fbpx
Wikipedia

Behavioural genetics

Behavioural genetics, also referred to as behaviour genetics, is a field of scientific research that uses genetic methods to investigate the nature and origins of individual differences in behaviour. While the name "behavioural genetics" connotes a focus on genetic influences, the field broadly investigates the extent to which genetic and environmental factors influence individual differences, and the development of research designs that can remove the confounding of genes and environment. Behavioural genetics was founded as a scientific discipline by Francis Galton in the late 19th century, only to be discredited through association with eugenics movements before and during World War II. In the latter half of the 20th century, the field saw renewed prominence with research on inheritance of behaviour and mental illness in humans (typically using twin and family studies), as well as research on genetically informative model organisms through selective breeding and crosses. In the late 20th and early 21st centuries, technological advances in molecular genetics made it possible to measure and modify the genome directly. This led to major advances in model organism research (e.g., knockout mice) and in human studies (e.g., genome-wide association studies), leading to new scientific discoveries.

Findings from behavioural genetic research have broadly impacted modern understanding of the role of genetic and environmental influences on behaviour. These include evidence that nearly all researched behaviours are under a significant degree of genetic influence, and that influence tends to increase as individuals develop into adulthood. Further, most researched human behaviours are influenced by a very large number of genes and the individual effects of these genes are very small. Environmental influences also play a strong role, but they tend to make family members more different from one another, not more similar.

History edit

 
Farmers with wheat and cattle—Ancient Egyptian art 1,422 BCE displaying domesticated animals

Selective breeding and the domestication of animals is perhaps the earliest evidence that humans considered the idea that individual differences in behaviour could be due to natural causes.[1] Plato and Aristotle each speculated on the basis and mechanisms of inheritance of behavioural characteristics.[2] Plato, for example, argued in The Republic that selective breeding among the citizenry to encourage the development of some traits and discourage others, what today might be called eugenics, was to be encouraged in the pursuit of an ideal society.[2][3] Behavioural genetic concepts also existed during the English renaissance, where William Shakespeare perhaps first coined the phrase "nature versus nurture" in The Tempest, where he wrote in Act IV, Scene I, that Caliban was "A devil, a born devil, on whose nature Nurture can never stick".[3][4]

Modern-day behavioural genetics began with Sir Francis Galton, a nineteenth-century intellectual and cousin of Charles Darwin.[3] Galton was a polymath who studied many subjects, including the heritability of human abilities and mental characteristics. One of Galton's investigations involved a large pedigree study of social and intellectual achievement in the English upper class. In 1869, 10 years after Darwin's On the Origin of Species, Galton published his results in Hereditary Genius.[5] In this work, Galton found that the rate of "eminence" was highest among close relatives of eminent individuals, and decreased as the degree of relationship to eminent individuals decreased. While Galton could not rule out the role of environmental influences on eminence, a fact which he acknowledged, the study served to initiate an important debate about the relative roles of genes and environment on behavioural characteristics. Through his work, Galton also "introduced multivariate analysis and paved the way towards modern Bayesian statistics" that are used throughout the sciences—launching what has been dubbed the "Statistical Enlightenment".[6]

 
Galton in his later years

The field of behavioural genetics, as founded by Galton, was ultimately undermined by another of Galton's intellectual contributions, the founding of the eugenics movement in 20th century society.[3] The primary idea behind eugenics was to use selective breeding combined with knowledge about the inheritance of behaviour to improve the human species.[3] The eugenics movement was subsequently discredited by scientific corruption and genocidal actions in Nazi Germany. Behavioural genetics was thereby discredited through its association to eugenics.[3] The field once again gained status as a distinct scientific discipline through the publication of early texts on behavioural genetics, such as Calvin S. Hall's 1951 book chapter on behavioural genetics, in which he introduced the term "psychogenetics",[7] which enjoyed some limited popularity in the 1960s and 1970s.[8][9] However, it eventually disappeared from usage in favour of "behaviour genetics".

The start of behaviour genetics as a well-identified field was marked by the publication in 1960 of the book Behavior Genetics by John L. Fuller and William Robert (Bob) Thompson.[1][10] It is widely accepted now that many if not most behaviours in animals and humans are under significant genetic influence, although the extent of genetic influence for any particular trait can differ widely.[11][12] A decade later, in February 1970, the first issue of the journal Behavior Genetics was published and in 1972 the Behavior Genetics Association was formed with Theodosius Dobzhansky elected as the association's first president. The field has since grown and diversified, touching many scientific disciplines.[3][13]

Methods edit

The primary goal of behavioural genetics is to investigate the nature and origins of individual differences in behaviour.[3] A wide variety of different methodological approaches are used in behavioural genetic research,[14] only a few of which are outlined below.

Animal studies edit

Investigators in animal behaviour genetics can carefully control for environmental factors and can experimentally manipulate genetic variants, allowing for a degree of causal inference that is not available in studies on human behavioural genetics.[15] In animal research selection experiments have often been employed. For example, laboratory house mice have been bred for open-field behaviour,[16] thermoregulatory nesting,[17] and voluntary wheel-running behaviour.[18] A range of methods in these designs are covered on those pages. Behavioural geneticists using model organisms employ a range of molecular techniques to alter, insert, or delete genes. These techniques include knockouts, floxing, gene knockdown, or genome editing using methods like CRISPR-Cas9.[19] These techniques allow behavioural geneticists different levels of control in the model organism's genome, to evaluate the molecular, physiological, or behavioural outcome of genetic changes.[20] Animals commonly used as model organisms in behavioural genetics include mice,[21] zebra fish,[22] and the nematode species C. elegans.[23]

Machine learning and A.I. developments are allowing researchers to design experiments that are able to manage the complexity and large data sets generated, allowing for increasingly complex behavioural experiments. [24]

Human studies edit

Some research designs used in behavioural genetic research are variations on family designs (also known as pedigree designs), including twin studies and adoption studies.[14] Quantitative genetic modelling of individuals with known genetic relationships (e.g., parent-child, sibling, dizygotic and monozygotic twins) allows one to estimate to what extent genes and environment contribute to phenotypic differences among individuals.[25]

Twin and family studies edit

 
Pedigree chart showing an inheritance pattern consistent with autosomal dominant transmission. Behavioural geneticists have used pedigree studies to investigate the genetic and environmental basis of behaviour.

The basic intuition of the twin study is that monozygotic twins share 100% of their genome and dizygotic twins share, on average, 50% of their segregating genome. Thus, differences between the two members of a monozygotic twin pair can only be due to differences in their environment, whereas dizygotic twins will differ from one another due to genes in addition to the environment. Under this simplistic model, if dizygotic twins differ more than monozygotic twins it can only be attributable to genetic influences. An important assumption of the twin model is the equal environment assumption[26] that monozygotic twins have the same shared environmental experiences as dizygotic twins. If, for example, monozygotic twins tend to have more similar experiences than dizygotic twins—and these experiences themselves are not genetically mediated through gene-environment correlation mechanisms—then monozygotic twins will tend to be more similar to one another than dizygotic twins for reasons that have nothing to do with genes.[27] While this assumption should be kept in mind when interpreting the results of twin studies, research tends to suppot the equal environment assumption.[28]

Twin studies of monozygotic and dizygotic twins use a biometrical formulation to describe the influences on twin similarity and to infer heritability.[25][29] The formulation rests on the basic observation that the variance in a phenotype is due to two sources, genes and environment. More formally,  , where   is the phenotype,   is the effect of genes,   is the effect of the environment, and   is a gene by environment interaction. The   term can be expanded to include additive ( ), dominance ( ), and epistatic ( ) genetic effects. Similarly, the environmental term   can be expanded to include shared environment ( ) and non-shared environment ( ), which includes any measurement error. Dropping the gene by environment interaction for simplicity (typical in twin studies) and fully decomposing the   and   terms, we now have  . Twin research then models the similarity in monozygotic twins and dizygotic twins using simplified forms of this decomposition, shown in the table.[25]

Decomposing the genetic and environmental contributions to twin similarity.[25]
Type of relationship Full decomposition Falconer's decomposition
Perfect similarity between siblings    
Monozygotic twin correlation( )    
Dizygotic twin correlation ( )    
Where   is an unknown (probably very small) quantity.

The simplified Falconer formulation can then be used to derive estimates of  ,  , and  . Rearranging and substituting the   and   equations one can obtain an estimate of the additive genetic variance, or heritability,  , the non-shared environmental effect   and, finally, the shared environmental effect  .[25] The Falconer formulation is presented here to illustrate how the twin model works. Modern approaches use maximum likelihood to estimate the genetic and environmental variance components.[30]

Genome-wide association studies edit

 
Study results about which and to what degree various traits, IQ and language-related skills appear to be influenced by genetics[31][32]

Behavioural genetics research also involves genome-wide association studies (GWAS). GWAS are observational studies which review hundreds of thousands of genetic variants from large samples of participants with the goal of finding alleles (genetic variants) that are associated with traits of interest. The goal may be to find genetic differences between a "disease" group and a "control" group, or to understand genetic contributions to traits that vary across a spectrum.[33] GWAS results showing genetic contributions to lifestyle behaviors and psychological conditions are particularly relevant to behavioural genetics.

The variants reviewed in GWAS can be as simple as a single nucleotide polymorphism (SNP), or can involve deletions or duplications of longer sequences of DNA. SNPs are the most common focus. GWAS do not involve sequencing every base pair of a person's DNA. Instead, they often rely on genotype imputation based on known groups of genes which tend to be inherited together.[34]

Genetic variants identified through GWAS can be used to improve disease risk predictions, and can be used as controls when studying the effectiveness of disease treatments.[33] However, the genetic variants identified through GWAS are most likely to have a moderate effect on disease risk or development of a given trait. This is different from the strong genetic contribution seen in single-gene diseases. Additionally, associations between alleles and traits can differ between groups of different ancestry. This means that GWAS results from one population cannot accurately be applied to another population, and comparing genetic variation between populations can be difficult and complicated.[35][36]

Measured genetic variants edit

The Human Genome Project has allowed scientists to directly genotype the sequence of human DNA nucleotides.[37] Once genotyped, genetic variants can be tested for association with a behavioural phenotype, such as mental disorder, cognitive ability, personality, and so on.[38]

  • Candidate Genes. One popular approach has been to test for association candidate genes with behavioural phenotypes, where the candidate gene is selected based on some a priori theory about biological mechanisms involved in the manifestation of a behavioural trait or phenotype.[39] In general, such studies have proven difficult to broadly replicate[40][41][42][43] and there has been concern raised that the false positive rate in this type of research is high.[39][44]
  • Genome-wide association studies. As discussed in the previous section, in genome-wide association studies, researchers test the relationship of millions of genetic polymorphisms with behavioural phenotypes across the genome.[38] This approach to genetic association studies is largely atheoretical, and typically not guided by a particular biological hypothesis regarding the phenotype.[38] Genetic association findings for behavioural traits and psychiatric disorders have been found to be highly polygenic (involving many small genetic effects).[45][46][47][48][49]
  • SNP heritability and co-heritability. Recently, researchers have begun to use similarity between classically unrelated people at their measured single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) to estimate genetic variation or covariation that is tagged by SNPs, using mixed effects models implemented in software such as genome-wide complex trait analysis (GCTA).[50][51] To do this, researchers find the average genetic relatedness over all SNPs between all individuals in a (typically large) sample, and use Haseman–Elston regression or restricted maximum likelihood to estimate the genetic variation that is "tagged" by, or predicted by, the SNPs. The proportion of phenotypic variation that is accounted for by the genetic relatedness has been called "SNP heritability".[52] Intuitively, SNP heritability increases to the degree that phenotypic similarity is predicted by genetic similarity at measured SNPs, and is expected to be lower than the true narrow-sense heritability to the degree that measured SNPs fail to tag (typically rare) causal variants.[53] The value of this method is that it is an independent way to estimate heritability that does not require the same assumptions as those in twin and family studies, and that it gives insight into the allelic frequency spectrum of the causal variants underlying trait variation.[54]

Quasi-experimental designs edit

Some behavioural genetic designs are useful not to understand genetic influences on behaviour, but to control for genetic influences to test environmentally-mediated influences on behaviour.[55] Such behavioural genetic designs may be considered a subset of natural experiments,[56] quasi-experiments that attempt to take advantage of naturally occurring situations that mimic true experiments by providing some control over an independent variable. Natural experiments can be particularly useful when experiments are infeasible, due to practical or ethical limitations.[56]

A general limitation of observational studies is that the relative influences of genes and environment are confounded. A simple demonstration of this fact is that measures of 'environmental' influence are heritable.[57] Thus, observing a correlation between an environmental risk factor and a health outcome is not necessarily evidence for environmental influence on the health outcome. Similarly, in observational studies of parent-child behavioural transmission, for example, it is impossible to know if the transmission is due to genetic or environmental influences, due to the problem of passive gene–environment correlation.[56] The simple observation that the children of parents who use drugs are more likely to use drugs as adults does not indicate why the children are more likely to use drugs when they grow up. It could be because the children are modelling their parents' behaviour. Equally plausible, it could be that the children inherited drug-use-predisposing genes from their parent, which put them at increased risk for drug use as adults regardless of their parents' behaviour. Adoption studies, which parse the relative effects of rearing environment and genetic inheritance, find a small to negligible effect of rearing environment on smoking, alcohol, and marijuana use in adopted children,[58] [non-primary source needed] but a larger effect of rearing environment on harder drug use.[59][non-primary source needed]

Other behavioural genetic designs include discordant twin studies,[55] children of twins designs,[60] and Mendelian randomization.[61]

General findings edit

There are many broad conclusions to be drawn from behavioural genetic research about the nature and origins of behaviour.[3][62] Three major conclusions include:[3]

  1. all behavioural traits and disorders are influenced by genes
  2. environmental influences tend to make members of the same family more different, rather than more similar
  3. the influence of genes tends to increase in relative importance as individuals age.

Genetic influences on behaviour are pervasive edit

It is clear from multiple lines of evidence that all researched behavioural traits and disorders are influenced by genes; that is, they are heritable. The single largest source of evidence comes from twin studies, where it is routinely observed that monozygotic (identical) twins are more similar to one another than are same-sex dizygotic (fraternal) twins.[11][12]

The conclusion that genetic influences are pervasive has also been observed in research designs that do not depend on the assumptions of the twin method. Adoption studies show that adoptees are routinely more similar to their biological relatives than their adoptive relatives for a wide variety of traits and disorders.[3] In the Minnesota Study of Twins Reared Apart, monozygotic twins separated shortly after birth were reunited in adulthood.[63] These adopted, reared-apart twins were as similar to one another as were twins reared together on a wide range of measures including general cognitive ability, personality, religious attitudes, and vocational interests, among others.[63] Approaches using genome-wide genotyping have allowed researchers to measure genetic relatedness between individuals and estimate heritability based on millions of genetic variants. Methods exist to test whether the extent of genetic similarity (aka, relatedness) between nominally unrelated individuals (individuals who are not close or even distant relatives) is associated with phenotypic similarity.[51] Such methods do not rely on the same assumptions as twin or adoption studies, and routinely find evidence for heritability of behavioural traits and disorders.[47][49][64]

Nature of environmental influence edit

Just as all researched human behavioural phenotypes are influenced by genes (i.e., are heritable), all such phenotypes are also influenced by the environment.[11][62] The basic fact that monozygotic twins are genetically identical but are never perfectly concordant for psychiatric disorder or perfectly correlated for behavioural traits, indicates that the environment shapes human behaviour.[62]

The nature of this environmental influence, however, is such that it tends to make individuals in the same family more different from one another, not more similar to one another.[3] That is, estimates of shared environmental effects ( ) in human studies are small, negligible, or zero for the vast majority of behavioural traits and psychiatric disorders, whereas estimates of non-shared environmental effects ( ) are moderate to large.[11] From twin studies   is typically estimated at 0 because the correlation ( ) between monozygotic twins is at least twice the correlation ( ) for dizygotic twins. When using the Falconer variance decomposition ( ) this difference between monozygotic and dizygotic twin similarity results in an estimated  . The Falconer decomposition is simplistic.[25] It removes the possible influence of dominance and epistatic effects which, if present, will tend to make monozygotic twins more similar than dizygotic twins and mask the influence of shared environmental effects.[25] This is a limitation of the twin design for estimating  . However, the general conclusion that shared environmental effects are negligible does not rest on twin studies alone. Adoption research also fails to find large ( ) components; that is, adoptive parents and their adopted children tend to show much less resemblance to one another than the adopted child and his or her non-rearing biological parent.[3] In studies of adoptive families with at least one biological child and one adopted child, the sibling resemblance also tends to be nearly zero for most traits that have been studied.[11][65]

 
Similarity in twins and adoptees indicates a small role for shared environment in personality.

The figure provides an example from personality research, where twin and adoption studies converge on the conclusion of zero to small influences of shared environment on broad personality traits measured by the Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire including positive emotionality, negative emotionality, and constraint.[66]

Given the conclusion that all researched behavioural traits and psychiatric disorders are heritable, biological siblings will always tend to be more similar to one another than will adopted siblings. However, for some traits, especially when measured during adolescence, adopted siblings do show some significant similarity (e.g., correlations of .20) to one another. Traits that have been demonstrated to have significant shared environmental influences include internalizing and externalizing psychopathology,[67] substance use[68] [non-primary source needed] and dependence,[59][non-primary source needed] and intelligence.[68] [non-primary source needed]

Nature of genetic influence edit

Genetic effects on human behavioural outcomes can be described in multiple ways.[25] One way to describe the effect is in terms of how much variance in the behaviour can be accounted for by alleles in the genetic variant, otherwise known as the coefficient of determination or  . An intuitive way to think about   is that it describes the extent to which the genetic variant makes individuals, who harbour different alleles, different from one another on the behavioural outcome. A complementary way to describe effects of individual genetic variants is in how much change one expects on the behavioural outcome given a change in the number of risk alleles an individual harbours, often denoted by the Greek letter   (denoting the slope in a regression equation), or, in the case of binary disease outcomes by the odds ratio   of disease given allele status. Note the difference:   describes the population-level effect of alleles within a genetic variant;   or   describe the effect of having a risk allele on the individual who harbours it, relative to an individual who does not harbour a risk allele.[69]

When described on the   metric, the effects of individual genetic variants on complex human behavioural traits and disorders are vanishingly small, with each variant accounting for   of variation in the phenotype.[3] This fact has been discovered primarily through genome-wide association studies of complex behavioural phenotypes, including results on substance use,[70][71] personality,[72] fertility,[73] schizophrenia,[46] depression,[72][74] and endophenotypes including brain structure[75] and function.[76] There are a small handful of replicated and robustly studied exceptions to this rule, including the effect of APOE on Alzheimer's disease,[77] and CHRNA5 on smoking behaviour,[70] and ALDH2 (in individuals of East Asian ancestry) on alcohol use.[78]

On the other hand, when assessing effects according to the   metric, there are a large number of genetic variants that have very large effects on complex behavioural phenotypes. The risk alleles within such variants are exceedingly rare, such that their large behavioural effects impact only a small number of individuals. Thus, when assessed at a population level using the   metric, they account for only a small amount of the differences in risk between individuals in the population. Examples include variants within APP that result in familial forms of severe early onset Alzheimer's disease but affect only relatively few individuals. Compare this to risk alleles within APOE, which pose much smaller risk compared to APP, but are far more common and therefore affect a much greater proportion of the population.[79]

Finally, there are classical behavioural disorders that are genetically simple in their etiology, such as Huntington's disease. Huntington's is caused by a single autosomal dominant variant in the HTT gene, which is the only variant that accounts for any differences among individuals in their risk for developing the disease, assuming they live long enough.[80] In the case of genetically simple and rare diseases such as Huntington's, the variant   and the   are simultaneously large.[69]

Additional general findings edit

In response to general concerns about the replicability of psychological research, behavioural geneticists Robert Plomin, John C. DeFries, Valerie Knopik, and Jenae Neiderhiser published a review of the ten most well-replicated findings from behavioural genetics research.[62] The ten findings were:

  1. "All psychological traits show significant and substantial genetic influence."
  2. "No behavioural traits are 100% heritable."
  3. "Heritability is caused by many genes of small effect."
  4. "Phenotypic correlations between psychological traits show significant and substantial genetic mediation."
  5. "The heritability of intelligence increases throughout development."
  6. "Age-to-age stability is mainly due to genetics."
  7. "Most measures of the 'environment' show significant genetic influence."
  8. "Most associations between environmental measures and psychological traits are significantly mediated genetically."
  9. "Most environmental effects are not shared by children growing up in the same family."
  10. "Abnormal is normal."

Criticisms and controversies edit

Behavioural genetic research and findings have at times been controversial. Some of this controversy has arisen because behavioural genetic findings can challenge societal beliefs about the nature of human behaviour and abilities. Major areas of controversy have included genetic research on topics such as racial differences, intelligence, violence, and human sexuality.[81] Other controversies have arisen due to misunderstandings of behavioural genetic research, whether by the lay public or the researchers themselves.[3] For example, the notion of heritability is easily misunderstood to imply causality, or that some behaviour or condition is determined by one's genetic endowment.[82] When behavioural genetics researchers say that a behaviour is X% heritable, that does not mean that genetics causes, determines, or fixes up to X% of the behaviour. Instead, heritability is a statement about genetic differences correlated with trait differences on the population level.[citation needed]

Historically, perhaps the most controversial subject has been on race and genetics.[81] Race is not a scientifically exact term, and its interpretation can depend on one's culture and country of origin.[83] Instead, geneticists use concepts such as ancestry, which is more rigorously defined.[84] For example, a so-called "Black" race may include all individuals of relatively recent African descent ("recent" because all humans are descended from African ancestors). However, there is more genetic diversity in Africa than the rest of the world combined,[85] so speaking of a "Black" race is without a precise genetic meaning.[84]

Qualitative research has fostered arguments that behavioural genetics is an ungovernable field without scientific norms or consensus, which fosters controversy. The argument continues that this state of affairs has led to controversies including race, intelligence, instances where variation within a single gene was found to very strongly influence a controversial phenotype (e.g., the "gay gene" controversy), and others. This argument further states that because of the persistence of controversy in behaviour genetics and the failure of disputes to be resolved, behaviour genetics does not conform to the standards of good science.[86]

The scientific assumptions on which parts of behavioural genetic research are based have also been criticized as flawed.[82] Genome wide association studies are often implemented with simplifying statistical assumptions, such as additivity, which may be statistically robust but unrealistic for some behaviours. Critics further contend that, in humans, behaviour genetics represents a misguided form of genetic reductionism based on inaccurate interpretations of statistical analyses.[87] Studies comparing monozygotic (MZ) and dizygotic (DZ) twins assume that environmental influences will be the same in both types of twins, but this assumption may also be unrealistic. MZ twins may be treated more alike than DZ twins,[82] which itself may be an example of evocative gene–environment correlation, suggesting that one's genes influence their treatment by others. It is also not possible in twin studies to eliminate effects of the shared womb environment, although studies comparing twins who experience monochorionic and dichorionic environments in utero do exist, and indicate limited impact.[88] Studies of twins separated in early life include children who were separated not at birth but part way through childhood.[82] The effect of early rearing environment can therefore be evaluated to some extent in such a study, by comparing twin similarity for those twins separated early and those separated later.[63]

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ a b Loehlin JC (2009). "History of Behavior Genetics". In Kim Y (ed.). Handbook of Behavior Genetics (1 ed.). New York, NY: Springer. pp. 3–11. doi:10.1007/978-0-387-76727-7_1. ISBN 978-0-387-76726-0.
  2. ^ a b Maxson SC (30 August 2006). "A History of Behavior Genetics". In Jones BC, Mormede P (eds.). Neurobehavioral Genetics: Methods and Applications, Second Edition. CRC Press. ISBN 978-1-4200-0356-7.
  3. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o McGue M, Gottesman II (2015). "Behavior Genetics". The Encyclopedia of Clinical Psychology. pp. 1–11. doi:10.1002/9781118625392.wbecp578. ISBN 9781118625392.
  4. ^ Vaughan V, Vaughan AT (1999). The Tempest. The Arden Shakespeare (Third ed.). The Arden Shakespeare. p. 60. ISBN 978-1-903436-08-0.
  5. ^ . London: MacMillan and Co. 1869. Archived from the original on 7 December 2019. Retrieved 17 December 2009.
  6. ^ Stigler SM (July 2010). "Darwin, Galton and the Statistical Enlightenment". Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, Series A. 173 (3): 469–482. doi:10.1111/j.1467-985X.2010.00643.x. S2CID 53333238.
  7. ^ Hall CS (1951). "The genetics of behavior". In Stevens SS (ed.). Handbook of Experimental Psychology. New York: John Wiley and Sons. pp. 304–329.
  8. ^ Grigorenko EL, Ravich-Shcherbo I (1997). "Russian psychogenetics". In Grigorenko EL (ed.). Psychology of Russia: Past, Present, Future. Commack, NY: Nova Science. pp. 83–124.
  9. ^ Broadhurst PL (July 1969). "Psychogenetics of emotionality in the rat". Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 159 (3): 806–24. Bibcode:1969NYASA.159..806B. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.1969.tb12980.x. PMID 5260300. S2CID 42323956.
  10. ^ Fuller JL, Thompson WR (1960). Behavior Genetics. New York: John Wiley and Sons.
  11. ^ a b c d e Polderman TJ, Benyamin B, de Leeuw CA, Sullivan PF, van Bochoven A, Visscher PM, et al. (July 2015). "Meta-analysis of the heritability of human traits based on fifty years of twin studies" (PDF). Nature Genetics. 47 (7): 702–9. doi:10.1038/ng.3285. PMID 25985137. S2CID 205349969. (PDF) from the original on 2022-04-20. Retrieved 2019-01-30.
  12. ^ a b Turkheimer E (2000). "Three Laws of Behavior Genetics and What They Mean" (PDF). Current Directions in Psychological Science. 9 (5): 160–164. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.00084. S2CID 2861437. (PDF) from the original on 2016-10-09. Retrieved 2016-04-12.
  13. ^ Ayorech Z, Selzam S, Smith-Woolley E, Knopik VS, Neiderhiser JM, DeFries JC, et al. (September 2016). "Publication Trends Over 55 Years of Behavioral Genetic Research". Behavior Genetics. 46 (5): 603–7. doi:10.1007/s10519-016-9786-2. PMC 5206393. PMID 26992731.
  14. ^ a b Plomin R, DeFries JC, Knopik VS, Neiderhiser M (24 September 2012). Behavioral Genetics. Worth Publishers. ISBN 978-1-4292-4215-8. from the original on 31 December 2013. Retrieved 27 January 2016.
  15. ^ Plomin R. "Behaviour genetics". Encyclopedia Britannica. from the original on 17 April 2021. Retrieved 15 June 2018.
  16. ^ DeFries JC, Hegmann JP, Halcomb RA (August 1974). "Response to 20 generations of selection for open-field activity in mice". Behavioral Biology. 11 (4): 481–95. doi:10.1016/s0091-6773(74)90800-1. PMID 4415597.
  17. ^ Lynch CB (November 1980). "Response to divergent selection for nesting behavior in Mus musculus". Genetics. 96 (3): 757–65. doi:10.1093/genetics/96.3.757. PMC 1214374. PMID 7196362.
  18. ^ Swallow JG, Carter PA, Garland T (May 1998). "Artificial selection for increased wheel-running behavior in house mice". Behavior Genetics. 28 (3): 227–37. doi:10.1023/A:1021479331779. PMID 9670598. S2CID 18336243.
  19. ^ Heidenreich M, Zhang F (January 2016). "Applications of CRISPR-Cas systems in neuroscience". Nature Reviews. Neuroscience. 17 (1): 36–44. doi:10.1038/nrn.2015.2. PMC 4899966. PMID 26656253.
  20. ^ Singh P, Schimenti JC, Bolcun-Filas E (January 2015). "A mouse geneticist's practical guide to CRISPR applications". Genetics. 199 (1): 1–15. doi:10.1534/genetics.114.169771. PMC 4286675. PMID 25271304.
  21. ^ Cryan JF, Holmes A (September 2005). "The ascent of mouse: advances in modelling human depression and anxiety". Nature Reviews. Drug Discovery. 4 (9): 775–790. doi:10.1038/nrd1825. PMID 16138108. S2CID 18207374.
  22. ^ Wolman M, Granato M (March 2012). "Behavioral genetics in larval zebrafish: learning from the young". Developmental Neurobiology. 72 (3): 366–372. doi:10.1002/dneu.20872. PMC 6430578. PMID 22328273.
  23. ^ Wolinsky E, Way J (March 1990). "The behavioral genetics of Caenorhabditis elegans". Behavior Genetics. 20 (2): 169–189. doi:10.1007/bf01067789. PMID 2191646. S2CID 23719167.
  24. ^ Stacher Hörndli CN, Wong E, Ferris E, Bennett K, Steinwand S, Rhodes AN, et al. (August 2019). "Complex Economic Behavior Patterns Are Constructed from Finite, Genetically Controlled Modules of Behavior". Cell Reports. 28 (7): 1814–1829.e6. doi:10.1016/j.celrep.2019.07.038. PMC 7476553. PMID 31412249. S2CID 199662477.
  25. ^ a b c d e f g h Falconer DS (1989). Introduction to quantitative genetics. Longman, Scientific & Technical. ISBN 978-0-470-21162-5. from the original on 2021-04-22. Retrieved 2016-12-02.
  26. ^ Eaves L, Foley D, Silberg J (2003). "Has the "Equal Environments" assumption been tested in twin studies?". Twin Research. 6 (6): 486–9. doi:10.1375/136905203322686473. PMID 14965458.
  27. ^ Kendler KS, Neale MC, Kessler RC, Heath AC, Eaves LJ (January 1993). "A test of the equal-environment assumption in twin studies of psychiatric illness". Behavior Genetics. 23 (1): 21–7. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.595.7413. doi:10.1007/BF01067551. PMID 8476388. S2CID 9034050.
  28. ^ Harden KP (2021-09-21). The Genetic Lottery: Why DNA Matters for Social Equality. Princeton University Press. p. 276. ISBN 978-0-691-22670-5.
  29. ^ Jinks JL, Fulker DW (1970). "Comparison of the biometrical genetical, MAVA, and classical approaches to the analysis of the human behavior". Psychological Bulletin. 73 (5): 311–349. doi:10.1037/h0029135. PMID 5528333.
  30. ^ Martin NG, Eaves LJ (February 1977). "The genetical analysis of covariance structure". Heredity. 38 (1): 79–95. doi:10.1038/hdy.1977.9. PMID 268313.
  31. ^ "Massive genome study informs the biology of reading and language". Max Planck Society via medicalxpress.com. Retrieved 18 September 2022.
  32. ^ Eising E, Mirza-Schreiber N, de Zeeuw EL, Wang CA, Truong DT, Allegrini AG, et al. (August 2022). "Genome-wide analyses of individual differences in quantitatively assessed reading- and language-related skills in up to 34,000 people". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 119 (35): e2202764119. Bibcode:2022PNAS..11902764E. doi:10.1073/pnas.2202764119. PMC 9436320. PMID 35998220.
  33. ^ a b Uffelmann E, Huang QQ, Munung NS, De Vries J, Okada Y, Martin AR, et al. (2021-08-26). "Genome-wide association studies". Nature Reviews Methods Primers. 1 (1): 1–21. doi:10.1038/s43586-021-00056-9. ISSN 2662-8449.
  34. ^ Marchini J, Howie B (July 2010). "Genotype imputation for genome-wide association studies". Nature Reviews. Genetics. 11 (7): 499–511. doi:10.1038/nrg2796. PMID 20517342.
  35. ^ Tam V, Patel N, Turcotte M, Bossé Y, Paré G, Meyre D (August 2019). "Benefits and limitations of genome-wide association studies". Nature Reviews. Genetics. 20 (8): 467–484. doi:10.1038/s41576-019-0127-1. PMID 31068683.
  36. ^ Plomin R (2019-07-02). Blueprint: How DNA Makes Us Who We Are. MIT Press. ISBN 978-0-262-53798-8.
  37. ^ Lander ES (February 2011). "Initial impact of the sequencing of the human genome". Nature. 470 (7333): 187–97. Bibcode:2011Natur.470..187L. doi:10.1038/nature09792. hdl:1721.1/69154. PMID 21307931. S2CID 4344403.
  38. ^ a b c McCarthy MI, Abecasis GR, Cardon LR, Goldstein DB, Little J, Ioannidis JP, et al. (May 2008). "Genome-wide association studies for complex traits: consensus, uncertainty and challenges". Nature Reviews Genetics. 9 (5): 356–69. doi:10.1038/nrg2344. PMID 18398418. S2CID 15032294.
  39. ^ a b Duncan LE, Keller MC (October 2011). "A critical review of the first 10 years of candidate gene-by-environment interaction research in psychiatry". The American Journal of Psychiatry. 168 (10): 1041–9. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.11020191. PMC 3222234. PMID 21890791.
  40. ^ Farrell MS, Werge T, Sklar P, Owen MJ, Ophoff RA, O'Donovan MC, et al. (May 2015). "Evaluating historical candidate genes for schizophrenia". Molecular Psychiatry. 20 (5): 555–562. doi:10.1038/mp.2015.16. PMC 4414705. PMID 25754081.
  41. ^ Hewitt JK (January 2012). "Editorial policy on candidate gene association and candidate gene-by-environment interaction studies of complex traits". Behavior Genetics. 42 (1): 1–2. doi:10.1007/s10519-011-9504-z. PMID 21928046. S2CID 11492871.
  42. ^ Johnson EC, Border R, Melroy-Greif WE, de Leeuw CA, Ehringer MA, Keller MC (November 2017). "No Evidence That Schizophrenia Candidate Genes Are More Associated With Schizophrenia Than Noncandidate Genes". Biological Psychiatry. Risk Genes and the Emergence of Schizophrenia. 82 (10): 702–708. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.06.033. PMC 5643230. PMID 28823710.
  43. ^ Border R, Johnson EC, Evans LM, Smolen A, Berley N, Sullivan PF, et al. (May 2019). "No Support for Historical Candidate Gene or Candidate Gene-by-Interaction Hypotheses for Major Depression Across Multiple Large Samples". The American Journal of Psychiatry. 176 (5): 376–387. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2018.18070881. PMC 6548317. PMID 30845820.
  44. ^ Colhoun HM, McKeigue PM, Davey Smith G (March 2003). "Problems of reporting genetic associations with complex outcomes". Lancet. 361 (9360): 865–872. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(03)12715-8. PMID 12642066. S2CID 15679561.
  45. ^ Visscher PM, Brown MA, McCarthy MI, Yang J (January 2012). "Five years of GWAS discovery". American Journal of Human Genetics. 90 (1): 7–24. doi:10.1016/j.ajhg.2011.11.029. PMC 3257326. PMID 22243964.
  46. ^ a b Ripke S, Neale BM, Corvin A, Walters JT, Farh KH, Holmans PA, et al. (Schizophrenia Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium) (July 2014). "Biological insights from 108 schizophrenia-associated genetic loci". Nature. 511 (7510): 421–7. Bibcode:2014Natur.511..421S. doi:10.1038/nature13595. PMC 4112379. PMID 25056061.
  47. ^ a b Lee SH, DeCandia TR, Ripke S, Yang J, Sullivan PF, Goddard ME, et al. (February 2012). "Estimating the proportion of variation in susceptibility to schizophrenia captured by common SNPs". Nature Genetics. 44 (3): 247–50. doi:10.1038/ng.1108. PMC 3327879. PMID 22344220.
  48. ^ Sullivan PF, Daly MJ, O'Donovan M (July 2012). "Genetic architectures of psychiatric disorders: the emerging picture and its implications". Nature Reviews. Genetics. 13 (8): 537–51. doi:10.1038/nrg3240. PMC 4110909. PMID 22777127.
  49. ^ a b de Moor MH, van den Berg SM, Verweij KJ, Krueger RF, Luciano M, Arias Vasquez A, et al. (July 2015). "Meta-analysis of Genome-wide Association Studies for Neuroticism, and the Polygenic Association With Major Depressive Disorder". JAMA Psychiatry. 72 (7): 642–50. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.0554. PMC 4667957. PMID 25993607.
  50. ^ Yang J, Benyamin B, McEvoy BP, Gordon S, Henders AK, Nyholt DR, et al. (July 2010). "Common SNPs explain a large proportion of the heritability for human height". Nature Genetics. 42 (7): 565–9. doi:10.1038/ng.608. PMC 3232052. PMID 20562875.
  51. ^ a b Yang J, Lee SH, Goddard ME, Visscher PM (January 2011). "GCTA: a tool for genome-wide complex trait analysis". American Journal of Human Genetics. 88 (1): 76–82. doi:10.1016/j.ajhg.2010.11.011. PMC 3014363. PMID 21167468.
  52. ^ Lee SH, Yang J, Chen GB, Ripke S, Stahl EA, Hultman CM, et al. (2013). "Estimation of SNP heritability from dense genotype data". American Journal of Human Genetics. 93 (6): 1151–5. doi:10.1016/j.ajhg.2013.10.015. PMC 3852919. PMID 24314550.
  53. ^ Visscher PM, Yang J, Goddard ME (2010). "A commentary on 'common SNPs explain a large proportion of the heritability for human height' by Yang et al. (2010)". Twin Research and Human Genetics. 13 (6): 517–24. doi:10.1375/twin.13.6.517. PMID 21142928. S2CID 15730955.
  54. ^ Wray NR, Lee SH, Mehta D, Vinkhuyzen AA, Dudbridge F, Middeldorp CM (2014). "Research review: Polygenic methods and their application to psychiatric traits" (PDF). Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines. 55 (10): 1068–87. doi:10.1111/jcpp.12295. PMID 25132410. (PDF) from the original on 2017-01-16. Retrieved 2019-07-01.
  55. ^ a b McGue M, Osler M, Christensen K (September 2010). "Causal Inference and Observational Research: The Utility of Twins". Perspectives on Psychological Science. 5 (5): 546–56. doi:10.1177/1745691610383511. PMC 3094752. PMID 21593989.
  56. ^ a b c Rutter M (December 2007). "Proceeding From Observed Correlation to Causal Inference: The Use of Natural Experiments". Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2 (4): 377–95. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.649.2804. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00050.x. PMID 26151974. S2CID 205908149.
  57. ^ Kendler KS, Baker JH (May 2007). "Genetic influences on measures of the environment: a systematic review". Psychological Medicine. 37 (5): 615–26. doi:10.1017/S0033291706009524. PMID 17176502. S2CID 43598144.
  58. ^ Keyes M, Legrand LN, Iacono WG, McGue M (October 2008). "Parental smoking and adolescent problem behavior: an adoption study of general and specific effects". The American Journal of Psychiatry. 165 (10): 1338–44. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08010125. PMC 2597022. PMID 18676589.
  59. ^ a b Kendler KS, Sundquist K, Ohlsson H, Palmér K, Maes H, Winkleby MA, et al. (July 2012). "Genetic and familial environmental influences on the risk for drug abuse: a national Swedish adoption study". Archives of General Psychiatry. 69 (7): 690–7. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.2112. PMC 3556483. PMID 22393206.
  60. ^ D'Onofrio BM, Turkheimer EN, Eaves LJ, Corey LA, Berg K, Solaas MH, et al. (November 2003). "The role of the children of twins design in elucidating causal relations between parent characteristics and child outcomes". Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines. 44 (8): 1130–44. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00196. PMID 14626455.
  61. ^ Smith GD, Ebrahim S (February 2004). "Mendelian randomization: prospects, potentials, and limitations". International Journal of Epidemiology. 33 (1): 30–42. doi:10.1093/ije/dyh132. PMID 15075143.
  62. ^ a b c d Plomin R, DeFries JC, Knopik VS, Neiderhiser JM (January 2016). "Top 10 Replicated Findings From Behavioral Genetics". Perspectives on Psychological Science. 11 (1) (published 27 January 2016): 3–23. doi:10.1177/1745691615617439. PMC 4739500. PMID 26817721.
  63. ^ a b c Bouchard TJ, Lykken DT, McGue M, Segal NL, Tellegen A (October 1990). "Sources of human psychological differences: the Minnesota Study of Twins Reared Apart". Science. 250 (4978): 223–8. Bibcode:1990Sci...250..223B. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.225.1769. doi:10.1126/science.2218526. PMID 2218526. S2CID 11794689.
  64. ^ Plomin R, Haworth CM, Meaburn EL, Price TS, Davis OS (April 2013). "Common DNA markers can account for more than half of the genetic influence on cognitive abilities". Psychological Science. 24 (4): 562–8. doi:10.1177/0956797612457952. PMC 3652710. PMID 23501967.
  65. ^ Plomin R, Daniels D (June 2011). "Why are children in the same family so different from one another?". International Journal of Epidemiology. 40 (3): 563–82. doi:10.1093/ije/dyq148. PMC 3147063. PMID 21807642.
  66. ^ Matteson LK, McGue M, Iacono WG (November 2013). "Shared environmental influences on personality: a combined twin and adoption approach". Behavior Genetics. 43 (6): 491–504. doi:10.1007/s10519-013-9616-8. PMC 3868213. PMID 24065564.
  67. ^ Burt SA (July 2009). "Rethinking environmental contributions to child and adolescent psychopathology: a meta-analysis of shared environmental influences". Psychological Bulletin. 135 (4): 608–37. doi:10.1037/a0015702. PMID 19586164.
  68. ^ a b Buchanan JP, McGue M, Keyes M, Iacono WG (September 2009). "Are there shared environmental influences on adolescent behavior? Evidence from a study of adoptive siblings". Behavior Genetics. 39 (5): 532–40. doi:10.1007/s10519-009-9283-y. PMC 2858574. PMID 19626434.
  69. ^ a b Bland JM, Altman DG (May 2000). "Statistics notes. The odds ratio". BMJ. 320 (7247): 1468. doi:10.1136/bmj.320.7247.1468. PMC 1127651. PMID 10827061.
  70. ^ a b Thorgeirsson TE, Gudbjartsson DF, Surakka I, Vink JM, Amin N, Geller F, et al. (May 2010). "Sequence variants at CHRNB3-CHRNA6 and CYP2A6 affect smoking behavior". Nature Genetics. 42 (5): 448–53. doi:10.1038/ng.573. PMC 3080600. PMID 20418888.
  71. ^ Schumann G, Coin LJ, Lourdusamy A, Charoen P, Berger KH, Stacey D, et al. (April 2011). "Genome-wide association and genetic functional studies identify autism susceptibility candidate 2 gene (AUTS2) in the regulation of alcohol consumption". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 108 (17): 7119–24. Bibcode:2011PNAS..108.7119S. doi:10.1073/pnas.1017288108. PMC 3084048. PMID 21471458.
  72. ^ a b Okbay A, Baselmans BM, De Neve JE, Turley P, Nivard MG, Fontana MA, et al. (June 2016). "Genetic variants associated with subjective well-being, depressive symptoms, and neuroticism identified through genome-wide analyses". Nature Genetics. 48 (6): 624–33. doi:10.1038/ng.3552. PMC 4884152. PMID 27089181.
  73. ^ Day FR, Helgason H, Chasman DI, Rose LM, Loh PR, Scott RA, et al. (June 2016). "Physical and neurobehavioral determinants of reproductive onset and success". Nature Genetics. 48 (6): 617–23. doi:10.1038/ng.3551. PMC 5238953. PMID 27089180.
  74. ^ CONVERGE consortium (July 2015). "Sparse whole-genome sequencing identifies two loci for major depressive disorder". Nature. 523 (7562): 588–91. Bibcode:2015Natur.523..588C. doi:10.1038/nature14659. PMC 4522619. PMID 26176920.
  75. ^ Hibar DP, Stein JL, Renteria ME, Arias-Vasquez A, Desrivières S, Jahanshad N, et al. (April 2015). "Common genetic variants influence human subcortical brain structures". Nature. 520 (7546): 224–9. Bibcode:2015Natur.520..224.. doi:10.1038/nature14101. PMC 4393366. PMID 25607358.
  76. ^ Iacono WG, Vaidyanathan U, Vrieze SI, Malone SM (December 2014). "Knowns and unknowns for psychophysiological endophenotypes: integration and response to commentaries". Psychophysiology. 51 (12): 1339–47. doi:10.1111/psyp.12358. PMC 4231488. PMID 25387720.
  77. ^ Corder EH, Saunders AM, Risch NJ, Strittmatter WJ, Schmechel DE, Gaskell PC, et al. (June 1994). "Protective effect of apolipoprotein E type 2 allele for late onset Alzheimer disease". Nature Genetics. 7 (2): 180–4. doi:10.1038/ng0694-180. PMID 7920638. S2CID 11137478.
  78. ^ Luczak SE, Glatt SJ, Wall TL (July 2006). "Meta-analyses of ALDH2 and ADH1B with alcohol dependence in Asians". Psychological Bulletin. 132 (4): 607–21. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.132.4.607. PMID 16822169.
  79. ^ Guerreiro RJ, Gustafson DR, Hardy J (March 2012). "The genetic architecture of Alzheimer's disease: beyond APP, PSENs and APOE". Neurobiology of Aging. 33 (3): 437–56. doi:10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2010.03.025. PMC 2980860. PMID 20594621.
  80. ^ Gusella JF, Wexler NS, Conneally PM, Naylor SL, Anderson MA, Tanzi RE, et al. (1983). "A polymorphic DNA marker genetically linked to Huntington's disease". Nature. 306 (5940): 234–8. Bibcode:1983Natur.306..234G. doi:10.1038/306234a0. PMID 6316146. S2CID 4320711.
  81. ^ a b Hayden EC (October 2013). "Ethics: Taboo genetics". Nature. 502 (7469): 26–8. Bibcode:2013Natur.502...26C. doi:10.1038/502026a. PMID 24091964.
  82. ^ a b c d Charney E (January 2017). "Genes, behavior, and behavior genetics". Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science. 8 (1–2): e1405. doi:10.1002/wcs.1405. hdl:10161/13337. PMID 27906529.
  83. ^ Yudell M, Roberts D, DeSalle R, Tishkoff S (February 2016). "Science and Society: Taking race out of human genetics". Science. 351 (6273): 564–5. Bibcode:2016Sci...351..564Y. doi:10.1126/science.aac4951. PMID 26912690. S2CID 206639306.
  84. ^ a b Bryc K, Durand EY, Macpherson JM, Reich D, Mountain JL (January 2015). "The genetic ancestry of African Americans, Latinos, and European Americans across the United States". American Journal of Human Genetics. 96 (1): 37–53. doi:10.1016/j.ajhg.2014.11.010. PMC 4289685. PMID 25529636.
  85. ^ Abecasis GR, Auton A, Brooks LD, DePristo MA, Durbin RM, Handsaker RE, et al. (November 2012). "An integrated map of genetic variation from 1,092 human genomes". Nature. 491 (7422): 56–65. Bibcode:2012Natur.491...56T. doi:10.1038/nature11632. PMC 3498066. PMID 23128226.
  86. ^ Panofsky A (7 July 2014). Misbehaving Science: Controversy and the Development of Behavior Genetics. University of Chicago Press. ISBN 978-0-226-05859-7. from the original on 22 April 2021. Retrieved 23 May 2018.
  87. ^ Lerner RM (27 August 2015). "Eliminating Genetic Reductionism from Developmental Science". Research in Human Development. 12 (3–4): 178–188. doi:10.1080/15427609.2015.1068058. ISSN 1542-7609. S2CID 143195504.
  88. ^ van Beijsterveldt CE, Overbeek LI, Rozendaal L, McMaster MT, Glasner TJ, Bartels M, et al. (May 2016). "Chorionicity and Heritability Estimates from Twin Studies: The Prenatal Environment of Twins and Their Resemblance Across a Large Number of Traits". Behavior Genetics. 46 (3): 304–14. doi:10.1007/s10519-015-9745-3. PMC 4858554. PMID 26410687.

Further reading edit

  • Crusio WE (2015). "Key issues in contemporary behavioral genetics". Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences. 2: 89–95. doi:10.1016/j.cobeha.2014.10.002. S2CID 53200512.
  • Crusio WE, Gerlai RT, eds. (1999). Handbook of Molecular-Genetic Techniques for Brain and Behavior Research. Techniques in the Behavioral and Neural Sciences. Vol. 13. Elsevier. ISBN 978-0-444-50239-1.
  • Crusio WE, Sluyter F, Gerlai RT, Pietropaolo S, eds. (2013). Behavioral Genetics of the Mouse: Genetics of Behavioral Phenotypes. Cambridge Handbooks in Behavioral Genetics. Vol. 1. Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-1-107-03481-5.
  • Flint J, Greenspan RJ, Kendler KS (28 January 2010). How Genes Influence Behavior. Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-955990-9.
  • Johnson W, Turkheimer E, Gottesman II, Bouchard TJ (August 2010). "Beyond Heritability: Twin Studies in Behavioral Research". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 18 (4): 217–220. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01639.x. PMC 2899491. PMID 20625474.
  • Johnson W, Penke L, Spinath FM (2011). "Understanding Heritability: What it is and What it is Not" (PDF). European Journal of Personality. 25 (4): 287–294. doi:10.1002/per.835. ISSN 0890-2070. S2CID 41842465. (PDF) from the original on 16 December 2013. Retrieved 15 December 2013.
  • Maxson SC (10 October 2012). "Chapter 1: Behavioral Genetics". In Weiner IB, Nelson RJ, Mizumori S (eds.). Handbook of Psychology (Volume 3: Behavioral Neuroscience). John Wiley & Sons. ISBN 978-0-470-89059-2. from the original on 16 December 2013. Retrieved 15 December 2013.
  • Spinath FM, Johnson W (2011). "Chapter 10: Behavior Genetics". In Chamorro-Premuzic T, von Stumm S, Furnham A (eds.). The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Individual Differences. United Kingdom: Blackwell Publishing Ltd. doi:10.1002/9781444343120. ISBN 978-1-4443-3438-8.

External links edit

  • McGue M (5 May 2014). "Introduction to Human Behavioral Genetics". Coursera. Retrieved 10 June 2014.
  • McGue M (December 2020). "Introduction to Behavioral Genetics". University of Minnesota. Retrieved 28 June 2021.
  • "Behavior Genetics Association". Behavior Genetics Association. Retrieved 8 May 2022.
  • Institute for Behavioral Genetics at the University of Colorado Boulder, University of Colorado Boulder
  • "Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics". Virginia Commonwealth University.

behavioural, genetics, genetic, psychology, redirects, here, franz, brentano, concept, genetic, psychology, genetic, psychology, brentano, journal, behavior, genetics, journal, also, referred, behaviour, genetics, field, scientific, research, that, uses, genet. Genetic psychology redirects here For Franz Brentano s concept of genetic psychology see Genetic psychology Brentano For the journal see Behavior Genetics journal Behavioural genetics also referred to as behaviour genetics is a field of scientific research that uses genetic methods to investigate the nature and origins of individual differences in behaviour While the name behavioural genetics connotes a focus on genetic influences the field broadly investigates the extent to which genetic and environmental factors influence individual differences and the development of research designs that can remove the confounding of genes and environment Behavioural genetics was founded as a scientific discipline by Francis Galton in the late 19th century only to be discredited through association with eugenics movements before and during World War II In the latter half of the 20th century the field saw renewed prominence with research on inheritance of behaviour and mental illness in humans typically using twin and family studies as well as research on genetically informative model organisms through selective breeding and crosses In the late 20th and early 21st centuries technological advances in molecular genetics made it possible to measure and modify the genome directly This led to major advances in model organism research e g knockout mice and in human studies e g genome wide association studies leading to new scientific discoveries Findings from behavioural genetic research have broadly impacted modern understanding of the role of genetic and environmental influences on behaviour These include evidence that nearly all researched behaviours are under a significant degree of genetic influence and that influence tends to increase as individuals develop into adulthood Further most researched human behaviours are influenced by a very large number of genes and the individual effects of these genes are very small Environmental influences also play a strong role but they tend to make family members more different from one another not more similar Contents 1 History 2 Methods 2 1 Animal studies 2 2 Human studies 2 2 1 Twin and family studies 2 2 2 Genome wide association studies 2 3 Measured genetic variants 2 4 Quasi experimental designs 3 General findings 3 1 Genetic influences on behaviour are pervasive 3 2 Nature of environmental influence 3 3 Nature of genetic influence 3 4 Additional general findings 4 Criticisms and controversies 5 See also 6 References 7 Further reading 8 External linksHistory edit nbsp Farmers with wheat and cattle Ancient Egyptian art 1 422 BCE displaying domesticated animals Selective breeding and the domestication of animals is perhaps the earliest evidence that humans considered the idea that individual differences in behaviour could be due to natural causes 1 Plato and Aristotle each speculated on the basis and mechanisms of inheritance of behavioural characteristics 2 Plato for example argued in The Republic that selective breeding among the citizenry to encourage the development of some traits and discourage others what today might be called eugenics was to be encouraged in the pursuit of an ideal society 2 3 Behavioural genetic concepts also existed during the English renaissance where William Shakespeare perhaps first coined the phrase nature versus nurture in The Tempest where he wrote in Act IV Scene I that Caliban was A devil a born devil on whose nature Nurture can never stick 3 4 Modern day behavioural genetics began with Sir Francis Galton a nineteenth century intellectual and cousin of Charles Darwin 3 Galton was a polymath who studied many subjects including the heritability of human abilities and mental characteristics One of Galton s investigations involved a large pedigree study of social and intellectual achievement in the English upper class In 1869 10 years after Darwin s On the Origin of Species Galton published his results in Hereditary Genius 5 In this work Galton found that the rate of eminence was highest among close relatives of eminent individuals and decreased as the degree of relationship to eminent individuals decreased While Galton could not rule out the role of environmental influences on eminence a fact which he acknowledged the study served to initiate an important debate about the relative roles of genes and environment on behavioural characteristics Through his work Galton also introduced multivariate analysis and paved the way towards modern Bayesian statistics that are used throughout the sciences launching what has been dubbed the Statistical Enlightenment 6 nbsp Galton in his later years The field of behavioural genetics as founded by Galton was ultimately undermined by another of Galton s intellectual contributions the founding of the eugenics movement in 20th century society 3 The primary idea behind eugenics was to use selective breeding combined with knowledge about the inheritance of behaviour to improve the human species 3 The eugenics movement was subsequently discredited by scientific corruption and genocidal actions in Nazi Germany Behavioural genetics was thereby discredited through its association to eugenics 3 The field once again gained status as a distinct scientific discipline through the publication of early texts on behavioural genetics such as Calvin S Hall s 1951 book chapter on behavioural genetics in which he introduced the term psychogenetics 7 which enjoyed some limited popularity in the 1960s and 1970s 8 9 However it eventually disappeared from usage in favour of behaviour genetics The start of behaviour genetics as a well identified field was marked by the publication in 1960 of the book Behavior Genetics by John L Fuller and William Robert Bob Thompson 1 10 It is widely accepted now that many if not most behaviours in animals and humans are under significant genetic influence although the extent of genetic influence for any particular trait can differ widely 11 12 A decade later in February 1970 the first issue of the journal Behavior Genetics was published and in 1972 the Behavior Genetics Association was formed with Theodosius Dobzhansky elected as the association s first president The field has since grown and diversified touching many scientific disciplines 3 13 Methods editThe primary goal of behavioural genetics is to investigate the nature and origins of individual differences in behaviour 3 A wide variety of different methodological approaches are used in behavioural genetic research 14 only a few of which are outlined below Animal studies edit This section needs expansion You can help by adding to it February 2020 Investigators in animal behaviour genetics can carefully control for environmental factors and can experimentally manipulate genetic variants allowing for a degree of causal inference that is not available in studies on human behavioural genetics 15 In animal research selection experiments have often been employed For example laboratory house mice have been bred for open field behaviour 16 thermoregulatory nesting 17 and voluntary wheel running behaviour 18 A range of methods in these designs are covered on those pages Behavioural geneticists using model organisms employ a range of molecular techniques to alter insert or delete genes These techniques include knockouts floxing gene knockdown or genome editing using methods like CRISPR Cas9 19 These techniques allow behavioural geneticists different levels of control in the model organism s genome to evaluate the molecular physiological or behavioural outcome of genetic changes 20 Animals commonly used as model organisms in behavioural genetics include mice 21 zebra fish 22 and the nematode species C elegans 23 Machine learning and A I developments are allowing researchers to design experiments that are able to manage the complexity and large data sets generated allowing for increasingly complex behavioural experiments 24 Human studies edit Main article Human behaviour genetics See also Heritability and Quantitative genetics Pedigree analysis Some research designs used in behavioural genetic research are variations on family designs also known as pedigree designs including twin studies and adoption studies 14 Quantitative genetic modelling of individuals with known genetic relationships e g parent child sibling dizygotic and monozygotic twins allows one to estimate to what extent genes and environment contribute to phenotypic differences among individuals 25 Twin and family studies edit Main article Twin studies nbsp Pedigree chart showing an inheritance pattern consistent with autosomal dominant transmission Behavioural geneticists have used pedigree studies to investigate the genetic and environmental basis of behaviour The basic intuition of the twin study is that monozygotic twins share 100 of their genome and dizygotic twins share on average 50 of their segregating genome Thus differences between the two members of a monozygotic twin pair can only be due to differences in their environment whereas dizygotic twins will differ from one another due to genes in addition to the environment Under this simplistic model if dizygotic twins differ more than monozygotic twins it can only be attributable to genetic influences An important assumption of the twin model is the equal environment assumption 26 that monozygotic twins have the same shared environmental experiences as dizygotic twins If for example monozygotic twins tend to have more similar experiences than dizygotic twins and these experiences themselves are not genetically mediated through gene environment correlation mechanisms then monozygotic twins will tend to be more similar to one another than dizygotic twins for reasons that have nothing to do with genes 27 While this assumption should be kept in mind when interpreting the results of twin studies research tends to suppot the equal environment assumption 28 Twin studies of monozygotic and dizygotic twins use a biometrical formulation to describe the influences on twin similarity and to infer heritability 25 29 The formulation rests on the basic observation that the variance in a phenotype is due to two sources genes and environment More formally V a r P g g ϵ ϵ displaystyle Var P g g times epsilon epsilon nbsp where P displaystyle P nbsp is the phenotype g displaystyle g nbsp is the effect of genes ϵ displaystyle epsilon nbsp is the effect of the environment and g ϵ displaystyle g times epsilon nbsp is a gene by environment interaction The g displaystyle g nbsp term can be expanded to include additive a 2 displaystyle a 2 nbsp dominance d 2 displaystyle d 2 nbsp and epistatic i 2 displaystyle i 2 nbsp genetic effects Similarly the environmental term ϵ displaystyle epsilon nbsp can be expanded to include shared environment c 2 displaystyle c 2 nbsp and non shared environment e 2 displaystyle e 2 nbsp which includes any measurement error Dropping the gene by environment interaction for simplicity typical in twin studies and fully decomposing the g displaystyle g nbsp and ϵ displaystyle epsilon nbsp terms we now have V a r P a 2 d 2 i 2 c 2 e 2 displaystyle Var P a 2 d 2 i 2 c 2 e 2 nbsp Twin research then models the similarity in monozygotic twins and dizygotic twins using simplified forms of this decomposition shown in the table 25 Decomposing the genetic and environmental contributions to twin similarity 25 Type of relationship Full decomposition Falconer s decomposition Perfect similarity between siblings 1 0 a 2 d 2 i 2 c 2 e 2 displaystyle 1 0 a 2 d 2 i 2 c 2 e 2 nbsp 1 0 a 2 c 2 e 2 displaystyle 1 0 a 2 c 2 e 2 nbsp Monozygotic twin correlation r M Z displaystyle r MZ nbsp r M Z a 2 d 2 i 2 c 2 displaystyle r MZ a 2 d 2 i 2 c 2 nbsp r M Z a 2 c 2 displaystyle r MZ a 2 c 2 nbsp Dizygotic twin correlation r D Z displaystyle r DZ nbsp r D Z 1 2 a 2 1 4 d 2 k i 2 c 2 displaystyle r DZ frac 1 2 a 2 frac 1 4 d 2 k i 2 c 2 nbsp r D Z 1 2 a 2 c 2 displaystyle r DZ frac 1 2 a 2 c 2 nbsp Where k displaystyle k nbsp is an unknown probably very small quantity The simplified Falconer formulation can then be used to derive estimates of a 2 displaystyle a 2 nbsp c 2 displaystyle c 2 nbsp and e 2 displaystyle e 2 nbsp Rearranging and substituting the r M Z displaystyle r MZ nbsp and r D Z displaystyle r DZ nbsp equations one can obtain an estimate of the additive genetic variance or heritability a 2 2 r M Z r D Z displaystyle a 2 2 r MZ r DZ nbsp the non shared environmental effect e 2 1 r M Z displaystyle e 2 1 r MZ nbsp and finally the shared environmental effect c 2 2 r D Z r M Z displaystyle c 2 2r DZ r MZ nbsp 25 The Falconer formulation is presented here to illustrate how the twin model works Modern approaches use maximum likelihood to estimate the genetic and environmental variance components 30 Genome wide association studies edit nbsp Study results about which and to what degree various traits IQ and language related skills appear to be influenced by genetics 31 32 Behavioural genetics research also involves genome wide association studies GWAS GWAS are observational studies which review hundreds of thousands of genetic variants from large samples of participants with the goal of finding alleles genetic variants that are associated with traits of interest The goal may be to find genetic differences between a disease group and a control group or to understand genetic contributions to traits that vary across a spectrum 33 GWAS results showing genetic contributions to lifestyle behaviors and psychological conditions are particularly relevant to behavioural genetics The variants reviewed in GWAS can be as simple as a single nucleotide polymorphism SNP or can involve deletions or duplications of longer sequences of DNA SNPs are the most common focus GWAS do not involve sequencing every base pair of a person s DNA Instead they often rely on genotype imputation based on known groups of genes which tend to be inherited together 34 Genetic variants identified through GWAS can be used to improve disease risk predictions and can be used as controls when studying the effectiveness of disease treatments 33 However the genetic variants identified through GWAS are most likely to have a moderate effect on disease risk or development of a given trait This is different from the strong genetic contribution seen in single gene diseases Additionally associations between alleles and traits can differ between groups of different ancestry This means that GWAS results from one population cannot accurately be applied to another population and comparing genetic variation between populations can be difficult and complicated 35 36 Measured genetic variants edit The Human Genome Project has allowed scientists to directly genotype the sequence of human DNA nucleotides 37 Once genotyped genetic variants can be tested for association with a behavioural phenotype such as mental disorder cognitive ability personality and so on 38 Candidate Genes One popular approach has been to test for association candidate genes with behavioural phenotypes where the candidate gene is selected based on some a priori theory about biological mechanisms involved in the manifestation of a behavioural trait or phenotype 39 In general such studies have proven difficult to broadly replicate 40 41 42 43 and there has been concern raised that the false positive rate in this type of research is high 39 44 Genome wide association studies As discussed in the previous section in genome wide association studies researchers test the relationship of millions of genetic polymorphisms with behavioural phenotypes across the genome 38 This approach to genetic association studies is largely atheoretical and typically not guided by a particular biological hypothesis regarding the phenotype 38 Genetic association findings for behavioural traits and psychiatric disorders have been found to be highly polygenic involving many small genetic effects 45 46 47 48 49 SNP heritability and co heritability Recently researchers have begun to use similarity between classically unrelated people at their measured single nucleotide polymorphisms SNPs to estimate genetic variation or covariation that is tagged by SNPs using mixed effects models implemented in software such as genome wide complex trait analysis GCTA 50 51 To do this researchers find the average genetic relatedness over all SNPs between all individuals in a typically large sample and use Haseman Elston regression or restricted maximum likelihood to estimate the genetic variation that is tagged by or predicted by the SNPs The proportion of phenotypic variation that is accounted for by the genetic relatedness has been called SNP heritability 52 Intuitively SNP heritability increases to the degree that phenotypic similarity is predicted by genetic similarity at measured SNPs and is expected to be lower than the true narrow sense heritability to the degree that measured SNPs fail to tag typically rare causal variants 53 The value of this method is that it is an independent way to estimate heritability that does not require the same assumptions as those in twin and family studies and that it gives insight into the allelic frequency spectrum of the causal variants underlying trait variation 54 Quasi experimental designs edit Some behavioural genetic designs are useful not to understand genetic influences on behaviour but to control for genetic influences to test environmentally mediated influences on behaviour 55 Such behavioural genetic designs may be considered a subset of natural experiments 56 quasi experiments that attempt to take advantage of naturally occurring situations that mimic true experiments by providing some control over an independent variable Natural experiments can be particularly useful when experiments are infeasible due to practical or ethical limitations 56 A general limitation of observational studies is that the relative influences of genes and environment are confounded A simple demonstration of this fact is that measures of environmental influence are heritable 57 Thus observing a correlation between an environmental risk factor and a health outcome is not necessarily evidence for environmental influence on the health outcome Similarly in observational studies of parent child behavioural transmission for example it is impossible to know if the transmission is due to genetic or environmental influences due to the problem of passive gene environment correlation 56 The simple observation that the children of parents who use drugs are more likely to use drugs as adults does not indicate why the children are more likely to use drugs when they grow up It could be because the children are modelling their parents behaviour Equally plausible it could be that the children inherited drug use predisposing genes from their parent which put them at increased risk for drug use as adults regardless of their parents behaviour Adoption studies which parse the relative effects of rearing environment and genetic inheritance find a small to negligible effect of rearing environment on smoking alcohol and marijuana use in adopted children 58 non primary source needed but a larger effect of rearing environment on harder drug use 59 non primary source needed Other behavioural genetic designs include discordant twin studies 55 children of twins designs 60 and Mendelian randomization 61 General findings editThere are many broad conclusions to be drawn from behavioural genetic research about the nature and origins of behaviour 3 62 Three major conclusions include 3 all behavioural traits and disorders are influenced by genes environmental influences tend to make members of the same family more different rather than more similar the influence of genes tends to increase in relative importance as individuals age Genetic influences on behaviour are pervasive edit It is clear from multiple lines of evidence that all researched behavioural traits and disorders are influenced by genes that is they are heritable The single largest source of evidence comes from twin studies where it is routinely observed that monozygotic identical twins are more similar to one another than are same sex dizygotic fraternal twins 11 12 The conclusion that genetic influences are pervasive has also been observed in research designs that do not depend on the assumptions of the twin method Adoption studies show that adoptees are routinely more similar to their biological relatives than their adoptive relatives for a wide variety of traits and disorders 3 In the Minnesota Study of Twins Reared Apart monozygotic twins separated shortly after birth were reunited in adulthood 63 These adopted reared apart twins were as similar to one another as were twins reared together on a wide range of measures including general cognitive ability personality religious attitudes and vocational interests among others 63 Approaches using genome wide genotyping have allowed researchers to measure genetic relatedness between individuals and estimate heritability based on millions of genetic variants Methods exist to test whether the extent of genetic similarity aka relatedness between nominally unrelated individuals individuals who are not close or even distant relatives is associated with phenotypic similarity 51 Such methods do not rely on the same assumptions as twin or adoption studies and routinely find evidence for heritability of behavioural traits and disorders 47 49 64 Nature of environmental influence edit Just as all researched human behavioural phenotypes are influenced by genes i e are heritable all such phenotypes are also influenced by the environment 11 62 The basic fact that monozygotic twins are genetically identical but are never perfectly concordant for psychiatric disorder or perfectly correlated for behavioural traits indicates that the environment shapes human behaviour 62 The nature of this environmental influence however is such that it tends to make individuals in the same family more different from one another not more similar to one another 3 That is estimates of shared environmental effects c 2 displaystyle c 2 nbsp in human studies are small negligible or zero for the vast majority of behavioural traits and psychiatric disorders whereas estimates of non shared environmental effects e 2 displaystyle e 2 nbsp are moderate to large 11 From twin studies c 2 displaystyle c 2 nbsp is typically estimated at 0 because the correlation r M Z displaystyle r MZ nbsp between monozygotic twins is at least twice the correlation r D Z displaystyle r DZ nbsp for dizygotic twins When using the Falconer variance decomposition 1 0 a 2 c 2 e 2 displaystyle 1 0 a 2 c 2 e 2 nbsp this difference between monozygotic and dizygotic twin similarity results in an estimated c 2 0 displaystyle c 2 0 nbsp The Falconer decomposition is simplistic 25 It removes the possible influence of dominance and epistatic effects which if present will tend to make monozygotic twins more similar than dizygotic twins and mask the influence of shared environmental effects 25 This is a limitation of the twin design for estimating c 2 displaystyle c 2 nbsp However the general conclusion that shared environmental effects are negligible does not rest on twin studies alone Adoption research also fails to find large c 2 displaystyle c 2 nbsp components that is adoptive parents and their adopted children tend to show much less resemblance to one another than the adopted child and his or her non rearing biological parent 3 In studies of adoptive families with at least one biological child and one adopted child the sibling resemblance also tends to be nearly zero for most traits that have been studied 11 65 nbsp Similarity in twins and adoptees indicates a small role for shared environment in personality The figure provides an example from personality research where twin and adoption studies converge on the conclusion of zero to small influences of shared environment on broad personality traits measured by the Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire including positive emotionality negative emotionality and constraint 66 Given the conclusion that all researched behavioural traits and psychiatric disorders are heritable biological siblings will always tend to be more similar to one another than will adopted siblings However for some traits especially when measured during adolescence adopted siblings do show some significant similarity e g correlations of 20 to one another Traits that have been demonstrated to have significant shared environmental influences include internalizing and externalizing psychopathology 67 substance use 68 non primary source needed and dependence 59 non primary source needed and intelligence 68 non primary source needed Nature of genetic influence edit Genetic effects on human behavioural outcomes can be described in multiple ways 25 One way to describe the effect is in terms of how much variance in the behaviour can be accounted for by alleles in the genetic variant otherwise known as the coefficient of determination or R 2 displaystyle R 2 nbsp An intuitive way to think about R 2 displaystyle R 2 nbsp is that it describes the extent to which the genetic variant makes individuals who harbour different alleles different from one another on the behavioural outcome A complementary way to describe effects of individual genetic variants is in how much change one expects on the behavioural outcome given a change in the number of risk alleles an individual harbours often denoted by the Greek letter b displaystyle beta nbsp denoting the slope in a regression equation or in the case of binary disease outcomes by the odds ratio O R displaystyle OR nbsp of disease given allele status Note the difference R 2 displaystyle R 2 nbsp describes the population level effect of alleles within a genetic variant b displaystyle beta nbsp or O R displaystyle OR nbsp describe the effect of having a risk allele on the individual who harbours it relative to an individual who does not harbour a risk allele 69 When described on the R 2 displaystyle R 2 nbsp metric the effects of individual genetic variants on complex human behavioural traits and disorders are vanishingly small with each variant accounting for R 2 lt 0 3 displaystyle R 2 lt 0 3 nbsp of variation in the phenotype 3 This fact has been discovered primarily through genome wide association studies of complex behavioural phenotypes including results on substance use 70 71 personality 72 fertility 73 schizophrenia 46 depression 72 74 and endophenotypes including brain structure 75 and function 76 There are a small handful of replicated and robustly studied exceptions to this rule including the effect of APOE on Alzheimer s disease 77 and CHRNA5 on smoking behaviour 70 and ALDH2 in individuals of East Asian ancestry on alcohol use 78 On the other hand when assessing effects according to the b displaystyle beta nbsp metric there are a large number of genetic variants that have very large effects on complex behavioural phenotypes The risk alleles within such variants are exceedingly rare such that their large behavioural effects impact only a small number of individuals Thus when assessed at a population level using the R 2 displaystyle R 2 nbsp metric they account for only a small amount of the differences in risk between individuals in the population Examples include variants within APP that result in familial forms of severe early onset Alzheimer s disease but affect only relatively few individuals Compare this to risk alleles within APOE which pose much smaller risk compared to APP but are far more common and therefore affect a much greater proportion of the population 79 Finally there are classical behavioural disorders that are genetically simple in their etiology such as Huntington s disease Huntington s is caused by a single autosomal dominant variant in the HTT gene which is the only variant that accounts for any differences among individuals in their risk for developing the disease assuming they live long enough 80 In the case of genetically simple and rare diseases such as Huntington s the variant R 2 displaystyle R 2 nbsp and the O R displaystyle OR nbsp are simultaneously large 69 Additional general findings edit In response to general concerns about the replicability of psychological research behavioural geneticists Robert Plomin John C DeFries Valerie Knopik and Jenae Neiderhiser published a review of the ten most well replicated findings from behavioural genetics research 62 The ten findings were All psychological traits show significant and substantial genetic influence No behavioural traits are 100 heritable Heritability is caused by many genes of small effect Phenotypic correlations between psychological traits show significant and substantial genetic mediation The heritability of intelligence increases throughout development Age to age stability is mainly due to genetics Most measures of the environment show significant genetic influence Most associations between environmental measures and psychological traits are significantly mediated genetically Most environmental effects are not shared by children growing up in the same family Abnormal is normal Criticisms and controversies editSee also Missing heritability problem Behavioural genetic research and findings have at times been controversial Some of this controversy has arisen because behavioural genetic findings can challenge societal beliefs about the nature of human behaviour and abilities Major areas of controversy have included genetic research on topics such as racial differences intelligence violence and human sexuality 81 Other controversies have arisen due to misunderstandings of behavioural genetic research whether by the lay public or the researchers themselves 3 For example the notion of heritability is easily misunderstood to imply causality or that some behaviour or condition is determined by one s genetic endowment 82 When behavioural genetics researchers say that a behaviour is X heritable that does not mean that genetics causes determines or fixes up to X of the behaviour Instead heritability is a statement about genetic differences correlated with trait differences on the population level citation needed Historically perhaps the most controversial subject has been on race and genetics 81 Race is not a scientifically exact term and its interpretation can depend on one s culture and country of origin 83 Instead geneticists use concepts such as ancestry which is more rigorously defined 84 For example a so called Black race may include all individuals of relatively recent African descent recent because all humans are descended from African ancestors However there is more genetic diversity in Africa than the rest of the world combined 85 so speaking of a Black race is without a precise genetic meaning 84 Qualitative research has fostered arguments that behavioural genetics is an ungovernable field without scientific norms or consensus which fosters controversy The argument continues that this state of affairs has led to controversies including race intelligence instances where variation within a single gene was found to very strongly influence a controversial phenotype e g the gay gene controversy and others This argument further states that because of the persistence of controversy in behaviour genetics and the failure of disputes to be resolved behaviour genetics does not conform to the standards of good science 86 The scientific assumptions on which parts of behavioural genetic research are based have also been criticized as flawed 82 Genome wide association studies are often implemented with simplifying statistical assumptions such as additivity which may be statistically robust but unrealistic for some behaviours Critics further contend that in humans behaviour genetics represents a misguided form of genetic reductionism based on inaccurate interpretations of statistical analyses 87 Studies comparing monozygotic MZ and dizygotic DZ twins assume that environmental influences will be the same in both types of twins but this assumption may also be unrealistic MZ twins may be treated more alike than DZ twins 82 which itself may be an example of evocative gene environment correlation suggesting that one s genes influence their treatment by others It is also not possible in twin studies to eliminate effects of the shared womb environment although studies comparing twins who experience monochorionic and dichorionic environments in utero do exist and indicate limited impact 88 Studies of twins separated in early life include children who were separated not at birth but part way through childhood 82 The effect of early rearing environment can therefore be evaluated to some extent in such a study by comparing twin similarity for those twins separated early and those separated later 63 See also editBehavior Genetics Behavior Genetics Association Behavioural neurogenetics Biocultural evolution Evolutionary psychology Genes Brain and Behavior Genome wide association study International Behavioural and Neural Genetics Society International Society of Psychiatric Genetics Journal of Neurogenetics Nature versus nurture Personality psychology Psychiatric genetics Psychiatric Genetics Quantitative geneticsReferences edit a b Loehlin JC 2009 History of Behavior Genetics In Kim Y ed Handbook of Behavior Genetics 1 ed New York NY Springer pp 3 11 doi 10 1007 978 0 387 76727 7 1 ISBN 978 0 387 76726 0 a b Maxson SC 30 August 2006 A History of Behavior Genetics In Jones BC Mormede P eds Neurobehavioral Genetics Methods and Applications Second Edition CRC Press ISBN 978 1 4200 0356 7 a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o McGue M Gottesman II 2015 Behavior Genetics The Encyclopedia of Clinical Psychology pp 1 11 doi 10 1002 9781118625392 wbecp578 ISBN 9781118625392 Vaughan V Vaughan AT 1999 The Tempest The Arden Shakespeare Third ed The Arden Shakespeare p 60 ISBN 978 1 903436 08 0 Hereditary Genius An Inquiry into Its Laws and Consequences London MacMillan and Co 1869 Archived from the original on 7 December 2019 Retrieved 17 December 2009 Stigler SM July 2010 Darwin Galton and the Statistical Enlightenment Journal of the Royal Statistical Society Series A 173 3 469 482 doi 10 1111 j 1467 985X 2010 00643 x S2CID 53333238 Hall CS 1951 The genetics of behavior In Stevens SS ed Handbook of Experimental Psychology New York John Wiley and Sons pp 304 329 Grigorenko EL Ravich Shcherbo I 1997 Russian psychogenetics In Grigorenko EL ed Psychology of Russia Past Present Future Commack NY Nova Science pp 83 124 Broadhurst PL July 1969 Psychogenetics of emotionality in the rat Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 159 3 806 24 Bibcode 1969NYASA 159 806B doi 10 1111 j 1749 6632 1969 tb12980 x PMID 5260300 S2CID 42323956 Fuller JL Thompson WR 1960 Behavior Genetics New York John Wiley and Sons a b c d e Polderman TJ Benyamin B de Leeuw CA Sullivan PF van Bochoven A Visscher PM et al July 2015 Meta analysis of the heritability of human traits based on fifty years of twin studies PDF Nature Genetics 47 7 702 9 doi 10 1038 ng 3285 PMID 25985137 S2CID 205349969 Archived PDF from the original on 2022 04 20 Retrieved 2019 01 30 a b Turkheimer E 2000 Three Laws of Behavior Genetics and What They Mean PDF Current Directions in Psychological Science 9 5 160 164 doi 10 1111 1467 8721 00084 S2CID 2861437 Archived PDF from the original on 2016 10 09 Retrieved 2016 04 12 Ayorech Z Selzam S Smith Woolley E Knopik VS Neiderhiser JM DeFries JC et al September 2016 Publication Trends Over 55 Years of Behavioral Genetic Research Behavior Genetics 46 5 603 7 doi 10 1007 s10519 016 9786 2 PMC 5206393 PMID 26992731 a b Plomin R DeFries JC Knopik VS Neiderhiser M 24 September 2012 Behavioral Genetics Worth Publishers ISBN 978 1 4292 4215 8 Archived from the original on 31 December 2013 Retrieved 27 January 2016 Plomin R Behaviour genetics Encyclopedia Britannica Archived from the original on 17 April 2021 Retrieved 15 June 2018 DeFries JC Hegmann JP Halcomb RA August 1974 Response to 20 generations of selection for open field activity in mice Behavioral Biology 11 4 481 95 doi 10 1016 s0091 6773 74 90800 1 PMID 4415597 Lynch CB November 1980 Response to divergent selection for nesting behavior in Mus musculus Genetics 96 3 757 65 doi 10 1093 genetics 96 3 757 PMC 1214374 PMID 7196362 Swallow JG Carter PA Garland T May 1998 Artificial selection for increased wheel running behavior in house mice Behavior Genetics 28 3 227 37 doi 10 1023 A 1021479331779 PMID 9670598 S2CID 18336243 Heidenreich M Zhang F January 2016 Applications of CRISPR Cas systems in neuroscience Nature Reviews Neuroscience 17 1 36 44 doi 10 1038 nrn 2015 2 PMC 4899966 PMID 26656253 Singh P Schimenti JC Bolcun Filas E January 2015 A mouse geneticist s practical guide to CRISPR applications Genetics 199 1 1 15 doi 10 1534 genetics 114 169771 PMC 4286675 PMID 25271304 Cryan JF Holmes A September 2005 The ascent of mouse advances in modelling human depression and anxiety Nature Reviews Drug Discovery 4 9 775 790 doi 10 1038 nrd1825 PMID 16138108 S2CID 18207374 Wolman M Granato M March 2012 Behavioral genetics in larval zebrafish learning from the young Developmental Neurobiology 72 3 366 372 doi 10 1002 dneu 20872 PMC 6430578 PMID 22328273 Wolinsky E Way J March 1990 The behavioral genetics of Caenorhabditis elegans Behavior Genetics 20 2 169 189 doi 10 1007 bf01067789 PMID 2191646 S2CID 23719167 Stacher Horndli CN Wong E Ferris E Bennett K Steinwand S Rhodes AN et al August 2019 Complex Economic Behavior Patterns Are Constructed from Finite Genetically Controlled Modules of Behavior Cell Reports 28 7 1814 1829 e6 doi 10 1016 j celrep 2019 07 038 PMC 7476553 PMID 31412249 S2CID 199662477 a b c d e f g h Falconer DS 1989 Introduction to quantitative genetics Longman Scientific amp Technical ISBN 978 0 470 21162 5 Archived from the original on 2021 04 22 Retrieved 2016 12 02 Eaves L Foley D Silberg J 2003 Has the Equal Environments assumption been tested in twin studies Twin Research 6 6 486 9 doi 10 1375 136905203322686473 PMID 14965458 Kendler KS Neale MC Kessler RC Heath AC Eaves LJ January 1993 A test of the equal environment assumption in twin studies of psychiatric illness Behavior Genetics 23 1 21 7 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 595 7413 doi 10 1007 BF01067551 PMID 8476388 S2CID 9034050 Harden KP 2021 09 21 The Genetic Lottery Why DNA Matters for Social Equality Princeton University Press p 276 ISBN 978 0 691 22670 5 Jinks JL Fulker DW 1970 Comparison of the biometrical genetical MAVA and classical approaches to the analysis of the human behavior Psychological Bulletin 73 5 311 349 doi 10 1037 h0029135 PMID 5528333 Martin NG Eaves LJ February 1977 The genetical analysis of covariance structure Heredity 38 1 79 95 doi 10 1038 hdy 1977 9 PMID 268313 Massive genome study informs the biology of reading and language Max Planck Society via medicalxpress com Retrieved 18 September 2022 Eising E Mirza Schreiber N de Zeeuw EL Wang CA Truong DT Allegrini AG et al August 2022 Genome wide analyses of individual differences in quantitatively assessed reading and language related skills in up to 34 000 people Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 119 35 e2202764119 Bibcode 2022PNAS 11902764E doi 10 1073 pnas 2202764119 PMC 9436320 PMID 35998220 a b Uffelmann E Huang QQ Munung NS De Vries J Okada Y Martin AR et al 2021 08 26 Genome wide association studies Nature Reviews Methods Primers 1 1 1 21 doi 10 1038 s43586 021 00056 9 ISSN 2662 8449 Marchini J Howie B July 2010 Genotype imputation for genome wide association studies Nature Reviews Genetics 11 7 499 511 doi 10 1038 nrg2796 PMID 20517342 Tam V Patel N Turcotte M Bosse Y Pare G Meyre D August 2019 Benefits and limitations of genome wide association studies Nature Reviews Genetics 20 8 467 484 doi 10 1038 s41576 019 0127 1 PMID 31068683 Plomin R 2019 07 02 Blueprint How DNA Makes Us Who We Are MIT Press ISBN 978 0 262 53798 8 Lander ES February 2011 Initial impact of the sequencing of the human genome Nature 470 7333 187 97 Bibcode 2011Natur 470 187L doi 10 1038 nature09792 hdl 1721 1 69154 PMID 21307931 S2CID 4344403 a b c McCarthy MI Abecasis GR Cardon LR Goldstein DB Little J Ioannidis JP et al May 2008 Genome wide association studies for complex traits consensus uncertainty and challenges Nature Reviews Genetics 9 5 356 69 doi 10 1038 nrg2344 PMID 18398418 S2CID 15032294 a b Duncan LE Keller MC October 2011 A critical review of the first 10 years of candidate gene by environment interaction research in psychiatry The American Journal of Psychiatry 168 10 1041 9 doi 10 1176 appi ajp 2011 11020191 PMC 3222234 PMID 21890791 Farrell MS Werge T Sklar P Owen MJ Ophoff RA O Donovan MC et al May 2015 Evaluating historical candidate genes for schizophrenia Molecular Psychiatry 20 5 555 562 doi 10 1038 mp 2015 16 PMC 4414705 PMID 25754081 Hewitt JK January 2012 Editorial policy on candidate gene association and candidate gene by environment interaction studies of complex traits Behavior Genetics 42 1 1 2 doi 10 1007 s10519 011 9504 z PMID 21928046 S2CID 11492871 Johnson EC Border R Melroy Greif WE de Leeuw CA Ehringer MA Keller MC November 2017 No Evidence That Schizophrenia Candidate Genes Are More Associated With Schizophrenia Than Noncandidate Genes Biological Psychiatry Risk Genes and the Emergence of Schizophrenia 82 10 702 708 doi 10 1016 j biopsych 2017 06 033 PMC 5643230 PMID 28823710 Border R Johnson EC Evans LM Smolen A Berley N Sullivan PF et al May 2019 No Support for Historical Candidate Gene or Candidate Gene by Interaction Hypotheses for Major Depression Across Multiple Large Samples The American Journal of Psychiatry 176 5 376 387 doi 10 1176 appi ajp 2018 18070881 PMC 6548317 PMID 30845820 Colhoun HM McKeigue PM Davey Smith G March 2003 Problems of reporting genetic associations with complex outcomes Lancet 361 9360 865 872 doi 10 1016 S0140 6736 03 12715 8 PMID 12642066 S2CID 15679561 Visscher PM Brown MA McCarthy MI Yang J January 2012 Five years of GWAS discovery American Journal of Human Genetics 90 1 7 24 doi 10 1016 j ajhg 2011 11 029 PMC 3257326 PMID 22243964 a b Ripke S Neale BM Corvin A Walters JT Farh KH Holmans PA et al Schizophrenia Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium July 2014 Biological insights from 108 schizophrenia associated genetic loci Nature 511 7510 421 7 Bibcode 2014Natur 511 421S doi 10 1038 nature13595 PMC 4112379 PMID 25056061 a b Lee SH DeCandia TR Ripke S Yang J Sullivan PF Goddard ME et al February 2012 Estimating the proportion of variation in susceptibility to schizophrenia captured by common SNPs Nature Genetics 44 3 247 50 doi 10 1038 ng 1108 PMC 3327879 PMID 22344220 Sullivan PF Daly MJ O Donovan M July 2012 Genetic architectures of psychiatric disorders the emerging picture and its implications Nature Reviews Genetics 13 8 537 51 doi 10 1038 nrg3240 PMC 4110909 PMID 22777127 a b de Moor MH van den Berg SM Verweij KJ Krueger RF Luciano M Arias Vasquez A et al July 2015 Meta analysis of Genome wide Association Studies for Neuroticism and the Polygenic Association With Major Depressive Disorder JAMA Psychiatry 72 7 642 50 doi 10 1001 jamapsychiatry 2015 0554 PMC 4667957 PMID 25993607 Yang J Benyamin B McEvoy BP Gordon S Henders AK Nyholt DR et al July 2010 Common SNPs explain a large proportion of the heritability for human height Nature Genetics 42 7 565 9 doi 10 1038 ng 608 PMC 3232052 PMID 20562875 a b Yang J Lee SH Goddard ME Visscher PM January 2011 GCTA a tool for genome wide complex trait analysis American Journal of Human Genetics 88 1 76 82 doi 10 1016 j ajhg 2010 11 011 PMC 3014363 PMID 21167468 Lee SH Yang J Chen GB Ripke S Stahl EA Hultman CM et al 2013 Estimation of SNP heritability from dense genotype data American Journal of Human Genetics 93 6 1151 5 doi 10 1016 j ajhg 2013 10 015 PMC 3852919 PMID 24314550 Visscher PM Yang J Goddard ME 2010 A commentary on common SNPs explain a large proportion of the heritability for human height by Yang et al 2010 Twin Research and Human Genetics 13 6 517 24 doi 10 1375 twin 13 6 517 PMID 21142928 S2CID 15730955 Wray NR Lee SH Mehta D Vinkhuyzen AA Dudbridge F Middeldorp CM 2014 Research review Polygenic methods and their application to psychiatric traits PDF Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines 55 10 1068 87 doi 10 1111 jcpp 12295 PMID 25132410 Archived PDF from the original on 2017 01 16 Retrieved 2019 07 01 a b McGue M Osler M Christensen K September 2010 Causal Inference and Observational Research The Utility of Twins Perspectives on Psychological Science 5 5 546 56 doi 10 1177 1745691610383511 PMC 3094752 PMID 21593989 a b c Rutter M December 2007 Proceeding From Observed Correlation to Causal Inference The Use of Natural Experiments Perspectives on Psychological Science 2 4 377 95 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 649 2804 doi 10 1111 j 1745 6916 2007 00050 x PMID 26151974 S2CID 205908149 Kendler KS Baker JH May 2007 Genetic influences on measures of the environment a systematic review Psychological Medicine 37 5 615 26 doi 10 1017 S0033291706009524 PMID 17176502 S2CID 43598144 Keyes M Legrand LN Iacono WG McGue M October 2008 Parental smoking and adolescent problem behavior an adoption study of general and specific effects The American Journal of Psychiatry 165 10 1338 44 doi 10 1176 appi ajp 2008 08010125 PMC 2597022 PMID 18676589 a b Kendler KS Sundquist K Ohlsson H Palmer K Maes H Winkleby MA et al July 2012 Genetic and familial environmental influences on the risk for drug abuse a national Swedish adoption study Archives of General Psychiatry 69 7 690 7 doi 10 1001 archgenpsychiatry 2011 2112 PMC 3556483 PMID 22393206 D Onofrio BM Turkheimer EN Eaves LJ Corey LA Berg K Solaas MH et al November 2003 The role of the children of twins design in elucidating causal relations between parent characteristics and child outcomes Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines 44 8 1130 44 doi 10 1111 1469 7610 00196 PMID 14626455 Smith GD Ebrahim S February 2004 Mendelian randomization prospects potentials and limitations International Journal of Epidemiology 33 1 30 42 doi 10 1093 ije dyh132 PMID 15075143 a b c d Plomin R DeFries JC Knopik VS Neiderhiser JM January 2016 Top 10 Replicated Findings From Behavioral Genetics Perspectives on Psychological Science 11 1 published 27 January 2016 3 23 doi 10 1177 1745691615617439 PMC 4739500 PMID 26817721 a b c Bouchard TJ Lykken DT McGue M Segal NL Tellegen A October 1990 Sources of human psychological differences the Minnesota Study of Twins Reared Apart Science 250 4978 223 8 Bibcode 1990Sci 250 223B CiteSeerX 10 1 1 225 1769 doi 10 1126 science 2218526 PMID 2218526 S2CID 11794689 Plomin R Haworth CM Meaburn EL Price TS Davis OS April 2013 Common DNA markers can account for more than half of the genetic influence on cognitive abilities Psychological Science 24 4 562 8 doi 10 1177 0956797612457952 PMC 3652710 PMID 23501967 Plomin R Daniels D June 2011 Why are children in the same family so different from one another International Journal of Epidemiology 40 3 563 82 doi 10 1093 ije dyq148 PMC 3147063 PMID 21807642 Matteson LK McGue M Iacono WG November 2013 Shared environmental influences on personality a combined twin and adoption approach Behavior Genetics 43 6 491 504 doi 10 1007 s10519 013 9616 8 PMC 3868213 PMID 24065564 Burt SA July 2009 Rethinking environmental contributions to child and adolescent psychopathology a meta analysis of shared environmental influences Psychological Bulletin 135 4 608 37 doi 10 1037 a0015702 PMID 19586164 a b Buchanan JP McGue M Keyes M Iacono WG September 2009 Are there shared environmental influences on adolescent behavior Evidence from a study of adoptive siblings Behavior Genetics 39 5 532 40 doi 10 1007 s10519 009 9283 y PMC 2858574 PMID 19626434 a b Bland JM Altman DG May 2000 Statistics notes The odds ratio BMJ 320 7247 1468 doi 10 1136 bmj 320 7247 1468 PMC 1127651 PMID 10827061 a b Thorgeirsson TE Gudbjartsson DF Surakka I Vink JM Amin N Geller F et al May 2010 Sequence variants at CHRNB3 CHRNA6 and CYP2A6 affect smoking behavior Nature Genetics 42 5 448 53 doi 10 1038 ng 573 PMC 3080600 PMID 20418888 Schumann G Coin LJ Lourdusamy A Charoen P Berger KH Stacey D et al April 2011 Genome wide association and genetic functional studies identify autism susceptibility candidate 2 gene AUTS2 in the regulation of alcohol consumption Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 108 17 7119 24 Bibcode 2011PNAS 108 7119S doi 10 1073 pnas 1017288108 PMC 3084048 PMID 21471458 a b Okbay A Baselmans BM De Neve JE Turley P Nivard MG Fontana MA et al June 2016 Genetic variants associated with subjective well being depressive symptoms and neuroticism identified through genome wide analyses Nature Genetics 48 6 624 33 doi 10 1038 ng 3552 PMC 4884152 PMID 27089181 Day FR Helgason H Chasman DI Rose LM Loh PR Scott RA et al June 2016 Physical and neurobehavioral determinants of reproductive onset and success Nature Genetics 48 6 617 23 doi 10 1038 ng 3551 PMC 5238953 PMID 27089180 CONVERGE consortium July 2015 Sparse whole genome sequencing identifies two loci for major depressive disorder Nature 523 7562 588 91 Bibcode 2015Natur 523 588C doi 10 1038 nature14659 PMC 4522619 PMID 26176920 Hibar DP Stein JL Renteria ME Arias Vasquez A Desrivieres S Jahanshad N et al April 2015 Common genetic variants influence human subcortical brain structures Nature 520 7546 224 9 Bibcode 2015Natur 520 224 doi 10 1038 nature14101 PMC 4393366 PMID 25607358 Iacono WG Vaidyanathan U Vrieze SI Malone SM December 2014 Knowns and unknowns for psychophysiological endophenotypes integration and response to commentaries Psychophysiology 51 12 1339 47 doi 10 1111 psyp 12358 PMC 4231488 PMID 25387720 Corder EH Saunders AM Risch NJ Strittmatter WJ Schmechel DE Gaskell PC et al June 1994 Protective effect of apolipoprotein E type 2 allele for late onset Alzheimer disease Nature Genetics 7 2 180 4 doi 10 1038 ng0694 180 PMID 7920638 S2CID 11137478 Luczak SE Glatt SJ Wall TL July 2006 Meta analyses of ALDH2 and ADH1B with alcohol dependence in Asians Psychological Bulletin 132 4 607 21 doi 10 1037 0033 2909 132 4 607 PMID 16822169 Guerreiro RJ Gustafson DR Hardy J March 2012 The genetic architecture of Alzheimer s disease beyond APP PSENs and APOE Neurobiology of Aging 33 3 437 56 doi 10 1016 j neurobiolaging 2010 03 025 PMC 2980860 PMID 20594621 Gusella JF Wexler NS Conneally PM Naylor SL Anderson MA Tanzi RE et al 1983 A polymorphic DNA marker genetically linked to Huntington s disease Nature 306 5940 234 8 Bibcode 1983Natur 306 234G doi 10 1038 306234a0 PMID 6316146 S2CID 4320711 a b Hayden EC October 2013 Ethics Taboo genetics Nature 502 7469 26 8 Bibcode 2013Natur 502 26C doi 10 1038 502026a PMID 24091964 a b c d Charney E January 2017 Genes behavior and behavior genetics Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews Cognitive Science 8 1 2 e1405 doi 10 1002 wcs 1405 hdl 10161 13337 PMID 27906529 Yudell M Roberts D DeSalle R Tishkoff S February 2016 Science and Society Taking race out of human genetics Science 351 6273 564 5 Bibcode 2016Sci 351 564Y doi 10 1126 science aac4951 PMID 26912690 S2CID 206639306 a b Bryc K Durand EY Macpherson JM Reich D Mountain JL January 2015 The genetic ancestry of African Americans Latinos and European Americans across the United States American Journal of Human Genetics 96 1 37 53 doi 10 1016 j ajhg 2014 11 010 PMC 4289685 PMID 25529636 Abecasis GR Auton A Brooks LD DePristo MA Durbin RM Handsaker RE et al November 2012 An integrated map of genetic variation from 1 092 human genomes Nature 491 7422 56 65 Bibcode 2012Natur 491 56T doi 10 1038 nature11632 PMC 3498066 PMID 23128226 Panofsky A 7 July 2014 Misbehaving Science Controversy and the Development of Behavior Genetics University of Chicago Press ISBN 978 0 226 05859 7 Archived from the original on 22 April 2021 Retrieved 23 May 2018 Lerner RM 27 August 2015 Eliminating Genetic Reductionism from Developmental Science Research in Human Development 12 3 4 178 188 doi 10 1080 15427609 2015 1068058 ISSN 1542 7609 S2CID 143195504 van Beijsterveldt CE Overbeek LI Rozendaal L McMaster MT Glasner TJ Bartels M et al May 2016 Chorionicity and Heritability Estimates from Twin Studies The Prenatal Environment of Twins and Their Resemblance Across a Large Number of Traits Behavior Genetics 46 3 304 14 doi 10 1007 s10519 015 9745 3 PMC 4858554 PMID 26410687 Further reading editCrusio WE 2015 Key issues in contemporary behavioral genetics Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 2 89 95 doi 10 1016 j cobeha 2014 10 002 S2CID 53200512 Crusio WE Gerlai RT eds 1999 Handbook of Molecular Genetic Techniques for Brain and Behavior Research Techniques in the Behavioral and Neural Sciences Vol 13 Elsevier ISBN 978 0 444 50239 1 Crusio WE Sluyter F Gerlai RT Pietropaolo S eds 2013 Behavioral Genetics of the Mouse Genetics of Behavioral Phenotypes Cambridge Handbooks in Behavioral Genetics Vol 1 Cambridge University Press ISBN 978 1 107 03481 5 Flint J Greenspan RJ Kendler KS 28 January 2010 How Genes Influence Behavior Oxford University Press ISBN 978 0 19 955990 9 Johnson W Turkheimer E Gottesman II Bouchard TJ August 2010 Beyond Heritability Twin Studies in Behavioral Research Current Directions in Psychological Science 18 4 217 220 doi 10 1111 j 1467 8721 2009 01639 x PMC 2899491 PMID 20625474 Johnson W Penke L Spinath FM 2011 Understanding Heritability What it is and What it is Not PDF European Journal of Personality 25 4 287 294 doi 10 1002 per 835 ISSN 0890 2070 S2CID 41842465 Archived PDF from the original on 16 December 2013 Retrieved 15 December 2013 Maxson SC 10 October 2012 Chapter 1 Behavioral Genetics In Weiner IB Nelson RJ Mizumori S eds Handbook of Psychology Volume 3 Behavioral Neuroscience John Wiley amp Sons ISBN 978 0 470 89059 2 Archived from the original on 16 December 2013 Retrieved 15 December 2013 Spinath FM Johnson W 2011 Chapter 10 Behavior Genetics In Chamorro Premuzic T von Stumm S Furnham A eds The Wiley Blackwell Handbook of Individual Differences United Kingdom Blackwell Publishing Ltd doi 10 1002 9781444343120 ISBN 978 1 4443 3438 8 External links edit nbsp Wikimedia Commons has media related to Behavioural genetics McGue M 5 May 2014 Introduction to Human Behavioral Genetics Coursera Retrieved 10 June 2014 McGue M December 2020 Introduction to Behavioral Genetics University of Minnesota Retrieved 28 June 2021 Behavior Genetics Association Behavior Genetics Association Retrieved 8 May 2022 Institute for Behavioral Genetics at the University of Colorado Boulder University of Colorado Boulder Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics Virginia Commonwealth University Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Behavioural genetics amp oldid 1225419566, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.