fbpx
Wikipedia

Language processing in the brain

In psycholinguistics, language processing refers to the way humans use words to communicate ideas and feelings, and how such communications are processed and understood. Language processing is considered to be a uniquely human ability that is not produced with the same grammatical understanding or systematicity in even human's closest primate relatives.[1]

Dual stream connectivity between the auditory cortex and frontal lobe of monkeys and humans. Top: The auditory cortex of the monkey (left) and human (right) is schematically depicted on the supratemporal plane and observed from above (with the parieto- frontal operculi removed). Bottom: The brain of the monkey (left) and human (right) is schematically depicted and displayed from the side. Orange frames mark the region of the auditory cortex, which is displayed in the top sub-figures. Top and Bottom: Blue colors mark regions affiliated with the ADS, and red colors mark regions affiliated with the AVS (dark red and blue regions mark the primary auditory fields). Material was copied from this source, which is available under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.

Throughout the 20th century the dominant model[2] for language processing in the brain was the Geschwind-Lichteim-Wernicke model, which is based primarily on the analysis of brain-damaged patients. However, due to improvements in intra-cortical electrophysiological recordings of monkey and human brains, as well non-invasive techniques such as fMRI, PET, MEG and EEG, a dual auditory pathway[3][4] has been revealed and a two-streams model has been developed. In accordance with this model, there are two pathways that connect the auditory cortex to the frontal lobe, each pathway accounting for different linguistic roles. The auditory ventral stream pathway is responsible for sound recognition, and is accordingly known as the auditory 'what' pathway. The auditory dorsal stream in both humans and non-human primates is responsible for sound localization, and is accordingly known as the auditory 'where' pathway. In humans, this pathway (especially in the left hemisphere) is also responsible for speech production, speech repetition, lip-reading, and phonological working memory and long-term memory. In accordance with the 'from where to what' model of language evolution,[5][6] the reason the ADS is characterized with such a broad range of functions is that each indicates a different stage in language evolution.

The division of the two streams first occurs in the auditory nerve where the anterior branch enters the anterior cochlear nucleus in the brainstem which gives rise to the auditory ventral stream. The posterior branch enters the dorsal and posteroventral cochlear nucleus to give rise to the auditory dorsal stream.[7]: 8 

Language processing can also occur in relation to signed languages or written content.

Early neurolinguistics models edit

 
Language Areas of the human brain. The angular gyrus is represented in orange, supramarginal gyrus is represented in yellow, Broca's area is represented in blue, Wernicke's area is represented in green and the primary auditory cortex is represented in pink.

Throughout the 20th century, our knowledge of language processing in the brain was dominated by the Wernicke-Lichtheim-Geschwind model.[8][2][9] The Wernicke-Lichtheim-Geschwind model is primarily based on research conducted on brain-damaged individuals who were reported to possess a variety of language related disorders. In accordance with this model, words are perceived via a specialized word reception center (Wernicke's area) that is located in the left temporoparietal junction. This region then projects to a word production center (Broca's area) that is located in the left inferior frontal gyrus. Because almost all language input was thought to funnel via Wernicke's area and all language output to funnel via Broca's area, it became extremely difficult to identify the basic properties of each region. This lack of clear definition for the contribution of Wernicke's and Broca's regions to human language rendered it extremely difficult to identify their homologues in other primates.[10] With the advent of the fMRI and its application for lesion mappings, however, it was shown that this model is based on incorrect correlations between symptoms and lesions.[11][12][13][14][15][16][17] The refutation of such an influential and dominant model opened the door to new models of language processing in the brain.

Current neurolinguistics models edit

Anatomy edit

In the last two decades, significant advances occurred in our understanding of the neural processing of sounds in primates. Initially by recording of neural activity in the auditory cortices of monkeys[18][19] and later elaborated via histological staining[20][21][22] and fMRI scanning studies,[23] 3 auditory fields were identified in the primary auditory cortex, and 9 associative auditory fields were shown to surround them (Figure 1 top left). Anatomical tracing and lesion studies further indicated of a separation between the anterior and posterior auditory fields, with the anterior primary auditory fields (areas R-RT) projecting to the anterior associative auditory fields (areas AL-RTL), and the posterior primary auditory field (area A1) projecting to the posterior associative auditory fields (areas CL-CM).[20][24][25][26] Recently, evidence accumulated that indicates homology between the human and monkey auditory fields. In humans, histological staining studies revealed two separate auditory fields in the primary auditory region of Heschl's gyrus,[27][28] and by mapping the tonotopic organization of the human primary auditory fields with high resolution fMRI and comparing it to the tonotopic organization of the monkey primary auditory fields, homology was established between the human anterior primary auditory field and monkey area R (denoted in humans as area hR) and the human posterior primary auditory field and the monkey area A1 (denoted in humans as area hA1).[29][30][31][32][33] Intra-cortical recordings from the human auditory cortex further demonstrated similar patterns of connectivity to the auditory cortex of the monkey. Recording from the surface of the auditory cortex (supra-temporal plane) reported that the anterior Heschl's gyrus (area hR) projects primarily to the middle-anterior superior temporal gyrus (mSTG-aSTG) and the posterior Heschl's gyrus (area hA1) projects primarily to the posterior superior temporal gyrus (pSTG) and the planum temporale (area PT; Figure 1 top right).[34][35] Consistent with connections from area hR to the aSTG and hA1 to the pSTG is an fMRI study of a patient with impaired sound recognition (auditory agnosia), who was shown with reduced bilateral activation in areas hR and aSTG but with spared activation in the mSTG-pSTG.[36] This connectivity pattern is also corroborated by a study that recorded activation from the lateral surface of the auditory cortex and reported of simultaneous non-overlapping activation clusters in the pSTG and mSTG-aSTG while listening to sounds.[37]

Downstream to the auditory cortex, anatomical tracing studies in monkeys delineated projections from the anterior associative auditory fields (areas AL-RTL) to ventral prefrontal and premotor cortices in the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG)[38][39] and amygdala.[40] Cortical recording and functional imaging studies in macaque monkeys further elaborated on this processing stream by showing that acoustic information flows from the anterior auditory cortex to the temporal pole (TP) and then to the IFG.[41][42][43][44][45][46] This pathway is commonly referred to as the auditory ventral stream (AVS; Figure 1, bottom left-red arrows). In contrast to the anterior auditory fields, tracing studies reported that the posterior auditory fields (areas CL-CM) project primarily to dorsolateral prefrontal and premotor cortices (although some projections do terminate in the IFG.[47][39] Cortical recordings and anatomical tracing studies in monkeys further provided evidence that this processing stream flows from the posterior auditory fields to the frontal lobe via a relay station in the intra-parietal sulcus (IPS).[48][49][50][51][52][53] This pathway is commonly referred to as the auditory dorsal stream (ADS; Figure 1, bottom left-blue arrows). Comparing the white matter pathways involved in communication in humans and monkeys with diffusion tensor imaging techniques indicates of similar connections of the AVS and ADS in the two species (Monkey,[52] Human[54][55][56][57][58][59]). In humans, the pSTG was shown to project to the parietal lobe (sylvian parietal-temporal junction-inferior parietal lobule; Spt-IPL), and from there to dorsolateral prefrontal and premotor cortices (Figure 1, bottom right-blue arrows), and the aSTG was shown to project to the anterior temporal lobe (middle temporal gyrus-temporal pole; MTG-TP) and from there to the IFG (Figure 1 bottom right-red arrows).

Auditory ventral stream edit

The auditory ventral stream (AVS) connects the auditory cortex with the middle temporal gyrus and temporal pole, which in turn connects with the inferior frontal gyrus. This pathway is responsible for sound recognition, and is accordingly known as the auditory 'what' pathway. The functions of the AVS include the following.

Sound recognition edit

Accumulative converging evidence indicates that the AVS is involved in recognizing auditory objects. At the level of the primary auditory cortex, recordings from monkeys showed higher percentage of neurons selective for learned melodic sequences in area R than area A1,[60] and a study in humans demonstrated more selectivity for heard syllables in the anterior Heschl's gyrus (area hR) than posterior Heschl's gyrus (area hA1).[61] In downstream associative auditory fields, studies from both monkeys and humans reported that the border between the anterior and posterior auditory fields (Figure 1-area PC in the monkey and mSTG in the human) processes pitch attributes that are necessary for the recognition of auditory objects.[18] The anterior auditory fields of monkeys were also demonstrated with selectivity for con-specific vocalizations with intra-cortical recordings.[41][19][62] and functional imaging[63][42][43] One fMRI monkey study further demonstrated a role of the aSTG in the recognition of individual voices.[42] The role of the human mSTG-aSTG in sound recognition was demonstrated via functional imaging studies that correlated activity in this region with isolation of auditory objects from background noise,[64][65] and with the recognition of spoken words,[66][67][68][69][70][71][72] voices,[73] melodies,[74][75] environmental sounds,[76][77][78] and non-speech communicative sounds.[79] A meta-analysis of fMRI studies[80] further demonstrated functional dissociation between the left mSTG and aSTG, with the former processing short speech units (phonemes) and the latter processing longer units (e.g., words, environmental sounds). A study that recorded neural activity directly from the left pSTG and aSTG reported that the aSTG, but not pSTG, was more active when the patient listened to speech in her native language than unfamiliar foreign language.[81] Consistently, electro stimulation to the aSTG of this patient resulted in impaired speech perception[81] (see also[82][83] for similar results). Intra-cortical recordings from the right and left aSTG further demonstrated that speech is processed laterally to music.[81] An fMRI study of a patient with impaired sound recognition (auditory agnosia) due to brainstem damage was also shown with reduced activation in areas hR and aSTG of both hemispheres when hearing spoken words and environmental sounds.[36] Recordings from the anterior auditory cortex of monkeys while maintaining learned sounds in working memory,[46] and the debilitating effect of induced lesions to this region on working memory recall,[84][85][86] further implicate the AVS in maintaining the perceived auditory objects in working memory. In humans, area mSTG-aSTG was also reported active during rehearsal of heard syllables with MEG.[87] and fMRI[88] The latter study further demonstrated that working memory in the AVS is for the acoustic properties of spoken words and that it is independent to working memory in the ADS, which mediates inner speech. Working memory studies in monkeys also suggest that in monkeys, in contrast to humans, the AVS is the dominant working memory store.[89]

In humans, downstream to the aSTG, the MTG and TP are thought to constitute the semantic lexicon, which is a long-term memory repository of audio-visual representations that are interconnected on the basis of semantic relationships. (See also the reviews by[3][4] discussing this topic). The primary evidence for this role of the MTG-TP is that patients with damage to this region (e.g., patients with semantic dementia or herpes simplex virus encephalitis) are reported[90][91] with an impaired ability to describe visual and auditory objects and a tendency to commit semantic errors when naming objects (i.e., semantic paraphasia). Semantic paraphasias were also expressed by aphasic patients with left MTG-TP damage[14][92] and were shown to occur in non-aphasic patients after electro-stimulation to this region.[93][83] or the underlying white matter pathway[94] Two meta-analyses of the fMRI literature also reported that the anterior MTG and TP were consistently active during semantic analysis of speech and text;[66][95] and an intra-cortical recording study correlated neural discharge in the MTG with the comprehension of intelligible sentences.[96]

Sentence comprehension edit

In addition to extracting meaning from sounds, the MTG-TP region of the AVS appears to have a role in sentence comprehension, possibly by merging concepts together (e.g., merging the concept 'blue' and 'shirt' to create the concept of a 'blue shirt'). The role of the MTG in extracting meaning from sentences has been demonstrated in functional imaging studies reporting stronger activation in the anterior MTG when proper sentences are contrasted with lists of words, sentences in a foreign or nonsense language, scrambled sentences, sentences with semantic or syntactic violations and sentence-like sequences of environmental sounds.[97][98][99][100][101][102][103][104] One fMRI study[105] in which participants were instructed to read a story further correlated activity in the anterior MTG with the amount of semantic and syntactic content each sentence contained. An EEG study[106] that contrasted cortical activity while reading sentences with and without syntactic violations in healthy participants and patients with MTG-TP damage, concluded that the MTG-TP in both hemispheres participate in the automatic (rule based) stage of syntactic analysis (ELAN component), and that the left MTG-TP is also involved in a later controlled stage of syntax analysis (P600 component). Patients with damage to the MTG-TP region have also been reported with impaired sentence comprehension.[14][107][108] See review[109] for more information on this topic.

Bilaterality edit

In contradiction to the Wernicke-Lichtheim-Geschwind model that implicates sound recognition to occur solely in the left hemisphere, studies that examined the properties of the right or left hemisphere in isolation via unilateral hemispheric anesthesia (i.e., the WADA procedure[110]) or intra-cortical recordings from each hemisphere[96] provided evidence that sound recognition is processed bilaterally. Moreover, a study that instructed patients with disconnected hemispheres (i.e., split-brain patients) to match spoken words to written words presented to the right or left hemifields, reported vocabulary in the right hemisphere that almost matches in size with the left hemisphere[111] (The right hemisphere vocabulary was equivalent to the vocabulary of a healthy 11-years old child). This bilateral recognition of sounds is also consistent with the finding that unilateral lesion to the auditory cortex rarely results in deficit to auditory comprehension (i.e., auditory agnosia), whereas a second lesion to the remaining hemisphere (which could occur years later) does.[112][113] Finally, as mentioned earlier, an fMRI scan of an auditory agnosia patient demonstrated bilateral reduced activation in the anterior auditory cortices,[36] and bilateral electro-stimulation to these regions in both hemispheres resulted with impaired speech recognition.[81]

Auditory dorsal stream edit

The auditory dorsal stream connects the auditory cortex with the parietal lobe, which in turn connects with inferior frontal gyrus. In both humans and non-human primates, the auditory dorsal stream is responsible for sound localization, and is accordingly known as the auditory 'where' pathway. In humans, this pathway (especially in the left hemisphere) is also responsible for speech production, speech repetition, lip-reading, and phonological working memory and long-term memory.

Speech production edit

Studies of present-day humans have demonstrated a role for the ADS in speech production, particularly in the vocal expression of the names of objects. For instance, in a series of studies in which sub-cortical fibers were directly stimulated[94] interference in the left pSTG and IPL resulted in errors during object-naming tasks, and interference in the left IFG resulted in speech arrest. Magnetic interference in the pSTG and IFG of healthy participants also produced speech errors and speech arrest, respectively[114][115] One study has also reported that electrical stimulation of the left IPL caused patients to believe that they had spoken when they had not and that IFG stimulation caused patients to unconsciously move their lips.[116] The contribution of the ADS to the process of articulating the names of objects could be dependent on the reception of afferents from the semantic lexicon of the AVS, as an intra-cortical recording study reported of activation in the posterior MTG prior to activation in the Spt-IPL region when patients named objects in pictures[117] Intra-cortical electrical stimulation studies also reported that electrical interference to the posterior MTG was correlated with impaired object naming[118][82]

Vocal mimicry edit

Although sound perception is primarily ascribed with the AVS, the ADS appears associated with several aspects of speech perception. For instance, in a meta-analysis of fMRI studies[119] (Turkeltaub and Coslett, 2010), in which the auditory perception of phonemes was contrasted with closely matching sounds, and the studies were rated for the required level of attention, the authors concluded that attention to phonemes correlates with strong activation in the pSTG-pSTS region. An intra-cortical recording study in which participants were instructed to identify syllables also correlated the hearing of each syllable with its own activation pattern in the pSTG.[120] The involvement of the ADS in both speech perception and production has been further illuminated in several pioneering functional imaging studies that contrasted speech perception with overt or covert speech production.[121][122][123] These studies demonstrated that the pSTS is active only during the perception of speech, whereas area Spt is active during both the perception and production of speech. The authors concluded that the pSTS projects to area Spt, which converts the auditory input into articulatory movements.[124][125] Similar results have been obtained in a study in which participants' temporal and parietal lobes were electrically stimulated. This study reported that electrically stimulating the pSTG region interferes with sentence comprehension and that stimulation of the IPL interferes with the ability to vocalize the names of objects.[83] The authors also reported that stimulation in area Spt and the inferior IPL induced interference during both object-naming and speech-comprehension tasks. The role of the ADS in speech repetition is also congruent with the results of the other functional imaging studies that have localized activation during speech repetition tasks to ADS regions.[126][127][128] An intra-cortical recording study that recorded activity throughout most of the temporal, parietal and frontal lobes also reported activation in the pSTG, Spt, IPL and IFG when speech repetition is contrasted with speech perception.[129] Neuropsychological studies have also found that individuals with speech repetition deficits but preserved auditory comprehension (i.e., conduction aphasia) suffer from circumscribed damage to the Spt-IPL area[130][131][132][133][134][135][136] or damage to the projections that emanate from this area and target the frontal lobe[137][138][139][140] Studies have also reported a transient speech repetition deficit in patients after direct intra-cortical electrical stimulation to this same region.[11][141][142] Insight into the purpose of speech repetition in the ADS is provided by longitudinal studies of children that correlated the learning of foreign vocabulary with the ability to repeat nonsense words.[143][144]

Speech monitoring edit

In addition to repeating and producing speech, the ADS appears to have a role in monitoring the quality of the speech output. Neuroanatomical evidence suggests that the ADS is equipped with descending connections from the IFG to the pSTG that relay information about motor activity (i.e., corollary discharges) in the vocal apparatus (mouth, tongue, vocal folds). This feedback marks the sound perceived during speech production as self-produced and can be used to adjust the vocal apparatus to increase the similarity between the perceived and emitted calls. Evidence for descending connections from the IFG to the pSTG has been offered by a study that electrically stimulated the IFG during surgical operations and reported the spread of activation to the pSTG-pSTS-Spt region[145] A study[146] that compared the ability of aphasic patients with frontal, parietal or temporal lobe damage to quickly and repeatedly articulate a string of syllables reported that damage to the frontal lobe interfered with the articulation of both identical syllabic strings ("Bababa") and non-identical syllabic strings ("Badaga"), whereas patients with temporal or parietal lobe damage only exhibited impairment when articulating non-identical syllabic strings. Because the patients with temporal and parietal lobe damage were capable of repeating the syllabic string in the first task, their speech perception and production appears to be relatively preserved, and their deficit in the second task is therefore due to impaired monitoring. Demonstrating the role of the descending ADS connections in monitoring emitted calls, an fMRI study instructed participants to speak under normal conditions or when hearing a modified version of their own voice (delayed first formant) and reported that hearing a distorted version of one's own voice results in increased activation in the pSTG.[147] Further demonstrating that the ADS facilitates motor feedback during mimicry is an intra-cortical recording study that contrasted speech perception and repetition.[129] The authors reported that, in addition to activation in the IPL and IFG, speech repetition is characterized by stronger activation in the pSTG than during speech perception.

Integration of phonemes with lip-movements edit

Although sound perception is primarily ascribed with the AVS, the ADS appears associated with several aspects of speech perception. For instance, in a meta-analysis of fMRI studies[119] in which the auditory perception of phonemes was contrasted with closely matching sounds, and the studies were rated for the required level of attention, the authors concluded that attention to phonemes correlates with strong activation in the pSTG-pSTS region. An intra-cortical recording study in which participants were instructed to identify syllables also correlated the hearing of each syllable with its own activation pattern in the pSTG.[148] Consistent with the role of the ADS in discriminating phonemes,[119] studies have ascribed the integration of phonemes and their corresponding lip movements (i.e., visemes) to the pSTS of the ADS. For example, an fMRI study[149] has correlated activation in the pSTS with the McGurk illusion (in which hearing the syllable "ba" while seeing the viseme "ga" results in the perception of the syllable "da"). Another study has found that using magnetic stimulation to interfere with processing in this area further disrupts the McGurk illusion.[150] The association of the pSTS with the audio-visual integration of speech has also been demonstrated in a study that presented participants with pictures of faces and spoken words of varying quality. The study reported that the pSTS selects for the combined increase of the clarity of faces and spoken words.[151] Corroborating evidence has been provided by an fMRI study[152] that contrasted the perception of audio-visual speech with audio-visual non-speech (pictures and sounds of tools). This study reported the detection of speech-selective compartments in the pSTS. In addition, an fMRI study[153] that contrasted congruent audio-visual speech with incongruent speech (pictures of still faces) reported pSTS activation. For a review presenting additional converging evidence regarding the role of the pSTS and ADS in phoneme-viseme integration see.[154]

Phonological long-term memory edit

A growing body of evidence indicates that humans, in addition to having a long-term store for word meanings located in the MTG-TP of the AVS (i.e., the semantic lexicon), also have a long-term store for the names of objects located in the Spt-IPL region of the ADS (i.e., the phonological lexicon). For example, a study[155][156] examining patients with damage to the AVS (MTG damage) or damage to the ADS (IPL damage) reported that MTG damage results in individuals incorrectly identifying objects (e.g., calling a "goat" a "sheep," an example of semantic paraphasia). Conversely, IPL damage results in individuals correctly identifying the object but incorrectly pronouncing its name (e.g., saying "gof" instead of "goat," an example of phonemic paraphasia). Semantic paraphasia errors have also been reported in patients receiving intra-cortical electrical stimulation of the AVS (MTG), and phonemic paraphasia errors have been reported in patients whose ADS (pSTG, Spt, and IPL) received intra-cortical electrical stimulation.[83][157][94] Further supporting the role of the ADS in object naming is an MEG study that localized activity in the IPL during the learning and during the recall of object names.[158] A study that induced magnetic interference in participants' IPL while they answered questions about an object reported that the participants were capable of answering questions regarding the object's characteristics or perceptual attributes but were impaired when asked whether the word contained two or three syllables.[159] An MEG study has also correlated recovery from anomia (a disorder characterized by an impaired ability to name objects) with changes in IPL activation.[160] Further supporting the role of the IPL in encoding the sounds of words are studies reporting that, compared to monolinguals, bilinguals have greater cortical density in the IPL but not the MTG.[161][162] Because evidence shows that, in bilinguals, different phonological representations of the same word share the same semantic representation,[163] this increase in density in the IPL verifies the existence of the phonological lexicon: the semantic lexicon of bilinguals is expected to be similar in size to the semantic lexicon of monolinguals, whereas their phonological lexicon should be twice the size. Consistent with this finding, cortical density in the IPL of monolinguals also correlates with vocabulary size.[164][165] Notably, the functional dissociation of the AVS and ADS in object-naming tasks is supported by cumulative evidence from reading research showing that semantic errors are correlated with MTG impairment and phonemic errors with IPL impairment. Based on these associations, the semantic analysis of text has been linked to the inferior-temporal gyrus and MTG, and the phonological analysis of text has been linked to the pSTG-Spt- IPL[166][167][168]

Phonological working memory edit

Working memory is often treated as the temporary activation of the representations stored in long-term memory that are used for speech (phonological representations). This sharing of resources between working memory and speech is evident by the finding[169][170] that speaking during rehearsal results in a significant reduction in the number of items that can be recalled from working memory (articulatory suppression). The involvement of the phonological lexicon in working memory is also evidenced by the tendency of individuals to make more errors when recalling words from a recently learned list of phonologically similar words than from a list of phonologically dissimilar words (the phonological similarity effect).[169] Studies have also found that speech errors committed during reading are remarkably similar to speech errors made during the recall of recently learned, phonologically similar words from working memory.[171] Patients with IPL damage have also been observed to exhibit both speech production errors and impaired working memory[172][173][174][175] Finally, the view that verbal working memory is the result of temporarily activating phonological representations in the ADS is compatible with recent models describing working memory as the combination of maintaining representations in the mechanism of attention in parallel to temporarily activating representations in long-term memory.[170][176][177][178] It has been argued that the role of the ADS in the rehearsal of lists of words is the reason this pathway is active during sentence comprehension[179] For a review of the role of the ADS in working memory, see.[180]

 
The 'from where to what' model of language evolution hypotheses 7 stages of language evolution.

Linguistic theories edit

Language-processing research informs theories of language. The primary theoretical question is whether linguistic structures follow from the brain structures or vice versa. Externalist models, such as Ferdinand de Saussure's structuralism, argue that language as a social phenomenon is external to the brain. The individual receives the linguistic system from the outside, and the given language shapes the individual's brain.[181]

This idea is opposed by internalist models including Noam Chomsky's transformational generative grammar, George Lakoff's Cognitive Linguistics, and John A. Hawkins's efficiency hypothesis. According to Chomsky, language is acquired from an innate brain structure independently of meaning.[182] Lakoff argues that language emerges from the sensory systems.[183] Hawkins hypothesizes that cross-linguistically prevalent patterns are based on the brain's natural processing preferences.[184]

Additionally, models inspired by Richard Dawkins's memetics, including Construction Grammar and Usage-Based Linguistics, advocate a two-way model arguing that the brain shapes language, and language shapes the brain.[185][186]

Evidence from neuroimaging studies points towards the externalist position. ERP studies suggest that language processing is based on the interaction of syntax and semantics, and the research does not support innate grammatical structures.[187][188] MRI studies suggest that the structural characteristics of the child's first language shapes the processing connectome of the brain.[189] Processing research has failed to find support for the inverse idea that syntactic structures reflect the brain's natural processing preferences cross-linguistically.[190]

The evolution of language edit

The auditory dorsal stream also has non-language related functions, such as sound localization[191][192][193][194][195] and guidance of eye movements.[196][197] Recent studies also indicate a role of the ADS in localization of family/tribe members, as a study[198] that recorded from the cortex of an epileptic patient reported that the pSTG, but not aSTG, is selective for the presence of new speakers. An fMRI[199] study of fetuses at their third trimester also demonstrated that area Spt is more selective to female speech than pure tones, and a sub-section of Spt is selective to the speech of their mother in contrast to unfamiliar female voices.

It is presently unknown why so many functions are ascribed to the human ADS. An attempt to unify these functions under a single framework was conducted in the 'From where to what' model of language evolution[200][201] In accordance with this model, each function of the ADS indicates of a different intermediate phase in the evolution of language. The roles of sound localization and integration of sound location with voices and auditory objects is interpreted as evidence that the origin of speech is the exchange of contact calls (calls used to report location in cases of separation) between mothers and offspring. The role of the ADS in the perception and production of intonations is interpreted as evidence that speech began by modifying the contact calls with intonations, possibly for distinguishing alarm contact calls from safe contact calls. The role of the ADS in encoding the names of objects (phonological long-term memory) is interpreted as evidence of gradual transition from modifying calls with intonations to complete vocal control. The role of the ADS in the integration of lip movements with phonemes and in speech repetition is interpreted as evidence that spoken words were learned by infants mimicking their parents' vocalizations, initially by imitating their lip movements. The role of the ADS in phonological working memory is interpreted as evidence that the words learned through mimicry remained active in the ADS even when not spoken. This resulted with individuals capable of rehearsing a list of vocalizations, which enabled the production of words with several syllables. Further developments in the ADS enabled the rehearsal of lists of words, which provided the infra-structure for communicating with sentences.

Sign language in the brain edit

Neuroscientific research has provided a scientific understanding of how sign language is processed in the brain. There are over 135 discrete sign languages around the world- making use of different accents formed by separate areas of a country.[202]

By resorting to lesion analyses and neuroimaging, neuroscientists have discovered that whether it be spoken or sign language, human brains process language in general, in a similar manner regarding which area of the brain is being used. [202]Lesion analyses are used to examine the consequences of damage to specific brain regions involved in language while neuroimaging explore regions that are engaged in the processing of language.[202]

Previous hypotheses have been made that damage to Broca's area or Wernicke’s area does not affect sign language being perceived; however, it is not the case. Studies have shown that damage to these areas are similar in results in spoken language where sign errors are present and/or repeated. [202]In both types of languages, they are affected by damage to the left hemisphere of the brain rather than the right -usually dealing with the arts.

There are obvious patterns for utilizing and processing language. In sign language, Broca’s area is activated while processing sign language employs Wernicke’s area similar to that of spoken language [202]

There have been other hypotheses about the lateralization of the two hemispheres. Specifically, the right hemisphere was thought to contribute to the overall communication of a language globally whereas the left hemisphere would be dominant in generating the language locally.[203] Through research in aphasias, RHD signers were found to have a problem maintaining the spatial portion of their signs, confusing similar signs at different locations necessary to communicate with another properly.[203] LHD signers, on the other hand, had similar results to those of hearing patients. Furthermore, other studies have emphasized that sign language is present bilaterally but will need to continue researching to reach a conclusion.[203]

Writing in the brain edit

There is a comparatively small body of research on the neurology of reading and writing.[204] Most of the studies performed deal with reading rather than writing or spelling, and the majority of both kinds focus solely on the English language.[205] English orthography is less transparent than that of other languages using a Latin script.[204] Another difficulty is that some studies focus on spelling words of English and omit the few logographic characters found in the script.[204]

In terms of spelling, English words can be divided into three categories – regular, irregular, and “novel words” or “nonwords.” Regular words are those in which there is a regular, one-to-one correspondence between grapheme and phoneme in spelling. Irregular words are those in which no such correspondence exists. Nonwords are those that exhibit the expected orthography of regular words but do not carry meaning, such as nonce words and onomatopoeia.[204]

An issue in the cognitive and neurological study of reading and spelling in English is whether a single-route or dual-route model best describes how literate speakers are able to read and write all three categories of English words according to accepted standards of orthographic correctness. Single-route models posit that lexical memory is used to store all spellings of words for retrieval in a single process. Dual-route models posit that lexical memory is employed to process irregular and high-frequency regular words, while low-frequency regular words and nonwords are processed using a sub-lexical set of phonological rules.[204]

The single-route model for reading has found support in computer modelling studies, which suggest that readers identify words by their orthographic similarities to phonologically alike words.[204] However, cognitive and lesion studies lean towards the dual-route model. Cognitive spelling studies on children and adults suggest that spellers employ phonological rules in spelling regular words and nonwords, while lexical memory is accessed to spell irregular words and high-frequency words of all types.[204] Similarly, lesion studies indicate that lexical memory is used to store irregular words and certain regular words, while phonological rules are used to spell nonwords.[204]

More recently, neuroimaging studies using positron emission tomography and fMRI have suggested a balanced model in which the reading of all word types begins in the visual word form area, but subsequently branches off into different routes depending upon whether or not access to lexical memory or semantic information is needed (which would be expected with irregular words under a dual-route model).[204] A 2007 fMRI study found that subjects asked to produce regular words in a spelling task exhibited greater activation in the left posterior STG, an area used for phonological processing, while the spelling of irregular words produced greater activation of areas used for lexical memory and semantic processing, such as the left IFG and left SMG and both hemispheres of the MTG.[204] Spelling nonwords was found to access members of both pathways, such as the left STG and bilateral MTG and ITG.[204] Significantly, it was found that spelling induces activation in areas such as the left fusiform gyrus and left SMG that are also important in reading, suggesting that a similar pathway is used for both reading and writing.[204]

Far less information exists on the cognition and neurology of non-alphabetic and non-English scripts. Every language has a morphological and a phonological component, either of which can be recorded by a writing system. Scripts recording words and morphemes are considered logographic, while those recording phonological segments, such as syllabaries and alphabets, are phonographic.[205] Most systems combine the two and have both logographic and phonographic characters.[205]

In terms of complexity, writing systems can be characterized as "transparent" or "opaque" and as "shallow" or "deep". A "transparent" system exhibits an obvious correspondence between grapheme and sound, while in an "opaque" system this relationship is less obvious. The terms "shallow" and "deep" refer to the extent that a system's orthography represents morphemes as opposed to phonological segments.[205] Systems that record larger morphosyntactic or phonological segments, such as logographic systems and syllabaries put greater demand on the memory of users.[205] It would thus be expected that an opaque or deep writing system would put greater demand on areas of the brain used for lexical memory than would a system with transparent or shallow orthography.

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ Seidenberg MS, Petitto LA (1987). "Communication, symbolic communication, and language: Comment on Savage-Rumbaugh, McDonald, Sevcik, Hopkins, and Rupert (1986)". Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 116 (3): 279–287. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.116.3.279. S2CID 18329599.
  2. ^ a b Geschwind N (June 1965). "Disconnexion syndromes in animals and man. I". review. Brain. 88 (2): 237–94. doi:10.1093/brain/88.2.237. PMID 5318481.
  3. ^ a b Hickok G, Poeppel D (May 2007). "The cortical organization of speech processing". review. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience. 8 (5): 393–402. doi:10.1038/nrn2113. PMID 17431404. S2CID 6199399.
  4. ^ a b Gow DW (June 2012). "The cortical organization of lexical knowledge: a dual lexicon model of spoken language processing". review. Brain and Language. 121 (3): 273–88. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2012.03.005. PMC 3348354. PMID 22498237.
  5. ^ Poliva O (2017-09-20). "From where to what: a neuroanatomically based evolutionary model of the emergence of speech in humans". review. F1000Research. 4: 67. doi:10.12688/f1000research.6175.3. PMC 5600004. PMID 28928931.   Material was copied from this source, which is available under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.
  6. ^ Poliva O (2016). "From Mimicry to Language: A Neuroanatomically Based Evolutionary Model of the Emergence of Vocal Language". review. Frontiers in Neuroscience. 10: 307. doi:10.3389/fnins.2016.00307. PMC 4928493. PMID 27445676.   Material was copied from this source, which is available under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.
  7. ^ Pickles JO (2015). "Chapter 1: Auditory pathways: anatomy and physiology". In Aminoff MJ, Boller F, Swaab DF (eds.). Handbook of Clinical Neurology. review. Vol. 129. pp. 3–25. doi:10.1016/B978-0-444-62630-1.00001-9. ISBN 978-0-444-62630-1. PMID 25726260.
  8. ^ Lichteim L (1885-01-01). "On Aphasia". Brain. 7 (4): 433–484. doi:10.1093/brain/7.4.433. hdl:11858/00-001M-0000-002C-5780-B.
  9. ^ Wernicke C (1974). Der aphasische Symptomenkomplex. Springer Berlin Heidelberg. pp. 1–70. ISBN 978-3-540-06905-8.
  10. ^ Aboitiz F, García VR (December 1997). "The evolutionary origin of the language areas in the human brain. A neuroanatomical perspective". Brain Research. Brain Research Reviews. 25 (3): 381–96. doi:10.1016/s0165-0173(97)00053-2. PMID 9495565. S2CID 20704891.
  11. ^ a b Anderson JM, Gilmore R, Roper S, Crosson B, Bauer RM, Nadeau S, Beversdorf DQ, Cibula J, Rogish M, Kortencamp S, Hughes JD, Gonzalez Rothi LJ, Heilman KM (October 1999). "Conduction aphasia and the arcuate fasciculus: A reexamination of the Wernicke-Geschwind model". Brain and Language. 70 (1): 1–12. doi:10.1006/brln.1999.2135. PMID 10534369. S2CID 12171982.
  12. ^ DeWitt I, Rauschecker JP (November 2013). "Wernicke's area revisited: parallel streams and word processing". Brain and Language. 127 (2): 181–91. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2013.09.014. PMC 4098851. PMID 24404576.
  13. ^ Dronkers NF (January 2000). "The pursuit of brain-language relationships". Brain and Language. 71 (1): 59–61. doi:10.1006/brln.1999.2212. PMID 10716807. S2CID 7224731.
  14. ^ a b c Dronkers NF, Wilkins DP, Van Valin RD, Redfern BB, Jaeger JJ (May 2004). "Lesion analysis of the brain areas involved in language comprehension". Cognition. 92 (1–2): 145–77. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2003.11.002. hdl:11858/00-001M-0000-0012-6912-A. PMID 15037129. S2CID 10919645.
  15. ^ Mesulam MM, Thompson CK, Weintraub S, Rogalski EJ (August 2015). "The Wernicke conundrum and the anatomy of language comprehension in primary progressive aphasia". Brain. 138 (Pt 8): 2423–37. doi:10.1093/brain/awv154. PMC 4805066. PMID 26112340.
  16. ^ Poeppel D, Emmorey K, Hickok G, Pylkkänen L (October 2012). "Towards a new neurobiology of language". The Journal of Neuroscience. 32 (41): 14125–31. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.3244-12.2012. PMC 3495005. PMID 23055482.
  17. ^ Vignolo LA, Boccardi E, Caverni L (March 1986). "Unexpected CT-scan findings in global aphasia". Cortex; A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior. 22 (1): 55–69. doi:10.1016/s0010-9452(86)80032-6. PMID 2423296. S2CID 4479679.
  18. ^ a b Bendor D, Wang X (August 2006). "Cortical representations of pitch in monkeys and humans". Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 16 (4): 391–9. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2006.07.001. PMC 4325365. PMID 16842992.
  19. ^ a b Rauschecker JP, Tian B, Hauser M (April 1995). "Processing of complex sounds in the macaque nonprimary auditory cortex". Science. 268 (5207): 111–4. Bibcode:1995Sci...268..111R. doi:10.1126/science.7701330. PMID 7701330. S2CID 19590708.
  20. ^ a b de la Mothe LA, Blumell S, Kajikawa Y, Hackett TA (May 2006). "Cortical connections of the auditory cortex in marmoset monkeys: core and medial belt regions". The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 496 (1): 27–71. doi:10.1002/cne.20923. PMID 16528722. S2CID 38393074.
  21. ^ de la Mothe LA, Blumell S, Kajikawa Y, Hackett TA (May 2012). "Cortical connections of auditory cortex in marmoset monkeys: lateral belt and parabelt regions". Anatomical Record. 295 (5): 800–21. doi:10.1002/ar.22451. PMC 3379817. PMID 22461313.
  22. ^ Kaas JH, Hackett TA (October 2000). "Subdivisions of auditory cortex and processing streams in primates". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 97 (22): 11793–9. Bibcode:2000PNAS...9711793K. doi:10.1073/pnas.97.22.11793. PMC 34351. PMID 11050211.
  23. ^ Petkov CI, Kayser C, Augath M, Logothetis NK (July 2006). "Functional imaging reveals numerous fields in the monkey auditory cortex". PLOS Biology. 4 (7): e215. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0040215. PMC 1479693. PMID 16774452.
  24. ^ Morel A, Garraghty PE, Kaas JH (September 1993). "Tonotopic organization, architectonic fields, and connections of auditory cortex in macaque monkeys". The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 335 (3): 437–59. doi:10.1002/cne.903350312. PMID 7693772. S2CID 22872232.
  25. ^ Rauschecker JP, Tian B (October 2000). "Mechanisms and streams for processing of "what" and "where" in auditory cortex". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 97 (22): 11800–6. Bibcode:2000PNAS...9711800R. doi:10.1073/pnas.97.22.11800. PMC 34352. PMID 11050212.
  26. ^ Rauschecker JP, Tian B, Pons T, Mishkin M (May 1997). "Serial and parallel processing in rhesus monkey auditory cortex". The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 382 (1): 89–103. doi:10.1002/(sici)1096-9861(19970526)382:1<89::aid-cne6>3.3.co;2-y. PMID 9136813.
  27. ^ Sweet RA, Dorph-Petersen KA, Lewis DA (October 2005). "Mapping auditory core, lateral belt, and parabelt cortices in the human superior temporal gyrus". The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 491 (3): 270–89. doi:10.1002/cne.20702. PMID 16134138. S2CID 40822276.
  28. ^ Wallace MN, Johnston PW, Palmer AR (April 2002). "Histochemical identification of cortical areas in the auditory region of the human brain". Experimental Brain Research. 143 (4): 499–508. doi:10.1007/s00221-002-1014-z. PMID 11914796. S2CID 24211906.
  29. ^ Da Costa S, van der Zwaag W, Marques JP, Frackowiak RS, Clarke S, Saenz M (October 2011). "Human primary auditory cortex follows the shape of Heschl's gyrus". The Journal of Neuroscience. 31 (40): 14067–75. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.2000-11.2011. PMC 6623669. PMID 21976491.
  30. ^ Humphries C, Liebenthal E, Binder JR (April 2010). "Tonotopic organization of human auditory cortex". NeuroImage. 50 (3): 1202–11. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.046. PMC 2830355. PMID 20096790.
  31. ^ Langers DR, van Dijk P (September 2012). "Mapping the tonotopic organization in human auditory cortex with minimally salient acoustic stimulation". Cerebral Cortex. 22 (9): 2024–38. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhr282. PMC 3412441. PMID 21980020.
  32. ^ Striem-Amit E, Hertz U, Amedi A (March 2011). "Extensive cochleotopic mapping of human auditory cortical fields obtained with phase-encoding fMRI". PLOS ONE. 6 (3): e17832. Bibcode:2011PLoSO...617832S. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0017832. PMC 3063163. PMID 21448274.
  33. ^ Woods DL, Herron TJ, Cate AD, Yund EW, Stecker GC, Rinne T, Kang X (2010). "Functional properties of human auditory cortical fields". Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience. 4: 155. doi:10.3389/fnsys.2010.00155. PMC 3001989. PMID 21160558.
  34. ^ Gourévitch B, Le Bouquin Jeannès R, Faucon G, Liégeois-Chauvel C (March 2008). "Temporal envelope processing in the human auditory cortex: response and interconnections of auditory cortical areas" (PDF). Hearing Research. 237 (1–2): 1–18. doi:10.1016/j.heares.2007.12.003. PMID 18255243. S2CID 15271578.
  35. ^ Guéguin M, Le Bouquin-Jeannès R, Faucon G, Chauvel P, Liégeois-Chauvel C (February 2007). "Evidence of functional connectivity between auditory cortical areas revealed by amplitude modulation sound processing". Cerebral Cortex. 17 (2): 304–13. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhj148. PMC 2111045. PMID 16514106.
  36. ^ a b c Poliva O, Bestelmeyer PE, Hall M, Bultitude JH, Koller K, Rafal RD (September 2015). "Functional Mapping of the Human Auditory Cortex: fMRI Investigation of a Patient with Auditory Agnosia from Trauma to the Inferior Colliculus" (PDF). Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology. 28 (3): 160–80. doi:10.1097/wnn.0000000000000072. PMID 26413744. S2CID 913296.
  37. ^ Chang EF, Edwards E, Nagarajan SS, Fogelson N, Dalal SS, Canolty RT, Kirsch HE, Barbaro NM, Knight RT (June 2011). "Cortical spatio-temporal dynamics underlying phonological target detection in humans". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 23 (6): 1437–46. doi:10.1162/jocn.2010.21466. PMC 3895406. PMID 20465359.
  38. ^ Muñoz M, Mishkin M, Saunders RC (September 2009). "Resection of the medial temporal lobe disconnects the rostral superior temporal gyrus from some of its projection targets in the frontal lobe and thalamus". Cerebral Cortex. 19 (9): 2114–30. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhn236. PMC 2722427. PMID 19150921.
  39. ^ a b Romanski LM, Bates JF, Goldman-Rakic PS (January 1999). "Auditory belt and parabelt projections to the prefrontal cortex in the rhesus monkey". The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 403 (2): 141–57. doi:10.1002/(sici)1096-9861(19990111)403:2<141::aid-cne1>3.0.co;2-v. PMID 9886040. S2CID 42482082.
  40. ^ Tanaka D (June 1976). "Thalamic projections of the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex in the rhesus monkey (Macaca mulatta)". Brain Research. 110 (1): 21–38. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(76)90206-7. PMID 819108. S2CID 21529048.
  41. ^ a b Perrodin C, Kayser C, Logothetis NK, Petkov CI (August 2011). "Voice cells in the primate temporal lobe". Current Biology. 21 (16): 1408–15. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2011.07.028. PMC 3398143. PMID 21835625.
  42. ^ a b c Petkov CI, Kayser C, Steudel T, Whittingstall K, Augath M, Logothetis NK (March 2008). "A voice region in the monkey brain". Nature Neuroscience. 11 (3): 367–74. doi:10.1038/nn2043. PMID 18264095. S2CID 5505773.
  43. ^ a b Poremba A, Malloy M, Saunders RC, Carson RE, Herscovitch P, Mishkin M (January 2004). "Species-specific calls evoke asymmetric activity in the monkey's temporal poles". Nature. 427 (6973): 448–51. Bibcode:2004Natur.427..448P. doi:10.1038/nature02268. PMID 14749833. S2CID 4402126.
  44. ^ Romanski LM, Averbeck BB, Diltz M (February 2005). "Neural representation of vocalizations in the primate ventrolateral prefrontal cortex". Journal of Neurophysiology. 93 (2): 734–47. doi:10.1152/jn.00675.2004. PMID 15371495.
  45. ^ Russ BE, Ackelson AL, Baker AE, Cohen YE (January 2008). "Coding of auditory-stimulus identity in the auditory non-spatial processing stream". Journal of Neurophysiology. 99 (1): 87–95. doi:10.1152/jn.01069.2007. PMC 4091985. PMID 18003874.
  46. ^ a b Tsunada J, Lee JH, Cohen YE (June 2011). "Representation of speech categories in the primate auditory cortex". Journal of Neurophysiology. 105 (6): 2634–46. doi:10.1152/jn.00037.2011. PMC 3118748. PMID 21346209.
  47. ^ Cusick CG, Seltzer B, Cola M, Griggs E (September 1995). "Chemoarchitectonics and corticocortical terminations within the superior temporal sulcus of the rhesus monkey: evidence for subdivisions of superior temporal polysensory cortex". The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 360 (3): 513–35. doi:10.1002/cne.903600312. PMID 8543656. S2CID 42281619.
  48. ^ Cohen YE, Russ BE, Gifford GW, Kiringoda R, MacLean KA (December 2004). "Selectivity for the spatial and nonspatial attributes of auditory stimuli in the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex". The Journal of Neuroscience. 24 (50): 11307–16. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.3935-04.2004. PMC 6730358. PMID 15601937.
  49. ^ Deacon TW (February 1992). "Cortical connections of the inferior arcuate sulcus cortex in the macaque brain". Brain Research. 573 (1): 8–26. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(92)90109-m. ISSN 0006-8993. PMID 1374284. S2CID 20670766.
  50. ^ Lewis JW, Van Essen DC (December 2000). "Corticocortical connections of visual, sensorimotor, and multimodal processing areas in the parietal lobe of the macaque monkey". The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 428 (1): 112–37. doi:10.1002/1096-9861(20001204)428:1<112::aid-cne8>3.0.co;2-9. PMID 11058227. S2CID 16153360.
  51. ^ Roberts AC, Tomic DL, Parkinson CH, Roeling TA, Cutter DJ, Robbins TW, Everitt BJ (May 2007). "Forebrain connectivity of the prefrontal cortex in the marmoset monkey (Callithrix jacchus): an anterograde and retrograde tract-tracing study". The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 502 (1): 86–112. doi:10.1002/cne.21300. PMID 17335041. S2CID 18262007.
  52. ^ a b Schmahmann JD, Pandya DN, Wang R, Dai G, D'Arceuil HE, de Crespigny AJ, Wedeen VJ (March 2007). "Association fibre pathways of the brain: parallel observations from diffusion spectrum imaging and autoradiography". Brain. 130 (Pt 3): 630–53. doi:10.1093/brain/awl359. PMID 17293361.
  53. ^ Seltzer B, Pandya DN (July 1984). "Further observations on parieto-temporal connections in the rhesus monkey". Experimental Brain Research. 55 (2): 301–12. doi:10.1007/bf00237280. PMID 6745368. S2CID 20167953.
  54. ^ Catani M, Jones DK, ffytche DH (January 2005). "Perisylvian language networks of the human brain". Annals of Neurology. 57 (1): 8–16. doi:10.1002/ana.20319. PMID 15597383. S2CID 17743067.
  55. ^ Frey S, Campbell JS, Pike GB, Petrides M (November 2008). "Dissociating the human language pathways with high angular resolution diffusion fiber tractography". The Journal of Neuroscience. 28 (45): 11435–44. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.2388-08.2008. PMC 6671318. PMID 18987180.
  56. ^ Makris N, Papadimitriou GM, Kaiser JR, Sorg S, Kennedy DN, Pandya DN (April 2009). "Delineation of the middle longitudinal fascicle in humans: a quantitative, in vivo, DT-MRI study". Cerebral Cortex. 19 (4): 777–85. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhn124. PMC 2651473. PMID 18669591.
  57. ^ Menjot de Champfleur N, Lima Maldonado I, Moritz-Gasser S, Machi P, Le Bars E, Bonafé A, Duffau H (January 2013). "Middle longitudinal fasciculus delineation within language pathways: a diffusion tensor imaging study in human". European Journal of Radiology. 82 (1): 151–7. doi:10.1016/j.ejrad.2012.05.034. PMID 23084876.
  58. ^ Turken AU, Dronkers NF (2011). "The neural architecture of the language comprehension network: converging evidence from lesion and connectivity analyses". Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience. 5: 1. doi:10.3389/fnsys.2011.00001. PMC 3039157. PMID 21347218.
  59. ^ Saur D, Kreher BW, Schnell S, Kümmerer D, Kellmeyer P, Vry MS, Umarova R, Musso M, Glauche V, Abel S, Huber W, Rijntjes M, Hennig J, Weiller C (November 2008). "Ventral and dorsal pathways for language". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 105 (46): 18035–40. Bibcode:2008PNAS..10518035S. doi:10.1073/pnas.0805234105. PMC 2584675. PMID 19004769.
  60. ^ Yin P, Mishkin M, Sutter M, Fritz JB (December 2008). "Early stages of melody processing: stimulus-sequence and task-dependent neuronal activity in monkey auditory cortical fields A1 and R". Journal of Neurophysiology. 100 (6): 3009–29. doi:10.1152/jn.00828.2007. PMC 2604844. PMID 18842950.
  61. ^ Steinschneider M, Volkov IO, Fishman YI, Oya H, Arezzo JC, Howard MA (February 2005). "Intracortical responses in human and monkey primary auditory cortex support a temporal processing mechanism for encoding of the voice onset time phonetic parameter". Cerebral Cortex. 15 (2): 170–86. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhh120. PMID 15238437.
  62. ^ Russ BE, Ackelson AL, Baker AE, Cohen YE (January 2008). "Coding of auditory-stimulus identity in the auditory non-spatial processing stream". Journal of Neurophysiology. 99 (1): 87–95. doi:10.1152/jn.01069.2007. PMC 4091985. PMID 18003874.
  63. ^ Joly O, Pallier C, Ramus F, Pressnitzer D, Vanduffel W, Orban GA (September 2012). "Processing of vocalizations in humans and monkeys: a comparative fMRI study" (PDF). NeuroImage. 62 (3): 1376–89. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.05.070. PMID 22659478. S2CID 9441377.
  64. ^ Scheich H, Baumgart F, Gaschler-Markefski B, Tegeler C, Tempelmann C, Heinze HJ, Schindler F, Stiller D (February 1998). "Functional magnetic resonance imaging of a human auditory cortex area involved in foreground-background decomposition". The European Journal of Neuroscience. 10 (2): 803–9. doi:10.1046/j.1460-9568.1998.00086.x. PMID 9749748. S2CID 42898063.
  65. ^ Zatorre RJ, Bouffard M, Belin P (April 2004). "Sensitivity to auditory object features in human temporal neocortex". The Journal of Neuroscience. 24 (14): 3637–42. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.5458-03.2004. PMC 6729744. PMID 15071112.
  66. ^ a b Binder JR, Desai RH, Graves WW, Conant LL (December 2009). "Where is the semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies". Cerebral Cortex. 19 (12): 2767–96. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhp055. PMC 2774390. PMID 19329570.
  67. ^ Davis MH, Johnsrude IS (April 2003). "Hierarchical processing in spoken language comprehension". The Journal of Neuroscience. 23 (8): 3423–31. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.23-08-03423.2003. PMC 6742313. PMID 12716950.
  68. ^ Liebenthal E, Binder JR, Spitzer SM, Possing ET, Medler DA (October 2005). "Neural substrates of phonemic perception". Cerebral Cortex. 15 (10): 1621–31. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhi040. PMID 15703256.
  69. ^ Narain C, Scott SK, Wise RJ, Rosen S, Leff A, Iversen SD, Matthews PM (December 2003). "Defining a left-lateralized response specific to intelligible speech using fMRI". Cerebral Cortex. 13 (12): 1362–8. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhg083. PMID 14615301.
  70. ^ Obleser J, Boecker H, Drzezga A, Haslinger B, Hennenlotter A, Roettinger M, Eulitz C, Rauschecker JP (July 2006). "Vowel sound extraction in anterior superior temporal cortex". Human Brain Mapping. 27 (7): 562–71. doi:10.1002/hbm.20201. PMC 6871493. PMID 16281283.
  71. ^ Obleser J, Zimmermann J, Van Meter J, Rauschecker JP (October 2007). "Multiple stages of auditory speech perception reflected in event-related FMRI". Cerebral Cortex. 17 (10): 2251–7. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhl133. PMID 17150986.
  72. ^ Scott SK, Blank CC, Rosen S, Wise RJ (December 2000). "Identification of a pathway for intelligible speech in the left temporal lobe". Brain. 123 (12): 2400–6. doi:10.1093/brain/123.12.2400. PMC 5630088. PMID 11099443.
  73. ^ Belin P, Zatorre RJ (November 2003). "Adaptation to speaker's voice in right anterior temporal lobe". NeuroReport. 14 (16): 2105–2109. doi:10.1097/00001756-200311140-00019. PMID 14600506. S2CID 34183900.
  74. ^ Benson RR, Whalen DH, Richardson M, Swainson B, Clark VP, Lai S, Liberman AM (September 2001). "Parametrically dissociating speech and nonspeech perception in the brain using fMRI". Brain and Language. 78 (3): 364–96. doi:10.1006/brln.2001.2484. PMID 11703063. S2CID 15328590.
  75. ^ Leaver AM, Rauschecker JP (June 2010). "Cortical representation of natural complex sounds: effects of acoustic features and auditory object category". The Journal of Neuroscience. 30 (22): 7604–12. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.0296-10.2010. PMC 2930617. PMID 20519535.
  76. ^ Lewis JW, Phinney RE, Brefczynski-Lewis JA, DeYoe EA (August 2006). "Lefties get it "right" when hearing tool sounds". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 18 (8): 1314–30. doi:10.1162/jocn.2006.18.8.1314. PMID 16859417. S2CID 14049095.
  77. ^ Maeder PP, Meuli RA, Adriani M, Bellmann A, Fornari E, Thiran JP, Pittet A, Clarke S (October 2001). "Distinct pathways involved in sound recognition and localization: a human fMRI study" (PDF). NeuroImage. 14 (4): 802–16. doi:10.1006/nimg.2001.0888. PMID 11554799. S2CID 1388647.
  78. ^ Viceic D, Fornari E, Thiran JP, Maeder PP, Meuli R, Adriani M, Clarke S (November 2006). "Human auditory belt areas specialized in sound recognition: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study" (PDF). NeuroReport. 17 (16): 1659–62. doi:10.1097/01.wnr.0000239962.75943.dd. PMID 17047449. S2CID 14482187.
  79. ^ Shultz S, Vouloumanos A, Pelphrey K (May 2012). "The superior temporal sulcus differentiates communicative and noncommunicative auditory signals". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 24 (5): 1224–32. doi:10.1162/jocn_a_00208. PMID 22360624. S2CID 10784270.
  80. ^ DeWitt I, Rauschecker JP (February 2012). "Phoneme and word recognition in the auditory ventral stream". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 109 (8): E505-14. Bibcode:2012PNAS..109E.505D. doi:10.1073/pnas.1113427109. PMC 3286918. PMID 22308358.
  81. ^ a b c d Lachaux JP, Jerbi K, Bertrand O, Minotti L, Hoffmann D, Schoendorff B, Kahane P (October 2007). "A blueprint for real-time functional mapping via human intracranial recordings". PLOS ONE. 2 (10): e1094. Bibcode:2007PLoSO...2.1094L. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0001094. PMC 2040217. PMID 17971857.
  82. ^ a b Matsumoto R, Imamura H, Inouchi M, Nakagawa T, Yokoyama Y, Matsuhashi M, Mikuni N, Miyamoto S, Fukuyama H, Takahashi R, Ikeda A (April 2011). "Left anterior temporal cortex actively engages in speech perception: A direct cortical stimulation study". Neuropsychologia. 49 (5): 1350–1354. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.01.023. hdl:2433/141342. PMID 21251921. S2CID 1831334.
  83. ^ a b c d Roux FE, Miskin K, Durand JB, Sacko O, Réhault E, Tanova R, Démonet JF (October 2015). "Electrostimulation mapping of comprehension of auditory and visual words". Cortex; A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior. 71: 398–408. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2015.07.001. PMID 26332785. S2CID 39964328.
  84. ^ Fritz J, Mishkin M, Saunders RC (June 2005). "In search of an auditory engram". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 102 (26): 9359–64. Bibcode:2005PNAS..102.9359F. doi:10.1073/pnas.0503998102. PMC 1166637. PMID 15967995.
  85. ^ Stepien LS, Cordeau JP, Rasmussen T (1960). "The effect of temporal lobe and hippocampal lesions on auditory and visual recent memory in monkeys". Brain. 83 (3): 470–489. doi:10.1093/brain/83.3.470. ISSN 0006-8950.
  86. ^ Strominger NL, Oesterreich RE, Neff WD (June 1980). "Sequential auditory and visual discriminations after temporal lobe ablation in monkeys". Physiology & Behavior. 24 (6): 1149–56. doi:10.1016/0031-9384(80)90062-1. PMID 6774349. S2CID 7494152.
  87. ^ Kaiser J, Ripper B, Birbaumer N, Lutzenberger W (October 2003). "Dynamics of gamma-band activity in human magnetoencephalogram during auditory pattern working memory". NeuroImage. 20 (2): 816–27. doi:10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00350-1. PMID 14568454. S2CID 19373941.
  88. ^ Buchsbaum BR, Olsen RK, Koch P, Berman KF (November 2005). "Human dorsal and ventral auditory streams subserve rehearsal-based and echoic processes during verbal working memory". Neuron. 48 (4): 687–97. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2005.09.029. PMID 16301183. S2CID 13202604.
  89. ^ Scott BH, Mishkin M, Yin P (July 2012). "Monkeys have a limited form of short-term memory in audition". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 109 (30): 12237–41. Bibcode:2012PNAS..10912237S. doi:10.1073/pnas.1209685109. PMC 3409773. PMID 22778411.
  90. ^ Noppeney U, Patterson K, Tyler LK, Moss H, Stamatakis EA, Bright P, Mummery C, Price CJ (April 2007). "Temporal lobe lesions and semantic impairment: a comparison of herpes simplex virus encephalitis and semantic dementia". Brain. 130 (Pt 4): 1138–47. doi:10.1093/brain/awl344. PMID 17251241.
  91. ^ Patterson K, Nestor PJ, Rogers TT (December 2007). "Where do you know what you know? The representation of semantic knowledge in the human brain". Nature Reviews. Neuroscience. 8 (12): 976–87. doi:10.1038/nrn2277. PMID 18026167. S2CID 7310189.
  92. ^ Schwartz MF, Kimberg DY, Walker GM, Faseyitan O, Brecher A, Dell GS, Coslett HB (December 2009). "Anterior temporal involvement in semantic word retrieval: voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping evidence from aphasia". Brain. 132 (Pt 12): 3411–27. doi:10.1093/brain/awp284. PMC 2792374. PMID 19942676.
  93. ^ Hamberger MJ, McClelland S, McKhann GM, Williams AC, Goodman RR (March 2007). "Distribution of auditory and visual naming sites in nonlesional temporal lobe epilepsy patients and patients with space-occupying temporal lobe lesions". Epilepsia. 48 (3): 531–8. doi:10.1111/j.1528-1167.2006.00955.x. PMID 17326797. S2CID 12642281.
  94. ^ a b c Duffau H (March 2008). "The anatomo-functional connectivity of language revisited. New insights provided by electrostimulation and tractography". Neuropsychologia. 46 (4): 927–34. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.10.025. PMID 18093622. S2CID 40514753.
  95. ^ Vigneau M, Beaucousin V, Hervé PY, Duffau H, Crivello F, Houdé O, Mazoyer B, Tzourio-Mazoyer N (May 2006). "Meta-analyzing left hemisphere language areas: phonology, semantics, and sentence processing". NeuroImage. 30 (4): 1414–32. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.11.002. PMID 16413796. S2CID 8870165.
  96. ^ a b Creutzfeldt O, Ojemann G, Lettich E (October 1989). "Neuronal activity in the human lateral temporal lobe. I. Responses to speech". Experimental Brain Research. 77 (3): 451–75. doi:10.1007/bf00249600. hdl:11858/00-001M-0000-002C-89EA-3. PMID 2806441. S2CID 19952034.
  97. ^ Mazoyer BM, Tzourio N, Frak V, Syrota A, Murayama N, Levrier O, Salamon G, Dehaene S, Cohen L, Mehler J (October 1993). "The cortical representation of speech" (PDF). Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 5 (4): 467–79. doi:10.1162/jocn.1993.5.4.467. PMID 23964919. S2CID 22265355.
  98. ^ Humphries C, Love T, Swinney D, Hickok G (October 2005). "Response of anterior temporal cortex to syntactic and prosodic manipulations during sentence processing". Human Brain Mapping. 26 (2): 128–38. doi:10.1002/hbm.20148. PMC 6871757. PMID 15895428.
  99. ^ Humphries C, Willard K, Buchsbaum B, Hickok G (June 2001). "Role of anterior temporal cortex in auditory sentence comprehension: an fMRI study". NeuroReport. 12 (8): 1749–52. doi:10.1097/00001756-200106130-00046. PMID 11409752. S2CID 13039857.
  100. ^ Vandenberghe R, Nobre AC, Price CJ (May 2002). "The response of left temporal cortex to sentences". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 14 (4): 550–60. doi:10.1162/08989290260045800. PMID 12126497. S2CID 21607482.
  101. ^ Friederici AD, Rüschemeyer SA, Hahne A, Fiebach CJ (February 2003). "The role of left inferior frontal and superior temporal cortex in sentence comprehension: localizing syntactic and semantic processes". Cerebral Cortex. 13 (2): 170–7. doi:10.1093/cercor/13.2.170. hdl:11858/00-001M-0000-0010-E3AB-B. PMID 12507948.
  102. ^ Xu J, Kemeny S, Park G, Frattali C, Braun A (2005). "Language in context: emergent features of word, sentence, and narrative comprehension". NeuroImage. 25 (3): 1002–15. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.013. PMID 15809000. S2CID 25570583.
  103. ^ Rogalsky C, Hickok G (April 2009). "Selective attention to semantic and syntactic features modulates sentence processing networks in anterior temporal cortex". Cerebral Cortex. 19 (4): 786–96. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhn126. PMC 2651476. PMID 18669589.
  104. ^ Pallier C, Devauchelle AD, Dehaene S (February 2011). "Cortical representation of the constituent structure of sentences". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 108 (6): 2522–7. doi:10.1073/pnas.1018711108. PMC 3038732. PMID 21224415.
  105. ^ Brennan J, Nir Y, Hasson U, Malach R, Heeger DJ, Pylkkänen L (February 2012). "Syntactic structure building in the anterior temporal lobe during natural story listening". Brain and Language. 120 (2): 163–73. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2010.04.002. PMC 2947556. PMID 20472279.
  106. ^ Kotz SA, von Cramon DY, Friederici AD (October 2003). "Differentiation of syntactic processes in the left and right anterior temporal lobe: Event-related brain potential evidence from lesion patients". Brain and Language. 87 (1): 135–136. doi:10.1016/s0093-934x(03)00236-0. S2CID 54320415.
  107. ^ Martin RC, Shelton JR, Yaffee LS (February 1994). "Language processing and working memory: Neuropsychological evidence for separate phonological and semantic capacities". Journal of Memory and Language. 33 (1): 83–111. doi:10.1006/jmla.1994.1005.
  108. ^ Magnusdottir S, Fillmore P, den Ouden DB, Hjaltason H, Rorden C, Kjartansson O, Bonilha L, Fridriksson J (October 2013). "Damage to left anterior temporal cortex predicts impairment of complex syntactic processing: a lesion-symptom mapping study". Human Brain Mapping. 34 (10): 2715–23. doi:10.1002/hbm.22096. PMC 6869931. PMID 22522937.
  109. ^ Bornkessel-Schlesewsky I, Schlesewsky M, Small SL, Rauschecker JP (March 2015). "Neurobiological roots of language in primate audition: common computational properties". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 19 (3): 142–50. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2014.12.008. PMC 4348204. PMID 25600585.
  110. ^ Hickok G, Okada K, Barr W, Pa J, Rogalsky C, Donnelly K, Barde L, Grant A (December 2008). "Bilateral capacity for speech sound processing in auditory comprehension: evidence from Wada procedures". Brain and Language. 107 (3): 179–84. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2008.09.006. PMC 2644214. PMID 18976806.
  111. ^ Zaidel E (September 1976). "Auditory Vocabulary of the Right Hemisphere Following Brain Bisection or Hemidecortication". Cortex. 12 (3): 191–211. doi:10.1016/s0010-9452(76)80001-9. ISSN 0010-9452. PMID 1000988. S2CID 4479925.
  112. ^ Poeppel D (October 2001). "Pure word deafness and the bilateral processing of the speech code". Cognitive Science. 25 (5): 679–693. doi:10.1016/s0364-0213(01)00050-7.
  113. ^ Ulrich G (May 1978). "Interhemispheric functional relationships in auditory agnosia. An analysis of the preconditions and a conceptual model". Brain and Language. 5 (3): 286–300. doi:10.1016/0093-934x(78)90027-5. PMID 656899. S2CID 33841186.
  114. ^ Stewart L, Walsh V, Frith U, Rothwell JC (March 2001). "TMS produces two dissociable types of speech disruption" (PDF). NeuroImage. 13 (3): 472–8. doi:10.1006/nimg.2000.0701. PMID 11170812. S2CID 10392466.
  115. ^ Acheson DJ, Hamidi M, Binder JR, Postle BR (June 2011). "A common neural substrate for language production and verbal working memory". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 23 (6): 1358–67. doi:10.1162/jocn.2010.21519. PMC 3053417. PMID 20617889.
  116. ^ Desmurget M, Reilly KT, Richard N, Szathmari A, Mottolese C, Sirigu A (May 2009). "Movement intention after parietal cortex stimulation in humans". Science. 324 (5928): 811–3. Bibcode:2009Sci...324..811D. doi:10.1126/science.1169896. PMID 19423830. S2CID 6555881.
  117. ^ Edwards E, Nagarajan SS, Dalal SS, Canolty RT, Kirsch HE, Barbaro NM, Knight RT (March 2010). "Spatiotemporal imaging of cortical activation during verb generation and picture naming". NeuroImage. 50 (1): 291–301. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.035. PMC 2957470. PMID 20026224.
  118. ^ Boatman D, Gordon B, Hart J, Selnes O, Miglioretti D, Lenz F (August 2000). "Transcortical sensory aphasia: revisited and revised". Brain. 123 (8): 1634–42. doi:10.1093/brain/123.8.1634. PMID 10908193.
  119. ^ a b c Turkeltaub PE, Coslett HB (July 2010). "Localization of sublexical speech perception components". Brain and Language. 114 (1): 1–15. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2010.03.008. PMC 2914564. PMID 20413149.
  120. ^ Chang EF, Rieger JW, Johnson K, Berger MS, Barbaro NM, Knight RT (November 2010). "Categorical speech representation in human superior temporal gyrus". Nature Neuroscience. 13 (11): 1428–32. doi:10.1038/nn.2641. PMC 2967728. PMID 20890293.
  121. ^ Buchsbaum BR, Hickok G, Humphries C (September 2001). "Role of left posterior superior temporal gyrus in phonological processing for speech perception and production". Cognitive Science. 25 (5): 663–678. doi:10.1207/s15516709cog2505_2. ISSN 0364-0213.
  122. ^ Wise RJ, Scott SK, Blank SC, Mummery CJ, Murphy K, Warburton EA (January 2001). "Separate neural subsystems within 'Wernicke's area'". Brain. 124 (Pt 1): 83–95. doi:10.1093/brain/124.1.83. PMID 11133789.
  123. ^ Hickok G, Buchsbaum B, Humphries C, Muftuler T (July 2003). "Auditory-motor interaction revealed by fMRI: speech, music, and working memory in area Spt". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 15 (5): 673–82. doi:10.1162/089892903322307393. PMID 12965041.
  124. ^ Warren JE, Wise RJ, Warren JD (December 2005). "Sounds do-able: auditory-motor transformations and the posterior temporal plane". Trends in Neurosciences. 28 (12): 636–43. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2005.09.010. PMID 16216346. S2CID 36678139.
  125. ^ Hickok G, Poeppel D (May 2007). "The cortical organization of speech processing". Nature Reviews. Neuroscience. 8 (5): 393–402. doi:10.1038/nrn2113. PMID 17431404. S2CID 6199399.
  126. ^ Karbe H, Herholz K, Weber-Luxenburger G, Ghaemi M, Heiss WD (June 1998). "Cerebral networks and functional brain asymmetry: evidence from regional metabolic changes during word repetition". Brain and Language. 63 (1): 108–21. doi:10.1006/brln.1997.1937. PMID 9642023. S2CID 31335617.
  127. ^ Giraud AL, Price CJ (August 2001). "The constraints functional neuroimaging places on classical models of auditory word processing". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 13 (6): 754–65. doi:10.1162/08989290152541421. PMID 11564320. S2CID 13916709.
  128. ^ Graves WW, Grabowski TJ, Mehta S, Gupta P (September 2008). "The left posterior superior temporal gyrus participates specifically in accessing lexical phonology". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 20 (9): 1698–710. doi:10.1162/jocn.2008.20113. PMC 2570618. PMID 18345989.
  129. ^ a b Towle VL, Yoon HA, Castelle M, Edgar JC, Biassou NM, Frim DM, Spire JP, Kohrman MH (August 2008). "ECoG gamma activity during a language task: differentiating expressive and receptive speech areas". Brain. 131 (Pt 8): 2013–27. doi:10.1093/brain/awn147. PMC 2724904. PMID 18669510.
  130. ^ Selnes OA, Knopman DS, Niccum N, Rubens AB (June 1985). "The critical role of Wernicke's area in sentence repetition". Annals of Neurology. 17 (6): 549–57. doi:10.1002/ana.410170604. PMID 4026225. S2CID 12914191.
  131. ^ Axer H, von Keyserlingk AG, Berks G, von Keyserlingk DG (March 2001). "Supra- and infrasylvian conduction aphasia". Brain and Language. 76 (3): 317–31. doi:10.1006/brln.2000.2425. PMID 11247647. S2CID 25406527.
  132. ^ Bartha L, Benke T (April 2003). "Acute conduction aphasia: an analysis of 20 cases". Brain and Language. 85 (1): 93–108. doi:10.1016/s0093-934x(02)00502-3. PMID 12681350. S2CID 18466425.
  133. ^ Baldo JV, Katseff S, Dronkers NF (March 2012). "Brain Regions Underlying Repetition and Auditory-Verbal Short-term Memory Deficits in Aphasia: Evidence from Voxel-based Lesion Symptom Mapping". Aphasiology. 26 (3–4): 338–354. doi:10.1080/02687038.2011.602391. PMC 4070523. PMID 24976669.
  134. ^ Baldo JV, Klostermann EC, Dronkers NF (May 2008). "It's either a cook or a baker: patients with conduction aphasia get the gist but lose the trace". Brain and Language. 105 (2): 134–40. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2007.12.007. PMID 18243294. S2CID 997735.
  135. ^ Fridriksson J, Kjartansson O, Morgan PS, Hjaltason H, Magnusdottir S, Bonilha L, Rorden C (August 2010). "Impaired speech repetition and left parietal lobe damage". The Journal of Neuroscience. 30 (33): 11057–61. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.1120-10.2010. PMC 2936270. PMID 20720112.
  136. ^ Buchsbaum BR, Baldo J, Okada K, Berman KF, Dronkers N, D'Esposito M, Hickok G (December 2011). "Conduction aphasia, sensory-motor integration, and phonological short-term memory - an aggregate analysis of lesion and fMRI data". Brain and Language. 119 (3): 119–28. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2010.12.001. PMC 3090694. PMID 21256582.
  137. ^ Yamada K, Nagakane Y, Mizuno T, Hosomi A, Nakagawa M, Nishimura T (March 2007). "MR tractography depicting damage to the arcuate fasciculus in a patient with conduction aphasia". Neurology. 68 (10): 789. doi:10.1212/01.wnl.0000256348.65744.b2. PMID 17339591.
  138. ^ Breier JI, Hasan KM, Zhang W, Men D, Papanicolaou AC (March 2008). "Language dysfunction after stroke and damage to white matter tracts evaluated using diffusion tensor imaging". AJNR. American Journal of Neuroradiology. 29 (3): 483–7. doi:10.3174/ajnr.A0846. PMC 3073452. PMID 18039757.
  139. ^ Zhang Y, Wang C, Zhao X, Chen H, Han Z, Wang Y (September 2010). "Diffusion tensor imaging depicting damage to the arcuate fasciculus in patients with conduction aphasia: a study of the Wernicke-Geschwind model". Neurological Research. 32 (7): 775–8. doi:10.1179/016164109x12478302362653. PMID 19825277. S2CID 22960870.
  140. ^ Jones OP, Prejawa S, Hope TM, Oberhuber M, Seghier ML, Leff AP, Green DW, Price CJ (2014). "Sensory-to-motor integration during auditory repetition: a combined fMRI and lesion study". Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 8: 24. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00024. PMC 3908611. PMID 24550807.
  141. ^ Quigg M, Fountain NB (March 1999). "Conduction aphasia elicited by stimulation of the left posterior superior temporal gyrus". Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry. 66 (3): 393–6. doi:10.1136/jnnp.66.3.393. PMC 1736266. PMID 10084542.
  142. ^ Quigg M, Geldmacher DS, Elias WJ (May 2006). "Conduction aphasia as a function of the dominant posterior perisylvian cortex. Report of two cases". Journal of Neurosurgery. 104 (5): 845–8. doi:10.3171/jns.2006.104.5.845. PMID 16703895.
  143. ^ Service E, Kohonen V (April 1995). "Is the relation between phonological memory and foreign language learning accounted for by vocabulary acquisition?". Applied Psycholinguistics. 16 (2): 155–172. doi:10.1017/S0142716400007062. S2CID 143974128.
  144. ^ Service E (July 1992). "Phonology, working memory, and foreign-language learning". The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. A, Human Experimental Psychology. 45 (1): 21–50. doi:10.1080/14640749208401314. PMID 1636010. S2CID 43268252.
  145. ^ Matsumoto R, Nair DR, LaPresto E, Najm I, Bingaman W, Shibasaki H, Lüders HO (October 2004). "Functional connectivity in the human language system: a cortico-cortical evoked potential study". Brain. 127 (Pt 10): 2316–30. doi:10.1093/brain/awh246. PMID 15269116.
  146. ^ Kimura D, Watson N (November 1989). "The relation between oral movement control and speech". Brain and Language. 37 (4): 565–90. doi:10.1016/0093-934x(89)90112-0. PMID 2479446. S2CID 39913744.
  147. ^ Tourville JA, Reilly KJ, Guenther FH (February 2008). "Neural mechanisms underlying auditory feedback control of speech". NeuroImage. 39 (3): 1429–43. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.09.054. PMC 3658624. PMID 18035557.
  148. ^ Chang EF, Rieger JW, Johnson K, Berger MS, Barbaro NM, Knight RT (November 2010). "Categorical speech representation in human superior temporal gyrus". Nature Neuroscience. 13 (11): 1428–32. doi:10.1038/nn.2641. PMC 2967728. PMID 20890293.
  149. ^ Nath AR, Beauchamp MS (January 2012). "A neural basis for interindividual differences in the McGurk effect, a multisensory speech illusion". NeuroImage. 59 (1): 781–7. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.07.024. PMC 3196040. PMID 21787869.
  150. ^ Beauchamp MS, Nath AR, Pasalar S (February 2010). "fMRI-Guided transcranial magnetic stimulation reveals that the superior temporal sulcus is a cortical locus of the McGurk effect". The Journal of Neuroscience. 30 (7): 2414–7. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4865-09.2010. PMC 2844713. PMID 20164324.
  151. ^ McGettigan C, Faulkner A, Altarelli I, Obleser J, Baverstock H, Scott SK (April 2012). "Speech comprehension aided by multiple modalities: behavioural and neural interactions". Neuropsychologia. 50 (5): 762–76. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.01.010. PMC 4050300. PMID 22266262.
  152. ^ Stevenson RA, James TW (February 2009). "Audiovisual integration in human superior temporal sulcus: Inverse effectiveness and the neural processing of speech and object recognition". NeuroImage. 44 (3): 1210–23. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.09.034. PMID 18973818. S2CID 8342349.
  153. ^ Bernstein LE, Jiang J, Pantazis D, Lu ZL, Joshi A (October 2011). "Visual phonetic processing localized using speech and nonspeech face gestures in video and point-light displays". Human Brain Mapping. 32 (10): 1660–76. doi:10.1002/hbm.21139. PMC 3120928. PMID 20853377.
  154. ^ Campbell R (March 2008). "The processing of audio-visual speech: empirical and neural bases". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences. 363 (1493): 1001–10. doi:10.1098/rstb.2007.2155. PMC 2606792. PMID 17827105.
  155. ^ Schwartz MF, Faseyitan O, Kim J, Coslett HB (December 2012). "The dorsal stream contribution to phonological retrieval in object naming". Brain. 135 (Pt 12): 3799–814. doi:10.1093/brain/aws300. PMC 3525060. PMID 23171662.
  156. ^ Schwartz MF, Kimberg DY, Walker GM, Faseyitan O, Brecher A, Dell GS, Coslett HB (December 2009). "Anterior temporal involvement in semantic word retrieval: voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping evidence from aphasia". Brain. 132 (Pt 12): 3411–27. doi:10.1093/brain/awp284. PMC 2792374. PMID 19942676.
  157. ^ Ojemann GA (June 1983). "Brain organization for language from the perspective of electrical stimulation mapping". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 6 (2): 189–206. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00015491. ISSN 1469-1825. S2CID 143189089.
  158. ^ Cornelissen K, Laine M, Renvall K, Saarinen T, Martin N, Salmelin R (June 2004). "Learning new names for new objects: cortical effects as measured by magnetoencephalography". Brain and Language. 89 (3): 617–22. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2003.12.007. PMID 15120553. S2CID 32224334.
  159. ^ Hartwigsen G, Baumgaertner A, Price CJ, Koehnke M, Ulmer S, Siebner HR (September 2010). "Phonological decisions require both the left and right supramarginal gyri". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 107 (38): 16494–9. Bibcode:2010PNAS..10716494H. doi:10.1073/pnas.1008121107. PMC 2944751. PMID 20807747.
  160. ^ Cornelissen K, Laine M, Tarkiainen A, Järvensivu T, Martin N, Salmelin R (April 2003). "Adult brain plasticity elicited by anomia treatment". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 15 (3): 444–61. doi:10.1162/089892903321593153. PMID 12729495. S2CID 1597939.
  161. ^ Mechelli A, Crinion JT, Noppeney U, O'Doherty J, Ashburner J, Frackowiak RS, Price CJ (October 2004). "Neurolinguistics: structural plasticity in the bilingual brain". Nature. 431 (7010): 757. Bibcode:2004Natur.431..757M. doi:10.1038/431757a. hdl:11858/00-001M-0000-0013-D79B-1. PMID 15483594. S2CID 4338340.
  162. ^ Green DW, Crinion J, Price CJ (July 2007). "Exploring cross-linguistic vocabulary effects on brain structures using voxel-based morphometry". Bilingualism. 10 (2): 189–199. doi:10.1017/S1366728907002933. PMC 2312335. PMID 18418473.
  163. ^ Willness C (2016-01-08). "The Oxford handbook of organizational climate and culture By Benjamin Schneider & Karen M. Barbera (Eds.) New York, NY: Oxford University Press, 2014. ISBN 978-0-19-986071-5". Book Reviews. British Journal of Psychology. 107 (1): 201–202. doi:10.1111/bjop.12170.
  164. ^ Lee H, Devlin JT, Shakeshaft C, Stewart LH, Brennan A, Glensman J, Pitcher K, Crinion J, Mechelli A, Frackowiak RS, Green DW, Price CJ (January 2007). "Anatomical traces of vocabulary acquisition in the adolescent brain". The Journal of Neuroscience. 27 (5): 1184–9. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4442-06.2007. PMC 6673201. PMID 17267574.
  165. ^ Richardson FM, Thomas MS, Filippi R, Harth H, Price CJ (May 2010). "Contrasting effects of vocabulary knowledge on temporal and parietal brain structure across lifespan". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 22 (5): 943–54. doi:10.1162/jocn.2009.21238. PMC 2860571. PMID 19366285.
  166. ^ Jobard G, Crivello F, Tzourio-Mazoyer N (October 2003). "Evaluation of the dual route theory of reading: a metanalysis of 35 neuroimaging studies". NeuroImage. 20 (2): 693–712. doi:10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00343-4. PMID 14568445. S2CID 739665.
  167. ^ Bolger DJ, Perfetti CA, Schneider W (May 2005). "Cross-cultural effect on the brain revisited: universal structures plus writing system variation". Human Brain Mapping (in French). 25 (1): 92–104. doi:10.1002/hbm.20124. PMC 6871743. PMID 15846818.
  168. ^ Brambati SM, Ogar J, Neuhaus J, Miller BL, Gorno-Tempini ML (July 2009). "Reading disorders in primary progressive aphasia: a behavioral and neuroimaging study". Neuropsychologia. 47 (8–9): 1893–900. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.02.033. PMC 2734967. PMID 19428421.
  169. ^ a b Baddeley A, Lewis V, Vallar G (May 1984). "Exploring the Articulatory Loop". The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A. 36 (2): 233–252. doi:10.1080/14640748408402157. S2CID 144313607.
  170. ^ a b Cowan N (February 2001). "The magical number 4 in short-term memory: a reconsideration of mental storage capacity". The Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 24 (1): 87–114, discussion 114–85. doi:10.1017/S0140525X01003922. PMID 11515286.
  171. ^ Caplan D, Rochon E, Waters GS (August 1992). "Articulatory and phonological determinants of word length effects in span tasks". The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. A, Human Experimental Psychology. 45 (2): 177–92. doi:10.1080/14640749208401323. PMID 1410554. S2CID 32594562.
  172. ^ Waters GS, Rochon E, Caplan D (February 1992). "The role of high-level speech planning in rehearsal: Evidence from patients with apraxia of speech". Journal of Memory and Language. 31 (1): 54–73. doi:10.1016/0749-596x(92)90005-i.
  173. ^ Cohen L, Bachoud-Levi AC (September 1995). "The role of the output phonological buffer in the control of speech timing: a single case study". Cortex; A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior. 31 (3): 469–86. doi:10.1016/s0010-9452(13)80060-3. PMID 8536476. S2CID 4480375.
  174. ^ Shallice T, Rumiati RI, Zadini A (September 2000). "The selective impairment of the phonological output buffer". Cognitive Neuropsychology. 17 (6): 517–46. doi:10.1080/02643290050110638. PMID 20945193. S2CID 14811413.
  175. ^ Shu H, Xiong H, Han Z, Bi Y, Bai X (2005). "The selective impairment of the phonological output buffer: evidence from a Chinese patient". Behavioural Neurology. 16 (2–3): 179–89. doi:10.1155/2005/647871. PMC 5478832. PMID 16410633.
  176. ^ Oberauer K (2002). "Access to information in working memory: Exploring the focus of attention". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 28 (3): 411–421. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.28.3.411. PMID 12018494.
  177. ^ Unsworth N, Engle RW (January 2007). "The nature of individual differences in working memory capacity: active maintenance in primary memory and controlled search from secondary memory". Psychological Review. 114 (1): 104–32. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.114.1.104. PMID 17227183.
  178. ^ Barrouillet P, Camos V (December 2012). "As Time Goes By". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 21 (6): 413–419. doi:10.1177/0963721412459513. S2CID 145540189.
  179. ^ Bornkessel-Schlesewsky I, Schlesewsky M, Small SL, Rauschecker JP (March 2015). "Neurobiological roots of language in primate audition: common computational properties". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 19 (3): 142–50. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2014.12.008. PMC 4348204. PMID 25600585.
  180. ^ Buchsbaum BR, D'Esposito M (May 2008). "The search for the phonological store: from loop to convolution". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 20 (5): 762–78. doi:10.1162/jocn.2008.20501. PMID 18201133. S2CID 17878480.
  181. ^ de Saussure, Ferdinand (1959) [First published 1916]. (PDF). New York: Philosophy Library. ISBN 9780231157278. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2020-04-14. Retrieved 2020-06-16.
  182. ^ Smith, Neil (2002). Chomsky: Ideas and Ideals (2nd ed.). Cambridge University Press. ISBN 0-521-47517-1.
  183. ^ Lakoff, George (1990). "Invariance hypothesis: is abstract reasoning based on image-schemas?". Cognitive Linguistics. 1 (1): 39–74. doi:10.1515/cogl.1990.1.1.39. S2CID 144380802.
  184. ^ Song, Jae Jung (2012). Word Order. Cambridge University Press. ISBN 9781139033930.
  185. ^ Christiansen, Morten H.; Chater, Nick (2008). "Language as shaped by the brain" (PDF). Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 31 (5): 489–558. doi:10.1017/S0140525X08004998. PMID 18826669. Retrieved 2020-12-22.
  186. ^ Blackmore, Susan (2008). "Memes shape brains shape memes". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 31 (5): 513. doi:10.1017/S0140525X08005037. Retrieved 2020-12-22.
  187. ^ Kluender, R.; Kutas, M. (1993). "Subjacency as a processing phenomenon" (PDF). Language and Cognitive Processes. 8 (4): 573–633. doi:10.1080/01690969308407588. Retrieved 2020-02-28.
  188. ^ Barkley, C.; Kluender, R.; Kutas, M. (2015). "Referential processing in the human brain: An Event-Related Potential (ERP) study" (PDF). Brain Research. 1629: 143–159. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2015.09.017. PMID 26456801. S2CID 17053154. Retrieved 2020-02-28.
  189. ^ Wei, Xuehu; Adamson, Helyne; Schwendemann, Matthias; Goucha, Tómas; Friederici, Angela D.; Anwander, Alfred (19 February 2023). "Native language differences in the structural connectome of the human brain". NeuroImage. 270 (270): 119955. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2023.119955. PMID 36805092.
  190. ^ Koizumi, Masatoshi; Yasugi, Yoshiho; Tamaoka, Katsuo; Kiyama, Sachiko; Kim, Jungho; Ajsivinac Sian, Juan Esteban; García Mátzar, Pedro Oscar (September 2014). "On the (non)universality of the preference for subject-object word order in sentence comprehension: A sentence-processing study in Kaqchikel Maya". Language. 90 (3): 722–736. doi:10.1353/lan.2014.0068. JSTOR 24672044. S2CID 146776347. Retrieved 2023-05-15.
  191. ^ Miller LM, Recanzone GH (April 2009). "Populations of auditory cortical neurons can accurately encode acoustic space across stimulus intensity". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 106 (14): 5931–5. Bibcode:2009PNAS..106.5931M. doi:10.1073/pnas.0901023106. PMC 2667094. PMID 19321750.
  192. ^ Tian B, Reser D, Durham A, Kustov A, Rauschecker JP (April 2001). "Functional specialization in rhesus monkey auditory cortex". Science. 292 (5515): 290–3. Bibcode:2001Sci...292..290T. doi:10.1126/science.1058911. PMID 11303104. S2CID 32846215.
  193. ^ Alain C, Arnott SR, Hevenor S, Graham S, Grady CL (October 2001). ""What" and "where" in the human auditory system". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 98 (21): 12301–6. Bibcode:2001PNAS...9812301A. doi:10.1073/pnas.211209098. PMC 59809. PMID 11572938.
  194. ^ De Santis L, Clarke S, Murray MM (January 2007). "Automatic and intrinsic auditory "what" and "where" processing in humans revealed by electrical neuroimaging". Cerebral Cortex. 17 (1): 9–17. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhj119. PMID 16421326.
  195. ^ Barrett DJ, Hall DA (August 2006). "Response preferences for "what" and "where" in human non-primary auditory cortex". NeuroImage. 32 (2): 968–77. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.03.050. PMID 16733092. S2CID 19988467.
  196. ^ Linden JF, Grunewald A, Andersen RA (July 1999). "Responses to auditory stimuli in macaque lateral intraparietal area. II. Behavioral modulation". Journal of Neurophysiology. 82 (1): 343–58. doi:10.1152/jn.1999.82.1.343. PMID 10400963. S2CID 5317446.
  197. ^ Mazzoni P, Bracewell RM, Barash S, Andersen RA (March 1996). "Spatially tuned auditory responses in area LIP of macaques performing delayed memory saccades to acoustic targets". Journal of Neurophysiology. 75 (3): 1233–41. doi:10.1152/jn.1996.75.3.1233. PMID 8867131.
  198. ^ Lachaux JP, Jerbi K, Bertrand O, Minotti L, Hoffmann D, Schoendorff B, Kahane P (October 2007). "A blueprint for real-time functional mapping via human intracranial recordings". PLOS ONE. 2 (10): e1094. Bibcode:2007PLoSO...2.1094L. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0001094. PMC 2040217. PMID 17971857.
  199. ^ Jardri R, Houfflin-Debarge V, Delion P, Pruvo JP, Thomas P, Pins D (April 2012). "Assessing fetal response to maternal speech using a noninvasive functional brain imaging technique". International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience. 30 (2): 159–61. doi:10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2011.11.002. PMID 22123457. S2CID 2603226.
  200. ^ Poliva O (2017-09-20). "From where to what: a neuroanatomically based evolutionary model of the emergence of speech in humans". F1000Research. 4: 67. doi:10.12688/f1000research.6175.3. PMC 5600004. PMID 28928931.
  201. ^ Poliva O (2016-06-30). "From Mimicry to Language: A Neuroanatomically Based Evolutionary Model of the Emergence of Vocal Language". Frontiers in Neuroscience. 10: 307. doi:10.3389/fnins.2016.00307. PMC 4928493. PMID 27445676.
  202. ^ a b c d e Suri, Sana (25 July 2014). "What sign language teaches us about the brain". The Conversation. Retrieved 2019-10-07.
  203. ^ a b c Scientific American. (2002). Sign language in the brain. [Brochure]. Retrieved from http://lcn.salk.edu/Brochure/SciAM%20ASL.pdf
  204. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l Norton ES, Kovelman I, Petitto LA (March 2007). "Are There Separate Neural Systems for Spelling? New Insights into the Role of Rules and Memory in Spelling from Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging". Mind, Brain and Education. 1 (1): 48–59. doi:10.1111/j.1751-228X.2007.00005.x. PMC 2790202. PMID 20011680.
  205. ^ a b c d e Treiman R, Kessler B (2007). Writing Systems and Spelling Development. Blackwell Publishing Ltd. pp. 120–134. doi:10.1002/9780470757642.ch7. ISBN 978-0-470-75764-2. {{cite book}}: |work= ignored (help)

language, processing, brain, language, processing, redirects, here, processing, language, computers, natural, language, processing, this, article, multiple, issues, please, help, improve, discuss, these, issues, talk, page, learn, when, remove, these, template. Language processing redirects here For the processing of language by computers see Natural language processing This article has multiple issues Please help improve it or discuss these issues on the talk page Learn how and when to remove these template messages This article relies excessively on references to primary sources Please improve this article by adding secondary or tertiary sources Find sources Language processing in the brain news newspapers books scholar JSTOR October 2018 Learn how and when to remove this template message This article needs to be updated Please help update this article to reflect recent events or newly available information October 2018 Learn how and when to remove this template message In psycholinguistics language processing refers to the way humans use words to communicate ideas and feelings and how such communications are processed and understood Language processing is considered to be a uniquely human ability that is not produced with the same grammatical understanding or systematicity in even human s closest primate relatives 1 Dual stream connectivity between the auditory cortex and frontal lobe of monkeys and humans Top The auditory cortex of the monkey left and human right is schematically depicted on the supratemporal plane and observed from above with the parieto frontal operculi removed Bottom The brain of the monkey left and human right is schematically depicted and displayed from the side Orange frames mark the region of the auditory cortex which is displayed in the top sub figures Top and Bottom Blue colors mark regions affiliated with the ADS and red colors mark regions affiliated with the AVS dark red and blue regions mark the primary auditory fields Material was copied from this source which is available under a Creative Commons Attribution 4 0 International License Throughout the 20th century the dominant model 2 for language processing in the brain was the Geschwind Lichteim Wernicke model which is based primarily on the analysis of brain damaged patients However due to improvements in intra cortical electrophysiological recordings of monkey and human brains as well non invasive techniques such as fMRI PET MEG and EEG a dual auditory pathway 3 4 has been revealed and a two streams model has been developed In accordance with this model there are two pathways that connect the auditory cortex to the frontal lobe each pathway accounting for different linguistic roles The auditory ventral stream pathway is responsible for sound recognition and is accordingly known as the auditory what pathway The auditory dorsal stream in both humans and non human primates is responsible for sound localization and is accordingly known as the auditory where pathway In humans this pathway especially in the left hemisphere is also responsible for speech production speech repetition lip reading and phonological working memory and long term memory In accordance with the from where to what model of language evolution 5 6 the reason the ADS is characterized with such a broad range of functions is that each indicates a different stage in language evolution The division of the two streams first occurs in the auditory nerve where the anterior branch enters the anterior cochlear nucleus in the brainstem which gives rise to the auditory ventral stream The posterior branch enters the dorsal and posteroventral cochlear nucleus to give rise to the auditory dorsal stream 7 8 Language processing can also occur in relation to signed languages or written content Contents 1 Early neurolinguistics models 2 Current neurolinguistics models 2 1 Anatomy 2 2 Auditory ventral stream 2 2 1 Sound recognition 2 2 2 Sentence comprehension 2 2 3 Bilaterality 2 3 Auditory dorsal stream 2 3 1 Speech production 2 3 2 Vocal mimicry 2 3 3 Speech monitoring 2 3 4 Integration of phonemes with lip movements 2 3 5 Phonological long term memory 2 3 6 Phonological working memory 3 Linguistic theories 4 The evolution of language 5 Sign language in the brain 6 Writing in the brain 7 See also 8 ReferencesEarly neurolinguistics models edit nbsp Language Areas of the human brain The angular gyrus is represented in orange supramarginal gyrus is represented in yellow Broca s area is represented in blue Wernicke s area is represented in green and the primary auditory cortex is represented in pink Throughout the 20th century our knowledge of language processing in the brain was dominated by the Wernicke Lichtheim Geschwind model 8 2 9 The Wernicke Lichtheim Geschwind model is primarily based on research conducted on brain damaged individuals who were reported to possess a variety of language related disorders In accordance with this model words are perceived via a specialized word reception center Wernicke s area that is located in the left temporoparietal junction This region then projects to a word production center Broca s area that is located in the left inferior frontal gyrus Because almost all language input was thought to funnel via Wernicke s area and all language output to funnel via Broca s area it became extremely difficult to identify the basic properties of each region This lack of clear definition for the contribution of Wernicke s and Broca s regions to human language rendered it extremely difficult to identify their homologues in other primates 10 With the advent of the fMRI and its application for lesion mappings however it was shown that this model is based on incorrect correlations between symptoms and lesions 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 The refutation of such an influential and dominant model opened the door to new models of language processing in the brain Current neurolinguistics models editAnatomy edit In the last two decades significant advances occurred in our understanding of the neural processing of sounds in primates Initially by recording of neural activity in the auditory cortices of monkeys 18 19 and later elaborated via histological staining 20 21 22 and fMRI scanning studies 23 3 auditory fields were identified in the primary auditory cortex and 9 associative auditory fields were shown to surround them Figure 1 top left Anatomical tracing and lesion studies further indicated of a separation between the anterior and posterior auditory fields with the anterior primary auditory fields areas R RT projecting to the anterior associative auditory fields areas AL RTL and the posterior primary auditory field area A1 projecting to the posterior associative auditory fields areas CL CM 20 24 25 26 Recently evidence accumulated that indicates homology between the human and monkey auditory fields In humans histological staining studies revealed two separate auditory fields in the primary auditory region of Heschl s gyrus 27 28 and by mapping the tonotopic organization of the human primary auditory fields with high resolution fMRI and comparing it to the tonotopic organization of the monkey primary auditory fields homology was established between the human anterior primary auditory field and monkey area R denoted in humans as area hR and the human posterior primary auditory field and the monkey area A1 denoted in humans as area hA1 29 30 31 32 33 Intra cortical recordings from the human auditory cortex further demonstrated similar patterns of connectivity to the auditory cortex of the monkey Recording from the surface of the auditory cortex supra temporal plane reported that the anterior Heschl s gyrus area hR projects primarily to the middle anterior superior temporal gyrus mSTG aSTG and the posterior Heschl s gyrus area hA1 projects primarily to the posterior superior temporal gyrus pSTG and the planum temporale area PT Figure 1 top right 34 35 Consistent with connections from area hR to the aSTG and hA1 to the pSTG is an fMRI study of a patient with impaired sound recognition auditory agnosia who was shown with reduced bilateral activation in areas hR and aSTG but with spared activation in the mSTG pSTG 36 This connectivity pattern is also corroborated by a study that recorded activation from the lateral surface of the auditory cortex and reported of simultaneous non overlapping activation clusters in the pSTG and mSTG aSTG while listening to sounds 37 Downstream to the auditory cortex anatomical tracing studies in monkeys delineated projections from the anterior associative auditory fields areas AL RTL to ventral prefrontal and premotor cortices in the inferior frontal gyrus IFG 38 39 and amygdala 40 Cortical recording and functional imaging studies in macaque monkeys further elaborated on this processing stream by showing that acoustic information flows from the anterior auditory cortex to the temporal pole TP and then to the IFG 41 42 43 44 45 46 This pathway is commonly referred to as the auditory ventral stream AVS Figure 1 bottom left red arrows In contrast to the anterior auditory fields tracing studies reported that the posterior auditory fields areas CL CM project primarily to dorsolateral prefrontal and premotor cortices although some projections do terminate in the IFG 47 39 Cortical recordings and anatomical tracing studies in monkeys further provided evidence that this processing stream flows from the posterior auditory fields to the frontal lobe via a relay station in the intra parietal sulcus IPS 48 49 50 51 52 53 This pathway is commonly referred to as the auditory dorsal stream ADS Figure 1 bottom left blue arrows Comparing the white matter pathways involved in communication in humans and monkeys with diffusion tensor imaging techniques indicates of similar connections of the AVS and ADS in the two species Monkey 52 Human 54 55 56 57 58 59 In humans the pSTG was shown to project to the parietal lobe sylvian parietal temporal junction inferior parietal lobule Spt IPL and from there to dorsolateral prefrontal and premotor cortices Figure 1 bottom right blue arrows and the aSTG was shown to project to the anterior temporal lobe middle temporal gyrus temporal pole MTG TP and from there to the IFG Figure 1 bottom right red arrows Auditory ventral stream edit The auditory ventral stream AVS connects the auditory cortex with the middle temporal gyrus and temporal pole which in turn connects with the inferior frontal gyrus This pathway is responsible for sound recognition and is accordingly known as the auditory what pathway The functions of the AVS include the following Sound recognition edit Accumulative converging evidence indicates that the AVS is involved in recognizing auditory objects At the level of the primary auditory cortex recordings from monkeys showed higher percentage of neurons selective for learned melodic sequences in area R than area A1 60 and a study in humans demonstrated more selectivity for heard syllables in the anterior Heschl s gyrus area hR than posterior Heschl s gyrus area hA1 61 In downstream associative auditory fields studies from both monkeys and humans reported that the border between the anterior and posterior auditory fields Figure 1 area PC in the monkey and mSTG in the human processes pitch attributes that are necessary for the recognition of auditory objects 18 The anterior auditory fields of monkeys were also demonstrated with selectivity for con specific vocalizations with intra cortical recordings 41 19 62 and functional imaging 63 42 43 One fMRI monkey study further demonstrated a role of the aSTG in the recognition of individual voices 42 The role of the human mSTG aSTG in sound recognition was demonstrated via functional imaging studies that correlated activity in this region with isolation of auditory objects from background noise 64 65 and with the recognition of spoken words 66 67 68 69 70 71 72 voices 73 melodies 74 75 environmental sounds 76 77 78 and non speech communicative sounds 79 A meta analysis of fMRI studies 80 further demonstrated functional dissociation between the left mSTG and aSTG with the former processing short speech units phonemes and the latter processing longer units e g words environmental sounds A study that recorded neural activity directly from the left pSTG and aSTG reported that the aSTG but not pSTG was more active when the patient listened to speech in her native language than unfamiliar foreign language 81 Consistently electro stimulation to the aSTG of this patient resulted in impaired speech perception 81 see also 82 83 for similar results Intra cortical recordings from the right and left aSTG further demonstrated that speech is processed laterally to music 81 An fMRI study of a patient with impaired sound recognition auditory agnosia due to brainstem damage was also shown with reduced activation in areas hR and aSTG of both hemispheres when hearing spoken words and environmental sounds 36 Recordings from the anterior auditory cortex of monkeys while maintaining learned sounds in working memory 46 and the debilitating effect of induced lesions to this region on working memory recall 84 85 86 further implicate the AVS in maintaining the perceived auditory objects in working memory In humans area mSTG aSTG was also reported active during rehearsal of heard syllables with MEG 87 and fMRI 88 The latter study further demonstrated that working memory in the AVS is for the acoustic properties of spoken words and that it is independent to working memory in the ADS which mediates inner speech Working memory studies in monkeys also suggest that in monkeys in contrast to humans the AVS is the dominant working memory store 89 In humans downstream to the aSTG the MTG and TP are thought to constitute the semantic lexicon which is a long term memory repository of audio visual representations that are interconnected on the basis of semantic relationships See also the reviews by 3 4 discussing this topic The primary evidence for this role of the MTG TP is that patients with damage to this region e g patients with semantic dementia or herpes simplex virus encephalitis are reported 90 91 with an impaired ability to describe visual and auditory objects and a tendency to commit semantic errors when naming objects i e semantic paraphasia Semantic paraphasias were also expressed by aphasic patients with left MTG TP damage 14 92 and were shown to occur in non aphasic patients after electro stimulation to this region 93 83 or the underlying white matter pathway 94 Two meta analyses of the fMRI literature also reported that the anterior MTG and TP were consistently active during semantic analysis of speech and text 66 95 and an intra cortical recording study correlated neural discharge in the MTG with the comprehension of intelligible sentences 96 Sentence comprehension edit In addition to extracting meaning from sounds the MTG TP region of the AVS appears to have a role in sentence comprehension possibly by merging concepts together e g merging the concept blue and shirt to create the concept of a blue shirt The role of the MTG in extracting meaning from sentences has been demonstrated in functional imaging studies reporting stronger activation in the anterior MTG when proper sentences are contrasted with lists of words sentences in a foreign or nonsense language scrambled sentences sentences with semantic or syntactic violations and sentence like sequences of environmental sounds 97 98 99 100 101 102 103 104 One fMRI study 105 in which participants were instructed to read a story further correlated activity in the anterior MTG with the amount of semantic and syntactic content each sentence contained An EEG study 106 that contrasted cortical activity while reading sentences with and without syntactic violations in healthy participants and patients with MTG TP damage concluded that the MTG TP in both hemispheres participate in the automatic rule based stage of syntactic analysis ELAN component and that the left MTG TP is also involved in a later controlled stage of syntax analysis P600 component Patients with damage to the MTG TP region have also been reported with impaired sentence comprehension 14 107 108 See review 109 for more information on this topic Bilaterality edit In contradiction to the Wernicke Lichtheim Geschwind model that implicates sound recognition to occur solely in the left hemisphere studies that examined the properties of the right or left hemisphere in isolation via unilateral hemispheric anesthesia i e the WADA procedure 110 or intra cortical recordings from each hemisphere 96 provided evidence that sound recognition is processed bilaterally Moreover a study that instructed patients with disconnected hemispheres i e split brain patients to match spoken words to written words presented to the right or left hemifields reported vocabulary in the right hemisphere that almost matches in size with the left hemisphere 111 The right hemisphere vocabulary was equivalent to the vocabulary of a healthy 11 years old child This bilateral recognition of sounds is also consistent with the finding that unilateral lesion to the auditory cortex rarely results in deficit to auditory comprehension i e auditory agnosia whereas a second lesion to the remaining hemisphere which could occur years later does 112 113 Finally as mentioned earlier an fMRI scan of an auditory agnosia patient demonstrated bilateral reduced activation in the anterior auditory cortices 36 and bilateral electro stimulation to these regions in both hemispheres resulted with impaired speech recognition 81 Auditory dorsal stream edit The auditory dorsal stream connects the auditory cortex with the parietal lobe which in turn connects with inferior frontal gyrus In both humans and non human primates the auditory dorsal stream is responsible for sound localization and is accordingly known as the auditory where pathway In humans this pathway especially in the left hemisphere is also responsible for speech production speech repetition lip reading and phonological working memory and long term memory Speech production edit Studies of present day humans have demonstrated a role for the ADS in speech production particularly in the vocal expression of the names of objects For instance in a series of studies in which sub cortical fibers were directly stimulated 94 interference in the left pSTG and IPL resulted in errors during object naming tasks and interference in the left IFG resulted in speech arrest Magnetic interference in the pSTG and IFG of healthy participants also produced speech errors and speech arrest respectively 114 115 One study has also reported that electrical stimulation of the left IPL caused patients to believe that they had spoken when they had not and that IFG stimulation caused patients to unconsciously move their lips 116 The contribution of the ADS to the process of articulating the names of objects could be dependent on the reception of afferents from the semantic lexicon of the AVS as an intra cortical recording study reported of activation in the posterior MTG prior to activation in the Spt IPL region when patients named objects in pictures 117 Intra cortical electrical stimulation studies also reported that electrical interference to the posterior MTG was correlated with impaired object naming 118 82 Vocal mimicry edit Although sound perception is primarily ascribed with the AVS the ADS appears associated with several aspects of speech perception For instance in a meta analysis of fMRI studies 119 Turkeltaub and Coslett 2010 in which the auditory perception of phonemes was contrasted with closely matching sounds and the studies were rated for the required level of attention the authors concluded that attention to phonemes correlates with strong activation in the pSTG pSTS region An intra cortical recording study in which participants were instructed to identify syllables also correlated the hearing of each syllable with its own activation pattern in the pSTG 120 The involvement of the ADS in both speech perception and production has been further illuminated in several pioneering functional imaging studies that contrasted speech perception with overt or covert speech production 121 122 123 These studies demonstrated that the pSTS is active only during the perception of speech whereas area Spt is active during both the perception and production of speech The authors concluded that the pSTS projects to area Spt which converts the auditory input into articulatory movements 124 125 Similar results have been obtained in a study in which participants temporal and parietal lobes were electrically stimulated This study reported that electrically stimulating the pSTG region interferes with sentence comprehension and that stimulation of the IPL interferes with the ability to vocalize the names of objects 83 The authors also reported that stimulation in area Spt and the inferior IPL induced interference during both object naming and speech comprehension tasks The role of the ADS in speech repetition is also congruent with the results of the other functional imaging studies that have localized activation during speech repetition tasks to ADS regions 126 127 128 An intra cortical recording study that recorded activity throughout most of the temporal parietal and frontal lobes also reported activation in the pSTG Spt IPL and IFG when speech repetition is contrasted with speech perception 129 Neuropsychological studies have also found that individuals with speech repetition deficits but preserved auditory comprehension i e conduction aphasia suffer from circumscribed damage to the Spt IPL area 130 131 132 133 134 135 136 or damage to the projections that emanate from this area and target the frontal lobe 137 138 139 140 Studies have also reported a transient speech repetition deficit in patients after direct intra cortical electrical stimulation to this same region 11 141 142 Insight into the purpose of speech repetition in the ADS is provided by longitudinal studies of children that correlated the learning of foreign vocabulary with the ability to repeat nonsense words 143 144 Speech monitoring edit In addition to repeating and producing speech the ADS appears to have a role in monitoring the quality of the speech output Neuroanatomical evidence suggests that the ADS is equipped with descending connections from the IFG to the pSTG that relay information about motor activity i e corollary discharges in the vocal apparatus mouth tongue vocal folds This feedback marks the sound perceived during speech production as self produced and can be used to adjust the vocal apparatus to increase the similarity between the perceived and emitted calls Evidence for descending connections from the IFG to the pSTG has been offered by a study that electrically stimulated the IFG during surgical operations and reported the spread of activation to the pSTG pSTS Spt region 145 A study 146 that compared the ability of aphasic patients with frontal parietal or temporal lobe damage to quickly and repeatedly articulate a string of syllables reported that damage to the frontal lobe interfered with the articulation of both identical syllabic strings Bababa and non identical syllabic strings Badaga whereas patients with temporal or parietal lobe damage only exhibited impairment when articulating non identical syllabic strings Because the patients with temporal and parietal lobe damage were capable of repeating the syllabic string in the first task their speech perception and production appears to be relatively preserved and their deficit in the second task is therefore due to impaired monitoring Demonstrating the role of the descending ADS connections in monitoring emitted calls an fMRI study instructed participants to speak under normal conditions or when hearing a modified version of their own voice delayed first formant and reported that hearing a distorted version of one s own voice results in increased activation in the pSTG 147 Further demonstrating that the ADS facilitates motor feedback during mimicry is an intra cortical recording study that contrasted speech perception and repetition 129 The authors reported that in addition to activation in the IPL and IFG speech repetition is characterized by stronger activation in the pSTG than during speech perception Integration of phonemes with lip movements edit Although sound perception is primarily ascribed with the AVS the ADS appears associated with several aspects of speech perception For instance in a meta analysis of fMRI studies 119 in which the auditory perception of phonemes was contrasted with closely matching sounds and the studies were rated for the required level of attention the authors concluded that attention to phonemes correlates with strong activation in the pSTG pSTS region An intra cortical recording study in which participants were instructed to identify syllables also correlated the hearing of each syllable with its own activation pattern in the pSTG 148 Consistent with the role of the ADS in discriminating phonemes 119 studies have ascribed the integration of phonemes and their corresponding lip movements i e visemes to the pSTS of the ADS For example an fMRI study 149 has correlated activation in the pSTS with the McGurk illusion in which hearing the syllable ba while seeing the viseme ga results in the perception of the syllable da Another study has found that using magnetic stimulation to interfere with processing in this area further disrupts the McGurk illusion 150 The association of the pSTS with the audio visual integration of speech has also been demonstrated in a study that presented participants with pictures of faces and spoken words of varying quality The study reported that the pSTS selects for the combined increase of the clarity of faces and spoken words 151 Corroborating evidence has been provided by an fMRI study 152 that contrasted the perception of audio visual speech with audio visual non speech pictures and sounds of tools This study reported the detection of speech selective compartments in the pSTS In addition an fMRI study 153 that contrasted congruent audio visual speech with incongruent speech pictures of still faces reported pSTS activation For a review presenting additional converging evidence regarding the role of the pSTS and ADS in phoneme viseme integration see 154 Phonological long term memory edit A growing body of evidence indicates that humans in addition to having a long term store for word meanings located in the MTG TP of the AVS i e the semantic lexicon also have a long term store for the names of objects located in the Spt IPL region of the ADS i e the phonological lexicon For example a study 155 156 examining patients with damage to the AVS MTG damage or damage to the ADS IPL damage reported that MTG damage results in individuals incorrectly identifying objects e g calling a goat a sheep an example of semantic paraphasia Conversely IPL damage results in individuals correctly identifying the object but incorrectly pronouncing its name e g saying gof instead of goat an example of phonemic paraphasia Semantic paraphasia errors have also been reported in patients receiving intra cortical electrical stimulation of the AVS MTG and phonemic paraphasia errors have been reported in patients whose ADS pSTG Spt and IPL received intra cortical electrical stimulation 83 157 94 Further supporting the role of the ADS in object naming is an MEG study that localized activity in the IPL during the learning and during the recall of object names 158 A study that induced magnetic interference in participants IPL while they answered questions about an object reported that the participants were capable of answering questions regarding the object s characteristics or perceptual attributes but were impaired when asked whether the word contained two or three syllables 159 An MEG study has also correlated recovery from anomia a disorder characterized by an impaired ability to name objects with changes in IPL activation 160 Further supporting the role of the IPL in encoding the sounds of words are studies reporting that compared to monolinguals bilinguals have greater cortical density in the IPL but not the MTG 161 162 Because evidence shows that in bilinguals different phonological representations of the same word share the same semantic representation 163 this increase in density in the IPL verifies the existence of the phonological lexicon the semantic lexicon of bilinguals is expected to be similar in size to the semantic lexicon of monolinguals whereas their phonological lexicon should be twice the size Consistent with this finding cortical density in the IPL of monolinguals also correlates with vocabulary size 164 165 Notably the functional dissociation of the AVS and ADS in object naming tasks is supported by cumulative evidence from reading research showing that semantic errors are correlated with MTG impairment and phonemic errors with IPL impairment Based on these associations the semantic analysis of text has been linked to the inferior temporal gyrus and MTG and the phonological analysis of text has been linked to the pSTG Spt IPL 166 167 168 Phonological working memory edit Working memory is often treated as the temporary activation of the representations stored in long term memory that are used for speech phonological representations This sharing of resources between working memory and speech is evident by the finding 169 170 that speaking during rehearsal results in a significant reduction in the number of items that can be recalled from working memory articulatory suppression The involvement of the phonological lexicon in working memory is also evidenced by the tendency of individuals to make more errors when recalling words from a recently learned list of phonologically similar words than from a list of phonologically dissimilar words the phonological similarity effect 169 Studies have also found that speech errors committed during reading are remarkably similar to speech errors made during the recall of recently learned phonologically similar words from working memory 171 Patients with IPL damage have also been observed to exhibit both speech production errors and impaired working memory 172 173 174 175 Finally the view that verbal working memory is the result of temporarily activating phonological representations in the ADS is compatible with recent models describing working memory as the combination of maintaining representations in the mechanism of attention in parallel to temporarily activating representations in long term memory 170 176 177 178 It has been argued that the role of the ADS in the rehearsal of lists of words is the reason this pathway is active during sentence comprehension 179 For a review of the role of the ADS in working memory see 180 nbsp The from where to what model of language evolution hypotheses 7 stages of language evolution Linguistic theories editLanguage processing research informs theories of language The primary theoretical question is whether linguistic structures follow from the brain structures or vice versa Externalist models such as Ferdinand de Saussure s structuralism argue that language as a social phenomenon is external to the brain The individual receives the linguistic system from the outside and the given language shapes the individual s brain 181 This idea is opposed by internalist models including Noam Chomsky s transformational generative grammar George Lakoff s Cognitive Linguistics and John A Hawkins s efficiency hypothesis According to Chomsky language is acquired from an innate brain structure independently of meaning 182 Lakoff argues that language emerges from the sensory systems 183 Hawkins hypothesizes that cross linguistically prevalent patterns are based on the brain s natural processing preferences 184 Additionally models inspired by Richard Dawkins s memetics including Construction Grammar and Usage Based Linguistics advocate a two way model arguing that the brain shapes language and language shapes the brain 185 186 Evidence from neuroimaging studies points towards the externalist position ERP studies suggest that language processing is based on the interaction of syntax and semantics and the research does not support innate grammatical structures 187 188 MRI studies suggest that the structural characteristics of the child s first language shapes the processing connectome of the brain 189 Processing research has failed to find support for the inverse idea that syntactic structures reflect the brain s natural processing preferences cross linguistically 190 The evolution of language editThe auditory dorsal stream also has non language related functions such as sound localization 191 192 193 194 195 and guidance of eye movements 196 197 Recent studies also indicate a role of the ADS in localization of family tribe members as a study 198 that recorded from the cortex of an epileptic patient reported that the pSTG but not aSTG is selective for the presence of new speakers An fMRI 199 study of fetuses at their third trimester also demonstrated that area Spt is more selective to female speech than pure tones and a sub section of Spt is selective to the speech of their mother in contrast to unfamiliar female voices It is presently unknown why so many functions are ascribed to the human ADS An attempt to unify these functions under a single framework was conducted in the From where to what model of language evolution 200 201 In accordance with this model each function of the ADS indicates of a different intermediate phase in the evolution of language The roles of sound localization and integration of sound location with voices and auditory objects is interpreted as evidence that the origin of speech is the exchange of contact calls calls used to report location in cases of separation between mothers and offspring The role of the ADS in the perception and production of intonations is interpreted as evidence that speech began by modifying the contact calls with intonations possibly for distinguishing alarm contact calls from safe contact calls The role of the ADS in encoding the names of objects phonological long term memory is interpreted as evidence of gradual transition from modifying calls with intonations to complete vocal control The role of the ADS in the integration of lip movements with phonemes and in speech repetition is interpreted as evidence that spoken words were learned by infants mimicking their parents vocalizations initially by imitating their lip movements The role of the ADS in phonological working memory is interpreted as evidence that the words learned through mimicry remained active in the ADS even when not spoken This resulted with individuals capable of rehearsing a list of vocalizations which enabled the production of words with several syllables Further developments in the ADS enabled the rehearsal of lists of words which provided the infra structure for communicating with sentences Sign language in the brain editNeuroscientific research has provided a scientific understanding of how sign language is processed in the brain There are over 135 discrete sign languages around the world making use of different accents formed by separate areas of a country 202 By resorting to lesion analyses and neuroimaging neuroscientists have discovered that whether it be spoken or sign language human brains process language in general in a similar manner regarding which area of the brain is being used 202 Lesion analyses are used to examine the consequences of damage to specific brain regions involved in language while neuroimaging explore regions that are engaged in the processing of language 202 Previous hypotheses have been made that damage to Broca s area or Wernicke s area does not affect sign language being perceived however it is not the case Studies have shown that damage to these areas are similar in results in spoken language where sign errors are present and or repeated 202 In both types of languages they are affected by damage to the left hemisphere of the brain rather than the right usually dealing with the arts There are obvious patterns for utilizing and processing language In sign language Broca s area is activated while processing sign language employs Wernicke s area similar to that of spoken language 202 There have been other hypotheses about the lateralization of the two hemispheres Specifically the right hemisphere was thought to contribute to the overall communication of a language globally whereas the left hemisphere would be dominant in generating the language locally 203 Through research in aphasias RHD signers were found to have a problem maintaining the spatial portion of their signs confusing similar signs at different locations necessary to communicate with another properly 203 LHD signers on the other hand had similar results to those of hearing patients Furthermore other studies have emphasized that sign language is present bilaterally but will need to continue researching to reach a conclusion 203 Writing in the brain editThere is a comparatively small body of research on the neurology of reading and writing 204 Most of the studies performed deal with reading rather than writing or spelling and the majority of both kinds focus solely on the English language 205 English orthography is less transparent than that of other languages using a Latin script 204 Another difficulty is that some studies focus on spelling words of English and omit the few logographic characters found in the script 204 In terms of spelling English words can be divided into three categories regular irregular and novel words or nonwords Regular words are those in which there is a regular one to one correspondence between grapheme and phoneme in spelling Irregular words are those in which no such correspondence exists Nonwords are those that exhibit the expected orthography of regular words but do not carry meaning such as nonce words and onomatopoeia 204 An issue in the cognitive and neurological study of reading and spelling in English is whether a single route or dual route model best describes how literate speakers are able to read and write all three categories of English words according to accepted standards of orthographic correctness Single route models posit that lexical memory is used to store all spellings of words for retrieval in a single process Dual route models posit that lexical memory is employed to process irregular and high frequency regular words while low frequency regular words and nonwords are processed using a sub lexical set of phonological rules 204 The single route model for reading has found support in computer modelling studies which suggest that readers identify words by their orthographic similarities to phonologically alike words 204 However cognitive and lesion studies lean towards the dual route model Cognitive spelling studies on children and adults suggest that spellers employ phonological rules in spelling regular words and nonwords while lexical memory is accessed to spell irregular words and high frequency words of all types 204 Similarly lesion studies indicate that lexical memory is used to store irregular words and certain regular words while phonological rules are used to spell nonwords 204 More recently neuroimaging studies using positron emission tomography and fMRI have suggested a balanced model in which the reading of all word types begins in the visual word form area but subsequently branches off into different routes depending upon whether or not access to lexical memory or semantic information is needed which would be expected with irregular words under a dual route model 204 A 2007 fMRI study found that subjects asked to produce regular words in a spelling task exhibited greater activation in the left posterior STG an area used for phonological processing while the spelling of irregular words produced greater activation of areas used for lexical memory and semantic processing such as the left IFG and left SMG and both hemispheres of the MTG 204 Spelling nonwords was found to access members of both pathways such as the left STG and bilateral MTG and ITG 204 Significantly it was found that spelling induces activation in areas such as the left fusiform gyrus and left SMG that are also important in reading suggesting that a similar pathway is used for both reading and writing 204 Far less information exists on the cognition and neurology of non alphabetic and non English scripts Every language has a morphological and a phonological component either of which can be recorded by a writing system Scripts recording words and morphemes are considered logographic while those recording phonological segments such as syllabaries and alphabets are phonographic 205 Most systems combine the two and have both logographic and phonographic characters 205 In terms of complexity writing systems can be characterized as transparent or opaque and as shallow or deep A transparent system exhibits an obvious correspondence between grapheme and sound while in an opaque system this relationship is less obvious The terms shallow and deep refer to the extent that a system s orthography represents morphemes as opposed to phonological segments 205 Systems that record larger morphosyntactic or phonological segments such as logographic systems and syllabaries put greater demand on the memory of users 205 It would thus be expected that an opaque or deep writing system would put greater demand on areas of the brain used for lexical memory than would a system with transparent or shallow orthography See also editSign language Phonology Auditory processing disorder Brodmann area Computational audiology Cognitive science Developmental verbal dyspraxia FOXP2 Language disorder Neurobiology Neurolinguistics Neuropsychology Neuroscience Origin of language Visual word form areaReferences edit Seidenberg MS Petitto LA 1987 Communication symbolic communication and language Comment on Savage Rumbaugh McDonald Sevcik Hopkins and Rupert 1986 Journal of Experimental Psychology General 116 3 279 287 doi 10 1037 0096 3445 116 3 279 S2CID 18329599 a b Geschwind N June 1965 Disconnexion syndromes in animals and man I review Brain 88 2 237 94 doi 10 1093 brain 88 2 237 PMID 5318481 a b Hickok G Poeppel D May 2007 The cortical organization of speech processing review Nature Reviews Neuroscience 8 5 393 402 doi 10 1038 nrn2113 PMID 17431404 S2CID 6199399 a b Gow DW June 2012 The cortical organization of lexical knowledge a dual lexicon model of spoken language processing review Brain and Language 121 3 273 88 doi 10 1016 j bandl 2012 03 005 PMC 3348354 PMID 22498237 Poliva O 2017 09 20 From where to what a neuroanatomically based evolutionary model of the emergence of speech in humans review F1000Research 4 67 doi 10 12688 f1000research 6175 3 PMC 5600004 PMID 28928931 nbsp Material was copied from this source which is available under a Creative Commons Attribution 4 0 International License Poliva O 2016 From Mimicry to Language A Neuroanatomically Based Evolutionary Model of the Emergence of Vocal Language review Frontiers in Neuroscience 10 307 doi 10 3389 fnins 2016 00307 PMC 4928493 PMID 27445676 nbsp Material was copied from this source which is available under a Creative Commons Attribution 4 0 International License Pickles JO 2015 Chapter 1 Auditory pathways anatomy and physiology In Aminoff MJ Boller F Swaab DF eds Handbook of Clinical Neurology review Vol 129 pp 3 25 doi 10 1016 B978 0 444 62630 1 00001 9 ISBN 978 0 444 62630 1 PMID 25726260 Lichteim L 1885 01 01 On Aphasia Brain 7 4 433 484 doi 10 1093 brain 7 4 433 hdl 11858 00 001M 0000 002C 5780 B Wernicke C 1974 Der aphasische Symptomenkomplex Springer Berlin Heidelberg pp 1 70 ISBN 978 3 540 06905 8 Aboitiz F Garcia VR December 1997 The evolutionary origin of the language areas in the human brain A neuroanatomical perspective Brain Research Brain Research Reviews 25 3 381 96 doi 10 1016 s0165 0173 97 00053 2 PMID 9495565 S2CID 20704891 a b Anderson JM Gilmore R Roper S Crosson B Bauer RM Nadeau S Beversdorf DQ Cibula J Rogish M Kortencamp S Hughes JD Gonzalez Rothi LJ Heilman KM October 1999 Conduction aphasia and the arcuate fasciculus A reexamination of the Wernicke Geschwind model Brain and Language 70 1 1 12 doi 10 1006 brln 1999 2135 PMID 10534369 S2CID 12171982 DeWitt I Rauschecker JP November 2013 Wernicke s area revisited parallel streams and word processing Brain and Language 127 2 181 91 doi 10 1016 j bandl 2013 09 014 PMC 4098851 PMID 24404576 Dronkers NF January 2000 The pursuit of brain language relationships Brain and Language 71 1 59 61 doi 10 1006 brln 1999 2212 PMID 10716807 S2CID 7224731 a b c Dronkers NF Wilkins DP Van Valin RD Redfern BB Jaeger JJ May 2004 Lesion analysis of the brain areas involved in language comprehension Cognition 92 1 2 145 77 doi 10 1016 j cognition 2003 11 002 hdl 11858 00 001M 0000 0012 6912 A PMID 15037129 S2CID 10919645 Mesulam MM Thompson CK Weintraub S Rogalski EJ August 2015 The Wernicke conundrum and the anatomy of language comprehension in primary progressive aphasia Brain 138 Pt 8 2423 37 doi 10 1093 brain awv154 PMC 4805066 PMID 26112340 Poeppel D Emmorey K Hickok G Pylkkanen L October 2012 Towards a new neurobiology of language The Journal of Neuroscience 32 41 14125 31 doi 10 1523 jneurosci 3244 12 2012 PMC 3495005 PMID 23055482 Vignolo LA Boccardi E Caverni L March 1986 Unexpected CT scan findings in global aphasia Cortex A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior 22 1 55 69 doi 10 1016 s0010 9452 86 80032 6 PMID 2423296 S2CID 4479679 a b Bendor D Wang X August 2006 Cortical representations of pitch in monkeys and humans Current Opinion in Neurobiology 16 4 391 9 doi 10 1016 j conb 2006 07 001 PMC 4325365 PMID 16842992 a b Rauschecker JP Tian B Hauser M April 1995 Processing of complex sounds in the macaque nonprimary auditory cortex Science 268 5207 111 4 Bibcode 1995Sci 268 111R doi 10 1126 science 7701330 PMID 7701330 S2CID 19590708 a b de la Mothe LA Blumell S Kajikawa Y Hackett TA May 2006 Cortical connections of the auditory cortex in marmoset monkeys core and medial belt regions The Journal of Comparative Neurology 496 1 27 71 doi 10 1002 cne 20923 PMID 16528722 S2CID 38393074 de la Mothe LA Blumell S Kajikawa Y Hackett TA May 2012 Cortical connections of auditory cortex in marmoset monkeys lateral belt and parabelt regions Anatomical Record 295 5 800 21 doi 10 1002 ar 22451 PMC 3379817 PMID 22461313 Kaas JH Hackett TA October 2000 Subdivisions of auditory cortex and processing streams in primates Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 97 22 11793 9 Bibcode 2000PNAS 9711793K doi 10 1073 pnas 97 22 11793 PMC 34351 PMID 11050211 Petkov CI Kayser C Augath M Logothetis NK July 2006 Functional imaging reveals numerous fields in the monkey auditory cortex PLOS Biology 4 7 e215 doi 10 1371 journal pbio 0040215 PMC 1479693 PMID 16774452 Morel A Garraghty PE Kaas JH September 1993 Tonotopic organization architectonic fields and connections of auditory cortex in macaque monkeys The Journal of Comparative Neurology 335 3 437 59 doi 10 1002 cne 903350312 PMID 7693772 S2CID 22872232 Rauschecker JP Tian B October 2000 Mechanisms and streams for processing of what and where in auditory cortex Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 97 22 11800 6 Bibcode 2000PNAS 9711800R doi 10 1073 pnas 97 22 11800 PMC 34352 PMID 11050212 Rauschecker JP Tian B Pons T Mishkin M May 1997 Serial and parallel processing in rhesus monkey auditory cortex The Journal of Comparative Neurology 382 1 89 103 doi 10 1002 sici 1096 9861 19970526 382 1 lt 89 aid cne6 gt 3 3 co 2 y PMID 9136813 Sweet RA Dorph Petersen KA Lewis DA October 2005 Mapping auditory core lateral belt and parabelt cortices in the human superior temporal gyrus The Journal of Comparative Neurology 491 3 270 89 doi 10 1002 cne 20702 PMID 16134138 S2CID 40822276 Wallace MN Johnston PW Palmer AR April 2002 Histochemical identification of cortical areas in the auditory region of the human brain Experimental Brain Research 143 4 499 508 doi 10 1007 s00221 002 1014 z PMID 11914796 S2CID 24211906 Da Costa S van der Zwaag W Marques JP Frackowiak RS Clarke S Saenz M October 2011 Human primary auditory cortex follows the shape of Heschl s gyrus The Journal of Neuroscience 31 40 14067 75 doi 10 1523 jneurosci 2000 11 2011 PMC 6623669 PMID 21976491 Humphries C Liebenthal E Binder JR April 2010 Tonotopic organization of human auditory cortex NeuroImage 50 3 1202 11 doi 10 1016 j neuroimage 2010 01 046 PMC 2830355 PMID 20096790 Langers DR van Dijk P September 2012 Mapping the tonotopic organization in human auditory cortex with minimally salient acoustic stimulation Cerebral Cortex 22 9 2024 38 doi 10 1093 cercor bhr282 PMC 3412441 PMID 21980020 Striem Amit E Hertz U Amedi A March 2011 Extensive cochleotopic mapping of human auditory cortical fields obtained with phase encoding fMRI PLOS ONE 6 3 e17832 Bibcode 2011PLoSO 617832S doi 10 1371 journal pone 0017832 PMC 3063163 PMID 21448274 Woods DL Herron TJ Cate AD Yund EW Stecker GC Rinne T Kang X 2010 Functional properties of human auditory cortical fields Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience 4 155 doi 10 3389 fnsys 2010 00155 PMC 3001989 PMID 21160558 Gourevitch B Le Bouquin Jeannes R Faucon G Liegeois Chauvel C March 2008 Temporal envelope processing in the human auditory cortex response and interconnections of auditory cortical areas PDF Hearing Research 237 1 2 1 18 doi 10 1016 j heares 2007 12 003 PMID 18255243 S2CID 15271578 Gueguin M Le Bouquin Jeannes R Faucon G Chauvel P Liegeois Chauvel C February 2007 Evidence of functional connectivity between auditory cortical areas revealed by amplitude modulation sound processing Cerebral Cortex 17 2 304 13 doi 10 1093 cercor bhj148 PMC 2111045 PMID 16514106 a b c Poliva O Bestelmeyer PE Hall M Bultitude JH Koller K Rafal RD September 2015 Functional Mapping of the Human Auditory Cortex fMRI Investigation of a Patient with Auditory Agnosia from Trauma to the Inferior Colliculus PDF Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology 28 3 160 80 doi 10 1097 wnn 0000000000000072 PMID 26413744 S2CID 913296 Chang EF Edwards E Nagarajan SS Fogelson N Dalal SS Canolty RT Kirsch HE Barbaro NM Knight RT June 2011 Cortical spatio temporal dynamics underlying phonological target detection in humans Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 23 6 1437 46 doi 10 1162 jocn 2010 21466 PMC 3895406 PMID 20465359 Munoz M Mishkin M Saunders RC September 2009 Resection of the medial temporal lobe disconnects the rostral superior temporal gyrus from some of its projection targets in the frontal lobe and thalamus Cerebral Cortex 19 9 2114 30 doi 10 1093 cercor bhn236 PMC 2722427 PMID 19150921 a b Romanski LM Bates JF Goldman Rakic PS January 1999 Auditory belt and parabelt projections to the prefrontal cortex in the rhesus monkey The Journal of Comparative Neurology 403 2 141 57 doi 10 1002 sici 1096 9861 19990111 403 2 lt 141 aid cne1 gt 3 0 co 2 v PMID 9886040 S2CID 42482082 Tanaka D June 1976 Thalamic projections of the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex in the rhesus monkey Macaca mulatta Brain Research 110 1 21 38 doi 10 1016 0006 8993 76 90206 7 PMID 819108 S2CID 21529048 a b Perrodin C Kayser C Logothetis NK Petkov CI August 2011 Voice cells in the primate temporal lobe Current Biology 21 16 1408 15 doi 10 1016 j cub 2011 07 028 PMC 3398143 PMID 21835625 a b c Petkov CI Kayser C Steudel T Whittingstall K Augath M Logothetis NK March 2008 A voice region in the monkey brain Nature Neuroscience 11 3 367 74 doi 10 1038 nn2043 PMID 18264095 S2CID 5505773 a b Poremba A Malloy M Saunders RC Carson RE Herscovitch P Mishkin M January 2004 Species specific calls evoke asymmetric activity in the monkey s temporal poles Nature 427 6973 448 51 Bibcode 2004Natur 427 448P doi 10 1038 nature02268 PMID 14749833 S2CID 4402126 Romanski LM Averbeck BB Diltz M February 2005 Neural representation of vocalizations in the primate ventrolateral prefrontal cortex Journal of Neurophysiology 93 2 734 47 doi 10 1152 jn 00675 2004 PMID 15371495 Russ BE Ackelson AL Baker AE Cohen YE January 2008 Coding of auditory stimulus identity in the auditory non spatial processing stream Journal of Neurophysiology 99 1 87 95 doi 10 1152 jn 01069 2007 PMC 4091985 PMID 18003874 a b Tsunada J Lee JH Cohen YE June 2011 Representation of speech categories in the primate auditory cortex Journal of Neurophysiology 105 6 2634 46 doi 10 1152 jn 00037 2011 PMC 3118748 PMID 21346209 Cusick CG Seltzer B Cola M Griggs E September 1995 Chemoarchitectonics and corticocortical terminations within the superior temporal sulcus of the rhesus monkey evidence for subdivisions of superior temporal polysensory cortex The Journal of Comparative Neurology 360 3 513 35 doi 10 1002 cne 903600312 PMID 8543656 S2CID 42281619 Cohen YE Russ BE Gifford GW Kiringoda R MacLean KA December 2004 Selectivity for the spatial and nonspatial attributes of auditory stimuli in the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex The Journal of Neuroscience 24 50 11307 16 doi 10 1523 jneurosci 3935 04 2004 PMC 6730358 PMID 15601937 Deacon TW February 1992 Cortical connections of the inferior arcuate sulcus cortex in the macaque brain Brain Research 573 1 8 26 doi 10 1016 0006 8993 92 90109 m ISSN 0006 8993 PMID 1374284 S2CID 20670766 Lewis JW Van Essen DC December 2000 Corticocortical connections of visual sensorimotor and multimodal processing areas in the parietal lobe of the macaque monkey The Journal of Comparative Neurology 428 1 112 37 doi 10 1002 1096 9861 20001204 428 1 lt 112 aid cne8 gt 3 0 co 2 9 PMID 11058227 S2CID 16153360 Roberts AC Tomic DL Parkinson CH Roeling TA Cutter DJ Robbins TW Everitt BJ May 2007 Forebrain connectivity of the prefrontal cortex in the marmoset monkey Callithrix jacchus an anterograde and retrograde tract tracing study The Journal of Comparative Neurology 502 1 86 112 doi 10 1002 cne 21300 PMID 17335041 S2CID 18262007 a b Schmahmann JD Pandya DN Wang R Dai G D Arceuil HE de Crespigny AJ Wedeen VJ March 2007 Association fibre pathways of the brain parallel observations from diffusion spectrum imaging and autoradiography Brain 130 Pt 3 630 53 doi 10 1093 brain awl359 PMID 17293361 Seltzer B Pandya DN July 1984 Further observations on parieto temporal connections in the rhesus monkey Experimental Brain Research 55 2 301 12 doi 10 1007 bf00237280 PMID 6745368 S2CID 20167953 Catani M Jones DK ffytche DH January 2005 Perisylvian language networks of the human brain Annals of Neurology 57 1 8 16 doi 10 1002 ana 20319 PMID 15597383 S2CID 17743067 Frey S Campbell JS Pike GB Petrides M November 2008 Dissociating the human language pathways with high angular resolution diffusion fiber tractography The Journal of Neuroscience 28 45 11435 44 doi 10 1523 jneurosci 2388 08 2008 PMC 6671318 PMID 18987180 Makris N Papadimitriou GM Kaiser JR Sorg S Kennedy DN Pandya DN April 2009 Delineation of the middle longitudinal fascicle in humans a quantitative in vivo DT MRI study Cerebral Cortex 19 4 777 85 doi 10 1093 cercor bhn124 PMC 2651473 PMID 18669591 Menjot de Champfleur N Lima Maldonado I Moritz Gasser S Machi P Le Bars E Bonafe A Duffau H January 2013 Middle longitudinal fasciculus delineation within language pathways a diffusion tensor imaging study in human European Journal of Radiology 82 1 151 7 doi 10 1016 j ejrad 2012 05 034 PMID 23084876 Turken AU Dronkers NF 2011 The neural architecture of the language comprehension network converging evidence from lesion and connectivity analyses Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience 5 1 doi 10 3389 fnsys 2011 00001 PMC 3039157 PMID 21347218 Saur D Kreher BW Schnell S Kummerer D Kellmeyer P Vry MS Umarova R Musso M Glauche V Abel S Huber W Rijntjes M Hennig J Weiller C November 2008 Ventral and dorsal pathways for language Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 105 46 18035 40 Bibcode 2008PNAS 10518035S doi 10 1073 pnas 0805234105 PMC 2584675 PMID 19004769 Yin P Mishkin M Sutter M Fritz JB December 2008 Early stages of melody processing stimulus sequence and task dependent neuronal activity in monkey auditory cortical fields A1 and R Journal of Neurophysiology 100 6 3009 29 doi 10 1152 jn 00828 2007 PMC 2604844 PMID 18842950 Steinschneider M Volkov IO Fishman YI Oya H Arezzo JC Howard MA February 2005 Intracortical responses in human and monkey primary auditory cortex support a temporal processing mechanism for encoding of the voice onset time phonetic parameter Cerebral Cortex 15 2 170 86 doi 10 1093 cercor bhh120 PMID 15238437 Russ BE Ackelson AL Baker AE Cohen YE January 2008 Coding of auditory stimulus identity in the auditory non spatial processing stream Journal of Neurophysiology 99 1 87 95 doi 10 1152 jn 01069 2007 PMC 4091985 PMID 18003874 Joly O Pallier C Ramus F Pressnitzer D Vanduffel W Orban GA September 2012 Processing of vocalizations in humans and monkeys a comparative fMRI study PDF NeuroImage 62 3 1376 89 doi 10 1016 j neuroimage 2012 05 070 PMID 22659478 S2CID 9441377 Scheich H Baumgart F Gaschler Markefski B Tegeler C Tempelmann C Heinze HJ Schindler F Stiller D February 1998 Functional magnetic resonance imaging of a human auditory cortex area involved in foreground background decomposition The European Journal of Neuroscience 10 2 803 9 doi 10 1046 j 1460 9568 1998 00086 x PMID 9749748 S2CID 42898063 Zatorre RJ Bouffard M Belin P April 2004 Sensitivity to auditory object features in human temporal neocortex The Journal of Neuroscience 24 14 3637 42 doi 10 1523 jneurosci 5458 03 2004 PMC 6729744 PMID 15071112 a b Binder JR Desai RH Graves WW Conant LL December 2009 Where is the semantic system A critical review and meta analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies Cerebral Cortex 19 12 2767 96 doi 10 1093 cercor bhp055 PMC 2774390 PMID 19329570 Davis MH Johnsrude IS April 2003 Hierarchical processing in spoken language comprehension The Journal of Neuroscience 23 8 3423 31 doi 10 1523 jneurosci 23 08 03423 2003 PMC 6742313 PMID 12716950 Liebenthal E Binder JR Spitzer SM Possing ET Medler DA October 2005 Neural substrates of phonemic perception Cerebral Cortex 15 10 1621 31 doi 10 1093 cercor bhi040 PMID 15703256 Narain C Scott SK Wise RJ Rosen S Leff A Iversen SD Matthews PM December 2003 Defining a left lateralized response specific to intelligible speech using fMRI Cerebral Cortex 13 12 1362 8 doi 10 1093 cercor bhg083 PMID 14615301 Obleser J Boecker H Drzezga A Haslinger B Hennenlotter A Roettinger M Eulitz C Rauschecker JP July 2006 Vowel sound extraction in anterior superior temporal cortex Human Brain Mapping 27 7 562 71 doi 10 1002 hbm 20201 PMC 6871493 PMID 16281283 Obleser J Zimmermann J Van Meter J Rauschecker JP October 2007 Multiple stages of auditory speech perception reflected in event related FMRI Cerebral Cortex 17 10 2251 7 doi 10 1093 cercor bhl133 PMID 17150986 Scott SK Blank CC Rosen S Wise RJ December 2000 Identification of a pathway for intelligible speech in the left temporal lobe Brain 123 12 2400 6 doi 10 1093 brain 123 12 2400 PMC 5630088 PMID 11099443 Belin P Zatorre RJ November 2003 Adaptation to speaker s voice in right anterior temporal lobe NeuroReport 14 16 2105 2109 doi 10 1097 00001756 200311140 00019 PMID 14600506 S2CID 34183900 Benson RR Whalen DH Richardson M Swainson B Clark VP Lai S Liberman AM September 2001 Parametrically dissociating speech and nonspeech perception in the brain using fMRI Brain and Language 78 3 364 96 doi 10 1006 brln 2001 2484 PMID 11703063 S2CID 15328590 Leaver AM Rauschecker JP June 2010 Cortical representation of natural complex sounds effects of acoustic features and auditory object category The Journal of Neuroscience 30 22 7604 12 doi 10 1523 jneurosci 0296 10 2010 PMC 2930617 PMID 20519535 Lewis JW Phinney RE Brefczynski Lewis JA DeYoe EA August 2006 Lefties get it right when hearing tool sounds Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 18 8 1314 30 doi 10 1162 jocn 2006 18 8 1314 PMID 16859417 S2CID 14049095 Maeder PP Meuli RA Adriani M Bellmann A Fornari E Thiran JP Pittet A Clarke S October 2001 Distinct pathways involved in sound recognition and localization a human fMRI study PDF NeuroImage 14 4 802 16 doi 10 1006 nimg 2001 0888 PMID 11554799 S2CID 1388647 Viceic D Fornari E Thiran JP Maeder PP Meuli R Adriani M Clarke S November 2006 Human auditory belt areas specialized in sound recognition a functional magnetic resonance imaging study PDF NeuroReport 17 16 1659 62 doi 10 1097 01 wnr 0000239962 75943 dd PMID 17047449 S2CID 14482187 Shultz S Vouloumanos A Pelphrey K May 2012 The superior temporal sulcus differentiates communicative and noncommunicative auditory signals Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 24 5 1224 32 doi 10 1162 jocn a 00208 PMID 22360624 S2CID 10784270 DeWitt I Rauschecker JP February 2012 Phoneme and word recognition in the auditory ventral stream Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 109 8 E505 14 Bibcode 2012PNAS 109E 505D doi 10 1073 pnas 1113427109 PMC 3286918 PMID 22308358 a b c d Lachaux JP Jerbi K Bertrand O Minotti L Hoffmann D Schoendorff B Kahane P October 2007 A blueprint for real time functional mapping via human intracranial recordings PLOS ONE 2 10 e1094 Bibcode 2007PLoSO 2 1094L doi 10 1371 journal pone 0001094 PMC 2040217 PMID 17971857 a b Matsumoto R Imamura H Inouchi M Nakagawa T Yokoyama Y Matsuhashi M Mikuni N Miyamoto S Fukuyama H Takahashi R Ikeda A April 2011 Left anterior temporal cortex actively engages in speech perception A direct cortical stimulation study Neuropsychologia 49 5 1350 1354 doi 10 1016 j neuropsychologia 2011 01 023 hdl 2433 141342 PMID 21251921 S2CID 1831334 a b c d Roux FE Miskin K Durand JB Sacko O Rehault E Tanova R Demonet JF October 2015 Electrostimulation mapping of comprehension of auditory and visual words Cortex A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior 71 398 408 doi 10 1016 j cortex 2015 07 001 PMID 26332785 S2CID 39964328 Fritz J Mishkin M Saunders RC June 2005 In search of an auditory engram Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 102 26 9359 64 Bibcode 2005PNAS 102 9359F doi 10 1073 pnas 0503998102 PMC 1166637 PMID 15967995 Stepien LS Cordeau JP Rasmussen T 1960 The effect of temporal lobe and hippocampal lesions on auditory and visual recent memory in monkeys Brain 83 3 470 489 doi 10 1093 brain 83 3 470 ISSN 0006 8950 Strominger NL Oesterreich RE Neff WD June 1980 Sequential auditory and visual discriminations after temporal lobe ablation in monkeys Physiology amp Behavior 24 6 1149 56 doi 10 1016 0031 9384 80 90062 1 PMID 6774349 S2CID 7494152 Kaiser J Ripper B Birbaumer N Lutzenberger W October 2003 Dynamics of gamma band activity in human magnetoencephalogram during auditory pattern working memory NeuroImage 20 2 816 27 doi 10 1016 s1053 8119 03 00350 1 PMID 14568454 S2CID 19373941 Buchsbaum BR Olsen RK Koch P Berman KF November 2005 Human dorsal and ventral auditory streams subserve rehearsal based and echoic processes during verbal working memory Neuron 48 4 687 97 doi 10 1016 j neuron 2005 09 029 PMID 16301183 S2CID 13202604 Scott BH Mishkin M Yin P July 2012 Monkeys have a limited form of short term memory in audition Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 109 30 12237 41 Bibcode 2012PNAS 10912237S doi 10 1073 pnas 1209685109 PMC 3409773 PMID 22778411 Noppeney U Patterson K Tyler LK Moss H Stamatakis EA Bright P Mummery C Price CJ April 2007 Temporal lobe lesions and semantic impairment a comparison of herpes simplex virus encephalitis and semantic dementia Brain 130 Pt 4 1138 47 doi 10 1093 brain awl344 PMID 17251241 Patterson K Nestor PJ Rogers TT December 2007 Where do you know what you know The representation of semantic knowledge in the human brain Nature Reviews Neuroscience 8 12 976 87 doi 10 1038 nrn2277 PMID 18026167 S2CID 7310189 Schwartz MF Kimberg DY Walker GM Faseyitan O Brecher A Dell GS Coslett HB December 2009 Anterior temporal involvement in semantic word retrieval voxel based lesion symptom mapping evidence from aphasia Brain 132 Pt 12 3411 27 doi 10 1093 brain awp284 PMC 2792374 PMID 19942676 Hamberger MJ McClelland S McKhann GM Williams AC Goodman RR March 2007 Distribution of auditory and visual naming sites in nonlesional temporal lobe epilepsy patients and patients with space occupying temporal lobe lesions Epilepsia 48 3 531 8 doi 10 1111 j 1528 1167 2006 00955 x PMID 17326797 S2CID 12642281 a b c Duffau H March 2008 The anatomo functional connectivity of language revisited New insights provided by electrostimulation and tractography Neuropsychologia 46 4 927 34 doi 10 1016 j neuropsychologia 2007 10 025 PMID 18093622 S2CID 40514753 Vigneau M Beaucousin V Herve PY Duffau H Crivello F Houde O Mazoyer B Tzourio Mazoyer N May 2006 Meta analyzing left hemisphere language areas phonology semantics and sentence processing NeuroImage 30 4 1414 32 doi 10 1016 j neuroimage 2005 11 002 PMID 16413796 S2CID 8870165 a b Creutzfeldt O Ojemann G Lettich E October 1989 Neuronal activity in the human lateral temporal lobe I Responses to speech Experimental Brain Research 77 3 451 75 doi 10 1007 bf00249600 hdl 11858 00 001M 0000 002C 89EA 3 PMID 2806441 S2CID 19952034 Mazoyer BM Tzourio N Frak V Syrota A Murayama N Levrier O Salamon G Dehaene S Cohen L Mehler J October 1993 The cortical representation of speech PDF Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 5 4 467 79 doi 10 1162 jocn 1993 5 4 467 PMID 23964919 S2CID 22265355 Humphries C Love T Swinney D Hickok G October 2005 Response of anterior temporal cortex to syntactic and prosodic manipulations during sentence processing Human Brain Mapping 26 2 128 38 doi 10 1002 hbm 20148 PMC 6871757 PMID 15895428 Humphries C Willard K Buchsbaum B Hickok G June 2001 Role of anterior temporal cortex in auditory sentence comprehension an fMRI study NeuroReport 12 8 1749 52 doi 10 1097 00001756 200106130 00046 PMID 11409752 S2CID 13039857 Vandenberghe R Nobre AC Price CJ May 2002 The response of left temporal cortex to sentences Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 14 4 550 60 doi 10 1162 08989290260045800 PMID 12126497 S2CID 21607482 Friederici AD Ruschemeyer SA Hahne A Fiebach CJ February 2003 The role of left inferior frontal and superior temporal cortex in sentence comprehension localizing syntactic and semantic processes Cerebral Cortex 13 2 170 7 doi 10 1093 cercor 13 2 170 hdl 11858 00 001M 0000 0010 E3AB B PMID 12507948 Xu J Kemeny S Park G Frattali C Braun A 2005 Language in context emergent features of word sentence and narrative comprehension NeuroImage 25 3 1002 15 doi 10 1016 j neuroimage 2004 12 013 PMID 15809000 S2CID 25570583 Rogalsky C Hickok G April 2009 Selective attention to semantic and syntactic features modulates sentence processing networks in anterior temporal cortex Cerebral Cortex 19 4 786 96 doi 10 1093 cercor bhn126 PMC 2651476 PMID 18669589 Pallier C Devauchelle AD Dehaene S February 2011 Cortical representation of the constituent structure of sentences Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 108 6 2522 7 doi 10 1073 pnas 1018711108 PMC 3038732 PMID 21224415 Brennan J Nir Y Hasson U Malach R Heeger DJ Pylkkanen L February 2012 Syntactic structure building in the anterior temporal lobe during natural story listening Brain and Language 120 2 163 73 doi 10 1016 j bandl 2010 04 002 PMC 2947556 PMID 20472279 Kotz SA von Cramon DY Friederici AD October 2003 Differentiation of syntactic processes in the left and right anterior temporal lobe Event related brain potential evidence from lesion patients Brain and Language 87 1 135 136 doi 10 1016 s0093 934x 03 00236 0 S2CID 54320415 Martin RC Shelton JR Yaffee LS February 1994 Language processing and working memory Neuropsychological evidence for separate phonological and semantic capacities Journal of Memory and Language 33 1 83 111 doi 10 1006 jmla 1994 1005 Magnusdottir S Fillmore P den Ouden DB Hjaltason H Rorden C Kjartansson O Bonilha L Fridriksson J October 2013 Damage to left anterior temporal cortex predicts impairment of complex syntactic processing a lesion symptom mapping study Human Brain Mapping 34 10 2715 23 doi 10 1002 hbm 22096 PMC 6869931 PMID 22522937 Bornkessel Schlesewsky I Schlesewsky M Small SL Rauschecker JP March 2015 Neurobiological roots of language in primate audition common computational properties Trends in Cognitive Sciences 19 3 142 50 doi 10 1016 j tics 2014 12 008 PMC 4348204 PMID 25600585 Hickok G Okada K Barr W Pa J Rogalsky C Donnelly K Barde L Grant A December 2008 Bilateral capacity for speech sound processing in auditory comprehension evidence from Wada procedures Brain and Language 107 3 179 84 doi 10 1016 j bandl 2008 09 006 PMC 2644214 PMID 18976806 Zaidel E September 1976 Auditory Vocabulary of the Right Hemisphere Following Brain Bisection or Hemidecortication Cortex 12 3 191 211 doi 10 1016 s0010 9452 76 80001 9 ISSN 0010 9452 PMID 1000988 S2CID 4479925 Poeppel D October 2001 Pure word deafness and the bilateral processing of the speech code Cognitive Science 25 5 679 693 doi 10 1016 s0364 0213 01 00050 7 Ulrich G May 1978 Interhemispheric functional relationships in auditory agnosia An analysis of the preconditions and a conceptual model Brain and Language 5 3 286 300 doi 10 1016 0093 934x 78 90027 5 PMID 656899 S2CID 33841186 Stewart L Walsh V Frith U Rothwell JC March 2001 TMS produces two dissociable types of speech disruption PDF NeuroImage 13 3 472 8 doi 10 1006 nimg 2000 0701 PMID 11170812 S2CID 10392466 Acheson DJ Hamidi M Binder JR Postle BR June 2011 A common neural substrate for language production and verbal working memory Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 23 6 1358 67 doi 10 1162 jocn 2010 21519 PMC 3053417 PMID 20617889 Desmurget M Reilly KT Richard N Szathmari A Mottolese C Sirigu A May 2009 Movement intention after parietal cortex stimulation in humans Science 324 5928 811 3 Bibcode 2009Sci 324 811D doi 10 1126 science 1169896 PMID 19423830 S2CID 6555881 Edwards E Nagarajan SS Dalal SS Canolty RT Kirsch HE Barbaro NM Knight RT March 2010 Spatiotemporal imaging of cortical activation during verb generation and picture naming NeuroImage 50 1 291 301 doi 10 1016 j neuroimage 2009 12 035 PMC 2957470 PMID 20026224 Boatman D Gordon B Hart J Selnes O Miglioretti D Lenz F August 2000 Transcortical sensory aphasia revisited and revised Brain 123 8 1634 42 doi 10 1093 brain 123 8 1634 PMID 10908193 a b c Turkeltaub PE Coslett HB July 2010 Localization of sublexical speech perception components Brain and Language 114 1 1 15 doi 10 1016 j bandl 2010 03 008 PMC 2914564 PMID 20413149 Chang EF Rieger JW Johnson K Berger MS Barbaro NM Knight RT November 2010 Categorical speech representation in human superior temporal gyrus Nature Neuroscience 13 11 1428 32 doi 10 1038 nn 2641 PMC 2967728 PMID 20890293 Buchsbaum BR Hickok G Humphries C September 2001 Role of left posterior superior temporal gyrus in phonological processing for speech perception and production Cognitive Science 25 5 663 678 doi 10 1207 s15516709cog2505 2 ISSN 0364 0213 Wise RJ Scott SK Blank SC Mummery CJ Murphy K Warburton EA January 2001 Separate neural subsystems within Wernicke s area Brain 124 Pt 1 83 95 doi 10 1093 brain 124 1 83 PMID 11133789 Hickok G Buchsbaum B Humphries C Muftuler T July 2003 Auditory motor interaction revealed by fMRI speech music and working memory in area Spt Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 15 5 673 82 doi 10 1162 089892903322307393 PMID 12965041 Warren JE Wise RJ Warren JD December 2005 Sounds do able auditory motor transformations and the posterior temporal plane Trends in Neurosciences 28 12 636 43 doi 10 1016 j tins 2005 09 010 PMID 16216346 S2CID 36678139 Hickok G Poeppel D May 2007 The cortical organization of speech processing Nature Reviews Neuroscience 8 5 393 402 doi 10 1038 nrn2113 PMID 17431404 S2CID 6199399 Karbe H Herholz K Weber Luxenburger G Ghaemi M Heiss WD June 1998 Cerebral networks and functional brain asymmetry evidence from regional metabolic changes during word repetition Brain and Language 63 1 108 21 doi 10 1006 brln 1997 1937 PMID 9642023 S2CID 31335617 Giraud AL Price CJ August 2001 The constraints functional neuroimaging places on classical models of auditory word processing Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 13 6 754 65 doi 10 1162 08989290152541421 PMID 11564320 S2CID 13916709 Graves WW Grabowski TJ Mehta S Gupta P September 2008 The left posterior superior temporal gyrus participates specifically in accessing lexical phonology Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 20 9 1698 710 doi 10 1162 jocn 2008 20113 PMC 2570618 PMID 18345989 a b Towle VL Yoon HA Castelle M Edgar JC Biassou NM Frim DM Spire JP Kohrman MH August 2008 ECoG gamma activity during a language task differentiating expressive and receptive speech areas Brain 131 Pt 8 2013 27 doi 10 1093 brain awn147 PMC 2724904 PMID 18669510 Selnes OA Knopman DS Niccum N Rubens AB June 1985 The critical role of Wernicke s area in sentence repetition Annals of Neurology 17 6 549 57 doi 10 1002 ana 410170604 PMID 4026225 S2CID 12914191 Axer H von Keyserlingk AG Berks G von Keyserlingk DG March 2001 Supra and infrasylvian conduction aphasia Brain and Language 76 3 317 31 doi 10 1006 brln 2000 2425 PMID 11247647 S2CID 25406527 Bartha L Benke T April 2003 Acute conduction aphasia an analysis of 20 cases Brain and Language 85 1 93 108 doi 10 1016 s0093 934x 02 00502 3 PMID 12681350 S2CID 18466425 Baldo JV Katseff S Dronkers NF March 2012 Brain Regions Underlying Repetition and Auditory Verbal Short term Memory Deficits in Aphasia Evidence from Voxel based Lesion Symptom Mapping Aphasiology 26 3 4 338 354 doi 10 1080 02687038 2011 602391 PMC 4070523 PMID 24976669 Baldo JV Klostermann EC Dronkers NF May 2008 It s either a cook or a baker patients with conduction aphasia get the gist but lose the trace Brain and Language 105 2 134 40 doi 10 1016 j bandl 2007 12 007 PMID 18243294 S2CID 997735 Fridriksson J Kjartansson O Morgan PS Hjaltason H Magnusdottir S Bonilha L Rorden C August 2010 Impaired speech repetition and left parietal lobe damage The Journal of Neuroscience 30 33 11057 61 doi 10 1523 jneurosci 1120 10 2010 PMC 2936270 PMID 20720112 Buchsbaum BR Baldo J Okada K Berman KF Dronkers N D Esposito M Hickok G December 2011 Conduction aphasia sensory motor integration and phonological short term memory an aggregate analysis of lesion and fMRI data Brain and Language 119 3 119 28 doi 10 1016 j bandl 2010 12 001 PMC 3090694 PMID 21256582 Yamada K Nagakane Y Mizuno T Hosomi A Nakagawa M Nishimura T March 2007 MR tractography depicting damage to the arcuate fasciculus in a patient with conduction aphasia Neurology 68 10 789 doi 10 1212 01 wnl 0000256348 65744 b2 PMID 17339591 Breier JI Hasan KM Zhang W Men D Papanicolaou AC March 2008 Language dysfunction after stroke and damage to white matter tracts evaluated using diffusion tensor imaging AJNR American Journal of Neuroradiology 29 3 483 7 doi 10 3174 ajnr A0846 PMC 3073452 PMID 18039757 Zhang Y Wang C Zhao X Chen H Han Z Wang Y September 2010 Diffusion tensor imaging depicting damage to the arcuate fasciculus in patients with conduction aphasia a study of the Wernicke Geschwind model Neurological Research 32 7 775 8 doi 10 1179 016164109x12478302362653 PMID 19825277 S2CID 22960870 Jones OP Prejawa S Hope TM Oberhuber M Seghier ML Leff AP Green DW Price CJ 2014 Sensory to motor integration during auditory repetition a combined fMRI and lesion study Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 8 24 doi 10 3389 fnhum 2014 00024 PMC 3908611 PMID 24550807 Quigg M Fountain NB March 1999 Conduction aphasia elicited by stimulation of the left posterior superior temporal gyrus Journal of Neurology Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 66 3 393 6 doi 10 1136 jnnp 66 3 393 PMC 1736266 PMID 10084542 Quigg M Geldmacher DS Elias WJ May 2006 Conduction aphasia as a function of the dominant posterior perisylvian cortex Report of two cases Journal of Neurosurgery 104 5 845 8 doi 10 3171 jns 2006 104 5 845 PMID 16703895 Service E Kohonen V April 1995 Is the relation between phonological memory and foreign language learning accounted for by vocabulary acquisition Applied Psycholinguistics 16 2 155 172 doi 10 1017 S0142716400007062 S2CID 143974128 Service E July 1992 Phonology working memory and foreign language learning The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A Human Experimental Psychology 45 1 21 50 doi 10 1080 14640749208401314 PMID 1636010 S2CID 43268252 Matsumoto R Nair DR LaPresto E Najm I Bingaman W Shibasaki H Luders HO October 2004 Functional connectivity in the human language system a cortico cortical evoked potential study Brain 127 Pt 10 2316 30 doi 10 1093 brain awh246 PMID 15269116 Kimura D Watson N November 1989 The relation between oral movement control and speech Brain and Language 37 4 565 90 doi 10 1016 0093 934x 89 90112 0 PMID 2479446 S2CID 39913744 Tourville JA Reilly KJ Guenther FH February 2008 Neural mechanisms underlying auditory feedback control of speech NeuroImage 39 3 1429 43 doi 10 1016 j neuroimage 2007 09 054 PMC 3658624 PMID 18035557 Chang EF Rieger JW Johnson K Berger MS Barbaro NM Knight RT November 2010 Categorical speech representation in human superior temporal gyrus Nature Neuroscience 13 11 1428 32 doi 10 1038 nn 2641 PMC 2967728 PMID 20890293 Nath AR Beauchamp MS January 2012 A neural basis for interindividual differences in the McGurk effect a multisensory speech illusion NeuroImage 59 1 781 7 doi 10 1016 j neuroimage 2011 07 024 PMC 3196040 PMID 21787869 Beauchamp MS Nath AR Pasalar S February 2010 fMRI Guided transcranial magnetic stimulation reveals that the superior temporal sulcus is a cortical locus of the McGurk effect The Journal of Neuroscience 30 7 2414 7 doi 10 1523 JNEUROSCI 4865 09 2010 PMC 2844713 PMID 20164324 McGettigan C Faulkner A Altarelli I Obleser J Baverstock H Scott SK April 2012 Speech comprehension aided by multiple modalities behavioural and neural interactions Neuropsychologia 50 5 762 76 doi 10 1016 j neuropsychologia 2012 01 010 PMC 4050300 PMID 22266262 Stevenson RA James TW February 2009 Audiovisual integration in human superior temporal sulcus Inverse effectiveness and the neural processing of speech and object recognition NeuroImage 44 3 1210 23 doi 10 1016 j neuroimage 2008 09 034 PMID 18973818 S2CID 8342349 Bernstein LE Jiang J Pantazis D Lu ZL Joshi A October 2011 Visual phonetic processing localized using speech and nonspeech face gestures in video and point light displays Human Brain Mapping 32 10 1660 76 doi 10 1002 hbm 21139 PMC 3120928 PMID 20853377 Campbell R March 2008 The processing of audio visual speech empirical and neural bases Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B Biological Sciences 363 1493 1001 10 doi 10 1098 rstb 2007 2155 PMC 2606792 PMID 17827105 Schwartz MF Faseyitan O Kim J Coslett HB December 2012 The dorsal stream contribution to phonological retrieval in object naming Brain 135 Pt 12 3799 814 doi 10 1093 brain aws300 PMC 3525060 PMID 23171662 Schwartz MF Kimberg DY Walker GM Faseyitan O Brecher A Dell GS Coslett HB December 2009 Anterior temporal involvement in semantic word retrieval voxel based lesion symptom mapping evidence from aphasia Brain 132 Pt 12 3411 27 doi 10 1093 brain awp284 PMC 2792374 PMID 19942676 Ojemann GA June 1983 Brain organization for language from the perspective of electrical stimulation mapping Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 2 189 206 doi 10 1017 S0140525X00015491 ISSN 1469 1825 S2CID 143189089 Cornelissen K Laine M Renvall K Saarinen T Martin N Salmelin R June 2004 Learning new names for new objects cortical effects as measured by magnetoencephalography Brain and Language 89 3 617 22 doi 10 1016 j bandl 2003 12 007 PMID 15120553 S2CID 32224334 Hartwigsen G Baumgaertner A Price CJ Koehnke M Ulmer S Siebner HR September 2010 Phonological decisions require both the left and right supramarginal gyri Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 107 38 16494 9 Bibcode 2010PNAS 10716494H doi 10 1073 pnas 1008121107 PMC 2944751 PMID 20807747 Cornelissen K Laine M Tarkiainen A Jarvensivu T Martin N Salmelin R April 2003 Adult brain plasticity elicited by anomia treatment Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 15 3 444 61 doi 10 1162 089892903321593153 PMID 12729495 S2CID 1597939 Mechelli A Crinion JT Noppeney U O Doherty J Ashburner J Frackowiak RS Price CJ October 2004 Neurolinguistics structural plasticity in the bilingual brain Nature 431 7010 757 Bibcode 2004Natur 431 757M doi 10 1038 431757a hdl 11858 00 001M 0000 0013 D79B 1 PMID 15483594 S2CID 4338340 Green DW Crinion J Price CJ July 2007 Exploring cross linguistic vocabulary effects on brain structures using voxel based morphometry Bilingualism 10 2 189 199 doi 10 1017 S1366728907002933 PMC 2312335 PMID 18418473 Willness C 2016 01 08 The Oxford handbook of organizational climate and culture By Benjamin Schneider amp Karen M Barbera Eds New York NY Oxford University Press 2014 ISBN 978 0 19 986071 5 Book Reviews British Journal of Psychology 107 1 201 202 doi 10 1111 bjop 12170 Lee H Devlin JT Shakeshaft C Stewart LH Brennan A Glensman J Pitcher K Crinion J Mechelli A Frackowiak RS Green DW Price CJ January 2007 Anatomical traces of vocabulary acquisition in the adolescent brain The Journal of Neuroscience 27 5 1184 9 doi 10 1523 JNEUROSCI 4442 06 2007 PMC 6673201 PMID 17267574 Richardson FM Thomas MS Filippi R Harth H Price CJ May 2010 Contrasting effects of vocabulary knowledge on temporal and parietal brain structure across lifespan Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 22 5 943 54 doi 10 1162 jocn 2009 21238 PMC 2860571 PMID 19366285 Jobard G Crivello F Tzourio Mazoyer N October 2003 Evaluation of the dual route theory of reading a metanalysis of 35 neuroimaging studies NeuroImage 20 2 693 712 doi 10 1016 s1053 8119 03 00343 4 PMID 14568445 S2CID 739665 Bolger DJ Perfetti CA Schneider W May 2005 Cross cultural effect on the brain revisited universal structures plus writing system variation Human Brain Mapping in French 25 1 92 104 doi 10 1002 hbm 20124 PMC 6871743 PMID 15846818 Brambati SM Ogar J Neuhaus J Miller BL Gorno Tempini ML July 2009 Reading disorders in primary progressive aphasia a behavioral and neuroimaging study Neuropsychologia 47 8 9 1893 900 doi 10 1016 j neuropsychologia 2009 02 033 PMC 2734967 PMID 19428421 a b Baddeley A Lewis V Vallar G May 1984 Exploring the Articulatory Loop The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A 36 2 233 252 doi 10 1080 14640748408402157 S2CID 144313607 a b Cowan N February 2001 The magical number 4 in short term memory a reconsideration of mental storage capacity The Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24 1 87 114 discussion 114 85 doi 10 1017 S0140525X01003922 PMID 11515286 Caplan D Rochon E Waters GS August 1992 Articulatory and phonological determinants of word length effects in span tasks The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A Human Experimental Psychology 45 2 177 92 doi 10 1080 14640749208401323 PMID 1410554 S2CID 32594562 Waters GS Rochon E Caplan D February 1992 The role of high level speech planning in rehearsal Evidence from patients with apraxia of speech Journal of Memory and Language 31 1 54 73 doi 10 1016 0749 596x 92 90005 i Cohen L Bachoud Levi AC September 1995 The role of the output phonological buffer in the control of speech timing a single case study Cortex A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior 31 3 469 86 doi 10 1016 s0010 9452 13 80060 3 PMID 8536476 S2CID 4480375 Shallice T Rumiati RI Zadini A September 2000 The selective impairment of the phonological output buffer Cognitive Neuropsychology 17 6 517 46 doi 10 1080 02643290050110638 PMID 20945193 S2CID 14811413 Shu H Xiong H Han Z Bi Y Bai X 2005 The selective impairment of the phonological output buffer evidence from a Chinese patient Behavioural Neurology 16 2 3 179 89 doi 10 1155 2005 647871 PMC 5478832 PMID 16410633 Oberauer K 2002 Access to information in working memory Exploring the focus of attention Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition 28 3 411 421 doi 10 1037 0278 7393 28 3 411 PMID 12018494 Unsworth N Engle RW January 2007 The nature of individual differences in working memory capacity active maintenance in primary memory and controlled search from secondary memory Psychological Review 114 1 104 32 doi 10 1037 0033 295x 114 1 104 PMID 17227183 Barrouillet P Camos V December 2012 As Time Goes By Current Directions in Psychological Science 21 6 413 419 doi 10 1177 0963721412459513 S2CID 145540189 Bornkessel Schlesewsky I Schlesewsky M Small SL Rauschecker JP March 2015 Neurobiological roots of language in primate audition common computational properties Trends in Cognitive Sciences 19 3 142 50 doi 10 1016 j tics 2014 12 008 PMC 4348204 PMID 25600585 Buchsbaum BR D Esposito M May 2008 The search for the phonological store from loop to convolution Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 20 5 762 78 doi 10 1162 jocn 2008 20501 PMID 18201133 S2CID 17878480 de Saussure Ferdinand 1959 First published 1916 Course in general linguistics PDF New York Philosophy Library ISBN 9780231157278 Archived from the original PDF on 2020 04 14 Retrieved 2020 06 16 Smith Neil 2002 Chomsky Ideas and Ideals 2nd ed Cambridge University Press ISBN 0 521 47517 1 Lakoff George 1990 Invariance hypothesis is abstract reasoning based on image schemas Cognitive Linguistics 1 1 39 74 doi 10 1515 cogl 1990 1 1 39 S2CID 144380802 Song Jae Jung 2012 Word Order Cambridge University Press ISBN 9781139033930 Christiansen Morten H Chater Nick 2008 Language as shaped by the brain PDF Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31 5 489 558 doi 10 1017 S0140525X08004998 PMID 18826669 Retrieved 2020 12 22 Blackmore Susan 2008 Memes shape brains shape memes Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31 5 513 doi 10 1017 S0140525X08005037 Retrieved 2020 12 22 Kluender R Kutas M 1993 Subjacency as a processing phenomenon PDF Language and Cognitive Processes 8 4 573 633 doi 10 1080 01690969308407588 Retrieved 2020 02 28 Barkley C Kluender R Kutas M 2015 Referential processing in the human brain An Event Related Potential ERP study PDF Brain Research 1629 143 159 doi 10 1016 j brainres 2015 09 017 PMID 26456801 S2CID 17053154 Retrieved 2020 02 28 Wei Xuehu Adamson Helyne Schwendemann Matthias Goucha Tomas Friederici Angela D Anwander Alfred 19 February 2023 Native language differences in the structural connectome of the human brain NeuroImage 270 270 119955 doi 10 1016 j neuroimage 2023 119955 PMID 36805092 Koizumi Masatoshi Yasugi Yoshiho Tamaoka Katsuo Kiyama Sachiko Kim Jungho Ajsivinac Sian Juan Esteban Garcia Matzar Pedro Oscar September 2014 On the non universality of the preference for subject object word order in sentence comprehension A sentence processing study in Kaqchikel Maya Language 90 3 722 736 doi 10 1353 lan 2014 0068 JSTOR 24672044 S2CID 146776347 Retrieved 2023 05 15 Miller LM Recanzone GH April 2009 Populations of auditory cortical neurons can accurately encode acoustic space across stimulus intensity Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 106 14 5931 5 Bibcode 2009PNAS 106 5931M doi 10 1073 pnas 0901023106 PMC 2667094 PMID 19321750 Tian B Reser D Durham A Kustov A Rauschecker JP April 2001 Functional specialization in rhesus monkey auditory cortex Science 292 5515 290 3 Bibcode 2001Sci 292 290T doi 10 1126 science 1058911 PMID 11303104 S2CID 32846215 Alain C Arnott SR Hevenor S Graham S Grady CL October 2001 What and where in the human auditory system Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 98 21 12301 6 Bibcode 2001PNAS 9812301A doi 10 1073 pnas 211209098 PMC 59809 PMID 11572938 De Santis L Clarke S Murray MM January 2007 Automatic and intrinsic auditory what and where processing in humans revealed by electrical neuroimaging Cerebral Cortex 17 1 9 17 doi 10 1093 cercor bhj119 PMID 16421326 Barrett DJ Hall DA August 2006 Response preferences for what and where in human non primary auditory cortex NeuroImage 32 2 968 77 doi 10 1016 j neuroimage 2006 03 050 PMID 16733092 S2CID 19988467 Linden JF Grunewald A Andersen RA July 1999 Responses to auditory stimuli in macaque lateral intraparietal area II Behavioral modulation Journal of Neurophysiology 82 1 343 58 doi 10 1152 jn 1999 82 1 343 PMID 10400963 S2CID 5317446 Mazzoni P Bracewell RM Barash S Andersen RA March 1996 Spatially tuned auditory responses in area LIP of macaques performing delayed memory saccades to acoustic targets Journal of Neurophysiology 75 3 1233 41 doi 10 1152 jn 1996 75 3 1233 PMID 8867131 Lachaux JP Jerbi K Bertrand O Minotti L Hoffmann D Schoendorff B Kahane P October 2007 A blueprint for real time functional mapping via human intracranial recordings PLOS ONE 2 10 e1094 Bibcode 2007PLoSO 2 1094L doi 10 1371 journal pone 0001094 PMC 2040217 PMID 17971857 Jardri R Houfflin Debarge V Delion P Pruvo JP Thomas P Pins D April 2012 Assessing fetal response to maternal speech using a noninvasive functional brain imaging technique International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience 30 2 159 61 doi 10 1016 j ijdevneu 2011 11 002 PMID 22123457 S2CID 2603226 Poliva O 2017 09 20 From where to what a neuroanatomically based evolutionary model of the emergence of speech in humans F1000Research 4 67 doi 10 12688 f1000research 6175 3 PMC 5600004 PMID 28928931 Poliva O 2016 06 30 From Mimicry to Language A Neuroanatomically Based Evolutionary Model of the Emergence of Vocal Language Frontiers in Neuroscience 10 307 doi 10 3389 fnins 2016 00307 PMC 4928493 PMID 27445676 a b c d e Suri Sana 25 July 2014 What sign language teaches us about the brain The Conversation Retrieved 2019 10 07 a b c Scientific American 2002 Sign language in the brain Brochure Retrieved from http lcn salk edu Brochure SciAM 20ASL pdf a b c d e f g h i j k l Norton ES Kovelman I Petitto LA March 2007 Are There Separate Neural Systems for Spelling New Insights into the Role of Rules and Memory in Spelling from Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Mind Brain and Education 1 1 48 59 doi 10 1111 j 1751 228X 2007 00005 x PMC 2790202 PMID 20011680 a b c d e Treiman R Kessler B 2007 Writing Systems and Spelling Development Blackwell Publishing Ltd pp 120 134 doi 10 1002 9780470757642 ch7 ISBN 978 0 470 75764 2 a href Template Cite book html title Template Cite book cite book a work ignored help Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Language processing in the brain amp oldid 1186046456, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.