fbpx
Wikipedia

Phonological change

In historical linguistics, phonological change is any sound change that alters the distribution of phonemes in a language. In other words, a language develops a new system of oppositions among its phonemes. Old contrasts may disappear, new ones may emerge, or they may simply be rearranged.[1] Sound change may be an impetus for changes in the phonological structures of a language (and likewise, phonological change may sway the process of sound change).[1] One process of phonological change is rephonemicization, in which the distribution of phonemes changes by either addition of new phonemes or a reorganization of existing phonemes.[2] Mergers and splits are types of rephonemicization and are discussed further below.

Types edit

In a typological scheme first systematized by Henry M. Hoenigswald in 1965, a historical sound law can only affect a phonological system in one of three ways:

  • Conditioned merger (which Hoenigswald calls "primary split"), in which some instances of phoneme A become an existing phoneme B; the number of phonemes does not change, only their distribution.
  • Phonemic split (which Hoenigswald calls "secondary split"), in which some instances of A become a new phoneme B; this is phonemic differentiation in which the number of phonemes increases.
  • Unconditioned merger, in which all instances of phonemes A and B become A; this is phonemic reduction, in which the number of phonemes decreases.

This classification does not consider mere changes in pronunciation, that is, phonetic change, even chain shifts, in which neither the number nor the distribution of phonemes is affected.

Phonetic vs. phonological change edit

Phonetic change can occur without any modification to the phoneme inventory or phonemic correspondences. This change is purely allophonic or subphonemic. This can entail one of two changes: either the phoneme turns into a new allophone—meaning the phonetic form changes—or the distribution of allophones of the phoneme changes.[2]

For the most part, phonetic changes are examples of allophonic differentiation or assimilation; i.e., sounds in specific environments acquire new phonetic features or perhaps lose phonetic features they originally had. For example, the devoicing of the vowels /i/ and /ɯ/ in certain environments in Japanese, the nasalization of vowels before nasals (common but not universal), changes in point of articulation of stops and nasals under the influence of adjacent vowels.

Phonetic change in this context refers to the lack of phonological restructuring, not a small degree of sound change. For example, chain shifts such as the Great Vowel Shift (in which nearly all of the vowels of the English language changed) or the allophonic differentiation of /s/, originally *[s], into [s z ʃ ʒ ʂ ʐ θ χ χʷ h], do not qualify as phonological change as long as all of the phones remain in complementary distribution.

Many phonetic changes provide the raw ingredients for later phonemic innovations. In Proto-Italic, for example, intervocalic */s/ became *[z]. It was a phonetic change, merely a mild and superficial complication in the phonological system, but when *[z] merged with */r/, the effect on the phonological system was greater. (The example will be discussed below, under conditioned merger.)

Similarly, in the prehistory of Indo-Iranian, the velars */k/ and */g/ acquired distinctively palatal articulation before front vowels (*/e/, */i/, */ē/ */ī/), so that */ke/ came to be pronounced *[t͡ʃe] and */ge/ *[d͡ʒe], but the phones *[t͡ʃ] and *[d͡ʒ] occurred only in that environment. However, when */e/, */o/, */a/ later fell together as Proto-Indo-Iranian */a/ (and */ē/ */ō/ */ā/ likewise fell together as */ā/), the result was that the allophonic palatal and velar stops now contrasted in identical environments: */ka/ and /ča/, /ga/ and /ǰa/, and so on. The difference became phonemic. (The "law of palatals" is an example of phonemic split.)

Sound changes generally operate for a limited period of time, and once established, new phonemic contrasts rarely remain tied to their ancestral environments. For example, Sanskrit acquired "new" /ki/ and /gi/ sequences via analogy and borrowing, and likewise /ču/, /ǰu/, /čm/, and similar novelties; and the reduction of the diphthong */ay/ to Sanskrit /ē/ had no effect at all on preceding velar stops.

Merger edit

Phonemic merger is a loss of distinction between phonemes. Occasionally, the term reduction refers to phonemic merger. It is not to be confused with the meaning of the word "reduction" in phonetics, such as vowel reduction, but phonetic changes may contribute to phonemic mergers. For example, in most North American English dialects, the vowel in the word lot and vowel in the word palm have become the same sound and thus undergone a merger. In most dialects of England, the words father and farther are pronounced the same due to a merger created by non-rhoticity or "R-dropping".

Conditioned merger edit

Conditioned merger, or primary split, takes place when some, but not all, allophones of a phoneme, say A, merge with some other phoneme, B. The immediate results are these:

  • there are the same number of contrasts as before.
  • there are fewer words with A than before.
  • there are more words of B than before.
  • there is at least one environment for which A, for the time being, no longer occurs, called a gap in the distribution of the phoneme.
  • there is, under certain circumstances, an alternation between A and B if inflection or derivation result in A sometimes but not always being in the environment in which it merged with B.

Example from Middle English edit

For a simple example, without alternation, early Middle English /d/ after stressed syllables followed by /r/ became /ð/: módor, fæder > mother, father /ðr/, weder > weather, and so on. Since /ð/ was already a structure-point in the language, the innovation resulted merely in more /ð/ and less /d/ and a gap in the distribution of /d/ (though not a very conspicuous one).

Note 1: thanks to borrowing, from dialects as well as other languages, the original distribution has been disturbed: rudder, adder in Standard English (but forms with /ð/ are attested in nonstandard dialects).
Note 2: one who knows German can figure out which cases of English /ð/ were originally /ð/ and which changed from /d/. Original /d/ corresponds to /t/ in German, and original /ð/ corresponds to /d/. Thus, leather = German Leder, brother = Bruder, whether = weder, wether = Widder, pointing to original /ð/ in English; weather = German Wetter, father = Vater, mother = Mutter pointing to original /d/.
Note 3: alternation between /d/ and /ð/ would have been a theoretical possibility in English, as in sets like hard, harder; ride, rider, but any such details have been erased by the commonplace diachronic process called morphological leveling.

Devoicing of voiced stops in German edit

A trivial (if all-pervasive) example of conditioned merger is the devoicing of voiced stops in German when in word-final position or immediately before a compound boundary (see: Help:IPA/Standard German):

  • *hand "hand" > /hant/ (cf. plural Hände /ˈhɛndə/)
  • Handgelenk "wrist" /ˈhantgəlɛŋk/
  • *bund "league, association" > /bʊnt/ (cf. plural Bünde /ˈbʏndə/)
  • *gold "gold" > /gɔlt/
  • *halb "half" > /halp/ (cf. halbieren "to halve" /halˈbiːʁən/)
  • halbamtlich "semi-official" /ˈhalpʔamtlɪç/
  • *berg "mountain" /bɛɐ̯k/ (cf. plural Berge /ˈbɛɐ̯gə/)
  • *klug "clever, wise" > /kluːk/ (cf. fem. kluge /ˈkluːgə/)

There were, of course, also many cases of original voiceless stops in final position: Bett "bed", bunt "colorful", Stock "(walking) stick, cane". To sum up: there are the same number of structure points as before, /p t k b d g/, but there are more cases of /p t k/ than before and fewer of /b d g/, and there is a gap in the distribution of /b d g/ (they are never found in word-final position or before a compound boundary).

Note 1: this split is easily recoverable by internal reconstruction because it results in alternations whose conditions are transparent. Thus Bund "bunch" (as in, keys) /bʊnt/ has a plural Bünde /ˈbʏndə/ in contrast to bunt "colorful" with /t/ in all environments (feminine /ˈbʊntə/, neuter /ˈbʊntəs/ and so on). In a neutralizing environment, such as a voiceless stop in word-final position, one cannot tell which of two possibilities was the original sound. The choice is resolved if the corresponding segment can be found in a non-neutralizing position, as when a suffix follows. Accordingly, a non-inflected form like und /ʔʊnt/ "and" is historically opaque (though as the spelling hints, the /t/ was originally *d).
Note 2: unlike most phonological changes, this one became a "surface" rule in German, so loan-words whose source had a voiced stop in the devoicing environment are taken into German with a voiceless one instead: Klub "club" (association) /klʊp/ from English club. The same goes for truncated forms: Bub (for formal Bube "boy") is /buːp/.
Note 2a: the surface alternation is what allows modern German orthography to write stops morphophonemically, thus Leib "loaf", Hand "hand", Weg "way", all with voiceless final stops in the simplex form and in compounds, but /b d g/ in inflected forms. In Old High and Middle High German, all voiceless stops were written as pronounced: hleip, hant, uuec and so on.
Note 3: the same distribution holds for /s/ vs. /z/, but it arose by a completely different process, the voicing of original */s/ between vowels: *mūs "mouse" > Maus /maʊs/, *mūsīz "mice" (for earlier *mūsiz) > Mäuse /ˈmɔʏzə/. Original long (now short) ss does not voice medially, as in küssen "to kiss" /ˈkʏsen/, nor does /s/ from Proto-West-Germanic *t, as in Wasser "water" /ˈvasɐ/, Fässer "kegs" /ˈfɛsɐ/ plural of Fass /fas/ (= English vat), müßig "idle" /ˈmyːsɪç/. German /ʃ/, as in Fisch "fish", reflects original *sk (in native words) and does not become voiced in any environment: Fischer "fisherman" /ˈfɪʃɐ/. (German has /ʒ/ only in loanwords: Genie /ʒeˈniː/ "genius", Gage /ˈɡaːʒə/ "salary".)

Rhotacism in Latin edit

More typical of the aftermath of a conditioned merger is the famous case of rhotacism in Latin (also seen in some Sabellian language spoken in the same area): Proto-Italic *s > Latin /r/ between vowels: *gesō "I do, act" > Lat. gerō (but perfect gessi < *ges-s- and participle gestus < *ges-to-, etc., with unchanged *s in all other environments, even in the same paradigm).

This sound law is quite complete and regular, and in its immediate wake there were no examples of /s/ between vowels except for a few words with a special condition (miser "wretched", caesariēs "bushy hair", diser(c)tus "eloquent": that is, rhotacism did not take place when an /r/ followed the *s). However, a new crop of /s/ between vowels soon arose from three sources. (1) a shortening of /ss/ after a diphthong or long vowel: causa "lawsuit" < *kawssā, cāsa "house' < *kāssā, fūsus "poured, melted" < *χewssos. (2) univerbation: nisi (nisī) "unless" < the phrase *ne sei, quasi (quasī) "as if" < the phrase *kʷam sei. (3) borrowings, such as rosa "rose" /rosa/, from a Sabellian source (the word is clearly somehow from Proto-Italic *ruθ- "red" but equally clearly not native Latin), and many words taken from or through Greek (philosophia, basis, casia, Mesopotamia, etc., etc.).

Nasal assimilation and "gn" in Latin edit

A particular example of a conditioned merger in Latin is the rule whereby syllable-final stops, when followed by a nasal consonant, assimilated with it in nasality, while preserving their original point of articulation:

  • *supimos > *supmos > summus "highest"
  • *sabnyom > Samnium "Samnium" (a region in the southern Apennines)
  • *swepnos > somnus "sleep"
  • *atnos > annus "year"

In some cases, the underlying (pre-assimilation) root can be retrieved from related lexical items in the language: e.g. superior "higher"; Sabīni "Samnites"; sopor "(deep) sleep". For some words, only comparative evidence can help retrieve the original consonant: for example, the etymology of annus “year” (as *atnos) is revealed by comparison with Gothic aþna “year”.

According to this rule of nasal assimilation, the sequences *-g-n and *-k-n would become [ŋn], with a velar nasal [ŋ]:

  • *dek-no- > dignus [diŋnus] "worthy"
  • *leg-no- (*leǵ- "gather") > lignum [liŋnum] "firewood"
  • *teg-no- (*(s)teg- "build") > tignum [tiŋnum] "timber"
  • *agʷnos > *ag-nos[3] > agnus [aŋnus] "lamb"

The sound [ŋ] was not a phoneme of Latin, but an allophone of /g/ before /n/.

The sequence [ŋn] was regularly rendered in the orthography as |gn|.[4] Some epigraphic inscriptions also feature non-standard spellings, e.g. SINNU for signum "sign, insigne", INGNEM for ignem "fire". These are witness to the speakers' hesitancy on how to best transcribe the sound [ŋ] in the sequence [ŋn].

The regular nasal assimilation of Latin can be seen as a form of "merger", insofar as it resulted in the contrast between oral stops (p, b, t, d) and nasal stops (m, n) being regularly neutralized.

Concerning the number of contrasts edit

One of the traits of conditioned merger, as outlined above, is that the total number of contrasts remains the same, but it is possible for such splits to reduce the number of contrasts. It happens if all of the conditioned merger products merge with one or another phoneme.

For example, in Latin, the Pre-Latin phoneme *θ (from Proto-Italic * < PIE *dh) disappears as such by merging with three other sounds: *f (from PIE *bh and *gʷh), *d, and *b:

Initially *θ > f:

  • PItal. *tʰi-n-kʰ- "model, shape" > *θi-n-χ- > Lat. fingō (PIE root *dheyǵh- "smear, work with the hands"; cf. Sanskrit dihanti "they smear", Avestan daēza- "wall" = Greek teîkhos; English dough < OE dāh besides dāg < PIE *dhoyǵh-)
  • PItal. *tʰwor- "door" > *θwor- > Lat. forēs "door" (PIE *dhwor-; like most reflexes plural only; cf Eng. door < *dhur-, Greek thúrā (probably < *dhwor-) usually thúrai pl.)
Cf. Latin ferō "carry" < Proto-Italic *pʰer- < PIE *bher-; Latin frāter "brother" < Proto-Italic *pʰrātēr < PIE *bhre-H₂ter-

Medially adjacent to *l, *r, or *u, *θ becomes b:

  • PItal. *wertʰom "word" > *werθom > *werðom (? *werβom) > Lat. verbum (cf. English word < *wurdaⁿ < PIE *wṛdhom, Lithuanian vaṙdas "name")
  • PItal. *rutʰros "red" > *ruθros > *ruðros (? *ruβros) > Latin ruber (via *rubers < *rubrs < *rubros), cf. rubra fem. rubrum neut.
  • PItal. *-tʰlo-/*-tʰlā- "tool suffix" > Latin -bulum, -bula: PIE *peH₂-dhlo- "nourishment" > PItal. *pā-tʰlo- > *pāθlo- > Latin pābulum; PIE *suH-dhleH₂- "sewing implement" > PItal. *sūtʰlā > *sūθlā > Latin sūbula "cobbler's awl"
Intervocalic Latin -b- is from PIE *bh, *s, and (rarely and problematically) *b: Lat. ambō "both" < PIE *ambh- or *H₂embh- (cf. Greek amphi-); Lat. crābrō "hornet" < *ḱṛHs-ron- (cf. Vedic śīrṣn- "hornet"); Lat. cannabis "hemp" (cf. Old English hænep "hemp"). The change of *-sr- to -br- is itself presumably via *-θr- > *-ðr- > *-βr-.

Elsewhere, *θ becomes d:

  • PItal. *metʰyo- "middle" > *meθyo- > Pre-Lat. *meðyo- > Lat. medius (three syllables; PIE *medhyo-, cf. Sanskrit madhya-, Greek més(s)os < *meth-yo-)
  • PItal. *pʰeytʰ- > *feyθ- > *feyð- > Lat. fīdus "trusting" (cf. Greek peíthomai "am persuaded", English bid "order, ask")
Intervocalic -d- in Latin comes from PIE *d in ped- "foot", sīdere "to sit down", cord- "heart"

There is no alternation to give away the historical story, there, via internal reconstruction; the evidence for these changes is almost entirely from comparative reconstruction. That reconstruction makes it easy to unriddle the story behind the weird forms of the Latin paradigm jubeō "order", jussī perfect, jussus participle. If the root is inherited, it would have to have been PIE *yewdh-.

Unconditioned merger edit

Unconditioned merger, that is, complete loss of a contrast between two or more phonemes, is not very common. Most mergers are conditioned. That is, most apparent mergers of A and B have an environment or two in which A did something else, such as drop or merge with C.

Typical is the unconditioned merger seen in the Celtic conflation of the PIE plain voiced series of stops with the breathy-voiced series: *bh, *dh, *ǵh, *gh are indistinguishable in Celtic etymology from the reflexes of *b *d *ǵ *g. The collapse of the contrast cannot be stated in whole-series terms because the labiovelars do not co-operate. PIE * everywhere falls together with the reflexes of *b and *bh as Proto-Celtic *b, but *gʷh seems to have become PCelt. *, lining up with PCelt. * < PIE *.

Examples edit

  • OE y and ý (short and long high front rounded vowels) fell together with i and í via a simple phonetic unrounding: OE hypp, cynn, cyssan, brycg, fyllan, fýr, mýs, brýd became modern hip, kin, kiss, bridge, fill, fire, mice, bride. There is no way to tell by inspection whether a modern /i ay/ goes back to a rounded or an unrounded vowel. The change is not even reflected in modern spelling since it took place too early to be captured in Middle English Spelling conventions. Of course, current spellings like type, thyme, psyche, etc., have nothing to do with OE y = /y/.
  • There is a massive, consistent body of evidence that PIE *l and *r merged totally in Proto-Indo-Iranian, as did PIE *e *o *a into Proto-Indo-Iranian *a.
  • The evolution of Romance shows a systematic collection of unconditioned mergers in connection with the loss of Latin vowel length. Latin had ten vowels, five long and five short (i, ī; e, ē; a, ā; and so on). In the variety of Romance underlying Sardo and some other dialects of the islands, the ten vowels simply fell together pairwise: in no way are Latin e, ē, say, reflected differently. In Proto-Western-Romance, the ancestor of French, Iberian, Italian north of the Spezia-Rimini line, etc., however, things happened differently: Latin /a ā/ merged totally, as in Sardo, but the other vowels all behaved differently. Upon losing the feature of length, Latin /ī ū/ merged with nothing, but the short high vowels, front and back, merged with the long mid vowels: thus, Latin /i ē/ are uniformly reflected as PWRom. * (in the standard Romance notation), and /u ō/ become *. PWRom. * is reflected in French (in open syllables) as /wa/ (spelled oi); voile "sail", foin "hay", doigt "finger", quoi "what", are from Latin vēlum, fēnum, digitus (via *dictu), quid, respectively. There is no way of telling in French which one of the two Latin vowels is the source of any given /wa/.

Another example is provided by Japonic languages. Proto-Japanese had 8 vowels; it has been reduced to 5 in modern Japanese, but in Yaeyama, the vowel mergers progressed further, to 3 vowels.

Split edit

In a split (Hoenigswald's "secondary split"), a new contrast arises when allophones of a phoneme cease being in complementary distribution and are therefore necessarily independent structure points, i.e. contrastive. This mostly comes about because of some loss of distinctiveness in the environment of one or more allophones of a phoneme.

A simple example is the rise of the contrast between nasal and oral vowels in French. A full account of this history is complicated by the subsequent changes in the phonetics of the nasal vowels, but the development can be compendiously illustrated via the present-day French phonemes /a/ and /ã/:

  • Step 1: *a > *ã when a nasal immediately follows: *čantu "song" > [tʃãntu] (still phonemically /tʃantu/);
  • Step 2: at some point in the history of French when speakers consistently stopped making an oral closure with the tongue, we had [tʃãt], that is /tʃãt/ (if not /ʃãt/) and finally, with the loss of the final stop, modern French /ʃã/ chant "song", distinct from French /ʃa/ chat "cat" solely by the contrast between the nasal and the oral articulation of the vowels, and thus with many other forms in which /a/ and /ã/ contrast.
Note 1: the nasalization of a vowel before a nasal is found very widely in the world's languages, but is not at all universal. In modern French, for example, vowels before a nasal are oral. That they used to be nasalized, like the vowels before lost nasals, is indicated by certain phonetic changes not always reflected in the orthography: Fr. femme "woman" /fam/ (with the lowering of [ɛ̃] (nasalized [ɛ]) to *ã prior to denasalization).
Note 2: unusually for a split, the history of the French innovation, even including some changes in vowel cavity features, can be readily inferred by internal reconstruction. This is because the contrastive feature [nasal] in a vowel system usually has a nasal in its history, which makes for straightforward surmises. There are also clear alternations, as /bɔ̃/ "good" (masc.) vs. /bɔn/ (fem.), while such pairs as /fin/ "fine" (fem.) and /fɛ̃/ (masc.) together with derivatives like raffiné /rafine/ "refined" indicate what happened to nasalized *i.

Phonemic split was a major factor in the creation of the contrast between voiced and voiceless fricatives in English. Originally, to oversimplify a bit, Old English fricatives were voiced between voiced sounds and voiceless elsewhere. Thus /f/ was [f] in fisc [fiʃ] "fish", fyllen "to fill" [fyllen], hæft "prisoner", ofþyrsted [ofθyrsted] "athirst", líf "life", wulf "wolf". But in say the dative singular of "life", that is lífe, the form was [li:ve] (as in English alive, being an old prepositional phrase on lífe); the plural of wulf, wulfas, was [wulvas], as still seen in wolves. The voiced fricative is typically seen in verbs, too (often with variations in vowel length of diverse sources): gift but give, shelf but shelve. Such alternations are to be seen even in loan words, as proof vs prove (though not as a rule in borrowed plurals, thus proofs, uses, with voiceless fricatives).

Note 1: unlike the French example, there is no chance of recovering the historical source of the alternations in English between /s θ f/ and /z ð v/ merely through inspection of the modern forms. The conditioning factor (original location of the voiced alternants between vowels, for example) is quite lost and with little reason to even suspect the original state of affairs; and anyway the original distributions have been much disturbed by analogical leveling. Worthy and (in some dialects) greasy have voiced fricative (next to the voiceless ones in worth and grease) but adjectives in -y otherwise do not alternate: bossy, glassy, leafy, earthy, breathy, saucy, etc (cf. glaze, leaves, breathe, and note that even in dialects with /z/ in greasy, the verb to grease always has /s/).
Note 2: the phoneme /ʃ/ does not alternate with /ʒ/ (and never did). In native words, /ʃ/ is from *sk, and either the change of this sequence to /ʃ/ postdated the rearranging of voicing in pre-Old English fricatives, or else it was phonetically long between vowels, originally, much like the /ʃ/ of present-day Italian (pesce "fish" is phonetically [peʃːe]) and long fricatives, just like sequences of fricatives, were always voiceless in Old English, as in cyssan "to kiss". The Early Modern English development of /ʃ/ < */sj/, as in nation, mission, assure, vastly postdated the period when fricatives became voiced between vowels.
Note 3: a common misstatement of cases like OE /f/ > Modern English /f, v/ is that a "new phoneme" has been created. Not so. A new contrast has been created. Both NE /f/ and /v/ are new phonemes, differing in phonetic specifications and distribution from OE /f/. Without doubt, one component in this misunderstanding is the orthography. If, instead of speaking of the development of Old English /f/ we said that OE /ɰ/ split into /f/ and /v/, there would presumably be less confused talk of "a" new phoneme arising in the process.

Loss edit

In Hoenigswald's original scheme, loss, the disappearance of a segment, or even of a whole phoneme, was treated as a form of merger, depending on whether the loss was conditioned or unconditioned. The "element" that a vanished segment or phoneme merged with was "zero".

The situation in which a highly inflected language has formations without any affix at all (Latin alter "(the) other", for example) is quite common, but it is the only one (nominative singular masculine: altera nominative singular feminine, alterum accusative singular masculine, etc.) of the 30 forms that make up the paradigm that is not explicitly marked with endings for gender, number, and case.

From a historical perspective, there is no problem since alter is from *alteros (overtly nominative singular and masculine), with the regular loss of the short vowel after *-r- and the truncation of the resulting word-final cluster *-rs. Descriptively, however, it is problematic to say that the "nominative singular masculine" is signaled by the absence of any affix. It is simpler to view alter as more than what it looks like, /alterØ/, "marked" for case, number, and gender by an affix, like the other 29 forms in the paradigm. It is merely that the "marker" in question is not a phoneme or sequence of phonemes but the element /Ø/.

Along the way, it is hard to know when to stop positing zeros and whether to regard one zero as different from another. For example, if the zero not-marking can (as in he can) as "third person singular" is the same zero that not-marks deer as "plural", or if are both basically a single morphological placeholder. If it is determined that there is a zero on the end of deer in three deer, it is uncertain whether English adjectives agree with the number of the noun they modify, using the same zero affix. (Deictics do so: compare this deer, these deer.) In some theories of syntax it is useful to have an overt marker on a singular noun in a sentence such as My head hurts because the syntactic mechanism needs something explicit to generate the singular suffix on the verb. Thus, all English singular nouns may be marked with yet another zero.

It seems possible to avoid all those issues by considering loss as a separate basic category of phonological change, and leave zero out of it.

As stated above, one can regard loss as both a kind of conditioned merger (when only some expressions of a phoneme are lost) and a disappearance of a whole structure point. The former is much more common than the latter.

  • In Latin are many consonant clusters that lose a member or two such as these: tostus "toasted, dried" < *torstos, multrum "milking stool" < *molktrom, scultus "carved" < *scolptos, cēna "dinner" < *kertsnā, lūna "moon" < *louwksnā ("lantern" or the like).
  • Greek lost all stops from the end of a word (so *kʷit "what" > Greek ti, *deḱṃt "ten" > déka, *wanakt "O prince" > ána), but stops generally survive elsewhere. PIE *s drops medially between voiced sounds in Greek but is preserved in final position and in some consonant clusters.
  • Old English [x] (voiceless velar fricative) is everywhere lost as such, but usually leaves traces behind (transphonologization). In furh "furrow" and mearh "marrow", it vocalizes. It is elided (with varying effects on the preceding vowel, such as lengthening) in night, knight, might, taught, naught, freight, fought, plow (Brit. plough, OE plōh), bought, through, though, slaughter; but /f/ in laugh, trough, tough, enough (and daughter can be found in The Pilgrim's Progress rhyming with after, and the spelling dafter is actually attested) The /x/ phoneme still exists in some onomatopoeiac words, like "ugh" (note the spelling uses gh, which indicates that when they were coined, there was still some understanding of the phonemic meaning of gh), "yech" and "chutzpah".
  • /g k/ are lost in English in word-initial position before /n/: gnaw, gnat, knight, know. /t/ is lost after fricatives before nasals and /l/: soften, castle, bristle, chestnut, Christmas, hasten
  • In many words, /f/ (that is, Old English [v]) was lost between vowels: auger, hawk, newt < OE nafogar, hafoc, efete ("lizard"), and in some alternative (poetic) forms: e'en "evening", o'er "over", e'er "ever"; Scottish siller "silver", and others.

The ends of words often have sound laws that apply there only, and many such special developments consist of the loss of a segment. The early history and prehistory of English has seen several waves of loss of elements, vowels and consonants alike, from the ends of words, first in Proto-Germanic, then to Proto-West-Germanic, then to Old and Middle and Modern English, shedding bits from the ends of words at every step of the way. There is in Modern English next to nothing left of the elaborate inflectional and derivational apparatus of PIE or of Proto-Germanic because of the successive ablation of the phonemes making up these suffixes.

Total unconditional loss is, as mentioned, not very common. Latin /h/ appears to have been lost everywhere in all varieties of Proto-Romance except Romanian. Proto-Indo-European laryngeals survived as consonants only in Anatolian languages but left plenty of traces of their former presence (see laryngeal theory).

Phonemic differentiation edit

Phonemic differentiation is the phenomenon of a language maximizing the acoustic distance between its phonemes.

Examples edit

For example, in many languages, including English, most front vowels are unrounded, while most back vowels are rounded. There are no languages in which all front vowels are rounded and all back vowels are unrounded. The most likely explanation for this[citation needed] is that front vowels have a higher second formant (F2) than back vowels, and unrounded vowels have a higher F2 than rounded vowels. Thus unrounded front vowels and rounded back vowels have maximally different F2s, enhancing their phonemic differentiation.

Phonemic differentiation can have an effect on diachronic sound change. In chain shifts, phonemic differentiation is maintained, while in phonemic mergers it is lost. Phonemic splits involve the creation of two phonemes out of one, which then tend to diverge because of phonemic differentiation.

Chain shifts edit

In a chain shift, one phoneme moves in acoustic space, causing other phonemes to move as well to maintain optimal phonemic differentiation. An example from American English is the Northern cities vowel shift [1], where the raising of /æ/ has triggered a fronting of /ɑ/, which in turn has triggered a lowering of /ɔ/, and so forth.

Phonemic mergers edit

If a phoneme moves in acoustic space, but its neighbors do not move in a chain shift, a phonemic merger may occur. In that case, a single phoneme results where an earlier stage of the language had two phonemes (that is also called phonetic neutralization). A well known example of a phonemic merger in American English is the cot–caught merger by which the vowel phonemes /ɑ/ and /ɔ/ (illustrated by the words cot and caught respectively) have merged into a single phoneme in some accents.

Phonemic splits edit

In a phonemic split, a phoneme at an earlier stage of the language is divided into two phonemes over time. Usually, it happens when a phoneme has two allophones appearing in different environments, but sound change eliminates the distinction between the two environments. For example, in umlaut in the Germanic languages, the back vowels /u, o/ originally had front rounded allophones [y, ø] before the vowel /i/ in a following syllable. When sound change caused the syllables containing /i/ to be lost, a phonemic split resulted, making /y, ø/ distinct phonemes.

It is sometimes difficult to determine whether a split or a merger has happened if one dialect has two phonemes corresponding to a single phoneme in another dialect; diachronic research is usually required to determine the dialect that is conservative and the one that is innovative.

When phonemic change occurs differently in the standard language and in dialects, the dialect pronunciation is considered nonstandard and may be stigmatized. In descriptive linguistics, however, the question of which splits and mergers are prestigious and which are stigmatized is irrelevant. However, such stigmatization can lead to hypercorrection, when the dialect speakers attempt to imitate the standard language but overshoot, as with the footstrut split, where failing to make the split is stigmatized in Northern England, and speakers of non-splitting accents often try to introduce it into their speech, sometimes resulting in hypercorrections such as pronouncing pudding /pʌdɪŋ/.

Occasionally, speakers of one accent may believe the speakers of another accent to have undergone a merger, when there has really been a chain shift.

See also edit

References edit

Notes edit

  1. ^ a b Henrich Hock, Hans (1991). Principles of Historical Linguistics (Second ed.). Berlin; New York: Walter de Gruyter. pp. 53–4.
  2. ^ a b Crowley, Terry; Bowern, Claire (2010). An Introduction to Historical Linguistics (4th ed.). Oxford; New York: Oxford University Press. p. 69. ISBN 9780195365542.
  3. ^ The same PIE root *H₂egʷnós yielded Greek ἀμνός ámnos "lamb". In Latin, PIE labiovelars were regularly delabialized before another consonant: relictus "left behind" < *likʷ-to- (cf. relinquō "leave behind", Greek leipō).
  4. ^ While Roman grammarians generally make some fairly fine observations about Latin phonetics, they do not mention g = [ŋ] despite being thoroughly familiar with the idea from Greek orthography, where |γ| = [ŋ] before /k/ and /g/, as in agkúlos "bent" /aŋkýlos/, ággellos "messenger" /áŋɡellos/. This is likely to be a mere oversight.

Sources edit

  1. Hale, M. (2007), Historical linguistics: Theory and method, Oxford, Blackwell[2][3]
  2. Hale, M., Kissock, M., & Reiss, C. (2014) An I-Language Approach to Phonologization and Lexification. Chapter 20. The Oxford Handbook of Historical Phonology. Edited by Patrick Honeybone and Joseph Salmons
  3. Hoenigswald, H. (1965). Language change and linguistic reconstruction. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

phonological, change, this, article, multiple, issues, please, help, improve, discuss, these, issues, talk, page, learn, when, remove, these, template, messages, this, article, needs, additional, citations, verification, please, help, improve, this, article, a. This article has multiple issues Please help improve it or discuss these issues on the talk page Learn how and when to remove these template messages This article needs additional citations for verification Please help improve this article by adding citations to reliable sources Unsourced material may be challenged and removed Find sources Phonological change news newspapers books scholar JSTOR July 2007 Learn how and when to remove this template message This article possibly contains original research Please improve it by verifying the claims made and adding inline citations Statements consisting only of original research should be removed June 2010 Learn how and when to remove this template message This article may be too technical for most readers to understand Please help improve it to make it understandable to non experts without removing the technical details April 2021 Learn how and when to remove this template message Learn how and when to remove this template message This article contains phonetic transcriptions in the International Phonetic Alphabet IPA For an introductory guide on IPA symbols see Help IPA For the distinction between and see IPA Brackets and transcription delimiters In historical linguistics phonological change is any sound change that alters the distribution of phonemes in a language In other words a language develops a new system of oppositions among its phonemes Old contrasts may disappear new ones may emerge or they may simply be rearranged 1 Sound change may be an impetus for changes in the phonological structures of a language and likewise phonological change may sway the process of sound change 1 One process of phonological change is rephonemicization in which the distribution of phonemes changes by either addition of new phonemes or a reorganization of existing phonemes 2 Mergers and splits are types of rephonemicization and are discussed further below Contents 1 Types 2 Phonetic vs phonological change 3 Merger 3 1 Conditioned merger 3 1 1 Example from Middle English 3 1 2 Devoicing of voiced stops in German 3 1 3 Rhotacism in Latin 3 1 4 Nasal assimilation and gn in Latin 3 1 5 Concerning the number of contrasts 3 2 Unconditioned merger 3 2 1 Examples 4 Split 5 Loss 6 Phonemic differentiation 6 1 Examples 6 2 Chain shifts 6 3 Phonemic mergers 6 4 Phonemic splits 7 See also 8 References 8 1 Notes 8 2 SourcesTypes editIn a typological scheme first systematized by Henry M Hoenigswald in 1965 a historical sound law can only affect a phonological system in one of three ways Conditioned merger which Hoenigswald calls primary split in which some instances of phoneme A become an existing phoneme B the number of phonemes does not change only their distribution Phonemic split which Hoenigswald calls secondary split in which some instances of A become a new phoneme B this is phonemic differentiation in which the number of phonemes increases Unconditioned merger in which all instances of phonemes A and B become A this is phonemic reduction in which the number of phonemes decreases This classification does not consider mere changes in pronunciation that is phonetic change even chain shifts in which neither the number nor the distribution of phonemes is affected Phonetic vs phonological change editPhonetic change can occur without any modification to the phoneme inventory or phonemic correspondences This change is purely allophonic or subphonemic This can entail one of two changes either the phoneme turns into a new allophone meaning the phonetic form changes or the distribution of allophones of the phoneme changes 2 For the most part phonetic changes are examples of allophonic differentiation or assimilation i e sounds in specific environments acquire new phonetic features or perhaps lose phonetic features they originally had For example the devoicing of the vowels i and ɯ in certain environments in Japanese the nasalization of vowels before nasals common but not universal changes in point of articulation of stops and nasals under the influence of adjacent vowels Phonetic change in this context refers to the lack of phonological restructuring not a small degree of sound change For example chain shifts such as the Great Vowel Shift in which nearly all of the vowels of the English language changed or the allophonic differentiation of s originally s into s z ʃ ʒ ʂ ʐ 8 x xʷ h do not qualify as phonological change as long as all of the phones remain in complementary distribution Many phonetic changes provide the raw ingredients for later phonemic innovations In Proto Italic for example intervocalic s became z It was a phonetic change merely a mild and superficial complication in the phonological system but when z merged with r the effect on the phonological system was greater The example will be discussed below under conditioned merger Similarly in the prehistory of Indo Iranian the velars k and g acquired distinctively palatal articulation before front vowels e i e i so that ke came to be pronounced t ʃe and ge d ʒe but the phones t ʃ and d ʒ occurred only in that environment However when e o a later fell together as Proto Indo Iranian a and e ō a likewise fell together as a the result was that the allophonic palatal and velar stops now contrasted in identical environments ka and ca ga and ǰa and so on The difference became phonemic The law of palatals is an example of phonemic split Sound changes generally operate for a limited period of time and once established new phonemic contrasts rarely remain tied to their ancestral environments For example Sanskrit acquired new ki and gi sequences via analogy and borrowing and likewise cu ǰu cm and similar novelties and the reduction of the diphthong ay to Sanskrit e had no effect at all on preceding velar stops Merger editPhonemic merger is a loss of distinction between phonemes Occasionally the term reduction refers to phonemic merger It is not to be confused with the meaning of the word reduction in phonetics such as vowel reduction but phonetic changes may contribute to phonemic mergers For example in most North American English dialects the vowel in the word lot and vowel in the word palm have become the same sound and thus undergone a merger In most dialects of England the words father and farther are pronounced the same due to a merger created by non rhoticity or R dropping Conditioned merger edit Conditioned merger or primary split takes place when some but not all allophones of a phoneme say A merge with some other phoneme B The immediate results are these there are the same number of contrasts as before there are fewer words with A than before there are more words of B than before there is at least one environment for which A for the time being no longer occurs called a gap in the distribution of the phoneme there is under certain circumstances an alternation between A and B if inflection or derivation result in A sometimes but not always being in the environment in which it merged with B Example from Middle English edit This section may be too technical for most readers to understand Please help improve it to make it understandable to non experts without removing the technical details April 2021 Learn how and when to remove this template message For a simple example without alternation early Middle English d after stressed syllables followed by r became d modor faeder gt mother father dr weder gt weather and so on Since d was already a structure point in the language the innovation resulted merely in more d and less d and a gap in the distribution of d though not a very conspicuous one Note 1 thanks to borrowing from dialects as well as other languages the original distribution has been disturbed rudder adder in Standard English but forms with d are attested in nonstandard dialects Note 2 one who knows German can figure out which cases of English d were originally d and which changed from d Original d corresponds to t in German and original d corresponds to d Thus leather German Leder brother Bruder whether weder wether Widder pointing to original d in English weather German Wetter father Vater mother Mutter pointing to original d Note 3 alternation between d and d would have been a theoretical possibility in English as in sets like hard harder ride rider but any such details have been erased by the commonplace diachronic process called morphological leveling Devoicing of voiced stops in German edit A trivial if all pervasive example of conditioned merger is the devoicing of voiced stops in German when in word final position or immediately before a compound boundary see Help IPA Standard German hand hand gt hant cf plural Hande ˈhɛnde Handgelenk wrist ˈhantgelɛŋk bund league association gt bʊnt cf plural Bunde ˈbʏnde gold gold gt gɔlt halb half gt halp cf halbieren to halve halˈbiːʁen halbamtlich semi official ˈhalpʔamtlɪc berg mountain bɛɐ k cf plural Berge ˈbɛɐ ge klug clever wise gt kluːk cf fem kluge ˈkluːge There were of course also many cases of original voiceless stops in final position Bett bed bunt colorful Stock walking stick cane To sum up there are the same number of structure points as before p t k b d g but there are more cases of p t k than before and fewer of b d g and there is a gap in the distribution of b d g they are never found in word final position or before a compound boundary Note 1 this split is easily recoverable by internal reconstruction because it results in alternations whose conditions are transparent Thus Bund bunch as in keys bʊnt has a plural Bunde ˈbʏnde in contrast to bunt colorful with t in all environments feminine ˈbʊnte neuter ˈbʊntes and so on In a neutralizing environment such as a voiceless stop in word final position one cannot tell which of two possibilities was the original sound The choice is resolved if the corresponding segment can be found in a non neutralizing position as when a suffix follows Accordingly a non inflected form like und ʔʊnt and is historically opaque though as the spelling hints the t was originally d Note 2 unlike most phonological changes this one became a surface rule in German so loan words whose source had a voiced stop in the devoicing environment are taken into German with a voiceless one instead Klub club association klʊp from English club The same goes for truncated forms Bub for formal Bube boy is buːp Note 2a the surface alternation is what allows modern German orthography to write stops morphophonemically thus Leib loaf Hand hand Weg way all with voiceless final stops in the simplex form and in compounds but b d g in inflected forms In Old High and Middle High German all voiceless stops were written as pronounced hleip hant uuec and so on dd Note 3 the same distribution holds for s vs z but it arose by a completely different process the voicing of original s between vowels mus mouse gt Maus maʊs musiz mice for earlier musiz gt Mause ˈmɔʏze Original long now short ss does not voice medially as inkussen to kiss ˈkʏsen nor does s from Proto West Germanic t as in Wasser water ˈvasɐ Fasser kegs ˈfɛsɐ plural of Fass fas English vat mussig idle ˈmyːsɪc German ʃ as in Fisch fish reflects original sk in native words and does not become voiced in any environment Fischer fisherman ˈfɪʃɐ German has ʒ only in loanwords Genie ʒeˈniː genius Gage ˈɡaːʒe salary Rhotacism in Latin edit More typical of the aftermath of a conditioned merger is the famous case of rhotacism in Latin also seen in some Sabellian language spoken in the same area Proto Italic s gt Latin r between vowels gesō I do act gt Lat gerō but perfect gessi lt ges s and participle gestus lt ges to etc with unchanged s in all other environments even in the same paradigm This sound law is quite complete and regular and in its immediate wake there were no examples of s between vowels except for a few words with a special condition miser wretched caesaries bushy hair diser c tus eloquent that is rhotacism did not take place when an r followed the s However a new crop of s between vowels soon arose from three sources 1 a shortening of ss after a diphthong or long vowel causa lawsuit lt kawssa casa house lt kassa fusus poured melted lt xewssos 2 univerbation nisi nisi unless lt the phrase ne sei quasi quasi as if lt the phrase kʷam sei 3 borrowings such as rosa rose rosa from a Sabellian source the word is clearly somehow from Proto Italic ru8 red but equally clearly not native Latin and many words taken from or through Greek philosophia basis casia Mesopotamia etc etc Nasal assimilation and gn in Latin edit This section needs additional citations for verification Please help improve this article by adding citations to reliable sources in this section Unsourced material may be challenged and removed Find sources Phonological change news newspapers books scholar JSTOR February 2018 Learn how and when to remove this template message A particular example of a conditioned merger in Latin is the rule whereby syllable final stops when followed by a nasal consonant assimilated with it in nasality while preserving their original point of articulation supimos gt supmos gt summus highest sabnyom gt Samnium Samnium a region in the southern Apennines swepnos gt somnus sleep atnos gt annus year In some cases the underlying pre assimilation root can be retrieved from related lexical items in the language e g superior higher Sabini Samnites sopor deep sleep For some words only comparative evidence can help retrieve the original consonant for example the etymology of annus year as atnos is revealed by comparison with Gothic athna year According to this rule of nasal assimilation the sequences g n and k n would become ŋn with a velar nasal ŋ dek no gt dignus diŋnus worthy leg no leǵ gather gt lignum liŋnum firewood teg no s teg build gt tignum tiŋnum timber agʷnos gt ag nos 3 gt agnus aŋnus lamb The sound ŋ was not a phoneme of Latin but an allophone of g before n The sequence ŋn was regularly rendered in the orthography as gn 4 Some epigraphic inscriptions also feature non standard spellings e g SINNU for signum sign insigne INGNEM for ignem fire These are witness to the speakers hesitancy on how to best transcribe the sound ŋ in the sequence ŋn The regular nasal assimilation of Latin can be seen as a form of merger insofar as it resulted in the contrast between oral stops p b t d and nasal stops m n being regularly neutralized Concerning the number of contrasts edit This section needs additional citations for verification Please help improve this article by adding citations to reliable sources in this section Unsourced material may be challenged and removed Find sources Phonological change news newspapers books scholar JSTOR February 2018 Learn how and when to remove this template message One of the traits of conditioned merger as outlined above is that the total number of contrasts remains the same but it is possible for such splits to reduce the number of contrasts It happens if all of the conditioned merger products merge with one or another phoneme For example in Latin the Pre Latin phoneme 8 from Proto Italic tʰ lt PIE dh disappears as such by merging with three other sounds f from PIE bh and gʷh d and b Initially 8 gt f PItal tʰi n kʰ model shape gt 8i n x gt Lat fingō PIE root dheyǵh smear work with the hands cf Sanskrit dihanti they smear Avestandaeza wall Greek teikhos English dough lt OE dah besides dag lt PIE dhoyǵh PItal tʰwor door gt 8wor gt Lat fores door PIE dhwor like most reflexes plural only cf Eng door lt dhur Greek thura probably lt dhwor usually thurai pl Cf Latin ferō carry lt Proto Italic pʰer lt PIE bher Latin frater brother lt Proto Italic pʰrater lt PIE bhre H ter dd Medially adjacent to l r or u 8 becomes b PItal wertʰom word gt wer8om gt werdom werbom gt Lat verbum cf English word lt wurdaⁿ lt PIE wṛdhom Lithuanian vaṙdas name PItal rutʰros red gt ru8ros gt rudros rubros gt Latin ruber via rubers lt rubrs lt rubros cf rubra fem rubrum neut PItal tʰlo tʰla tool suffix gt Latin bulum bula PIE peH dhlo nourishment gt PItal pa tʰlo gt pa8lo gt Latin pabulum PIE suH dhleH sewing implement gt PItal sutʰla gt su8la gt Latin subula cobbler s awl Intervocalic Latin b is from PIE bh s and rarely and problematically b Lat ambō both lt PIE ambh or H embh cf Greek amphi Lat crabrō hornet lt ḱṛHs ron cf Vedic sirṣn hornet Lat cannabis hemp cf Old English haenep hemp The change of sr to br is itself presumably via 8r gt dr gt br dd Elsewhere 8 becomes d PItal metʰyo middle gt me8yo gt Pre Lat medyo gt Lat medius three syllables PIE medhyo cf Sanskrit madhya Greek mes s os lt meth yo PItal pʰeytʰ gt fey8 gt feyd gt Lat fidus trusting cf Greek peithomai am persuaded English bid order ask Intervocalic d in Latin comes from PIE d in ped foot sidere to sit down cord heart dd There is no alternation to give away the historical story there via internal reconstruction the evidence for these changes is almost entirely from comparative reconstruction That reconstruction makes it easy to unriddle the story behind the weird forms of the Latin paradigm jubeō order jussi perfect jussus participle If the root is inherited it would have to have been PIE yewdh Unconditioned merger edit Unconditioned merger that is complete loss of a contrast between two or more phonemes is not very common Most mergers are conditioned That is most apparent mergers of A and B have an environment or two in which A did something else such as drop or merge with C Typical is the unconditioned merger seen in the Celtic conflation of the PIE plain voiced series of stops with the breathy voiced series bh dh ǵh gh are indistinguishable in Celtic etymology from the reflexes of b d ǵ g The collapse of the contrast cannot be stated in whole series terms because the labiovelars do not co operate PIE gʷ everywhere falls together with the reflexes of b and bh as Proto Celtic b but gʷh seems to have become PCelt gʷ lining up with PCelt kʷ lt PIE kʷ Examples edit OEy andy short and long high front rounded vowels fell together with i andi via a simple phonetic unrounding OEhypp cynn cyssan brycg fyllan fyr mys bryd became modern hip kin kiss bridge fill fire mice bride There is no way to tell by inspection whether a modern i ay goes back to a rounded or an unrounded vowel The change is not even reflected in modern spelling since it took place too early to be captured in Middle English Spelling conventions Of course current spellings like type thyme psyche etc have nothing to do with OE y y There is a massive consistent body of evidence that PIE l and r merged totally in Proto Indo Iranian as did PIE e o a into Proto Indo Iranian a The evolution of Romance shows a systematic collection of unconditioned mergers in connection with the loss of Latin vowel length Latin had ten vowels five long and five short i i e e a a and so on In the variety of Romance underlying Sardo and some other dialects of the islands the ten vowels simply fell together pairwise in no way are Latin e e say reflected differently In Proto Western Romance the ancestor of French Iberian Italian north of the Spezia Rimini line etc however things happened differently Latin a a merged totally as in Sardo but the other vowels all behaved differently Upon losing the feature of length Latin i u merged with nothing but the short high vowels front and back merged with the long mid vowels thus Latin i e are uniformly reflected as PWRom ẹ in the standard Romance notation and u ō become ọ PWRom ẹ is reflected in French in open syllables as wa spelled oi voile sail foin hay doigt finger quoi what are from Latin velum fenum digitus via dictu quid respectively There is no way of telling in French which one of the two Latin vowels is the source of any given wa Another example is provided by Japonic languages Proto Japanese had 8 vowels it has been reduced to 5 in modern Japanese but in Yaeyama the vowel mergers progressed further to 3 vowels Split editIn a split Hoenigswald s secondary split a new contrast arises when allophones of a phoneme cease being in complementary distribution and are therefore necessarily independent structure points i e contrastive This mostly comes about because of some loss of distinctiveness in the environment of one or more allophones of a phoneme A simple example is the rise of the contrast between nasal and oral vowels in French A full account of this history is complicated by the subsequent changes in the phonetics of the nasal vowels but the development can be compendiously illustrated via the present day French phonemes a and a Step 1 a gt a when a nasal immediately follows cantu song gt tʃantu still phonemically tʃantu Step 2 at some point in the history of French when speakers consistently stopped making an oral closure with the tongue we had tʃat that is tʃat if not ʃat and finally with the loss of the final stop modern French ʃa chant song distinct from French ʃa chat cat solely by the contrast between the nasal and the oral articulation of the vowels and thus with many other forms in which a and a contrast Note 1 the nasalization of a vowel before a nasal is found very widely in the world s languages but is not at all universal In modern French for example vowels before a nasal are oral That they used to be nasalized like the vowels before lost nasals is indicated by certain phonetic changes not always reflected in the orthography Fr femme woman fam with the lowering of ɛ nasalized ɛ to a prior to denasalization dd Note 2 unusually for a split the history of the French innovation even including some changes in vowel cavity features can be readily inferred by internal reconstruction This is because the contrastive feature nasal in a vowel system usually has a nasal in its history which makes for straightforward surmises There are also clear alternations as bɔ good masc vs bɔn fem while such pairs as fin fine fem and fɛ masc together with derivatives like raffine rafine refined indicate what happened to nasalized i dd Phonemic split was a major factor in the creation of the contrast between voiced and voiceless fricatives in English Originally to oversimplify a bit Old English fricatives were voiced between voiced sounds and voiceless elsewhere Thus f was f in fisc fiʃ fish fyllen to fill fyllen haeft prisoner ofthyrsted of8yrsted athirst lif life wulf wolf But in say the dative singular of life that is life the form was li ve as in English alive being an old prepositional phrase on life the plural of wulf wulfas was wulvas as still seen inwolves The voiced fricative is typically seen in verbs too often with variations in vowel length of diverse sources gift but give shelf but shelve Such alternations are to be seen even in loan words as proof vs prove though not as a rule in borrowed plurals thus proofs uses with voiceless fricatives Note 1 unlike the French example there is no chance of recovering the historical source of the alternations in English between s 8 f and z d v merely through inspection of the modern forms The conditioning factor original location of the voiced alternants between vowels for example is quite lost and with little reason to even suspect the original state of affairs and anyway the original distributions have been much disturbed by analogical leveling Worthy and in some dialects greasy have voiced fricative next to the voiceless ones in worth andgrease but adjectives in y otherwise do not alternate bossy glassy leafy earthy breathy saucy etc cf glaze leaves breathe and note that even in dialects with z ingreasy the verb to grease always has s Note 2 the phoneme ʃ does not alternate with ʒ and never did In native words ʃ is from sk and either the change of this sequence to ʃ postdated the rearranging of voicing in pre Old English fricatives or else it was phonetically long between vowels originally much like the ʃ of present day Italian pesce fish is phonetically peʃːe and long fricatives just like sequences of fricatives were always voiceless in Old English as in cyssan to kiss The Early Modern English development of ʃ lt sj as in nation mission assure vastly postdated the period when fricatives became voiced between vowels Note 3 a common misstatement of cases like OE f gt Modern English f v is that a new phoneme has been created Not so A new contrast has been created Both NE f and v are new phonemes differing in phonetic specifications and distribution from OE f Without doubt one component in this misunderstanding is the orthography If instead of speaking of the development of Old English f we said that OE ɰ split into f and v there would presumably be less confused talk of a new phoneme arising in the process Loss editIn Hoenigswald s original scheme loss the disappearance of a segment or even of a whole phoneme was treated as a form of merger depending on whether the loss was conditioned or unconditioned The element that a vanished segment or phoneme merged with was zero The situation in which a highly inflected language has formations without any affix at all Latin alter the other for example is quite common but it is the only one nominative singular masculine altera nominative singular feminine alterum accusative singular masculine etc of the 30 forms that make up the paradigm that is not explicitly marked with endings for gender number and case From a historical perspective there is no problem since alter is from alteros overtly nominative singular and masculine with the regular loss of the short vowel after r and the truncation of the resulting word final cluster rs Descriptively however it is problematic to say that the nominative singular masculine is signaled by the absence of any affix It is simpler to view alter as more than what it looks like alterO marked for case number and gender by an affix like the other 29 forms in the paradigm It is merely that the marker in question is not a phoneme or sequence of phonemes but the element O Along the way it is hard to know when to stop positing zeros and whether to regard one zero as different from another For example if the zero not marking can as in he can as third person singular is the same zero that not marks deer as plural or if are both basically a single morphological placeholder If it is determined that there is a zero on the end of deer in three deer it is uncertain whether English adjectives agree with the number of the noun they modify using the same zero affix Deictics do so compare this deer these deer In some theories of syntax it is useful to have an overt marker on a singular noun in a sentence such as My head hurts because the syntactic mechanism needs something explicit to generate the singular suffix on the verb Thus all English singular nouns may be marked with yet another zero It seems possible to avoid all those issues by considering loss as a separate basic category of phonological change and leave zero out of it As stated above one can regard loss as both a kind of conditioned merger when only some expressions of a phoneme are lost and a disappearance of a whole structure point The former is much more common than the latter In Latin are many consonant clusters that lose a member or two such as these tostus toasted dried lt torstos multrum milking stool lt molktrom scultus carved lt scolptos cena dinner lt kertsna luna moon lt louwksna lantern or the like Greek lost all stops from the end of a word so kʷit what gt Greek ti deḱṃt ten gt deka wanakt O prince gt ana but stops generally survive elsewhere PIE s drops medially between voiced sounds in Greek but is preserved in final position and in some consonant clusters Old English x voiceless velar fricative is everywhere lost as such but usually leaves traces behind transphonologization In furh furrow and mearh marrow it vocalizes It is elided with varying effects on the preceding vowel such as lengthening in night knight might taught naught freight fought plow Brit plough OE plōh bought through though slaughter but f in laugh trough tough enough and daughter can be found in The Pilgrim s Progress rhyming with after and the spelling dafter is actually attested The x phoneme still exists in some onomatopoeiac words like ugh note the spelling uses gh which indicates that when they were coined there was still some understanding of the phonemic meaning of gh yech and chutzpah g k are lost in English in word initial position before n gnaw gnat knight know t is lost after fricatives before nasals and l soften castle bristle chestnut Christmas hasten In many words f that is Old English v was lost between vowels auger hawk newt lt OE nafogar hafoc efete lizard and in some alternative poetic forms e en evening o er over e er ever Scottish siller silver and others The ends of words often have sound laws that apply there only and many such special developments consist of the loss of a segment The early history and prehistory of English has seen several waves of loss of elements vowels and consonants alike from the ends of words first in Proto Germanic then to Proto West Germanic then to Old and Middle and Modern English shedding bits from the ends of words at every step of the way There is in Modern English next to nothing left of the elaborate inflectional and derivational apparatus of PIE or of Proto Germanic because of the successive ablation of the phonemes making up these suffixes Total unconditional loss is as mentioned not very common Latin h appears to have been lost everywhere in all varieties of Proto Romance except Romanian Proto Indo European laryngeals survived as consonants only in Anatolian languages but left plenty of traces of their former presence see laryngeal theory Phonemic differentiation editPhonemic differentiation is the phenomenon of a language maximizing the acoustic distance between its phonemes Examples edit For example in many languages including English most front vowels are unrounded while most back vowels are rounded There are no languages in which all front vowels are rounded and all back vowels are unrounded The most likely explanation for this citation needed is that front vowels have a higher second formant F2 than back vowels and unrounded vowels have a higher F2 than rounded vowels Thus unrounded front vowels and rounded back vowels have maximally different F2s enhancing their phonemic differentiation Phonemic differentiation can have an effect on diachronic sound change In chain shifts phonemic differentiation is maintained while in phonemic mergers it is lost Phonemic splits involve the creation of two phonemes out of one which then tend to diverge because of phonemic differentiation Chain shifts edit Main article Chain shift In a chain shift one phoneme moves in acoustic space causing other phonemes to move as well to maintain optimal phonemic differentiation An example from American English is the Northern cities vowel shift 1 where the raising of ae has triggered a fronting of ɑ which in turn has triggered a lowering of ɔ and so forth Phonemic mergers edit If a phoneme moves in acoustic space but its neighbors do not move in a chain shift a phonemic merger may occur In that case a single phoneme results where an earlier stage of the language had two phonemes that is also called phonetic neutralization A well known example of a phonemic merger in American English is the cot caught merger by which the vowel phonemes ɑ and ɔ illustrated by the words cot and caught respectively have merged into a single phoneme in some accents Phonemic splits edit In a phonemic split a phoneme at an earlier stage of the language is divided into two phonemes over time Usually it happens when a phoneme has two allophones appearing in different environments but sound change eliminates the distinction between the two environments For example in umlaut in the Germanic languages the back vowels u o originally had front rounded allophones y o before the vowel i in a following syllable When sound change caused the syllables containing i to be lost a phonemic split resulted making y o distinct phonemes It is sometimes difficult to determine whether a split or a merger has happened if one dialect has two phonemes corresponding to a single phoneme in another dialect diachronic research is usually required to determine the dialect that is conservative and the one that is innovative When phonemic change occurs differently in the standard language and in dialects the dialect pronunciation is considered nonstandard and may be stigmatized In descriptive linguistics however the question of which splits and mergers are prestigious and which are stigmatized is irrelevant However such stigmatization can lead to hypercorrection when the dialect speakers attempt to imitate the standard language but overshoot as with the foot strut split where failing to make the split is stigmatized in Northern England and speakers of non splitting accents often try to introduce it into their speech sometimes resulting in hypercorrections such as pronouncing pudding pʌdɪŋ Occasionally speakers of one accent may believe the speakers of another accent to have undergone a merger when there has really been a chain shift See also editChain shift Drift linguistics Language change Phonological history of English consonants Phonological history of English vowels Phonological history of the English language Sound change Vowel shift Index DiachronicaReferences editNotes edit a b Henrich Hock Hans 1991 Principles of Historical Linguistics Second ed Berlin New York Walter de Gruyter pp 53 4 a b Crowley Terry Bowern Claire 2010 An Introduction to Historical Linguistics 4th ed Oxford New York Oxford University Press p 69 ISBN 9780195365542 The same PIE root H egʷnos yielded Greek ἀmnos amnos lamb In Latin PIE labiovelars were regularly delabialized before another consonant relictus left behind lt likʷ to cf relinquō leave behind Greek leipō While Roman grammarians generally make some fairly fine observations about Latin phonetics they do not mention g ŋ despite being thoroughly familiar with the idea from Greek orthography where g ŋ before k and g as in agkulos bent aŋkylos aggellos messenger aŋɡellos This is likely to be a mere oversight Sources edit Hale M 2007 Historical linguistics Theory and method Oxford Blackwell 2 3 Hale M Kissock M amp Reiss C 2014 An I Language Approach to Phonologization and Lexification Chapter 20 The Oxford Handbook of Historical Phonology Edited by Patrick Honeybone and Joseph Salmons Hoenigswald H 1965 Language change and linguistic reconstruction Chicago University of Chicago Press Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Phonological change amp oldid 1176174403 Split, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.