fbpx
Wikipedia

Verner's law

Verner's law describes a historical sound change in the Proto-Germanic language whereby consonants that would usually have been the voiceless fricatives *f, *þ, *s, *h, *, following an unstressed syllable, became the voiced fricatives *β, *ð, *z, *ɣ, *ɣʷ.[1] The law was formulated by Karl Verner, and first published in 1877.

Problem

A seminal insight into how the Germanic languages diverged from their Indo-European ancestor had been established in the early nineteenth century, and had been formulated as Grimm's law. Amongst other things, Grimm's law described how the Proto-Indo-European voiceless stops *p, *t, *k, and * regularly changed into Proto-Germanic *f (bilabial fricative [ɸ]), *þ (dental fricative [θ]), *h (velar fricative [x]), and * (velar fricative [xw]).[2]

However, there appeared to be a large set of words in which the agreement of Latin, Greek, Sanskrit, Baltic, Slavic etc. guaranteed Proto-Indo-European *p, *t or *k, and yet the Germanic reflex was not the expected, unvoiced fricatives *f, *þ, *h, * but rather their voiced counterparts *β, *ð, *ɣ, *ɣʷ. A similar problem obtained with Proto-Indo-European *s, which sometimes appeared as Proto-Germanic *z.[3]

At first, irregularities did not cause concern for scholars since there were many examples of the regular outcome. Increasingly, however, it became the ambition of linguists like the Neogrammarians to formulate general and exceptionless rules of sound change that would account for all the data (or as close to all the data as possible), not merely for a well-behaved subset of it.

One classic example of Proto-Indo-European *t → Proto-Germanic *ð is the word for 'father'. Proto-Indo-European *ph₂tḗr (here, the macron marks vowel length) → Proto-Germanic *faðēr (instead of expected *faþēr).[3] In the structurally similar family term *bʰréh₂tēr 'brother', Proto-Indo-European *t did indeed develop as predicted by Grimm's Law (Germanic *brōþēr).[4] Even more curiously, scholars often found both *þ and *ð as reflexes of Proto-Indo-European *t in different forms of one and the same root, e.g. *werþaną 'to turn', preterite third-person singular *warþ 'he turned', but preterite third-person plural *wurðun and past participle *wurðanaz.

Solution

Karl Verner is traditionally credited as the first scholar to note the factor governing the distribution of the two outcomes. Verner observed that the apparently unexpected voicing of Proto-Indo-European voiceless stops occurred if they were non-word-initial and if the vowel preceding them carried no stress in Proto-Indo-European. The original location of stress was often retained in Greek and early Sanskrit; in Germanic, though, stress eventually became fixed on the initial (root) syllable of all words.

The following table illustrates the sound changes according to Verner. In the bottom row, for each pair, the sound on the right represents the sound changed according to Verner's Law.

PIE *p *t *k *kʷ *s
Grimm *x *xʷ
Verner *x *xʷ *ɣʷ *s *z

The crucial difference between *patḗr and *bʰrā́tēr was therefore one of second-syllable versus first-syllable stress (compare Sanskrit pitā́ versus bhrā́tā).[3]

The *werþaną : *wurðun contrast is likewise explained as due to stress on the root versus stress on the inflectional suffix (leaving the first syllable unstressed). There are also other Vernerian alternations, as illustrated by modern German ziehen 'to draw, pull': Old High German zogōn 'to tug, drag' ← Proto-Germanic *teuhaną : *tugōną ← Pre-Germanic *déwk-o-nom : *duk-éh₂-yo-nom 'lead'.[3]

The change described by Verner's Law also accounts for Proto-Germanic *z as the development of Proto-Indo-European *s in some words. Since this *z changed to *r in the Scandinavian languages and in West Germanic (German, Dutch, English, Frisian), Verner's Law resulted in alternation of *s and *r in some inflectional paradigms, known as grammatischer Wechsel.[5] For example, the Old English verb ceosan 'choose' had the past plural form curon and the past participle (ge)coren. These three forms derived from Proto-Germanic *keusaną : *kuzun ~ *kuzanaz, which again derived from Pre-Germanic *géws-o-nom : *gus-únt ~ *gus-o-nós 'taste, try'. We would have **corn for chosen in Modern English if the consonants of choose and chose had not been morphologically levelled (compare the Dutch kiezen 'to choose' : verkoren 'chosen'). On the other hand, Vernerian *r has not been levelled out in English were ← Proto-Germanic *wēzun, related to English was. Similarly, English lose, though it has the weak form lost, also has the archaic form †lorn (now seen in the compound forlorn) (compare Dutch verliezen : verloren); in German, on the other hand, the *s has been levelled out both in war 'was' (plural waren 'were') and verlieren 'lose' (participle verloren 'lost').

Verner's law in Gothic

Whereas the North Germanic and West Germanic languages clearly show the effects of Verner's law, those patterns seldom appear in Gothic, the representative of East Germanic. This is usually thought to be because Gothic eliminated most Verner's law variants through analogy with the unaffected consonants.[6]

Significance

Karl Verner published his discovery in the article "Eine Ausnahme der ersten Lautverschiebung" (an exception to the first sound shift) in Kuhn's Journal of Comparative Linguistic Research in 1877,[7] but he had already presented his theory on 1 May 1875 in a comprehensive personal letter to his friend and mentor, Vilhelm Thomsen.[citation needed]

A letter shows that Eduard Sievers had hit on the same explanation by 1874, but did not publish it.[8]

Verner's theory was received with great enthusiasm by the young generation of comparative philologists, the so-called Junggrammatiker, because it was an important argument in favour of the Neogrammarian dogma that the sound laws were without exceptions ("die Ausnahmslosigkeit der Lautgesetze").

Dating the change described by Verner's law

The change in pronunciation described by Verner's Law must have occurred before the shift of stress to the first syllable: the voicing of the new consonant in Proto-Germanic is conditioned by which syllable is stressed in Proto-Indo-European, yet this syllabic stress has disappeared in Proto-Germanic, so the change in the consonant must have occurred at a time when the syllabic stress in earlier Proto-Germanic still conformed to the Indo-European pattern. However, the syllabic stress shift erased the conditioning environment, and made the variation between voiceless fricatives and their voiced alternants look mysteriously haphazard.

Which applied first: Grimm's law or Verner's law?

Until recently it was assumed that Verner's law was productive after Grimm's Law, and this remains the standard account: R. D. Fulk's 2018 Comparative Grammar of the Early Germanic Languages, for example, finds that 'Grimm's law should be assumed to antecede Verner's law'.[9]

But it has been pointed out that, even if the sequence is reversed, the result can be just the same given certain conditions, and the thesis that Verner's Law might have been valid before Grimm's Law—maybe long before it—has been finding more and more acceptance.[10] Accordingly, this order now would have to be assumed:

  1. Verner's law (possible boundary for Indo-European/Germanic)
  2. Grimm's law/First Sound Shift (does not mark the formation of Germanic accordingly)
  3. Appearance of initial stress (third possible boundary for Indo-European/Germanic)

This chronological reordering would have far-reaching implications for the shape and development of the Proto-Germanic language. If Verner's law operated before Grimm's law, one would expect the voicing of Proto-Indo-European *p, *t, *k, and * to produce *b, *d, *g, and *, which would have been identical with the existing Proto-Indo-European voiced stops. Yet it is clear that consonants affected by Verner's law merged with the descendants of the Proto-Indo-European voiced aspirate stops, not of the plain voiced stops. The usual proposed explanation for this is to postulate aspiration in the voiceless stops of the dialect of Indo-European that gave rise to Proto-Germanic.

Here is a table describing the sequence of changes in this alternative ordering:

PrePG *pʰ *tʰ *kʰ *kʷʰ *s
Verner *pʰ *bʱ *tʰ *dʱ *kʰ *ɡʱ *kʷʰ *ɡʷʱ *s *z
Grimm *x *xʷ *ɣʷ

(This can however be bypassed in the glottalic theory framework, where the voiced aspirate stops are replaced with plain voiced stops, and plain voiced stops with glottalized stops.)

Having Verner's law act before Grimm's law may help explain the little evidence that there is for the earliest Germanic phonology. There is some evidence for dating Grimm's law changes only to the end of the first century BCE. In particular, the tribal name recorded as Cimbri by the Romans and the river-name recorded as Vaculus (now known as the Waal) suggest that the change from k to h had still not happened around the first century BCE, when Romans were rendering those words into Latin (unless they were rendering the early Germanic *h (/x/) sound as a /k/ because their own /h/ did not often occur between vowels and was at any rate already in the process of going silent).[citation needed] If Grimm's law was operative only in the first century BCE and Verner's law applied after it (followed in turn by the shift of stress to initial syllables), then three dramatic changes would have had to happen in quick succession. Such a rapid set of language changes seems implausible to some scholars.[who?]

Meanwhile, Noske (2012) argues that Grimm's Law and Verner's Law must have been part of a single bifurcating chain shift.

Areal connections

An exact parallel to Verner's law is found in the neighboring Finnic languages, where it forms a part of the system of consonant gradation: a single voiceless consonant (*p, *t, *k, *s) becomes weakened (*b, *d, *g; *h < *z) when occurring after an unstressed syllable. As word stress in Finnic is predictable (primary stress on the initial syllable, secondary stress on odd-numbered non-final syllables), and has remained so since Proto-Uralic, this change did not produce any alternation in the shape of word roots. However, it manifests in the shape of numerous inflectional or derivational suffixes, and is therefore called "suffixal gradation".[citation needed]

Suffixal gradation in the Finnish partitive case
Meaning Pre-Proto-Finnic Proto-Finnic modern Finnish
'tree' (nom. : part.) *puu : *ˈpuu-ta *puu : *puuta puu : puuta
'hut, teepee' (nom. : part.) kota : *ˈkota-ta *ˈkota : *ˈkotada kota : kotaa
'blind' (nom. : part.) soketa : *ˈsokeˌta-ta *ˈsokeda : *ˈsokeˌdata sokea : sokeata

Lauri Posti argued that suffixal gradation in Finnic represents Germanic influence, in particular reflecting the pronunciation of Proto-Finnic by a hypothetical Germanic-speaking superstrate (often assumed to account for the great number of Germanic loanwords already in Proto-Finnic).[11] On the contrary, consonant gradation has also been viewed as inheritance from Proto-Uralic, as it occurs also in other Uralic languages. In particular, suffixal gradation under identical conditions also exists in Nganasan. The possibility of the opposite direction of influence – from Finnic to Germanic – has also been suggested.[12]

See also

References

  1. ^ In Proto-Germanic, voiced fricatives *[β ð ɣ] were allophones of their corresponding voiced plosives *[b d ɡ] when they occurred between vowels, semivowels, and liquid consonants. The situations where Verner's law applied resulted in fricatives in these very circumstances, so fricative *[b d ɡ] can be used in this context.
  2. ^ R.D. Fulk, A Comparative Grammar of the Early Germanic Languages, Studies in Germanic Linguistics, 3 (Amsterdam: Benjamins, 2018), doi:10.1075/sigl.3, ISBN 978 90 272 6312 4, p. 102.  
  3. ^ a b c d R.D. Fulk, A Comparative Grammar of the Early Germanic Languages, Studies in Germanic Linguistics, 3 (Amsterdam: Benjamins, 2018), doi:10.1075/sigl.3, ISBN 978 90 272 6312 4, pp. 107-8.  
  4. ^ R.D. Fulk, A Comparative Grammar of the Early Germanic Languages, Studies in Germanic Linguistics, 3 (Amsterdam: Benjamins, 2018), doi:10.1075/sigl.3, ISBN 978 90 272 6312 4, p. 106.  
  5. ^ R.D. Fulk, A Comparative Grammar of the Early Germanic Languages, Studies in Germanic Linguistics, 3 (Amsterdam: Benjamins, 2018), doi:10.1075/sigl.3, ISBN 978 90 272 6312 4, p. 106-9.  
  6. ^ R.D. Fulk, A Comparative Grammar of the Early Germanic Languages, Studies in Germanic Linguistics, 3 (Amsterdam: Benjamins, 2018), doi:10.1075/sigl.3, ISBN 978 90 272 6312 4, p. 108-9.  
  7. ^ Verner, K. 1877. Eine Ausnahme der ersten Lautverschiebung. Zeitschrift für vergleichende Sprach-forschung auf dem Gebiete der indo-germanischen Sprachen, 23.97–130.
  8. ^ R.D. Fulk, A Comparative Grammar of the Early Germanic Languages, Studies in Germanic Linguistics, 3 (Amsterdam: Benjamins, 2018), doi:10.1075/sigl.3, ISBN 978 90 272 6312 4, p. 110, n. 1.  
  9. ^ R.D. Fulk, A Comparative Grammar of the Early Germanic Languages, Studies in Germanic Linguistics, 3 (Amsterdam: Benjamins, 2018), doi:10.1075/sigl.3, ISBN 978 90 272 6312 4, p. 111.  
  10. ^ Vennemann 1984:21, Kortlandt 1988:5–6, Euler and Badenheuer 2009:54-55 and 61-64.
  11. ^ Posti, Lauri (1953). "From Pre-Finnic to Late Proto-Finnic". Finnisch-Ugrische Forschungen. XXXI: 1–91 (74–82).
  12. ^ Kallio, Petri (2000). "Posti's Superstrate Theory at the Threshold of a New Millennium". In Laakso, Johanna (ed.). Facing Finnic: Some Challenges to Historical and Contact Linguistics. Castrenianumin toimitteita. Vol. 59.

Further reference

  1. Ramat, Paolo, Einführung in das Germanische (Linguistische Arbeiten 95) (Tübingen, 1981)
  2. Wolfram Euler, Konrad Badenheuer: Sprache und Herkunft der Germanen — Abriss des Protogermanischen vor der Ersten Lautverschiebung [Language and Origin of the Germanic Peoples — Compendium of the Proto-Germanic Language prior to First Sound Shift], 244 p., ISBN 978-3-9812110-1-6, London/Hamburg 2009
  3. Kortlandt, Frederik, Proto-Germanic obstruents. — in: Amsterdamer Beiträge zur älteren Germanistik 27, p. 3–10 (1988).
  4. Koivulehto, Jorma / Vennemann, Theo, Der finnische Stufenwechsel und das Vernersche Gesetz. - in: Beiträge zur Geschichte der deutschen Sprache und Literatur 118, p. 163–182 (esp. 170–174) (1996)
  5. Noske, Roland, The Grimm–Verner Chain Shift and Contrast Preservation Theory. — in: Botma, Bert & Roland Noske (eds.), Phonological Explorations. Empirical, Theoretical and Diachronic Issues (Linguistische Arbeiten 548), p. 63–86. Berlin: De Gruyter, 2012.
  6. Vennemann, Theo, Hochgermanisch und Niedergermanisch. — in: Beiträge zur Geschichte der deutschen Sprache und Literatur 106, p. 1–45 (1984)

External links

  • "An exception to the first sound shift" by Winfred P. Lehmann — From the Linguistics Research Center at the University of Texas at Austin
  • Verner, Karl A. (1877). "Eine Ausnahme der ersten Lautverschiebung." Zeitschrift für vergleichende Sprachforschung auf dem Gebiete der Indogermanischen Sprachen 23.2: 97–130 The original article (German)

verner, this, article, contains, characters, used, write, reconstructed, proto, indo, european, words, explanation, notation, proto, indo, european, phonology, without, proper, rendering, support, question, marks, boxes, other, symbols, instead, unicode, combi. This article contains characters used to write reconstructed Proto Indo European words for an explanation of the notation see Proto Indo European phonology Without proper rendering support you may see question marks boxes or other symbols instead of Unicode combining characters and Latin characters Verner s law describes a historical sound change in the Proto Germanic language whereby consonants that would usually have been the voiceless fricatives f th s h hʷ following an unstressed syllable became the voiced fricatives b d z ɣ ɣʷ 1 The law was formulated by Karl Verner and first published in 1877 Contents 1 Problem 2 Solution 3 Verner s law in Gothic 4 Significance 5 Dating the change described by Verner s law 5 1 Which applied first Grimm s law or Verner s law 6 Areal connections 7 See also 8 References 9 Further reference 10 External linksProblem EditA seminal insight into how the Germanic languages diverged from their Indo European ancestor had been established in the early nineteenth century and had been formulated as Grimm s law Amongst other things Grimm s law described how the Proto Indo European voiceless stops p t k and kʷ regularly changed into Proto Germanic f bilabial fricative ɸ th dental fricative 8 h velar fricative x and hʷ velar fricative xw 2 However there appeared to be a large set of words in which the agreement of Latin Greek Sanskrit Baltic Slavic etc guaranteed Proto Indo European p t or k and yet the Germanic reflex was not the expected unvoiced fricatives f th h hʷ but rather their voiced counterparts b d ɣ ɣʷ A similar problem obtained with Proto Indo European s which sometimes appeared as Proto Germanic z 3 At first irregularities did not cause concern for scholars since there were many examples of the regular outcome Increasingly however it became the ambition of linguists like the Neogrammarians to formulate general and exceptionless rules of sound change that would account for all the data or as close to all the data as possible not merely for a well behaved subset of it One classic example of Proto Indo European t Proto Germanic d is the word for father Proto Indo European ph tḗr here the macron marks vowel length Proto Germanic fader instead of expected father 3 In the structurally similar family term bʰreh ter brother Proto Indo European t did indeed develop as predicted by Grimm s Law Germanic brōther 4 Even more curiously scholars often found both th and d as reflexes of Proto Indo European t in different forms of one and the same root e g werthana to turn preterite third person singular warth he turned but preterite third person plural wurdun and past participle wurdanaz Solution EditKarl Verner is traditionally credited as the first scholar to note the factor governing the distribution of the two outcomes Verner observed that the apparently unexpected voicing of Proto Indo European voiceless stops occurred if they were non word initial and if the vowel preceding them carried no stress in Proto Indo European The original location of stress was often retained in Greek and early Sanskrit in Germanic though stress eventually became fixed on the initial root syllable of all words The following table illustrates the sound changes according to Verner In the bottom row for each pair the sound on the right represents the sound changed according to Verner s Law PIE p t k kʷ sGrimm ɸ 8 x xʷVerner ɸ b 8 d x ɣ xʷ ɣʷ s zThe crucial difference between patḗr and bʰra ter was therefore one of second syllable versus first syllable stress compare Sanskrit pita versus bhra ta 3 The werthana wurdun contrast is likewise explained as due to stress on the root versus stress on the inflectional suffix leaving the first syllable unstressed There are also other Vernerian alternations as illustrated by modern German ziehen to draw pull Old High German zogōn to tug drag Proto Germanic teuhana tugōna Pre Germanic dewk o nom duk eh yo nom lead 3 The change described by Verner s Law also accounts for Proto Germanic z as the development of Proto Indo European s in some words Since this z changed to r in the Scandinavian languages and in West Germanic German Dutch English Frisian Verner s Law resulted in alternation of s and r in some inflectional paradigms known as grammatischer Wechsel 5 For example the Old English verb ceosan choose had the past plural form curon and the past participle ge coren These three forms derived from Proto Germanic keusana kuzun kuzanaz which again derived from Pre Germanic gews o nom gus unt gus o nos taste try We would have corn for chosen in Modern English if the consonants of choose and chose had not been morphologically levelled compare the Dutch kiezen to choose verkoren chosen On the other hand Vernerian r has not been levelled out in English were Proto Germanic wezun related to English was Similarly English lose though it has the weak form lost also has the archaic form lorn now seen in the compound forlorn compare Dutch verliezen verloren in German on the other hand the s has been levelled out both in war was plural waren were and verlieren lose participle verloren lost Verner s law in Gothic EditWhereas the North Germanic and West Germanic languages clearly show the effects of Verner s law those patterns seldom appear in Gothic the representative of East Germanic This is usually thought to be because Gothic eliminated most Verner s law variants through analogy with the unaffected consonants 6 Significance EditKarl Verner published his discovery in the article Eine Ausnahme der ersten Lautverschiebung an exception to the first sound shift in Kuhn s Journal of Comparative Linguistic Research in 1877 7 but he had already presented his theory on 1 May 1875 in a comprehensive personal letter to his friend and mentor Vilhelm Thomsen citation needed A letter shows that Eduard Sievers had hit on the same explanation by 1874 but did not publish it 8 Verner s theory was received with great enthusiasm by the young generation of comparative philologists the so called Junggrammatiker because it was an important argument in favour of the Neogrammarian dogma that the sound laws were without exceptions die Ausnahmslosigkeit der Lautgesetze Dating the change described by Verner s law EditThe change in pronunciation described by Verner s Law must have occurred before the shift of stress to the first syllable the voicing of the new consonant in Proto Germanic is conditioned by which syllable is stressed in Proto Indo European yet this syllabic stress has disappeared in Proto Germanic so the change in the consonant must have occurred at a time when the syllabic stress in earlier Proto Germanic still conformed to the Indo European pattern However the syllabic stress shift erased the conditioning environment and made the variation between voiceless fricatives and their voiced alternants look mysteriously haphazard Which applied first Grimm s law or Verner s law Edit Until recently it was assumed that Verner s law was productive after Grimm s Law and this remains the standard account R D Fulk s 2018 Comparative Grammar of the Early Germanic Languages for example finds that Grimm s law should be assumed to antecede Verner s law 9 But it has been pointed out that even if the sequence is reversed the result can be just the same given certain conditions and the thesis that Verner s Law might have been valid before Grimm s Law maybe long before it has been finding more and more acceptance 10 Accordingly this order now would have to be assumed Verner s law possible boundary for Indo European Germanic Grimm s law First Sound Shift does not mark the formation of Germanic accordingly Appearance of initial stress third possible boundary for Indo European Germanic This chronological reordering would have far reaching implications for the shape and development of the Proto Germanic language If Verner s law operated before Grimm s law one would expect the voicing of Proto Indo European p t k and kʷ to produce b d g and gʷ which would have been identical with the existing Proto Indo European voiced stops Yet it is clear that consonants affected by Verner s law merged with the descendants of the Proto Indo European voiced aspirate stops not of the plain voiced stops The usual proposed explanation for this is to postulate aspiration in the voiceless stops of the dialect of Indo European that gave rise to Proto Germanic Here is a table describing the sequence of changes in this alternative ordering PrePG pʰ tʰ kʰ kʷʰ sVerner pʰ bʱ tʰ dʱ kʰ ɡʱ kʷʰ ɡʷʱ s zGrimm ɸ b 8 d x ɣ xʷ ɣʷ This can however be bypassed in the glottalic theory framework where the voiced aspirate stops are replaced with plain voiced stops and plain voiced stops with glottalized stops Having Verner s law act before Grimm s law may help explain the little evidence that there is for the earliest Germanic phonology There is some evidence for dating Grimm s law changes only to the end of the first century BCE In particular the tribal name recorded as Cimbri by the Romans and the river name recorded as Vaculus now known as the Waal suggest that the change from k to h had still not happened around the first century BCE when Romans were rendering those words into Latin unless they were rendering the early Germanic h x sound as a k because their own h did not often occur between vowels and was at any rate already in the process of going silent citation needed If Grimm s law was operative only in the first century BCE and Verner s law applied after it followed in turn by the shift of stress to initial syllables then three dramatic changes would have had to happen in quick succession Such a rapid set of language changes seems implausible to some scholars who Meanwhile Noske 2012 argues that Grimm s Law and Verner s Law must have been part of a single bifurcating chain shift Areal connections EditAn exact parallel to Verner s law is found in the neighboring Finnic languages where it forms a part of the system of consonant gradation a single voiceless consonant p t k s becomes weakened b d g h lt z when occurring after an unstressed syllable As word stress in Finnic is predictable primary stress on the initial syllable secondary stress on odd numbered non final syllables and has remained so since Proto Uralic this change did not produce any alternation in the shape of word roots However it manifests in the shape of numerous inflectional or derivational suffixes and is therefore called suffixal gradation citation needed Suffixal gradation in the Finnish partitive case Meaning Pre Proto Finnic Proto Finnic modern Finnish tree nom part puu ˈpuu ta puu puuta puu puuta hut teepee nom part ˈkota ˈkota ta ˈkota ˈkotada kota kotaa blind nom part ˈsoketa ˈsokeˌta ta ˈsokeda ˈsokeˌdata sokea sokeataLauri Posti argued that suffixal gradation in Finnic represents Germanic influence in particular reflecting the pronunciation of Proto Finnic by a hypothetical Germanic speaking superstrate often assumed to account for the great number of Germanic loanwords already in Proto Finnic 11 On the contrary consonant gradation has also been viewed as inheritance from Proto Uralic as it occurs also in other Uralic languages In particular suffixal gradation under identical conditions also exists in Nganasan The possibility of the opposite direction of influence from Finnic to Germanic has also been suggested 12 See also EditHigh German consonant shift Grimm s lawReferences Edit In Proto Germanic voiced fricatives b d ɣ were allophones of their corresponding voiced plosives b d ɡ when they occurred between vowels semivowels and liquid consonants The situations where Verner s law applied resulted in fricatives in these very circumstances so fricative b d ɡ can be used in this context R D Fulk A Comparative Grammar of the Early Germanic Languages Studies in Germanic Linguistics 3 Amsterdam Benjamins 2018 doi 10 1075 sigl 3 ISBN 978 90 272 6312 4 p 102 a b c d R D Fulk A Comparative Grammar of the Early Germanic Languages Studies in Germanic Linguistics 3 Amsterdam Benjamins 2018 doi 10 1075 sigl 3 ISBN 978 90 272 6312 4 pp 107 8 R D Fulk A Comparative Grammar of the Early Germanic Languages Studies in Germanic Linguistics 3 Amsterdam Benjamins 2018 doi 10 1075 sigl 3 ISBN 978 90 272 6312 4 p 106 R D Fulk A Comparative Grammar of the Early Germanic Languages Studies in Germanic Linguistics 3 Amsterdam Benjamins 2018 doi 10 1075 sigl 3 ISBN 978 90 272 6312 4 p 106 9 R D Fulk A Comparative Grammar of the Early Germanic Languages Studies in Germanic Linguistics 3 Amsterdam Benjamins 2018 doi 10 1075 sigl 3 ISBN 978 90 272 6312 4 p 108 9 Verner K 1877 Eine Ausnahme der ersten Lautverschiebung Zeitschrift fur vergleichende Sprach forschung auf dem Gebiete der indo germanischen Sprachen 23 97 130 R D Fulk A Comparative Grammar of the Early Germanic Languages Studies in Germanic Linguistics 3 Amsterdam Benjamins 2018 doi 10 1075 sigl 3 ISBN 978 90 272 6312 4 p 110 n 1 R D Fulk A Comparative Grammar of the Early Germanic Languages Studies in Germanic Linguistics 3 Amsterdam Benjamins 2018 doi 10 1075 sigl 3 ISBN 978 90 272 6312 4 p 111 Vennemann 1984 21 Kortlandt 1988 5 6 Euler and Badenheuer 2009 54 55 and 61 64 Posti Lauri 1953 From Pre Finnic to Late Proto Finnic Finnisch Ugrische Forschungen XXXI 1 91 74 82 Kallio Petri 2000 Posti s Superstrate Theory at the Threshold of a New Millennium In Laakso Johanna ed Facing Finnic Some Challenges to Historical and Contact Linguistics Castrenianumin toimitteita Vol 59 Further reference EditRamat Paolo Einfuhrung in das Germanische Linguistische Arbeiten 95 Tubingen 1981 Wolfram Euler Konrad Badenheuer Sprache und Herkunft der Germanen Abriss des Protogermanischen vor der Ersten Lautverschiebung Language and Origin of the Germanic Peoples Compendium of the Proto Germanic Language prior to First Sound Shift 244 p ISBN 978 3 9812110 1 6 London Hamburg 2009 Kortlandt Frederik Proto Germanic obstruents in Amsterdamer Beitrage zur alteren Germanistik 27 p 3 10 1988 Koivulehto Jorma Vennemann Theo Der finnische Stufenwechsel und das Vernersche Gesetz in Beitrage zur Geschichte der deutschen Sprache und Literatur 118 p 163 182 esp 170 174 1996 Noske Roland The Grimm Verner Chain Shift and Contrast Preservation Theory in Botma Bert amp Roland Noske eds Phonological Explorations Empirical Theoretical and Diachronic Issues Linguistische Arbeiten 548 p 63 86 Berlin De Gruyter 2012 Vennemann Theo Hochgermanisch und Niedergermanisch in Beitrage zur Geschichte der deutschen Sprache und Literatur 106 p 1 45 1984 External links EditA Reader in Nineteenth Century Historical IE Linguistics Ch 11 An exception to the first sound shift by Winfred P Lehmann From the Linguistics Research Center at the University of Texas at Austin Verner Karl A 1877 Eine Ausnahme der ersten Lautverschiebung Zeitschrift fur vergleichende Sprachforschung auf dem Gebiete der Indogermanischen Sprachen 23 2 97 130 The original article German Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Verner 27s law amp oldid 1118570891, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.