fbpx
Wikipedia

Synesthesia

Synesthesia (American English) or synaesthesia (British English) is a perceptual phenomenon in which stimulation of one sensory or cognitive pathway leads to involuntary experiences in a second sensory or cognitive pathway.[1][2][3][4] People who report a lifelong history of such experiences are known as synesthetes. Awareness of synesthetic perceptions varies from person to person.[5] In one common form of synesthesia, known as grapheme–color synesthesia or color–graphemic synesthesia, letters or numbers are perceived as inherently colored.[6][7] In spatial-sequence, or number form synesthesia, numbers, months of the year, or days of the week elicit precise locations in space (e.g., 1980 may be "farther away" than 1990), or may appear as a three-dimensional map (clockwise or counterclockwise).[8][9] Synesthetic associations can occur in any combination and any number of senses or cognitive pathways.[10]

Synesthesia
How someone with synesthesia might perceive certain letters and numbers. Most synesthetes see characters just as others do (in whichever color actually displayed) but may simultaneously perceive colors as associated with or evoked by each one.
SpecialtyPsychology, Psychiatry, Neurology

Little is known about how synesthesia develops. It has been suggested that synesthesia develops during childhood when children are intensively engaged with abstract concepts for the first time.[11] This hypothesis—referred to as semantic vacuum hypothesis—could explain why the most common forms of synesthesia are grapheme-color, spatial sequence, and number form. These are usually the first abstract concepts that educational systems require children to learn.

The earliest recorded case of synesthesia is attributed to the Oxford University academic and philosopher John Locke, who, in 1690, made a report about a blind man who said he experienced the color scarlet when he heard the sound of a trumpet.[12] However, there is disagreement as to whether Locke described an actual instance of synesthesia or was using a metaphor.[13] The first medical account came from German physician Georg Tobias Ludwig Sachs in 1812.[13][14][15] The term is from the Ancient Greek σύν syn, 'together', and αἴσθησις aisthēsis, 'sensation'.[12]

Types Edit

There are two overall forms of synesthesia:

  • projective synesthesia: seeing colors, forms, or shapes when stimulated (the widely understood version of synesthesia)
  • associative synesthesia: feeling a very strong and involuntary connection between the stimulus and the sense that it triggers

For example, in chromesthesia (sound to color), a projector may hear a trumpet, and see an orange triangle in space, while an associator might hear a trumpet, and think very strongly that it sounds "orange".[citation needed]

Synesthesia can occur between nearly any two senses or perceptual modes, and at least one synesthete, Solomon Shereshevsky, experienced synesthesia that linked all five senses.[16] Types of synesthesia are indicated by using the notation x → y, where x is the "inducer" or trigger experience, and y is the "concurrent" or additional experience. For example, perceiving letters and numbers (collectively called graphemes) as colored would be indicated as grapheme-color synesthesia. Similarly, when synesthetes see colors and movement as a result of hearing musical tones, it would be indicated as tone → (color, movement) synesthesia.

While nearly every logically possible combination of experiences can occur, several types are more common than others.

Grapheme–color synesthesia Edit

 
From the 2009 non-fiction book Wednesday Is Indigo Blue

In one of the most common forms of synesthesia, individual letters of the alphabet and numbers (collectively referred to as "graphemes") are "shaded" or "tinged" with a color. While different individuals usually do not report the same colors for all letters and numbers, studies with large numbers of synesthetes find some commonalities across letters (e.g., A is likely to be red).[17]

Some authors had argued that the term synaesthesia may not be correct when applied to the so-called grapheme-colour synesthesia and similar phenomena in which the inducer is conceptual (e.g. a letter or number) rather than sensory (e.g. sound or color). They have postulated that the term ideasthesia is a more accurate description.[18][19]

Chromesthesia Edit

Another common form of synesthesia is the association of sounds with colors. For some, everyday sounds can trigger seeing colors. For others, colors are triggered when musical notes or keys are being played. People with synesthesia related to music may also have perfect pitch because their ability to see and hear colors aids them in identifying notes or keys.[20]

The colors triggered by certain sounds, and any other synesthetic visual experiences, are referred to as photisms.

According to Richard Cytowic,[3] chromesthesia is "something like fireworks": voice, music, and assorted environmental sounds such as clattering dishes or dog barks trigger color and firework shapes that arise, move around, and then fade when the sound ends. Sound often changes the perceived hue, brightness, scintillation, and directional movement. Some individuals see music on a "screen" in front of their faces. For Deni Simon, music produces waving lines "like oscilloscope configurations – lines moving in color, often metallic with height, width, and, most importantly, depth. My favorite music has lines that extend horizontally beyond the 'screen' area."

Individuals rarely agree on what color a given sound is. Composers Franz Liszt and Nikolai Rimsky-Korsakov famously disagreed on the colors of musical keys.

Spatial sequence synesthesia Edit

Those with spatial sequence synesthesia (SSS) tend to see ordinal sequences as points in space. People with SSS may have superior memories; in one study, they were able to recall past events and memories far better and in far greater detail than those without the condition. They can also see months or dates in the space around them, but most synesthetes "see" these sequences in their mind's eye. Some people see time like a clock above and around them.[21][22][23]

Number form Edit

 
A number form from one of Francis Galton's subjects (1881).[8] Note how the first 4 digits roughly correspond to their positions on a clock face.

A number form is a mental map of numbers that automatically and involuntarily appear whenever someone who experiences number-forms synesthesia thinks of numbers. These numbers might appear in different locations and the mapping changes and varies between individuals. Number forms were first documented and named in 1881 by Francis Galton in "The Visions of Sane Persons".[24]

Auditory–tactile synesthesia Edit

In auditory–tactile synesthesia, certain sounds can induce sensations in parts of the body. For example, someone with auditory–tactile synesthesia may experience that hearing a specific word or sound feels like touch in one specific part of the body or may experience that certain sounds can create a sensation in the skin without being touched (not to be confused with the milder general reaction known as frisson, which affects approximately 50% of the population). It is one of the least common forms of synesthesia.[25]

Ordinal linguistic personification Edit

Ordinal-linguistic personification (OLP, or personification) is a form of synesthesia in which ordered sequences, such as ordinal numbers, week-day names, months, and alphabetical letters are associated with personalities or genders (Simner & Hubbard 2006). Although this form of synesthesia was documented as early as the 1890s,[26][27] researchers have, until recently, paid little attention to it (see History of synesthesia research). This form of synesthesia was named "OLP" in the contemporary literature by Julia Simner and colleagues[28] although it is now also widely recognised by the term "sequence-personality" synesthesia. Ordinal linguistic personification normally co-occurs with other forms of synesthesia such as grapheme–color synesthesia.

Misophonia Edit

Misophonia is a neurological disorder in which negative experiences (anger, fright, hatred, disgust) are triggered by specific sounds. Cytowic suggests that misophonia is related to, or perhaps a variety of, synesthesia.[1] Edelstein and her colleagues have compared misophonia to synesthesia in terms of connectivity between different brain regions as well as specific symptoms.[1] They formed the hypothesis that "a pathological distortion of connections between the auditory cortex and limbic structures could cause a form of sound-emotion synesthesia."[29] Studies suggest that individuals with misophonia have a normal hearing sensitivity level but the limbic system and autonomic nervous system are constantly in a "heightened state of arousal" where abnormal reactions to sounds will be more prevalent.[30]

Newer studies suggest that depending on its severity, misophonia could be associated with lower cognitive control when individuals are exposed to certain associations and triggers.[31]

It is unclear what causes misophonia. Some scientists believe it could be genetic, others believe it to be present with other additional conditions however there is not enough evidence to conclude what causes it.[32] There are no current treatments for the condition but could be managed with different types of coping strategies.[32] These strategies vary from person to person, some have reported the avoidance of certain situations that could trigger the reaction: mimicking the sounds, cancelling out the sounds by using different methods like earplugs, music, internal dialog and many other tactics. Most misophonics use these to "overwrite" these sounds produced by others.[33]

Mirror-touch synesthesia Edit

This is a form of synesthesia where individuals feel the same sensation that another person feels (such as touch). For instance, when such a synesthete observes someone being tapped on their shoulder, the synesthete involuntarily feels a tap on their own shoulder as well. People with this type of synesthesia have been shown to have higher empathy levels compared to the general population. This may be related to the so-called mirror neurons present in the motor areas of the brain, which have also been linked to empathy.[34]

Lexical–gustatory synesthesia Edit

This is another form of synesthesia where certain tastes are experienced when hearing words. For example, the word basketball might taste like waffles. The documentary 'Derek Tastes of Earwax' gets its name from this phenomenon, in references to pub owner James Wannerton who experiences this particular sensation whenever he hears the name spoken.[35][36] It is estimated that 0.2% of the synesthesia population has this form of synesthesia, making it one of the rarest forms.[37]

Kinesthetic synesthesia Edit

Kinesthetic synesthesia is one of the rarest documented forms of synesthesia in the world.[38] This form of synesthesia is a combination of various different types of synesthesia. Features appear similar to auditory–tactile synesthesia but sensations are not isolated to individual numbers or letters but complex systems of relationships. The result is the ability to memorize and model complex relationships between numerous variables by feeling physical sensations around the kinesthetic movement of related variables. Reports include feeling sensations in the hands or feet, coupled with visualizations of shapes or objects when analyzing mathematical equations, physical systems, or music. In another case, a person described seeing interactions between physical shapes causing sensations in the feet when solving a math problem. Generally, those with this type of synesthesia can memorize and visualize complicated systems, and with a high degree of accuracy, predict the results of changes to the system. Examples include predicting the results of computer simulations in subjects such as quantum mechanics or fluid dynamics when results are not naturally intuitive.[17][39]

Other forms Edit

Other forms of synesthesia have been reported, but little has been done to analyze them scientifically. There are at least 80 types of synesthesia.[38]

In August 2017 a research article in the journal Social Neuroscience reviewed studies with fMRI to determine if persons who experience autonomous sensory meridian response are experiencing a form of synesthesia. While a determination has not yet been made, there is anecdotal evidence that this may be the case, based on significant and consistent differences from the control group, in terms of functional connectivity within neural pathways. It is unclear whether this will lead to ASMR being included as a form of existing synesthesia, or if a new type will be considered.[40]

Signs and symptoms Edit

Some synesthetes often report that they were unaware their experiences were unusual until they realized other people did not have them, while others report feeling as if they had been keeping a secret their entire lives.[41] The automatic and ineffable nature of a synesthetic experience means that the pairing may not seem out of the ordinary. This involuntary and consistent nature helps define synesthesia as a real experience. Most synesthetes report that their experiences are pleasant or neutral, although, in rare cases, synesthetes report that their experiences can lead to a degree of sensory overload.[17]

Though often stereotyped in the popular media as a medical condition or neurological aberration,[citation needed] many synesthetes themselves do not perceive their synesthetic experiences as a handicap. On the contrary, some report it as a gift – an additional "hidden" sense – something they would not want to miss. Most synesthetes become aware of their distinctive mode of perception in their childhood. Some have learned how to apply their ability in daily life and work. Synesthetes have used their abilities in memorization of names and telephone numbers, mental arithmetic, and more complex creative activities like producing visual art, music, and theater.[41]

Despite the commonalities which permit the definition of the broad phenomenon of synesthesia, individual experiences vary in numerous ways. This variability was first noticed early in synesthesia research.[42] Some synesthetes report that vowels are more strongly colored, while for others consonants are more strongly colored.[17] Self-reports, interviews, and autobiographical notes by synesthetes demonstrate a great degree of variety in types of synesthesia, the intensity of synesthetic perceptions, awareness of the perceptual discrepancies between synesthetes and non-synesthetes, and the ways synesthesia is used in work, creative processes, and daily life.[41][43]

Synesthetes are very likely to participate in creative activities.[39] It has been suggested that individual development of perceptual and cognitive skills, in addition to one's cultural environment, produces the variety in awareness and practical use of synesthetic phenomena.[5][43] Synesthesia may also give a memory advantage. In one study, conducted by Julia Simner of the University of Edinburgh, it was found that spatial sequence synesthetes have a built-in and automatic mnemonic reference. Whereas a non-synesthete will need to create a mnemonic device to remember a sequence (like dates in a diary), a synesthete can simply reference their spatial visualizations.[44]

Mechanism Edit

 
Regions thought to be cross-activated in grapheme–color synesthesia (green=grapheme recognition area, red=V4 color area)[45]

As of 2015, the neurological correlates of synesthesia had not been established.[46]

Dedicated regions of the brain are specialized for given functions. Increased cross-talk between regions specialized for different functions may account for the many types of synesthesia. For example, the additive experience of seeing color when looking at graphemes might be due to cross-activation of the grapheme-recognition area and the color area called V4 (see figure).[45] This is supported by the fact that grapheme–color synesthetes can identify the color of a grapheme in their peripheral vision even when they cannot consciously identify the shape of the grapheme.[45]

An alternative possibility is disinhibited feedback, or a reduction in the amount of inhibition along normally existing feedback pathways.[47] Normally, excitation and inhibition are balanced. However, if normal feedback were not inhibited as usual, then signals feeding back from late stages of multi-sensory processing might influence earlier stages such that tones could activate vision. Cytowic and Eagleman find support for the disinhibition idea in the so-called acquired forms[3] of synesthesia that occur in non-synesthetes under certain conditions: temporal lobe epilepsy,[48] head trauma, stroke, and brain tumors. They also note that it can likewise occur during stages of meditation, deep concentration, sensory deprivation, or with use of psychedelics such as LSD or mescaline, and even, in some cases, marijuana.[3] However, synesthetes report that common stimulants, like caffeine and cigarettes do not affect the strength of their synesthesia, nor does alcohol.[3]: 137–40 

A very different theoretical approach to synesthesia is that based on ideasthesia. According to this account, synesthesia is a phenomenon mediated by the extraction of the meaning of the inducing stimulus. Thus, synesthesia may be fundamentally a semantic phenomenon. Therefore, to understand neural mechanisms of synesthesia the mechanisms of semantics and the extraction of meaning need to be understood better. This is a non-trivial issue because it is not only a question of a location in the brain at which meaning is "processed" but pertains also to the question of understanding – epitomized in e.g., the Chinese room problem. Thus, the question of the neural basis of synesthesia is deeply entrenched into the general mind–body problem and the problem of the explanatory gap.[49]

Genetics Edit

Due to the prevalence of synesthesia among the first-degree relatives of people affected,[50] there may be a genetic basis, as indicated by the monozygotic twins studies showing an epigenetic component.[medical citation needed] Synesthesia might also be an oligogenic condition, with locus heterogeneity, multiple forms of inheritance, and continuous variation in gene expression.[medical citation needed] While the exact genetic loci for this trait haven't been identified, research indicates that the genetic constructs underlying synesthesia are most likely more complex than the simple X-linked mode of inheritance that early researchers believed it to be.[7] Further, it remains uncertain as to whether synesthesia perseveres in the genetic pool because it provides a selective advantage, or because it has become a byproduct of some other useful selected trait.[51] Women have a higher chance of developing synesthesia, as demonstrated in population studies conducted in the city of Cambridge, England where females were 6 times more likely to have it.[50] As technological equipment continues to advance, the search for clearer answers regarding the genetics behind synesthesia will become more promising.

Although often termed a "neurological condition," synesthesia is not listed in either the DSM-IV or the ICD since it usually does not interfere with normal daily functioning.[52] Indeed, most synesthetes report that their experiences are neutral or even pleasant.[17] Like perfect pitch, synesthesia is simply a difference in perceptual experience.

 
Reaction times for answers that are congruent with a synesthete's automatic colors are shorter than those whose answers are incongruent.[3]

The simplest approach is test-retest reliability over long periods of time, using stimuli of color names, color chips, or a computer-screen color picker providing 16.7 million choices. Synesthetes consistently score around 90% on the reliability of associations, even with years between tests.[1] In contrast, non-synesthetes score just 30–40%, even with only a few weeks between tests and a warning that they would be retested.[1]

Many tests exist for synesthesia. Each common type has a specific test. When testing for grapheme–color synesthesia, a visual test is given. The person is shown a picture that includes black letters and numbers. A synesthete will associate the letters and numbers with a specific color. An auditory test is another way to test for synesthesia. A sound is turned on and one will either identify it with a taste or envision shapes. The audio test correlates with chromesthesia (sounds with colors). Since people question whether or not synesthesia is tied to memory, the "retest" is given. One is given a set of objects and is asked to assign colors, tastes, personalities, or more. After a period of time, the same objects are presented and the person is asked again to do the same task. The synesthete can assign the same characteristics because that person has permanent neural associations in the brain, rather than memories of a certain object.[medical citation needed]

 
The automaticity of synesthetic experience. A synesthete might perceive the left panel like the panel on the right.[45]

Grapheme–color synesthetes, as a group, share significant preferences for the color of each letter (e.g., A tends to be red; O tends to be white or black; S tends to be yellow, etc.)[17] Nonetheless, there is a great variety in types of synesthesia, and within each type, individuals report differing triggers for their sensations and differing intensities of experiences. This variety means that defining synesthesia in an individual is difficult, and the majority of synesthetes are completely unaware that their experiences have a name.[17]

Neurologist Richard Cytowic identifies the following diagnostic criteria for synesthesia in his first edition book. However, the criteria are different in the second book:[1][2][3]

  1. Synesthesia is involuntary and automatic.
  2. Synesthetic perceptions are spatially extended, meaning they often have a sense of "location." For example, synesthetes speak of "looking at" or "going to" a particular place to attend to the experience.
  3. Synesthetic percepts are consistent and generic (i.e., simple rather than pictorial).
  4. Synesthesia is highly memorable.
  5. Synesthesia is laden with affect.

Cytowic's early cases mainly included individuals whose synesthesia was frankly projected outside the body (e.g., on a "screen" in front of one's face). Later research showed that such stark externalization occurs in a minority of synesthetes. Refining this concept, Cytowic and Eagleman differentiated between "localizers" and "non-localizers" to distinguish those synesthetes whose perceptions have a definite sense of spatial quality from those whose perceptions do not.[3]

Prevalence Edit

Estimates of prevalence of synesthesia have ranged widely, from 1 in 4 to 1 in 25,000–100,000. However, most studies have relied on synesthetes reporting themselves, introducing self-referral bias.[53] In what is cited as the most accurate prevalence study so far,[53] self-referral bias was avoided by studying 500 people recruited from the communities of Edinburgh and Glasgow Universities; it showed a prevalence of 4.4%, with 9 different variations of synesthesia.[54] This study also concluded that one common form of synesthesia – grapheme–color synesthesia (colored letters and numbers) – is found in more than one percent of the population, and this latter prevalence of graphemes–color synesthesia has since been independently verified in a sample of nearly 3,000 people in the University of Edinburgh.[55]

The most common forms of synesthesia are those that trigger colors, and the most prevalent of all is day–color.[54] Also relatively common is grapheme–color synesthesia. We can think of "prevalence" both in terms of how common is synesthesia (or different forms of synesthesia) within the population, or how common are different forms of synesthesia within synesthetes. So within synesthetes, forms of synesthesia that trigger color also appear to be the most common forms of synesthesia with a prevalence rate of 86% within synesthetes.[54] In another study, music–color is also prevalent at 18–41%.[citation needed] Some of the rarest are reported to be auditory–tactile, mirror-touch, and lexical–gustatory.[56]

There is research to suggest that the likelihood of having synesthesia is greater in people with autism spectrum condition.[57]

History Edit

The interest in colored hearing dates back to Greek antiquity when philosophers asked if the color (chroia, what we now call timbre) of music was a quantifiable quality.[58] Isaac Newton proposed that musical tones and color tones shared common frequencies, as did Goethe in his book Theory of Colours.[59] There is a long history of building color organs such as the clavier à lumières on which to perform colored music in concert halls.[60][61]

The first medical description of "colored hearing" is in an 1812 thesis by the German physician Georg Tobias Ludwig Sachs.[62][13][14] The "father of psychophysics," Gustav Fechner, reported the first empirical survey of colored letter photisms among 73 synesthetes in 1876,[63][64] followed in the 1880s by Francis Galton.[8][65][66] Carl Jung refers to "color hearing" in his Symbols of Transformation in 1912.[67]

In the early 1920s, the Bauhaus teacher and musician Gertrud Grunow researched the relationships between sound, color, and movement and developed a 'twelve-tone circle of colour' which was analogous with the twelve-tone music of the Austrian composer Arnold Schönberg (1874-1951).[68] She was a participant in at least one of the Congresses for Colour-Sound Research (German:Kongreß für Farbe-Ton-Forschung) held in Hamburg in the late 1920s and early 1930s.[69]

Research into synesthesia proceeded briskly in several countries, but due to the difficulties in measuring subjective experiences and the rise of behaviorism, which made the study of any subjective experience taboo, synesthesia faded into scientific oblivion between 1930 and 1980.[citation needed]

As the 1980s cognitive revolution made inquiry into internal subjective states respectable again, scientists returned to synesthesia. Led in the United States by Larry Marks and Richard Cytowic, and later in England by Simon Baron-Cohen and Jeffrey Gray, researchers explored the reality, consistency, and frequency of synesthetic experiences. In the late 1990s, the focus settled on grapheme → color synesthesia, one of the most common[17] and easily studied types. Psychologists and neuroscientists study synesthesia not only for its inherent appeal but also for the insights it may give into cognitive and perceptual processes that occur in synesthetes and non-synesthetes alike. Synesthesia is now the topic of scientific books and papers, Ph.D. theses, documentary films, and even novels.[citation needed]

Since the rise of the Internet in the 1990s, synesthetes began contacting one another and creating websites devoted to the condition. These rapidly grew into international organizations such as the American Synesthesia Association, the UK Synaesthesia Association, the Belgian Synesthesia Association, the Canadian Synesthesia Association, the German Synesthesia Association, and the Netherlands Synesthesia Web Community.[citation needed]

Society and culture Edit

Notable cases Edit

Solomon Shereshevsky, a newspaper reporter turned mnemonist, was discovered by Russian neuropsychologist Alexander Luria to have a rare fivefold form of synesthesia,[16] of which he is the only known case. Words and text were not only associated with highly vivid visuospatial imagery but also sound, taste, color, and sensation.[16] Shereshevsky could recount endless details of many things without form, from lists of names to decades-old conversations, but he had great difficulty grasping abstract concepts. The automatic, and nearly permanent, retention of every detail due to synesthesia greatly inhibited Shereshevsky's ability to understand what he read or heard.[16]

Neuroscientist and author V.S. Ramachandran studied the case of a grapheme–color synesthete who was also color blind. While he couldn't see certain colors with his eyes, he could still "see" those colors when looking at certain letters. Because he didn't have a name for those colors, he called them "Martian colors."[70]

Art Edit

Other notable synesthetes come particularly from artistic professions and backgrounds. Synesthetic art historically refers to multi-sensory experiments in the genres of visual music, music visualization, audiovisual art, abstract film, and intermedia.[41][71][72][73][74][75] Distinct from neuroscience, the concept of synesthesia in the arts is regarded as the simultaneous perception of multiple stimuli in one gestalt experience.[76] Neurological synesthesia has been a source of inspiration for artists, composers, poets, novelists, and digital artists.

Writers Edit

Vladimir Nabokov wrote explicitly about synesthesia in several novels. Nabokov described his grapheme–color synesthesia at length in his autobiography, Speak, Memory:[77]

I present a fine case of colored hearing. Perhaps "hearing" is not quite accurate, since the color sensations seem to be produced by the very act of my orally forming a given letter while I imagine its outline. The long a of the English alphabet (and it is this alphabet I have in mind farther on unless otherwise stated) has for me the tint of weathered wood, but the French a evokes polished ebony. This black group also includes hard g (vulcanized rubber) and r (a sooty rag being ripped). Oatmeal n, noodle-limp l, and the ivory-backed hand mirror of o take care of the whites. I am puzzled by my French on which I see as the brimming tension-surface of alcohol in a small glass. Passing on to the blue group, there is steely x, thundercloud z, and huckleberry k. Since a subtle interaction exists between sound and shape, I see q as browner than k, while s is not the light blue of c, but a curious mixture of azure and mother-of-pearl.

Daniel Tammet wrote a book on his experiences with synesthesia called Born on a Blue Day.[78] Joanne Harris, author of Chocolat, is a synesthete who says she experiences colors as scents.[79] Her novel Blueeyedboy features various aspects of synesthesia.

Painters and photographers Edit

Wassily Kandinsky (a synesthete) and Piet Mondrian (not a synesthete) both experimented with image–music congruence in their paintings. Contemporary artists with synesthesia, such as Carol Steen[80] and Marcia Smilack[81] (a photographer who waits until she gets a synesthetic response from what she sees and then takes the picture), use their synesthesia to create their artwork. Linda Anderson, according to NPR considered "one of the foremost living memory painters", creates with oil crayons on fine-grain sandpaper representations of the auditory-visual synaesthesia she experiences during severe migraine attacks.[82][83] Brandy Gale, a Canadian visual artist, experiences an involuntary joining or crossing of any of her senses – hearing, vision, taste, touch, smell and movement. Gale paints from life rather than from photographs and by exploring the sensory panorama of each locale attempts to capture, select, and transmit these personal experiences.[84][85][86] David Hockney perceives music as color, shape, and configuration and uses these perceptions when painting opera stage sets (though not while creating his other artworks). Kandinsky combined four senses: color, hearing, touch, and smell.[1][3]

Composers Edit

 
The music of his "Symphonic Poem "The Sea" (eventually skip to 0:45 seconds) and the matching painting "Sonata of the Sea. "Finale" (1908) by synestheet - Lithuanian painter, composer and writer Mikalojus Konstantinas Čiurlionis.

Multiple composers had experienced synesthesia.

Mikalojus Konstantinas Čiurlionis, a Lithuanian painter, composer and writer, perceived colors and music simultaneously. Many of his paintings bear the names of matching musical pieces: sonatas, fugues, and preludes.

Alexander Scriabin composed colored music that was deliberately contrived and based on the circle of fifths, whereas Olivier Messiaen invented a new method of composition (the modes of limited transposition) specifically to render his bi-directional sound–color synesthesia.[3][87] For example, the red rocks of Bryce Canyon are depicted in his symphony Des canyons aux étoiles... ("From the Canyons to the Stars"). New art movements such as literary symbolism, non-figurative art, and visual music have profited from experiments with synesthetic perception and contributed to the public awareness of synesthetic and multi-sensory ways of perceiving.[41] Other composers who reported synesthesia include Duke Ellington,[88] Nikolay Rimsky-Korsakov,[89] and Jean Sibelius.[72]

Several contemporary composers with a synesthesia are Michael Torke,[72] and Ramin Djawadi, best known for his work on composing the theme songs and scores for such TV series as Game of Thrones, Westworld and for the Iron Man movie. He says he tends to "associate colors with music, or music with colors."[90]

British composer Daniel Liam Glyn created the classical-contemporary music project Changing Stations using Grapheme Colour Synaesthesia. Based on the 11 main lines of the London Underground, the eleven tracks featured on the album represent the eleven main tube line colours.[91] Each track focuses heavily on the different speeds, sounds, and mood of each line, and are composed in the key signature synaesthetically assigned by Glyn with reference to the colour of the tube line on the map.[92]

Musicians Edit

The producer, rapper, and fashion designer Kanye West is a prominent interdisciplinary case. In an impromptu speech he gave during an Ellen interview, he described his condition, saying that he sees sounds, and that everything he sonically makes is a painting.[93] Other notable synesthetes include musicians Billy Joel,[94]: 89, 91  Andy Partridge,[95] Itzhak Perlman,[94]: 53  Lorde,[96] Billie Eilish,[97] Brendon Urie,[98][99] Ida Maria,[100] Brian Chase,[101][102]and classical pianist Hélène Grimaud. Musician Kristin Hersh sees music in colors.[103] Drummer Mickey Hart of The Grateful Dead wrote about his experiences with synaesthesia in his autobiography Drumming at the Edge of Magic.[104] John Frusciante, guitarist of the Red Hot Chili Peppers, talks about his experiences with synesthesia in a podcast with Rick Rubin.[105] Pharrell Williams, of the groups The Neptunes and N.E.R.D., also experiences synesthesia[106][107] and used it as the basis of the album Seeing Sounds. Singer/songwriter Marina and the Diamonds experiences music → color synesthesia and reports colored days of the week.[108] Awsten Knight from Waterparks has chromesthesia, which influences many of the band's artistic choices.[109]

Artists without synesthesia Edit

Some artists frequently mentioned as synesthetes did not, in fact, have the neurological condition. Scriabin's 1911 Prometheus, for example, is a deliberate contrivance whose color choices are based on the circle of fifths and appear to have been taken from Madame Blavatsky.[3][110] The musical score has a separate staff marked luce whose "notes" are played on a color organ. Technical reviews appear in period volumes of Scientific American.[3] On the other hand, his older colleague Rimsky-Korsakov (who was perceived as a fairly conservative composer) was, in fact, a synesthete.[111]

French poets Arthur Rimbaud and Charles Baudelaire wrote of synesthetic experiences, but there is no evidence they were synesthetes themselves. Baudelaire's 1857 Correspondances introduced the notion that the senses can and should intermingle. Baudelaire participated in a hashish experiment by psychiatrist Jacques-Joseph Moreau and became interested in how the senses might affect each other.[41] Rimbaud later wrote Voyelles (1871), which was perhaps more important than Correspondances in popularizing synesthesia. He later boasted "J'inventais la couleur des voyelles!" (I invented the colors of the vowels!).[112]

Science Edit

Some technologists, like inventor Nikola Tesla,[113] and scientists also reported being synesthetic. Physicist Richard Feynman describes his colored equations in his autobiography, What Do You Care What Other People Think?:[114] "When I see equations, I see the letters in colors. I don't know why. I see vague pictures of Bessel functions with light-tan j's, slightly violet-bluish n's, and dark brown x's flying around."[115]

Literature Edit

Synesthesia is sometimes used as a plot device or way of developing a character's inner life. Author and synesthete Pat Duffy describes four ways in which synesthetic characters have been used in modern fiction.[116][117]

  • Synesthesia as Romantic ideal: in which the condition illustrates the Romantic ideal of transcending one's experience of the world. Books in this category include The Gift by Vladimir Nabokov.
  • Synesthesia as pathology: in which the trait is pathological. Books in this category include The Whole World Over by Julia Glass.
  • Synesthesia as Romantic pathology: in which synesthesia is pathological but also provides an avenue to the Romantic ideal of transcending quotidian experience. Books in this category include Holly Payne's The Sound of Blue and Anna Ferrara's The Woman Who Tried To Be Normal.
  • Synesthesia as psychological health and balance: Painting Ruby Tuesday by Jane Yardley, and A Mango-Shaped Space by Wendy Mass.

Literary depictions of synesthesia are criticized as often being more of a reflection of an author's interpretation of synesthesia than of the phenomenon itself.[citation needed]

Research Edit

 
Tests like this demonstrate that people do not attach sounds to visual shapes arbitrarily. When people are given a choice between the words "Bouba" and "Kiki", the left shape is almost always called "Kiki" while the right is called "Bouba"

Research on synesthesia raises questions about how the brain combines information from different sensory modalities, referred to as crossmodal perception or multisensory integration.[citation needed]

An example of this is the bouba/kiki effect. In an experiment first designed by Wolfgang Köhler, people are asked to choose which of two shapes is named bouba and which kiki. The angular shape, kiki, is chosen by 95–98% and bouba for the rounded one. Individuals on the island of Tenerife showed a similar preference between shapes called takete and maluma. Even 2.5-year-old children (too young to read) show this effect.[118] Research indicated that in the background of this effect may operate a form of ideasthesia.[119]

Researchers hope that the study of synesthesia will provide better understanding of consciousness and its neural correlates. In particular, synesthesia might be relevant to the philosophical problem of qualia,[4][120] given that synesthetes experience extra qualia (e.g., colored sound). An important insight for qualia research may come from the findings that synesthesia has the properties of ideasthesia,[18] which then suggest a crucial role of conceptualization processes in generating qualia.[11]

Technological applications Edit

Synesthesia also has a number of practical applications, including 'intentional synesthesia' in technology,[121] and sensory prosthetics.[122]

The Voice (vOICe) Edit

Peter Meijer developed a sensory substitution device for the visually impaired called The vOICe (the capital letters "O," "I," and "C" in "vOICe" are intended to evoke the expression "Oh I see"). The vOICe is a privately owned research project, running without venture capital, that was first implemented using low-cost hardware in 1991.[123] The vOICe is a visual-to-auditory sensory substitution device (SSD) preserving visual detail at high resolution (up to 25,344 pixels).[124] The device consists of a laptop, head-mounted camera or computer camera, and headphones. The vOICe converts visual stimuli of the surroundings captured by the camera into corresponding aural representations (soundscapes) delivered to the user through headphones at a default rate of one soundscape per second. Each soundscape is a left-to-right scan, with height represented by pitch, and brightness by loudness.[125] The vOICe compensates for the loss of vision by converting information from the lost sensory modality into stimuli in a remaining modality.[126]

See also Edit

References Edit

  1. ^ a b c d e f g Cytowic RE (2002). Synesthesia: A Union of the Senses (2nd ed.). Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press. ISBN 978-0-262-03296-4. OCLC 49395033.[page needed]
  2. ^ a b Cytowic RE (2003). The Man Who Tasted Shapes. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press. ISBN 978-0-262-53255-6. OCLC 53186027.[page needed]
  3. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l Cytowic RE, Eagleman DM (2009). Wednesday is Indigo Blue: Discovering the Brain of Synesthesia (with an afterword by Dmitri Nabokov). Cambridge: MIT Press. ISBN 978-0-262-01279-9.[page needed]
  4. ^ a b Harrison JE, Baron-Cohen S (1996). Synaesthesia: classic and contemporary readings. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing. ISBN 978-0-631-19764-5. OCLC 59664610.[page needed]
  5. ^ a b van Campen C (2009). (PDF). Teccogs. 1: 1–13. Archived from the original (PDF) on 8 July 2009.
  6. ^ Rich AN, Mattingley JB (January 2002). "Anomalous perception in synaesthesia: a cognitive neuroscience perspective". Nature Reviews. Neuroscience (Review). 3 (1): 43–52. doi:10.1038/nrn702. PMID 11823804. S2CID 11477960.
  7. ^ a b Hubbard EM, Ramachandran VS (November 2005). "Neurocognitive mechanisms of synesthesia". Neuron (Review). 48 (3): 509–520. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2005.10.012. PMID 16269367. S2CID 18730779.
  8. ^ a b c Galton F (1880). "Visualized Numerals". Nature. 21 (543): 494–5. Bibcode:1880Natur..21..494G. doi:10.1038/021494e0. S2CID 4074444.
  9. ^ Seron X, Pesenti M, Noël MP, Deloche G, Cornet JA (August 1992). "Images of numbers, or "When 98 is upper left and 6 sky blue"". Cognition. 44 (1–2): 159–196. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(92)90053-K. PMID 1511585. S2CID 26687757.
  10. ^ "How Synesthesia Works". HowStuffWorks. 1 January 1970. Retrieved 2 May 2016.
  11. ^ a b Mroczko-Wąsowicz A, Nikolić D (2014). "Semantic mechanisms may be responsible for developing synesthesia". Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 8: 509. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00509. PMC 4137691. PMID 25191239.
  12. ^ a b Ward, Ossian (10 June 2006). The man who heard his paint box hiss. The Telegraph. Retrieved 3 December 2018.
  13. ^ a b c Jewanski J, Day SA, Ward J (July 2009). "A colorful albino: the first documented case of synaesthesia, by Georg Tobias Ludwig Sachs in 1812". Journal of the History of the Neurosciences. 18 (3): 293–303. doi:10.1080/09647040802431946. PMID 20183209. S2CID 8641750.
  14. ^ a b Herman LM (28 December 2018). "Synesthesia". Encyclopaedia Britannica. Retrieved 25 January 2019.
  15. ^ Konnikova M (26 February 2013). . Scientific American Blog Network. Archived from the original on 20 September 2016. Retrieved 25 January 2019.
  16. ^ a b c d Schacter D (1996). Searching for Memory: The Brain, The Mind, And The Past. Basic Books. pp. 81. ISBN 978-0-465-07552-2.
  17. ^ a b c d e f g h Sagiv N, Robertson LC (2005). Synesthesia: perspectives from cognitive neuroscience. Oxford: Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-516623-1. OCLC 53020292.[page needed]
  18. ^ a b Nikolić D (2009). "Is synaesthesia actually ideaesthesia? An inquiry into the nature of the phenomenon" (PDF). Proceedings of the Third International Congress on Synaesthesia, Science & Art, Granada, Spain, April 26–29.
  19. ^ Simner J (February 2012). "Defining synaesthesia" (PDF). British Journal of Psychology (Review). 103 (1): 1–15. doi:10.1348/000712610X528305. PMID 22229768. S2CID 9038571.
  20. ^ "Absolute Pitch and Synesthesia | The Feinstein Institute for Medical Research". The Feinstein Institute for Medical Research. Retrieved 2 May 2016.
  21. ^ Jonas CN, Price MC (30 October 2014). "Not all synesthetes are alike: spatial vs. visual dimensions of sequence-space synesthesia". Frontiers in Psychology. 5: 1171. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01171. PMC 4214186. PMID 25400596.
  22. ^ [unreliable medical source?]Do sequence-space synaesthetes have better spatial imagery skills? Maybe not, The National Center for Biotechnology Information
  23. ^ [unreliable source?] A Mind That Touches the Past, Sciencemag.org
  24. ^ Seaton M (2013). "The Visions of Sane Persons". Ploughshares. 39 (1): 144–145. doi:10.1353/plo.2013.0012. ISSN 2162-0903. S2CID 161837195.
  25. ^ Naumer MJ, van den Bosch JJ (July 2009). (PDF). Journal of Neurophysiology. 102 (1): 7–8. doi:10.1152/jn.00209.2009. PMID 19403745. S2CID 1712367. Archived from the original (PDF) on 20 February 2019.
  26. ^ Flournoy 2001
  27. ^ Calkins 1893
  28. ^ Simner J, Hubbard EM (December 2006). "Variants of synesthesia interact in cognitive tasks: evidence for implicit associations and late connectivity in cross-talk theories". Neuroscience. 143 (3): 805–814. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.08.018. PMID 16996695. S2CID 18922608.
  29. ^ Edelstein M, Brang D, Rouw R, Ramachandran VS (2013). "Misophonia: physiological investigations and case descriptions". Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 7: 296. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00296. PMC 3691507. PMID 23805089.
  30. ^ Schröder A, van Diepen R, Mazaheri A, Petropoulos-Petalas D, Soto de Amesti V, Vulink N, Denys D (9 April 2014). "Diminished n1 auditory evoked potentials to oddball stimuli in misophonia patients". Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 8: 123. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00123. PMC 3988356. PMID 24782731.
  31. ^ Daniels EC, Rodriguez A, Zabelina DL (16 January 2020). Langguth B (ed.). "Severity of misophonia symptoms is associated with worse cognitive control when exposed to misophonia trigger sounds". PLOS ONE. 15 (1): e0227118. Bibcode:2020PLoSO..1527118D. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0227118. PMC 6964854. PMID 31945068.
  32. ^ a b Duddy D, Oeding K (29 April 2014). "Misophonia: An Overview". Seminars in Hearing. 35 (2): 084–091. doi:10.1055/s-0034-1372525. ISSN 0734-0451. S2CID 76090599.
  33. ^ Edelstein M, Brang D, Rouw R, Ramachandran VS (2013). "Misophonia: physiological investigations and case descriptions". Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 7: 296. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00296. PMC 3691507. PMID 23805089.
  34. ^ Heyes C (March 2010). "Where do mirror neurons come from?". Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 34 (4): 575–583. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.11.007. PMID 19914284. S2CID 2578537.
  35. ^ "Derek Tastes of Ear Wax". Top Documentary Films. Retrieved 2 February 2015.
  36. ^ "BBC – Science & Nature – Horizon". bbc.co.uk. Retrieved 2 February 2015.
  37. ^ Simner J (2009). "Lexical–Gustatory Synesthesia". Encyclopedia of Neuroscience. Springer Berlin Heidelberg. pp. 2149–2152. doi:10.1007/978-3-540-29678-2_2766. ISBN 978-3-540-23735-8.
  38. ^ a b "Types-of-Syn". DaySyn.com. Retrieved 26 January 2016.
  39. ^ a b Dailey A, Martindale C, Borkum J (1997). "Creativity, synesthesia, and physiognomic perception". Creativity Research Journal. 10 (1): 1–8. doi:10.1207/s15326934crj1001_1.
  40. ^ Smith SD, Katherine Fredborg B, Kornelsen J (August 2017). "An examination of the default mode network in individuals with autonomous sensory meridian response (ASMR)". Social Neuroscience. 12 (4): 361–365. doi:10.1080/17470919.2016.1188851. PMID 27196787.
  41. ^ a b c d e f van Campen C (2007). The Hidden Sense: Synesthesia in Art and Science. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press. ISBN 978-0-262-22081-1. OCLC 80179991.[page needed]
  42. ^ Flournoy T (2001). Des phénomènes de synopsie (Audition colorée). Adamant Media Corporation. ISBN 978-0-543-94462-7.[page needed]
  43. ^ a b [full citation needed] Dittmar A, ed. (2007). Synästhesien. Roter Faden durchs Leben? [Synesthesia. A red thread through life?] (in German). Essen: Verlag Die Blaue Eule.
  44. ^ "Investigating Spatial Sequence Synesthesia". Synesthesia Test. 26 June 2012. Retrieved 2 May 2016.
  45. ^ a b c d [non-primary source needed][dead link]Ramachandran VS, Hubbard EM (2001). (PDF). Journal of Consciousness Studies. 8 (12): 3–34. Archived from the original (PDF) on 27 May 2006.
  46. ^ Hupé JM, Dojat M (2015). "A critical review of the neuroimaging literature on synesthesia". Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 9: 103. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2015.00103. PMC 4379872. PMID 25873873.
  47. ^ Grossenbacher PG, Lovelace CT (January 2001). "Mechanisms of synesthesia: cognitive and physiological constraints". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 5 (1): 36–41. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01571-0. PMID 11164734. S2CID 15092606.
  48. ^ Neckar M, Bob P (11 January 2016). "Synesthetic associations and psychosensory symptoms of temporal epilepsy". Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment. 12: 109–112. doi:10.2147/NDT.S95464. PMC 4714732. PMID 26811683.
  49. ^ Gray JA, Parslow DM, Brammer MJ, Chopping S, Vythelingum GN, ffytche DH (February 2006). "Evidence against functionalism from neuroimaging of the alien colour effect in synaesthesia". Cortex; A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior. 42 (2): 309–318. doi:10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70357-5. PMID 16683506. S2CID 4475077.
  50. ^ a b Baron-Cohen S, Burt L, Smith-Laittan F, Harrison J, Bolton P (1 September 1996). "Synaesthesia: prevalence and familiality". Perception. 25 (9): 1073–1079. doi:10.1068/p251073. PMID 8983047. S2CID 25954158.
  51. ^ Brang D, Ramachandran VS (November 2011). "Survival of the synesthesia gene: why do people hear colors and taste words?". PLOS Biology. 9 (11): e1001205. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.1001205. PMC 3222625. PMID 22131906.
  52. ^ Hubbard EM (June 2007). "Neurophysiology of synesthesia" (PDF). Current Psychiatry Reports. 9 (3): 193–199. doi:10.1007/s11920-007-0018-6. PMID 17521514. S2CID 13370073.
  53. ^ a b Simner J, Hubbard EM, eds. (2013). "A brief history of synesthesia research". Oxford Handbook of Synesthesia. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. pp. 13–17. ISBN 978-0-19-960332-9. Retrieved 25 February 2018.
  54. ^ a b c Simner J, Mulvenna C, Sagiv N, Tsakanikos E, Witherby SA, Fraser C, et al. (2006). "Synaesthesia: the prevalence of atypical cross-modal experiences" (PDF). Perception. 35 (8): 1024–1033. doi:10.1068/p5469. PMID 17076063. S2CID 2508540.
  55. ^ Carmichael DA, Down MP, Shillcock RC, Eagleman DM, Simner J (May 2015). "Validating a standardised test battery for synesthesia: Does the Synesthesia Battery reliably detect synesthesia?". Consciousness and Cognition. 33: 375–385. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2015.02.001. PMC 5047354. PMID 25734257.
  56. ^ Safran AB, Sanda N (February 2015). "Color synesthesia. Insight into perception, emotion, and consciousness". Current Opinion in Neurology. 28 (1): 36–44. doi:10.1097/WCO.0000000000000169. PMC 4286234. PMID 25545055.
  57. ^ Baron-Cohen S, Johnson D, Asher J, Wheelwright S, Fisher SE, Gregerson PK, Allison C, "Is synaesthesia more common in autism?", Molecular Autism, 20 November 2013
  58. ^ Gage J (1993). Colour and Culture. Practice and Meaning from Antiquity to Abstraction. London: Thames & Hudson.[page needed]
  59. ^ Goethe JW (1840). Theory of Colours. J. Murray.
  60. ^ Peacock K (1988). "Instruments to Perform Color-Music: Two Centuries of Technological Experimentation" (PDF). Leonardo. 21 (4): 397–406. doi:10.2307/1578702. JSTOR 1578702. S2CID 54178977.
  61. ^ Jewanski J, Sidler N, eds. (2006). Farbe – Licht – Musik. Synaesthesie und Farblichtmusik. Bern: Peter Lang.[page needed]
  62. ^ Mahling F (1926). "Das Problem der 'audition colorée': Eine historisch-kritische Untersuchung". Archiv für die gesamte Psychologie. 57: 165–301.
  63. ^ Fechner G (1876). Vorschule der Aesthetik. Leipzig: Breitkopf und Hartel.
  64. ^ von Campen C (1996). "De verwarring der zintuigen. Artistieke en psychologische experimenten met synesthesie". Psychologie & Maatschappij. 20 (1): 10–26.
  65. ^ Galton F (1880). "Visualized Numerals". Nature. 21 (533): 252–6. Bibcode:1880Natur..21..252G. doi:10.1038/021252a0.
  66. ^ Galton F (1883). Inquiries into Human Faculty and Its Development. Macmillan. Retrieved 17 June 2008.[page needed]
  67. ^ Jung CG (1956) [1912]. "The Transformation of Libido". Symbols of Transformation. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul. p. 237.
  68. ^ Bauhaus100.Curriculum. Harmonisation theory 1919-1924. Retrieved 2 December 2018
  69. ^ Farbe-Ton-Forschungen. III. Band. Bericht über den II. Kongreß für Farbe-Ton-Forschung (Hamburg 1. - 5. Oktober 1930). Published 1931.
  70. ^ Carroll S (5 November 2007). "Martian Colors - Cosmic Variance". Discover. Retrieved 12 April 2018.
  71. ^ Campen C (1999). "Artistic and psychological experiments with synesthesia". Leonardo. 32 (1): 9–14. doi:10.1162/002409499552948. S2CID 57568389.
  72. ^ a b c Berman G (1999). "Synesthesia and the Arts". Leonardo. 32 (1): 15–22. doi:10.1162/002409499552957. S2CID 57565862.
  73. ^ von Maur K (1999). The Sound of Painting: Music in Modern Art (Pegasus Library). Munich: Prestel. ISBN 978-3-7913-2082-3.[page needed]
  74. ^ Gage JD (1993). Colour and culture: practice and meaning from antiquity to abstraction. London: Thames and Hudson. ISBN 978-0-500-27818-5.[page needed]
  75. ^ Gage JD (1999). Color and meaning: art, science, and symbolism. Berkeley: University of California Press. ISBN 978-0-520-22611-1.[page needed]
  76. ^ van Campen C (2009). "Visual Music and Musical Paintings. The Quest for Synesthesia in the Arts.". In Bacci F, Melcher D (eds.). Making Sense of Art, making Art of Sense. Oxford: Oxford University Press.[page needed]
  77. ^ Bouchet, Marie; Loison-Charles, Julie; Poulin, Isabelle (19 June 2020). The Five Senses in Nabokov's Works. Springer Nature. pp. 255–256. ISBN 978-3-030-45406-7.
  78. ^ Tammet D (2007). Born on a Blue Day. Free Press. ISBN 978-1-4165-3507-2.[page needed]
  79. ^ "Chocolat author Joanne Harris talks about her latest novel Blue Eyed Boy". Metro. 7 April 2010.
  80. ^ Steen C (2001). "Visions Shared: A Firsthand Look into Synesthesia and Art". Leonardo. 34 (3): 203–208. doi:10.1162/002409401750286949. S2CID 57570552.
  81. ^ Marcia Smilack Website Accessed 20 August 2006.
  82. ^ . 7 April 2019. Archived from the original on 7 April 2019. Retrieved 27 December 2021.
  83. ^ . NPR. 7 April 2019. Archived from the original on 7 April 2019. Retrieved 27 December 2021.
  84. ^ . gualalaarts.org. Archived from the original on 2 February 2015. Retrieved 2 February 2015.
  85. ^ EG. . FORA.tv. Archived from the original on 2 February 2015. Retrieved 2 February 2015.
  86. ^ brandygale.com
  87. ^ Samuel C (1994) [1986]. Olivier Messiaen: Music and Color. Conversations with Claude Samuel. Translated by Glasow ET. Portland, Oregon: Amadeus Press.
  88. ^ Duke Ellington as quoted in George D (1981). Sweet man: The real Duke Ellington. New York: G.P. Putnam's Sons. p. 226.
  89. ^ Yastrebtsev V (1908). "On N.A.Rimsky-Korsakov's color sound- contemplation". Russkaya Muzykalnaya Gazeta (in Russian) (39–40): 842–845. cited by Bulat Galeyev (1999).
  90. ^ "Meet the musical genius behind the 'Game of Thrones' soundtrack who watches each season before anyone else". Business Insider. Retrieved 11 January 2018.
  91. ^ "Sounds of the Underground: Synaesthetic Musician Creates LP Based on Tube Map". The Big Issue. 27 April 2017. Retrieved 15 May 2018.
  92. ^ "Do all London Underground lines have a unique sound?" (PDF). Norwegian Air Magazine. Retrieved 3 April 2017.
  93. ^ Kanye West FULL Banned Ellen Interview HD May 19 2016, retrieved 20 August 2022
  94. ^ a b Seaberg M (2011). Tasting the Universe. New Page Books. ISBN 978-1-60163-159-6.
  95. ^ Enos, Morgan (9 April 2020). "Chords Become Colors: Inside Synesthesia". Tidal. Retrieved 11 May 2023.
  96. ^ Trendell A (11 May 2017). "Lorde explains the experience of having synaesthesia". NME. Retrieved 15 June 2021.
  97. ^ Nattress K. "Billie Eilish Explains How Synesthesia Affects Her Music". iHeartRadio. Retrieved 15 June 2021.
  98. ^ Spanos B (15 January 2016). "Panic! at the Disco: Band Is 'Outlet for Nonchalant Chaos'". Rolling Stone. Retrieved 13 November 2019.
  99. ^ "4 Singers Who Draw Inspiration From Synesthesia To Write Music". Cultura Colectiva. 23 January 2018. Retrieved 16 April 2020.
  100. ^ Cairns D (24 February 2008). "Times Online interview". The Times. London. Retrieved 24 July 2008.
  101. ^ Forrest E (30 March 2009). "Emma Forrest meets New York's favourite art-punk rockers Yeah Yeah Yeahs". guardian.co.uk. London: The Guardian. Retrieved 7 May 2009.
  102. ^ Chase B. . yeahyeahyeahs.com. Archived from the original on 25 January 2009. Retrieved 7 May 2009.
  103. ^ Seaman D (8 May 2021). "Strange Angels: Kristin Hersh On Music & Motherhood". The Quietus. Retrieved 8 November 2021.
  104. ^ Hart M, Stevens J, Lieberman F (1990). Drumming at the edge of magic: a journey into the spirit of percussion. San Francisco, CA: Harper. p. 133.
  105. ^ John Frusciante of the Red Hot Chili Peppers Returns, Part 1 | Broken Record (Hosted by Rick Rubin), retrieved 20 May 2023
  106. ^ [unreliable source?] It just always stuck out in my mind, and I could always see it. I don't know if that makes sense, but I could always visualize what I was hearing... Yeah, it was always like weird colors." From a Nightline interview with Pharrell
  107. ^ "Synesthetes: "People of the Future"". Psychology Today. 3 March 2012. Retrieved 15 May 2014.
  108. ^ "Loose Women: Marina and the Diamonds". ITV Lifestyle. ITV Network. 27 April 2010. Retrieved 28 April 2010.
  109. ^ Waterparks' Awsten Can 'See' Music, So We Had Him Paint His Songs 🎨 Seeing Sounds | MTV, retrieved 14 February 2023
  110. ^ [page needed] Dann KT (1998). Bright colors falsely seen: synaesthesia and the search for transcendental knowledge. New Haven, Conn: Yale University Press. ISBN 978-0-300-06619-7.
  111. ^ This is according to an article in the Russian press, Yastrebtsev V. "On N.A.Rimsky-Korsakov's color sound- contemplation." Russkaya muzykalnaya gazeta, 1908, N 39–40, pp. 842–845 (in Russian), cited by Bulat Galeyev (1999).
  112. ^ Barth F, Giampieri-Deutsch P, Hans-Dieter K (2012). Sensory Perception: Mind & Matter. Vienna: Springer Vienna. p. 221. ISBN 978-3-211-99750-5. I invented the colours of the vowels!
  113. ^ Tesla N. (PDF). pitt.edu. Archived from the original (PDF) on 17 July 2012. Retrieved 4 September 2012.
  114. ^ Feynman R (1988). What Do You Care What Other People Think?. New York: Norton. p. 59.
  115. ^ "Colourful language: U of T psychologists discover enhanced language learning in synesthetes". University of Toronto News. Retrieved 20 March 2023.
  116. ^ Duffy PL (2006). . 6th Annual Meeting of the American Synesthesia Association. University of South Florida. Archived from the original on 17 January 2012.
  117. ^ Duffy PL, Simner J (February 2010). "Synaesthesia in fiction". Cortex; A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior. 46 (2): 277–278. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2008.11.003. PMID 19081086. S2CID 11224620.
  118. ^ [non-primary source needed] Maurer D, Pathman T, Mondloch CJ (May 2006). "The shape of boubas: sound-shape correspondences in toddlers and adults". Developmental Science. 9 (3): 316–322. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00495.x. PMID 16669803. S2CID 7297731.
  119. ^ Milan E, Iborra O, de Cordoba M, Juárez-Ramos V, Artacho MR, Rubio JL (2013). "The Kiki-Bouba effect: A case of personification and ideaesthesia". Journal of Consciousness Studies. 20 (1–2): 84–102.
  120. ^ Gray JA, Chopping S, Nunn J, et al. (2002). "Implications of synaesthesia for functionalism: Theory and experiments". Journal of Consciousness. 9 (12): 5–31.
  121. ^ Suslick KS (December 2012). "Synesthesia in science and technology: more than making the unseen visible". Current Opinion in Chemical Biology. 16 (5–6): 557–563. doi:10.1016/j.cbpa.2012.10.030. PMC 3606019. PMID 23183411.
  122. ^ Scheff CM (1 January 1986). "Experimental model for the study of changes in the organization of human sensory information processing through the design and testing of non-invasive prosthetic devices for sensory impaired people". ACM SIGCAPH Computers and the Physically Handicapped (36): 3–10. doi:10.1145/15711.15713. ISSN 0163-5727. S2CID 11924232.
  123. ^ Meijer P. "Augmented Reality for the Totally Blind". Retrieved 4 February 2014.
  124. ^ Striem-Amit E, Guendelman M, Amedi A (16 March 2012). "'Visual' acuity of the congenitally blind using visual-to-auditory sensory substitution". PLOS ONE. 7 (3): e33136. Bibcode:2012PLoSO...733136S. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0033136. PMC 3306374. PMID 22438894.
  125. ^ Carmichael J (10 July 2013). "Device Trains Blind People To 'See' By Listening". Retrieved 4 February 2014.
  126. ^ Haigh A, Brown DJ, Meijer P, Proulx MJ (2013). "How well do you see what you hear? The acuity of visual-to-auditory sensory substitution". Frontiers in Psychology. 4: 330. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00330. PMC 3684791. PMID 23785345.

Further reading Edit

  • de Broucker T (April 2013). "[Synaesthesia, an augmented sensory world: phenomenology and literature review]". Revue Neurologique (Review) (in French). 169 (4): 328–334. doi:10.1016/j.neurol.2012.09.016. PMID 23434143.
  • Cohen Kadosh R, Terhune DB (February 2012). "Redefining synaesthesia?" (PDF). British Journal of Psychology (Review). 103 (1): 20–23. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.2010.02003.x. PMID 22229770.
  • De Cordoba MJ, Riccò D, Day S (July 2014). Synaesthesia: Theoretical, artistic and scientific foundations. Granada, Spain. p. 372. ISBN 978-84-939054-9-1.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: location missing publisher (link)
  • Dael N, Sierro G, Mohr C (October 2013). "Affect-related synesthesias: a prospective view on their existence, expression and underlying mechanisms". Frontiers in Psychology (Review). 4: 754. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00754. PMC 3798864. PMID 24151478.
  • Fitzgibbon BM, Enticott PG, Rich AN, Giummarra MJ, Georgiou-Karistianis N, Bradshaw JL (January 2012). "Mirror-sensory synaesthesia: exploring 'shared' sensory experiences as synaesthesia". Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews (Review). 36 (1): 645–657. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.09.006. PMID 21986634. S2CID 10536190.
  • Luke DP, Terhune DB (October 2013). "The induction of synaesthesia with chemical agents: a systematic review". Frontiers in Psychology (Review). 4: 753. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00753. PMC 3797969. PMID 24146659.
  • Mylopoulos MI, Ro T (October 2013). "Synesthesia: a colorful word with a touching sound?". Frontiers in Psychology (Review). 4: 763. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00763. PMC 3804765. PMID 24155733.
  • Price MC, Mattingley JB (May 2013). "Automaticity in sequence-space synaesthesia: a critical appraisal of the evidence" (PDF). Cortex; A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior (Review). 49 (5): 1165–1186. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2012.10.013. PMID 23237480. S2CID 8151536.
  • Rothen N, Meier B, Ward J (September 2012). "Enhanced memory ability: Insights from synaesthesia". Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews (Review). 36 (8): 1952–1963. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.05.004. PMID 22634573. S2CID 9541065.
  • Sinke C, Halpern JH, Zedler M, Neufeld J, Emrich HM, Passie T (September 2012). "Genuine and drug-induced synesthesia: a comparison". Consciousness and Cognition (Review). 21 (3): 1419–1434. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2012.03.009. PMID 22521474. S2CID 15807455.
  • Simner J (February 2012). "Defining synaesthesia" (PDF). British Journal of Psychology (Review). 103 (1): 1–15. doi:10.1348/000712610X528305. PMID 22229768. S2CID 9038571.
  • Suslick KS (December 2012). "Synesthesia in science and technology: more than making the unseen visible". Current Opinion in Chemical Biology (Review). 16 (5–6): 557–563. doi:10.1016/j.cbpa.2012.10.030. PMC 3606019. PMID 23183411.
  • Ward J (2013). "Synesthesia". Annual Review of Psychology (Review). 64: 49–75. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143840. PMID 22747246. S2CID 241155101.
  • Danis A. . Numberphile. Brady Haran. Archived from the original on 18 September 2015. Retrieved 6 April 2013.
  • Palmer S, Schloss KB (21 August 2015). "What's the Color of Your Favorite Song? – The Crux". Discover Magazine Blogs. Kalmbach Publishing Co.
  • "Synesthesia". Internet Encyclopedia of Philosophy.

External links Edit

synesthesia, other, uses, disambiguation, confused, with, synthesia, american, english, synaesthesia, british, english, perceptual, phenomenon, which, stimulation, sensory, cognitive, pathway, leads, involuntary, experiences, second, sensory, cognitive, pathwa. For other uses see Synesthesia disambiguation Not to be confused with Synthesia Synesthesia American English or synaesthesia British English is a perceptual phenomenon in which stimulation of one sensory or cognitive pathway leads to involuntary experiences in a second sensory or cognitive pathway 1 2 3 4 People who report a lifelong history of such experiences are known as synesthetes Awareness of synesthetic perceptions varies from person to person 5 In one common form of synesthesia known as grapheme color synesthesia or color graphemic synesthesia letters or numbers are perceived as inherently colored 6 7 In spatial sequence or number form synesthesia numbers months of the year or days of the week elicit precise locations in space e g 1980 may be farther away than 1990 or may appear as a three dimensional map clockwise or counterclockwise 8 9 Synesthetic associations can occur in any combination and any number of senses or cognitive pathways 10 SynesthesiaHow someone with synesthesia might perceive certain letters and numbers Most synesthetes see characters just as others do in whichever color actually displayed but may simultaneously perceive colors as associated with or evoked by each one SpecialtyPsychology Psychiatry NeurologyLittle is known about how synesthesia develops It has been suggested that synesthesia develops during childhood when children are intensively engaged with abstract concepts for the first time 11 This hypothesis referred to as semantic vacuum hypothesis could explain why the most common forms of synesthesia are grapheme color spatial sequence and number form These are usually the first abstract concepts that educational systems require children to learn The earliest recorded case of synesthesia is attributed to the Oxford University academic and philosopher John Locke who in 1690 made a report about a blind man who said he experienced the color scarlet when he heard the sound of a trumpet 12 However there is disagreement as to whether Locke described an actual instance of synesthesia or was using a metaphor 13 The first medical account came from German physician Georg Tobias Ludwig Sachs in 1812 13 14 15 The term is from the Ancient Greek syn syn together and aἴs8hsis aisthesis sensation 12 Contents 1 Types 1 1 Grapheme color synesthesia 1 2 Chromesthesia 1 3 Spatial sequence synesthesia 1 4 Number form 1 5 Auditory tactile synesthesia 1 6 Ordinal linguistic personification 1 7 Misophonia 1 8 Mirror touch synesthesia 1 9 Lexical gustatory synesthesia 1 10 Kinesthetic synesthesia 1 11 Other forms 2 Signs and symptoms 3 Mechanism 3 1 Genetics 4 Prevalence 5 History 6 Society and culture 6 1 Notable cases 6 2 Art 6 2 1 Writers 6 2 2 Painters and photographers 6 2 3 Composers 6 2 4 Musicians 6 2 5 Artists without synesthesia 6 3 Science 6 4 Literature 7 Research 7 1 Technological applications 7 1 1 The Voice vOICe 8 See also 9 References 10 Further reading 11 External linksTypes EditThere are two overall forms of synesthesia projective synesthesia seeing colors forms or shapes when stimulated the widely understood version of synesthesia associative synesthesia feeling a very strong and involuntary connection between the stimulus and the sense that it triggersFor example in chromesthesia sound to color a projector may hear a trumpet and see an orange triangle in space while an associator might hear a trumpet and think very strongly that it sounds orange citation needed Synesthesia can occur between nearly any two senses or perceptual modes and at least one synesthete Solomon Shereshevsky experienced synesthesia that linked all five senses 16 Types of synesthesia are indicated by using the notation x y where x is the inducer or trigger experience and y is the concurrent or additional experience For example perceiving letters and numbers collectively called graphemes as colored would be indicated as grapheme color synesthesia Similarly when synesthetes see colors and movement as a result of hearing musical tones it would be indicated as tone color movement synesthesia While nearly every logically possible combination of experiences can occur several types are more common than others Grapheme color synesthesia Edit Main article Grapheme color synesthesia From the 2009 non fiction book Wednesday Is Indigo BlueIn one of the most common forms of synesthesia individual letters of the alphabet and numbers collectively referred to as graphemes are shaded or tinged with a color While different individuals usually do not report the same colors for all letters and numbers studies with large numbers of synesthetes find some commonalities across letters e g A is likely to be red 17 Some authors had argued that the term synaesthesia may not be correct when applied to the so called grapheme colour synesthesia and similar phenomena in which the inducer is conceptual e g a letter or number rather than sensory e g sound or color They have postulated that the term ideasthesia is a more accurate description 18 19 Chromesthesia Edit Main article Chromesthesia Another common form of synesthesia is the association of sounds with colors For some everyday sounds can trigger seeing colors For others colors are triggered when musical notes or keys are being played People with synesthesia related to music may also have perfect pitch because their ability to see and hear colors aids them in identifying notes or keys 20 The colors triggered by certain sounds and any other synesthetic visual experiences are referred to as photisms According to Richard Cytowic 3 chromesthesia is something like fireworks voice music and assorted environmental sounds such as clattering dishes or dog barks trigger color and firework shapes that arise move around and then fade when the sound ends Sound often changes the perceived hue brightness scintillation and directional movement Some individuals see music on a screen in front of their faces For Deni Simon music produces waving lines like oscilloscope configurations lines moving in color often metallic with height width and most importantly depth My favorite music has lines that extend horizontally beyond the screen area Individuals rarely agree on what color a given sound is Composers Franz Liszt and Nikolai Rimsky Korsakov famously disagreed on the colors of musical keys Spatial sequence synesthesia Edit Those with spatial sequence synesthesia SSS tend to see ordinal sequences as points in space People with SSS may have superior memories in one study they were able to recall past events and memories far better and in far greater detail than those without the condition They can also see months or dates in the space around them but most synesthetes see these sequences in their mind s eye Some people see time like a clock above and around them 21 22 23 Number form Edit Main article Number form A number form from one of Francis Galton s subjects 1881 8 Note how the first 4 digits roughly correspond to their positions on a clock face A number form is a mental map of numbers that automatically and involuntarily appear whenever someone who experiences number forms synesthesia thinks of numbers These numbers might appear in different locations and the mapping changes and varies between individuals Number forms were first documented and named in 1881 by Francis Galton in The Visions of Sane Persons 24 Auditory tactile synesthesia Edit In auditory tactile synesthesia certain sounds can induce sensations in parts of the body For example someone with auditory tactile synesthesia may experience that hearing a specific word or sound feels like touch in one specific part of the body or may experience that certain sounds can create a sensation in the skin without being touched not to be confused with the milder general reaction known as frisson which affects approximately 50 of the population It is one of the least common forms of synesthesia 25 Ordinal linguistic personification Edit Main article Ordinal linguistic personification Ordinal linguistic personification OLP or personification is a form of synesthesia in which ordered sequences such as ordinal numbers week day names months and alphabetical letters are associated with personalities or genders Simner amp Hubbard 2006 Although this form of synesthesia was documented as early as the 1890s 26 27 researchers have until recently paid little attention to it see History of synesthesia research This form of synesthesia was named OLP in the contemporary literature by Julia Simner and colleagues 28 although it is now also widely recognised by the term sequence personality synesthesia Ordinal linguistic personification normally co occurs with other forms of synesthesia such as grapheme color synesthesia Misophonia Edit Main article Misophonia Misophonia is a neurological disorder in which negative experiences anger fright hatred disgust are triggered by specific sounds Cytowic suggests that misophonia is related to or perhaps a variety of synesthesia 1 Edelstein and her colleagues have compared misophonia to synesthesia in terms of connectivity between different brain regions as well as specific symptoms 1 They formed the hypothesis that a pathological distortion of connections between the auditory cortex and limbic structures could cause a form of sound emotion synesthesia 29 Studies suggest that individuals with misophonia have a normal hearing sensitivity level but the limbic system and autonomic nervous system are constantly in a heightened state of arousal where abnormal reactions to sounds will be more prevalent 30 Newer studies suggest that depending on its severity misophonia could be associated with lower cognitive control when individuals are exposed to certain associations and triggers 31 It is unclear what causes misophonia Some scientists believe it could be genetic others believe it to be present with other additional conditions however there is not enough evidence to conclude what causes it 32 There are no current treatments for the condition but could be managed with different types of coping strategies 32 These strategies vary from person to person some have reported the avoidance of certain situations that could trigger the reaction mimicking the sounds cancelling out the sounds by using different methods like earplugs music internal dialog and many other tactics Most misophonics use these to overwrite these sounds produced by others 33 Mirror touch synesthesia Edit Main article Mirror touch synesthesia This is a form of synesthesia where individuals feel the same sensation that another person feels such as touch For instance when such a synesthete observes someone being tapped on their shoulder the synesthete involuntarily feels a tap on their own shoulder as well People with this type of synesthesia have been shown to have higher empathy levels compared to the general population This may be related to the so called mirror neurons present in the motor areas of the brain which have also been linked to empathy 34 Lexical gustatory synesthesia Edit Main article Lexical gustatory synesthesia This is another form of synesthesia where certain tastes are experienced when hearing words For example the word basketball might taste like waffles The documentary Derek Tastes of Earwax gets its name from this phenomenon in references to pub owner James Wannerton who experiences this particular sensation whenever he hears the name spoken 35 36 It is estimated that 0 2 of the synesthesia population has this form of synesthesia making it one of the rarest forms 37 Kinesthetic synesthesia Edit Kinesthetic synesthesia is one of the rarest documented forms of synesthesia in the world 38 This form of synesthesia is a combination of various different types of synesthesia Features appear similar to auditory tactile synesthesia but sensations are not isolated to individual numbers or letters but complex systems of relationships The result is the ability to memorize and model complex relationships between numerous variables by feeling physical sensations around the kinesthetic movement of related variables Reports include feeling sensations in the hands or feet coupled with visualizations of shapes or objects when analyzing mathematical equations physical systems or music In another case a person described seeing interactions between physical shapes causing sensations in the feet when solving a math problem Generally those with this type of synesthesia can memorize and visualize complicated systems and with a high degree of accuracy predict the results of changes to the system Examples include predicting the results of computer simulations in subjects such as quantum mechanics or fluid dynamics when results are not naturally intuitive 17 39 Other forms Edit Other forms of synesthesia have been reported but little has been done to analyze them scientifically There are at least 80 types of synesthesia 38 In August 2017 a research article in the journal Social Neuroscience reviewed studies with fMRI to determine if persons who experience autonomous sensory meridian response are experiencing a form of synesthesia While a determination has not yet been made there is anecdotal evidence that this may be the case based on significant and consistent differences from the control group in terms of functional connectivity within neural pathways It is unclear whether this will lead to ASMR being included as a form of existing synesthesia or if a new type will be considered 40 Signs and symptoms EditSome synesthetes often report that they were unaware their experiences were unusual until they realized other people did not have them while others report feeling as if they had been keeping a secret their entire lives 41 The automatic and ineffable nature of a synesthetic experience means that the pairing may not seem out of the ordinary This involuntary and consistent nature helps define synesthesia as a real experience Most synesthetes report that their experiences are pleasant or neutral although in rare cases synesthetes report that their experiences can lead to a degree of sensory overload 17 Though often stereotyped in the popular media as a medical condition or neurological aberration citation needed many synesthetes themselves do not perceive their synesthetic experiences as a handicap On the contrary some report it as a gift an additional hidden sense something they would not want to miss Most synesthetes become aware of their distinctive mode of perception in their childhood Some have learned how to apply their ability in daily life and work Synesthetes have used their abilities in memorization of names and telephone numbers mental arithmetic and more complex creative activities like producing visual art music and theater 41 Despite the commonalities which permit the definition of the broad phenomenon of synesthesia individual experiences vary in numerous ways This variability was first noticed early in synesthesia research 42 Some synesthetes report that vowels are more strongly colored while for others consonants are more strongly colored 17 Self reports interviews and autobiographical notes by synesthetes demonstrate a great degree of variety in types of synesthesia the intensity of synesthetic perceptions awareness of the perceptual discrepancies between synesthetes and non synesthetes and the ways synesthesia is used in work creative processes and daily life 41 43 Synesthetes are very likely to participate in creative activities 39 It has been suggested that individual development of perceptual and cognitive skills in addition to one s cultural environment produces the variety in awareness and practical use of synesthetic phenomena 5 43 Synesthesia may also give a memory advantage In one study conducted by Julia Simner of the University of Edinburgh it was found that spatial sequence synesthetes have a built in and automatic mnemonic reference Whereas a non synesthete will need to create a mnemonic device to remember a sequence like dates in a diary a synesthete can simply reference their spatial visualizations 44 Mechanism EditMain article Neural basis of synesthesia Regions thought to be cross activated in grapheme color synesthesia green grapheme recognition area red V4 color area 45 As of 2015 the neurological correlates of synesthesia had not been established 46 Dedicated regions of the brain are specialized for given functions Increased cross talk between regions specialized for different functions may account for the many types of synesthesia For example the additive experience of seeing color when looking at graphemes might be due to cross activation of the grapheme recognition area and the color area called V4 see figure 45 This is supported by the fact that grapheme color synesthetes can identify the color of a grapheme in their peripheral vision even when they cannot consciously identify the shape of the grapheme 45 An alternative possibility is disinhibited feedback or a reduction in the amount of inhibition along normally existing feedback pathways 47 Normally excitation and inhibition are balanced However if normal feedback were not inhibited as usual then signals feeding back from late stages of multi sensory processing might influence earlier stages such that tones could activate vision Cytowic and Eagleman find support for the disinhibition idea in the so called acquired forms 3 of synesthesia that occur in non synesthetes under certain conditions temporal lobe epilepsy 48 head trauma stroke and brain tumors They also note that it can likewise occur during stages of meditation deep concentration sensory deprivation or with use of psychedelics such as LSD or mescaline and even in some cases marijuana 3 However synesthetes report that common stimulants like caffeine and cigarettes do not affect the strength of their synesthesia nor does alcohol 3 137 40 A very different theoretical approach to synesthesia is that based on ideasthesia According to this account synesthesia is a phenomenon mediated by the extraction of the meaning of the inducing stimulus Thus synesthesia may be fundamentally a semantic phenomenon Therefore to understand neural mechanisms of synesthesia the mechanisms of semantics and the extraction of meaning need to be understood better This is a non trivial issue because it is not only a question of a location in the brain at which meaning is processed but pertains also to the question of understanding epitomized in e g the Chinese room problem Thus the question of the neural basis of synesthesia is deeply entrenched into the general mind body problem and the problem of the explanatory gap 49 Genetics Edit Main article Genetics of synesthesia Due to the prevalence of synesthesia among the first degree relatives of people affected 50 there may be a genetic basis as indicated by the monozygotic twins studies showing an epigenetic component medical citation needed Synesthesia might also be an oligogenic condition with locus heterogeneity multiple forms of inheritance and continuous variation in gene expression medical citation needed While the exact genetic loci for this trait haven t been identified research indicates that the genetic constructs underlying synesthesia are most likely more complex than the simple X linked mode of inheritance that early researchers believed it to be 7 Further it remains uncertain as to whether synesthesia perseveres in the genetic pool because it provides a selective advantage or because it has become a byproduct of some other useful selected trait 51 Women have a higher chance of developing synesthesia as demonstrated in population studies conducted in the city of Cambridge England where females were 6 times more likely to have it 50 As technological equipment continues to advance the search for clearer answers regarding the genetics behind synesthesia will become more promising Although often termed a neurological condition synesthesia is not listed in either the DSM IV or the ICD since it usually does not interfere with normal daily functioning 52 Indeed most synesthetes report that their experiences are neutral or even pleasant 17 Like perfect pitch synesthesia is simply a difference in perceptual experience Reaction times for answers that are congruent with a synesthete s automatic colors are shorter than those whose answers are incongruent 3 The simplest approach is test retest reliability over long periods of time using stimuli of color names color chips or a computer screen color picker providing 16 7 million choices Synesthetes consistently score around 90 on the reliability of associations even with years between tests 1 In contrast non synesthetes score just 30 40 even with only a few weeks between tests and a warning that they would be retested 1 Many tests exist for synesthesia Each common type has a specific test When testing for grapheme color synesthesia a visual test is given The person is shown a picture that includes black letters and numbers A synesthete will associate the letters and numbers with a specific color An auditory test is another way to test for synesthesia A sound is turned on and one will either identify it with a taste or envision shapes The audio test correlates with chromesthesia sounds with colors Since people question whether or not synesthesia is tied to memory the retest is given One is given a set of objects and is asked to assign colors tastes personalities or more After a period of time the same objects are presented and the person is asked again to do the same task The synesthete can assign the same characteristics because that person has permanent neural associations in the brain rather than memories of a certain object medical citation needed The automaticity of synesthetic experience A synesthete might perceive the left panel like the panel on the right 45 Grapheme color synesthetes as a group share significant preferences for the color of each letter e g A tends to be red O tends to be white or black S tends to be yellow etc 17 Nonetheless there is a great variety in types of synesthesia and within each type individuals report differing triggers for their sensations and differing intensities of experiences This variety means that defining synesthesia in an individual is difficult and the majority of synesthetes are completely unaware that their experiences have a name 17 Neurologist Richard Cytowic identifies the following diagnostic criteria for synesthesia in his first edition book However the criteria are different in the second book 1 2 3 Synesthesia is involuntary and automatic Synesthetic perceptions are spatially extended meaning they often have a sense of location For example synesthetes speak of looking at or going to a particular place to attend to the experience Synesthetic percepts are consistent and generic i e simple rather than pictorial Synesthesia is highly memorable Synesthesia is laden with affect Cytowic s early cases mainly included individuals whose synesthesia was frankly projected outside the body e g on a screen in front of one s face Later research showed that such stark externalization occurs in a minority of synesthetes Refining this concept Cytowic and Eagleman differentiated between localizers and non localizers to distinguish those synesthetes whose perceptions have a definite sense of spatial quality from those whose perceptions do not 3 Prevalence EditEstimates of prevalence of synesthesia have ranged widely from 1 in 4 to 1 in 25 000 100 000 However most studies have relied on synesthetes reporting themselves introducing self referral bias 53 In what is cited as the most accurate prevalence study so far 53 self referral bias was avoided by studying 500 people recruited from the communities of Edinburgh and Glasgow Universities it showed a prevalence of 4 4 with 9 different variations of synesthesia 54 This study also concluded that one common form of synesthesia grapheme color synesthesia colored letters and numbers is found in more than one percent of the population and this latter prevalence of graphemes color synesthesia has since been independently verified in a sample of nearly 3 000 people in the University of Edinburgh 55 The most common forms of synesthesia are those that trigger colors and the most prevalent of all is day color 54 Also relatively common is grapheme color synesthesia We can think of prevalence both in terms of how common is synesthesia or different forms of synesthesia within the population or how common are different forms of synesthesia within synesthetes So within synesthetes forms of synesthesia that trigger color also appear to be the most common forms of synesthesia with a prevalence rate of 86 within synesthetes 54 In another study music color is also prevalent at 18 41 citation needed Some of the rarest are reported to be auditory tactile mirror touch and lexical gustatory 56 There is research to suggest that the likelihood of having synesthesia is greater in people with autism spectrum condition 57 History EditMain article History of synesthesia research The interest in colored hearing dates back to Greek antiquity when philosophers asked if the color chroia what we now call timbre of music was a quantifiable quality 58 Isaac Newton proposed that musical tones and color tones shared common frequencies as did Goethe in his book Theory of Colours 59 There is a long history of building color organs such as the clavier a lumieres on which to perform colored music in concert halls 60 61 The first medical description of colored hearing is in an 1812 thesis by the German physician Georg Tobias Ludwig Sachs 62 13 14 The father of psychophysics Gustav Fechner reported the first empirical survey of colored letter photisms among 73 synesthetes in 1876 63 64 followed in the 1880s by Francis Galton 8 65 66 Carl Jung refers to color hearing in his Symbols of Transformation in 1912 67 In the early 1920s the Bauhaus teacher and musician Gertrud Grunow researched the relationships between sound color and movement and developed a twelve tone circle of colour which was analogous with the twelve tone music of the Austrian composer Arnold Schonberg 1874 1951 68 She was a participant in at least one of the Congresses for Colour Sound Research German Kongress fur Farbe Ton Forschung held in Hamburg in the late 1920s and early 1930s 69 Research into synesthesia proceeded briskly in several countries but due to the difficulties in measuring subjective experiences and the rise of behaviorism which made the study of any subjective experience taboo synesthesia faded into scientific oblivion between 1930 and 1980 citation needed As the 1980s cognitive revolution made inquiry into internal subjective states respectable again scientists returned to synesthesia Led in the United States by Larry Marks and Richard Cytowic and later in England by Simon Baron Cohen and Jeffrey Gray researchers explored the reality consistency and frequency of synesthetic experiences In the late 1990s the focus settled on grapheme color synesthesia one of the most common 17 and easily studied types Psychologists and neuroscientists study synesthesia not only for its inherent appeal but also for the insights it may give into cognitive and perceptual processes that occur in synesthetes and non synesthetes alike Synesthesia is now the topic of scientific books and papers Ph D theses documentary films and even novels citation needed Since the rise of the Internet in the 1990s synesthetes began contacting one another and creating websites devoted to the condition These rapidly grew into international organizations such as the American Synesthesia Association the UK Synaesthesia Association the Belgian Synesthesia Association the Canadian Synesthesia Association the German Synesthesia Association and the Netherlands Synesthesia Web Community citation needed Society and culture EditNotable cases Edit Main article List of people with synesthesia Solomon Shereshevsky a newspaper reporter turned mnemonist was discovered by Russian neuropsychologist Alexander Luria to have a rare fivefold form of synesthesia 16 of which he is the only known case Words and text were not only associated with highly vivid visuospatial imagery but also sound taste color and sensation 16 Shereshevsky could recount endless details of many things without form from lists of names to decades old conversations but he had great difficulty grasping abstract concepts The automatic and nearly permanent retention of every detail due to synesthesia greatly inhibited Shereshevsky s ability to understand what he read or heard 16 Neuroscientist and author V S Ramachandran studied the case of a grapheme color synesthete who was also color blind While he couldn t see certain colors with his eyes he could still see those colors when looking at certain letters Because he didn t have a name for those colors he called them Martian colors 70 Art Edit Main article Synesthesia in art Other notable synesthetes come particularly from artistic professions and backgrounds Synesthetic art historically refers to multi sensory experiments in the genres of visual music music visualization audiovisual art abstract film and intermedia 41 71 72 73 74 75 Distinct from neuroscience the concept of synesthesia in the arts is regarded as the simultaneous perception of multiple stimuli in one gestalt experience 76 Neurological synesthesia has been a source of inspiration for artists composers poets novelists and digital artists Writers Edit Vladimir Nabokov wrote explicitly about synesthesia in several novels Nabokov described his grapheme color synesthesia at length in his autobiography Speak Memory 77 I present a fine case of colored hearing Perhaps hearing is not quite accurate since the color sensations seem to be produced by the very act of my orally forming a given letter while I imagine its outline The long a of the English alphabet and it is this alphabet I have in mind farther on unless otherwise stated has for me the tint of weathered wood but the French a evokes polished ebony This black group also includes hard g vulcanized rubber and r a sooty rag being ripped Oatmeal n noodle limp l and the ivory backed hand mirror of o take care of the whites I am puzzled by my French on which I see as the brimming tension surface of alcohol in a small glass Passing on to the blue group there is steely x thundercloud z and huckleberry k Since a subtle interaction exists between sound and shape I see q as browner than k while s is not the light blue of c but a curious mixture of azure and mother of pearl Daniel Tammet wrote a book on his experiences with synesthesia called Born on a Blue Day 78 Joanne Harris author of Chocolat is a synesthete who says she experiences colors as scents 79 Her novel Blueeyedboy features various aspects of synesthesia Painters and photographers Edit Wassily Kandinsky a synesthete and Piet Mondrian not a synesthete both experimented with image music congruence in their paintings Contemporary artists with synesthesia such as Carol Steen 80 and Marcia Smilack 81 a photographer who waits until she gets a synesthetic response from what she sees and then takes the picture use their synesthesia to create their artwork Linda Anderson according to NPR considered one of the foremost living memory painters creates with oil crayons on fine grain sandpaper representations of the auditory visual synaesthesia she experiences during severe migraine attacks 82 83 Brandy Gale a Canadian visual artist experiences an involuntary joining or crossing of any of her senses hearing vision taste touch smell and movement Gale paints from life rather than from photographs and by exploring the sensory panorama of each locale attempts to capture select and transmit these personal experiences 84 85 86 David Hockney perceives music as color shape and configuration and uses these perceptions when painting opera stage sets though not while creating his other artworks Kandinsky combined four senses color hearing touch and smell 1 3 Composers Edit The music of his Symphonic Poem The Sea eventually skip to 0 45 seconds and the matching painting Sonata of the Sea Finale 1908 by synestheet Lithuanian painter composer and writer Mikalojus Konstantinas Ciurlionis Multiple composers had experienced synesthesia Mikalojus Konstantinas Ciurlionis a Lithuanian painter composer and writer perceived colors and music simultaneously Many of his paintings bear the names of matching musical pieces sonatas fugues and preludes Alexander Scriabin composed colored music that was deliberately contrived and based on the circle of fifths whereas Olivier Messiaen invented a new method of composition the modes of limited transposition specifically to render his bi directional sound color synesthesia 3 87 For example the red rocks of Bryce Canyon are depicted in his symphony Des canyons aux etoiles From the Canyons to the Stars New art movements such as literary symbolism non figurative art and visual music have profited from experiments with synesthetic perception and contributed to the public awareness of synesthetic and multi sensory ways of perceiving 41 Other composers who reported synesthesia include Duke Ellington 88 Nikolay Rimsky Korsakov 89 and Jean Sibelius 72 Several contemporary composers with a synesthesia are Michael Torke 72 and Ramin Djawadi best known for his work on composing the theme songs and scores for such TV series as Game of Thrones Westworld and for the Iron Man movie He says he tends to associate colors with music or music with colors 90 British composer Daniel Liam Glyn created the classical contemporary music project Changing Stations using Grapheme Colour Synaesthesia Based on the 11 main lines of the London Underground the eleven tracks featured on the album represent the eleven main tube line colours 91 Each track focuses heavily on the different speeds sounds and mood of each line and are composed in the key signature synaesthetically assigned by Glyn with reference to the colour of the tube line on the map 92 Musicians Edit The producer rapper and fashion designer Kanye West is a prominent interdisciplinary case In an impromptu speech he gave during an Ellen interview he described his condition saying that he sees sounds and that everything he sonically makes is a painting 93 Other notable synesthetes include musicians Billy Joel 94 89 91 Andy Partridge 95 Itzhak Perlman 94 53 Lorde 96 Billie Eilish 97 Brendon Urie 98 99 Ida Maria 100 Brian Chase 101 102 and classical pianist Helene Grimaud Musician Kristin Hersh sees music in colors 103 Drummer Mickey Hart of The Grateful Dead wrote about his experiences with synaesthesia in his autobiography Drumming at the Edge of Magic 104 John Frusciante guitarist of the Red Hot Chili Peppers talks about his experiences with synesthesia in a podcast with Rick Rubin 105 Pharrell Williams of the groups The Neptunes and N E R D also experiences synesthesia 106 107 and used it as the basis of the album Seeing Sounds Singer songwriter Marina and the Diamonds experiences music color synesthesia and reports colored days of the week 108 Awsten Knight from Waterparks has chromesthesia which influences many of the band s artistic choices 109 Artists without synesthesia Edit Some artists frequently mentioned as synesthetes did not in fact have the neurological condition Scriabin s 1911 Prometheus for example is a deliberate contrivance whose color choices are based on the circle of fifths and appear to have been taken from Madame Blavatsky 3 110 The musical score has a separate staff marked luce whose notes are played on a color organ Technical reviews appear in period volumes of Scientific American 3 On the other hand his older colleague Rimsky Korsakov who was perceived as a fairly conservative composer was in fact a synesthete 111 French poets Arthur Rimbaud and Charles Baudelaire wrote of synesthetic experiences but there is no evidence they were synesthetes themselves Baudelaire s 1857 Correspondances introduced the notion that the senses can and should intermingle Baudelaire participated in a hashish experiment by psychiatrist Jacques Joseph Moreau and became interested in how the senses might affect each other 41 Rimbaud later wrote Voyelles 1871 which was perhaps more important than Correspondances in popularizing synesthesia He later boasted J inventais la couleur des voyelles I invented the colors of the vowels 112 Science Edit Some technologists like inventor Nikola Tesla 113 and scientists also reported being synesthetic Physicist Richard Feynman describes his colored equations in his autobiography What Do You Care What Other People Think 114 When I see equations I see the letters in colors I don t know why I see vague pictures of Bessel functions with light tan j s slightly violet bluish n s and dark brown x s flying around 115 Literature Edit Main articles Synesthesia in literature and Synesthesia in fiction Synesthesia is sometimes used as a plot device or way of developing a character s inner life Author and synesthete Pat Duffy describes four ways in which synesthetic characters have been used in modern fiction 116 117 Synesthesia as Romantic ideal in which the condition illustrates the Romantic ideal of transcending one s experience of the world Books in this category include The Gift by Vladimir Nabokov Synesthesia as pathology in which the trait is pathological Books in this category include The Whole World Over by Julia Glass Synesthesia as Romantic pathology in which synesthesia is pathological but also provides an avenue to the Romantic ideal of transcending quotidian experience Books in this category include Holly Payne s The Sound of Blue and Anna Ferrara s The Woman Who Tried To Be Normal Synesthesia as psychological health and balance Painting Ruby Tuesday by Jane Yardley and A Mango Shaped Space by Wendy Mass Literary depictions of synesthesia are criticized as often being more of a reflection of an author s interpretation of synesthesia than of the phenomenon itself citation needed Research Edit Tests like this demonstrate that people do not attach sounds to visual shapes arbitrarily When people are given a choice between the words Bouba and Kiki the left shape is almost always called Kiki while the right is called Bouba Research on synesthesia raises questions about how the brain combines information from different sensory modalities referred to as crossmodal perception or multisensory integration citation needed An example of this is the bouba kiki effect In an experiment first designed by Wolfgang Kohler people are asked to choose which of two shapes is named bouba and which kiki The angular shape kiki is chosen by 95 98 and bouba for the rounded one Individuals on the island of Tenerife showed a similar preference between shapes called takete and maluma Even 2 5 year old children too young to read show this effect 118 Research indicated that in the background of this effect may operate a form of ideasthesia 119 Researchers hope that the study of synesthesia will provide better understanding of consciousness and its neural correlates In particular synesthesia might be relevant to the philosophical problem of qualia 4 120 given that synesthetes experience extra qualia e g colored sound An important insight for qualia research may come from the findings that synesthesia has the properties of ideasthesia 18 which then suggest a crucial role of conceptualization processes in generating qualia 11 Technological applications Edit Synesthesia also has a number of practical applications including intentional synesthesia in technology 121 and sensory prosthetics 122 The Voice vOICe Edit Peter Meijer developed a sensory substitution device for the visually impaired called The vOICe the capital letters O I and C in vOICe are intended to evoke the expression Oh I see The vOICe is a privately owned research project running without venture capital that was first implemented using low cost hardware in 1991 123 The vOICe is a visual to auditory sensory substitution device SSD preserving visual detail at high resolution up to 25 344 pixels 124 The device consists of a laptop head mounted camera or computer camera and headphones The vOICe converts visual stimuli of the surroundings captured by the camera into corresponding aural representations soundscapes delivered to the user through headphones at a default rate of one soundscape per second Each soundscape is a left to right scan with height represented by pitch and brightness by loudness 125 The vOICe compensates for the loss of vision by converting information from the lost sensory modality into stimuli in a remaining modality 126 See also EditAllochiria Apophenia Autonomous Sensory Meridian Response Fantasy prone personality Hallucination Ideasthesia Ideophone Vibration theory of olfaction Parosmia Psychedelic drug Sensory substitution Thought Forms Visual music The Yellow Sound McCollough effectReferences Edit a b c d e f g Cytowic RE 2002 Synesthesia A Union of the Senses 2nd ed Cambridge Massachusetts MIT Press ISBN 978 0 262 03296 4 OCLC 49395033 page needed a b Cytowic RE 2003 The Man Who Tasted Shapes Cambridge Massachusetts MIT Press ISBN 978 0 262 53255 6 OCLC 53186027 page needed a b c d e f g h i j k l Cytowic RE Eagleman DM 2009 Wednesday is Indigo Blue Discovering the Brain of Synesthesia with an afterword by Dmitri Nabokov Cambridge MIT Press ISBN 978 0 262 01279 9 page needed a b Harrison JE Baron Cohen S 1996 Synaesthesia classic and contemporary readings Oxford Blackwell Publishing ISBN 978 0 631 19764 5 OCLC 59664610 page needed a b van Campen C 2009 The Hidden Sense On Becoming Aware of Synesthesia PDF Teccogs 1 1 13 Archived from the original PDF on 8 July 2009 Rich AN Mattingley JB January 2002 Anomalous perception in synaesthesia a cognitive neuroscience perspective Nature Reviews Neuroscience Review 3 1 43 52 doi 10 1038 nrn702 PMID 11823804 S2CID 11477960 a b Hubbard EM Ramachandran VS November 2005 Neurocognitive mechanisms of synesthesia Neuron Review 48 3 509 520 doi 10 1016 j neuron 2005 10 012 PMID 16269367 S2CID 18730779 a b c Galton F 1880 Visualized Numerals Nature 21 543 494 5 Bibcode 1880Natur 21 494G doi 10 1038 021494e0 S2CID 4074444 Seron X Pesenti M Noel MP Deloche G Cornet JA August 1992 Images of numbers or When 98 is upper left and 6 sky blue Cognition 44 1 2 159 196 doi 10 1016 0010 0277 92 90053 K PMID 1511585 S2CID 26687757 How Synesthesia Works HowStuffWorks 1 January 1970 Retrieved 2 May 2016 a b Mroczko Wasowicz A Nikolic D 2014 Semantic mechanisms may be responsible for developing synesthesia Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 8 509 doi 10 3389 fnhum 2014 00509 PMC 4137691 PMID 25191239 a b Ward Ossian 10 June 2006 The man who heard his paint box hiss The Telegraph Retrieved 3 December 2018 a b c Jewanski J Day SA Ward J July 2009 A colorful albino the first documented case of synaesthesia by Georg Tobias Ludwig Sachs in 1812 Journal of the History of the Neurosciences 18 3 293 303 doi 10 1080 09647040802431946 PMID 20183209 S2CID 8641750 a b Herman LM 28 December 2018 Synesthesia Encyclopaedia Britannica Retrieved 25 January 2019 Konnikova M 26 February 2013 From the words of an albino a brilliant blend of color Scientific American Blog Network Archived from the original on 20 September 2016 Retrieved 25 January 2019 a b c d Schacter D 1996 Searching for Memory The Brain The Mind And The Past Basic Books pp 81 ISBN 978 0 465 07552 2 a b c d e f g h Sagiv N Robertson LC 2005 Synesthesia perspectives from cognitive neuroscience Oxford Oxford University Press ISBN 978 0 19 516623 1 OCLC 53020292 page needed a b Nikolic D 2009 Is synaesthesia actually ideaesthesia An inquiry into the nature of the phenomenon PDF Proceedings of the Third International Congress on Synaesthesia Science amp Art Granada Spain April 26 29 Simner J February 2012 Defining synaesthesia PDF British Journal of Psychology Review 103 1 1 15 doi 10 1348 000712610X528305 PMID 22229768 S2CID 9038571 Absolute Pitch and Synesthesia The Feinstein Institute for Medical Research The Feinstein Institute for Medical Research Retrieved 2 May 2016 Jonas CN Price MC 30 October 2014 Not all synesthetes are alike spatial vs visual dimensions of sequence space synesthesia Frontiers in Psychology 5 1171 doi 10 3389 fpsyg 2014 01171 PMC 4214186 PMID 25400596 unreliable medical source Do sequence space synaesthetes have better spatial imagery skills Maybe not The National Center for Biotechnology Information unreliable source A Mind That Touches the Past Sciencemag org Seaton M 2013 The Visions of Sane Persons Ploughshares 39 1 144 145 doi 10 1353 plo 2013 0012 ISSN 2162 0903 S2CID 161837195 Naumer MJ van den Bosch JJ July 2009 Touching sounds thalamocortical plasticity and the neural basis of multisensory integration PDF Journal of Neurophysiology 102 1 7 8 doi 10 1152 jn 00209 2009 PMID 19403745 S2CID 1712367 Archived from the original PDF on 20 February 2019 Flournoy 2001 Calkins 1893harvnb error no target CITEREFCalkins1893 help Simner J Hubbard EM December 2006 Variants of synesthesia interact in cognitive tasks evidence for implicit associations and late connectivity in cross talk theories Neuroscience 143 3 805 814 doi 10 1016 j neuroscience 2006 08 018 PMID 16996695 S2CID 18922608 Edelstein M Brang D Rouw R Ramachandran VS 2013 Misophonia physiological investigations and case descriptions Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7 296 doi 10 3389 fnhum 2013 00296 PMC 3691507 PMID 23805089 Schroder A van Diepen R Mazaheri A Petropoulos Petalas D Soto de Amesti V Vulink N Denys D 9 April 2014 Diminished n1 auditory evoked potentials to oddball stimuli in misophonia patients Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 8 123 doi 10 3389 fnbeh 2014 00123 PMC 3988356 PMID 24782731 Daniels EC Rodriguez A Zabelina DL 16 January 2020 Langguth B ed Severity of misophonia symptoms is associated with worse cognitive control when exposed to misophonia trigger sounds PLOS ONE 15 1 e0227118 Bibcode 2020PLoSO 1527118D doi 10 1371 journal pone 0227118 PMC 6964854 PMID 31945068 a b Duddy D Oeding K 29 April 2014 Misophonia An Overview Seminars in Hearing 35 2 084 091 doi 10 1055 s 0034 1372525 ISSN 0734 0451 S2CID 76090599 Edelstein M Brang D Rouw R Ramachandran VS 2013 Misophonia physiological investigations and case descriptions Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7 296 doi 10 3389 fnhum 2013 00296 PMC 3691507 PMID 23805089 Heyes C March 2010 Where do mirror neurons come from Neuroscience amp Biobehavioral Reviews 34 4 575 583 doi 10 1016 j neubiorev 2009 11 007 PMID 19914284 S2CID 2578537 Derek Tastes of Ear Wax Top Documentary Films Retrieved 2 February 2015 BBC Science amp Nature Horizon bbc co uk Retrieved 2 February 2015 Simner J 2009 Lexical Gustatory Synesthesia Encyclopedia of Neuroscience Springer Berlin Heidelberg pp 2149 2152 doi 10 1007 978 3 540 29678 2 2766 ISBN 978 3 540 23735 8 a b Types of Syn DaySyn com Retrieved 26 January 2016 a b Dailey A Martindale C Borkum J 1997 Creativity synesthesia and physiognomic perception Creativity Research Journal 10 1 1 8 doi 10 1207 s15326934crj1001 1 Smith SD Katherine Fredborg B Kornelsen J August 2017 An examination of the default mode network in individuals with autonomous sensory meridian response ASMR Social Neuroscience 12 4 361 365 doi 10 1080 17470919 2016 1188851 PMID 27196787 a b c d e f van Campen C 2007 The Hidden Sense Synesthesia in Art and Science Cambridge Massachusetts MIT Press ISBN 978 0 262 22081 1 OCLC 80179991 page needed Flournoy T 2001 Des phenomenes de synopsie Audition coloree Adamant Media Corporation ISBN 978 0 543 94462 7 page needed a b full citation needed Dittmar A ed 2007 Synasthesien Roter Faden durchs Leben Synesthesia A red thread through life in German Essen Verlag Die Blaue Eule Investigating Spatial Sequence Synesthesia Synesthesia Test 26 June 2012 Retrieved 2 May 2016 a b c d non primary source needed dead link Ramachandran VS Hubbard EM 2001 Synaesthesia A window into perception thought and language PDF Journal of Consciousness Studies 8 12 3 34 Archived from the original PDF on 27 May 2006 Hupe JM Dojat M 2015 A critical review of the neuroimaging literature on synesthesia Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 9 103 doi 10 3389 fnhum 2015 00103 PMC 4379872 PMID 25873873 Grossenbacher PG Lovelace CT January 2001 Mechanisms of synesthesia cognitive and physiological constraints Trends in Cognitive Sciences 5 1 36 41 doi 10 1016 S1364 6613 00 01571 0 PMID 11164734 S2CID 15092606 Neckar M Bob P 11 January 2016 Synesthetic associations and psychosensory symptoms of temporal epilepsy Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment 12 109 112 doi 10 2147 NDT S95464 PMC 4714732 PMID 26811683 Gray JA Parslow DM Brammer MJ Chopping S Vythelingum GN ffytche DH February 2006 Evidence against functionalism from neuroimaging of the alien colour effect in synaesthesia Cortex A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior 42 2 309 318 doi 10 1016 s0010 9452 08 70357 5 PMID 16683506 S2CID 4475077 a b Baron Cohen S Burt L Smith Laittan F Harrison J Bolton P 1 September 1996 Synaesthesia prevalence and familiality Perception 25 9 1073 1079 doi 10 1068 p251073 PMID 8983047 S2CID 25954158 Brang D Ramachandran VS November 2011 Survival of the synesthesia gene why do people hear colors and taste words PLOS Biology 9 11 e1001205 doi 10 1371 journal pbio 1001205 PMC 3222625 PMID 22131906 Hubbard EM June 2007 Neurophysiology of synesthesia PDF Current Psychiatry Reports 9 3 193 199 doi 10 1007 s11920 007 0018 6 PMID 17521514 S2CID 13370073 a b Simner J Hubbard EM eds 2013 A brief history of synesthesia research Oxford Handbook of Synesthesia Oxford UK Oxford University Press pp 13 17 ISBN 978 0 19 960332 9 Retrieved 25 February 2018 a b c Simner J Mulvenna C Sagiv N Tsakanikos E Witherby SA Fraser C et al 2006 Synaesthesia the prevalence of atypical cross modal experiences PDF Perception 35 8 1024 1033 doi 10 1068 p5469 PMID 17076063 S2CID 2508540 Carmichael DA Down MP Shillcock RC Eagleman DM Simner J May 2015 Validating a standardised test battery for synesthesia Does the Synesthesia Battery reliably detect synesthesia Consciousness and Cognition 33 375 385 doi 10 1016 j concog 2015 02 001 PMC 5047354 PMID 25734257 Safran AB Sanda N February 2015 Color synesthesia Insight into perception emotion and consciousness Current Opinion in Neurology 28 1 36 44 doi 10 1097 WCO 0000000000000169 PMC 4286234 PMID 25545055 Baron Cohen S Johnson D Asher J Wheelwright S Fisher SE Gregerson PK Allison C Is synaesthesia more common in autism Molecular Autism 20 November 2013 Gage J 1993 Colour and Culture Practice and Meaning from Antiquity to Abstraction London Thames amp Hudson page needed Goethe JW 1840 Theory of Colours J Murray Peacock K 1988 Instruments to Perform Color Music Two Centuries of Technological Experimentation PDF Leonardo 21 4 397 406 doi 10 2307 1578702 JSTOR 1578702 S2CID 54178977 Jewanski J Sidler N eds 2006 Farbe Licht Musik Synaesthesie und Farblichtmusik Bern Peter Lang page needed Mahling F 1926 Das Problem der audition coloree Eine historisch kritische Untersuchung Archiv fur die gesamte Psychologie 57 165 301 Fechner G 1876 Vorschule der Aesthetik Leipzig Breitkopf und Hartel von Campen C 1996 De verwarring der zintuigen Artistieke en psychologische experimenten met synesthesie Psychologie amp Maatschappij 20 1 10 26 Galton F 1880 Visualized Numerals Nature 21 533 252 6 Bibcode 1880Natur 21 252G doi 10 1038 021252a0 Galton F 1883 Inquiries into Human Faculty and Its Development Macmillan Retrieved 17 June 2008 page needed Jung CG 1956 1912 The Transformation of Libido Symbols of Transformation London Routledge amp Kegan Paul p 237 Bauhaus100 Curriculum Harmonisation theory 1919 1924 Retrieved 2 December 2018 Farbe Ton Forschungen III Band Bericht uber den II Kongress fur Farbe Ton Forschung Hamburg 1 5 Oktober 1930 Published 1931 Carroll S 5 November 2007 Martian Colors Cosmic Variance Discover Retrieved 12 April 2018 Campen C 1999 Artistic and psychological experiments with synesthesia Leonardo 32 1 9 14 doi 10 1162 002409499552948 S2CID 57568389 a b c Berman G 1999 Synesthesia and the Arts Leonardo 32 1 15 22 doi 10 1162 002409499552957 S2CID 57565862 von Maur K 1999 The Sound of Painting Music in Modern Art Pegasus Library Munich Prestel ISBN 978 3 7913 2082 3 page needed Gage JD 1993 Colour and culture practice and meaning from antiquity to abstraction London Thames and Hudson ISBN 978 0 500 27818 5 page needed Gage JD 1999 Color and meaning art science and symbolism Berkeley University of California Press ISBN 978 0 520 22611 1 page needed van Campen C 2009 Visual Music and Musical Paintings The Quest for Synesthesia in the Arts In Bacci F Melcher D eds Making Sense of Art making Art of Sense Oxford Oxford University Press page needed Bouchet Marie Loison Charles Julie Poulin Isabelle 19 June 2020 The Five Senses in Nabokov s Works Springer Nature pp 255 256 ISBN 978 3 030 45406 7 Tammet D 2007 Born on a Blue Day Free Press ISBN 978 1 4165 3507 2 page needed Chocolat author Joanne Harris talks about her latest novel Blue Eyed Boy Metro 7 April 2010 Steen C 2001 Visions Shared A Firsthand Look into Synesthesia and Art Leonardo 34 3 203 208 doi 10 1162 002409401750286949 S2CID 57570552 Marcia Smilack Website Accessed 20 August 2006 Linda Anderson MOCA GA 7 April 2019 Archived from the original on 7 April 2019 Retrieved 27 December 2021 Linda Anderson NPR 7 April 2019 Archived from the original on 7 April 2019 Retrieved 27 December 2021 Coastal Synaesthesia Paintings and Photographs of Hawaii Fiji and California by Brandy Gale Gualala Arts Center exhibit January 2015 gualalaarts org Archived from the original on 2 February 2015 Retrieved 2 February 2015 EG The Wondrous Sensory Spectrum of Brandy Gale FORA tv Archived from the original on 2 February 2015 Retrieved 2 February 2015 brandygale wbr com Samuel C 1994 1986 Olivier Messiaen Music and Color Conversations with Claude Samuel Translated by Glasow ET Portland Oregon Amadeus Press Duke Ellington as quoted in George D 1981 Sweet man The real Duke Ellington New York G P Putnam s Sons p 226 Yastrebtsev V 1908 On N A Rimsky Korsakov s color sound contemplation Russkaya Muzykalnaya Gazeta in Russian 39 40 842 845 cited by Bulat Galeyev 1999 Meet the musical genius behind the Game of Thrones soundtrack who watches each season before anyone else Business Insider Retrieved 11 January 2018 Sounds of the Underground Synaesthetic Musician Creates LP Based on Tube Map The Big Issue 27 April 2017 Retrieved 15 May 2018 Do all London Underground lines have a unique sound PDF Norwegian Air Magazine Retrieved 3 April 2017 Kanye West FULL Banned Ellen Interview HD May 19 2016 retrieved 20 August 2022 a b Seaberg M 2011 Tasting the Universe New Page Books ISBN 978 1 60163 159 6 Enos Morgan 9 April 2020 Chords Become Colors Inside Synesthesia Tidal Retrieved 11 May 2023 Trendell A 11 May 2017 Lorde explains the experience of having synaesthesia NME Retrieved 15 June 2021 Nattress K Billie Eilish Explains How Synesthesia Affects Her Music iHeartRadio Retrieved 15 June 2021 Spanos B 15 January 2016 Panic at the Disco Band Is Outlet for Nonchalant Chaos Rolling Stone Retrieved 13 November 2019 4 Singers Who Draw Inspiration From Synesthesia To Write Music Cultura Colectiva 23 January 2018 Retrieved 16 April 2020 Cairns D 24 February 2008 Times Online interview The Times London Retrieved 24 July 2008 Forrest E 30 March 2009 Emma Forrest meets New York s favourite art punk rockers Yeah Yeah Yeahs guardian co uk London The Guardian Retrieved 7 May 2009 Chase B Brian Chase s blog yeahyeahyeahs com Archived from the original on 25 January 2009 Retrieved 7 May 2009 Seaman D 8 May 2021 Strange Angels Kristin Hersh On Music amp Motherhood The Quietus Retrieved 8 November 2021 Hart M Stevens J Lieberman F 1990 Drumming at the edge of magic a journey into the spirit of percussion San Francisco CA Harper p 133 John Frusciante of the Red Hot Chili Peppers Returns Part 1 Broken Record Hosted by Rick Rubin retrieved 20 May 2023 unreliable source It just always stuck out in my mind and I could always see it I don t know if that makes sense but I could always visualize what I was hearing Yeah it was always like weird colors From a Nightline interview with Pharrell Synesthetes People of the Future Psychology Today 3 March 2012 Retrieved 15 May 2014 Loose Women Marina and the Diamonds ITV Lifestyle ITV Network 27 April 2010 Retrieved 28 April 2010 Waterparks Awsten Can See Music So We Had Him Paint His Songs Seeing Sounds MTV retrieved 14 February 2023 page needed Dann KT 1998 Bright colors falsely seen synaesthesia and the search for transcendental knowledge New Haven Conn Yale University Press ISBN 978 0 300 06619 7 This is according to an article in the Russian press Yastrebtsev V On N A Rimsky Korsakov s color sound contemplation Russkaya muzykalnaya gazeta 1908 N 39 40 pp 842 845 in Russian cited by Bulat Galeyev 1999 Barth F Giampieri Deutsch P Hans Dieter K 2012 Sensory Perception Mind amp Matter Vienna Springer Vienna p 221 ISBN 978 3 211 99750 5 I invented the colours of the vowels Tesla N The Strange Life of Nikola Tesla PDF pitt edu Archived from the original PDF on 17 July 2012 Retrieved 4 September 2012 Feynman R 1988 What Do You Care What Other People Think New York Norton p 59 Colourful language U of T psychologists discover enhanced language learning in synesthetes University of Toronto News Retrieved 20 March 2023 Duffy PL 2006 Images of Synesthetes and their Perceptions of Language in Fiction 6th Annual Meeting of the American Synesthesia Association University of South Florida Archived from the original on 17 January 2012 Duffy PL Simner J February 2010 Synaesthesia in fiction Cortex A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior 46 2 277 278 doi 10 1016 j cortex 2008 11 003 PMID 19081086 S2CID 11224620 non primary source needed Maurer D Pathman T Mondloch CJ May 2006 The shape of boubas sound shape correspondences in toddlers and adults Developmental Science 9 3 316 322 doi 10 1111 j 1467 7687 2006 00495 x PMID 16669803 S2CID 7297731 Milan E Iborra O de Cordoba M Juarez Ramos V Artacho MR Rubio JL 2013 The Kiki Bouba effect A case of personification and ideaesthesia Journal of Consciousness Studies 20 1 2 84 102 Gray JA Chopping S Nunn J et al 2002 Implications of synaesthesia for functionalism Theory and experiments Journal of Consciousness 9 12 5 31 Suslick KS December 2012 Synesthesia in science and technology more than making the unseen visible Current Opinion in Chemical Biology 16 5 6 557 563 doi 10 1016 j cbpa 2012 10 030 PMC 3606019 PMID 23183411 Scheff CM 1 January 1986 Experimental model for the study of changes in the organization of human sensory information processing through the design and testing of non invasive prosthetic devices for sensory impaired people ACM SIGCAPH Computers and the Physically Handicapped 36 3 10 doi 10 1145 15711 15713 ISSN 0163 5727 S2CID 11924232 Meijer P Augmented Reality for the Totally Blind Retrieved 4 February 2014 Striem Amit E Guendelman M Amedi A 16 March 2012 Visual acuity of the congenitally blind using visual to auditory sensory substitution PLOS ONE 7 3 e33136 Bibcode 2012PLoSO 733136S doi 10 1371 journal pone 0033136 PMC 3306374 PMID 22438894 Carmichael J 10 July 2013 Device Trains Blind People To See By Listening Retrieved 4 February 2014 Haigh A Brown DJ Meijer P Proulx MJ 2013 How well do you see what you hear The acuity of visual to auditory sensory substitution Frontiers in Psychology 4 330 doi 10 3389 fpsyg 2013 00330 PMC 3684791 PMID 23785345 Further reading Editde Broucker T April 2013 Synaesthesia an augmented sensory world phenomenology and literature review Revue Neurologique Review in French 169 4 328 334 doi 10 1016 j neurol 2012 09 016 PMID 23434143 Cohen Kadosh R Terhune DB February 2012 Redefining synaesthesia PDF British Journal of Psychology Review 103 1 20 23 doi 10 1111 j 2044 8295 2010 02003 x PMID 22229770 De Cordoba MJ Ricco D Day S July 2014 Synaesthesia Theoretical artistic and scientific foundations Granada Spain p 372 ISBN 978 84 939054 9 1 a href Template Cite book html title Template Cite book cite book a CS1 maint location missing publisher link Dael N Sierro G Mohr C October 2013 Affect related synesthesias a prospective view on their existence expression and underlying mechanisms Frontiers in Psychology Review 4 754 doi 10 3389 fpsyg 2013 00754 PMC 3798864 PMID 24151478 Fitzgibbon BM Enticott PG Rich AN Giummarra MJ Georgiou Karistianis N Bradshaw JL January 2012 Mirror sensory synaesthesia exploring shared sensory experiences as synaesthesia Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews Review 36 1 645 657 doi 10 1016 j neubiorev 2011 09 006 PMID 21986634 S2CID 10536190 Luke DP Terhune DB October 2013 The induction of synaesthesia with chemical agents a systematic review Frontiers in Psychology Review 4 753 doi 10 3389 fpsyg 2013 00753 PMC 3797969 PMID 24146659 Mylopoulos MI Ro T October 2013 Synesthesia a colorful word with a touching sound Frontiers in Psychology Review 4 763 doi 10 3389 fpsyg 2013 00763 PMC 3804765 PMID 24155733 Price MC Mattingley JB May 2013 Automaticity in sequence space synaesthesia a critical appraisal of the evidence PDF Cortex A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior Review 49 5 1165 1186 doi 10 1016 j cortex 2012 10 013 PMID 23237480 S2CID 8151536 Rothen N Meier B Ward J September 2012 Enhanced memory ability Insights from synaesthesia Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews Review 36 8 1952 1963 doi 10 1016 j neubiorev 2012 05 004 PMID 22634573 S2CID 9541065 Sinke C Halpern JH Zedler M Neufeld J Emrich HM Passie T September 2012 Genuine and drug induced synesthesia a comparison Consciousness and Cognition Review 21 3 1419 1434 doi 10 1016 j concog 2012 03 009 PMID 22521474 S2CID 15807455 Simner J February 2012 Defining synaesthesia PDF British Journal of Psychology Review 103 1 1 15 doi 10 1348 000712610X528305 PMID 22229768 S2CID 9038571 Suslick KS December 2012 Synesthesia in science and technology more than making the unseen visible Current Opinion in Chemical Biology Review 16 5 6 557 563 doi 10 1016 j cbpa 2012 10 030 PMC 3606019 PMID 23183411 Ward J 2013 Synesthesia Annual Review of Psychology Review 64 49 75 doi 10 1146 annurev psych 113011 143840 PMID 22747246 S2CID 241155101 Danis A Grapheme colour synesthesia Numberphile Brady Haran Archived from the original on 18 September 2015 Retrieved 6 April 2013 Palmer S Schloss KB 21 August 2015 What s the Color of Your Favorite Song The Crux Discover Magazine Blogs Kalmbach Publishing Co Synesthesia Internet Encyclopedia of Philosophy Wikimedia Commons has media related to Synesthesia External links Edit Look up synaesthesia in Wiktionary the free dictionary Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Synesthesia amp oldid 1172312472, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.