyer, Paul Georg (2005). Synchronic English Linguistics: An Introduction. Tübingen: Gunter Narr. p. 130.
  2. ^ For examples, see: Slis, Iman Hans. 1985. The voiced-voiceless distinction and assimilation of voice in Dutch. Helmond: Wibro. 2-3.
  3. ^ a b Sihler, Andrew L. 2000. Language History: An Introduction. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 21–22.
  4. ^ Magloughlin, Lyra (2018-05-07). /tɹ/ and /dɹ/ in North American English: Phonologization of a Coarticulatory Effect (Thesis thesis). Université d'Ottawa / University of Ottawa. doi:10.20381/ruor-21856.
  5. ^ a b c Smith, Bridget J.; Mielke, Jeff; Magloughlin, Lyra; Wilbanks, Eric (2019-06-19). "Sound change and coarticulatory variability involving English /ɹ/". Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics. 4 (1). doi:10.5334/gjgl.650. ISSN 2397-1835. S2CID 197750120.
  6. ^ a b c Read, Charles (1971-04-01). "Pre-School Children's Knowledge of English Phonology". Harvard Educational Review. 41 (1): 1–34. doi:10.17763/haer.41.1.91367v0h80051573. ISSN 0017-8055.
  7. ^ Savnik, Roman, ed. 1971. Krajevni leksikon Slovenije, vol. 2. Ljubljana: Državna založba Slovenije, p. 266.
  8. ^ a b Snoj, Marko (2009). Etimološki slovar slovenskih zemljepisnih imen. Ljubljana: Modrijan. pp. 179, 347–348.

Sources

  • Crowley, Terry. (1997) An Introduction to Historical Linguistics. 3rd edition. Oxford University Press.

assimilation, phonology, assimilation, linguistics, redirects, here, assimilation, speakers, different, languages, language, assimilation, this, article, multiple, issues, please, help, improve, discuss, these, issues, talk, page, learn, when, remove, these, t. Assimilation linguistics redirects here For assimilation of speakers of two different languages see Language assimilation This article has multiple issues Please help improve it or discuss these issues on the talk page Learn how and when to remove these template messages This article may be too technical for most readers to understand Please help improve it to make it understandable to non experts without removing the technical details September 2016 Learn how and when to remove this template message This article needs additional citations for verification Please help improve this article by adding citations to reliable sources Unsourced material may be challenged and removed Find sources Assimilation phonology news newspapers books scholar JSTOR September 2009 Learn how and when to remove this template message Learn how and when to remove this template message This article contains phonetic transcriptions in the International Phonetic Alphabet IPA For an introductory guide on IPA symbols see Help IPA For the distinction between and see IPA Brackets and transcription delimiters Assimilation is a sound change in which some phonemes typically consonants or vowels change to become more similar to other nearby sounds A common type of phonological process across languages assimilation can occur either within a word or between words It occurs in normal speech but becomes more common in more rapid speech In some cases assimilation causes the sound spoken to differ from the normal pronunciation in isolation such as the prefix in of English input pronounced with phonetic m rather than n In other cases the change is accepted as canonical for that word or phrase especially if it is recognized in standard spelling implant pronounced with m composed historically of in plant English handbag canonically ˈ h ae n d b ae ɡ is often pronounced ˈ h ae m b ae ɡ in rapid speech because the m and b sounds are both bilabial consonants and their places of articulation are similar However the sequence d b has different places but similar manner of articulation voiced stop and is sometimes elided which sometimes causes the canonical n phoneme to assimilate to m before the b The pronunciations ˈ h ae n b ae ɡ or ˈ h ae n d b ae ɡ are however common in normal speech In contrast the word cupboard although it is historically a compound of cup k ʌ p and board b ɔːr d is always pronounced ˈ k ʌ b er d never ˈ k ʌ p b ɔːr d even in slow highly articulated speech Like in those examples sound segments typically assimilate to a following sound note 1 but they may also assimilate to a preceding one note 2 Assimilation most commonly occurs between immediately adjacent sounds but may occur between sounds that are separated by others note 3 Assimilation can be synchronic an active process in a language at a given point in time or diachronic a historical sound change A related process is coarticulation in which one segment influences another to produce an allophonic variation such as vowels becoming nasalized before nasal consonants n m ŋ when the soft palate velum opens prematurely or b becoming labialized as in boot bʷuːt or ball bʷɔːɫ in some accents This article describes both processes under the term assimilation Contents 1 Concept 2 Examples 2 1 Anticipatory assimilation to an adjacent segment 2 1 1 Affrication in English 2 2 Anticipatory assimilation at a distance 2 3 Lag assimilation to an adjacent segment 2 4 Lag assimilation at a distance 2 5 Coalescence fusion 3 See also 4 Notes 5 References 5 1 Citations 5 2 SourcesConcept EditThe physiological or psychological mechanisms of coarticulation are unknown and coarticulation is often loosely referred to as a segment being triggered by an assimilatory change in another segment In assimilation the phonological patterning of the language discourse styles and accent are some of the factors contributing to changes observed There are four configurations found in assimilations Between adjacent segments Between segments separated by one or more intervening segments Changes made in reference to a preceding segment Changes made in reference to a following segmentAlthough all four occur changes in regard to a following adjacent segment account for virtually all assimilatory changes and most of the regular ones citation needed Assimilations to an adjacent segment are vastly more frequent than assimilations to a nonadjacent one Those radical asymmetries might contain hints about the mechanisms involved but they are not obvious If a sound changes with reference to a following segment it is traditionally called regressive assimilation Changes with reference to a preceding segment are traditionally called progressive 1 Many 2 find those terms confusing as they seem to mean the opposite of the intended meaning Accordingly a variety of alternative terms have arisen not all of which avoid the problem of the traditional terms Regressive assimilation is also known as right to left leading or anticipatory assimilation Progressive assimilation is also known as left to right perseveratory preservative lagging or lag assimilation The terms anticipatory and lag are used here Occasionally two sounds invariably adjacent may influence each other in reciprocal assimilation When such a change results in a single segment with some of the features of both components it is known as coalescence or fusion Assimilation occurs in two different types complete assimilation in which the sound affected by assimilation becomes exactly the same as the sound causing assimilation and partial assimilation in which the sound becomes the same in one or more features but remains different in other features Tonal languages may exhibit tone assimilation in effect tonal umlaut but sign languages also exhibit assimilation when the characteristics of neighbouring cheremes may be mixed Examples EditAnticipatory assimilation to an adjacent segment Edit Anticipatory assimilation to an adjacent segment 3 is the most common type of assimilation by far and typically has the character of a conditioned sound change i e it applies to the whole lexicon or part of it For example in English the place of articulation of nasals assimilates to that of a following stop handkerchief is pronounced haeŋkɚtʃif handbag in rapid speech is pronounced haembaeɡ In Italian voiceless stops assimilated historically to a following t Latin octo eight gt It otto Latin lectus bed gt letto Latin subtus pronounced suptus under gt sottoItalian otto letto and sotto are examples of historical restructuring otto and letto no longer contain kt pronounced tt and sotto is no longer the structure bt subject to the partial assimilation of devoicing of b and full assimilation to produce tt Over time phonetic tt as a frequent assimilation of kt and bt was rather reinterpreted as reflecting tt The structural sequence kt is now all but absent in Italian since all items in popular speech underwent the same restructuring kt gt tt On the rare occasion that Italian kt is encountered however the same assimilation that triggered the restructuring can occur at the phonetic level For example the medical term ictus stroke a relatively recent direct borrowing from Latin is usually pronounced ˈiktus in deliberate speech but ˈittus is frequent in more casual registers Latin ictus gt Italian ictus pronounced either ˈiktus or ˈittus Affrication in English Edit There has been a notable change recognized across a variety of English dialects regarding the pronunciation of the tr and dr consonant clusters Starting around the mid 20th century 4 the alveolar stop in tr dr has slowly been replaced by a post alveolar affricate instead resulting in the all postalveolar consonant clusters tʃɹ and dʒɹ 5 This phenomenon also occurs in str resulting in the all postalveolar consonant cluster ʃtʃɹ The affrication of tr dɹ has been seen in American English British English Australian English and New Zealand English 5 It is suspected that this change has occurred due to assimilation 5 One of the first papers that discussed the affrication of tɹ dr is Pre School Children s Knowledge of English Phonology by Charles Read published in 1971 6 The study discussed in this paper focuses on how children in pre school analyze the phonetic aspect of language in order to determine the proper spelling of English words Read noticed that many of the children involved in the study misspelled words that began with tr dr spelling words like troubles and dragon as chribls and jragin respectively 6 In a different test Read also found that many of the children believed that words like train and chicken both started with tʃ 6 Anticipatory assimilation at a distance Edit Anticipatory assimilation at a distance is rare and usually merely an accident in the history of a specific word Standard Slovene Jevnica a toponym gt Slovene dialect Vevnica 7 8 Slovene Rakitovec gt Ratitovec a mountain ridge 8 However the diverse and common assimilations known as umlaut in which the phonetics of a vowel are influenced by the phonetics of a vowel in a following syllable are common and in the nature of sound laws Such changes abound in the histories of Germanic languages Romance Insular Celtic Albanian and many others For example in the history of English a back vowel became front if a high front vowel or semivowel i i j was in the following syllable and a front vowel became higher unless it was already high Proto Germanic musiz mice gt Old English mys myːs gt Modern English mice PGmc batizōn better gt OE bettre PGmc fōtiz feet gt OE fet gt ME feetOn the other hand Proto Germanic i and u gt e o respectively before a in the following syllable Germanic a mutation although that had already happened significantly earlier PGmc wurda gt OE word PGmc nestaz gt OE nestAnother example of a regular change is the sibilant assimilation of Sanskrit in which if there were two different sibilants as the onset of successive syllables a plain s was always replaced by the palatal ɕ Proto Indo European smeḱru beard gt Skt smasru PIE ḱoso gray gt Skt sasa rabbit PIE sweḱru husband s mother gt Skt svasru Lag assimilation to an adjacent segment Edit Lag assimilation to an adjacent segment 3 is tolerably common and often has the nature of a sound law Proto Indo European ln becomes ll in both Germanic and Italic ḱl nis hill gt PreLat kolnis gt Lat collis gt PGmc hulliz gt OE hyll hyll gt hill The enclitic form of English is eliding the vowel becomes voiceless when adjacent to a word final voiceless nonsibilant it is ɪtɪz that is daetɪz gt it s ɪts that s daets In Polish v regularly becomes f after a voiceless obstruent kwiat flower pronounced kfjat instead of kvjat twarz face pronounced tfaʂ instead of tvaʂ Because of a similar process Proto Indo Iranian cw became sp in Avestan Old Avestan aspa horse corresponds to Sanskrit asva Lag assimilation at a distance Edit Lag assimilation at a distance is rare and usually sporadic except when part of something broader as for the Sanskrit sasa example above Greek leirion gt Lat lilium lily In vowel harmony a vowel s phonetics is often influenced by that of a preceding vowel Thus for example most Finnish case markers come in two flavors with ɑ written a and ae written a depending on whether the preceding vowel is back or front However it is difficult to know where and how in the history of Finnish an actual assimilatory change took place The distribution of pairs of endings in Finnish is just that and is not in any sense the operation of an assimilatory innovation but it was probably the outbirth of such an innovation long ago In the opposite direction in umlaut a vowel is modified to conform more closely to the vowel in the next syllable Coalescence fusion Edit Proto Italic dw gt Latin b as in dwis twice gt Lat bis Also Old Latin duellum gt Latin bellum war Proto Celtic sw shows up in Old Irish in initial position as s thus swesōr sister gt OIr siur ʃuɾ spenyo gt swinea gt swine nipple gt sine However when preceded by a vowel the sw sequence becomes f ma fiur my sister bo tri fne a cow with three teats There is also the famous change in P Celtic of kʷ gt p Proto Celtic also underwent the change gʷ gt b See also EditAssibilation Phonological history of English consonant clusters Co articulated consonant Consonant harmony Crasis Deletion phonology Dissimilation Epenthesis Labialization Palatalization Pharyngealization Secondary articulation VelarizationNotes Edit Assimilation to a following sound is called regressive or anticipatory assimilation Assimilation to a preceding sound is called progressive assimilation This is called assimilation at a distance References EditCitations Edit Meyer Paul Georg 2005 Synchronic English Linguistics An Introduction Tubingen Gunter Narr p 130 For examples see Slis Iman Hans 1985 The voiced voiceless distinction and assimilation of voice in Dutch Helmond Wibro 2 3 a b Sihler Andrew L 2000 Language History An Introduction Amsterdam John Benjamins pp 21 22 Magloughlin Lyra 2018 05 07 tɹ and dɹ in North American English Phonologization of a Coarticulatory Effect Thesis thesis Universite d Ottawa University of Ottawa doi 10 20381 ruor 21856 a b c Smith Bridget J Mielke Jeff Magloughlin Lyra Wilbanks Eric 2019 06 19 Sound change and coarticulatory variability involving English ɹ Glossa A Journal of General Linguistics 4 1 doi 10 5334 gjgl 650 ISSN 2397 1835 S2CID 197750120 a b c Read Charles 1971 04 01 Pre School Children s Knowledge of English Phonology Harvard Educational Review 41 1 1 34 doi 10 17763 haer 41 1 91367v0h80051573 ISSN 0017 8055 Savnik Roman ed 1971 Krajevni leksikon Slovenije vol 2 Ljubljana Drzavna zalozba Slovenije p 266 a b Snoj Marko 2009 Etimoloski slovar slovenskih zemljepisnih imen Ljubljana Modrijan pp 179 347 348 Sources Edit Crowley Terry 1997 An Introduction to Historical Linguistics 3rd edition Oxford University Press Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Assimilation phonology amp oldid 1127212813, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.