fbpx
Wikipedia

Unami language

Unami (Delaware: Wënami èlixsuwakàn)[4] was an Algonquian language spoken by the Lenape people in the late 17th century and the early 18th century, in what later became the southern two-thirds of New Jersey, southeastern Pennsylvania and the northern two-thirds of Delaware. Later the Lenape peoples migrated and settled in Ontario, Canada and Oklahoma. It is one of the two Delaware languages; the other is Munsee. The last fluent Unami speaker in the United States, Edward Thompson, of the federally recognized Delaware Tribe of Indians, died on August 31, 2002.[1] His sister Nora Thompson Dean (1907–1984) provided valuable information about the language to linguists and other scholars.

Unami
Native toEastern United States
RegionAround the lower Delaware and Hudson rivers in the United States; some Unami groups in Oklahoma
Extinct2002[1]
Language codes
ISO 639-3unm
Glottologunam1242
ELPUnami
Map showing the aboriginal boundaries of Delaware territories, with Munsee territory and Unami dialectal divisions indicated. The territory of the poorly known Unalachtigo dialect of Unami is not clearly indicated, but is presumed to be approximately in the area of "Sankhikan" on the map.
This article contains IPA phonetic symbols. Without proper rendering support, you may see question marks, boxes, or other symbols instead of Unicode characters. For an introductory guide on IPA symbols, see Help:IPA.

Lenni-Lenape literally means 'Men of Men', but is translated to mean 'Original People'. The Lenape names for the areas they inhabited were Scheyichbi (i.e. New Jersey), which means 'water's edge', and Lenapehoking, meaning 'in the land of the Delaware Indians'. It describes the ancient homeland of all Delaware Indians, both Unami and Munsee.[5][6] The English named the river running through much of the traditional range of the Lenape after the first governor of the Jamestown Colony, Lord De La Warr, and consequently referred to the people who lived around the river as "Delaware Indians".

History edit

Unami is an Eastern Algonquian language. The hypothetical common ancestor language from which the Eastern Algonquian languages descend is Proto-Eastern Algonquian (PEA). An intermediate group, Delawarean, that is a descendant of Proto-Eastern Algonquian consists of Mahican and Common Delaware, the latter being a further subgroup comprising Munsee Delaware and Unami Delaware.[7] The justification for Delawarean as an intermediate subgroup rests upon the high degree of similarity between Mahican and the two Delaware languages, but relatively little detailed argumentation in support of Delawarean has been adduced.[7][8][9]

Compared to Munsee, Unami has undergone extensive phonological innovation, coupled with morphological regularization.[10]

The PEA vowel system consisted of four long vowels *i·, *o·, *e·, *a·, and two short vowels *a and ə. The vowel history is as follows: *i· (from PEA merger of Proto-Algonquian (PA *i· and *i to PEA *i·), *o· (from PEA merger of PA *o· and *o ), *e· (from Proto-Algonquian *e·), and *a· (from Proto-Algonquian *a·; the short vowels are (from Proto-Algonquian *e), and *a (from Proto-Algonquian *a). This system was continued down to Common Delaware,[11] but Munsee and Unami have innovated separately with respect to the vowel systems.

Revival attempts edit

The dominant modern version of the Southern Unami dialect called Lenape is taught by the Delaware Tribe of Indians, headquartered in Bartlesville, Oklahoma. They have the Lenape Language Preservation Project.[12] The same dialect was spoken by the Delaware Nation in Anadarko in the southwestern part of Oklahoma. Both Oklahoma and Delaware tribes have recorded native speakers and produced written lessons for instruction, which are available for sale from Various Indian Peoples Publishing Company, which is in Texas. These efforts, in conjunction with other community efforts comprise an attempt to preserve the language.

Some descriptions of the Northern Unami dialect as spoken during the 18th century are given by Moravian missionary John Heckewelder.[13]

Phonology edit

Consonants edit

Unami has been analyzed as having contrastive geminate and non-geminate obstruent consonants, although this contrast is relatively weak.[14] A full analysis of the status of the geminates, also known as long consonants, is not available, and more than one analysis of Delaware consonants has been proposed. The long consonants are described as having low functional yield, and they differentiate relatively few pairs of words but occur in contrasting environments.[15] Some examples of contrastive geminate pairs include: k·ə́ntka·n 'then you (sg.) danced' versus kə́ntka·n 'then there was dancing'; p·ɔ́·m 'his thigh' versus pɔ́·m 'the ham'; and nsa·s·a·k·ənə́mən 'I stuck it out repeatedly' versus nsa·sa·k·ənə́mən 'I stuck it out slowly'.

There are also rules that lengthen consonants in certain environments.[16] The length mark (ː) is used to indicate gemination of a preceding consonant or vowel length, although in the literature on Unami the raised dot (·) is often used for these purposes, as other diacritics may be used above vowels (see below).[17]

In the following chart, the usual transcription used in the sources is given with the IPA in brackets.

Consonants
Bilabial Dental Postalveolar Velar Glottal
Plosive p [p] t [t̪] č [tʃ] k [k]
Fricative s [s] š [ʃ] x [x] h [h]
Nasal m [m] n [n]
Lateral l [l]
Semivowel w [w] j [j]

Vowels edit

Unami vowels are presented as organized into contrasting long–short pairs.[18] One asymmetry is that high short /u/ is paired with long /oː/, and the pairing of long and short /ə/ is noteworthy. /ə/ and /o/ are not distinguishable before /w/, /m/, and /kw/. Additionally, vowels are classified as strong and weak, which plays an important role in determining stress (see below). Long vowels and vowels before consonant clusters are automatically strong. Certain short vowels, which are differentiated with a breve/ĭ, ĕ, ŏ/ – are also strong vowels because they are treated morphophonemically as long vowels, even though they are pronounced as short. In a sequence of syllables containing a short vowel followed by a consonant (C) or consonant and /w/ (Cw), the odd-numbered vowels are weak, and the even numbered vowels are strong.[vague]

Furthermore, some short vowels are strong even in a weakening environment; such exceptions are often marked with a grave accent. Additionally, some vowels which are unaffected by predicted vowel syncope are marked with an acute accent.[19] There is a predictable tendency, additionally, to nasalize and lengthen a vowel before /ns/ and /nš/, so that /lowé·nso/ ('his name is [such]') is realized closer to [luwé̹·su] from underlying /ələwe·nsəw/.[20]

Long vowels
Front Central Back
Close
Mid əː ɔː
Open
Short vowels
Front Central Back
Close i u
Mid e ə ɔ
Open a

Syllable structure edit

Syllable structure is diverse, permitting a certain amount of consonant clustering. The following consonant clusters can occur:[21]

  • /m, n/ (which are realized as homorganic nasals) + /p, t, k, s, tʃ, ʃ/
  • /h/ + /p, t, tʃ, k, m, l/
  • /s/ + /p, k/
  • /x/ + /p, k/
  • /tʃ/ + /k/

Additionally, certain consonants may combine with the semivowel /w/. Some underlying forms may also contain /sw/ and /šw/, but these are always removed by morphophonemic processes.[22]

  • /p, m, k, h/ + /w/

Stress edit

Stress is generally predictable in Unami. The rightmost nonfinal strong vowel is stressed, or a strong vowel in final position if it is the only one in the word. Often when stress would be expected to fall on the antepenult it is shifted to the penult. This change is found in three conjunct endings: /-ak/, /-at/, and /-an/. In the last case, the accent shifts to the penultimate /-an/ only if it would otherwise fall on an antepenultimate short vowel, and if the consonant between them is voiced.[23]

Phonological processes edit

Unami phonology is extremely complex, with various morphophonological rules, and a theoretical form usually undergoes a set of predictable phonological processes to produce the true form found in speech. There are about 17 such rules common to both Munsee and Unami, and another 28 unique to Unami, though this analysis ignores predictable exceptions, such as the class of static words which may skip many of these rules. These rules govern things such as consonant lengthening/shortening, vowel syncopation, metathesis, vowel coloring, etc.[24]

A list of processes unique to Unami follow.[25] These are written in linguistic notation. Thus, {ə,a} → ∅ / _{h, x}V when {ə,a} are weak means that the sounds /ə/ and /a/ become null (disappear) in the context of when they are weak and appear before either /h/ or /x/ and another vowel. The slash means 'in the context of', and the underscore _ indicates where the /ə/ or /a/ must occur. In some notations the pound symbol (#) appears, indicating word boundaries (either the beginning or end). Regular parenthesis indicate optional conditions when framing phonemes or additional information about phonemes: "C=stop". The capital letters C, V, and N mean 'consonant', 'vowel', and 'nasal' respectively.

  • U-1: Weak and strong vowel marking
  • U-2: Weak short vowel loss before gutturals /h/ and /x/: /kənalhó·xwe/ ('you walk upstream') versus /nalahó·xwe·(w)/ ('he walks upstream')
    • {ə,a} when /ə,a/ are weak→ ∅ / _{h, x}V
  • U-3: Vowel-coloring; underlying /ə/ may be color to /i/ /o/ or /a/ in various environments.
    • ə → o / _ {(k)w} -- although Goddard notes that this orthography may be imperfect because surface-phonemic /ə/ and /o/ are not distinct before /w/, /m/ and /kw/. Cf. /pko/ ('vegetable gum', underlying /pəkəw/) with /mpok·ó·yom/ ('my gum') but /mpək·ó·he/ ('I gather gum').
    • ə → i / _ {y}
    • ə → o / _ h{p, kw, m, w}
    • ə → i / _ h elsewhere
    • ə → o / _ {Np, Nkw} and // w_Nk
    • ə → i / _ Nk elsewhere
    • ə → o / _x{p, kw}
    • ə → a / _x elsewhere
  • U-4: Vowel shortening before primary cluster of a nasal and another consonant
    • V̅ → V̌ / _ NC
  • U-5: Semivowel assimilation
    • {w, y}h → hh / V̌_
  • U-6: Stop lengthening
    • C^(vl≠h) → C· / V_(s, š, x) where V is strong
  • U-7: /h/-metathesis
    • V̌hC → hV̌C except / VC_ where V is weak
  • U-8: An adjustment in vowel length before /hC/ (an /h/ and another consonant)
    • V → V̌ / _hC (voiced C)
    • V → V̅ / _hC (voiceless C)
  • U-9: /h/-loss before stop consonant
    • hC (C=stop) → C
  • U-10: Nasal assimilation, part 1
    • NC (C=stop) → homorganic nasal + voiced stop
    • NC (C=continuant)nC / V_
  • U-11: Vowel-weakening and syncope (with certain exceptions)
    • a → ah / CV (/a/ is weak; C=voiceless) except some a (a=weak) → ∅ / #n_CV (C=voiceless)
    • ə → ∅ / _CV (/ə/ is weak; C=voiceless) and / l_{n, l}, y_l, w_w, m_m and / #(n)_{n, l}V
  • U-12: Nasal assimilation, part 2
    • nC (C=voiceless; C≠/x,h/) → homorganic nasal + C (C=voiced) / #_
    • VnC → Ṽ·C (most speakers)
  • U-13: Voiced consonant assimilation
    • C(x)C(y) (C=voiced constituant) → CyCy
  • U-14: Vowel syncope before /xCV/, in which exceptions are marked with a grave accent
    • V̌ → ∅ / _{x, s, š}CV except / VC_ (where V=weak)
  • U-15: /h/-loss in clusters
    • h → ∅ / _{CC, C#, Cah}
  • U-16: Vowel shortening
    • V̅ → V̌ / _h(ə)CV and // _CC (C=voiced) and / _C·ah
  • U-17: /mə/-loss
    • V́hməna· → V́hna·
  • U-18: /ə/-insertion
    • ∅ → ə / h_{l, n, m} and / #{l, m}_C (C≠h)
  • U-19: Metathesis of /w/. Does not affect the roots /wəl-/ 'well' and /wət-/ 'pull'.
    • w(ə)C1(w) → Cw / #_V if C1=/p, m, k, h/
    • w(ə)C → Cw / #_{ah, a, a·}
    • {/nəw/, /kəw/} → {nw, kw} / #_{ah, a, a·}
    • w{h, x} → {hw, xw} / C_
    • x → xw / o(·)_
    • (k)wx(k) → (k)x(k)w / #_
  • U-20: /w/-coloring of /a/-vowels and /w/-loss
    • w{ah, a, a·} → {oh, ɔ, ɔ·} / {#,C,V}_ where C is not /w/ and V is not /o(·)/
    • w → ∅ / V1_V2 where V1 is not /o(·)/ and V2 is not /ə/
    • w → ∅ / (C)_o(·)
  • U-21: /y/-adjustment
    • y → ∅ / V̄_V1 where V1 is not /ə/
    • ∅ → y / V1_V2 where V1 is a front vowel and V2 is a back vowel
    • w → y / _k (only in the suffixes 1/-əkw/ and 1/-əke·/)
  • U-22: Final /l/-loss. The dropping of the /l/ is optional, but the option is exploited differently by the two morphemes it affects.
    • l → (l) / _#
  • U-23: Final-vowel shortening
    • V̄ → V̆ / _#
  • U-24: Final /h/-, /w/-, and /y/-loss
    • h → ∅ / _#
    • w → (w) / V̄_#
    • w → ∅ / o(·)_#
    • w → ∅ / #_tə{l, n}
    • y → ∅ / i_#
  • U-25: Initial cluster and syllable loss. Initial clusters arising from morphophonemic rules U-11 and U-14 above are simplified; many initial weak-vowel syllables are lost. There are many exceptions, however, such that Goddard does not attempt to describe the pattern.
  • U-26: Consonant-shortening
    • C·(w) → C(w) / _{ah, oh, C, #}
  • U-27: Consonant-lengthening
    • C (C=stop) → C· / #(C)hV̆_V(C)#
  • U-28: Negative vowel assimilation in forms with 5/-(o·)w(i·)/, which was a recent innovation at the time Goddard was writing.
    • x(w)i → V̄xx / _#

Morphology edit

Nouns edit

Third person participants are marked for gender (animate versus inanimate), obviation (proximate versus obviative), and presence (nonabsentative versus absentative). Generally, the inanimate, obviative, and absentative categories are more marked than their opposites (i.e. animate, proximate, and nonabsentative), but it is not clear whether animacy or inanimacy is the more marked of the opposition. The first and second persons are not marked for presence or obviation and are always animate.[26]

Obviation edit

The first mentioned and/or primary animate third person is proximate; all other third persons are obviative, unless they act in conjunction with the proximate participant. Verbs are also inflected to indicate whether the verbal action is proximate on obviate or obviate on proximate.

Presence edit

Third-person participants can be marked by a special set of endings indicating their absence from the general area of the focus of discourse. For example, absentative endings are used when speaking of the deceased (even if the corpse is physically present), as in the sentence no·lăčahko·ná·na nkahe·səná·na ('our (excl.) mother (abv.) treated us well'), in which both verb and noun are marked with the absentative /-a/ ending.

Gender edit

Nouns in Unami are classified as animate or inanimate, which is reflected in verbal conjugation. Animate nouns denote human beings, animals, spirits, trees, and certain fruits, tubers, root vegetables, and other unpredictable exceptions like ko·n ('snow') and nhíkaš ('my fingernail'). (However, berries, nuts, and vegetables growing above ground are generally inanimate.) Thus, té·hi·m ('strawberry'), xáskwi·m ('corn'), ke·skúnthak ('pumpkin'), mpi ('water'), and nhíka·t ('my leg') are inanimate, while lə́nu ('man'), xho·k ('snake'), mahtán'tu ('Devil') and hɔ́pəni·s ('potato') are animate. However, traditionally inanimate nouns which are directly addressed or personified are treated as animate. Thus, traditionally inanimate ăsǝ́n ('stone') is treated as animate in the sentence šá·i a· ăsǝ́nak kǝnčí·mowak ('the stones would immediately cry out').[27]

Verbs edit

Unami is a highly agglutinative, polysynthetic language. Verbs in Unami are marked for person and number, and contain inflectional elements of order (independent, conjunct, and imperative), aspect, and the negative.[28]

A table of the personal pronouns is given below. The first person plural ("we") may be either inclusive (including the addressee) or exclusive.

Personal pronouns[29]
Person Singular Plural
First ni· ni·ló·na (exclusive) ki·ló·na (inclusive)
Second ki· ki·ló·wa
Third né·k·a ne·k·a·ɔ
Indefinite ∅ (zero ending) ∅ (zero ending)

Following are tables exemplifying verbal paradigms in Unami in the independent order, indicative mood and present tense.[30]

Animate intransitive (AI) verbs edit

Animate intransitive independent indicative
Person /kəntəka·-/
'dance'
/wəm-/
'come from'
1,2 nkə́ntka
/n-,k--/
no·m
/n-,k--/
3 kə́ntke·(w)
/-w/
wəm
/-w/
1p,12 nkəntkáhəna
/n-,k--hməna·/
no·mhóməna
/n-,k--hməna·/
2p kkəntkáhəmɔ
/k--hmwa·/
ko·mhómɔ
/k--hmwa·/
3p kəntké·yɔk
/-wak/
mo·k
/-wak/
X kə́ntkan
/-n/
xahé·lən
/-n/

Transitive animate (TA) verbs edit

Transitive animate independent indicative, theme 1
Objective mi·l-
'give to'
Absolute lo·sw
'burn'
Person 3 3p -
1,2 nǝmí·la
/n-,k--a·/
nǝmi·lá·ɔk
/n-,k--a·wak/
nló·sa
/n-,k--a·/
3 mwi·lá·ɔ
/w--a·wa(l)/
mwi·lá·ɔ
/w--a·wa(l)/
ló·se·(w)
/-e·w/
1p,12 nǝmi·lá·wǝna
/n-,k--a·wǝna·/
nǝmi·la·wǝná·na(·)k
/n-,k--a·wǝnă̆·k/
nlo·sáhǝna
/n-,k--a·hmǝna·/
2p kǝmi·láwwa
/k--a·wǝwa·/
kǝmi·láwwa·ɔk
/k--a·wǝwa·wak/
kǝlo·sáhǝmɔ
/k--a·hmwa·/
3p mwi·lawwá·ɔ
/w--a·wǝwa·wa(l)/
mwi·lawwá·ɔ
/w--a·wǝwa·wa(l)/
lo·sé·yɔk
/-ǝ·wak/
X mí·la·(w)
/a·w/
mí·l·á·ɔk
/a·wak/
N/A

Inanimate intransitive (II) verbs edit

Inanimate intransitive independent indicative
Person màxhe·-
'be red'
alǝt-
'rot'
IN máxke·(w)
/-w/
alǝ́t
/-w/
IN pl. maxké·yɔ(l)
/-wa(l)/
alǝ́t·o(·l)
/-wa(l)/

Transitive inanimate (TI) verbs edit

The TI themes have the same inflection as AI stems for all conjuncts. (Indefinite subject forms of consonant-final themes are not attested, but the vowel-final themes follow the AI pattern.) Three forms are illustrated from each type.

Transitive inanimate independent indicative
Type Subjects
2 3 3pl
1a pǝnáman
/-aman/
pǝnánk
/-ank/
pǝnamhíti·t
/-amǝh(ǝ)ti·t/
1b ló·sǝman
/-ǝman/
ló·sink
/-ǝnk/
lo·sǝmíhti·t
/-ǝmǝh(ǝ)ti·t/
2 wǝlí·taɔn
/-awan/
wǝlí·ta·kw
/-a·kw/
wǝli·tóhti·t
/-o·h(ǝ)ti·t/
3(/-C/) né·man
/-an/
nenk
/-ǝk/
ne·mhíti·t
/-ǝ·h(ǝ)ti·t/
3(/-V) mí·č·iyan
/-yan/
mí·č·i·t
/-t/
mi·č·íhti·t
/-h(ǝ)ti·t/

Affixes edit

Prefixes edit

Verbal prefixes are used only in the independent order, although some forms of the independent order lack a prefix. There are three of them: /n-/ (first person), /k-/ (second person), and /w-/ (third person). If a stem has an underlying initial vowel, a /t/ is inserted after the prefix, and before this and other stem-initial consonants a /ə/ is inserted. Sometimes, this /ə/ contracts with a stem-initial /wə/ to /o·/ except when the /o·/ would be phonetically shortened via rule U-4a. Examples include: /ntá·mwi/ ('I get up from lying') versus /á·mwi·(w)/ ('he gets up'). Two roots with initial /t/ extend the syllable with /-ən/ when adding prefixes; these roots are /tal-/ ('there') and /tax-/ ('so many'), e.g. náni ntəntala·wsí·ne·n ('that is where we live [our lives]') from the animate intransitive stem /tala·wəsi·/.[31]

Prefixes are mutually exclusive and are selected based on the following rule: if one of the participants is second person, the second person prefix is used; if not, if one of the participants is the first person, then the first person prefix is used; if none of these applies, other forms, if they take a prefix, take the third person prefix. This is the well-known Algonquian 2-1-3 precedence rule.[32]

Suffixes edit

Suffixes are grouped into eight positional classes. These are:[31]

  • Theme
    1. Theme signs
  • Thematic affixes
  • Diminutive and pejorative
    1. Obviative and plural
    2. Negative; imperative modes
  • Desinences (inflectional suffixes)
    1. Central endings
    2. Aspect
    3. Peripheral endings
    4. Mode
First position edit

The first position (theme signs) is filled only for transitive verbs and help describe the relationship between the two participants by indicating which is the agent and which is the object. The direct and inverse theme signs indicate the direction of the verb along a spectrum what might be called distance. From least to most distant the participants are: (1) first or second; (2) indefinite (only as subject); (3) proximate third person; (4) obviative third person; (5) farther obviative third person; (6) inanimate (subject only). If the subject is less distant than the object, the direct theme is used; if the subject is more distant, the inverse signs are used. After transitive animate (TA) verb stems appear one of the four following theme signs:

  • Theme 1: 1/-a·/ ~ 1/-∅/; makes direct forms
  • Theme 2: 1/-əkw/ ~ 1/-əke·/; makes inverse forms
  • Theme 3: 1/-i·/; makes first person object forms
  • Theme 4: 1/-əl/; makes second person forms

For transitive inanimate (TI) verbs, there appears the theme sign pertaining to the appropriate TI class:

  • Class 1a: 1/-am/
  • Class 1b: 1/-əm/
  • Class 2: 1/-o·/ ~ 1/-aw/
  • Class 3: no theme sign

For Class 2 TI theme signs, in certain derivational categories, the theme sign is retained even when the thematic element is dropped. The contrast between both categories is sharper in the Central Algonquian languages, whose theme sign has a more complex series of alternants.[33]

Second position edit

The second position consists of diminutives in 2/-tī/ and pejoratives in 2/-šī/ (in both of these, the /-ī/ is unstable.) An example of a diminutive is /pé·t·o/ ('the little one comes'), contrasted with the pejorative /pé·šo/ ('the undesirable one came'). The use of /t/ in the formation of diminutives seems to be an innovation of Unami, as many other Algonquian languages use */s/ or */ʃ/ (in fact, the diminutive of Munsee is /-šī/).

Third position edit

In position three are the affixes 3/-lī/, which marks the obviative third person, and 3/-h(ə)tī/, which marks the plural.

Fourth position edit

Position four contains the negative affix 4/-(ō)w(ī)/ and prohibitive imperative and future imperative forms, which have complex series of alternants.

Fifth position edit

Position five contains the central endings which index the central participant of each form, except those using TA theme signs 1/-i·/ and 1/-əl/;

Sixth position edit

Position six contains the affix endings: /-pan/ marks the preterite, and /sa/ ~ /shan/ mark the present.

Seventh position edit

Position seven contains peripheral endings, which are used to mark the nominal category of some 3rd person participants in forms in the independent and conjunct (but not imperative) orders.

Eighth position edit

Position eight reflects the subjunctive, prohibitive, and future modes.

Syntax edit

Unami is, like many Algonquian languages, polysynthetic and highly agglutinative. This means that most of the information is encoded in the verb (sometimes with whole words being incorporated into the stem), making word order more fluid than in English. Syntax is one of the least studied aspects of the Unami language; there is much more data on morphology, because of an especial focus on reconstructing Proto-Algonquian.

Some examples of complex sentences in Unami include:[34]

  • ta heč tɔllí·ksi·n ne·k ma·nšá·p·iyak? ('what color are those beads?')
  • še· lah ni e·k·aɔ́·kwe [tali-]phɔkhakéhɔ·n ní·ša awé·ni·k ('over there under the trees two people were buried')
  • mi·mə́nsak šokw təli-ahi-pe·nháto·n ('but the children's footprints are numerous')
  • na hont náni tənnə́mən níkahke pi·lae·č·əč·ínka ('then that's what those boys did')

Notes edit

  1. ^ a b Chambers, Steve. Star-Ledger. 17 Nov 2002. Retrieved 3 Dec 2022, archived from the original on 4 Jun 2019
  2. ^ Pentland, David, 1992, p. 15; Goddard, Ives, 1996, p. 5
  3. ^ Hammarström, Harald; Forkel, Robert; Haspelmath, Martin; Bank, Sebastian (2022-05-24). "Delawaran". Glottolog. Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology. from the original on 2022-10-30. Retrieved 2022-10-29.
  4. ^ Online Lenape Talking Dictionary, "Unami (a group of the Lenape or Delaware Indians)", [1]
  5. ^ Kraft, Herbert C., The Lenape-Delaware Indian Heritage: 10,000 BC to AD 2000, Lenape Books, p.9, n.2
  6. ^ Lenapehoking Map 2006-01-13 at the Wayback Machine, showing approximate locations of Lenape Indian Bands, Lenape Lifeways website (accessed December 16, 2007)
  7. ^ a b Goddard, Ives. 1996. "Introduction." Ives Goddard, ed., The Handbook of North American Indians, Volume 17. Languages, pp. 1-16. Washington, D.C.: The Smithsonian Institution.
  8. ^ Goddard, Ives, 1978
  9. ^ Pentland, David, 1982
  10. ^ Goddard, Ives. 1982. "Munsee historical phonology." International Journal of American Linguistics 48: 16-48.
  11. ^ Goddard, Ives, 1982; Goddard, Ives, 1979, p. 11,
  12. ^ "Lenape Language Preservation Project." Delaware Tribe of Indians. Retrieved 8 Dec 2013.
  13. ^ Heckewelder, Rev. John: History, Manners, and Customs of the Indian Nations Who Once Inhabited Pennsylvania and the Neighboring States, pages 353-432. Facsimile reprint by Heritage Books, Bowie, MD, 1990, ISBN 1-55613-411-8
  14. ^ Goddard, Ives, 1979, p. vi; for a consonant chart see Ives Goddard, 1997, pp. 43-98.
  15. ^ Goddard, Ives, 1979, pp. 22, xii (rule U-6); p. 26 (rule U-27)
  16. ^ Goddard, Ives, 1979, p. viii
  17. ^ Goddard, Ives, 1979, p. 28
  18. ^ Goddard, Ives, 1997: 45
  19. ^ Goddard, Ives, 1979, p. 21
  20. ^ Goddard, Ives, 1979, p. xv
  21. ^ Goddard, Ives, 1979, pp. 12, 22
  22. ^ Goddard, Ives, 1979, pp. 18-19
  23. ^ Goddard, Ives, 1979, pp. 21, 130
  24. ^ Goddard, Ives, 1979, pp. 12-13
  25. ^ Goddard, Ives, 1979, pp. 20-26
  26. ^ Goddard, Ives, 1979, p. 31-34
  27. ^ Goddard, Ives, 1979, pp. 30–31
  28. ^ Goddard, Ives, 1979, p. 47
  29. ^ Goddard, Ives, 1979, p. 29
  30. ^ Goddard, Ives, 1979, p. 167-191
  31. ^ a b Goddard, Ives, 1979, p. 76
  32. ^ Goddard, Ives, 1979, p. 78
  33. ^ Goddard, Ives, 1979, p. 73
  34. ^ Goddard, Ives, 1979

References edit

  • Goddard, Ives. 1979. Delaware Verbal Morphology. New York: Garland. [published version of 1969 Harvard University dissertation in linguistics] ISBN 978-0-8240-9685-4
  • Goddard, Ives. 1997. "Pidgin Delaware." Sarah G. Thomason, ed., Contact Languages: A Wider Perspective, pp. 43–98. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company. ISBN 978-1-55619-172-5
  • Zeisberger, David (1827). Grammar of the Language of the Lenni Lenape Or Delaware Indians. AMS Press. ISBN 978-0-404-15803-3. Retrieved 25 August 2012.
  • Brinton, Daniel Garrison; Constantine Samuel Rafinesque (1885). The Lenâpé and their legends: with the complete text and symbols of the Walam olum, a new translation, and an inquiry into its authenticity. D.G. Brinton. pp. 97–. Retrieved 25 August 2012.
  • Lenape Texts & Studies - Northern Unami (Mission Delaware) works

External links edit

  • Delaware Nation of Oklahoma
  • Lenape Talking Dictionary
  • Native Languages page for Lenape
  • History of our lord and saviour Jesus Christ Translation of the Gospels into the Delaware language. Printed by Jotham Meeker, Shawnee Baptist Mission.
  • OLAC resources in and about the Unami language

unami, language, unami, delaware, wënami, èlixsuwakàn, algonquian, language, spoken, lenape, people, late, 17th, century, early, 18th, century, what, later, became, southern, thirds, jersey, southeastern, pennsylvania, northern, thirds, delaware, later, lenape. Unami Delaware Wenami elixsuwakan 4 was an Algonquian language spoken by the Lenape people in the late 17th century and the early 18th century in what later became the southern two thirds of New Jersey southeastern Pennsylvania and the northern two thirds of Delaware Later the Lenape peoples migrated and settled in Ontario Canada and Oklahoma It is one of the two Delaware languages the other is Munsee The last fluent Unami speaker in the United States Edward Thompson of the federally recognized Delaware Tribe of Indians died on August 31 2002 1 His sister Nora Thompson Dean 1907 1984 provided valuable information about the language to linguists and other scholars UnamiNative toEastern United StatesRegionAround the lower Delaware and Hudson rivers in the United States some Unami groups in OklahomaExtinct2002 1 Language familyAlgic AlgonquianEastern AlgonquianDelawaran 2 3 DelawareUnamiLanguage codesISO 639 3 a href https iso639 3 sil org code unm class extiw title iso639 3 unm unm a Glottologunam1242ELPUnamiMap showing the aboriginal boundaries of Delaware territories with Munsee territory and Unami dialectal divisions indicated The territory of the poorly known Unalachtigo dialect of Unami is not clearly indicated but is presumed to be approximately in the area of Sankhikan on the map This article contains IPA phonetic symbols Without proper rendering support you may see question marks boxes or other symbols instead of Unicode characters For an introductory guide on IPA symbols see Help IPA Lenni Lenape literally means Men of Men but is translated to mean Original People The Lenape names for the areas they inhabited were Scheyichbi i e New Jersey which means water s edge and Lenapehoking meaning in the land of the Delaware Indians It describes the ancient homeland of all Delaware Indians both Unami and Munsee 5 6 The English named the river running through much of the traditional range of the Lenape after the first governor of the Jamestown Colony Lord De La Warr and consequently referred to the people who lived around the river as Delaware Indians Contents 1 History 2 Revival attempts 3 Phonology 3 1 Consonants 3 2 Vowels 3 3 Syllable structure 3 4 Stress 3 5 Phonological processes 4 Morphology 4 1 Nouns 4 1 1 Obviation 4 1 2 Presence 4 1 3 Gender 4 2 Verbs 4 2 1 Animate intransitive AI verbs 4 2 2 Transitive animate TA verbs 4 2 3 Inanimate intransitive II verbs 4 2 4 Transitive inanimate TI verbs 4 3 Affixes 4 3 1 Prefixes 4 3 2 Suffixes 4 3 2 1 First position 4 3 2 2 Second position 4 3 2 3 Third position 4 3 2 4 Fourth position 4 3 2 5 Fifth position 4 3 2 6 Sixth position 4 3 2 7 Seventh position 4 3 2 8 Eighth position 5 Syntax 6 Notes 7 References 8 External linksHistory editUnami is an Eastern Algonquian language The hypothetical common ancestor language from which the Eastern Algonquian languages descend is Proto Eastern Algonquian PEA An intermediate group Delawarean that is a descendant of Proto Eastern Algonquian consists of Mahican and Common Delaware the latter being a further subgroup comprising Munsee Delaware and Unami Delaware 7 The justification for Delawarean as an intermediate subgroup rests upon the high degree of similarity between Mahican and the two Delaware languages but relatively little detailed argumentation in support of Delawarean has been adduced 7 8 9 Compared to Munsee Unami has undergone extensive phonological innovation coupled with morphological regularization 10 The PEA vowel system consisted of four long vowels i o e a and two short vowels a and e The vowel history is as follows i from PEA merger of Proto Algonquian PA i and i to PEA i o from PEA merger of PA o and o e from Proto Algonquian e and a from Proto Algonquian a the short vowels are e from Proto Algonquian e and a from Proto Algonquian a This system was continued down to Common Delaware 11 but Munsee and Unami have innovated separately with respect to the vowel systems Revival attempts editThe dominant modern version of the Southern Unami dialect called Lenape is taught by the Delaware Tribe of Indians headquartered in Bartlesville Oklahoma They have the Lenape Language Preservation Project 12 The same dialect was spoken by the Delaware Nation in Anadarko in the southwestern part of Oklahoma Both Oklahoma and Delaware tribes have recorded native speakers and produced written lessons for instruction which are available for sale from Various Indian Peoples Publishing Company which is in Texas These efforts in conjunction with other community efforts comprise an attempt to preserve the language Some descriptions of the Northern Unami dialect as spoken during the 18th century are given by Moravian missionary John Heckewelder 13 Phonology editConsonants edit Unami has been analyzed as having contrastive geminate and non geminate obstruent consonants although this contrast is relatively weak 14 A full analysis of the status of the geminates also known as long consonants is not available and more than one analysis of Delaware consonants has been proposed The long consonants are described as having low functional yield and they differentiate relatively few pairs of words but occur in contrasting environments 15 Some examples of contrastive geminate pairs include na k e ntka n then you sg danced versus na ke ntka n then there was dancing ni p ɔ m his thigh versus ni pɔ m the ham and nsa s a k ene men I stuck it out repeatedly versus nsa sa k ene men I stuck it out slowly There are also rules that lengthen consonants in certain environments 16 The length mark ː is used to indicate gemination of a preceding consonant or vowel length although in the literature on Unami the raised dot is often used for these purposes as other diacritics may be used above vowels see below 17 In the following chart the usual transcription used in the sources is given with the IPA in brackets Consonants Bilabial Dental Postalveolar Velar GlottalPlosive p p t t c tʃ k k Fricative s s s ʃ x x h h Nasal m m n n Lateral l l Semivowel w w j j Vowels edit Unami vowels are presented as organized into contrasting long short pairs 18 One asymmetry is that high short u is paired with long oː and the pairing of long and short e is noteworthy e and o are not distinguishable before w m and kw Additionally vowels are classified as strong and weak which plays an important role in determining stress see below Long vowels and vowels before consonant clusters are automatically strong Certain short vowels which are differentiated with a breve ĭ ĕ ŏ are also strong vowels because they are treated morphophonemically as long vowels even though they are pronounced as short In a sequence of syllables containing a short vowel followed by a consonant C or consonant and w Cw the odd numbered vowels are weak and the even numbered vowels are strong vague Furthermore some short vowels are strong even in a weakening environment such exceptions are often marked with a grave accent Additionally some vowels which are unaffected by predicted vowel syncope are marked with an acute accent 19 There is a predictable tendency additionally to nasalize and lengthen a vowel before ns and ns so that lowe nso his name is such is realized closer to luwe su from underlying elewe nsew 20 Long vowels Front Central BackClose iː oːMid eː eː ɔːOpen aːShort vowels Front Central BackClose i uMid e e ɔOpen aSyllable structure edit Syllable structure is diverse permitting a certain amount of consonant clustering The following consonant clusters can occur 21 m n which are realized as homorganic nasals p t k s tʃ ʃ h p t tʃ k m l s p k x p k tʃ k Additionally certain consonants may combine with the semivowel w Some underlying forms may also contain sw and sw but these are always removed by morphophonemic processes 22 p m k h w Stress edit Stress is generally predictable in Unami The rightmost nonfinal strong vowel is stressed or a strong vowel in final position if it is the only one in the word Often when stress would be expected to fall on the antepenult it is shifted to the penult This change is found in three conjunct endings ak at and an In the last case the accent shifts to the penultimate an only if it would otherwise fall on an antepenultimate short vowel and if the consonant between them is voiced 23 Phonological processes edit Unami phonology is extremely complex with various morphophonological rules and a theoretical form usually undergoes a set of predictable phonological processes to produce the true form found in speech There are about 17 such rules common to both Munsee and Unami and another 28 unique to Unami though this analysis ignores predictable exceptions such as the class of static words which may skip many of these rules These rules govern things such as consonant lengthening shortening vowel syncopation metathesis vowel coloring etc 24 A list of processes unique to Unami follow 25 These are written in linguistic notation Thus e a h x V when e a are weak means that the sounds e and a become null disappear in the context of when they are weak and appear before either h or x and another vowel The slash means in the context of and the underscore indicates where the e or a must occur In some notations the pound symbol appears indicating word boundaries either the beginning or end Regular parenthesis indicate optional conditions when framing phonemes or additional information about phonemes C stop The capital letters C V and N mean consonant vowel and nasal respectively U 1 Weak and strong vowel marking U 2 Weak short vowel loss before gutturals h and x kenalho xwe you walk upstream versus nalaho xwe w he walks upstream e a when e a are weak h x V U 3 Vowel coloring underlying e may be color to i o or a in various environments e o k w although Goddard notes that this orthography may be imperfect because surface phonemic e and o are not distinct before w m and kw Cf pko vegetable gum underlying pekew with mpok o yom my gum but mpek o he I gather gum e i y e o h p kw m w e i h elsewhere e o Np Nkw and w Nk e i Nk elsewhere e o x p kw e a x elsewhere U 4 Vowel shortening before primary cluster of a nasal and another consonant V V NC U 5 Semivowel assimilation w y h hh V U 6 Stop lengthening C vl h C V s s x where V is strong U 7 h metathesis V hC hV C except VC where V is weak U 8 An adjustment in vowel length before hC an h and another consonant V V hC voiced C V V hC voiceless C U 9 h loss before stop consonant hC C stop C U 10 Nasal assimilation part 1 NC C stop homorganic nasal voiced stop NC C continuant nC V U 11 Vowel weakening and syncope with certain exceptions a ah CV a is weak C voiceless except some a a weak n CV C voiceless e CV e is weak C voiceless and l n l y l w w m m and n n l V U 12 Nasal assimilation part 2 nC C voiceless C x h homorganic nasal C C voiced VnC Ṽ C most speakers U 13 Voiced consonant assimilation C x C y C voiced constituant CyCy U 14 Vowel syncope before xCV in which exceptions are marked with a grave accent V x s s CV except VC where V weak U 15 h loss in clusters h CC C Cah U 16 Vowel shortening V V h e CV and CC C voiced and C ah U 17 me loss V hmena V hna U 18 e insertion e h l n m and l m C C h U 19 Metathesis of w Does not affect the roots wel well and wet pull w e C1 w Cw V if C1 p m k h w e C Cw ah a a new kew nw kw ah a a w h x hw xw C x xw o k wx k k x k w U 20 w coloring of a vowels and w loss w ah a a oh ɔ ɔ C V where C is not w and V is not o w V1 V2 where V1 is not o and V2 is not e w C o U 21 y adjustment y V V1 where V1 is not e y V1 V2 where V1 is a front vowel and V2 is a back vowel w y k only in the suffixes 1 ekw and 1 eke U 22 Final l loss The dropping of the l is optional but the option is exploited differently by the two morphemes it affects l l U 23 Final vowel shortening V V U 24 Final h w and y loss h w w V w o w te l n y i U 25 Initial cluster and syllable loss Initial clusters arising from morphophonemic rules U 11 and U 14 above are simplified many initial weak vowel syllables are lost There are many exceptions however such that Goddard does not attempt to describe the pattern U 26 Consonant shortening C w C w ah oh C U 27 Consonant lengthening C C stop C C hV V C U 28 Negative vowel assimilation in forms with 5 o w i which was a recent innovation at the time Goddard was writing V x w i V xV x Morphology editNouns edit Third person participants are marked for gender animate versus inanimate obviation proximate versus obviative and presence nonabsentative versus absentative Generally the inanimate obviative and absentative categories are more marked than their opposites i e animate proximate and nonabsentative but it is not clear whether animacy or inanimacy is the more marked of the opposition The first and second persons are not marked for presence or obviation and are always animate 26 Obviation edit The first mentioned and or primary animate third person is proximate all other third persons are obviative unless they act in conjunction with the proximate participant Verbs are also inflected to indicate whether the verbal action is proximate on obviate or obviate on proximate Presence edit Third person participants can be marked by a special set of endings indicating their absence from the general area of the focus of discourse For example absentative endings are used when speaking of the deceased even if the corpse is physically present as in the sentence no lăcahko na na nkahe sena na our excl mother abv treated us well in which both verb and noun are marked with the absentative a ending Gender edit Nouns in Unami are classified as animate or inanimate which is reflected in verbal conjugation Animate nouns denote human beings animals spirits trees and certain fruits tubers root vegetables and other unpredictable exceptions like ko n snow and nhikas my fingernail However berries nuts and vegetables growing above ground are generally inanimate Thus te hi m strawberry xaskwi m corn ke skunthak pumpkin mpi water and nhika t my leg are inanimate while le nu man xho k snake mahtan tu Devil and hɔ peni s potato are animate However traditionally inanimate nouns which are directly addressed or personified are treated as animate Thus traditionally inanimate ăsǝ n stone is treated as animate in the sentence sa i a ăsǝ nak kǝnci mowak the stones would immediately cry out 27 Verbs edit Unami is a highly agglutinative polysynthetic language Verbs in Unami are marked for person and number and contain inflectional elements of order independent conjunct and imperative aspect and the negative 28 A table of the personal pronouns is given below The first person plural we may be either inclusive including the addressee or exclusive Personal pronouns 29 Person Singular PluralFirst ni ni lo na exclusive ki lo na inclusive Second ki ki lo waThird ne k a ne k a ɔIndefinite zero ending zero ending Following are tables exemplifying verbal paradigms in Unami in the independent order indicative mood and present tense 30 Animate intransitive AI verbs edit Animate intransitive independent indicative Person kenteka dance wem come from 1 2 nke ntka n k no m n k 3 ke ntke w w wem w 1p 12 nkentkahena n k hmena no mhomena n k hmena 2p kkentkahemɔ k hmwa ko mhomɔ k hmwa 3p kentke yɔk wak mo k wak X ke ntkan n xahe len n Transitive animate TA verbs edit Transitive animate independent indicative theme 1 Objective mi l give to Absolute lo sw burn Person 3 3p 1 2 nǝmi la n k a nǝmi la ɔk n k a wak nlo sa n k a 3 mwi la ɔ w a wa l mwi la ɔ w a wa l lo se w e w 1p 12 nǝmi la wǝna n k a wǝna nǝmi la wǝna na k n k a wǝnă k nlo sahǝna n k a hmǝna 2p kǝmi lawwa k a wǝwa kǝmi lawwa ɔk k a wǝwa wak kǝlo sahǝmɔ k a hmwa 3p mwi lawwa ɔ w a wǝwa wa l mwi lawwa ɔ w a wǝwa wa l lo se yɔk ǝ wak X mi la w a w mi l a ɔk a wak N AInanimate intransitive II verbs edit Inanimate intransitive independent indicative Person maxhe be red alǝt rot IN maxke w w alǝ t w IN pl maxke yɔ l wa l alǝ t o l wa l Transitive inanimate TI verbs edit The TI themes have the same inflection as AI stems for all conjuncts Indefinite subject forms of consonant final themes are not attested but the vowel final themes follow the AI pattern Three forms are illustrated from each type Transitive inanimate independent indicative Type Subjects2 3 3pl1a pǝnaman aman pǝnank ank pǝnamhiti t amǝh ǝ ti t 1b lo sǝman ǝman lo sink ǝnk lo sǝmihti t ǝmǝh ǝ ti t 2 wǝli taɔn awan wǝli ta kw a kw wǝli tohti t o h ǝ ti t 3 C ne man an nenk ǝk ne mhiti t ǝ h ǝ ti t 3 V mi c iyan yan mi c i t t mi c ihti t h ǝ ti t Affixes edit Prefixes edit Verbal prefixes are used only in the independent order although some forms of the independent order lack a prefix There are three of them n first person k second person and w third person If a stem has an underlying initial vowel a t is inserted after the prefix and before this and other stem initial consonants a e is inserted Sometimes this e contracts with a stem initial we to o except when the o would be phonetically shortened via rule U 4a Examples include nta mwi I get up from lying versus a mwi w he gets up Two roots with initial t extend the syllable with en when adding prefixes these roots are tal there and tax so many e g nani ntentala wsi ne n that is where we live our lives from the animate intransitive stem tala wesi 31 Prefixes are mutually exclusive and are selected based on the following rule if one of the participants is second person the second person prefix is used if not if one of the participants is the first person then the first person prefix is used if none of these applies other forms if they take a prefix take the third person prefix This is the well known Algonquian 2 1 3 precedence rule 32 Suffixes edit Suffixes are grouped into eight positional classes These are 31 Theme Theme signs Thematic affixes Diminutive and pejorative Obviative and plural Negative imperative modes Desinences inflectional suffixes Central endings Aspect Peripheral endings ModeFirst position edit The first position theme signs is filled only for transitive verbs and help describe the relationship between the two participants by indicating which is the agent and which is the object The direct and inverse theme signs indicate the direction of the verb along a spectrum what might be called distance From least to most distant the participants are 1 first or second 2 indefinite only as subject 3 proximate third person 4 obviative third person 5 farther obviative third person 6 inanimate subject only If the subject is less distant than the object the direct theme is used if the subject is more distant the inverse signs are used After transitive animate TA verb stems appear one of the four following theme signs Theme 1 1 a 1 makes direct forms Theme 2 1 ekw 1 eke makes inverse forms Theme 3 1 i makes first person object forms Theme 4 1 el makes second person formsFor transitive inanimate TI verbs there appears the theme sign pertaining to the appropriate TI class Class 1a 1 am Class 1b 1 em Class 2 1 o 1 aw Class 3 no theme signFor Class 2 TI theme signs in certain derivational categories the theme sign is retained even when the thematic element is dropped The contrast between both categories is sharper in the Central Algonquian languages whose theme sign has a more complex series of alternants 33 Second position edit The second position consists of diminutives in 2 ti and pejoratives in 2 si in both of these the i is unstable An example of a diminutive is pe t o the little one comes contrasted with the pejorative pe so the undesirable one came The use of t in the formation of diminutives seems to be an innovation of Unami as many other Algonquian languages use s or ʃ in fact the diminutive of Munsee is si Third position edit In position three are the affixes 3 li which marks the obviative third person and 3 h e ti which marks the plural Fourth position edit Position four contains the negative affix 4 ō w i and prohibitive imperative and future imperative forms which have complex series of alternants Fifth position edit Position five contains the central endings which index the central participant of each form except those using TA theme signs 1 i and 1 el Sixth position edit Position six contains the affix endings pan marks the preterite and sa shan mark the present Seventh position edit Position seven contains peripheral endings which are used to mark the nominal category of some 3rd person participants in forms in the independent and conjunct but not imperative orders Eighth position edit Position eight reflects the subjunctive prohibitive and future modes Syntax editUnami is like many Algonquian languages polysynthetic and highly agglutinative This means that most of the information is encoded in the verb sometimes with whole words being incorporated into the stem making word order more fluid than in English Syntax is one of the least studied aspects of the Unami language there is much more data on morphology because of an especial focus on reconstructing Proto Algonquian Some examples of complex sentences in Unami include 34 ta hec tɔlli ksi n ne k ma nsa p iyak what color are those beads se lah ni e k aɔ kwe tali phɔkhakehɔ n ni sa awe ni k over there under the trees two people were buried mi me nsak sokw teli ahi pe nhato n but the children s footprints are numerous na hont nani tenne men nikahke pi lae c ec inka then that s what those boys did Notes edit a b Chambers Steve The vanishing voice of the Lenape Star Ledger 17 Nov 2002 Retrieved 3 Dec 2022 archived from the original on 4 Jun 2019 Pentland David 1992 p 15 Goddard Ives 1996 p 5 Hammarstrom Harald Forkel Robert Haspelmath Martin Bank Sebastian 2022 05 24 Delawaran Glottolog Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology Archived from the original on 2022 10 30 Retrieved 2022 10 29 Online Lenape Talking Dictionary Unami a group of the Lenape or Delaware Indians 1 Kraft Herbert C The Lenape Delaware Indian Heritage 10 000 BC to AD 2000 Lenape Books p 9 n 2 Lenapehoking Map Archived 2006 01 13 at the Wayback Machine showing approximate locations of Lenape Indian Bands Lenape Lifeways website accessed December 16 2007 a b Goddard Ives 1996 Introduction Ives Goddard ed The Handbook of North American Indians Volume 17 Languages pp 1 16 Washington D C The Smithsonian Institution Goddard Ives 1978 Pentland David 1982 Goddard Ives 1982 Munsee historical phonology International Journal of American Linguistics 48 16 48 Goddard Ives 1982 Goddard Ives 1979 p 11 Lenape Language Preservation Project Delaware Tribe of Indians Retrieved 8 Dec 2013 Heckewelder Rev John History Manners and Customs of the Indian Nations Who Once Inhabited Pennsylvania and the Neighboring States pages 353 432 Facsimile reprint by Heritage Books Bowie MD 1990 ISBN 1 55613 411 8 Goddard Ives 1979 p vi for a consonant chart see Ives Goddard 1997 pp 43 98 Goddard Ives 1979 pp 22 xii rule U 6 p 26 rule U 27 Goddard Ives 1979 p viii Goddard Ives 1979 p 28 Goddard Ives 1997 45 Goddard Ives 1979 p 21 Goddard Ives 1979 p xv Goddard Ives 1979 pp 12 22 Goddard Ives 1979 pp 18 19 Goddard Ives 1979 pp 21 130 Goddard Ives 1979 pp 12 13 Goddard Ives 1979 pp 20 26 Goddard Ives 1979 p 31 34 Goddard Ives 1979 pp 30 31 Goddard Ives 1979 p 47 Goddard Ives 1979 p 29 Goddard Ives 1979 p 167 191 a b Goddard Ives 1979 p 76 Goddard Ives 1979 p 78 Goddard Ives 1979 p 73 Goddard Ives 1979References editGoddard Ives 1979 Delaware Verbal Morphology New York Garland published version of 1969 Harvard University dissertation in linguistics ISBN 978 0 8240 9685 4 Goddard Ives 1997 Pidgin Delaware Sarah G Thomason ed Contact Languages A Wider Perspective pp 43 98 Amsterdam John Benjamins Publishing Company ISBN 978 1 55619 172 5 Zeisberger David 1827 Grammar of the Language of the Lenni Lenape Or Delaware Indians AMS Press ISBN 978 0 404 15803 3 Retrieved 25 August 2012 Brinton Daniel Garrison Constantine Samuel Rafinesque 1885 The Lenape and their legends with the complete text and symbols of the Walam olum a new translation and an inquiry into its authenticity D G Brinton pp 97 Retrieved 25 August 2012 Lenape Texts amp Studies Northern Unami Mission Delaware worksExternal links edit nbsp Unami language test of Wikipedia at Wikimedia Incubator Delaware Lenape Tribe of Indians Delaware Nation of Oklahoma Lenape Talking Dictionary Native Languages page for Lenape History of our lord and saviour Jesus Christ Translation of the Gospels into the Delaware language Printed by Jotham Meeker Shawnee Baptist Mission OLAC resources in and about the Unami language Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Unami language amp oldid 1186987009, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.