fbpx
Wikipedia

Primate archaeology

Primate archaeology is a field of research established in 2008 that combines research interests and foci from primatology and archaeology. The main aim of primate archaeology is to study behavior of extant and extinct primates and the associated material records. The discipline attempts to move beyond archaeology's anthropocentric perspective by placing the focus on both past and present primate tool use.[1]

Primate archaeology is characterized by the combination of archaeological and primatological methods, and researchers consider both non-human primate tools and their behaviour in tandem.[2] Primate archaeology has the unique opportunity to observe the tool-use behaviors of extant non-human primates and the formation of the material record that emerges from that behavior. It is this ability to observe behavior and the subsequent material deposition resulting in a material record recoverable using standard archaeological field methods that gives this new field of research the possibility of reconstructing, predicting, and interpreting extant primates' tool use spatial patterns.[3] Overall, primate archaeology helps researchers understand how early hominins used material culture, what these patterns reveal about ancient hominin cognition, as well as patterns of landscape use that could allow researchers to identify behaviors that are difficult to detect archaeologically.[1][2][3] Primate archaeology's main focus is on the study of the non-human primates that have been observed using tools in the wild: chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), robust capuchins (Sapajus spp.) and long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis aurea). Since its conception primate archaeology has also implemented the use of captive studies[4] akin to archaeological experiments with non-human primates looking into stone tool manufacture.[5][6][7]

Origins edit

The establishment of primate archaeology as an official discipline took place around 2008[8] and 2009.[1] However, the two-fold nature of this relatively new subject area can be traced back to some major events in the development of both archaeology and primatology.

Archaeology edit

The search for the earliest evidence of material culture in the archaeological record has been one of archaeology's major aims almost since its inception as a discipline. With the classification of stone tools as human-made artifacts and not fossils, towards the end of the seventeenth century, stone tools soon became key evidence for arranging the archaeological record into sequential cultural phases.[9] The discovery of Eoliths, which were thought to be flint tools in the Oligocene deposits of Thenay in France led to the establishment of the Eolithic culture and Homopithecus bourgeoisi.[9][10] The Eolithic tools were described as being directly used in their natural state or being poorly worked. Although some influential scholars tried to discredit the Eolith hypothesis,[11] the discovery of the Piltdown man in 1912 only helped to reinforce its credibility because the fossil remains were found in association with Eolithic tools.[12] Both the Eolithic culture and Piltdown man remained controversial until they were finally discredited around the 1950s.[9]

 
OH 7, Homo habilis
 
Example of Oldowan stone tools

Disregarding the Eolithic controversy, the earliest stone tools (also found in the European continent) were the Chellean being followed by the Acheulean.[9] Thus, before the twentieth century, most archaeologists believed that Europe was where hominins began to use and manufacture stone tools. However, this narrative began to change when in 1931 Louis Leakey discovered in Olduvai Gorge stone tools that seemed to be older than those found in Europe.[13] Soon after, Leakey formally named this discovery the Oldowan industry.[14] Decades later, Oldupai was the focus of two more major findings. First, in 1959 a skull identified as Zinjanthropus (now Paranthropus) boisei was found near Oldowan stone tools, and this led to the argument that this hominin could be a direct ancestor of modern humans.[15] However, in 1964 a new fossil (OH 7) was also found in association with the aforementioned stone tools.[16] The connection of this hominin with Oldowan stone tools and the fact that it seemed to have a larger brain than the australopiths led to its classification as Homo habilis (meaning "the handyman") and the assumption that stone tools were a distinctive feature of the genus Homo.[16] The idea that the use of (stone) tools, and thus culture, was a unique characteristic of the genus Homo was not empirically challenged until 2010 and 2015 with the discovery of what could be the earliest archaeological evidence of cut-marked bones[17] and stone tools[18] respectively. However, the classification of H. habilis as part of the genus Homo became controversial soon after its discovery due to the use of cultural traits to justify hominin taxonomy.[19] Recently, the idea that human ancestors have been responsible for intentionally making stone tools has also been put into question through primate archaeology research.[20]

Towards the end of the twentieth-century, archaeology shifted from a focus on typological classifications towards an interest in understanding the mechanisms behind the origins and subsequent evolution of stone tools. This conceptual turn led to the initial establishment of experiments bringing archaeology and primatology together. Inspired by Jane Goodall's report of chimpanzee tool use,[21] in 1972 Wright decided to teach a captive orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus) named Abang to make Oldowan flakes by striking a flint core to then use the flakes to open a box through imitative learning and some hand molding.[22] Abang was capable of making and using the flakes only after multiple trials and not very efficiently, yet it has been suggested that this inefficiency could be the result of his upbringing in captivity.[23] A couple of decades later, Kathy Schick and Nicholas Toth developed a similar experiment initially with a single captive, enculturated bonobo (Pan paniscus) named Kanzi to see whether he could copy Oldowan stone tool making.[5] In subsequent experiments, more enculturatd bonobos started to make and use stone tools after seeing Kanzi.[6] The skills of these bonobos were said to be rudimentary, and even though they were taught to use freehand knapping (one stone core is held in one hand and another stone in the other hand) their preferred method to make flakes involved throwing the stone core against a hard surface.[5] In 1994, Westergaard and Suomi decided to redo the Abang and Kanzi type of experiments but this time they ran a baseline study (no demonstrations) with a group of unenculturated monkeys - capuchins (Sapajus [Cebus] apella). These capuchins had no exposure to stone tool making prior or during the study, and they were unenculturated - and so anystone tool use they would show should therefore be considered spontaneous.[24] In this experiment, the unenculturated capuchins spontaneously began to produce flakes by striking stones against hard surfaces. Like Kanzi, they did not use the freehand knapping technique normally associated with the Oldowan. Researchers did not attempt further experiments involving non-human primates trying to make and use stone tools until around 2020 - when further baseline studies were run, this time with a focus on the performance of unenculturated apes. In this way, orangutans got tested,[25] as did chimpanzees [26] and gorillas.[27] These experiments combined the use of baselines with that of demonstration conditions.

Primatology edit

 
Japanese macaque (Macaca fuscata)

Although there are prior examples of studies looking into non-human primates across Western countries,[28] the modern study of primate behavior originated in Japan.[29] The first international journal in primatology, Primates, was created in Japan in 1957.[30] Japanese primatology was established through the observation of free-ranging Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata) by Jun'ichiro Itani, Kinji Imanishi and Shunzo Kawamura.[31] The main aim of these first primatologists was to understand the social structure of the macaques groups they were observing.[30] Following their prior expertise in the individual identification of wild horses, they began to give nicknames to each monkey.[32] Furthermore, to habituate the monkeys to their presence, they began to give them food while they established a long-term observation method.[32] A few years later, in one of the Japanese macaque sites, a young female, later nicknamed "Imo", was observed washing a sweet potato in a small stream.[32] After this instance, Kawamura began more systematic observations of this behavior discovering the propagation of sweet-potato washing among other members of the group.[32] As a result of the sweet-potato washing phenomenon, Japanese primatologists started to draw the first direct associations of the terms culture and subculture in association with non-human animals.[33] Another major contribution of Japanese primatology took place with the foundation in 1978 of the ape-language project (later named Ai project) at the Primate Research Institute of Kyoto University with the collaboration of Kiyoko Murofushi, Toshio Asano and Tetsuro Matsuzawa.[31] Although the Ai project had a focus on language acquisition their methods emphasized the cognitive capabilities more than the communicative skills of the monkeys. Through projects of this kind, Japanese primatology highlighted the importance of merging both field and experimental settings for the study of primate behavior and cognition.[31]

Turning to primatology in other areas of the globe, the contribution of Louis Leakey in the configuration of contemporary primatology is also notable.[editorializing] Even though Louis Leakey was not a primatologist (he was an archaeologist), he believed in the importance of studying non-human primates to better understand human evolution. Thus, it was Louis Leakey who encouraged Jane Goodall, Dian Fossey, and Birutė Galdikas to study chimpanzee, gorilla and orangutan socio-ecology and behavior.[34] These three women are seen as pioneers in the establishment of very long-term primatological sites where the habituation of the primates through trust became very important.[35] Their work continues to leave an impact on the way non-human primate sites are managed and how their behavior is seen as a model to past study human behavior.[35]

The publication of Andrew Whiten and colleagues' paper about chimpanzee cultures reinforced the prior conversations about culture in non-human primates that began during the 1950s and 1960s.[36] However, the question as to how to define culture is a focus of debate across primatologists and other researchers such as anthropologists and archaeologists even today. The main disagreement is whether only humans can have culture, or if it is possible to see cultural traits in non-human animals.[37] Overall, most definitions of a culture accepting its existence amongst non-human primates agree that culture includes behaviors that can vary in their expression between social groups, are maintained across generations, and are socially transmitted between the members of the same group.[38] However, the opponents to this perspective believe that there are different types of culture, and that - among primates - humans are unique in their ability to accumulate cultural traits over time.[39]

Official birth edit

On top of the experimental work with non-human primates towards the end of the twentieth century, archaeologists began to observe chimpanzees in the wild to try to understand their behaviour and possible spatial patterns present in their living spaces.[40] However, the real breakthrough[according to whom?] took place with the discovery of Panda 100, the first excavated chimpanzee nut-cracking archaeological site located in the Taï Forest, Ivory Coast and dated to around 4000 years ago.[41] This site finally reinforced the idea that the combination of primatological and archaeological methods could shed light on both contemporary and past primate (human and non-human) behavior. Researchers realized that chimpanzees, and probably other non-human primates too, could create long-lasting patterns of material evidence that could be linked to their current behavior while they are creating that material record.[8]

After the publication of Carvalho, Cunha, Sousa and Matsuzawa in 2008[8] of what is considered one of the first studies merging archaeology and ethology from an evolutionary perspective,[3] primate archaeology began to formalize. Discussions about primate archaeology developing into a discipline became more concrete in 2009 at the Paleoanthropology meets Primatology conference.[42] It was this conference that led to the publication of the first article encouraging the official establishment of primate archaeology as a research field.[1] After this first publication, studies and discoveries in primate archaeology started to grow exponentially.[43][4]

Tool-use in non-human primates edit

 
Locations of wild non-human primates using tools: (A) white-faced capuchins (Cebus capucinus); (B) bearded capuchins (Sapajus libidinosus); (C) West African chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes); (D) Burmese long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis aurea).
 
Bonobo (sister taxon of the chimpanzee) doing termite fishing.

The earliest written report of tool use in non-human primates dates to 1844 and it describes chimpanzees cracking nuts.[44] In The Descent of Man, Charles Darwin used this initial report to mention the possible existence of culture and art in non-human primates.[45] Furthermore, in 1887 Carpenter observed long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis aurea) pounding oysters using stones.[46] After these anecdotal reports, most of the early accounts of non-human primates’ tool use came from the work of psychologists with captive subjects.[47] Most of this work took place in Europe during the early twentieth century with examples such as the Anthropoid Station in Tenerife (Spain) dedicated to experimental research with large prosimians.[28] Nonetheless, the most well-known example of the initial recognition of tool use in our closest relatives is Jane Goodall's report on chimpanzee termite fishing.[21]

When it comes to reconstructing past hominin behaviors, the use of non-human primates engaging in similar activities as the ones in the archaeological record is considered by some researchers as a viable methodology.[48][8][49][50]

However, there are still some skeptics that question the use of extant non-human primates as analogies of hominin tool use behaviors.[51][52] This scepticism is based on anatomical differences between extant and extinct primates, the possible convergent evolution of similar behaviors, the diversity of past and present environments, and the amount of time that has gone by since the last common ancestor of different non-human primates and humans.[53] However, there is a mid-ground to this disagreement, which involves discerning which parts of different non-human primate species could be used as valid evolutionary models.[3][53] Overall, the consensus points towards the use of multi-species comparative primate models given that different taxa provide distinct benefits when it comes to understanding early hominins.[3][53]

For example, a recent[when?] primate archaeology example of the use of non-human primates as viable models is the consolidation of the "by-product hypothesis" for the origins of stone tools.[54] The observation of chimpanzees causing the detachment of fractured stones from stone anvils as a consequence of missed hits[55] led some researchers to propose the idea that early hominins could have found themselves in similar situations and saw the mis-hits as opportunities to directly use the by-products or as examples of how to make flaked stone tools.[56] However, the "by-product hypothesis" has gained more attention after primate archaeology field experiments demonstrated that nut-cracking behavior can facilitate the production of reasonable numbers of sharp-edged flakes.[54]

Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) edit

 
Chimpanzee at the Bossou forest.

Chimpanzees are the most widely studied non-human primates both in the wild[8] and in captivity[4] particularly when it comes to tool use. After more than a century of intensive observation, researchers began to see the broad range of behavioral variations present across chimpanzee populations. During the late 1990s, chimpanzees were thought to have 39 behavioral variants (not all associated with tool use) across six habituated African populations (Bossou, Taï forest, Kibale, Gombe, Budongo, Mahale M-group, Mahale K-group).[36] Some of these behavioral variants were only present in certain groups whereas others were shared amongst multiple groups independent of ecological factors. These findings served as the foundation for the argument of culture in chimpanzees, and thus in other non-human primates.[36]

 
Representation of chimpanzee nut-cracking.

Some of the most well-known tool use examples in chimpanzees include ant dipping, wood boring, honey fishing, leaf sponging, and nut-cracking.[57] However, chimpanzees also use tools for accessing the bone marrow of other normally smaller non-human primates,[57] and even for medicinal purposes by swallowing leaves.[58] Chimpanzees are also capable of using two or more tools in a sequential order to achieve an end goal such as termite fishing or obtaining honey.[59] Moreover, chimpanzees can create compound tools in which two or more individual implements are combined to form a single working tool.[59]

Chimpanzees' use of stone tools in foraging activities is extensive, yet the most well-known example is nut-cracking. Although some chimpanzees in the Ebo Forest of Cameroon have been indirectly observed cracking nuts,[60] it is generally assumed[by whom?] that chimpanzee nut-cracking in the wild is restricted to West Africa.[49] The reasons behind this phenomenon are not clear. Researchers have suggested that this could be the result of cultural transmission,[48] more than ecological pressures[61] given that both nut-cracking and the absence of it can be seen in closely located chimpanzee populations we can. Moreover, nut-cracking techniques can vary between chimpanzee populations with some groups like those in Bossou using stones as anvils[62] and groups like those in the Taï forest using tree roots.[63] Likewise, nut-cracking efficiency also differs across groups.[64] Chimpanzees can actively choose different stone types depending on the distance they had to travel to the selected anvil (heavier stones the shorter the distance), or the type of anvil they were going to use showing an ability to anticipate future sequential events.[65]

Robust capuchins (genus Sapajus) edit

Capuchin stone tool use in the wild has been known at least since the eighteenth century,[66] yet studies focusing on wild capuchin stone tool use became more common after the systematic observation of this behavior from the early 2000s.[67][68] Prior to these observations in the wild, capuchin stone tool use was thought to only occur in captivity.[69] Moreover, even though there has been indirect evidence of capuchins using stones for foraging in rainforest habitats,[70][71] pounding behaviors such as nut-cracking seem to be associated with groups that spend most of their time on the ground or in savanna-like environments.[69][72][73] Tool use in this taxon is mostly limited to wild robust (genus Sapajus) capuchins with the exception of one instance in which white-faced capuchins (Cebus capucinus imitator) have been observed using stones to forage coastal resources on the Coiba National Park, Panama.[74][75] Traditionally capuchins have been assigned to a single genus (Cebus). However, from 2012[76] there has been a growing consensus on separating capuchins species between robust (genus Sapajus[77]) and gracile (genus Cebus) mostly based on morphological and behavioral differences.,[78][79] yet this classification still remains controversial[80]

 
Bearded capuchin nut-cracking

There are four species of robust capuchins that have been observed using tools: S. apella, S. libidinosus, S. xanthosternos, and S. nigritus.[69] For example, indirect evidence of several nut-cracking sites of S. xanthosternos has been reported in the State of Bahia, Brazil[81] However, tool use is more frequently observed amongst populations of S. apella[67][82][83][7] and S. libidinosus. In fact, most accounts of tool use in wild capuchin populations involve S. libidinosus,[84][85][86] commonly known as black-stripped capuchins. This species of robust capuchins are found in the savanna-like environments of the Brazilian caatinga and cerrado.[69] Even though black-stripped capuchins at the Serra da Capivara National Park have been observed using stones to cut wood for insects and to dig for tubers.,[87][88][89] the most widely studied tool use behavior in black-stripped capuchins is the use of stones to crack open encapsulated foods[89] Capuchins are capable of actively selecting different stones depending on the type of encased foods they want to open.[90] Moreover, this type of tool use takes multiple years of learning and it can involve not just conspecifics but also other members of the social group who are the most efficient at nut-cracking.[91] Overall, the costs and benefits of cracking open encased foods using stones for capuchins vary across individuals.[92] However, capuchins tend to be very efficient in this percussive behavior even though they can use stones that weight half their body mass.[93]

Black-stripped capuchins at the Serra da Capivara National Park have also been observed engaging in "stone on stone" percussion which consists on striking a stone against a conglomerate.[94] This behavior allows the capuchins to unintentionally break stones in a way that allows them to produce sharp-edged flakes that show similar traits to those observed in the Oldowan.[94] The purpose of this "stone on stone" behavior is unclear, but it has been suggested that the capuchins consume the minerals that result from breaking the stones.[94]

Long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis aurea) edit

 
Long-tailed macaque tool use

Although tool use has also been reported in other macaque species (e.g., Macaca fuscata)[95][96] primate archaeological research has focused on Thai long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis aurea)[20][97][98] given that stone tool use in this species is associated with foraging. After Carpenter's mention of long-tailed macaques doing oyster-cracking,[46] there was virtually no mention of this type of behavior in the literature associated with this taxon. However, in 2007 this foraging behavior was observed in two long-tailed macaque populations inhabiting the Piak Nam Yai Island, Laem Son National Park in southern Thailand.[99] It was this re-discovery of stone tool use for foraging that sparked the exponential growth of systematic studies on macaque tool use.[97][100][101] A few years later, long-tailed macaques were also observed using stones to crack open oil palm nuts.[102][103] Recent primate archaeology studies have discovered that long-tailed macaques choose different stone tool shapes based on individual preference and the different types of food they want to open.[101][104] This prompted researchers to propose different terminology depending on the type of stone tools the macaques use. They normally differentiate between “point/axe hammering” associated with a smaller size with pointy ends, “pound/face hammering” associated with larger and rounder stones, and “edge hammering” mostly related to the use of the edge of the stone tool and applied both for cracking oysters and molluscs.[105][106]

In long-tailed macaques, stone tool use seems to be restricted to the Macaca fascicularis aurea subspecies because Macaca fascicularis fascicularis has never been observed using tools in the wild or captivity.[99] Both subspecies share the same habitat, but they have developed different strategies to forage similar resources. It is still not clear why this happens, but it has been suggested that it could be the result of a genetic component because long-tail hybrids of both Macaca fascicularis aurea and Macaca fascicularis fascicularis with a stronger component of the former show a bias towards the use of stones.[107]

Other non-human primates edit

Although they have never been seen to undertake stone tool percussive activities in the wild, recent studies trying to comprehend the possible mechanisms behind the emergence of stone tool technology have started to run experiments with non-human primates in captivity, or in free-ranging environments. There seems to be a particular focus on great apes such as gorillas (Gorilla spp.), orangutans (Pongo spp.) and bonobos (Pan paniscus) due to their closer phylogenetic association with the genus Homo.[108] However, experimental studies trying to understand the emergence of stone tool technology still show a clear bias towards bonobos and orangutans given that gorillas are yet to be seen to use stones as tools both in the wild and in captivity.[53]

 
Bonobo (Pan paniscus)

Wild orangutans have never been seen using stone tools, but orangutans in captivity have been the focus of experimental studies testing their ability to use stone tools. For example, captive naïve orangutans have been reported to spontaneously use wooden hammers to crack open nuts.[109] Following a similar pattern to Kanzi's experiments, experiments with captive orangutans demonstrated that when presented with the right learning stimulus this great ape can engage in stone tool use and manufacture.[25] However, the flakes made by the captive orangutans were low in number and did not resemble stone flakes made by early hominins.[citation needed]

Although bonobos have been observed using tools in association with thirteen different behaviors only one of them, leaf sponging for drinking water, has a foraging purpose.[110][111] Moreover, no stone tool use has been reported in wild bonobos. This contrasts with the forty-two different tool use behaviors observed in chimpanzees of which more than half are associated with foraging.[112] The reason why bonobos and chimpanzees are so distinct in their tool use behaviors is still not clear. One possibility is that there are very few long-term wild-habituated bonobo sites (LuiKotale, Lomako and Wamba[113]) which are very difficult to access.[114] Another possible explanation is that chimpanzees and bonobos have different motivations (social, ecological or innate) that lead them to different predispositions when it comes to using tools.[112][115] However, despite the absence of stone tool use in the wild, studies of bonobo tool use in captivity and free-ranging communities are becoming more common.[116][117]

Methods edit

Excavation and site formation processes edit

 
Serra da Capivara National Park.

In archaeology excavation is an essential element of the discipline because it allows archaeologists to trace when specific behaviors originated and their subsequent changes over time.[2] Through excavation, archaeologists can obtain data on the chronology, the spatial distribution of the artefacts and the stratigraphic and depositional contexts of the site allowing them to reconstruct the natural and cultural events.[118] that led to the formation of that particular site[2] Particularly, in primate archaeology the excavation of non-human primate tool sites has proved to be valuable for tracing back the history of nut-cracking sites and investigating the changes of this particular percussive behavior.[119][98][120] The first non-human primate archaeological site was found in 2002 in the Taï Forest, Ivory Coast, and it was associated with chimpanzee tool use dating back 4000 years.[41] After this, in 2016 a group of primate archaeologists using archaeological methods discovered a long-tailed macaque archaeological site mapping shellfish processing activities at Piak Nam Yai Island, southern Thailand.[98] Finally, another group of primate archaeologists discovered a capuchin nut-cracking site dated to around 2400 years ago at Serra da Capivara National Park, Brazil.[120]

Archaeological excavation techniques can be time-consuming and costly. Furthermore, excavations are very invasive given that once you excavate something you will not be able to put it back into its original state.[2] Primate archaeology has the advantage of being able to do present-day surface surveys because they study extant non-human primate behavior and their tools.[8] Surface surveys allow primate archaeologists to look into the spatial distribution of the tools used by present non-human primates; thus, providing useful models to reconstruct past site formation processes. For example, a recent landscape agent-based modelling of chimpanzee nut-cracking stone tool fragments showed that nut-cracking sites have the potential to form dense clusters of stone tools that scatter across the landscape.[121]

Use-wear and residue analysis edit

Use-wear is a technique that consists of identifying the different damage marks present on stone tool edges to understand how toolmakers used these implements. Before the expansion of major technological advances in archaeology, use-wear analyses were mostly done from a qualitative and macroscopic perspective.[122] However, with the inclusion of microscopes and 3D scanners, use-wear analyses began to be approached more from a microscopic and quantitative perspective.[123][124] Through use-wear analyses, primate archaeologists can design a protocol that can allow them to differentiate stones that have been used as tools from those that were not even if they cannot observe the primates' behavior.[2][123][43] Likewise, in some instances they are also capable of reconstructing the particular behavior the tool was used for. For example, through the use-wear analysis of percussive tools using 3D surface morphometrics, archaeologists discovered that different macaque behaviours leave specific traces on the stones.[104]

Residue analysis adds another dimension to use-wear studies by looking into residual evidence that is left on tool edges. Normally, tools are inspected using use-wear techniques and if relevant residues are found within traces they will be extracted and further analysed.[125] This method allows primate archaeologists to know the exact foods that non-human primates are processing with stones, even if they cannot observe the behavior. For example, a recent study looked into bearded capuchin monkey pounding tools from Serra da Capivara, Brazil. In this study, researchers found that traces on capuchin stone tools reflect behavioral differences.[126] The application of these techniques in primate archaeology allows researchers to begin to draw comparisons between the tools used by different non-human primate species,[104] and contrast those tools with the stones found in the archaeological record.[2][123]

Technological analysis edit

Lithic technological analysis entails the study of the different attributes present in stone tools such as flakes through measurements, qualitative observations and possible subsequent typological classifications.[127] With the added method of 3D geometric morphometrics, archaeologists can develop a more in-depth and quantitative analysis of the morphological variation present in stone tools.

The application of these methods in primate archaeology has generated studies that are capable of comparing the flakes unintentionally produced by non-human primate species with flakes associated with hominins in the archaeological record.[128] Chimpanzees, robust capuchins and long-tailed macaques use stone tools to crack encased foods, and even though they can be quite efficient they can still have mishits.[55] These mishits can end up unintentionally producing flakes that could resemble the flakes produced by hominins.[54] Thus, to test the extent of the differences and similarities between non-human primate and hominin flakes, primate archaeologists have used technological and 3D geometric morphometric analyses to compare capuchin quartzite "stone on stone" flakes[94] with flakes produced by a contemporary human knapper using freehand and passive hammer knapping.[128] Moreover, in this study primate archaeologists also compared the capuchin unintentional flakes with Oldowan flakes. Overall, these analyses concluded that most of the technological attributes present in the capuchin unintentional flakes are shared with the intentionally made flakes, yet some substantial differences (e.g., external platform angle) could help to differentiate these non-human primate flakes from those found in the archaeological record.[128] Likewise, technological analysis has been used to study the attributes of archaeological flakes found in the chimpanzee nut-cracking site of Panda 100 allowing primate archaeologists to create data that can be used to find more sites and also to better understand modern chimpanzees tool use.[129]

Implications for the study of human evolution edit

Most of the major contributions of primate archaeology are associated with the importance of finding the earliest stone tools.[20][128][54] The search for the origins of stone tool use and making has been one of the major aims of archaeology almost since it became a discipline.[9] Moreover, reconstructing the emergence of stone tool technology in human evolution goes hand in hand with questions in biological anthropology about the behavior and anatomical constraints of early hominins.[130][131] Through the analysis of present-day non-human primate stone tool use primate archaeology provides a unique opportunity to compare observed behaviors with the tools and the traces they leave behind.[1][3][2]

Primate archaeological research has been able to challenge the view that only stone tools could have played an essential role in shaping human evolution.[1][3] Organic tool use such as leaves and twigs in non-human primates[36] points towards the also likely existence on these types of tools in all hominins. Moreover, the occurrence of tool use and even the unintentional production of flakes in extant non-human primates questions the idea that only hominins were the sole creators of archaeological sites.[1][3] The discovery of chimpanzee,[41] macaque[98] and capuchin[120] archaeological sites further strengthen that perspective. The use of tools by this wide range of non-human primate taxa also provides data in the reconstruction of the different environmental, anatomical, behavioral and/or genetic factors that could lead to the emergence and subsequent evolution of stone tool manufacture and use across different species.[1][3]

The question of whether non-human animals have human-like culture has been the focus of debate in disciplines such as primatology for many decades.[38][132] Although the non-human animal culture controversy is far from being over,[133][37] primate archaeology has contributed to this debate with several recent studies. A primate archaeology study in 2015 suggested the existence of social learning and cumulative culture in chimpanzees given that they could not introduce nut-cracking independently.[134] To test the hypothesis of cumulative culture in chimpanzees, a recent study tested whether a group of chimpanzees at Seringbara (Nimba Mountains, Guinea) could independently re-invent nut-cracking.[135] This study concluded that chimpanzee nut-cracking requires social learning because the observed chimpanzees could not re-invent nut-cracking despite being in ecologically valid conditions.[135] However, these primate archaeology studies still find opposition from scholars that firmly believe in the absence of cumulative culture in non-human primates, who pointed out that the tested chimpanzees lacked a food interest in the provided nuts.[136]

Moreover, the involvement of primate archaeology in the non-human animal culture debate has been extended to questions on the existence or lack thereof of human-like culture in the early archaeological record.[132][137][138] Through the use of non-human primate tool-use behaviors, researchers try to reconstruct when, how, and why humans began to rely on high-fidelity cultural transmission. Previous experiments run with unenculturated capuchin monkeys[24] and orangutans[25] which found spontaneous reinnovation of stone-tool making inspired the possibility that early stone tools such as the Oldowan, and even the Acheulean, may not have been know-how requiring social learning - thus, they may not have been early examples of cumulative culture.[139] The possible re-innovation of stone tool making was tested in a recent experiment with naïve humans who were never exposed to stone tool knapping know-how.[140] Overall, most subjects of this experiment were able to independently make stone tools - using all four known early knapping techniques. Nonetheless, the debate as to whether early stone tools reflect cumulative culture or not is still not settled.[132][133][139][135][136][37]

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ a b c d e f g h Haslam, Michael; Hernandez-Aguilar, Adriana; Ling, Victoria; Carvalho, Susana; de la Torre, Ignacio; DeStefano, April; Du, Andrew; Hardy, Bruce; Harris, Jack; Marchant, Linda; Matsuzawa, Tetsuro; McGrew, William; Mercader, Julio; Mora, Rafael; Petraglia, Michael (2009). "Primate archaeology". Nature. 460 (7253): 339–344. Bibcode:2009Natur.460..339H. doi:10.1038/nature08188. ISSN 0028-0836. PMID 19606139. S2CID 30108964.
  2. ^ a b c d e f g h Carvalho, S. and Almeida-Warren, K., (2019). Primate archaeology. In Encyclopedia of animal behavior. Academic Press, 397–407.
  3. ^ a b c d e f g h i Haslam, Michael; Hernandez-Aguilar, R. Adriana; Proffitt, Tomos; Arroyo, Adrian; Falótico, Tiago; Fragaszy, Dorothy; Gumert, Michael; Harris, John W. K.; Huffman, Michael A.; Kalan, Ammie K.; Malaivijitnond, Suchinda; Matsuzawa, Tetsuro; McGrew, William; Ottoni, Eduardo B.; Pascual-Garrido, Alejandra (2017). "Primate archaeology evolves". Nature Ecology & Evolution. 1 (10): 1431–1437. doi:10.1038/s41559-017-0286-4. ISSN 2397-334X. PMID 29185525. S2CID 256708334.
  4. ^ a b c Arroyo, Adrián; Hirata, Satoshi; Matsuzawa, Tetsuro; de la Torre, Ignacio (2016). "Nut Cracking Tools Used by Captive Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and Their Comparison with Early Stone Age Percussive Artefacts from Olduvai Gorge". PLOS ONE. 11 (11): e0166788. Bibcode:2016PLoSO..1166788A. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0166788. ISSN 1932-6203. PMC 5117719. PMID 27870877.
  5. ^ a b c Toth, Nicholas; Schick, Kathy D.; Savage-Rumbaugh, E.Sue; Sevcik, Rose A.; Rumbaugh, Duane M. (1993). "Pan the Tool-Maker: Investigations into the Stone Tool-Making and Tool-Using Capabilities of a Bonobo (Pan paniscus)". Journal of Archaeological Science. 20 (1): 81–91. Bibcode:1993JArSc..20...81T. doi:10.1006/jasc.1993.1006.
  6. ^ a b Schick, Kathy D.; Toth, Nicholas; Garufi, Gary; Savage-Rumbaugh, E.Sue; Rumbaugh, Duane; Sevcik, Rose (1999). "Continuing Investigations into the Stone Tool-making and Tool-using Capabilities of a Bonobo (Pan paniscus)". Journal of Archaeological Science. 26 (7): 821–832. Bibcode:1999JArSc..26..821S. doi:10.1006/jasc.1998.0350.
  7. ^ a b Westergaard, Gregory C.; Fragaszy, Dorothy M. (1987). "The manufacture and use of tools by capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella)". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 101 (2): 159–168. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.101.2.159. ISSN 1939-2087.
  8. ^ a b c d e f Carvalho, Susana; Cunha, Eugénia; Sousa, Cláudia; Matsuzawa, Tetsuro (2008). "Chaînes opératoires and resource-exploitation strategies in chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) nut cracking". Journal of Human Evolution. 55 (1): 148–163. doi:10.1016/j.jhevol.2008.02.005. hdl:10316/3758. PMID 18359504.
  9. ^ a b c d e de la Torre, Ignacio (2011). "The origins of stone tool technology in Africa: a historical perspective". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 366 (1567): 1028–1037. doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0350. ISSN 0962-8436. PMC 3049100. PMID 21357225.
  10. ^ Romeo, Luigi (1979-01-01). Ecce Homo! A Lexicon of Man. John Benjamins Publishing. ISBN 978-90-272-7452-6.
  11. ^ Warren, S. H., 1905. On the origin of Eoliths. Man, 5, 179-183.
  12. ^ Dawson, C. and Woodward, A.S., 1913. On the discovery of a Palaeolithic human skull and mandible in a flint-bearing gravel overlying the Wealden (Hastings Beds) at Piltdown, Fletching (Sussex). Quarterly journal of the geological society, 69(1-4), pp.117-123.
  13. ^ Leakey, L. S. B.; Hopwood, Arthur T.; Reck, Hans (1931). "New Yields from the Oldoway Bone Beds, Tanganyika Territory". Nature. 128 (3243): 1075. Bibcode:1931Natur.128.1075L. doi:10.1038/1281075a0. ISSN 0028-0836. S2CID 4077834.
  14. ^ Leakey L. S. B. (1934). The sequence of Stone Age cultures in east Africa. In Evans-Pritchard E. E., Firth R., Malinowski B., Schapera I. (Eds.) Essays presented to CG Seligman. London, UK: Kegan Paul, Trench, Trubner & Co. Limited, pp. 143-146.
  15. ^ Leakey, L. S. B. (1961). "Africa's Contribution to the Evolution of Man". The South African Archaeological Bulletin. 16 (61): 3–7. doi:10.2307/3887411. JSTOR 3887411.
  16. ^ a b Leakey L. S. B., Tobias P. V., Napier J. R. (1964). A new species of the genus Homo from Olduvai Gorge. Nature 202: 5–7.
  17. ^ McPherron, Shannon P.; Alemseged, Zeresenay; Marean, Curtis W.; Wynn, Jonathan G.; Reed, Denné; Geraads, Denis; Bobe, René; Béarat, Hamdallah A. (2010). "Evidence for stone-tool-assisted consumption of animal tissues before 3.39 million years ago at Dikika, Ethiopia". Nature. 466 (7308): 857–860. Bibcode:2010Natur.466..857M. doi:10.1038/nature09248. ISSN 0028-0836. PMID 20703305. S2CID 4356816.
  18. ^ Harmand, Sonia; Lewis, Jason E.; Feibel, Craig S.; Lepre, Christopher J.; Prat, Sandrine; Lenoble, Arnaud; Boës, Xavier; Quinn, Rhonda L.; Brenet, Michel; Arroyo, Adrian; Taylor, Nicholas; Clément, Sophie; Daver, Guillaume; Brugal, Jean-Philip; Leakey, Louise (2015). "3.3-million-year-old stone tools from Lomekwi 3, West Turkana, Kenya". Nature. 521 (7552): 310–315. Bibcode:2015Natur.521..310H. doi:10.1038/nature14464. ISSN 0028-0836. PMID 25993961. S2CID 1207285.
  19. ^ Wood, B. and Collard, M., (1999). The changing face of genus Homo. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews: Issues, News, and Reviews, 8(6): 195-207.
  20. ^ a b c Proffitt, Tomos; Reeves, Jonathan S.; Braun, David R.; Malaivijitnond, Suchinda; Luncz, Lydia V. (2023). "Wild macaques challenge the origin of intentional tool production". Science Advances. 9 (10): eade8159. Bibcode:2023SciA....9E8159P. doi:10.1126/sciadv.ade8159. PMC 10005173. PMID 36897944. S2CID 257437132.
  21. ^ a b Van Lawick-Goodall, Jane (1968). "The Behaviour of Free-living Chimpanzees in the Gombe Stream Reserve". Animal Behaviour Monographs. 1: 161–IN12. doi:10.1016/s0066-1856(68)80003-2. ISSN 0066-1856.
  22. ^ Wright, R.V., (1972). Imitative learning of a flaked stone technology-the case of an orangutan. The Australian Journal of Anthropology, 8(4): 296-306.
  23. ^ Haslam, Michael (2013). "'Captivity bias' in animal tool use and its implications for the evolution of hominin technology". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 368 (1630): 20120421. doi:10.1098/rstb.2012.0421. ISSN 0962-8436. PMC 4027414. PMID 24101629.
  24. ^ a b Westergaard, Gregory Charles; Suomi, Stephen J. (1994). "A simple stone-tool technology in monkeys". Journal of Human Evolution. 27 (5): 399–404. doi:10.1006/jhev.1994.1055.
  25. ^ a b c Motes-Rodrigo, Alba; McPherron, Shannon P.; Archer, Will; Hernandez-Aguilar, R. Adriana; Tennie, Claudio (2022). "Experimental investigation of orangutans' lithic percussive and sharp stone tool behaviours". PLOS ONE. 17 (2): e0263343. Bibcode:2022PLoSO..1763343M. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0263343. ISSN 1932-6203. PMC 8849460. PMID 35171926.
  26. ^ https://open-research-europe.ec.europa.eu/articles/1-20/v2
  27. ^ https://osf.io/preprints/qj3kn/
  28. ^ a b Köhler W., (1925). The Mentality of Apes. Kegan Paul, Trench and Trubner, London.
  29. ^ de Waal, Frans B. M. (2003). "Silent invasion: Imanishi's primatology and cultural bias in science". Animal Cognition. 6 (4): 293–299. doi:10.1007/s10071-003-0197-4. ISSN 1435-9448. PMID 14551801. S2CID 45665875.
  30. ^ a b Matsuzawa, Tetsuro; Yamagiwa, Juichi (2018). "Primatology: the beginning". Primates. 59 (4): 313–326. doi:10.1007/s10329-018-0672-9. ISSN 0032-8332. PMID 29982936. S2CID 254157644.
  31. ^ a b c Matsuzawa, Tetsuro (2003). "The Ai project: historical and ecological contexts". Animal Cognition. 6 (4): 199–211. doi:10.1007/s10071-003-0199-2. ISSN 1435-9448. PMID 14566577. S2CID 8928490.
  32. ^ a b c d Matsuzawa, Tetsuro; McGrew, William C. (2008). "Kinji Imanishi and 60 years of Japanese primatology". Current Biology. 18 (14): R587–R591. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2008.05.040. ISSN 0960-9822. PMID 18644329. S2CID 13572608.
  33. ^ Kawamura, S., 1959. The process of sub-culture propagation among Japanese macaques. Primates, 2(1), pp.43-60.
  34. ^ Morell, Virginia (1993). "Called 'Trimates,' Three Bold Women Shaped Their Field". Science. 260 (5106): 420–425. Bibcode:1993Sci...260..420M. doi:10.1126/science.260.5106.420. ISSN 0036-8075. PMID 17838264. S2CID 5060859.
  35. ^ a b Montgomery, Sy (2009). Walking with the Great Apes : Jane Goodall, Dian Fossey, Biruté Galdikas. Chelsea Green Publishing. ISBN 978-1-60358-244-5. OCLC 1021804170.
  36. ^ a b c d Whiten, A.; Goodall, J.; McGrew, W. C.; Nishida, T.; Reynolds, V.; Sugiyama, Y.; Tutin, C. E. G.; Wrangham, R. W.; Boesch, C. (1999). "Cultures in chimpanzees". Nature. 399 (6737): 682–685. Bibcode:1999Natur.399..682W. doi:10.1038/21415. ISSN 1476-4687. PMID 10385119. S2CID 4385871.
  37. ^ a b c Boesch, Christophe; Kalan, Ammie K.; Mundry, Roger; Arandjelovic, Mimi; Pika, Simone; Dieguez, Paula; Ayimisin, Emmanuel Ayuk; Barciela, Amanda; Coupland, Charlotte; Egbe, Villard Ebot; Eno-Nku, Manasseh; Michael Fay, J.; Fine, David; Adriana Hernandez-Aguilar, R.; Hermans, Veerle (2020). "Chimpanzee ethnography reveals unexpected cultural diversity". Nature Human Behaviour. 4 (9): 910–916. doi:10.1038/s41562-020-0890-1. ISSN 2397-3374. PMID 32451479. S2CID 256706922.
  38. ^ a b Humle, Tatyana; Newton-Fisher, Nicholas E. (2013), "Chapter 2 Culture in Non-human Primates: Definitions and Evidence", Understanding Cultural Transmission in Anthropology, Berghahn Books, pp. 80–101, doi:10.1515/9780857459947-006, ISBN 978-0-85745-994-7
  39. ^ Tomasello, Michael; Carpenter, Malinda; Call, Josep; Behne, Tanya; Moll, Henrike (2005). "Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 28 (5): 675–691. doi:10.1017/S0140525X05000129. ISSN 0140-525X. PMID 16262930. S2CID 3900485.
  40. ^ Sept, Jeanne M.; King, Barbara J.; McGrew, W. C.; Moore, Jim; Paterson, James D.; Strier, Karen B.; Uehara, Shigeo; Whiten, Andrew; Wrangham, Richard W. (1992). "Was There No Place Like Home?: A New Perspective on Early Hominid Archaeological Sites From the Mapping of Chimpanzee Nests [and Comments and Reply]". Current Anthropology. 33 (2): 187–207. doi:10.1086/204050. ISSN 0011-3204. S2CID 145013951.
  41. ^ a b c Mercader, Julio; Panger, Melissa; Boesch, Christophe (2002). "Excavation of a Chimpanzee Stone Tool Site in the African Rainforest". Science. 296 (5572): 1452–1455. Bibcode:2002Sci...296.1452M. doi:10.1126/science.1070268. ISSN 0036-8075. PMID 12029130. S2CID 21042746.
  42. ^ Ling, Victoria; Hernandez-Aguilar, Adriana; Haslam, Michael; Carvalho, Susana (2009). "The origins of percussive technology: A smashing time in Cambridge". Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews. 18 (2): 48–49. doi:10.1002/evan.20197. S2CID 85989573.
  43. ^ a b Benito-Calvo, Alfonso; Carvalho, Susana; Arroyo, Adrian; Matsuzawa, Tetsuro; de la Torre, Ignacio (2015). Addessi, Elsa (ed.). "First GIS Analysis of Modern Stone Tools Used by Wild Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) in Bossou, Guinea, West Africa". PLOS ONE. 10 (3): e0121613. Bibcode:2015PLoSO..1021613B. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0121613. ISSN 1932-6203. PMC 4368754. PMID 25793642.
  44. ^ Savage, Thomas S. (1844). "Observations on the habits of the python natalensis". Annals and Magazine of Natural History. 14 (89): 148–150. doi:10.1080/037454809495158. ISSN 0374-5481.
  45. ^ Darwin, Charles (1871). The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex. New York: D. Appleton and company. doi:10.5962/bhl.title.24784.
  46. ^ a b Carpenter, Alfred (1887). "Monkeys opening Oysters". Nature. 36 (916): 53. Bibcode:1887Natur..36...53C. doi:10.1038/036053d0. ISSN 0028-0836. S2CID 4112014.
  47. ^ Beck, B.B. (1980). Animal Tool Behavior. Garland STPM Pub.
  48. ^ a b Whiten, Andrew; Schick, Kathy; Toth, Nicholas (2009). "The evolution and cultural transmission of percussive technology: integrating evidence from palaeoanthropology and primatology". Journal of Human Evolution. 57 (4): 420–435. doi:10.1016/j.jhevol.2008.12.010. ISSN 0047-2484. PMID 19740521.
  49. ^ a b Carvalho, Susana; Matsuzawa, Tetsuro; McGrew, William C. (2013), Boesch, Christophe; Sanz, Crickette M.; Call, Josep (eds.), "From pounding to knapping: How chimpanzees can help us to model hominin lithics", Tool Use in Animals: Cognition and Ecology, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 225–241, ISBN 978-0-511-89480-0, retrieved 2023-04-10
  50. ^ Wynn, Thomas; Hernandez-Aguilar, R. Adriana; Marchant, Linda F.; Mcgrew, William C. (2011). ""An ape's view of the Oldowan" revisited". Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews. 20 (5): 181–197. doi:10.1002/evan.20323. ISSN 1060-1538. PMID 22034236. S2CID 23910905.
  51. ^ Sayers, K., and Lovejoy, C. O., 2008. The chimpanzee has no clothes: A critical examination of Pan troglodytes in models of human evolution. Current Anthropology, 49(1), pp.87–99
  52. ^ Sayers, Ken; Raghanti, Mary Ann; Lovejoy, C. Owen (2012). "Human Evolution and the Chimpanzee Referential Doctrine". Annual Review of Anthropology. 41 (1): 119–138. doi:10.1146/annurev-anthro-092611-145815. ISSN 0084-6570.
  53. ^ a b c d Bandini, Elisa; Harrison, Rachel A.; Motes-Rodrigo, Alba (2022). "Examining the suitability of extant primates as models of hominin stone tool culture". Humanities and Social Sciences Communications. 9 (1): 74. doi:10.1057/s41599-022-01091-x. ISSN 2662-9992. S2CID 257090939.
  54. ^ a b c d Luncz, Lydia V.; Arroyo, Adrián; Falótico, Tiago; Quinn, Patrick; Proffitt, Tomos (2022). "A primate model for the origin of flake technology". Journal of Human Evolution. 171: 103250. doi:10.1016/j.jhevol.2022.103250. PMID 36122461. S2CID 252347187.
  55. ^ a b Hannah, Alison C.; McGrew, W. C. (1987). "Chimpanzees using stones to crack open oil palm nuts in Liberia". Primates. 28 (1): 31–46. doi:10.1007/BF02382181. ISSN 1610-7365. S2CID 24738945.
  56. ^ Davidson, Iain; McGrew, William C. (2005). "Stone tools and the uniqueness of human culture". Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute. 11 (4): 793–817. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9655.2005.00262.x. ISSN 1359-0987.
  57. ^ a b Boesch, Christophe; Boesch, Hedwige (1990). "Tool Use and Tool Making in Wild Chimpanzees". Folia Primatologica. 54 (1–2): 86–99. doi:10.1159/000156428. ISSN 0015-5713. PMID 2157651.
  58. ^ Wrangham, R.W. and Goodall, J., 2013. Chimpanzee use of medicinal leaves. In Understanding chimpanzees (pp. 22-37). Harvard University Press
  59. ^ a b McGrew, William C. (2010). "Chimpanzee Technology". Science. 328 (5978): 579–580. Bibcode:2010Sci...328..579M. doi:10.1126/science.1187921. ISSN 0036-8075. PMID 20431004. S2CID 8772765.
  60. ^ Morgan, Bethan J.; Abwe, Ekwoge E. (2006). "Chimpanzees use stone hammers in Cameroon". Current Biology. 16 (16): R632–R633. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2006.07.045. ISSN 0960-9822. PMID 16920608. S2CID 31639484.
  61. ^ Biro, Dora; Inoue-Nakamura, Noriko; Tonooka, Rikako; Yamakoshi, Gen; Sousa, Claudia; Matsuzawa, Tetsuro (2003). "Cultural innovation and transmission of tool use in wild chimpanzees: evidence from field experiments". Animal Cognition. 6 (4): 213–223. doi:10.1007/s10071-003-0183-x. ISSN 1435-9448. PMID 12898285. S2CID 651756.
  62. ^ Carvalho, Susana; Biro, Dora; McGrew, William C.; Matsuzawa, Tetsuro (2009). "Tool-composite reuse in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): archaeologically invisible steps in the technological evolution of early hominins?". Animal Cognition. 12 (S1): 103–114. doi:10.1007/s10071-009-0271-7. ISSN 1435-9448. PMID 19680699. S2CID 22737853.
  63. ^ Luncz, Lydia V.; Mundry, Roger; Boesch, Christophe (2012). "Evidence for Cultural Differences between Neighboring Chimpanzee Communities". Current Biology. 22 (10): 922–926. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2012.03.031. ISSN 0960-9822. PMID 22578420. S2CID 3191082.
  64. ^ Luncz, Lydia V.; Sirianni, Giulia; Mundry, Roger; Boesch, Christophe (2018). "Costly culture: differences in nut-cracking efficiency between wild chimpanzee groups". Animal Behaviour. 137: 63–73. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2017.12.017. ISSN 0003-3472. S2CID 53205528.
  65. ^ Sirianni, Giulia; Mundry, Roger; Boesch, Christophe (2015). "When to choose which tool: multidimensional and conditional selection of nut-cracking hammers in wild chimpanzees". Animal Behaviour. 100: 152–165. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.11.022. ISSN 0003-3472. S2CID 53192010.
  66. ^ Visalberghi, Elisabetta (1990). "Tool Use in Cebus". Folia Primatologica. 54 (3–4): 146–154. doi:10.1159/000156438. ISSN 0015-5713. PMID 2202616.
  67. ^ a b Ottoni, Eduardo B.; Mannu, Massimo (2001). "Semifree-ranging tufted capuchins (Cebus apella) spontaneously use tools to crack open nuts". International Journal of Primatology. 22 (3): 347–358. doi:10.1023/A:1010747426841. S2CID 29768277.
  68. ^ Fragaszy, Dorothy; Izar, Patricia; Visalberghi, Elisabetta; Ottoni, Eduardo B.; de Oliveira, Marino Gomes (2004). "Wild capuchin monkeys (Cebus libidinosus) use anvils and stone pounding tools". American Journal of Primatology. 64 (4): 359–366. doi:10.1002/ajp.20085. ISSN 0275-2565. PMID 15580579. S2CID 16222308.
  69. ^ a b c d Ottoni, Eduardo B.; Izar, Patrícia (2008). "Capuchin monkey tool use: Overview and implications". Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews. 17 (4): 171–178. doi:10.1002/evan.20185. ISSN 1060-1538. S2CID 83604383.
  70. ^ A, Langguth (1997). "Capuchin monkeys in the Caatinga : tool use and food habits during drought". Neo Primates. 5 (3): 77–78.
  71. ^ Fernandes, Marcus E. B. (1991). "Tool use and predation of oysters (Crassostrea rhizophorae) by the tufted capuchin, Cebus apella appella, in brackish water mangrove swamp". Primates. 32 (4): 529–531. doi:10.1007/BF02381944. ISSN 1610-7365. S2CID 19951712.
  72. ^ Visalberghi, Elisabetta; Fragaszy, Dorothy Munkenbeck; Izar, Patrícia; Ottoni, Eduardo B. (2005). "Terrestriality and Tool Use". Science. 308 (5724): 951–952. doi:10.1126/science.308.5724.951c. ISSN 0036-8075. PMID 15890860. S2CID 29819009.
  73. ^ Meulman, Ellen J.M.; Sanz, Crickette M.; Visalberghi, Elisabetta; van Schaik, Carel P. (2012). "The Role of Terrestriality in Promoting Primate Technology". Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews. 21 (2): 58–68. doi:10.1002/evan.21304. ISSN 1060-1538. PMID 22499440. S2CID 31022370.
  74. ^ Barrett, Brendan J.; Monteza-moreno, Claudio M.; Dogandžić, Tamara; Zwyns, Nicolas; Ibáñez, Alicia; Crofoot, Margaret C. (2018). "Habitual Stone-Tool Aided Extractive Foraging in White-Faced Capuchins, Cebus Capucinus". Royal Society Open Science. 5 (8): 351619. bioRxiv 10.1101/351619. doi:10.1098/rsos.181002. PMC 6124021. PMID 30225086. S2CID 52286411.
  75. ^ Monteza-Moreno, Claudio M.; Dogandžić, Tamara; McLean, Kevin A.; Castillo-Caballero, Pedro L.; Mijango-Ramos, Zarluis; Del Rosario-Vargas, Evelyn; Crofoot, Margaret C.; Barrett, Brendan J. (2020). "White-Faced Capuchin, Cebus capucinus imitator, Hammerstone and Anvil Tool Use in Riparian Habitats on Coiba Island, Panama". International Journal of Primatology. 41 (3): 429–433. doi:10.1007/s10764-020-00156-5. ISSN 0164-0291. S2CID 218773276.
  76. ^ Alfaro, Jessica W. Lynch; Silva, José de Sousa E; Rylands, Anthony B. (2012). "How Different Are Robust and Gracile Capuchin Monkeys? An Argument for the Use of Sapajus and Cebus". American Journal of Primatology. 74 (4): 273–286. doi:10.1002/ajp.22007. ISSN 0275-2565. PMID 22328205. S2CID 18840598.
  77. ^ Linné, Carl von (1792). The animal kingdom, or zoological system, of the celebrated Sir Charles Linnæus. containing a complete systematic description, arrangement, and nomenclature, of all the known species and varieties of the mammalia, or animals which give suck to their young. Printed for A. Strahan, and T. Cadell, London, and W. Creech, Edinburgh. OCLC 1039518575.
  78. ^ Alfaro, Jessica W. Lynch; Matthews, Luke; Boyette, Adam H.; Macfarlan, Shane J.; Phillips, Kimberley A.; Falótico, Tiago; Ottoni, Eduardo; Verderane, Michele; Izar, Patrícia; Schulte, Meredith; Melin, Amanda; Fedigan, Linda; Janson, Charles; Alfaro, Michael E. (2011). "Anointing variation across wild capuchin populations: a review of material preferences, bout frequency and anointing sociality in Cebus and Sapajus". American Journal of Primatology. 74 (4): 299–314. doi:10.1002/ajp.20971. ISSN 0275-2565. PMID 21769906. S2CID 14560688.
  79. ^ Hogg, Russell T.; Elokda, Abdallah (2021). "Quantification of enamel decussation in gracile and robust capuchins (Cebus, Sapajus, Cebidae, Platyrrhini)". American Journal of Primatology. 83 (5): e23246. doi:10.1002/ajp.23246. ISSN 0275-2565. PMID 33638563. S2CID 232064804.
  80. ^ Balolia, Katharine L.; Wulff, Alexandra (2022). "One Genus or Two? Evaluating Whether Gracile and Robust Capuchin Monkeys are Validly Classified as Separate Genera Based on Craniofacial Shape". International Journal of Primatology. 43 (5): 798–821. doi:10.1007/s10764-022-00300-3. hdl:1885/298237. ISSN 0164-0291. S2CID 251008044.
  81. ^ Canale, Gustavo Rodrigues; Guidorizzi, Carlos Eduardo; Kierulff, Maria Cecília Martins; Gatto, Cassiano Augusto Ferreira Rodrigues (2009). "First record of tool use by wild populations of the yellow-breasted capuchin monkey (Cebus xanthosternos) and new records for the bearded capuchin (Cebus libidinosus)". American Journal of Primatology. 71 (5): 366–372. doi:10.1002/ajp.20648. ISSN 0275-2565. PMID 19206141. S2CID 38883635.
  82. ^ Boinski, Sue; Quatrone, Robert P.; Swartz, Hilary (2000). "Substrate and Tool Use by Brown Capuchins in Suriname: Ecological Contexts and Cognitive Bases". American Anthropologist. 102 (4): 741–761. doi:10.1525/aa.2000.102.4.741. ISSN 0002-7294.
  83. ^ Ottoni, Eduardo B.; de Resende, Briseida Dogo; Izar, Patrícia (2005). "Watching the best nutcrackers: what capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) know about others' tool-using skills". Animal Cognition. 8 (4): 215–219. doi:10.1007/s10071-004-0245-8. ISSN 1435-9456. PMID 15719240. S2CID 51862020.
  84. ^ Falótico, Tiago; Coutinho, Paulo Henrique M.; Bueno, Carolina Q.; Rufo, Henrique P.; Ottoni, Eduardo B. (2018). "Stone tool use by wild capuchin monkeys (Sapajus libidinosus) at Serra das Confusões National Park, Brazil". Primates. 59 (4): 385–394. doi:10.1007/s10329-018-0660-0. ISSN 0032-8332. PMID 29550951. S2CID 254158889.
  85. ^ Visalberghi, Elisabetta; Barca, Virginia; Izar, Patricia; Fragaszy, Dorothy; Truppa, Valentina (2021). "Optional tool use: The case of wild bearded capuchins (Sapajus libidinosus) cracking cashew nuts by biting or by using percussors". American Journal of Primatology. 83 (1): e23221. doi:10.1002/ajp.23221. ISSN 0275-2565. PMID 33300618. S2CID 228090909.
  86. ^ Truppa, Valentina; Sabbatini, Gloria; Izar, Patricia; Fragaszy, Dorothy M.; Visalberghi, Elisabetta (2021). "Anticipating future actions: Motor planning improves with age in wild bearded capuchin monkeys (Sapajus libidinosus)". Developmental Science. 24 (4): e13077. doi:10.1111/desc.13077. ISSN 1363-755X. PMID 33342007. S2CID 229342997.
  87. ^ Mannu, Massimo; Ottoni, Eduardo B. (2009). "The enhanced tool-kit of two groups of wild bearded capuchin monkeys in the Caatinga: tool making, associative use, and secondary tools". American Journal of Primatology. 71 (3): 242–251. doi:10.1002/ajp.20642. ISSN 0275-2565. PMID 19051323. S2CID 3479020.
  88. ^ de A. Moura, A. C.; Lee, P. C. (2004). "Capuchin Stone Tool Use in Caatinga Dry Forest". Science. 306 (5703): 1909. doi:10.1126/science.1102558. ISSN 0036-8075. PMID 15591195. S2CID 36793833.
  89. ^ a b Falótico, Tiago; Ottoni, Eduardo B. (2016). "The manifold use of pounding stone tools by wild capuchin monkeys of Serra da Capivara National Park, Brazil". Behaviour. 153 (4): 421–442. doi:10.1163/1568539X-00003357. ISSN 0005-7959.
  90. ^ Visalberghi, Elisabetta; Addessi, Elsa; Truppa, Valentina; Spagnoletti, Noemi; Ottoni, Eduardo; Izar, Patricia; Fragaszy, Dorothy (2009). "Selection of Effective Stone Tools by Wild Bearded Capuchin Monkeys". Current Biology. 19 (3): 213–217. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2008.11.064. PMID 19147358. S2CID 7258497.
  91. ^ Coelho, C. G.; Falótico, T.; Izar, P.; Mannu, M.; Resende, B. D.; Siqueira, J. O.; Ottoni, E. B. (2015). "Social learning strategies for nut-cracking by tufted capuchin monkeys (Sapajus spp.)". Animal Cognition. 18 (4): 911–919. doi:10.1007/s10071-015-0861-5. ISSN 1435-9448. PMID 25800169. S2CID 254134412.
  92. ^ Fragaszy, D.; Pickering, T.; Liu, Q.; Izar, P.; Ottoni, E.; Visalberghi, E. (2010-02-01). "Bearded capuchin monkeys' and a human's efficiency at cracking palm nuts with stone tools: field experiments". Animal Behaviour. 79 (2): 321–332. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.11.004. ISSN 0003-3472. S2CID 11137182.
  93. ^ Liu, Q.; Simpson, K.; Izar, P.; Ottoni, E.; Visalberghi, E.; Fragaszy, D. (2009). "Kinematics and energetics of nut-cracking in wild capuchin monkeys (Cebus libidinosus) in Piauí, Brazil". American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 138 (2): 210–220. doi:10.1002/ajpa.20920. ISSN 0002-9483. PMID 18785652.
  94. ^ a b c d Proffitt, Tomos; Luncz, Lydia V.; Falótico, Tiago; Ottoni, Eduardo B.; de la Torre, Ignacio; Haslam, Michael (2016-11-03). "Wild monkeys flake stone tools". Nature. 539 (7627): 85–88. Bibcode:2016Natur.539...85P. doi:10.1038/nature20112. ISSN 0028-0836. PMID 27760117. S2CID 205251548.
  95. ^ Huffman, Michael A.; Quiatt, Duane (1986). "Stone handling by Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata): Implications for tool use of stone". Primates. 27 (4): 413–423. doi:10.1007/BF02381887. ISSN 1610-7365. S2CID 25426387.
  96. ^ Gunst, Noëlle; Huffman, Michael; Leca, Jean-Baptiste (2007). "Japanese macaque cultures: Inter- and intra-troop behavioural variability of stone handling patterns across 10 troops". Behaviour. 144 (3): 251–281. doi:10.1163/156853907780425712. ISSN 0005-7959.
  97. ^ a b Gumert, Michael D.; Kluck, Marius; Malaivijitnond, Suchinda (2009). "The physical characteristics and usage patterns of stone axe and pounding hammers used by long-tailed macaques in the Andaman Sea region of Thailand". American Journal of Primatology. 71 (7): 594–608. doi:10.1002/ajp.20694. PMID 19405083. S2CID 22384150.
  98. ^ a b c d Haslam, Michael; Luncz, Lydia; Pascual-Garrido, Alejandra; Falótico, Tiago; Malaivijitnond, Suchinda; Gumert, Michael (2016). "Archaeological excavation of wild macaque stone tools". Journal of Human Evolution. 96: 134–138. doi:10.1016/j.jhevol.2016.05.002. PMID 27256780.
  99. ^ a b Malaivijitnond, Suchinda; Lekprayoon, Chariya; Tandavanittj, Nontivich; Panha, Somsak; Cheewatham, Cheewapap; Hamada, Yuzuru (2007). "Stone-tool usage by Thai long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis)". American Journal of Primatology. 69 (2): 227–233. doi:10.1002/ajp.20342. PMID 17146796. S2CID 24117181.
  100. ^ Gumert, Michael D.; Malaivijitnond, Suchinda (2013). "Long-tailed macaques select mass of stone tools according to food type". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 368 (1630): 20120413. doi:10.1098/rstb.2012.0413. ISSN 0962-8436. PMC 4027418. PMID 24101623. S2CID 12011847.
  101. ^ a b Proffitt, T.; Luncz, V. L.; Malaivijitnond, S.; Gumert, M.; Svensson, M. S.; Haslam, M. (2018). "Analysis of wild macaque stone tools used to crack oil palm nuts". Royal Society Open Science. 5 (3): 171904. Bibcode:2018RSOS....571904P. doi:10.1098/rsos.171904. ISSN 2054-5703. PMC 5882716. PMID 29657792.
  102. ^ Falótico, Tiago; Spagnoletti, Noemi; Haslam, Michael; Luncz, Lydia V.; Malaivijitnond, Suchinda; Gumert, Michael (2017). "Analysis of sea almond (Terminalia catappa) cracking sites used by wild Burmese long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis aurea)". American Journal of Primatology. 79 (5): e22629. doi:10.1002/ajp.22629. PMID 28056164. S2CID 3882235.
  103. ^ Luncz, Lydia V.; Svensson, Magdalena S.; Haslam, Michael; Malaivijitnond, Suchinda; Proffitt, Tomos; Gumert, Michael (2017). "Technological Response of Wild Macaques (Macaca fascicularis) to Anthropogenic Change". International Journal of Primatology. 38 (5): 872–880. doi:10.1007/s10764-017-9985-6. ISSN 0164-0291. PMC 5629225. PMID 29056799. S2CID 254539613.
  104. ^ a b c Proffitt, Tomos; Reeves, Jonathan S.; Benito-Calvo, Alfonso; Sánchez-Romero, Laura; Arroyo, Adrián; Malaijivitnond, Suchinda; Luncz, Lydia V. (2021). "Three-dimensional surface morphometry differentiates behaviour on primate percussive stone tools". Journal of the Royal Society Interface. 18 (184). doi:10.1098/rsif.2021.0576. ISSN 1742-5662. PMC 8564602. PMID 34727711.
  105. ^ Tan, Amanda; Tan, Say Hoon; Vyas, Dhaval; Malaivijitnond, Suchinda; Gumert, Michael D. (2015). "There Is More than One Way to Crack an Oyster: Identifying Variation in Burmese Long-Tailed Macaque (Macaca fascicularis aurea) Stone-Tool Use". PLOS ONE. 10 (5): e0124733. Bibcode:2015PLoSO..1024733T. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0124733. ISSN 1932-6203. PMC 4430286. PMID 25970286.
  106. ^ Haslam, Michael; Gumert, Michael D.; Biro, Dora; Carvalho, Susana; Malaivijitnond, Suchinda (2013). "Use-Wear Patterns on Wild Macaque Stone Tools Reveal Their Behavioural History". PLOS ONE. 8 (8): e72872. Bibcode:2013PLoSO...872872H. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0072872. ISSN 1932-6203. PMC 3745380. PMID 23977365.
  107. ^ Gumert, Michael D.; Tan, Amanda Wei Yi; Luncz, Lydia V.; Chua, Constance Ting; Kulik, Lars; Switzer, Adam D.; Haslam, Michael; Iriki, Atsushi; Malaivijitnond, Suchinda (2019). "Prevalence of tool behaviour is associated with pelage phenotype in intraspecific hybrid long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis aurea × M. f. fascicularis)". Behaviour. 156 (11): 1083–1125. doi:10.1163/1568539X-00003557. hdl:10220/49527. ISSN 0005-7959. S2CID 164644293.
  108. ^ "Primate Speciation: A Case Study of African Apes | Learn Science at Scitable". www.nature.com. Retrieved 2023-03-16.
  109. ^ Bandini, Elisa; Motes-Rodrigo, Alba; Archer, William; Minchin, Tanya; Axelsen, Helene; Hernandez-Aguilar, Raquel Adriana; McPherron, Shannon P.; Tennie, Claudio (2021-07-15). "Naïve, unenculturated chimpanzees fail to make and use flaked stone tools". Open Research Europe. 1: 20. doi:10.12688/openreseurope.13186.2. ISSN 2732-5121. PMC 7612464. PMID 35253007.
  110. ^ Gruber, Thibaud; Clay, Zanna (2016). "A Comparison Between Bonobos and Chimpanzees: A Review and Update". Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews. 25 (5): 239–252. doi:10.1002/evan.21501. PMID 27753219. S2CID 20712022.
  111. ^ Samuni, Liran; Lemieux, David; Lamb, Alicia; Galdino, Daiane; Surbeck, Martin (2022). "Tool use behavior in three wild bonobo communities at Kokolopori". American Journal of Primatology. 84 (1): e23342. doi:10.1002/ajp.23342. ISSN 0275-2565. PMID 34694658. S2CID 239767503.
  112. ^ a b Furuichi, T.; Sanz, C.; Koops, K.; Sakamaki, T.; Ryu, H.; Tokuyama, N.; Morgan, D. (2015). "Why do wild bonobos not use tools like chimpanzees do?". Behaviour. 152 (3–4): 425–460. doi:10.1163/1568539X-00003226. ISSN 0005-7959. S2CID 84339010.
  113. ^ Narat, Victor; Pennec, Flora; Simmen, Bruno; Ngawolo, Jean Christophe Bokika; Krief, Sabrina (2015). "Bonobo habituation in a forest–savanna mosaic habitat: influence of ape species, habitat type, and sociocultural context". Primates. 56 (4): 339–349. doi:10.1007/s10329-015-0476-0. ISSN 1610-7365. PMID 26159325. S2CID 254162786.
  114. ^ Gruber, Thibaud; Clay, Zanna; Zuberbühler, Klaus (2010). "A comparison of bonobo and chimpanzee tool use: evidence for a female bias in the Pan lineage". Animal Behaviour. 80 (6): 1023–1033. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.09.005. ISSN 0003-3472. S2CID 14923158.
  115. ^ Koops, Kathelijne; Furuichi, Takeshi; Hashimoto, Chie (2015). "Chimpanzees and bonobos differ in intrinsic motivation for tool use". Scientific Reports. 5 (1): 11356. Bibcode:2015NatSR...511356K. doi:10.1038/srep11356. ISSN 2045-2322. PMC 4468814. PMID 26079292.
  116. ^ Bardo, Ameline; Borel, Antony; Meunier, Hélène; Guéry, Jean-Pascal; Pouydebat, Emmanuelle (2016). "Behavioral and functional strategies during tool use tasks in bonobos: B ardo et al ". American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 161 (1): 125–140. doi:10.1002/ajpa.23015. PMID 27311774.
  117. ^ Neufuss, Johanna; Humle, Tatyana; Cremaschi, Andrea; Kivell, Tracy L. (2017). "Nut-cracking behaviour in wild-born, rehabilitated bonobos (Pan paniscus ): a comprehensive study of hand-preference, hand grips and efficiency". American Journal of Primatology. 79 (2): e22589. doi:10.1002/ajp.22589. PMID 27564429. S2CID 3767070.
  118. ^ Schiffer, Michael B. (1983). "Toward the Identification of Formation Processes". American Antiquity. 48 (4): 675–706. doi:10.2307/279771. ISSN 0002-7316. JSTOR 279771. S2CID 160026999.
  119. ^ Mercader, Julio; Panger, Melissa; Boesch, Christophe (2002-05-24). "Excavation of a Chimpanzee Stone Tool Site in the African Rainforest". Science. 296 (5572): 1452–1455. Bibcode:2002Sci...296.1452M. doi:10.1126/science.1070268. ISSN 0036-8075. PMID 12029130. S2CID 21042746.
  120. ^ a b c Falótico, Tiago; Proffitt, Tomos; Ottoni, Eduardo B.; Staff, Richard A.; Haslam, Michael (2019). "Three thousand years of wild capuchin stone tool use". Nature Ecology & Evolution. 3 (7): 1034–1038. doi:10.1038/s41559-019-0904-4. ISSN 2397-334X. PMID 31235926. S2CID 256705176.
  121. ^ Reeves, Jonathan S.; Proffitt, Tomos; Luncz, Lydia V. (2021). "Modeling a primate technological niche". Scientific Reports. 11 (1): 23139. Bibcode:2021NatSR..1123139R. doi:10.1038/s41598-021-01849-4. ISSN 2045-2322. PMC 8632893. PMID 34848740.
  122. ^ Van Gijn, A.L. (2014). "Science and interpretation in microwear studies". Journal of Archaeological Science. 48: 166–169. Bibcode:2014JArSc..48..166V. doi:10.1016/j.jas.2013.10.024. ISSN 0305-4403.
  123. ^ a b c Caruana, Matthew V.; Carvalho, Susana; Braun, David R.; Presnyakova, Darya; Haslam, Michael; Archer, Will; Bobe, Rene; Harris, John W. K. (2014). Bicho, Nuno (ed.). "Quantifying Traces of Tool Use: A Novel Morphometric Analysis of Damage Patterns on Percussive Tools". PLOS ONE. 9 (11): e113856. Bibcode:2014PLoSO...9k3856C. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0113856. ISSN 1932-6203. PMC 4240665. PMID 25415303.
  124. ^ Borel, Antony; Ollé, Andreu; Vergès, Josep Maria; Sala, Robert (2014). "Scanning Electron and Optical Light Microscopy: two complementary approaches for the understanding and interpretation of usewear and residues on stone tools". Journal of Archaeological Science. 48: 46–59. Bibcode:2014JArSc..48...46B. doi:10.1016/j.jas.2013.06.031. ISSN 0305-4403.
  125. ^ Evershed, R.P., 2008. Organic residue analysis in archaeology: the archaeological biomarker revolution. Archaeometry, 50(6), pp.895-924.
  126. ^ Arroyo, Adrián; Falótico, Tiago; Burguet-Coca, Aitor; Expósito, Isabel; Quinn, Patrick; Proffitt, Tomos (2021). "Use-wear and residue analysis of pounding tools used by wild capuchin monkeys (Sapajus libidinosus) from Serra da Capivara (Piauí, Brazil)". Journal of Archaeological Science: Reports. 35: 102690. Bibcode:2021JArSR..35j2690A. doi:10.1016/j.jasrep.2020.102690. S2CID 230555705.
  127. ^ Inizan, M.L., Reduron-Ballinger, M., Roche, H. and Tixier, J., 1999. Technology and terminology of knapped stone (Préhistoire de la Pierre taillée 5). Nanterre: CREP.
  128. ^ a b c d Proffitt, Tomos; Reeves, Jonathan S.; Falótico, Tiago; Arroyo, Adrián; Torre, Ignacio de la; Ottoni, Eduardo B.; Luncz, Lydia V. (2023). "Identifying intentional flake production at the dawn of technology: A technological and 3D geometric morphometric study". Journal of Archaeological Science. 152: 105740. Bibcode:2023JArSc.152j5740P. doi:10.1016/j.jas.2023.105740. hdl:10261/296180. S2CID 257190876.
  129. ^ Proffitt, T.; Haslam, M.; Mercader, J.F.; Boesch, C.; Luncz, L.V. (2018). "Revisiting Panda 100, the first archaeological chimpanzee nut-cracking site". Journal of Human Evolution. 124: 117–139. doi:10.1016/j.jhevol.2018.04.016. PMID 30236627. S2CID 52308880.
  130. ^ Macchi, R.; Daver, G.; Brenet, M.; Prat, S.; Hugheville, L.; Harmand, S.; Lewis, J.; Domalain, M. (2021). "Biomechanical demands of percussive techniques in the context of early stone toolmaking". Journal of the Royal Society Interface. 18 (178): 20201044. doi:10.1098/rsif.2020.1044. ISSN 1742-5662. PMC 8150015. PMID 34034530.
  131. ^ Heldstab, Sandra A.; Kosonen, Zaida K.; Koski, Sonja E.; Burkart, Judith M.; van Schaik, Carel P.; Isler, Karin (2016). "Manipulation complexity in primates coevolved with brain size and terrestriality". Scientific Reports. 6 (1): 24528. Bibcode:2016NatSR...624528H. doi:10.1038/srep24528. ISSN 2045-2322. PMC 4830942. PMID 27075921.
  132. ^ a b c Tennie, Claudio; Call, Josep; Tomasello, Michael (2009). "Ratcheting up the ratchet: on the evolution of cumulative culture". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 364 (1528): 2405–2415. doi:10.1098/rstb.2009.0052. ISSN 0962-8436. PMC 2865079. PMID 19620111.
  133. ^ a b Tennie, Claudio; Bandini, Elisa; van Schaik, Carel P.; Hopper, Lydia M. (2020). "The zone of latent solutions and its relevance to understanding ape cultures". Biology & Philosophy. 35 (5): 55. doi:10.1007/s10539-020-09769-9. ISSN 1572-8404. PMC 7548278. PMID 33093737.
  134. ^ Luncz, Lydia V.; Wittig, Roman M.; Boesch, Christophe (2015). "Primate archaeology reveals cultural transmission in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus)". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 370 (1682): 20140348. doi:10.1098/rstb.2014.0348. ISSN 0962-8436. PMC 4614712. PMID 26483527.
  135. ^ a b c Koops, Kathelijne; Soumah, Aly Gaspard; van Leeuwen, Kelly L.; Camara, Henry Didier; Matsuzawa, Tetsuro (2022). "Field experiments find no evidence that chimpanzee nut cracking can be independently innovated". Nature Human Behaviour. 6 (4): 487–494. doi:10.1038/s41562-021-01272-9. ISSN 2397-3374. PMID 35075258. S2CID 246277257.
  136. ^ a b Tennie, Claudio; Call, Josep (2023). "Unmotivated Subjects Cannot Provide Interpretable Data and Tasks with Sensitive Learning Periods Require Appropriately Aged Subjects: A Commentary on Koops et al. (2022) "Field experiments find no evidence that chimpanzee nut cracking can be independently innovated"" (PDF). Animal Behavior and Cognition. 10 (1): 89–94. doi:10.26451/abc.10.01.05.2023. S2CID 257262799.
  137. ^ Tennie, Claudio; Premo, L. S.; Braun, David R.; McPherron, Shannon P. (2017). "Early Stone Tools and Cultural Transmission: Resetting the Null Hypothesis". Current Anthropology. 58 (5): 652–672. doi:10.1086/693846. ISSN 0011-3204. S2CID 148701377.
  138. ^ Pradhan, Gauri R.; Tennie, Claudio; van Schaik, Carel P. (2012-07-01). "Social organization and the evolution of cumulative technology in apes and hominins". Journal of Human Evolution. 63 (1): 180–190. doi:10.1016/j.jhevol.2012.04.008. ISSN 0047-2484. PMID 22658335.
  139. ^ a b Tennie, C. (2023) ‘The Earliest Tools and Cultures of Hominins’, in J.J. Tehrani, J. Kendal, and R. Kendal (Eds) The Oxford Handbook of Cultural Evolution. 1st ed. Oxford University Press, p. C33P1-C33N40.
  140. ^ Snyder, William D.; Reeves, Jonathan S.; Tennie, Claudio (2022). "Early knapping techniques do not necessitate cultural transmission". Science Advances. 8 (27): eabo2894. Bibcode:2022SciA....8O2894S. doi:10.1126/sciadv.abo2894. ISSN 2375-2548. PMC 9258951. PMID 35857472.

primate, archaeology, field, research, established, 2008, that, combines, research, interests, foci, from, primatology, archaeology, main, primate, archaeology, study, behavior, extant, extinct, primates, associated, material, records, discipline, attempts, mo. Primate archaeology is a field of research established in 2008 that combines research interests and foci from primatology and archaeology The main aim of primate archaeology is to study behavior of extant and extinct primates and the associated material records The discipline attempts to move beyond archaeology s anthropocentric perspective by placing the focus on both past and present primate tool use 1 Primate archaeology is characterized by the combination of archaeological and primatological methods and researchers consider both non human primate tools and their behaviour in tandem 2 Primate archaeology has the unique opportunity to observe the tool use behaviors of extant non human primates and the formation of the material record that emerges from that behavior It is this ability to observe behavior and the subsequent material deposition resulting in a material record recoverable using standard archaeological field methods that gives this new field of research the possibility of reconstructing predicting and interpreting extant primates tool use spatial patterns 3 Overall primate archaeology helps researchers understand how early hominins used material culture what these patterns reveal about ancient hominin cognition as well as patterns of landscape use that could allow researchers to identify behaviors that are difficult to detect archaeologically 1 2 3 Primate archaeology s main focus is on the study of the non human primates that have been observed using tools in the wild chimpanzees Pan troglodytes robust capuchins Sapajus spp and long tailed macaques Macaca fascicularis aurea Since its conception primate archaeology has also implemented the use of captive studies 4 akin to archaeological experiments with non human primates looking into stone tool manufacture 5 6 7 Contents 1 Origins 1 1 Archaeology 1 2 Primatology 1 3 Official birth 2 Tool use in non human primates 2 1 Chimpanzees Pan troglodytes 2 2 Robust capuchins genus Sapajus 2 3 Long tailed macaques Macaca fascicularis aurea 2 4 Other non human primates 3 Methods 3 1 Excavation and site formation processes 3 2 Use wear and residue analysis 3 3 Technological analysis 4 Implications for the study of human evolution 5 See also 6 ReferencesOrigins editThe establishment of primate archaeology as an official discipline took place around 2008 8 and 2009 1 However the two fold nature of this relatively new subject area can be traced back to some major events in the development of both archaeology and primatology Archaeology edit The search for the earliest evidence of material culture in the archaeological record has been one of archaeology s major aims almost since its inception as a discipline With the classification of stone tools as human made artifacts and not fossils towards the end of the seventeenth century stone tools soon became key evidence for arranging the archaeological record into sequential cultural phases 9 The discovery of Eoliths which were thought to be flint tools in the Oligocene deposits of Thenay in France led to the establishment of the Eolithic culture and Homopithecus bourgeoisi 9 10 The Eolithic tools were described as being directly used in their natural state or being poorly worked Although some influential scholars tried to discredit the Eolith hypothesis 11 the discovery of the Piltdown man in 1912 only helped to reinforce its credibility because the fossil remains were found in association with Eolithic tools 12 Both the Eolithic culture and Piltdown man remained controversial until they were finally discredited around the 1950s 9 nbsp OH 7 Homo habilis nbsp Example of Oldowan stone toolsDisregarding the Eolithic controversy the earliest stone tools also found in the European continent were the Chellean being followed by the Acheulean 9 Thus before the twentieth century most archaeologists believed that Europe was where hominins began to use and manufacture stone tools However this narrative began to change when in 1931 Louis Leakey discovered in Olduvai Gorge stone tools that seemed to be older than those found in Europe 13 Soon after Leakey formally named this discovery the Oldowan industry 14 Decades later Oldupai was the focus of two more major findings First in 1959 a skull identified as Zinjanthropus now Paranthropus boisei was found near Oldowan stone tools and this led to the argument that this hominin could be a direct ancestor of modern humans 15 However in 1964 a new fossil OH 7 was also found in association with the aforementioned stone tools 16 The connection of this hominin with Oldowan stone tools and the fact that it seemed to have a larger brain than the australopiths led to its classification as Homo habilis meaning the handyman and the assumption that stone tools were a distinctive feature of the genus Homo 16 The idea that the use of stone tools and thus culture was a unique characteristic of the genus Homo was not empirically challenged until 2010 and 2015 with the discovery of what could be the earliest archaeological evidence of cut marked bones 17 and stone tools 18 respectively However the classification of H habilis as part of the genus Homo became controversial soon after its discovery due to the use of cultural traits to justify hominin taxonomy 19 Recently the idea that human ancestors have been responsible for intentionally making stone tools has also been put into question through primate archaeology research 20 Towards the end of the twentieth century archaeology shifted from a focus on typological classifications towards an interest in understanding the mechanisms behind the origins and subsequent evolution of stone tools This conceptual turn led to the initial establishment of experiments bringing archaeology and primatology together Inspired by Jane Goodall s report of chimpanzee tool use 21 in 1972 Wright decided to teach a captive orangutan Pongo pygmaeus named Abang to make Oldowan flakes by striking a flint core to then use the flakes to open a box through imitative learning and some hand molding 22 Abang was capable of making and using the flakes only after multiple trials and not very efficiently yet it has been suggested that this inefficiency could be the result of his upbringing in captivity 23 A couple of decades later Kathy Schick and Nicholas Toth developed a similar experiment initially with a single captive enculturated bonobo Pan paniscus named Kanzi to see whether he could copy Oldowan stone tool making 5 In subsequent experiments more enculturatd bonobos started to make and use stone tools after seeing Kanzi 6 The skills of these bonobos were said to be rudimentary and even though they were taught to use freehand knapping one stone core is held in one hand and another stone in the other hand their preferred method to make flakes involved throwing the stone core against a hard surface 5 In 1994 Westergaard and Suomi decided to redo the Abang and Kanzi type of experiments but this time they ran a baseline study no demonstrations with a group of unenculturated monkeys capuchins Sapajus Cebus apella These capuchins had no exposure to stone tool making prior or during the study and they were unenculturated and so anystone tool use they would show should therefore be considered spontaneous 24 In this experiment the unenculturated capuchins spontaneously began to produce flakes by striking stones against hard surfaces Like Kanzi they did not use the freehand knapping technique normally associated with the Oldowan Researchers did not attempt further experiments involving non human primates trying to make and use stone tools until around 2020 when further baseline studies were run this time with a focus on the performance of unenculturated apes In this way orangutans got tested 25 as did chimpanzees 26 and gorillas 27 These experiments combined the use of baselines with that of demonstration conditions Primatology edit nbsp Japanese macaque Macaca fuscata Although there are prior examples of studies looking into non human primates across Western countries 28 the modern study of primate behavior originated in Japan 29 The first international journal in primatology Primates was created in Japan in 1957 30 Japanese primatology was established through the observation of free ranging Japanese macaques Macaca fuscata by Jun ichiro Itani Kinji Imanishi and Shunzo Kawamura 31 The main aim of these first primatologists was to understand the social structure of the macaques groups they were observing 30 Following their prior expertise in the individual identification of wild horses they began to give nicknames to each monkey 32 Furthermore to habituate the monkeys to their presence they began to give them food while they established a long term observation method 32 A few years later in one of the Japanese macaque sites a young female later nicknamed Imo was observed washing a sweet potato in a small stream 32 After this instance Kawamura began more systematic observations of this behavior discovering the propagation of sweet potato washing among other members of the group 32 As a result of the sweet potato washing phenomenon Japanese primatologists started to draw the first direct associations of the terms culture and subculture in association with non human animals 33 Another major contribution of Japanese primatology took place with the foundation in 1978 of the ape language project later named Ai project at the Primate Research Institute of Kyoto University with the collaboration of Kiyoko Murofushi Toshio Asano and Tetsuro Matsuzawa 31 Although the Ai project had a focus on language acquisition their methods emphasized the cognitive capabilities more than the communicative skills of the monkeys Through projects of this kind Japanese primatology highlighted the importance of merging both field and experimental settings for the study of primate behavior and cognition 31 Turning to primatology in other areas of the globe the contribution of Louis Leakey in the configuration of contemporary primatology is also notable editorializing Even though Louis Leakey was not a primatologist he was an archaeologist he believed in the importance of studying non human primates to better understand human evolution Thus it was Louis Leakey who encouraged Jane Goodall Dian Fossey and Birute Galdikas to study chimpanzee gorilla and orangutan socio ecology and behavior 34 These three women are seen as pioneers in the establishment of very long term primatological sites where the habituation of the primates through trust became very important 35 Their work continues to leave an impact on the way non human primate sites are managed and how their behavior is seen as a model to past study human behavior 35 The publication of Andrew Whiten and colleagues paper about chimpanzee cultures reinforced the prior conversations about culture in non human primates that began during the 1950s and 1960s 36 However the question as to how to define culture is a focus of debate across primatologists and other researchers such as anthropologists and archaeologists even today The main disagreement is whether only humans can have culture or if it is possible to see cultural traits in non human animals 37 Overall most definitions of a culture accepting its existence amongst non human primates agree that culture includes behaviors that can vary in their expression between social groups are maintained across generations and are socially transmitted between the members of the same group 38 However the opponents to this perspective believe that there are different types of culture and that among primates humans are unique in their ability to accumulate cultural traits over time 39 Official birth edit On top of the experimental work with non human primates towards the end of the twentieth century archaeologists began to observe chimpanzees in the wild to try to understand their behaviour and possible spatial patterns present in their living spaces 40 However the real breakthrough according to whom took place with the discovery of Panda 100 the first excavated chimpanzee nut cracking archaeological site located in the Tai Forest Ivory Coast and dated to around 4000 years ago 41 This site finally reinforced the idea that the combination of primatological and archaeological methods could shed light on both contemporary and past primate human and non human behavior Researchers realized that chimpanzees and probably other non human primates too could create long lasting patterns of material evidence that could be linked to their current behavior while they are creating that material record 8 After the publication of Carvalho Cunha Sousa and Matsuzawa in 2008 8 of what is considered one of the first studies merging archaeology and ethology from an evolutionary perspective 3 primate archaeology began to formalize Discussions about primate archaeology developing into a discipline became more concrete in 2009 at the Paleoanthropology meets Primatology conference 42 It was this conference that led to the publication of the first article encouraging the official establishment of primate archaeology as a research field 1 After this first publication studies and discoveries in primate archaeology started to grow exponentially 43 4 Tool use in non human primates editSee also Tool use by non human animals nbsp Locations of wild non human primates using tools A white faced capuchins Cebus capucinus B bearded capuchins Sapajus libidinosus C West African chimpanzees Pan troglodytes D Burmese long tailed macaques Macaca fascicularis aurea nbsp Bonobo sister taxon of the chimpanzee doing termite fishing The earliest written report of tool use in non human primates dates to 1844 and it describes chimpanzees cracking nuts 44 In The Descent of Man Charles Darwin used this initial report to mention the possible existence of culture and art in non human primates 45 Furthermore in 1887 Carpenter observed long tailed macaques Macaca fascicularis aurea pounding oysters using stones 46 After these anecdotal reports most of the early accounts of non human primates tool use came from the work of psychologists with captive subjects 47 Most of this work took place in Europe during the early twentieth century with examples such as the Anthropoid Station in Tenerife Spain dedicated to experimental research with large prosimians 28 Nonetheless the most well known example of the initial recognition of tool use in our closest relatives is Jane Goodall s report on chimpanzee termite fishing 21 When it comes to reconstructing past hominin behaviors the use of non human primates engaging in similar activities as the ones in the archaeological record is considered by some researchers as a viable methodology 48 8 49 50 However there are still some skeptics that question the use of extant non human primates as analogies of hominin tool use behaviors 51 52 This scepticism is based on anatomical differences between extant and extinct primates the possible convergent evolution of similar behaviors the diversity of past and present environments and the amount of time that has gone by since the last common ancestor of different non human primates and humans 53 However there is a mid ground to this disagreement which involves discerning which parts of different non human primate species could be used as valid evolutionary models 3 53 Overall the consensus points towards the use of multi species comparative primate models given that different taxa provide distinct benefits when it comes to understanding early hominins 3 53 For example a recent when primate archaeology example of the use of non human primates as viable models is the consolidation of the by product hypothesis for the origins of stone tools 54 The observation of chimpanzees causing the detachment of fractured stones from stone anvils as a consequence of missed hits 55 led some researchers to propose the idea that early hominins could have found themselves in similar situations and saw the mis hits as opportunities to directly use the by products or as examples of how to make flaked stone tools 56 However the by product hypothesis has gained more attention after primate archaeology field experiments demonstrated that nut cracking behavior can facilitate the production of reasonable numbers of sharp edged flakes 54 Chimpanzees Pan troglodytes edit nbsp Chimpanzee at the Bossou forest Chimpanzees are the most widely studied non human primates both in the wild 8 and in captivity 4 particularly when it comes to tool use After more than a century of intensive observation researchers began to see the broad range of behavioral variations present across chimpanzee populations During the late 1990s chimpanzees were thought to have 39 behavioral variants not all associated with tool use across six habituated African populations Bossou Tai forest Kibale Gombe Budongo Mahale M group Mahale K group 36 Some of these behavioral variants were only present in certain groups whereas others were shared amongst multiple groups independent of ecological factors These findings served as the foundation for the argument of culture in chimpanzees and thus in other non human primates 36 nbsp Representation of chimpanzee nut cracking Some of the most well known tool use examples in chimpanzees include ant dipping wood boring honey fishing leaf sponging and nut cracking 57 However chimpanzees also use tools for accessing the bone marrow of other normally smaller non human primates 57 and even for medicinal purposes by swallowing leaves 58 Chimpanzees are also capable of using two or more tools in a sequential order to achieve an end goal such as termite fishing or obtaining honey 59 Moreover chimpanzees can create compound tools in which two or more individual implements are combined to form a single working tool 59 Chimpanzees use of stone tools in foraging activities is extensive yet the most well known example is nut cracking Although some chimpanzees in the Ebo Forest of Cameroon have been indirectly observed cracking nuts 60 it is generally assumed by whom that chimpanzee nut cracking in the wild is restricted to West Africa 49 The reasons behind this phenomenon are not clear Researchers have suggested that this could be the result of cultural transmission 48 more than ecological pressures 61 given that both nut cracking and the absence of it can be seen in closely located chimpanzee populations we can Moreover nut cracking techniques can vary between chimpanzee populations with some groups like those in Bossou using stones as anvils 62 and groups like those in the Tai forest using tree roots 63 Likewise nut cracking efficiency also differs across groups 64 Chimpanzees can actively choose different stone types depending on the distance they had to travel to the selected anvil heavier stones the shorter the distance or the type of anvil they were going to use showing an ability to anticipate future sequential events 65 Robust capuchins genus Sapajus edit Capuchin stone tool use in the wild has been known at least since the eighteenth century 66 yet studies focusing on wild capuchin stone tool use became more common after the systematic observation of this behavior from the early 2000s 67 68 Prior to these observations in the wild capuchin stone tool use was thought to only occur in captivity 69 Moreover even though there has been indirect evidence of capuchins using stones for foraging in rainforest habitats 70 71 pounding behaviors such as nut cracking seem to be associated with groups that spend most of their time on the ground or in savanna like environments 69 72 73 Tool use in this taxon is mostly limited to wild robust genus Sapajus capuchins with the exception of one instance in which white faced capuchins Cebus capucinus imitator have been observed using stones to forage coastal resources on the Coiba National Park Panama 74 75 Traditionally capuchins have been assigned to a single genus Cebus However from 2012 76 there has been a growing consensus on separating capuchins species between robust genus Sapajus 77 and gracile genus Cebus mostly based on morphological and behavioral differences 78 79 yet this classification still remains controversial 80 nbsp Bearded capuchin nut crackingThere are four species of robust capuchins that have been observed using tools S apella S libidinosus S xanthosternos and S nigritus 69 For example indirect evidence of several nut cracking sites of S xanthosternos has been reported in the State of Bahia Brazil 81 However tool use is more frequently observed amongst populations of S apella 67 82 83 7 and S libidinosus In fact most accounts of tool use in wild capuchin populations involve S libidinosus 84 85 86 commonly known as black stripped capuchins This species of robust capuchins are found in the savanna like environments of the Brazilian caatinga and cerrado 69 Even though black stripped capuchins at the Serra da Capivara National Park have been observed using stones to cut wood for insects and to dig for tubers 87 88 89 the most widely studied tool use behavior in black stripped capuchins is the use of stones to crack open encapsulated foods 89 Capuchins are capable of actively selecting different stones depending on the type of encased foods they want to open 90 Moreover this type of tool use takes multiple years of learning and it can involve not just conspecifics but also other members of the social group who are the most efficient at nut cracking 91 Overall the costs and benefits of cracking open encased foods using stones for capuchins vary across individuals 92 However capuchins tend to be very efficient in this percussive behavior even though they can use stones that weight half their body mass 93 Black stripped capuchins at the Serra da Capivara National Park have also been observed engaging in stone on stone percussion which consists on striking a stone against a conglomerate 94 This behavior allows the capuchins to unintentionally break stones in a way that allows them to produce sharp edged flakes that show similar traits to those observed in the Oldowan 94 The purpose of this stone on stone behavior is unclear but it has been suggested that the capuchins consume the minerals that result from breaking the stones 94 Long tailed macaques Macaca fascicularis aurea edit nbsp Long tailed macaque tool useAlthough tool use has also been reported in other macaque species e g Macaca fuscata 95 96 primate archaeological research has focused on Thai long tailed macaques Macaca fascicularis aurea 20 97 98 given that stone tool use in this species is associated with foraging After Carpenter s mention of long tailed macaques doing oyster cracking 46 there was virtually no mention of this type of behavior in the literature associated with this taxon However in 2007 this foraging behavior was observed in two long tailed macaque populations inhabiting the Piak Nam Yai Island Laem Son National Park in southern Thailand 99 It was this re discovery of stone tool use for foraging that sparked the exponential growth of systematic studies on macaque tool use 97 100 101 A few years later long tailed macaques were also observed using stones to crack open oil palm nuts 102 103 Recent primate archaeology studies have discovered that long tailed macaques choose different stone tool shapes based on individual preference and the different types of food they want to open 101 104 This prompted researchers to propose different terminology depending on the type of stone tools the macaques use They normally differentiate between point axe hammering associated with a smaller size with pointy ends pound face hammering associated with larger and rounder stones and edge hammering mostly related to the use of the edge of the stone tool and applied both for cracking oysters and molluscs 105 106 In long tailed macaques stone tool use seems to be restricted to the Macaca fascicularis aurea subspecies because Macaca fascicularis fascicularis has never been observed using tools in the wild or captivity 99 Both subspecies share the same habitat but they have developed different strategies to forage similar resources It is still not clear why this happens but it has been suggested that it could be the result of a genetic component because long tail hybrids of both Macaca fascicularis aurea and Macaca fascicularis fascicularis with a stronger component of the former show a bias towards the use of stones 107 Other non human primates edit Although they have never been seen to undertake stone tool percussive activities in the wild recent studies trying to comprehend the possible mechanisms behind the emergence of stone tool technology have started to run experiments with non human primates in captivity or in free ranging environments There seems to be a particular focus on great apes such as gorillas Gorilla spp orangutans Pongo spp and bonobos Pan paniscus due to their closer phylogenetic association with the genus Homo 108 However experimental studies trying to understand the emergence of stone tool technology still show a clear bias towards bonobos and orangutans given that gorillas are yet to be seen to use stones as tools both in the wild and in captivity 53 nbsp Bonobo Pan paniscus Wild orangutans have never been seen using stone tools but orangutans in captivity have been the focus of experimental studies testing their ability to use stone tools For example captive naive orangutans have been reported to spontaneously use wooden hammers to crack open nuts 109 Following a similar pattern to Kanzi s experiments experiments with captive orangutans demonstrated that when presented with the right learning stimulus this great ape can engage in stone tool use and manufacture 25 However the flakes made by the captive orangutans were low in number and did not resemble stone flakes made by early hominins citation needed Although bonobos have been observed using tools in association with thirteen different behaviors only one of them leaf sponging for drinking water has a foraging purpose 110 111 Moreover no stone tool use has been reported in wild bonobos This contrasts with the forty two different tool use behaviors observed in chimpanzees of which more than half are associated with foraging 112 The reason why bonobos and chimpanzees are so distinct in their tool use behaviors is still not clear One possibility is that there are very few long term wild habituated bonobo sites LuiKotale Lomako and Wamba 113 which are very difficult to access 114 Another possible explanation is that chimpanzees and bonobos have different motivations social ecological or innate that lead them to different predispositions when it comes to using tools 112 115 However despite the absence of stone tool use in the wild studies of bonobo tool use in captivity and free ranging communities are becoming more common 116 117 Methods editExcavation and site formation processes edit nbsp Serra da Capivara National Park In archaeology excavation is an essential element of the discipline because it allows archaeologists to trace when specific behaviors originated and their subsequent changes over time 2 Through excavation archaeologists can obtain data on the chronology the spatial distribution of the artefacts and the stratigraphic and depositional contexts of the site allowing them to reconstruct the natural and cultural events 118 that led to the formation of that particular site 2 Particularly in primate archaeology the excavation of non human primate tool sites has proved to be valuable for tracing back the history of nut cracking sites and investigating the changes of this particular percussive behavior 119 98 120 The first non human primate archaeological site was found in 2002 in the Tai Forest Ivory Coast and it was associated with chimpanzee tool use dating back 4000 years 41 After this in 2016 a group of primate archaeologists using archaeological methods discovered a long tailed macaque archaeological site mapping shellfish processing activities at Piak Nam Yai Island southern Thailand 98 Finally another group of primate archaeologists discovered a capuchin nut cracking site dated to around 2400 years ago at Serra da Capivara National Park Brazil 120 Archaeological excavation techniques can be time consuming and costly Furthermore excavations are very invasive given that once you excavate something you will not be able to put it back into its original state 2 Primate archaeology has the advantage of being able to do present day surface surveys because they study extant non human primate behavior and their tools 8 Surface surveys allow primate archaeologists to look into the spatial distribution of the tools used by present non human primates thus providing useful models to reconstruct past site formation processes For example a recent landscape agent based modelling of chimpanzee nut cracking stone tool fragments showed that nut cracking sites have the potential to form dense clusters of stone tools that scatter across the landscape 121 Use wear and residue analysis edit Use wear is a technique that consists of identifying the different damage marks present on stone tool edges to understand how toolmakers used these implements Before the expansion of major technological advances in archaeology use wear analyses were mostly done from a qualitative and macroscopic perspective 122 However with the inclusion of microscopes and 3D scanners use wear analyses began to be approached more from a microscopic and quantitative perspective 123 124 Through use wear analyses primate archaeologists can design a protocol that can allow them to differentiate stones that have been used as tools from those that were not even if they cannot observe the primates behavior 2 123 43 Likewise in some instances they are also capable of reconstructing the particular behavior the tool was used for For example through the use wear analysis of percussive tools using 3D surface morphometrics archaeologists discovered that different macaque behaviours leave specific traces on the stones 104 Residue analysis adds another dimension to use wear studies by looking into residual evidence that is left on tool edges Normally tools are inspected using use wear techniques and if relevant residues are found within traces they will be extracted and further analysed 125 This method allows primate archaeologists to know the exact foods that non human primates are processing with stones even if they cannot observe the behavior For example a recent study looked into bearded capuchin monkey pounding tools from Serra da Capivara Brazil In this study researchers found that traces on capuchin stone tools reflect behavioral differences 126 The application of these techniques in primate archaeology allows researchers to begin to draw comparisons between the tools used by different non human primate species 104 and contrast those tools with the stones found in the archaeological record 2 123 Technological analysis edit Lithic technological analysis entails the study of the different attributes present in stone tools such as flakes through measurements qualitative observations and possible subsequent typological classifications 127 With the added method of 3D geometric morphometrics archaeologists can develop a more in depth and quantitative analysis of the morphological variation present in stone tools The application of these methods in primate archaeology has generated studies that are capable of comparing the flakes unintentionally produced by non human primate species with flakes associated with hominins in the archaeological record 128 Chimpanzees robust capuchins and long tailed macaques use stone tools to crack encased foods and even though they can be quite efficient they can still have mishits 55 These mishits can end up unintentionally producing flakes that could resemble the flakes produced by hominins 54 Thus to test the extent of the differences and similarities between non human primate and hominin flakes primate archaeologists have used technological and 3D geometric morphometric analyses to compare capuchin quartzite stone on stone flakes 94 with flakes produced by a contemporary human knapper using freehand and passive hammer knapping 128 Moreover in this study primate archaeologists also compared the capuchin unintentional flakes with Oldowan flakes Overall these analyses concluded that most of the technological attributes present in the capuchin unintentional flakes are shared with the intentionally made flakes yet some substantial differences e g external platform angle could help to differentiate these non human primate flakes from those found in the archaeological record 128 Likewise technological analysis has been used to study the attributes of archaeological flakes found in the chimpanzee nut cracking site of Panda 100 allowing primate archaeologists to create data that can be used to find more sites and also to better understand modern chimpanzees tool use 129 Implications for the study of human evolution editMost of the major contributions of primate archaeology are associated with the importance of finding the earliest stone tools 20 128 54 The search for the origins of stone tool use and making has been one of the major aims of archaeology almost since it became a discipline 9 Moreover reconstructing the emergence of stone tool technology in human evolution goes hand in hand with questions in biological anthropology about the behavior and anatomical constraints of early hominins 130 131 Through the analysis of present day non human primate stone tool use primate archaeology provides a unique opportunity to compare observed behaviors with the tools and the traces they leave behind 1 3 2 Primate archaeological research has been able to challenge the view that only stone tools could have played an essential role in shaping human evolution 1 3 Organic tool use such as leaves and twigs in non human primates 36 points towards the also likely existence on these types of tools in all hominins Moreover the occurrence of tool use and even the unintentional production of flakes in extant non human primates questions the idea that only hominins were the sole creators of archaeological sites 1 3 The discovery of chimpanzee 41 macaque 98 and capuchin 120 archaeological sites further strengthen that perspective The use of tools by this wide range of non human primate taxa also provides data in the reconstruction of the different environmental anatomical behavioral and or genetic factors that could lead to the emergence and subsequent evolution of stone tool manufacture and use across different species 1 3 The question of whether non human animals have human like culture has been the focus of debate in disciplines such as primatology for many decades 38 132 Although the non human animal culture controversy is far from being over 133 37 primate archaeology has contributed to this debate with several recent studies A primate archaeology study in 2015 suggested the existence of social learning and cumulative culture in chimpanzees given that they could not introduce nut cracking independently 134 To test the hypothesis of cumulative culture in chimpanzees a recent study tested whether a group of chimpanzees at Seringbara Nimba Mountains Guinea could independently re invent nut cracking 135 This study concluded that chimpanzee nut cracking requires social learning because the observed chimpanzees could not re invent nut cracking despite being in ecologically valid conditions 135 However these primate archaeology studies still find opposition from scholars that firmly believe in the absence of cumulative culture in non human primates who pointed out that the tested chimpanzees lacked a food interest in the provided nuts 136 Moreover the involvement of primate archaeology in the non human animal culture debate has been extended to questions on the existence or lack thereof of human like culture in the early archaeological record 132 137 138 Through the use of non human primate tool use behaviors researchers try to reconstruct when how and why humans began to rely on high fidelity cultural transmission Previous experiments run with unenculturated capuchin monkeys 24 and orangutans 25 which found spontaneous reinnovation of stone tool making inspired the possibility that early stone tools such as the Oldowan and even the Acheulean may not have been know how requiring social learning thus they may not have been early examples of cumulative culture 139 The possible re innovation of stone tool making was tested in a recent experiment with naive humans who were never exposed to stone tool knapping know how 140 Overall most subjects of this experiment were able to independently make stone tools using all four known early knapping techniques Nonetheless the debate as to whether early stone tools reflect cumulative culture or not is still not settled 132 133 139 135 136 37 See also editLomekwi History of archaeologyReferences edit a b c d e f g h Haslam Michael Hernandez Aguilar Adriana Ling Victoria Carvalho Susana de la Torre Ignacio DeStefano April Du Andrew Hardy Bruce Harris Jack Marchant Linda Matsuzawa Tetsuro McGrew William Mercader Julio Mora Rafael Petraglia Michael 2009 Primate archaeology Nature 460 7253 339 344 Bibcode 2009Natur 460 339H doi 10 1038 nature08188 ISSN 0028 0836 PMID 19606139 S2CID 30108964 a b c d e f g h Carvalho S and Almeida Warren K 2019 Primate archaeology In Encyclopedia of animal behavior Academic Press 397 407 a b c d e f g h i Haslam Michael Hernandez Aguilar R Adriana Proffitt Tomos Arroyo Adrian Falotico Tiago Fragaszy Dorothy Gumert Michael Harris John W K Huffman Michael A Kalan Ammie K Malaivijitnond Suchinda Matsuzawa Tetsuro McGrew William Ottoni Eduardo B Pascual Garrido Alejandra 2017 Primate archaeology evolves Nature Ecology amp Evolution 1 10 1431 1437 doi 10 1038 s41559 017 0286 4 ISSN 2397 334X PMID 29185525 S2CID 256708334 a b c Arroyo Adrian Hirata Satoshi Matsuzawa Tetsuro de la Torre Ignacio 2016 Nut Cracking Tools Used by Captive Chimpanzees Pan troglodytes and Their Comparison with Early Stone Age Percussive Artefacts from Olduvai Gorge PLOS ONE 11 11 e0166788 Bibcode 2016PLoSO 1166788A doi 10 1371 journal pone 0166788 ISSN 1932 6203 PMC 5117719 PMID 27870877 a b c Toth Nicholas Schick Kathy D Savage Rumbaugh E Sue Sevcik Rose A Rumbaugh Duane M 1993 Pan the Tool Maker Investigations into the Stone Tool Making and Tool Using Capabilities of a Bonobo Pan paniscus Journal of Archaeological Science 20 1 81 91 Bibcode 1993JArSc 20 81T doi 10 1006 jasc 1993 1006 a b Schick Kathy D Toth Nicholas Garufi Gary Savage Rumbaugh E Sue Rumbaugh Duane Sevcik Rose 1999 Continuing Investigations into the Stone Tool making and Tool using Capabilities of a Bonobo Pan paniscus Journal of Archaeological Science 26 7 821 832 Bibcode 1999JArSc 26 821S doi 10 1006 jasc 1998 0350 a b Westergaard Gregory C Fragaszy Dorothy M 1987 The manufacture and use of tools by capuchin monkeys Cebus apella Journal of Comparative Psychology 101 2 159 168 doi 10 1037 0735 7036 101 2 159 ISSN 1939 2087 a b c d e f Carvalho Susana Cunha Eugenia Sousa Claudia Matsuzawa Tetsuro 2008 Chaines operatoires and resource exploitation strategies in chimpanzee Pan troglodytes nut cracking Journal of Human Evolution 55 1 148 163 doi 10 1016 j jhevol 2008 02 005 hdl 10316 3758 PMID 18359504 a b c d e de la Torre Ignacio 2011 The origins of stone tool technology in Africa a historical perspective Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences 366 1567 1028 1037 doi 10 1098 rstb 2010 0350 ISSN 0962 8436 PMC 3049100 PMID 21357225 Romeo Luigi 1979 01 01 Ecce Homo A Lexicon of Man John Benjamins Publishing ISBN 978 90 272 7452 6 Warren S H 1905 On the origin of Eoliths Man 5 179 183 Dawson C and Woodward A S 1913 On the discovery of a Palaeolithic human skull and mandible in a flint bearing gravel overlying the Wealden Hastings Beds at Piltdown Fletching Sussex Quarterly journal of the geological society 69 1 4 pp 117 123 Leakey L S B Hopwood Arthur T Reck Hans 1931 New Yields from the Oldoway Bone Beds Tanganyika Territory Nature 128 3243 1075 Bibcode 1931Natur 128 1075L doi 10 1038 1281075a0 ISSN 0028 0836 S2CID 4077834 Leakey L S B 1934 The sequence of Stone Age cultures in east Africa In Evans Pritchard E E Firth R Malinowski B Schapera I Eds Essays presented to CG Seligman London UK Kegan Paul Trench Trubner amp Co Limited pp 143 146 Leakey L S B 1961 Africa s Contribution to the Evolution of Man The South African Archaeological Bulletin 16 61 3 7 doi 10 2307 3887411 JSTOR 3887411 a b Leakey L S B Tobias P V Napier J R 1964 A new species of the genus Homo from Olduvai Gorge Nature 202 5 7 McPherron Shannon P Alemseged Zeresenay Marean Curtis W Wynn Jonathan G Reed Denne Geraads Denis Bobe Rene Bearat Hamdallah A 2010 Evidence for stone tool assisted consumption of animal tissues before 3 39 million years ago at Dikika Ethiopia Nature 466 7308 857 860 Bibcode 2010Natur 466 857M doi 10 1038 nature09248 ISSN 0028 0836 PMID 20703305 S2CID 4356816 Harmand Sonia Lewis Jason E Feibel Craig S Lepre Christopher J Prat Sandrine Lenoble Arnaud Boes Xavier Quinn Rhonda L Brenet Michel Arroyo Adrian Taylor Nicholas Clement Sophie Daver Guillaume Brugal Jean Philip Leakey Louise 2015 3 3 million year old stone tools from Lomekwi 3 West Turkana Kenya Nature 521 7552 310 315 Bibcode 2015Natur 521 310H doi 10 1038 nature14464 ISSN 0028 0836 PMID 25993961 S2CID 1207285 Wood B and Collard M 1999 The changing face of genus Homo Evolutionary Anthropology Issues News and Reviews Issues News and Reviews 8 6 195 207 a b c Proffitt Tomos Reeves Jonathan S Braun David R Malaivijitnond Suchinda Luncz Lydia V 2023 Wild macaques challenge the origin of intentional tool production Science Advances 9 10 eade8159 Bibcode 2023SciA 9E8159P doi 10 1126 sciadv ade8159 PMC 10005173 PMID 36897944 S2CID 257437132 a b Van Lawick Goodall Jane 1968 The Behaviour of Free living Chimpanzees in the Gombe Stream Reserve Animal Behaviour Monographs 1 161 IN12 doi 10 1016 s0066 1856 68 80003 2 ISSN 0066 1856 Wright R V 1972 Imitative learning of a flaked stone technology the case of an orangutan The Australian Journal of Anthropology 8 4 296 306 Haslam Michael 2013 Captivity bias in animal tool use and its implications for the evolution of hominin technology Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences 368 1630 20120421 doi 10 1098 rstb 2012 0421 ISSN 0962 8436 PMC 4027414 PMID 24101629 a b Westergaard Gregory Charles Suomi Stephen J 1994 A simple stone tool technology in monkeys Journal of Human Evolution 27 5 399 404 doi 10 1006 jhev 1994 1055 a b c Motes Rodrigo Alba McPherron Shannon P Archer Will Hernandez Aguilar R Adriana Tennie Claudio 2022 Experimental investigation of orangutans lithic percussive and sharp stone tool behaviours PLOS ONE 17 2 e0263343 Bibcode 2022PLoSO 1763343M doi 10 1371 journal pone 0263343 ISSN 1932 6203 PMC 8849460 PMID 35171926 https open research europe ec europa eu articles 1 20 v2 https osf io preprints qj3kn a b Kohler W 1925 The Mentality of Apes Kegan Paul Trench and Trubner London de Waal Frans B M 2003 Silent invasion Imanishi s primatology and cultural bias in science Animal Cognition 6 4 293 299 doi 10 1007 s10071 003 0197 4 ISSN 1435 9448 PMID 14551801 S2CID 45665875 a b Matsuzawa Tetsuro Yamagiwa Juichi 2018 Primatology the beginning Primates 59 4 313 326 doi 10 1007 s10329 018 0672 9 ISSN 0032 8332 PMID 29982936 S2CID 254157644 a b c Matsuzawa Tetsuro 2003 The Ai project historical and ecological contexts Animal Cognition 6 4 199 211 doi 10 1007 s10071 003 0199 2 ISSN 1435 9448 PMID 14566577 S2CID 8928490 a b c d Matsuzawa Tetsuro McGrew William C 2008 Kinji Imanishi and 60 years of Japanese primatology Current Biology 18 14 R587 R591 doi 10 1016 j cub 2008 05 040 ISSN 0960 9822 PMID 18644329 S2CID 13572608 Kawamura S 1959 The process of sub culture propagation among Japanese macaques Primates 2 1 pp 43 60 Morell Virginia 1993 Called Trimates Three Bold Women Shaped Their Field Science 260 5106 420 425 Bibcode 1993Sci 260 420M doi 10 1126 science 260 5106 420 ISSN 0036 8075 PMID 17838264 S2CID 5060859 a b Montgomery Sy 2009 Walking with the Great Apes Jane Goodall Dian Fossey Birute Galdikas Chelsea Green Publishing ISBN 978 1 60358 244 5 OCLC 1021804170 a b c d Whiten A Goodall J McGrew W C Nishida T Reynolds V Sugiyama Y Tutin C E G Wrangham R W Boesch C 1999 Cultures in chimpanzees Nature 399 6737 682 685 Bibcode 1999Natur 399 682W doi 10 1038 21415 ISSN 1476 4687 PMID 10385119 S2CID 4385871 a b c Boesch Christophe Kalan Ammie K Mundry Roger Arandjelovic Mimi Pika Simone Dieguez Paula Ayimisin Emmanuel Ayuk Barciela Amanda Coupland Charlotte Egbe Villard Ebot Eno Nku Manasseh Michael Fay J Fine David Adriana Hernandez Aguilar R Hermans Veerle 2020 Chimpanzee ethnography reveals unexpected cultural diversity Nature Human Behaviour 4 9 910 916 doi 10 1038 s41562 020 0890 1 ISSN 2397 3374 PMID 32451479 S2CID 256706922 a b Humle Tatyana Newton Fisher Nicholas E 2013 Chapter 2 Culture in Non human Primates Definitions and Evidence Understanding Cultural Transmission in Anthropology Berghahn Books pp 80 101 doi 10 1515 9780857459947 006 ISBN 978 0 85745 994 7 Tomasello Michael Carpenter Malinda Call Josep Behne Tanya Moll Henrike 2005 Understanding and sharing intentions The origins of cultural cognition Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28 5 675 691 doi 10 1017 S0140525X05000129 ISSN 0140 525X PMID 16262930 S2CID 3900485 Sept Jeanne M King Barbara J McGrew W C Moore Jim Paterson James D Strier Karen B Uehara Shigeo Whiten Andrew Wrangham Richard W 1992 Was There No Place Like Home A New Perspective on Early Hominid Archaeological Sites From the Mapping of Chimpanzee Nests and Comments and Reply Current Anthropology 33 2 187 207 doi 10 1086 204050 ISSN 0011 3204 S2CID 145013951 a b c Mercader Julio Panger Melissa Boesch Christophe 2002 Excavation of a Chimpanzee Stone Tool Site in the African Rainforest Science 296 5572 1452 1455 Bibcode 2002Sci 296 1452M doi 10 1126 science 1070268 ISSN 0036 8075 PMID 12029130 S2CID 21042746 Ling Victoria Hernandez Aguilar Adriana Haslam Michael Carvalho Susana 2009 The origins of percussive technology A smashing time in Cambridge Evolutionary Anthropology Issues News and Reviews 18 2 48 49 doi 10 1002 evan 20197 S2CID 85989573 a b Benito Calvo Alfonso Carvalho Susana Arroyo Adrian Matsuzawa Tetsuro de la Torre Ignacio 2015 Addessi Elsa ed First GIS Analysis of Modern Stone Tools Used by Wild Chimpanzees Pan troglodytes verus in Bossou Guinea West Africa PLOS ONE 10 3 e0121613 Bibcode 2015PLoSO 1021613B doi 10 1371 journal pone 0121613 ISSN 1932 6203 PMC 4368754 PMID 25793642 Savage Thomas S 1844 Observations on the habits of the python natalensis Annals and Magazine of Natural History 14 89 148 150 doi 10 1080 037454809495158 ISSN 0374 5481 Darwin Charles 1871 The descent of man and selection in relation to sex New York D Appleton and company doi 10 5962 bhl title 24784 a b Carpenter Alfred 1887 Monkeys opening Oysters Nature 36 916 53 Bibcode 1887Natur 36 53C doi 10 1038 036053d0 ISSN 0028 0836 S2CID 4112014 Beck B B 1980 Animal Tool Behavior Garland STPM Pub a b Whiten Andrew Schick Kathy Toth Nicholas 2009 The evolution and cultural transmission of percussive technology integrating evidence from palaeoanthropology and primatology Journal of Human Evolution 57 4 420 435 doi 10 1016 j jhevol 2008 12 010 ISSN 0047 2484 PMID 19740521 a b Carvalho Susana Matsuzawa Tetsuro McGrew William C 2013 Boesch Christophe Sanz Crickette M Call Josep eds From pounding to knapping How chimpanzees can help us to model hominin lithics Tool Use in Animals Cognition and Ecology Cambridge Cambridge University Press pp 225 241 ISBN 978 0 511 89480 0 retrieved 2023 04 10 Wynn Thomas Hernandez Aguilar R Adriana Marchant Linda F Mcgrew William C 2011 An ape s view of the Oldowan revisited Evolutionary Anthropology Issues News and Reviews 20 5 181 197 doi 10 1002 evan 20323 ISSN 1060 1538 PMID 22034236 S2CID 23910905 Sayers K and Lovejoy C O 2008 The chimpanzee has no clothes A critical examination of Pan troglodytes in models of human evolution Current Anthropology 49 1 pp 87 99 Sayers Ken Raghanti Mary Ann Lovejoy C Owen 2012 Human Evolution and the Chimpanzee Referential Doctrine Annual Review of Anthropology 41 1 119 138 doi 10 1146 annurev anthro 092611 145815 ISSN 0084 6570 a b c d Bandini Elisa Harrison Rachel A Motes Rodrigo Alba 2022 Examining the suitability of extant primates as models of hominin stone tool culture Humanities and Social Sciences Communications 9 1 74 doi 10 1057 s41599 022 01091 x ISSN 2662 9992 S2CID 257090939 a b c d Luncz Lydia V Arroyo Adrian Falotico Tiago Quinn Patrick Proffitt Tomos 2022 A primate model for the origin of flake technology Journal of Human Evolution 171 103250 doi 10 1016 j jhevol 2022 103250 PMID 36122461 S2CID 252347187 a b Hannah Alison C McGrew W C 1987 Chimpanzees using stones to crack open oil palm nuts in Liberia Primates 28 1 31 46 doi 10 1007 BF02382181 ISSN 1610 7365 S2CID 24738945 Davidson Iain McGrew William C 2005 Stone tools and the uniqueness of human culture Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute 11 4 793 817 doi 10 1111 j 1467 9655 2005 00262 x ISSN 1359 0987 a b Boesch Christophe Boesch Hedwige 1990 Tool Use and Tool Making in Wild Chimpanzees Folia Primatologica 54 1 2 86 99 doi 10 1159 000156428 ISSN 0015 5713 PMID 2157651 Wrangham R W and Goodall J 2013 Chimpanzee use of medicinal leaves In Understanding chimpanzees pp 22 37 Harvard University Press a b McGrew William C 2010 Chimpanzee Technology Science 328 5978 579 580 Bibcode 2010Sci 328 579M doi 10 1126 science 1187921 ISSN 0036 8075 PMID 20431004 S2CID 8772765 Morgan Bethan J Abwe Ekwoge E 2006 Chimpanzees use stone hammers in Cameroon Current Biology 16 16 R632 R633 doi 10 1016 j cub 2006 07 045 ISSN 0960 9822 PMID 16920608 S2CID 31639484 Biro Dora Inoue Nakamura Noriko Tonooka Rikako Yamakoshi Gen Sousa Claudia Matsuzawa Tetsuro 2003 Cultural innovation and transmission of tool use in wild chimpanzees evidence from field experiments Animal Cognition 6 4 213 223 doi 10 1007 s10071 003 0183 x ISSN 1435 9448 PMID 12898285 S2CID 651756 Carvalho Susana Biro Dora McGrew William C Matsuzawa Tetsuro 2009 Tool composite reuse in wild chimpanzees Pan troglodytes archaeologically invisible steps in the technological evolution of early hominins Animal Cognition 12 S1 103 114 doi 10 1007 s10071 009 0271 7 ISSN 1435 9448 PMID 19680699 S2CID 22737853 Luncz Lydia V Mundry Roger Boesch Christophe 2012 Evidence for Cultural Differences between Neighboring Chimpanzee Communities Current Biology 22 10 922 926 doi 10 1016 j cub 2012 03 031 ISSN 0960 9822 PMID 22578420 S2CID 3191082 Luncz Lydia V Sirianni Giulia Mundry Roger Boesch Christophe 2018 Costly culture differences in nut cracking efficiency between wild chimpanzee groups Animal Behaviour 137 63 73 doi 10 1016 j anbehav 2017 12 017 ISSN 0003 3472 S2CID 53205528 Sirianni Giulia Mundry Roger Boesch Christophe 2015 When to choose which tool multidimensional and conditional selection of nut cracking hammers in wild chimpanzees Animal Behaviour 100 152 165 doi 10 1016 j anbehav 2014 11 022 ISSN 0003 3472 S2CID 53192010 Visalberghi Elisabetta 1990 Tool Use in Cebus Folia Primatologica 54 3 4 146 154 doi 10 1159 000156438 ISSN 0015 5713 PMID 2202616 a b Ottoni Eduardo B Mannu Massimo 2001 Semifree ranging tufted capuchins Cebus apella spontaneously use tools to crack open nuts International Journal of Primatology 22 3 347 358 doi 10 1023 A 1010747426841 S2CID 29768277 Fragaszy Dorothy Izar Patricia Visalberghi Elisabetta Ottoni Eduardo B de Oliveira Marino Gomes 2004 Wild capuchin monkeys Cebus libidinosus use anvils and stone pounding tools American Journal of Primatology 64 4 359 366 doi 10 1002 ajp 20085 ISSN 0275 2565 PMID 15580579 S2CID 16222308 a b c d Ottoni Eduardo B Izar Patricia 2008 Capuchin monkey tool use Overview and implications Evolutionary Anthropology Issues News and Reviews 17 4 171 178 doi 10 1002 evan 20185 ISSN 1060 1538 S2CID 83604383 A Langguth 1997 Capuchin monkeys in the Caatinga tool use and food habits during drought Neo Primates 5 3 77 78 Fernandes Marcus E B 1991 Tool use and predation of oysters Crassostrea rhizophorae by the tufted capuchin Cebus apella appella in brackish water mangrove swamp Primates 32 4 529 531 doi 10 1007 BF02381944 ISSN 1610 7365 S2CID 19951712 Visalberghi Elisabetta Fragaszy Dorothy Munkenbeck Izar Patricia Ottoni Eduardo B 2005 Terrestriality and Tool Use Science 308 5724 951 952 doi 10 1126 science 308 5724 951c ISSN 0036 8075 PMID 15890860 S2CID 29819009 Meulman Ellen J M Sanz Crickette M Visalberghi Elisabetta van Schaik Carel P 2012 The Role of Terrestriality in Promoting Primate Technology Evolutionary Anthropology Issues News and Reviews 21 2 58 68 doi 10 1002 evan 21304 ISSN 1060 1538 PMID 22499440 S2CID 31022370 Barrett Brendan J Monteza moreno Claudio M Dogandzic Tamara Zwyns Nicolas Ibanez Alicia Crofoot Margaret C 2018 Habitual Stone Tool Aided Extractive Foraging in White Faced Capuchins Cebus Capucinus Royal Society Open Science 5 8 351619 bioRxiv 10 1101 351619 doi 10 1098 rsos 181002 PMC 6124021 PMID 30225086 S2CID 52286411 Monteza Moreno Claudio M Dogandzic Tamara McLean Kevin A Castillo Caballero Pedro L Mijango Ramos Zarluis Del Rosario Vargas Evelyn Crofoot Margaret C Barrett Brendan J 2020 White Faced Capuchin Cebus capucinus imitator Hammerstone and Anvil Tool Use in Riparian Habitats on Coiba Island Panama International Journal of Primatology 41 3 429 433 doi 10 1007 s10764 020 00156 5 ISSN 0164 0291 S2CID 218773276 Alfaro Jessica W Lynch Silva Jose de Sousa E Rylands Anthony B 2012 How Different Are Robust and Gracile Capuchin Monkeys An Argument for the Use of Sapajus and Cebus American Journal of Primatology 74 4 273 286 doi 10 1002 ajp 22007 ISSN 0275 2565 PMID 22328205 S2CID 18840598 Linne Carl von 1792 The animal kingdom or zoological system of the celebrated Sir Charles Linnaeus containing a complete systematic description arrangement and nomenclature of all the known species and varieties of the mammalia or animals which give suck to their young Printed for A Strahan and T Cadell London and W Creech Edinburgh OCLC 1039518575 Alfaro Jessica W Lynch Matthews Luke Boyette Adam H Macfarlan Shane J Phillips Kimberley A Falotico Tiago Ottoni Eduardo Verderane Michele Izar Patricia Schulte Meredith Melin Amanda Fedigan Linda Janson Charles Alfaro Michael E 2011 Anointing variation across wild capuchin populations a review of material preferences bout frequency and anointing sociality in Cebus and Sapajus American Journal of Primatology 74 4 299 314 doi 10 1002 ajp 20971 ISSN 0275 2565 PMID 21769906 S2CID 14560688 Hogg Russell T Elokda Abdallah 2021 Quantification of enamel decussation in gracile and robust capuchins Cebus Sapajus Cebidae Platyrrhini American Journal of Primatology 83 5 e23246 doi 10 1002 ajp 23246 ISSN 0275 2565 PMID 33638563 S2CID 232064804 Balolia Katharine L Wulff Alexandra 2022 One Genus or Two Evaluating Whether Gracile and Robust Capuchin Monkeys are Validly Classified as Separate Genera Based on Craniofacial Shape International Journal of Primatology 43 5 798 821 doi 10 1007 s10764 022 00300 3 hdl 1885 298237 ISSN 0164 0291 S2CID 251008044 Canale Gustavo Rodrigues Guidorizzi Carlos Eduardo Kierulff Maria Cecilia Martins Gatto Cassiano Augusto Ferreira Rodrigues 2009 First record of tool use by wild populations of the yellow breasted capuchin monkey Cebus xanthosternos and new records for the bearded capuchin Cebus libidinosus American Journal of Primatology 71 5 366 372 doi 10 1002 ajp 20648 ISSN 0275 2565 PMID 19206141 S2CID 38883635 Boinski Sue Quatrone Robert P Swartz Hilary 2000 Substrate and Tool Use by Brown Capuchins in Suriname Ecological Contexts and Cognitive Bases American Anthropologist 102 4 741 761 doi 10 1525 aa 2000 102 4 741 ISSN 0002 7294 Ottoni Eduardo B de Resende Briseida Dogo Izar Patricia 2005 Watching the best nutcrackers what capuchin monkeys Cebus apella know about others tool using skills Animal Cognition 8 4 215 219 doi 10 1007 s10071 004 0245 8 ISSN 1435 9456 PMID 15719240 S2CID 51862020 Falotico Tiago Coutinho Paulo Henrique M Bueno Carolina Q Rufo Henrique P Ottoni Eduardo B 2018 Stone tool use by wild capuchin monkeys Sapajus libidinosus at Serra das Confusoes National Park Brazil Primates 59 4 385 394 doi 10 1007 s10329 018 0660 0 ISSN 0032 8332 PMID 29550951 S2CID 254158889 Visalberghi Elisabetta Barca Virginia Izar Patricia Fragaszy Dorothy Truppa Valentina 2021 Optional tool use The case of wild bearded capuchins Sapajus libidinosus cracking cashew nuts by biting or by using percussors American Journal of Primatology 83 1 e23221 doi 10 1002 ajp 23221 ISSN 0275 2565 PMID 33300618 S2CID 228090909 Truppa Valentina Sabbatini Gloria Izar Patricia Fragaszy Dorothy M Visalberghi Elisabetta 2021 Anticipating future actions Motor planning improves with age in wild bearded capuchin monkeys Sapajus libidinosus Developmental Science 24 4 e13077 doi 10 1111 desc 13077 ISSN 1363 755X PMID 33342007 S2CID 229342997 Mannu Massimo Ottoni Eduardo B 2009 The enhanced tool kit of two groups of wild bearded capuchin monkeys in the Caatinga tool making associative use and secondary tools American Journal of Primatology 71 3 242 251 doi 10 1002 ajp 20642 ISSN 0275 2565 PMID 19051323 S2CID 3479020 de A Moura A C Lee P C 2004 Capuchin Stone Tool Use in Caatinga Dry Forest Science 306 5703 1909 doi 10 1126 science 1102558 ISSN 0036 8075 PMID 15591195 S2CID 36793833 a b Falotico Tiago Ottoni Eduardo B 2016 The manifold use of pounding stone tools by wild capuchin monkeys of Serra da Capivara National Park Brazil Behaviour 153 4 421 442 doi 10 1163 1568539X 00003357 ISSN 0005 7959 Visalberghi Elisabetta Addessi Elsa Truppa Valentina Spagnoletti Noemi Ottoni Eduardo Izar Patricia Fragaszy Dorothy 2009 Selection of Effective Stone Tools by Wild Bearded Capuchin Monkeys Current Biology 19 3 213 217 doi 10 1016 j cub 2008 11 064 PMID 19147358 S2CID 7258497 Coelho C G Falotico T Izar P Mannu M Resende B D Siqueira J O Ottoni E B 2015 Social learning strategies for nut cracking by tufted capuchin monkeys Sapajus spp Animal Cognition 18 4 911 919 doi 10 1007 s10071 015 0861 5 ISSN 1435 9448 PMID 25800169 S2CID 254134412 Fragaszy D Pickering T Liu Q Izar P Ottoni E Visalberghi E 2010 02 01 Bearded capuchin monkeys and a human s efficiency at cracking palm nuts with stone tools field experiments Animal Behaviour 79 2 321 332 doi 10 1016 j anbehav 2009 11 004 ISSN 0003 3472 S2CID 11137182 Liu Q Simpson K Izar P Ottoni E Visalberghi E Fragaszy D 2009 Kinematics and energetics of nut cracking in wild capuchin monkeys Cebus libidinosus in Piaui Brazil American Journal of Physical Anthropology 138 2 210 220 doi 10 1002 ajpa 20920 ISSN 0002 9483 PMID 18785652 a b c d Proffitt Tomos Luncz Lydia V Falotico Tiago Ottoni Eduardo B de la Torre Ignacio Haslam Michael 2016 11 03 Wild monkeys flake stone tools Nature 539 7627 85 88 Bibcode 2016Natur 539 85P doi 10 1038 nature20112 ISSN 0028 0836 PMID 27760117 S2CID 205251548 Huffman Michael A Quiatt Duane 1986 Stone handling by Japanese macaques Macaca fuscata Implications for tool use of stone Primates 27 4 413 423 doi 10 1007 BF02381887 ISSN 1610 7365 S2CID 25426387 Gunst Noelle Huffman Michael Leca Jean Baptiste 2007 Japanese macaque cultures Inter and intra troop behavioural variability of stone handling patterns across 10 troops Behaviour 144 3 251 281 doi 10 1163 156853907780425712 ISSN 0005 7959 a b Gumert Michael D Kluck Marius Malaivijitnond Suchinda 2009 The physical characteristics and usage patterns of stone axe and pounding hammers used by long tailed macaques in the Andaman Sea region of Thailand American Journal of Primatology 71 7 594 608 doi 10 1002 ajp 20694 PMID 19405083 S2CID 22384150 a b c d Haslam Michael Luncz Lydia Pascual Garrido Alejandra Falotico Tiago Malaivijitnond Suchinda Gumert Michael 2016 Archaeological excavation of wild macaque stone tools Journal of Human Evolution 96 134 138 doi 10 1016 j jhevol 2016 05 002 PMID 27256780 a b Malaivijitnond Suchinda Lekprayoon Chariya Tandavanittj Nontivich Panha Somsak Cheewatham Cheewapap Hamada Yuzuru 2007 Stone tool usage by Thai long tailed macaques Macaca fascicularis American Journal of Primatology 69 2 227 233 doi 10 1002 ajp 20342 PMID 17146796 S2CID 24117181 Gumert Michael D Malaivijitnond Suchinda 2013 Long tailed macaques select mass of stone tools according to food type Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences 368 1630 20120413 doi 10 1098 rstb 2012 0413 ISSN 0962 8436 PMC 4027418 PMID 24101623 S2CID 12011847 a b Proffitt T Luncz V L Malaivijitnond S Gumert M Svensson M S Haslam M 2018 Analysis of wild macaque stone tools used to crack oil palm nuts Royal Society Open Science 5 3 171904 Bibcode 2018RSOS 571904P doi 10 1098 rsos 171904 ISSN 2054 5703 PMC 5882716 PMID 29657792 Falotico Tiago Spagnoletti Noemi Haslam Michael Luncz Lydia V Malaivijitnond Suchinda Gumert Michael 2017 Analysis of sea almond Terminalia catappa cracking sites used by wild Burmese long tailed macaques Macaca fascicularis aurea American Journal of Primatology 79 5 e22629 doi 10 1002 ajp 22629 PMID 28056164 S2CID 3882235 Luncz Lydia V Svensson Magdalena S Haslam Michael Malaivijitnond Suchinda Proffitt Tomos Gumert Michael 2017 Technological Response of Wild Macaques Macaca fascicularis to Anthropogenic Change International Journal of Primatology 38 5 872 880 doi 10 1007 s10764 017 9985 6 ISSN 0164 0291 PMC 5629225 PMID 29056799 S2CID 254539613 a b c Proffitt Tomos Reeves Jonathan S Benito Calvo Alfonso Sanchez Romero Laura Arroyo Adrian Malaijivitnond Suchinda Luncz Lydia V 2021 Three dimensional surface morphometry differentiates behaviour on primate percussive stone tools Journal of the Royal Society Interface 18 184 doi 10 1098 rsif 2021 0576 ISSN 1742 5662 PMC 8564602 PMID 34727711 Tan Amanda Tan Say Hoon Vyas Dhaval Malaivijitnond Suchinda Gumert Michael D 2015 There Is More than One Way to Crack an Oyster Identifying Variation in Burmese Long Tailed Macaque Macaca fascicularis aurea Stone Tool Use PLOS ONE 10 5 e0124733 Bibcode 2015PLoSO 1024733T doi 10 1371 journal pone 0124733 ISSN 1932 6203 PMC 4430286 PMID 25970286 Haslam Michael Gumert Michael D Biro Dora Carvalho Susana Malaivijitnond Suchinda 2013 Use Wear Patterns on Wild Macaque Stone Tools Reveal Their Behavioural History PLOS ONE 8 8 e72872 Bibcode 2013PLoSO 872872H doi 10 1371 journal pone 0072872 ISSN 1932 6203 PMC 3745380 PMID 23977365 Gumert Michael D Tan Amanda Wei Yi Luncz Lydia V Chua Constance Ting Kulik Lars Switzer Adam D Haslam Michael Iriki Atsushi Malaivijitnond Suchinda 2019 Prevalence of tool behaviour is associated with pelage phenotype in intraspecific hybrid long tailed macaques Macaca fascicularis aurea M f fascicularis Behaviour 156 11 1083 1125 doi 10 1163 1568539X 00003557 hdl 10220 49527 ISSN 0005 7959 S2CID 164644293 Primate Speciation A Case Study of African Apes Learn Science at Scitable www nature com Retrieved 2023 03 16 Bandini Elisa Motes Rodrigo Alba Archer William Minchin Tanya Axelsen Helene Hernandez Aguilar Raquel Adriana McPherron Shannon P Tennie Claudio 2021 07 15 Naive unenculturated chimpanzees fail to make and use flaked stone tools Open Research Europe 1 20 doi 10 12688 openreseurope 13186 2 ISSN 2732 5121 PMC 7612464 PMID 35253007 Gruber Thibaud Clay Zanna 2016 A Comparison Between Bonobos and Chimpanzees A Review and Update Evolutionary Anthropology Issues News and Reviews 25 5 239 252 doi 10 1002 evan 21501 PMID 27753219 S2CID 20712022 Samuni Liran Lemieux David Lamb Alicia Galdino Daiane Surbeck Martin 2022 Tool use behavior in three wild bonobo communities at Kokolopori American Journal of Primatology 84 1 e23342 doi 10 1002 ajp 23342 ISSN 0275 2565 PMID 34694658 S2CID 239767503 a b Furuichi T Sanz C Koops K Sakamaki T Ryu H Tokuyama N Morgan D 2015 Why do wild bonobos not use tools like chimpanzees do Behaviour 152 3 4 425 460 doi 10 1163 1568539X 00003226 ISSN 0005 7959 S2CID 84339010 Narat Victor Pennec Flora Simmen Bruno Ngawolo Jean Christophe Bokika Krief Sabrina 2015 Bonobo habituation in a forest savanna mosaic habitat influence of ape species habitat type and sociocultural context Primates 56 4 339 349 doi 10 1007 s10329 015 0476 0 ISSN 1610 7365 PMID 26159325 S2CID 254162786 Gruber Thibaud Clay Zanna Zuberbuhler Klaus 2010 A comparison of bonobo and chimpanzee tool use evidence for a female bias in the Pan lineage Animal Behaviour 80 6 1023 1033 doi 10 1016 j anbehav 2010 09 005 ISSN 0003 3472 S2CID 14923158 Koops Kathelijne Furuichi Takeshi Hashimoto Chie 2015 Chimpanzees and bonobos differ in intrinsic motivation for tool use Scientific Reports 5 1 11356 Bibcode 2015NatSR 511356K doi 10 1038 srep11356 ISSN 2045 2322 PMC 4468814 PMID 26079292 Bardo Ameline Borel Antony Meunier Helene Guery Jean Pascal Pouydebat Emmanuelle 2016 Behavioral and functional strategies during tool use tasks in bonobos B ardo et al American Journal of Physical Anthropology 161 1 125 140 doi 10 1002 ajpa 23015 PMID 27311774 Neufuss Johanna Humle Tatyana Cremaschi Andrea Kivell Tracy L 2017 Nut cracking behaviour in wild born rehabilitated bonobos Pan paniscus a comprehensive study of hand preference hand grips and efficiency American Journal of Primatology 79 2 e22589 doi 10 1002 ajp 22589 PMID 27564429 S2CID 3767070 Schiffer Michael B 1983 Toward the Identification of Formation Processes American Antiquity 48 4 675 706 doi 10 2307 279771 ISSN 0002 7316 JSTOR 279771 S2CID 160026999 Mercader Julio Panger Melissa Boesch Christophe 2002 05 24 Excavation of a Chimpanzee Stone Tool Site in the African Rainforest Science 296 5572 1452 1455 Bibcode 2002Sci 296 1452M doi 10 1126 science 1070268 ISSN 0036 8075 PMID 12029130 S2CID 21042746 a b c Falotico Tiago Proffitt Tomos Ottoni Eduardo B Staff Richard A Haslam Michael 2019 Three thousand years of wild capuchin stone tool use Nature Ecology amp Evolution 3 7 1034 1038 doi 10 1038 s41559 019 0904 4 ISSN 2397 334X PMID 31235926 S2CID 256705176 Reeves Jonathan S Proffitt Tomos Luncz Lydia V 2021 Modeling a primate technological niche Scientific Reports 11 1 23139 Bibcode 2021NatSR 1123139R doi 10 1038 s41598 021 01849 4 ISSN 2045 2322 PMC 8632893 PMID 34848740 Van Gijn A L 2014 Science and interpretation in microwear studies Journal of Archaeological Science 48 166 169 Bibcode 2014JArSc 48 166V doi 10 1016 j jas 2013 10 024 ISSN 0305 4403 a b c Caruana Matthew V Carvalho Susana Braun David R Presnyakova Darya Haslam Michael Archer Will Bobe Rene Harris John W K 2014 Bicho Nuno ed Quantifying Traces of Tool Use A Novel Morphometric Analysis of Damage Patterns on Percussive Tools PLOS ONE 9 11 e113856 Bibcode 2014PLoSO 9k3856C doi 10 1371 journal pone 0113856 ISSN 1932 6203 PMC 4240665 PMID 25415303 Borel Antony Olle Andreu Verges Josep Maria Sala Robert 2014 Scanning Electron and Optical Light Microscopy two complementary approaches for the understanding and interpretation of usewear and residues on stone tools Journal of Archaeological Science 48 46 59 Bibcode 2014JArSc 48 46B doi 10 1016 j jas 2013 06 031 ISSN 0305 4403 Evershed R P 2008 Organic residue analysis in archaeology the archaeological biomarker revolution Archaeometry 50 6 pp 895 924 Arroyo Adrian Falotico Tiago Burguet Coca Aitor Exposito Isabel Quinn Patrick Proffitt Tomos 2021 Use wear and residue analysis of pounding tools used by wild capuchin monkeys Sapajus libidinosus from Serra da Capivara Piaui Brazil Journal of Archaeological Science Reports 35 102690 Bibcode 2021JArSR 35j2690A doi 10 1016 j jasrep 2020 102690 S2CID 230555705 Inizan M L Reduron Ballinger M Roche H and Tixier J 1999 Technology and terminology of knapped stone Prehistoire de la Pierre taillee 5 Nanterre CREP a b c d Proffitt Tomos Reeves Jonathan S Falotico Tiago Arroyo Adrian Torre Ignacio de la Ottoni Eduardo B Luncz Lydia V 2023 Identifying intentional flake production at the dawn of technology A technological and 3D geometric morphometric study Journal of Archaeological Science 152 105740 Bibcode 2023JArSc 152j5740P doi 10 1016 j jas 2023 105740 hdl 10261 296180 S2CID 257190876 Proffitt T Haslam M Mercader J F Boesch C Luncz L V 2018 Revisiting Panda 100 the first archaeological chimpanzee nut cracking site Journal of Human Evolution 124 117 139 doi 10 1016 j jhevol 2018 04 016 PMID 30236627 S2CID 52308880 Macchi R Daver G Brenet M Prat S Hugheville L Harmand S Lewis J Domalain M 2021 Biomechanical demands of percussive techniques in the context of early stone toolmaking Journal of the Royal Society Interface 18 178 20201044 doi 10 1098 rsif 2020 1044 ISSN 1742 5662 PMC 8150015 PMID 34034530 Heldstab Sandra A Kosonen Zaida K Koski Sonja E Burkart Judith M van Schaik Carel P Isler Karin 2016 Manipulation complexity in primates coevolved with brain size and terrestriality Scientific Reports 6 1 24528 Bibcode 2016NatSR 624528H doi 10 1038 srep24528 ISSN 2045 2322 PMC 4830942 PMID 27075921 a b c Tennie Claudio Call Josep Tomasello Michael 2009 Ratcheting up the ratchet on the evolution of cumulative culture Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences 364 1528 2405 2415 doi 10 1098 rstb 2009 0052 ISSN 0962 8436 PMC 2865079 PMID 19620111 a b Tennie Claudio Bandini Elisa van Schaik Carel P Hopper Lydia M 2020 The zone of latent solutions and its relevance to understanding ape cultures Biology amp Philosophy 35 5 55 doi 10 1007 s10539 020 09769 9 ISSN 1572 8404 PMC 7548278 PMID 33093737 Luncz Lydia V Wittig Roman M Boesch Christophe 2015 Primate archaeology reveals cultural transmission in wild chimpanzees Pan troglodytes verus Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences 370 1682 20140348 doi 10 1098 rstb 2014 0348 ISSN 0962 8436 PMC 4614712 PMID 26483527 a b c Koops Kathelijne Soumah Aly Gaspard van Leeuwen Kelly L Camara Henry Didier Matsuzawa Tetsuro 2022 Field experiments find no evidence that chimpanzee nut cracking can be independently innovated Nature Human Behaviour 6 4 487 494 doi 10 1038 s41562 021 01272 9 ISSN 2397 3374 PMID 35075258 S2CID 246277257 a b Tennie Claudio Call Josep 2023 Unmotivated Subjects Cannot Provide Interpretable Data and Tasks with Sensitive Learning Periods Require Appropriately Aged Subjects A Commentary on Koops et al 2022 Field experiments find no evidence that chimpanzee nut cracking can be independently innovated PDF Animal Behavior and Cognition 10 1 89 94 doi 10 26451 abc 10 01 05 2023 S2CID 257262799 Tennie Claudio Premo L S Braun David R McPherron Shannon P 2017 Early Stone Tools and Cultural Transmission Resetting the Null Hypothesis Current Anthropology 58 5 652 672 doi 10 1086 693846 ISSN 0011 3204 S2CID 148701377 Pradhan Gauri R Tennie Claudio van Schaik Carel P 2012 07 01 Social organization and the evolution of cumulative technology in apes and hominins Journal of Human Evolution 63 1 180 190 doi 10 1016 j jhevol 2012 04 008 ISSN 0047 2484 PMID 22658335 a b Tennie C 2023 The Earliest Tools and Cultures of Hominins in J J Tehrani J Kendal and R Kendal Eds The Oxford Handbook of Cultural Evolution 1st ed Oxford University Press p C33P1 C33N40 Snyder William D Reeves Jonathan S Tennie Claudio 2022 Early knapping techniques do not necessitate cultural transmission Science Advances 8 27 eabo2894 Bibcode 2022SciA 8O2894S doi 10 1126 sciadv abo2894 ISSN 2375 2548 PMC 9258951 PMID 35857472 This article needs additional or more specific categories Please help out by adding categories to it so that it can be listed with similar articles May 2023 Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Primate archaeology amp oldid 1195608752, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.