fbpx
Wikipedia

Event-related potential

An event-related potential (ERP) is the measured brain response that is the direct result of a specific sensory, cognitive, or motor event.[1] More formally, it is any stereotyped electrophysiological response to a stimulus. The study of the brain in this way provides a noninvasive means of evaluating brain functioning.

A waveform showing several ERP components, including the N100 (labeled N1) and P300 (labeled P3). The ERP is plotted with negative voltages upward, a common, but not universal, practice in ERP research

ERPs are measured by means of electroencephalography (EEG). The magnetoencephalography (MEG) equivalent of ERP is the ERF, or event-related field.[2] Evoked potentials and induced potentials are subtypes of ERPs.

History edit

With the discovery of the electroencephalogram (EEG) in 1924, Hans Berger revealed that one could measure the electrical activity of the human brain by placing electrodes on the scalp and amplifying the signal. Changes in voltage can then be plotted over a period of time. He observed that the voltages could be influenced by external events that stimulated the senses. The EEG proved to be a useful source in recording brain activity over the ensuing decades. However, it tended to be very difficult to assess the highly specific neural process that are the focus of cognitive neuroscience because using pure EEG data made it difficult to isolate individual neurocognitive processes. Event-related potentials (ERPs) offered a more sophisticated method of extracting more specific sensory, cognitive, and motor events by using simple averaging techniques. In 1935–1936, Pauline and Hallowell Davis recorded the first known ERPs on awake humans and their findings were published a few years later, in 1939. Due to World War II not much research was conducted in the 1940s, but research focusing on sensory issues picked back up again in the 1950s. In 1964, research by Grey Walter and colleagues began the modern era of ERP component discoveries when they reported the first cognitive ERP component, called the contingent negative variation (CNV).[3] Sutton, Braren, and Zubin (1965) made another advancement with the discovery of the P3 component.[4] Over the next fifteen years, ERP component research became increasingly popular. The 1980s, with the introduction of inexpensive computers, opened up a new door for cognitive neuroscience research. Currently, ERP is one of the most widely used methods in cognitive neuroscience research to study the physiological correlates of sensory, perceptual and cognitive activity associated with processing information.[5]

Calculation edit

ERPs can be reliably measured using electroencephalography (EEG), a procedure that measures electrical activity of the brain over time using electrodes placed on the scalp. The EEG reflects thousands of simultaneously ongoing brain processes. This means that the brain response to a single stimulus or event of interest is not usually visible in the EEG recording of a single trial. To see the brain's response to a stimulus, the experimenter must conduct many trials and average the results together, causing random brain activity to be averaged out and the relevant waveform to remain, called the ERP.[6]

The random (background) brain activity together with other bio-signals (e.g., EOG, EMG, EKG) and electromagnetic interference (e.g., line noise, fluorescent lamps) constitute the noise contribution to the recorded ERP. This noise obscures the signal of interest, which is the sequence of underlying ERPs under study. From an engineering point of view it is possible to define the signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) of the recorded ERPs. Averaging increases the SNR of the recorded ERPs making them discernible and allowing for their interpretation. This has a simple mathematical explanation provided that some simplifying assumptions are made. These assumptions are:

  1. The signal of interest is made of a sequence of event-locked ERPs with invariable latency and shape
  2. The noise can be approximated by a zero-mean Gaussian random process of variance   which is uncorrelated between trials and not time-locked to the event (this assumption can be easily violated, for example in the case of a subject doing little tongue movements while mentally counting the targets in an experiment).

Having defined  , the trial number, and  , the time elapsed after the  th event, each recorded trial can be written as   where   is the signal and   is the noise (Under the assumptions above, the signal does not depend on the specific trial while the noise does).

The average of   trials is

  .

The expected value of   is (as hoped) the signal itself,  .

Its variance is

 .

For this reason the noise amplitude of the average of   trials is expected to deviate from the mean (which is  ) by less or equal than   in 68% of the cases. In particular, the deviation wherein 68% of the noise amplitudes lie is   times that of a single trial. A larger deviation of   can already be expected to encompass 95% of all noise amplitudes.

Wide amplitude noise (such as eye blinks or movement artifacts) are often several orders of magnitude larger than the underlying ERPs. Therefore, trials containing such artifacts should be removed before averaging. Artifact rejection can be performed manually by visual inspection or using an automated procedure based on predefined fixed thresholds (limiting the maximum EEG amplitude or slope) or on time-varying thresholds derived from the statistics of the set of trials.[citation needed]

Nomenclature edit

ERP waveforms consist of a series of positive and negative voltage deflections, which are related to a set of underlying components.[7] Though some ERP components are referred to with acronyms (e.g., contingent negative variation – CNV, error-related negativity – ERN), most components are referred to by a letter (N/P) indicating polarity (negative/positive), followed by a number indicating either the latency in milliseconds or the component's ordinal position in the waveform. For instance, a negative-going peak that is the first substantial peak in the waveform and often occurs about 100 milliseconds after a stimulus is presented is often called the N100 (indicating its latency is 100 ms after the stimulus and that it is negative) or N1 (indicating that it is the first peak and is negative); it is often followed by a positive peak, usually called the P200 or P2. The stated latencies for ERP components are often quite variable, particularly so for the later components that are related to the cognitive processing of the stimulus. For example, the P300 component may exhibit a peak anywhere between 250 ms – 700 ms.

Advantages and disadvantages edit

Relative to behavioral measures edit

Compared with behavioral procedures, ERPs provide a continuous measure of processing between a stimulus and a response, making it possible to determine which stage(s) are being affected by a specific experimental manipulation. Another advantage over behavioral measures is that they can provide a measure of processing of stimuli even when there is no behavioral change. However, because of the significantly small size of an ERP, it usually takes a large number of trials to accurately measure it correctly.[8]

Relative to other neurophysiological measures edit

Invasiveness edit

Unlike microelectrodes, which require an electrode to be inserted into the brain, and PET scans that expose humans to radiation, ERPs use EEG, a non-invasive procedure.

Spatial and temporal resolution edit

ERPs provide excellent temporal resolution—as the speed of ERP recording is only constrained by the sampling rate that the recording equipment can feasibly support, whereas hemodynamic measures (such as fMRI, PET, and fNIRS) are inherently limited by the slow speed of the BOLD response. The spatial resolution of an ERP, however, is much poorer than that of hemodynamic methods—in fact, the location of ERP sources is an inverse problem that cannot be exactly solved, only estimated. Thus, ERPs are well suited to research questions about the speed of neural activity, and are less well suited to research questions about the location of such activity.[1]

Cost edit

ERP research is much cheaper to do than other imaging techniques such as fMRI, PET, and MEG. This is because purchasing and maintaining an EEG system is less expensive than the other systems.

Clinical edit

Physicians and neurologists will sometimes use a flashing visual checkerboard stimulus to test for any damage or trauma in the visual system. In a healthy person, this stimulus will elicit a strong response over the primary visual cortex located in the occipital lobe, in the back of the brain.

ERP component abnormalities in clinical research have been shown in neurological conditions such as:

Research edit

ERPs are used extensively in neuroscience, cognitive psychology, cognitive science, and psycho-physiological research. Experimental psychologists and neuroscientists have discovered many different stimuli that elicit reliable ERPs from participants. The timing of these responses is thought to provide a measure of the timing of the brain's communication or timing of information processing. For example, in the checkerboard paradigm described above, healthy participants' first response of the visual cortex is around 50–70 ms. This would seem to indicate that this is the amount of time it takes for the transduced visual stimulus to reach the cortex after light first enters the eye. Alternatively, the P300 response occurs at around 300ms in the oddball paradigm, for example, regardless of the type of stimulus presented: visual, tactile, auditory, olfactory, gustatory, etc. Because of this general invariance with regard to stimulus type, the P300 component is understood to reflect a higher cognitive response to unexpected and/or cognitively salient stimuli. The P300 response has also been studied in the context of information and memory detection.[22] In addition, there are studies on abnormalities of P300 in depression. Depressed patients tend to have a reduced P200 and P300 amplitude and a prolonged P300 latency.[19]

Due to the consistency of the P300 response to novel stimuli, a brain–computer interface can be constructed which relies on it. By arranging many signals in a grid, randomly flashing the rows of the grid as in the previous paradigm, and observing the P300 responses of a subject staring at the grid, the subject may communicate which stimulus he is looking at, and thus slowly "type" words.[23]

Another area of research in the field of ERP lies in the efference copy. This predictive mechanism plays a central role in for example human verbalization.[24][25] Efference copies, however, do not only occur with spoken words, but also with inner language - i.e. the quiet production of words - which has also been proven by event-related potentials.[26]

Other ERPs used frequently in research, especially neurolinguistics research, include the ELAN, the N400, and the P600/SPS. The analysis of ERP data is also increasingly supported by machine learning algorithms.[27][28]

Number of trials edit

A common issue in ERP studies is whether the observed data have a sufficient number of trials to support statistical analysis.[29] The background noise in any ERP for any individual can vary. Therefore simply characterizing the number of ERP trials needed for a robust component response is inadequate. ERP researchers can use metrics like the standardized measurement error (SME) to justify the examination of between-condition or between-group differences[30] or estimates of internal consistency to justify the examination of individual differences.[31][32][29]

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ a b Luck SJ (2005). An Introduction to the Event-Related Potential Technique. The MIT Press. ISBN 978-0-262-12277-1.[page needed]
  2. ^ Brown CM, Hagoort P (1999). "The cognitive neuroscience of language". In Brown CM, Hagoort P (eds.). The Neurocognition of Language. New York: Oxford University Press. p. 6.
  3. ^ Walter WG, Cooper R, Aldridge VJ, Mccallum WC, Winter AL (July 1964). "Contingent Negative Variation: An Electric Sign of Sensori-Motor Association and Expectancy in the Human Brain". Nature. 203 (4943): 380–4. Bibcode:1964Natur.203..380W. doi:10.1038/203380a0. PMID 14197376. S2CID 26808780.
  4. ^ Sutton S, Braren M, Zubin J, John ER (November 1965). "Evoked-potential correlates of stimulus uncertainty". Science. 150 (3700): 1187–8. Bibcode:1965Sci...150.1187S. doi:10.1126/science.150.3700.1187. PMID 5852977. S2CID 39822117.
  5. ^ Handy, T. C. (2005). Event Related Potentials: A Methods Handbook. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Bradford/MIT Press.[page needed]
  6. ^ Coles MG, Rugg MD (1995). "Event-related brain potentials: An introduction". In Rugg MD, Coles MG (eds.). Electrophysiology of mind: Event-related brain potentials and cognition. Oxford psychology series, No. 25. New York: Oxford University Press. pp. 1–26.
  7. ^ Luck SJ, Kappenman ES, eds. (2012). The Oxford Handbook of Event-Related Potential Components. Oxford University Press. p. 664. ISBN 9780195374148.
  8. ^ Luck S (2005). "Comparison with Behavioral Measures". An Introduction to the Event-Related Potential Technique. MIT Press. pp. 21–23.
  9. ^ Johnstone SJ, Barry RJ, Clarke AR (April 2013). "Ten years on: a follow-up review of ERP research in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder". Clinical Neurophysiology. 124 (4): 644–57. doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2012.09.006. PMID 23063669. S2CID 13867965.
  10. ^ Barry RJ, Johnstone SJ, Clarke AR (February 2003). "A review of electrophysiology in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: II. Event-related potentials". Clinical Neurophysiology. 114 (2): 184–98. doi:10.1016/S1388-2457(02)00363-2. PMID 12559225. S2CID 9239459.
  11. ^ Boutros N, Torello MW, Burns EM, Wu SS, Nasrallah HA (June 1995). "Evoked potentials in subjects at risk for Alzheimer's disease". Psychiatry Research. 57 (1): 57–63. doi:10.1016/0165-1781(95)02597-P. PMID 7568559. S2CID 17010156.
  12. ^ Prabhakar S, Syal P, Srivastava T (September 2000). "P300 in newly diagnosed non-dementing Parkinson's disease: effect of dopaminergic drugs". Neurology India. 48 (3): 239–42. PMID 11025627.
  13. ^ Boose MA, Cranford JL (January 1996). "Auditory event-related potentials in multiple sclerosis". The American Journal of Otology. 17 (1): 165–70. PMID 8694124.
  14. ^ Duncan CC, Kosmidis MH, Mirsky AF (January 2003). "Event-related potential assessment of information processing after closed head injury". Psychophysiology. 40 (1): 45–59. doi:10.1111/1469-8986.00006. PMID 12751803.
  15. ^ D'Arcy RC, Marchand Y, Eskes GA, Harrison ER, Phillips SJ, Major A, Connolly JF (April 2003). "Electrophysiological assessment of language function following stroke". Clinical Neurophysiology. 114 (4): 662–72. doi:10.1016/S1388-2457(03)00007-5. PMID 12686275. S2CID 27955719.
  16. ^ Hanna GL, Carrasco M, Harbin SM, Nienhuis JK, LaRosa CE, Chen P, et al. (September 2012). "Error-related negativity and tic history in pediatric obsessive-compulsive disorder". Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 51 (9): 902–10. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2012.06.019. PMC 3427894. PMID 22917203.
  17. ^ Ford JM, Palzes VA, Roach BJ, Mathalon DH (July 2014). "Did I do that? Abnormal predictive processes in schizophrenia when button pressing to deliver a tone". Schizophrenia Bulletin. 40 (4): 804–12. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbt072. PMC 4059422. PMID 23754836.
  18. ^ Clayson PE, Wynn JK, Infantolino ZP, Hajcak G, Green MF, Horan WP (November 2019). "Reward processing in certain versus uncertain contexts in schizophrenia: An event-related potential (ERP) study". Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 128 (8): 867–880. doi:10.1037/abn0000469. PMC 6822386. PMID 31657597.
  19. ^ a b Zhou L, Wang G, Nan C, Wang H, Liu Z, Bai H (January 2019). "Abnormalities in P300 components in depression: an ERP-sLORETA study". Nordic Journal of Psychiatry. 73 (1): 1–8. doi:10.1080/08039488.2018.1478991. PMID 30636465. S2CID 58664019.
  20. ^ Casanova MF, Sokhadze EM, Casanova EL, Li X (October 2020). "Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation in Autism Spectrum Disorders: Neuropathological Underpinnings and Clinical Correlations". Seminars in Pediatric Neurology. 35: 100832. doi:10.1016/j.spen.2020.100832. PMC 7477302. PMID 32892959.
  21. ^ Derkowski, Wojciech (2012). "Event-related potentials in patients with epilepsy treated with levetiracetam". Epilepsia. 53 (s5 p670): 195. doi:10.1111/j.1528-1167.2012.03677.x. ISSN 0013-9580.
  22. ^ McCormick B (2006). "Your Thoughts May Deceive You: The Constitutional Implications of Brain Fingerprinting Technology and How It May Be Used to Secure Our Skies". Law & Psychology Review. 30: 171–84.
  23. ^ Farwell LA, Donchin E (December 1988). "Talking off the top of your head: toward a mental prosthesis utilizing event-related brain potentials". Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. 70 (6): 510–23. doi:10.1016/0013-4694(88)90149-6. PMID 2461285. S2CID 4547500.
  24. ^ Roach BJ, Ford JM, Biagianti B, Hamilton HK, Ramsay IS, Fisher M, et al. (November 2019). "Efference copy/corollary discharge function and targeted cognitive training in patients with schizophrenia". International Journal of Psychophysiology. 145: 91–98. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2018.12.015. PMC 6616012. PMID 30599145.
  25. ^ Brumberg JS, Pitt KM (July 2019). "Motor-Induced Suppression of the N100 Event-Related Potential During Motor Imagery Control of a Speech Synthesizer Brain–Computer Interface". Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 62 (7): 2133–2140. doi:10.1044/2019_JSLHR-S-MSC18-18-0198. PMC 6808362. PMID 31306609.
  26. ^ Whitford TJ, Jack BN, Pearson D, Griffiths O, Luque D, Harris AW, et al. (December 2017). "Neurophysiological evidence of efference copies to inner speech". eLife. 6. doi:10.7554/eLife.28197. PMC 5714499. PMID 29199947.
  27. ^ Mueller A, Candrian G, Kropotov JD, Ponomarev VA, Baschera GM (June 2010). "Classification of ADHD patients on the basis of independent ERP components using a machine learning system". Nonlinear Biomedical Physics. 4 (Suppl 1): S1. doi:10.1186/1753-4631-4-S1-S1. PMC 2880795. PMID 20522259.
  28. ^ Frick J, Rieg T, Buettner R (2021). Detection of schizophrenia: a machine learning algorithm for potential early detection and prevention based on event-related potentials. Proceedings of the 54th Hawaii International Conference on System Sciences. doi:10.24251/HICSS.2021.460. hdl:10125/71076.
  29. ^ a b Clayson, Peter E. (2024). "The psychometric upgrade psychophysiology needs". Psychophysiology. 61 (3). doi:10.1111/psyp.14522. ISSN 0048-5772.
  30. ^ Luck SJ, Stewart AX, Simmons AM, Rhemtulla M (June 2021). "Standardized measurement error: A universal metric of data quality for averaged event-related potentials". Psychophysiology. 58 (6): e13793. doi:10.1111/psyp.13793. PMC 8169536. PMID 33782996.
  31. ^ Clayson PE, Miller GA (January 2017). "Psychometric considerations in the measurement of event-related brain potentials: Guidelines for measurement and reporting". International Journal of Psychophysiology. 111: 57–67. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2016.09.005. PMID 27619493.
  32. ^ Clayson PE, Brush CJ, Hajcak G (July 2021). "Data quality and reliability metrics for event-related potentials (ERPs): The utility of subject-level reliability". International Journal of Psychophysiology. 165: 121–136. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2021.04.004. PMID 33901510. S2CID 233408794.

Further reading edit

  • Luck SJ (2014). (Second ed.). Cambridge, Massachusetts: The MIT Press. ISBN 978-0-262-52585-5. Archived from the original on 2018-03-17. Retrieved 2017-03-28.
  • Luck SJ, Kappenman ES, eds. (2005). The Oxford Handbook of Event-Related Potential Components. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press. ISBN 978-0-262-08333-1.
  • Fabiani M, Gratton G, Federmeier KD (2007). "Event-Related Brain Potentials: Methods, Theory, and Applications". In Cacioppo JT, Tassinary LG, Berntson GG (eds.). Handbook of Psychophysiology (3rd ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University. pp. 85–119. ISBN 978-0-521-84471-0.
  • Polich J, Corey-Bloom J (December 2005). "Alzheimer's disease and P300: review and evaluation of task and modality". Current Alzheimer Research. 2 (5): 515–25. doi:10.2174/156720505774932214. PMID 16375655.
  • Zani A, Proverbio AM (2003). Cognitive Electrophysiology of Mind and Brain. Amsterdam: Academic Press. ISBN 978-0-12-775421-5.
  • Kropotov J (2009). Quantitative EEG, Event-Related Potentials and Neurotherapy (1st ed.). Amsterdam: Elsevier/Academic. ISBN 978-0-12-374512-5.

External links edit

  • [1] – ERP Summer School 2017 was held in The School of Psychology, Bangor University from 25–30 June 2017
  • EEGLAB Toolbox – A freely available, open-source, Matlab toolbox for processing and analyzing EEG data
  • ERPLAB Toolbox – A freely available, open-source, Matlab toolbox for processing and analyzing ERP data
  • The ERP Boot Camp 2016-11-28 at the Wayback Machine – A series of training workshops for ERP researchers led by Steve Luck and Emily Kappenman
  • Virtual ERP Boot Camp – A blog with information, announcements, and tips about ERP methodology

event, related, potential, also, evoked, potential, this, article, includes, list, general, references, lacks, sufficient, corresponding, inline, citations, please, help, improve, this, article, introducing, more, precise, citations, january, 2009, learn, when. See also Evoked potential This article includes a list of general references but it lacks sufficient corresponding inline citations Please help to improve this article by introducing more precise citations January 2009 Learn how and when to remove this template message An event related potential ERP is the measured brain response that is the direct result of a specific sensory cognitive or motor event 1 More formally it is any stereotyped electrophysiological response to a stimulus The study of the brain in this way provides a noninvasive means of evaluating brain functioning A waveform showing several ERP components including the N100 labeled N1 and P300 labeled P3 The ERP is plotted with negative voltages upward a common but not universal practice in ERP research ERPs are measured by means of electroencephalography EEG The magnetoencephalography MEG equivalent of ERP is the ERF or event related field 2 Evoked potentials and induced potentials are subtypes of ERPs Contents 1 History 2 Calculation 3 Nomenclature 4 Advantages and disadvantages 4 1 Relative to behavioral measures 4 2 Relative to other neurophysiological measures 4 2 1 Invasiveness 4 2 2 Spatial and temporal resolution 4 3 Cost 5 Clinical 6 Research 6 1 Number of trials 7 See also 8 References 9 Further reading 10 External linksHistory editWith the discovery of the electroencephalogram EEG in 1924 Hans Berger revealed that one could measure the electrical activity of the human brain by placing electrodes on the scalp and amplifying the signal Changes in voltage can then be plotted over a period of time He observed that the voltages could be influenced by external events that stimulated the senses The EEG proved to be a useful source in recording brain activity over the ensuing decades However it tended to be very difficult to assess the highly specific neural process that are the focus of cognitive neuroscience because using pure EEG data made it difficult to isolate individual neurocognitive processes Event related potentials ERPs offered a more sophisticated method of extracting more specific sensory cognitive and motor events by using simple averaging techniques In 1935 1936 Pauline and Hallowell Davis recorded the first known ERPs on awake humans and their findings were published a few years later in 1939 Due to World War II not much research was conducted in the 1940s but research focusing on sensory issues picked back up again in the 1950s In 1964 research by Grey Walter and colleagues began the modern era of ERP component discoveries when they reported the first cognitive ERP component called the contingent negative variation CNV 3 Sutton Braren and Zubin 1965 made another advancement with the discovery of the P3 component 4 Over the next fifteen years ERP component research became increasingly popular The 1980s with the introduction of inexpensive computers opened up a new door for cognitive neuroscience research Currently ERP is one of the most widely used methods in cognitive neuroscience research to study the physiological correlates of sensory perceptual and cognitive activity associated with processing information 5 Calculation editERPs can be reliably measured using electroencephalography EEG a procedure that measures electrical activity of the brain over time using electrodes placed on the scalp The EEG reflects thousands of simultaneously ongoing brain processes This means that the brain response to a single stimulus or event of interest is not usually visible in the EEG recording of a single trial To see the brain s response to a stimulus the experimenter must conduct many trials and average the results together causing random brain activity to be averaged out and the relevant waveform to remain called the ERP 6 The random background brain activity together with other bio signals e g EOG EMG EKG and electromagnetic interference e g line noise fluorescent lamps constitute the noise contribution to the recorded ERP This noise obscures the signal of interest which is the sequence of underlying ERPs under study From an engineering point of view it is possible to define the signal to noise ratio SNR of the recorded ERPs Averaging increases the SNR of the recorded ERPs making them discernible and allowing for their interpretation This has a simple mathematical explanation provided that some simplifying assumptions are made These assumptions are The signal of interest is made of a sequence of event locked ERPs with invariable latency and shape The noise can be approximated by a zero mean Gaussian random process of variance s 2 displaystyle sigma 2 nbsp which is uncorrelated between trials and not time locked to the event this assumption can be easily violated for example in the case of a subject doing little tongue movements while mentally counting the targets in an experiment Having defined k displaystyle k nbsp the trial number and t displaystyle t nbsp the time elapsed after the k displaystyle k nbsp th event each recorded trial can be written as x t k s t n t k displaystyle x t k s t n t k nbsp where s t displaystyle s t nbsp is the signal and n t k displaystyle n t k nbsp is the noise Under the assumptions above the signal does not depend on the specific trial while the noise does The average of N displaystyle N nbsp trials is x t 1 N k 1 N x t k s t 1 N k 1 N n t k displaystyle bar x t frac 1 N sum k 1 N x t k s t frac 1 N sum k 1 N n t k nbsp The expected value of x t displaystyle bar x t nbsp is as hoped the signal itself E x t s t displaystyle operatorname E bar x t s t nbsp Its variance is Var x t E x t E x t 2 1 N 2 E k 1 N n t k 2 1 N 2 k 1 N E n t k 2 s 2 N displaystyle operatorname Var bar x t operatorname E left left bar x t operatorname E bar x t right 2 right frac 1 N 2 operatorname E left left sum k 1 N n t k right 2 right frac 1 N 2 sum k 1 N operatorname E left n t k 2 right frac sigma 2 N nbsp For this reason the noise amplitude of the average of N displaystyle N nbsp trials is expected to deviate from the mean which is s t displaystyle s t nbsp by less or equal than s N displaystyle sigma sqrt N nbsp in 68 of the cases In particular the deviation wherein 68 of the noise amplitudes lie is 1 N displaystyle 1 sqrt N nbsp times that of a single trial A larger deviation of 2 s N displaystyle 2 sigma sqrt N nbsp can already be expected to encompass 95 of all noise amplitudes Wide amplitude noise such as eye blinks or movement artifacts are often several orders of magnitude larger than the underlying ERPs Therefore trials containing such artifacts should be removed before averaging Artifact rejection can be performed manually by visual inspection or using an automated procedure based on predefined fixed thresholds limiting the maximum EEG amplitude or slope or on time varying thresholds derived from the statistics of the set of trials citation needed Nomenclature editERP waveforms consist of a series of positive and negative voltage deflections which are related to a set of underlying components 7 Though some ERP components are referred to with acronyms e g contingent negative variation CNV error related negativity ERN most components are referred to by a letter N P indicating polarity negative positive followed by a number indicating either the latency in milliseconds or the component s ordinal position in the waveform For instance a negative going peak that is the first substantial peak in the waveform and often occurs about 100 milliseconds after a stimulus is presented is often called the N100 indicating its latency is 100 ms after the stimulus and that it is negative or N1 indicating that it is the first peak and is negative it is often followed by a positive peak usually called the P200 or P2 The stated latencies for ERP components are often quite variable particularly so for the later components that are related to the cognitive processing of the stimulus For example the P300 component may exhibit a peak anywhere between 250 ms 700 ms Advantages and disadvantages editRelative to behavioral measures edit Compared with behavioral procedures ERPs provide a continuous measure of processing between a stimulus and a response making it possible to determine which stage s are being affected by a specific experimental manipulation Another advantage over behavioral measures is that they can provide a measure of processing of stimuli even when there is no behavioral change However because of the significantly small size of an ERP it usually takes a large number of trials to accurately measure it correctly 8 Relative to other neurophysiological measures edit Invasiveness edit Unlike microelectrodes which require an electrode to be inserted into the brain and PET scans that expose humans to radiation ERPs use EEG a non invasive procedure Spatial and temporal resolution edit ERPs provide excellent temporal resolution as the speed of ERP recording is only constrained by the sampling rate that the recording equipment can feasibly support whereas hemodynamic measures such as fMRI PET and fNIRS are inherently limited by the slow speed of the BOLD response The spatial resolution of an ERP however is much poorer than that of hemodynamic methods in fact the location of ERP sources is an inverse problem that cannot be exactly solved only estimated Thus ERPs are well suited to research questions about the speed of neural activity and are less well suited to research questions about the location of such activity 1 Cost edit ERP research is much cheaper to do than other imaging techniques such as fMRI PET and MEG This is because purchasing and maintaining an EEG system is less expensive than the other systems Clinical editPhysicians and neurologists will sometimes use a flashing visual checkerboard stimulus to test for any damage or trauma in the visual system In a healthy person this stimulus will elicit a strong response over the primary visual cortex located in the occipital lobe in the back of the brain ERP component abnormalities in clinical research have been shown in neurological conditions such as AD HD 9 10 Dementia 11 Parkinson s disease 12 Multiple sclerosis 13 Head injuries 14 Stroke 15 Obsessive compulsive disorder 16 Schizophrenia 17 18 Depression 19 Autism spectrum disorder 20 Epilepsy to monitor the efficiency of cognitive processes 21 Research editERPs are used extensively in neuroscience cognitive psychology cognitive science and psycho physiological research Experimental psychologists and neuroscientists have discovered many different stimuli that elicit reliable ERPs from participants The timing of these responses is thought to provide a measure of the timing of the brain s communication or timing of information processing For example in the checkerboard paradigm described above healthy participants first response of the visual cortex is around 50 70 ms This would seem to indicate that this is the amount of time it takes for the transduced visual stimulus to reach the cortex after light first enters the eye Alternatively the P300 response occurs at around 300ms in the oddball paradigm for example regardless of the type of stimulus presented visual tactile auditory olfactory gustatory etc Because of this general invariance with regard to stimulus type the P300 component is understood to reflect a higher cognitive response to unexpected and or cognitively salient stimuli The P300 response has also been studied in the context of information and memory detection 22 In addition there are studies on abnormalities of P300 in depression Depressed patients tend to have a reduced P200 and P300 amplitude and a prolonged P300 latency 19 Due to the consistency of the P300 response to novel stimuli a brain computer interface can be constructed which relies on it By arranging many signals in a grid randomly flashing the rows of the grid as in the previous paradigm and observing the P300 responses of a subject staring at the grid the subject may communicate which stimulus he is looking at and thus slowly type words 23 Another area of research in the field of ERP lies in the efference copy This predictive mechanism plays a central role in for example human verbalization 24 25 Efference copies however do not only occur with spoken words but also with inner language i e the quiet production of words which has also been proven by event related potentials 26 Other ERPs used frequently in research especially neurolinguistics research include the ELAN the N400 and the P600 SPS The analysis of ERP data is also increasingly supported by machine learning algorithms 27 28 Number of trials edit A common issue in ERP studies is whether the observed data have a sufficient number of trials to support statistical analysis 29 The background noise in any ERP for any individual can vary Therefore simply characterizing the number of ERP trials needed for a robust component response is inadequate ERP researchers can use metrics like the standardized measurement error SME to justify the examination of between condition or between group differences 30 or estimates of internal consistency to justify the examination of individual differences 31 32 29 See also editBereitschaftspotential C1 and P1 Contingent negative variation Difference due to memory Early left anterior negativity Erich Schroger Error related negativity Evoked potential Induced activity Lateralized readiness potential Mismatch negativity Negativity N100 Visual N1 N170 N200 N2pc N400 Positivity P200 P300 P3a P3b Late positive component P600 Somatosensory evoked potentialReferences edit a b Luck SJ 2005 An Introduction to the Event Related Potential Technique The MIT Press ISBN 978 0 262 12277 1 page needed Brown CM Hagoort P 1999 The cognitive neuroscience of language In Brown CM Hagoort P eds The Neurocognition of Language New York Oxford University Press p 6 Walter WG Cooper R Aldridge VJ Mccallum WC Winter AL July 1964 Contingent Negative Variation An Electric Sign of Sensori Motor Association and Expectancy in the Human Brain Nature 203 4943 380 4 Bibcode 1964Natur 203 380W doi 10 1038 203380a0 PMID 14197376 S2CID 26808780 Sutton S Braren M Zubin J John ER November 1965 Evoked potential correlates of stimulus uncertainty Science 150 3700 1187 8 Bibcode 1965Sci 150 1187S doi 10 1126 science 150 3700 1187 PMID 5852977 S2CID 39822117 Handy T C 2005 Event Related Potentials A Methods Handbook Cambridge Massachusetts Bradford MIT Press page needed Coles MG Rugg MD 1995 Event related brain potentials An introduction In Rugg MD Coles MG eds Electrophysiology of mind Event related brain potentials and cognition Oxford psychology series No 25 New York Oxford University Press pp 1 26 Luck SJ Kappenman ES eds 2012 The Oxford Handbook of Event Related Potential Components Oxford University Press p 664 ISBN 9780195374148 Luck S 2005 Comparison with Behavioral Measures An Introduction to the Event Related Potential Technique MIT Press pp 21 23 Johnstone SJ Barry RJ Clarke AR April 2013 Ten years on a follow up review of ERP research in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder Clinical Neurophysiology 124 4 644 57 doi 10 1016 j clinph 2012 09 006 PMID 23063669 S2CID 13867965 Barry RJ Johnstone SJ Clarke AR February 2003 A review of electrophysiology in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder II Event related potentials Clinical Neurophysiology 114 2 184 98 doi 10 1016 S1388 2457 02 00363 2 PMID 12559225 S2CID 9239459 Boutros N Torello MW Burns EM Wu SS Nasrallah HA June 1995 Evoked potentials in subjects at risk for Alzheimer s disease Psychiatry Research 57 1 57 63 doi 10 1016 0165 1781 95 02597 P PMID 7568559 S2CID 17010156 Prabhakar S Syal P Srivastava T September 2000 P300 in newly diagnosed non dementing Parkinson s disease effect of dopaminergic drugs Neurology India 48 3 239 42 PMID 11025627 Boose MA Cranford JL January 1996 Auditory event related potentials in multiple sclerosis The American Journal of Otology 17 1 165 70 PMID 8694124 Duncan CC Kosmidis MH Mirsky AF January 2003 Event related potential assessment of information processing after closed head injury Psychophysiology 40 1 45 59 doi 10 1111 1469 8986 00006 PMID 12751803 D Arcy RC Marchand Y Eskes GA Harrison ER Phillips SJ Major A Connolly JF April 2003 Electrophysiological assessment of language function following stroke Clinical Neurophysiology 114 4 662 72 doi 10 1016 S1388 2457 03 00007 5 PMID 12686275 S2CID 27955719 Hanna GL Carrasco M Harbin SM Nienhuis JK LaRosa CE Chen P et al September 2012 Error related negativity and tic history in pediatric obsessive compulsive disorder Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 51 9 902 10 doi 10 1016 j jaac 2012 06 019 PMC 3427894 PMID 22917203 Ford JM Palzes VA Roach BJ Mathalon DH July 2014 Did I do that Abnormal predictive processes in schizophrenia when button pressing to deliver a tone Schizophrenia Bulletin 40 4 804 12 doi 10 1093 schbul sbt072 PMC 4059422 PMID 23754836 Clayson PE Wynn JK Infantolino ZP Hajcak G Green MF Horan WP November 2019 Reward processing in certain versus uncertain contexts in schizophrenia An event related potential ERP study Journal of Abnormal Psychology 128 8 867 880 doi 10 1037 abn0000469 PMC 6822386 PMID 31657597 a b Zhou L Wang G Nan C Wang H Liu Z Bai H January 2019 Abnormalities in P300 components in depression an ERP sLORETA study Nordic Journal of Psychiatry 73 1 1 8 doi 10 1080 08039488 2018 1478991 PMID 30636465 S2CID 58664019 Casanova MF Sokhadze EM Casanova EL Li X October 2020 Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation in Autism Spectrum Disorders Neuropathological Underpinnings and Clinical Correlations Seminars in Pediatric Neurology 35 100832 doi 10 1016 j spen 2020 100832 PMC 7477302 PMID 32892959 Derkowski Wojciech 2012 Event related potentials in patients with epilepsy treated with levetiracetam Epilepsia 53 s5 p670 195 doi 10 1111 j 1528 1167 2012 03677 x ISSN 0013 9580 McCormick B 2006 Your Thoughts May Deceive You The Constitutional Implications of Brain Fingerprinting Technology and How It May Be Used to Secure Our Skies Law amp Psychology Review 30 171 84 Farwell LA Donchin E December 1988 Talking off the top of your head toward a mental prosthesis utilizing event related brain potentials Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 70 6 510 23 doi 10 1016 0013 4694 88 90149 6 PMID 2461285 S2CID 4547500 Roach BJ Ford JM Biagianti B Hamilton HK Ramsay IS Fisher M et al November 2019 Efference copy corollary discharge function and targeted cognitive training in patients with schizophrenia International Journal of Psychophysiology 145 91 98 doi 10 1016 j ijpsycho 2018 12 015 PMC 6616012 PMID 30599145 Brumberg JS Pitt KM July 2019 Motor Induced Suppression of the N100 Event Related Potential During Motor Imagery Control of a Speech Synthesizer Brain Computer Interface Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research 62 7 2133 2140 doi 10 1044 2019 JSLHR S MSC18 18 0198 PMC 6808362 PMID 31306609 Whitford TJ Jack BN Pearson D Griffiths O Luque D Harris AW et al December 2017 Neurophysiological evidence of efference copies to inner speech eLife 6 doi 10 7554 eLife 28197 PMC 5714499 PMID 29199947 Mueller A Candrian G Kropotov JD Ponomarev VA Baschera GM June 2010 Classification of ADHD patients on the basis of independent ERP components using a machine learning system Nonlinear Biomedical Physics 4 Suppl 1 S1 doi 10 1186 1753 4631 4 S1 S1 PMC 2880795 PMID 20522259 Frick J Rieg T Buettner R 2021 Detection of schizophrenia a machine learning algorithm for potential early detection and prevention based on event related potentials Proceedings of the 54th Hawaii International Conference on System Sciences doi 10 24251 HICSS 2021 460 hdl 10125 71076 a b Clayson Peter E 2024 The psychometric upgrade psychophysiology needs Psychophysiology 61 3 doi 10 1111 psyp 14522 ISSN 0048 5772 Luck SJ Stewart AX Simmons AM Rhemtulla M June 2021 Standardized measurement error A universal metric of data quality for averaged event related potentials Psychophysiology 58 6 e13793 doi 10 1111 psyp 13793 PMC 8169536 PMID 33782996 Clayson PE Miller GA January 2017 Psychometric considerations in the measurement of event related brain potentials Guidelines for measurement and reporting International Journal of Psychophysiology 111 57 67 doi 10 1016 j ijpsycho 2016 09 005 PMID 27619493 Clayson PE Brush CJ Hajcak G July 2021 Data quality and reliability metrics for event related potentials ERPs The utility of subject level reliability International Journal of Psychophysiology 165 121 136 doi 10 1016 j ijpsycho 2021 04 004 PMID 33901510 S2CID 233408794 Further reading editLuck SJ 2014 An Introduction to the Event Related Potential Technique Second ed Cambridge Massachusetts The MIT Press ISBN 978 0 262 52585 5 Archived from the original on 2018 03 17 Retrieved 2017 03 28 Luck SJ Kappenman ES eds 2005 The Oxford Handbook of Event Related Potential Components Cambridge Mass MIT Press ISBN 978 0 262 08333 1 Fabiani M Gratton G Federmeier KD 2007 Event Related Brain Potentials Methods Theory and Applications In Cacioppo JT Tassinary LG Berntson GG eds Handbook of Psychophysiology 3rd ed Cambridge Cambridge University pp 85 119 ISBN 978 0 521 84471 0 Polich J Corey Bloom J December 2005 Alzheimer s disease and P300 review and evaluation of task and modality Current Alzheimer Research 2 5 515 25 doi 10 2174 156720505774932214 PMID 16375655 Zani A Proverbio AM 2003 Cognitive Electrophysiology of Mind and Brain Amsterdam Academic Press ISBN 978 0 12 775421 5 Kropotov J 2009 Quantitative EEG Event Related Potentials and Neurotherapy 1st ed Amsterdam Elsevier Academic ISBN 978 0 12 374512 5 External links edit 1 ERP Summer School 2017 was held in The School of Psychology Bangor University from 25 30 June 2017 EEGLAB Toolbox A freely available open source Matlab toolbox for processing and analyzing EEG data ERPLAB Toolbox A freely available open source Matlab toolbox for processing and analyzing ERP data The ERP Boot Camp Archived 2016 11 28 at the Wayback Machine A series of training workshops for ERP researchers led by Steve Luck and Emily Kappenman Virtual ERP Boot Camp A blog with information announcements and tips about ERP methodology Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Event related potential amp oldid 1214912714, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.