fbpx
Wikipedia

Temperament

In psychology, temperament broadly refers to consistent individual differences in behavior that are biologically based and are relatively independent of learning, system of values and attitudes.

Some researchers point to association of temperament with formal dynamical features of behavior, such as energetic aspects, plasticity, sensitivity to specific reinforcers and emotionality.[1] Temperament traits (such as neuroticism, sociability, impulsivity, etc.) are distinct patterns in behavior throughout a lifetime, but they are most noticeable and most studied in children. Babies are typically described by temperament, but longitudinal research in the 1920s began to establish temperament as something which is stable across the lifespan.[2]

Definition

Temperament has been defined as "the constellation of inborn traits that determine a child's unique behavioral style and the way he or she experiences and reacts to the world."[3]

Classification schemes

Many classification schemes for temperament have been developed, and there is no consensus.[4][5] The Latin word temperamentum means 'mixture'.

Temperament vs personality

Some commentators see temperament as one factor underlying personality.[6]

Main models

Four temperaments model

Historically, in the second century AD, the physician Galen described four classical temperaments (melancholic, phlegmatic, sanguine and choleric), corresponding to the four humors or bodily fluids.[7] This historical concept was explored by philosophers, psychologists, psychiatrists and psycho-physiologists from very early times of psychological science, with theories proposed by Immanuel Kant, Hermann Lotze, Ivan Pavlov, Carl Jung, Gerardus Heymans among others. In more recent history, Rudolf Steiner had emphasized the importance of the four classical temperaments in elementary education, the time when he believed the influence of temperament on the personality to be at its strongest.[8][9] Neither Galen nor Steiner are generally applied to the contemporary study of temperament in the approaches of modern medicine or contemporary psychology.

Rusalov-Trofimova neurophysiological model of temperament

This model based on the longest tradition of neurophysiological experiments started within the investigations of types and properties of nervous systems by Ivan Pavlov's school. This experimental tradition started on studies with animals in 1910–20s but expanded its methodology to humans since 1930s and especially since 1960s, including EEG, caffeine tests, evoked potentials, behavioral tasks and other psychophysiological methods.[10]

The latest version of this model is based on the "Activity-specific approach in temperament research, on Alexander Luria's research in clinical neurophysiology and on the neurochemical model Functional Ensemble of Temperament.[5][11][12] At the present time the model is associated with the Structure of Temperament Questionnaire and has 12 scales:

Endurance-related scales

  • Motor-physical Endurance: the ability of an individual to sustain prolonged physical activity using well-defined behavioral elements
  • Social-verbal Endurance (sociability): the ability of an individual to sustain prolonged social-verbal activities using well-defined behavioral elements.
  • Mental Endurance, or Attention: the ability to stay focused on selected features of objects with suppression of behavioral reactivity to other features.

Scales related to speed of integration of behavior

  • Motor-physical Tempo: speed of integration of an action in physical manipulations with objects with well-defined scripts of actions
  • Plasticity: the ability to adapt quickly to changes in situations, to change the program of action, and to shift between different tasks
  • Social-verbal Tempo: the preferred speed of speech and ability to understand fast speech on well-known topics, reading and sorting of known verbal material

Scales related to type of orientation of behavior

  • Sensation Seeking (SS): behavioral orientation to well-defined and existing sensational objects and events, underestimation of outcomes of risky behavior.
  • Empathy: behavioral orientation to the emotional states/needs of others (ranging from empathic deafness in autism and schizophrenia disorders to social dependency).
  • Sensitivity to Probabilities: the drive to gather information about uniqueness, frequency and values of objects/events, to differentiate their specific features, to project these features in future actions.

Emotionality scales

  • Satisfaction (Self-Confidence): A disposition to be satisfied with current state of events, a sense of security, confidence in the future. In spite of the optimism about outcomes of his or her activities, the respondent might be negligent in details.
  • Impulsivity: Initiation of actions based on immediate emotional reactivity rather than by planning or rational reasoning.
  • Neuroticism: A tendency to avoid novelty, unpredictable situations and uncertainty. Preference of well-known settings and people over unknown ones and a need for approval and feedback from people around.

Kagan's research

Jerome Kagan and his colleagues have concentrated empirical research on a temperamental category termed "reactivity." Four-month-old infants who became "motorically aroused and distressed" to presentations of novel stimuli were termed highly reactive. Those who remained "motorically relaxed and did not cry or fret to the same set of unfamiliar events" were termed low reactive.[13] These high and low reactive infants were tested again at 14 and 21 months "in a variety of unfamiliar laboratory situations." Highly reactive infants were predominantly characterized by a profile of high fear to unfamiliar events, which Kagan termed inhibited. Contrastingly, low reactive children were minimally fearful to novel situations, and were characterized by an uninhibited profile (Kagan). However, when observed again at age 4.5, only a modest proportion of children maintained their expected profile due to mediating factors such as intervening family experiences. Those who remained highly inhibited or uninhibited after age 4.5 were at higher risk for developing anxiety and conduct disorders, respectively.[14]

Kagan also used two additional classifications, one for infants who were inactive but cried frequently (distressed) and one for those who showed vigorous activity but little crying (aroused). Followed to age 14–17 years, these groups of children showed differing outcomes, including some differences in central nervous system activity. Teenagers who had been classed as high reactives when they were babies were more likely to be "subdued in unfamiliar situations, to report a dour mood and anxiety over the future, [and] to be more religious."[15]

Thomas and Chess's nine temperament characteristics

Alexander Thomas, Stella Chess, Herbert G. Birch, Margaret Hertzig and Sam Korn began the classic New York Longitudinal study in the early 1950s regarding infant temperament (Thomas, Chess & Birch, 1968). The study focused on how temperamental qualities influence adjustment throughout life. Chess, Thomas et al. rated young infants on nine temperament characteristics, each of which, by itself, or with connection to another, affects how well a child fits in at school, with their friends, and at home. Behaviors for each one of these traits are on a continuum. If a child leans towards the high or low end of the scale, it could be a cause for concern. The specific behaviors are: activity level, regularity of sleeping and eating patterns, initial reaction, adaptability, intensity of emotion, mood, distractibility, persistence and attention span, and sensory sensitivity. Redundancies between the categories have been found and a reduced list is normally used by psychologists today.[16]

Research by Thomas and Chess used the following nine temperament traits in children[17][18][19][20][21] based on a classification scheme developed by Dr. Herbert Birch:[22] Thomas, Chess, Birch, Hertzig and Korn found that many babies could be categorized into one of three groups: easy, difficult, and slow-to-warm-up (Thomas & Chess 1977).[17] Not all children can be placed in one of these groups. Approximately 65% of children fit one of the patterns. Of the 65%, 40% fit the easy pattern, 10% fell into the difficult pattern, and 15% were slow to warm up. Each category has its own strength and weakness and one is not superior to another.[23]

Thomas, Chess, Birch, Hertzig and Korn showed that easy babies readily adapt to new experiences, generally display positive moods and emotions and also have normal eating and sleeping patterns. Difficult babies tend to be very emotional, irritable and fussy, and cry a lot. They also tend to have irregular eating and sleeping patterns. Slow-to-warm-up babies have a low activity level, and tend to withdraw from new situations and people. They are slow to adapt to new experiences, but accept them after repeated exposure.

Thomas, Chess, Birch, Hertzig and Korn found that these broad patterns of temperamental qualities are remarkably stable through childhood. These traits are also found in children across all cultures.

Thomas and Chess also studied temperament and environment. One sample consisted of white middle-class families with high educational status and the other was of Puerto Rican working-class families. They found several differences. Among those were:[17]

  • Parents of middle class children were more likely to report behavior problems before the age of nine and the children had sleep problems. This may be because children start preschool between the ages of three and four. Puerto Rican children under the age of five showed rare signs of sleep problems, however, sleep problems became more common at the age of six.
  • Middle-class parents also placed great stress on the child's early development, believing that problems in early ages were indicative of later problems in psychological development, whereas Puerto Rican parents felt their children would outgrow any problems.
  • At the age of nine, the report of new problems dropped for middle class children but they rose in Puerto Rican children, possibly due to the demands of school.

Observed traits:

  • Activity: refers to the child's physical energy. Is the child constantly moving, or does the child have a relaxing approach? A high-energy child may have difficulty sitting still in class, whereas a child with low energy can tolerate a very structured environment. The former may use gross motor skills like running and jumping more frequently. Conversely, a child with a lower activity level may rely more on fine motor skills, such as drawing and putting puzzles together. This trait can also refer to mental activity, such as deep thinking or reading—activities which become more significant as the person matures.
  • Regularity: also known as rhythmicity, refers to the level of predictability in a child's biological functions, such as waking, becoming tired, hunger, and bowel movements. Does the child have a routine in eating and sleeping habits, or are these events more random? For example, a child with a high regularity rating may want to eat at 2 p.m. every day, whereas a child lower on the regularity scale may eat at sporadic times throughout the day.
  • Initial reaction: also known as approach or withdrawal. This refers to how the child responds (whether positively or negatively) to new people or environments. Does the child approach people or things in the environment without hesitation, or does the child shy away? A bold child tends to approach things quickly, as if without thinking, whereas a cautious child typically prefers to watch for a while before engaging in new experiences.
  • Adaptability: refers to how long it takes the child to adjust to change over time (as opposed to an initial reaction). Does the child adjust to the changes in their environment easily, or is the child resistant? A child who adjusts easily may be quick to settle into a new routine, whereas a resistant child may take a long time to adjust to the situation.
  • Intensity: refers to the energy level of a positive or negative response. Does the child react intensely to a situation, or does the child respond in a calm and quiet manner? A more intense child may jump up and down screaming with excitement, whereas a mild-mannered child may smile or show no emotion.
  • Mood: refers to the child's general tendency towards a happy or unhappy demeanor. All children have a variety of emotions and reactions, such as cheerful and stormy, happy and unhappy. Yet each child biologically tends to have a generally positive or negative outlook. A baby who frequently smiles and coos could be considered a cheerful baby, whereas a baby who frequently cries or fusses might be considered a stormy baby.
  • Distractibility: refers to the child's tendency to be sidetracked by other things going on around them. Does the child get easily distracted by what is happening in the environment, or can the child concentrate despite the interruptions? An easily distracted child is engaged by external events and has difficulty returning to the task at hand, whereas a rarely distracted child stays focused and completes the task at hand.
  • Persistence and attention span: refer to the child's length of time on a task and ability to stay with the task through frustrations—whether the child stays with an activity for a long period of time or loses interest quickly.
  • Sensitivity: refers to how easily a child is disturbed by changes in the environment. This is also called sensory threshold or threshold of responsiveness. Is the child bothered by external stimuli like noises, textures, or lights, or does the child seem to ignore them? A sensitive child may lose focus when a door slams, whereas a child less sensitive to external noises will be able to maintain focus.

Mary K. Rothbart's three dimensions of temperament

Mary K. Rothbart views temperament as the individual personality differences in infants and young children that are present prior to the development of higher cognitive and social aspects of personality.[24] Rothbart further defines temperament as individual differences in reactivity and self-regulation that manifest in the domains of emotion, activity and attention. Moving away from classifying infants into categories, Mary Rothbart identified three underlying dimensions of temperament.[25] Using factor analysis on data from 3- to 12-month-old children, three broad factors emerged and were labeled surgency/extraversion, negative affect, and effortful control.[24]

Surgency/extraversion

Surgency/extraversion includes positive anticipation, impulsivity, increased levels of activity and a desire for sensation seeking.[26] This factor reflects the degree to which a child is generally happy, active, and enjoys vocalizing and seeking stimulation.[25] Increased levels of smiling and laughter are observed in babies high in surgency/extraversion.[24] 10- to 11-year-olds with higher levels of surgency/extraversion are more likely to develop externalizing problems like acting out; however, they are less likely to develop internalizing problems such as shyness and low self-esteem.[27]

Negative affect

Negative affect includes fear, frustration, sadness, discomfort,[26] and anger.[24] This factor reflects the degree to which a child is shy and not easily calmed.[25] Anger and frustration is seen as early as 2 to 3 months of age. Anger and frustration, together, predict externalizing and internalizing difficulties. Anger, alone, is later related to externalizing problems, while fear is associated with internalizing difficulties. Fear as evidenced by behavioral inhibition is seen as early as 7–10 months of age, and later predicts children's fearfulness and lower levels of aggression.[28]

Effortful control

Effortful control includes the focusing and shifting of attention, inhibitory control, perceptual sensitivity, and a low threshold for pleasure.[26] This factor reflects the degree to which a child can focus attention, is not easily distracted,[25] can restrain a dominant response in order to execute a non-dominant response, and employ planning. When high in effortful control, six- to seven-year-olds tend to be more empathetic and lower in aggressiveness.[26] Higher levels of effortful control at age seven also predict lower externalizing problems at age 11 years.[29] Children high on negative affect show decreased internalizing and externalizing problems when they are also high on effortful control.[28] Rothbart suggests that effortful control is dependent on the development of executive attention skills in the early years. In turn, executive attention skills allows greater self-control over reactive tendencies. Effortful control shows stability from infancy into the school years[28] and also predicts conscience.[30]

Others

Solomon Diamond described temperaments based upon characteristics found in the animal world: fearfulness, aggressiveness, affiliativeness, and impulsiveness. His work has been carried forward by Arnold Buss and Robert Plomin, who developed two measures of temperament: The Colorado Child Temperament Inventory, which includes aspects of Thomas and Chess's schema, and the EAS Survey for Children.[16]

H. Hill Goldsmith and Joseph Campos used emotional characteristics to define temperament, originally analyzing five emotional qualities: motor activity, anger, fearfulness, pleasure/joy, and interest/persistence, but later expanding to include other emotions. They developed several measures of temperament: Lab-TAB and TBAQ.[16]

Other temperament systems include those based upon theories of adult temperament (e.g. Gray and Martin's Temperament Assessment Battery for Children), or adult personality (e.g.the Big Five personality traits).

Causal and correlating factors

Biological basis for temperament

Scientists seeking evidence of a biological basis of personality have examined the relationship between temperament and neurotransmitter systems[31][5][11][12] and character (defined in this context as developmental aspects of personality). Temperament is hypothesized to be associated with biological factors, but these have proven to be complex and diverse,[14][5] and biological correlations have proven hard to confirm.[14]

Temperament vs. psychiatric disorders

Several psychiatrists and differential psychologists have suggested that temperament and mental illness represent varying degrees along the same continuum of neurotransmitter imbalances in neurophysiological systems of behavioral regulation.[32][33][34]

In fact, the original four types of temperament (choleric, melancholic, phlegmatic and sanguine) suggested by Hippocrates and Galen resemble mild forms of types of psychiatric disorders described in modern classifications. Moreover, Hippocrates-Galen hypothesis of chemical imbalances as factors of consistent individual differences has also been validated by research in neurochemistry and psychopharmacology, though modern studies attribute this to different compounds. Many studies have examined the relationships between temperament traits (such as impulsivity, sensation seeking, neuroticism, endurance, plasticity, sociability or extraversion) and various neurotransmitter and hormonal systems (i.e., the very same systems implicated in mental disorders).

Even though temperament and psychiatric disorders can be presented as, correspondingly, weak and strong imbalances within the same regulatory systems, it is incorrect to say that temperament is a weak degree of these disorders. Temperament might be a disposition to develop a mental disorder, but it should not be treated as a guaranteed marker for disorders.

Family life

Influences

Most experts agree that temperament has a genetic and biological basis, although environmental factors and maturation modify the ways a child's personality is expressed.[35] The term "goodness of fit" refers to the match or mismatch between temperament and other personal characteristics and the specific features of the environment. Differences of temperament or behavior styles between individuals are important in family life. They affect the interactions among family members. While some children can adapt quickly and easily to family routines and get along with siblings, others who are more active or intense may have a difficult time adjusting. The interactions between these children and their parents or siblings are among a number of factors that can lead to stress and friction within the family.

The temperament mix between parents and children also affects family life. For example, a slow-paced parent may be irritated by a highly active child; or if both parent and child are highly active and intense, conflict could result. This knowledge can help parents figure out how temperaments affect family relationships. What may appear to be a behavioral problem may actually be a mismatch between the parent's temperament and their child's. By taking a closer look at the nine traits that Thomas and Chess revealed from their study, parents can gain a better understanding of their child's temperament and their own. Parents may also notice that situational factors cause a child's temperament to seem problematic; for example, a child with low rhythmicity can cause difficulties for a family with a highly scheduled life, and a child with a high activity level may be difficult to cope with if the family lives in a crowded apartment upstairs from sensitive neighbors.

Parents can encourage new behaviors in their children, and with enough support a slow-to-warm-up child can become less shy, or a difficult baby can become easier to handle. More recently infants and children with temperament issues have been called "spirited" to avoid negative connotations of "difficult" and "slow to warm up". Numerous books have been written advising parents how to raise their spirited youngsters.

Understanding for improvement

Understanding a child's temperament can help reframe how parents interpret children's behavior and the way parents think about the reasons for behaviors. By parents having access to this knowledge now helps them to guide their child in ways that respect the child's individual differences. By understanding children's temperaments and our own helps adults to work with them rather than try to change them. It is an opportunity to anticipate and understand a child's reaction. It is also important to know that temperament does not excuse a child's unacceptable behavior, but it does provide direction to how parents can respond to it. Making small and reasonable accommodations to routines can reduce tension. For example, a child who is slow-paced in the mornings may need an extra half-hour to get ready. Knowing who or what may affect the child's behavior can help to alleviate potential problems. Although children obtain their temperament behaviors innately, a large part that helps determine a child's ability to develop and act in certain ways is determined by the parents. When a parent takes the time to identify and more importantly respond to the temperaments they are faced with in a positive way it will help them guide their child in trying to figure out the world.

Recognizing the child's temperament and helping them to understand how it impacts his/her life as well as others is important. It is just as important for parents to recognize their own temperaments. Recognizing each individual's temperament, will help to prevent and manage problems that may arise from the differences among family members.

Temperament continues into adulthood, and later studies by Chess and Thomas have shown that these characteristics continue to influence behavior and adjustment throughout the life-span.

In addition to the initial clinical studies, academic psychologists have developed an interest in the field and researchers such as Bates, Buss & Plomin, Kagan, Rusalov, Cloninger, Trofimova and Rothbart have generated large bodies of research in the areas of personality, neuroscience, and behavioral genetics.

Determination of temperament type

Temperament is determined through specific behavioral profiles, usually focusing on those that are both easily measurable and testable early in childhood. Commonly tested factors include traits related to energetic capacities (named as "Activity", "Endurance", "Extraversion"), traits related to emotionality (such as irritability, frequency of smiling), and approach or avoidance of unfamiliar events.[14][5] There is generally a low correlation between descriptions by teachers and behavioral observations by scientists of features used in determining temperament.[36]

See also

References

  1. ^ Rusalov, VM; Trofimova, IN (2007). Structure of Temperament and Its Measurement. Toronto, Canada: Psychological Services Press.
  2. ^ Friedman, Schustack, Howard S., Miriam W. (2016). Personality: Classic Theories and Modern Research. USA: Pearson Education. ISBN 9780133829808.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  3. ^ Kristal, J. (2005). The temperament perspective: Working with children's behavior styles. New York, NY: Brookes Publishing Co.
  4. ^ Kagan, Jerome. "Temperament". Encyclopedia of Early Childhood Development. Retrieved 17 March 2018.
  5. ^ a b c d e Trofimova, IN (2016). "The interlocking between functional aspects of activities and a neurochemical model of adult temperament". In Arnold, M.C. (ed.). Temperaments: Individual Differences, Social and Environmental Influences and Impact on Quality of Life. New York: Nova Science Publishers. pp. 77–147.
  6. ^ Irina Trofimova (2010). "An investigation into differences between the structure of temperament and the structure of personality". The American Journal of Psychology. 123 (4): 467–480. doi:10.5406/amerjpsyc.123.4.0467. JSTOR 10.5406/amerjpsyc.123.4.0467. PMID 21291163.
  7. ^ "Temperament | personality".
  8. ^ Steiner, Rudolf (1909). "The Four Temperaments: Lecture in Berlin". Retrieved 2009-04-21.
  9. ^ Steiner, Rudolf (1985). The Four Temperaments. ISBN 978-0-910142-11-3.
  10. ^ Rusalov, Vladimir (26 February 2018). "Functional systems theory and the activity-specific approach in psychological taxonomies". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 373 (1744): 20170166. doi:10.1098/rstb.2017.0166. PMC 5832690. PMID 29483350.
  11. ^ a b Trofimova, IN; Robbins, TW (2016). "Temperament and arousal systems: a new synthesis of differential psychology and functional neurochemistry". Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 64: 382–402. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.03.008. hdl:11375/26202. PMID 26969100. S2CID 13937324.
  12. ^ a b Trofimova, IN (2018). "Functionality vs dimensionality in psychological taxonomies, and a puzzle of emotional valence". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 373 (1744): 20170167. doi:10.1098/rstb.2017.0167. PMC 5832691. PMID 29483351.
  13. ^ Kagan, J. (1997). "Temperament and the reactions to unfamiliarity". Child Development. 68 (1): 139–143. doi:10.2307/1131931. JSTOR 1131931. PMID 9084130.
  14. ^ a b c d Jerome Kagan (2005), Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development
  15. ^ Kagan, Jerome; Snidman, Nancy; Kahn, Vali; Towsley, Sara; Steinberg, Laurence; Fox, Nathan A (2007). "The Preservation of Two Infant Temperaments into Adolescence". Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 72 (2): i–95. JSTOR 30163598.
  16. ^ a b c Zentner, Marcel; Bates, John E. (1 January 2008). "Child Temperament: An Integrative Review of Concepts, Research Programs, and Measures". International Journal of Developmental Science. 2 (1–2): 7–37. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.598.1517. doi:10.3233/DEV-2008-21203.
  17. ^ a b c "PsychPage.com Child Temperament". Retrieved 2009-04-21.
  18. ^ Firchow, Nancy M.L.S. . Archived from the original on May 17, 2006. Retrieved 2009-04-21.
  19. ^ . Archived from the original on 2009-04-21. Retrieved 2009-04-21.
  20. ^ . Archived from the original on 2009-01-07. Retrieved 2009-04-21.
  21. ^ . Archived from the original on 2009-02-07. Retrieved 2009-04-21.
  22. ^ Stella Chess and Alexander Thomas, Temperament, Routledge: 1996. ISBN 978-0-87630-835-6. p. 31
  23. ^ "Raising Children – Temperament and Personality". Rookie Parenting. 29 September 2014. Retrieved 12 October 2014.
  24. ^ a b c d Rothbart, M.K & Hwang, J. (2005). Temperament and the development of competence and motivation. In A.J. Elliot & A.C. Dweck (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation. New York: Guilford Press. pp. 167–184. ISBN 978-1-59385-606-9.
  25. ^ a b c d Kail RV, Barnfield A (2011). Children and Their Development, Second Canadian Edition with MyDevelopmentLab. Toronto: Pearson Education Canada. ISBN 978-0-13-255770-2.
  26. ^ a b c d Rothbart, M K (Oct 2004). "Temperament and the pursuit of an integrated developmental psychology". Merrill-Palmer Quarterly. 50 (4): 492–505. doi:10.1353/mpq.2004.0035. S2CID 144970654.
  27. ^ Ormel, J; Oldehinkel, A. J; Ferdinand, R. F; Hartman, C. A; De Winter, A. F; Veenstra, R; Vollebergh, W; Minderaa, R. B; Buitelaar, J. K; Verhulst, F. C (2005). "Internalizing and externalizing problems in adolescence: General and dimension-specific effects of familial loadings and preadolescent temperament traits". Psychological Medicine. 35 (12): 1825–35. doi:10.1017/S0033291705005829. PMID 16300695. S2CID 10109488.
  28. ^ a b c Rothbart, M. K. (2007). "Temperament, development and personality" (PDF). Current Directions in Psychological Science. 16 (4): 207–212. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00505.x. S2CID 15994633.
  29. ^ Valiente, C; Eisenberg, N; Smith, C. L; Reiser, M; Fabes, R. A; Losoya, S; Guthrie, I. K; Murphy, B. C (2003). "The relations of effortful control and reactive control to children's externalizing problems: A longitudinal assessment". Journal of Personality. 71 (6): 1171–96. doi:10.1111/1467-6494.7106011. PMID 14633062.
  30. ^ Kochanska, G; Murray, K. T; Harlan, E. T (2000). "Effortful control in early childhood: Continuity and change, antecedents, and implications for social development". Developmental Psychology. 36 (2): 220–32. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.36.2.220. PMID 10749079.
  31. ^ Depue, R. & Fu, Y. (2012) Neurobiology and neurochemistry of temperament in adults. In: Zentner, M. & Shiner, R. (Eds.) Handbook of Temperament. NY: Guilford Publications, 368-399. (2012).
  32. ^ Sulis, W. (2018). "Assessing the continuum between temperament and affective illness: Psychiatric and mathematical perspectives". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 373 (1744): 20170168. doi:10.1098/rstb.2017.0168. PMC 5832692. PMID 29483352.
  33. ^ Watson, D; Naragon-Gainey, K (2014). "Personality, Emotions, and the Emotional Disorders". Clinical Psychological Science. 2 (4): 422–442. doi:10.1177/2167702614536162. PMC 4370336. PMID 25815243.
  34. ^ Trofimova, I.; Sulis, W (2018). "There is more to mental illness than negative affect: Comprehensive temperament profiles in depression and generalized anxiety". BMC Psychiatry. 18 (1): 125. doi:10.1186/s12888-018-1695-x. PMC 5946468. PMID 29747614.
  35. ^ Development of Temperament Symposium (2008). Philoctetes Center, New York; with panelists Sue Carter, Francis Champagne, Susan Coates, Ed Nercessian, Donald Pfaff, Daniel Schechter, Nadia Bruschweiler Stern
  36. ^ Bishop, Gillian; Spence, Susan H.; Mcdonald, Casey (November 2003). "Can Parents and Teachers Provide a Reliable and Valid Report of Behavioral Inhibition?". Child Development. 74 (6): 1899–1917. doi:10.1046/j.1467-8624.2003.00645.x. JSTOR 3696311. PMID 14669903. S2CID 16653275.

Additional References

  • Anschütz, Marieke, Children and Their Temperaments. ISBN 0-86315-175-2.
  • Carey, William B., Understanding Your Child's Temperament. ISBN 1-4134-7028-9.
  • Diamond, S. (1957). Personality and temperament New York: Harper
  • Kagan J. Galen's prophecy: temperament in human nature. New York, NY: Basic Books; 1994.
  • Kagan J, Snidman NC. The long shadow of temperament. Cambridge, Mass: Harvard University Press; 2004.
  • Kohnstamm GA, Bates JE, Rothbart MK, eds. Temperament in childhood Oxford, United Kingdom: John Wiley and Sons; 1989:59-73.
  • Neville, Helen F., and Diane Clark Johnson, "Temperament Tools: Working with Your Child's Inborn Traits". ISBN 1-884734-34-0.
  • Shick, Lyndall,"Understanding Temperament: Strategies for Creating Family Harmony". ISBN 1-884734-32-4.
  • Seifer, RA; Sameroff, AJ; Barrette, LC; Krafchuk, E. (1994). "Infant temperament measured by multiple observations and mother report". Child Development. 65 (5): 1478–1490. doi:10.2307/1131512. JSTOR 1131512. PMID 7982363.
  • Thomas, Chess & Birch (1968). Temperament and Behavior Disorders in Children. New York, New York University Press

External links

  • Cloninger, C. Robert; Cloninger, Kevin M.; Zwir, Igor; Keltikangas-Järvinen, Liisa (11 November 2019). "The complex genetics and biology of human temperament: a review of traditional concepts in relation to new molecular findings". Translational Psychiatry. 9 (1): 290. doi:10.1038/s41398-019-0621-4. PMC 6848211. PMID 31712636.
  • Henig, Robin Marantz. "Understanding the Anxious Mind". New York Times Magazine, September 29, 2009. Retrieved October 3, 2009.

temperament, this, article, about, psychological, temperament, musical, temperament, musical, temperament, psychology, temperament, broadly, refers, consistent, individual, differences, behavior, that, biologically, based, relatively, independent, learning, sy. This article is about psychological temperament For musical temperament see Musical temperament In psychology temperament broadly refers to consistent individual differences in behavior that are biologically based and are relatively independent of learning system of values and attitudes Some researchers point to association of temperament with formal dynamical features of behavior such as energetic aspects plasticity sensitivity to specific reinforcers and emotionality 1 Temperament traits such as neuroticism sociability impulsivity etc are distinct patterns in behavior throughout a lifetime but they are most noticeable and most studied in children Babies are typically described by temperament but longitudinal research in the 1920s began to establish temperament as something which is stable across the lifespan 2 Contents 1 Definition 1 1 Classification schemes 1 2 Temperament vs personality 2 Main models 2 1 Four temperaments model 2 2 Rusalov Trofimova neurophysiological model of temperament 2 2 1 Endurance related scales 2 2 2 Scales related to speed of integration of behavior 2 2 3 Scales related to type of orientation of behavior 2 2 4 Emotionality scales 2 3 Kagan s research 2 4 Thomas and Chess s nine temperament characteristics 2 5 Mary K Rothbart s three dimensions of temperament 2 5 1 Surgency extraversion 2 5 2 Negative affect 2 5 3 Effortful control 2 6 Others 3 Causal and correlating factors 3 1 Biological basis for temperament 3 2 Temperament vs psychiatric disorders 3 3 Family life 3 3 1 Influences 3 3 2 Understanding for improvement 4 Determination of temperament type 5 See also 6 References 7 Additional References 8 External linksDefinitionTemperament has been defined as the constellation of inborn traits that determine a child s unique behavioral style and the way he or she experiences and reacts to the world 3 Classification schemes Many classification schemes for temperament have been developed and there is no consensus 4 5 The Latin word temperamentum means mixture Temperament vs personality Some commentators see temperament as one factor underlying personality 6 Main modelsFour temperaments model Historically in the second century AD the physician Galen described four classical temperaments melancholic phlegmatic sanguine and choleric corresponding to the four humors or bodily fluids 7 This historical concept was explored by philosophers psychologists psychiatrists and psycho physiologists from very early times of psychological science with theories proposed by Immanuel Kant Hermann Lotze Ivan Pavlov Carl Jung Gerardus Heymans among others In more recent history Rudolf Steiner had emphasized the importance of the four classical temperaments in elementary education the time when he believed the influence of temperament on the personality to be at its strongest 8 9 Neither Galen nor Steiner are generally applied to the contemporary study of temperament in the approaches of modern medicine or contemporary psychology Rusalov Trofimova neurophysiological model of temperament This model based on the longest tradition of neurophysiological experiments started within the investigations of types and properties of nervous systems by Ivan Pavlov s school This experimental tradition started on studies with animals in 1910 20s but expanded its methodology to humans since 1930s and especially since 1960s including EEG caffeine tests evoked potentials behavioral tasks and other psychophysiological methods 10 The latest version of this model is based on the Activity specific approach in temperament research on Alexander Luria s research in clinical neurophysiology and on the neurochemical model Functional Ensemble of Temperament 5 11 12 At the present time the model is associated with the Structure of Temperament Questionnaire and has 12 scales Endurance related scales Motor physical Endurance the ability of an individual to sustain prolonged physical activity using well defined behavioral elements Social verbal Endurance sociability the ability of an individual to sustain prolonged social verbal activities using well defined behavioral elements Mental Endurance or Attention the ability to stay focused on selected features of objects with suppression of behavioral reactivity to other features Scales related to speed of integration of behavior Motor physical Tempo speed of integration of an action in physical manipulations with objects with well defined scripts of actions Plasticity the ability to adapt quickly to changes in situations to change the program of action and to shift between different tasks Social verbal Tempo the preferred speed of speech and ability to understand fast speech on well known topics reading and sorting of known verbal materialScales related to type of orientation of behavior Sensation Seeking SS behavioral orientation to well defined and existing sensational objects and events underestimation of outcomes of risky behavior Empathy behavioral orientation to the emotional states needs of others ranging from empathic deafness in autism and schizophrenia disorders to social dependency Sensitivity to Probabilities the drive to gather information about uniqueness frequency and values of objects events to differentiate their specific features to project these features in future actions Emotionality scales Satisfaction Self Confidence A disposition to be satisfied with current state of events a sense of security confidence in the future In spite of the optimism about outcomes of his or her activities the respondent might be negligent in details Impulsivity Initiation of actions based on immediate emotional reactivity rather than by planning or rational reasoning Neuroticism A tendency to avoid novelty unpredictable situations and uncertainty Preference of well known settings and people over unknown ones and a need for approval and feedback from people around Kagan s research Jerome Kagan and his colleagues have concentrated empirical research on a temperamental category termed reactivity Four month old infants who became motorically aroused and distressed to presentations of novel stimuli were termed highly reactive Those who remained motorically relaxed and did not cry or fret to the same set of unfamiliar events were termed low reactive 13 These high and low reactive infants were tested again at 14 and 21 months in a variety of unfamiliar laboratory situations Highly reactive infants were predominantly characterized by a profile of high fear to unfamiliar events which Kagan termed inhibited Contrastingly low reactive children were minimally fearful to novel situations and were characterized by an uninhibited profile Kagan However when observed again at age 4 5 only a modest proportion of children maintained their expected profile due to mediating factors such as intervening family experiences Those who remained highly inhibited or uninhibited after age 4 5 were at higher risk for developing anxiety and conduct disorders respectively 14 Kagan also used two additional classifications one for infants who were inactive but cried frequently distressed and one for those who showed vigorous activity but little crying aroused Followed to age 14 17 years these groups of children showed differing outcomes including some differences in central nervous system activity Teenagers who had been classed as high reactives when they were babies were more likely to be subdued in unfamiliar situations to report a dour mood and anxiety over the future and to be more religious 15 Thomas and Chess s nine temperament characteristics Alexander Thomas Stella Chess Herbert G Birch Margaret Hertzig and Sam Korn began the classic New York Longitudinal study in the early 1950s regarding infant temperament Thomas Chess amp Birch 1968 The study focused on how temperamental qualities influence adjustment throughout life Chess Thomas et al rated young infants on nine temperament characteristics each of which by itself or with connection to another affects how well a child fits in at school with their friends and at home Behaviors for each one of these traits are on a continuum If a child leans towards the high or low end of the scale it could be a cause for concern The specific behaviors are activity level regularity of sleeping and eating patterns initial reaction adaptability intensity of emotion mood distractibility persistence and attention span and sensory sensitivity Redundancies between the categories have been found and a reduced list is normally used by psychologists today 16 Research by Thomas and Chess used the following nine temperament traits in children 17 18 19 20 21 based on a classification scheme developed by Dr Herbert Birch 22 Thomas Chess Birch Hertzig and Korn found that many babies could be categorized into one of three groups easy difficult and slow to warm up Thomas amp Chess 1977 17 Not all children can be placed in one of these groups Approximately 65 of children fit one of the patterns Of the 65 40 fit the easy pattern 10 fell into the difficult pattern and 15 were slow to warm up Each category has its own strength and weakness and one is not superior to another 23 Thomas Chess Birch Hertzig and Korn showed that easy babies readily adapt to new experiences generally display positive moods and emotions and also have normal eating and sleeping patterns Difficult babies tend to be very emotional irritable and fussy and cry a lot They also tend to have irregular eating and sleeping patterns Slow to warm up babies have a low activity level and tend to withdraw from new situations and people They are slow to adapt to new experiences but accept them after repeated exposure Thomas Chess Birch Hertzig and Korn found that these broad patterns of temperamental qualities are remarkably stable through childhood These traits are also found in children across all cultures Thomas and Chess also studied temperament and environment One sample consisted of white middle class families with high educational status and the other was of Puerto Rican working class families They found several differences Among those were 17 Parents of middle class children were more likely to report behavior problems before the age of nine and the children had sleep problems This may be because children start preschool between the ages of three and four Puerto Rican children under the age of five showed rare signs of sleep problems however sleep problems became more common at the age of six Middle class parents also placed great stress on the child s early development believing that problems in early ages were indicative of later problems in psychological development whereas Puerto Rican parents felt their children would outgrow any problems At the age of nine the report of new problems dropped for middle class children but they rose in Puerto Rican children possibly due to the demands of school Observed traits Activity refers to the child s physical energy Is the child constantly moving or does the child have a relaxing approach A high energy child may have difficulty sitting still in class whereas a child with low energy can tolerate a very structured environment The former may use gross motor skills like running and jumping more frequently Conversely a child with a lower activity level may rely more on fine motor skills such as drawing and putting puzzles together This trait can also refer to mental activity such as deep thinking or reading activities which become more significant as the person matures Regularity also known as rhythmicity refers to the level of predictability in a child s biological functions such as waking becoming tired hunger and bowel movements Does the child have a routine in eating and sleeping habits or are these events more random For example a child with a high regularity rating may want to eat at 2 p m every day whereas a child lower on the regularity scale may eat at sporadic times throughout the day Initial reaction also known as approach or withdrawal This refers to how the child responds whether positively or negatively to new people or environments Does the child approach people or things in the environment without hesitation or does the child shy away A bold child tends to approach things quickly as if without thinking whereas a cautious child typically prefers to watch for a while before engaging in new experiences Adaptability refers to how long it takes the child to adjust to change over time as opposed to an initial reaction Does the child adjust to the changes in their environment easily or is the child resistant A child who adjusts easily may be quick to settle into a new routine whereas a resistant child may take a long time to adjust to the situation Intensity refers to the energy level of a positive or negative response Does the child react intensely to a situation or does the child respond in a calm and quiet manner A more intense child may jump up and down screaming with excitement whereas a mild mannered child may smile or show no emotion Mood refers to the child s general tendency towards a happy or unhappy demeanor All children have a variety of emotions and reactions such as cheerful and stormy happy and unhappy Yet each child biologically tends to have a generally positive or negative outlook A baby who frequently smiles and coos could be considered a cheerful baby whereas a baby who frequently cries or fusses might be considered a stormy baby Distractibility refers to the child s tendency to be sidetracked by other things going on around them Does the child get easily distracted by what is happening in the environment or can the child concentrate despite the interruptions An easily distracted child is engaged by external events and has difficulty returning to the task at hand whereas a rarely distracted child stays focused and completes the task at hand Persistence and attention span refer to the child s length of time on a task and ability to stay with the task through frustrations whether the child stays with an activity for a long period of time or loses interest quickly Sensitivity refers to how easily a child is disturbed by changes in the environment This is also called sensory threshold or threshold of responsiveness Is the child bothered by external stimuli like noises textures or lights or does the child seem to ignore them A sensitive child may lose focus when a door slams whereas a child less sensitive to external noises will be able to maintain focus Mary K Rothbart s three dimensions of temperament Mary K Rothbart views temperament as the individual personality differences in infants and young children that are present prior to the development of higher cognitive and social aspects of personality 24 Rothbart further defines temperament as individual differences in reactivity and self regulation that manifest in the domains of emotion activity and attention Moving away from classifying infants into categories Mary Rothbart identified three underlying dimensions of temperament 25 Using factor analysis on data from 3 to 12 month old children three broad factors emerged and were labeled surgency extraversion negative affect and effortful control 24 Surgency extraversion Surgency extraversion includes positive anticipation impulsivity increased levels of activity and a desire for sensation seeking 26 This factor reflects the degree to which a child is generally happy active and enjoys vocalizing and seeking stimulation 25 Increased levels of smiling and laughter are observed in babies high in surgency extraversion 24 10 to 11 year olds with higher levels of surgency extraversion are more likely to develop externalizing problems like acting out however they are less likely to develop internalizing problems such as shyness and low self esteem 27 Negative affect Negative affect includes fear frustration sadness discomfort 26 and anger 24 This factor reflects the degree to which a child is shy and not easily calmed 25 Anger and frustration is seen as early as 2 to 3 months of age Anger and frustration together predict externalizing and internalizing difficulties Anger alone is later related to externalizing problems while fear is associated with internalizing difficulties Fear as evidenced by behavioral inhibition is seen as early as 7 10 months of age and later predicts children s fearfulness and lower levels of aggression 28 Effortful control Effortful control includes the focusing and shifting of attention inhibitory control perceptual sensitivity and a low threshold for pleasure 26 This factor reflects the degree to which a child can focus attention is not easily distracted 25 can restrain a dominant response in order to execute a non dominant response and employ planning When high in effortful control six to seven year olds tend to be more empathetic and lower in aggressiveness 26 Higher levels of effortful control at age seven also predict lower externalizing problems at age 11 years 29 Children high on negative affect show decreased internalizing and externalizing problems when they are also high on effortful control 28 Rothbart suggests that effortful control is dependent on the development of executive attention skills in the early years In turn executive attention skills allows greater self control over reactive tendencies Effortful control shows stability from infancy into the school years 28 and also predicts conscience 30 Others Solomon Diamond described temperaments based upon characteristics found in the animal world fearfulness aggressiveness affiliativeness and impulsiveness His work has been carried forward by Arnold Buss and Robert Plomin who developed two measures of temperament The Colorado Child Temperament Inventory which includes aspects of Thomas and Chess s schema and the EAS Survey for Children 16 H Hill Goldsmith and Joseph Campos used emotional characteristics to define temperament originally analyzing five emotional qualities motor activity anger fearfulness pleasure joy and interest persistence but later expanding to include other emotions They developed several measures of temperament Lab TAB and TBAQ 16 Other temperament systems include those based upon theories of adult temperament e g Gray and Martin s Temperament Assessment Battery for Children or adult personality e g the Big Five personality traits Causal and correlating factorsBiological basis for temperament Scientists seeking evidence of a biological basis of personality have examined the relationship between temperament and neurotransmitter systems 31 5 11 12 and character defined in this context as developmental aspects of personality Temperament is hypothesized to be associated with biological factors but these have proven to be complex and diverse 14 5 and biological correlations have proven hard to confirm 14 Temperament vs psychiatric disorders Several psychiatrists and differential psychologists have suggested that temperament and mental illness represent varying degrees along the same continuum of neurotransmitter imbalances in neurophysiological systems of behavioral regulation 32 33 34 In fact the original four types of temperament choleric melancholic phlegmatic and sanguine suggested by Hippocrates and Galen resemble mild forms of types of psychiatric disorders described in modern classifications Moreover Hippocrates Galen hypothesis of chemical imbalances as factors of consistent individual differences has also been validated by research in neurochemistry and psychopharmacology though modern studies attribute this to different compounds Many studies have examined the relationships between temperament traits such as impulsivity sensation seeking neuroticism endurance plasticity sociability or extraversion and various neurotransmitter and hormonal systems i e the very same systems implicated in mental disorders Even though temperament and psychiatric disorders can be presented as correspondingly weak and strong imbalances within the same regulatory systems it is incorrect to say that temperament is a weak degree of these disorders Temperament might be a disposition to develop a mental disorder but it should not be treated as a guaranteed marker for disorders Family life Influences Most experts agree that temperament has a genetic and biological basis although environmental factors and maturation modify the ways a child s personality is expressed 35 The term goodness of fit refers to the match or mismatch between temperament and other personal characteristics and the specific features of the environment Differences of temperament or behavior styles between individuals are important in family life They affect the interactions among family members While some children can adapt quickly and easily to family routines and get along with siblings others who are more active or intense may have a difficult time adjusting The interactions between these children and their parents or siblings are among a number of factors that can lead to stress and friction within the family The temperament mix between parents and children also affects family life For example a slow paced parent may be irritated by a highly active child or if both parent and child are highly active and intense conflict could result This knowledge can help parents figure out how temperaments affect family relationships What may appear to be a behavioral problem may actually be a mismatch between the parent s temperament and their child s By taking a closer look at the nine traits that Thomas and Chess revealed from their study parents can gain a better understanding of their child s temperament and their own Parents may also notice that situational factors cause a child s temperament to seem problematic for example a child with low rhythmicity can cause difficulties for a family with a highly scheduled life and a child with a high activity level may be difficult to cope with if the family lives in a crowded apartment upstairs from sensitive neighbors Parents can encourage new behaviors in their children and with enough support a slow to warm up child can become less shy or a difficult baby can become easier to handle More recently infants and children with temperament issues have been called spirited to avoid negative connotations of difficult and slow to warm up Numerous books have been written advising parents how to raise their spirited youngsters Understanding for improvement Understanding a child s temperament can help reframe how parents interpret children s behavior and the way parents think about the reasons for behaviors By parents having access to this knowledge now helps them to guide their child in ways that respect the child s individual differences By understanding children s temperaments and our own helps adults to work with them rather than try to change them It is an opportunity to anticipate and understand a child s reaction It is also important to know that temperament does not excuse a child s unacceptable behavior but it does provide direction to how parents can respond to it Making small and reasonable accommodations to routines can reduce tension For example a child who is slow paced in the mornings may need an extra half hour to get ready Knowing who or what may affect the child s behavior can help to alleviate potential problems Although children obtain their temperament behaviors innately a large part that helps determine a child s ability to develop and act in certain ways is determined by the parents When a parent takes the time to identify and more importantly respond to the temperaments they are faced with in a positive way it will help them guide their child in trying to figure out the world Recognizing the child s temperament and helping them to understand how it impacts his her life as well as others is important It is just as important for parents to recognize their own temperaments Recognizing each individual s temperament will help to prevent and manage problems that may arise from the differences among family members Temperament continues into adulthood and later studies by Chess and Thomas have shown that these characteristics continue to influence behavior and adjustment throughout the life span In addition to the initial clinical studies academic psychologists have developed an interest in the field and researchers such as Bates Buss amp Plomin Kagan Rusalov Cloninger Trofimova and Rothbart have generated large bodies of research in the areas of personality neuroscience and behavioral genetics Determination of temperament typeTemperament is determined through specific behavioral profiles usually focusing on those that are both easily measurable and testable early in childhood Commonly tested factors include traits related to energetic capacities named as Activity Endurance Extraversion traits related to emotionality such as irritability frequency of smiling and approach or avoidance of unfamiliar events 14 5 There is generally a low correlation between descriptions by teachers and behavioral observations by scientists of features used in determining temperament 36 See alsoFour temperaments Functional Ensemble of Temperament Keirsey Temperament Sorter Socionics temperaments Structure of Temperament Questionnaire Big Five Personality Traits Blood type personality theoryReferences Rusalov VM Trofimova IN 2007 Structure of Temperament and Its Measurement Toronto Canada Psychological Services Press Friedman Schustack Howard S Miriam W 2016 Personality Classic Theories and Modern Research USA Pearson Education ISBN 9780133829808 a href Template Cite book html title Template Cite book cite book a CS1 maint multiple names authors list link Kristal J 2005 The temperament perspective Working with children s behavior styles New York NY Brookes Publishing Co Kagan Jerome Temperament Encyclopedia of Early Childhood Development Retrieved 17 March 2018 a b c d e Trofimova IN 2016 The interlocking between functional aspects of activities and a neurochemical model of adult temperament In Arnold M C ed Temperaments Individual Differences Social and Environmental Influences and Impact on Quality of Life New York Nova Science Publishers pp 77 147 Irina Trofimova 2010 An investigation into differences between the structure of temperament and the structure of personality The American Journal of Psychology 123 4 467 480 doi 10 5406 amerjpsyc 123 4 0467 JSTOR 10 5406 amerjpsyc 123 4 0467 PMID 21291163 Temperament personality Steiner Rudolf 1909 The Four Temperaments Lecture in Berlin Retrieved 2009 04 21 Steiner Rudolf 1985 The Four Temperaments ISBN 978 0 910142 11 3 Rusalov Vladimir 26 February 2018 Functional systems theory and the activity specific approach in psychological taxonomies Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences 373 1744 20170166 doi 10 1098 rstb 2017 0166 PMC 5832690 PMID 29483350 a b Trofimova IN Robbins TW 2016 Temperament and arousal systems a new synthesis of differential psychology and functional neurochemistry Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 64 382 402 doi 10 1016 j neubiorev 2016 03 008 hdl 11375 26202 PMID 26969100 S2CID 13937324 a b Trofimova IN 2018 Functionality vs dimensionality in psychological taxonomies and a puzzle of emotional valence Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences 373 1744 20170167 doi 10 1098 rstb 2017 0167 PMC 5832691 PMID 29483351 Kagan J 1997 Temperament and the reactions to unfamiliarity Child Development 68 1 139 143 doi 10 2307 1131931 JSTOR 1131931 PMID 9084130 a b c d Jerome Kagan 2005 Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development Kagan Jerome Snidman Nancy Kahn Vali Towsley Sara Steinberg Laurence Fox Nathan A 2007 The Preservation of Two Infant Temperaments into Adolescence Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 72 2 i 95 JSTOR 30163598 a b c Zentner Marcel Bates John E 1 January 2008 Child Temperament An Integrative Review of Concepts Research Programs and Measures International Journal of Developmental Science 2 1 2 7 37 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 598 1517 doi 10 3233 DEV 2008 21203 a b c PsychPage com Child Temperament Retrieved 2009 04 21 Firchow Nancy M L S Your Child s Temperament Some Basics Archived from the original on May 17 2006 Retrieved 2009 04 21 Ohio State University Extension Understanding Your Child s Temperament Archived from the original on 2009 04 21 Retrieved 2009 04 21 University of Maine Cooperative Extension Family Issues Facts Archived from the original on 2009 01 07 Retrieved 2009 04 21 KeepKidsHealthy com Temperament and Personality Archived from the original on 2009 02 07 Retrieved 2009 04 21 Stella Chess and Alexander Thomas Temperament Routledge 1996 ISBN 978 0 87630 835 6 p 31 Raising Children Temperament and Personality Rookie Parenting 29 September 2014 Retrieved 12 October 2014 a b c d Rothbart M K amp Hwang J 2005 Temperament and the development of competence and motivation In A J Elliot amp A C Dweck Eds Handbook of competence and motivation New York Guilford Press pp 167 184 ISBN 978 1 59385 606 9 a b c d Kail RV Barnfield A 2011 Children and Their Development Second Canadian Edition with MyDevelopmentLab Toronto Pearson Education Canada ISBN 978 0 13 255770 2 a b c d Rothbart M K Oct 2004 Temperament and the pursuit of an integrated developmental psychology Merrill Palmer Quarterly 50 4 492 505 doi 10 1353 mpq 2004 0035 S2CID 144970654 Ormel J Oldehinkel A J Ferdinand R F Hartman C A De Winter A F Veenstra R Vollebergh W Minderaa R B Buitelaar J K Verhulst F C 2005 Internalizing and externalizing problems in adolescence General and dimension specific effects of familial loadings and preadolescent temperament traits Psychological Medicine 35 12 1825 35 doi 10 1017 S0033291705005829 PMID 16300695 S2CID 10109488 a b c Rothbart M K 2007 Temperament development and personality PDF Current Directions in Psychological Science 16 4 207 212 doi 10 1111 j 1467 8721 2007 00505 x S2CID 15994633 Valiente C Eisenberg N Smith C L Reiser M Fabes R A Losoya S Guthrie I K Murphy B C 2003 The relations of effortful control and reactive control to children s externalizing problems A longitudinal assessment Journal of Personality 71 6 1171 96 doi 10 1111 1467 6494 7106011 PMID 14633062 Kochanska G Murray K T Harlan E T 2000 Effortful control in early childhood Continuity and change antecedents and implications for social development Developmental Psychology 36 2 220 32 doi 10 1037 0012 1649 36 2 220 PMID 10749079 Depue R amp Fu Y 2012 Neurobiology and neurochemistry of temperament in adults In Zentner M amp Shiner R Eds Handbook of Temperament NY Guilford Publications 368 399 2012 Sulis W 2018 Assessing the continuum between temperament and affective illness Psychiatric and mathematical perspectives Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences 373 1744 20170168 doi 10 1098 rstb 2017 0168 PMC 5832692 PMID 29483352 Watson D Naragon Gainey K 2014 Personality Emotions and the Emotional Disorders Clinical Psychological Science 2 4 422 442 doi 10 1177 2167702614536162 PMC 4370336 PMID 25815243 Trofimova I Sulis W 2018 There is more to mental illness than negative affect Comprehensive temperament profiles in depression and generalized anxiety BMC Psychiatry 18 1 125 doi 10 1186 s12888 018 1695 x PMC 5946468 PMID 29747614 Development of Temperament Symposium 2008 Philoctetes Center New York with panelists Sue Carter Francis Champagne Susan Coates Ed Nercessian Donald Pfaff Daniel Schechter Nadia Bruschweiler Stern Bishop Gillian Spence Susan H Mcdonald Casey November 2003 Can Parents and Teachers Provide a Reliable and Valid Report of Behavioral Inhibition Child Development 74 6 1899 1917 doi 10 1046 j 1467 8624 2003 00645 x JSTOR 3696311 PMID 14669903 S2CID 16653275 Additional ReferencesAnschutz Marieke Children and Their Temperaments ISBN 0 86315 175 2 Carey William B Understanding Your Child s Temperament ISBN 1 4134 7028 9 Diamond S 1957 Personality and temperament New York Harper Kagan J Galen s prophecy temperament in human nature New York NY Basic Books 1994 Kagan J Snidman NC The long shadow of temperament Cambridge Mass Harvard University Press 2004 Kohnstamm GA Bates JE Rothbart MK eds Temperament in childhood Oxford United Kingdom John Wiley and Sons 1989 59 73 Neville Helen F and Diane Clark Johnson Temperament Tools Working with Your Child s Inborn Traits ISBN 1 884734 34 0 Shick Lyndall Understanding Temperament Strategies for Creating Family Harmony ISBN 1 884734 32 4 Seifer RA Sameroff AJ Barrette LC Krafchuk E 1994 Infant temperament measured by multiple observations and mother report Child Development 65 5 1478 1490 doi 10 2307 1131512 JSTOR 1131512 PMID 7982363 Thomas Chess amp Birch 1968 Temperament and Behavior Disorders in Children New York New York University PressExternal links nbsp Wikimedia Commons has media related to Temperaments Cloninger C Robert Cloninger Kevin M Zwir Igor Keltikangas Jarvinen Liisa 11 November 2019 The complex genetics and biology of human temperament a review of traditional concepts in relation to new molecular findings Translational Psychiatry 9 1 290 doi 10 1038 s41398 019 0621 4 PMC 6848211 PMID 31712636 Henig Robin Marantz Understanding the Anxious Mind New York Times Magazine September 29 2009 Retrieved October 3 2009 Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Temperament amp oldid 1205131631, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.