fbpx
Wikipedia

Sound symbolism

In linguistics, sound symbolism is the perception of speech sounds as concept meanings. It is a form of linguistic iconicity. For example, the English word ding may sound similar to the actual sound of a bell.

Linguistic sound may be perceived as similar to not only sounds, but also to other sensory properties, such as size, vision, touch, or smell, or abstract domains, such as emotion or value judgment. Such correspondence between linguistic sound and meaning may significantly affect the form of spoken languages.

History Edit

Plato and the Cratylus Dialogue Edit

In Cratylus, Plato has Socrates commenting on the origins and correctness of various names and words. When Hermogenes asks if he can provide another hypothesis on how signs come into being (his own is simply 'convention'), Socrates initially suggests that they fit their referents in virtue of the sounds they are made of:

Now the letter rho, as I was saying, appeared to the imposer of names an excellent instrument for the expression of motion; and he frequently uses the letter for this purpose: for example, in the actual words rein and roe he represents motion by rho; also in the words tromos (trembling), trachus (rugged); and again, in words such as krouein (strike), thrauein (crush), ereikein (bruise), thruptein (break), kermatixein (crumble), rumbein (whirl): of all these sorts of movements he generally finds an expression in the letter R, because, as I imagine, he had observed that the tongue was most agitated and least at rest in the pronunciation of this letter, which he therefore used in order to express motion

— Cratylus.[1]

However, faced by an overwhelming number of counterexamples given by Hermogenes, Socrates has to admit that "my first notions of original names are truly wild and ridiculous".

Upanishads Edit

The Upanishads and Vyākaraṇa contain a lot of material about sound symbolism, for instance:

The mute consonants represent the earth, the sibilants the sky, the vowels heaven. The mute consonants represent fire, the sibilants air, the vowels the sun… The mute consonants represent the eye, the sibilants the ear, the vowels the mind.

— Aitareya Aranyaka III.2.6.2.[2]

The concept of Sphota and Nirukta is also based on this.

Shingon Buddhism Edit

Kūkai, the founder of Shingon, wrote his Sound, word, reality in the 9th century which relates all sounds to the voice of the Dharmakaya Buddha.

Early Western phonosemantics Edit

The idea of phonosemantics was sporadically discussed during the Middle Ages and the Renaissance. In 1690, Locke wrote against the idea in An Essay Concerning Human Understanding. His argument was that if there were any connection between sounds and ideas, then we would all be speaking the same language, but this is an over-generalisation. Leibniz's book New Essays on Human Understanding published in 1765 contains a point by point critique of Locke's essay. Leibniz picks up on the generalization used by Locke and adopts a less rigid approach: clearly there is no perfect correspondence between words and things, but neither is the relationship completely arbitrary, although he seems vague about what that relationship might be.[3]

Modern linguistics Edit

Modern linguistics has been seen as opposing sound symbolism, beginning with Ferdinand de Saussure (1857–1913), considered the founder of modern 'scientific' linguistics. Central to what de Saussure says about words are two related statements: First, he says that "the sign is arbitrary". He considers the words that we use to indicate things and concepts could be any words – they are essentially just a consensus agreed upon by the speakers of a language and have no discernible pattern or relationship to the thing. Second, he says that, because words are arbitrary, they have meaning only in relation to other words. A dog is a dog because it is not a cat or a mouse or a horse, etc. These ideas have permeated the study of words since the 19th century.[citation needed]

Types Edit

Onomatopoeia Edit

Onomatopoeia refers to words that imitate sounds. Some examples in English are bow-wow or meow, each representing the sound of a dog or a cat.

Ideophone Edit

An ideophone is "a member of an open lexical class of marked words that depict sensory imagery".[4] Unlike onomatopoeia, an ideophone refers to words that depict any sensory domain, such as vision or touch. Examples are Korean mallang-mallang 말랑말랑 'soft' and Japanese kira-kira キラキラ 'shiny'. Ideophones are heavily present in many African and East/Southeast Asian languages, such as Japanese, Thai, and Xhosa. Their form is very often reduplicated, but not necessarily so.

Phonaesthemes Edit

A phonaestheme is a sub-morphemic sequence of sounds that are associated to a certain range of meanings. A well-known example is English gl-, which is present in many words related to light or vision, such as gleam, glow, or glare. Since it is submorphemic, gl- itself is not a morpheme, and it does not form compounds with other morphemes: -eam, -ow, and -are have no meaning of their own. Phonaesthemes, however, are not necessarily iconic, as they may be language-specific and may not iconically resemble the meaning they are associated to.

Sound symbolism in basic vocabulary Edit

Blasi et al. (2016),[5] Joo (2020),[6] and Johansson et al. (2020)[7] demonstrated that in the languages around the world, certain concepts in the basic vocabulary (such as the Swadesh list or the Leipzig-Jakarta list) tend to be represented by words containing certain sounds. Below are some of the phonosemantic associations confirmed by the three studies:

Concept Sound
Breast Nasal sounds (e. g. /m/)
Knee Rounded vowels (e. g. /o/)
Tongue Lateral consonants (e. g. /l/)

Magnitude symbolism Edit

High front vowels, such as /i/, are known to be perceptually associated to small size, whereas low and/or back vowels, such as /u/ or /a/, are usually associated with big size. This phenomenon is known as magnitude symbolism.

Sapir (1929)[8] showed that, when asked which of the two tables, named mil and mal, is bigger than the other, many people choose mal to be bigger than mil. This phenomenon is not only observable in pseudowords, but present throughout English vocabulary as well.[9]

Deictic symbolism Edit

In many languages, the proximal demonstrative pronoun ('this') tends to have high front vowels (such as /i/), whereas the distal demonstrative pronoun ('that') tends to have low and/or back vowels (such as /u/). [10] Examples include: English this and that, French ceci and cela, and Indonesian ini and itu.

Pronominal symbolism Edit

First person pronouns (me) and second person pronouns (you) tend to contain a nasal sound.[11] Joo (2020)[12] suggests that this may be related to the infant's tendency of using the nasal sound to seek the attention of the caretaker.

Bouba-Kiki effect Edit

 
Which of the two shapes is bouba, and which one is kiki?

Köhler (1929, 1947, 1970) [13] introduced what is known as the Takete-Maluma phenomenon. When presented two shapes, one being curvy and another being spiky, and asked which one is called Takete and which one is called Maluma, participants are more likely to associate the name Takete to the spiky shape and the name Maluma to the curvy shape.

Following Ramachandran and Hubbard (2001), [14] this phenomenon is now more commonly known as the Bouba/kiki effect, and has been demonstrated to be valid across different cultures and languages.[15][16]

Tactile sound symbolism Edit

Bilabial consonants have been demonstrated to be linked to the perception of softness, arguably due to the soft texture of human lips.[17][18]

The trilled R is frequent in words for 'rough' while infrequent in words for 'smooth'.[19]

Use in commerce Edit

Sound symbolism is used in commerce for the names of products and even companies themselves.[20] For example, a car company may be interested in how to name their car to make it sound faster or stronger. Furthermore, sound symbolism can be used to create a meaningful relationship between a company's brand name and the brand mark itself. Sound symbolism can relate to the color, shade, shape, and size of the brand mark.[21]

See also Edit

References Edit

  1. ^ (This is an open source translation available at Internet Classics Archive
  2. ^ [1] The Upanishads, translated by Max Müller, 1879.
  3. ^ adapted from a literature review by Margaret Magnus
  4. ^ Dingemanse, Mark (15 May 2019). "Chapter 1. 'Ideophone' as a comparative concept". Iconicity in Language and Literature. 16: 13–33. doi:10.1075/ill.16.02din. hdl:21.11116/0000-0002-6949-7. ISBN 978-90-272-0311-3. S2CID 171541216.
  5. ^ Blasi, Damián E.; Wichmann, Søren; Hammarström, Harald; Stadler, Peter F.; Christiansen, Morten H. (27 September 2016). "Sound–meaning association biases evidenced across thousands of languages". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 113 (39): 10818–10823. Bibcode:2016PNAS..11310818B. doi:10.1073/pnas.1605782113. PMC 5047153. PMID 27621455.
  6. ^ Joo, Ian (27 May 2020). "Phonosemantic biases found in Leipzig-Jakarta lists of 66 languages". Linguistic Typology. 24 (1): 1–12. doi:10.1515/lingty-2019-0030. hdl:21.11116/0000-0004-EBB1-B. S2CID 209962593.
  7. ^ Erben Johansson, Niklas; Anikin, Andrey; Carling, Gerd; Holmer, Arthur (27 August 2020). "The typology of sound symbolism: Defining macro-concepts via their semantic and phonetic features". Linguistic Typology. 24 (2): 253–310. doi:10.1515/lingty-2020-2034. S2CID 209913202.
  8. ^ Sapir, E. (1929). "A study in phonetic symbolism". Journal of Experimental Psychology. 12 (3): 225–239. doi:10.1037/h0070931. hdl:11858/00-001M-0000-002C-4324-D. S2CID 37090694.
  9. ^ Winter, Bodo; Perlman, Marcus (28 June 2021). "Size sound symbolism in the English lexicon". Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics. 6 (1). doi:10.5334/gjgl.1646. S2CID 238832121.
  10. ^ Johansson, Niklas; Zlatev, Jordan (1 January 1970). "Motivations for Sound Symbolism in Spatial Deixis: A Typological Study of 101 Languages". Public Journal of Semiotics. 5 (1): 3–20. doi:10.37693/pjos.2013.5.9668.
  11. ^ Nichols, Johanna; Peterson, David A. (June 1996). "The Amerind Personal Pronouns". Language. 72 (2): 336. doi:10.2307/416653. JSTOR 416653.
  12. ^ Joo, Ian (27 May 2020). "Phonosemantic biases found in Leipzig-Jakarta lists of 66 languages". Linguistic Typology. 24 (1): 1–12. doi:10.1515/lingty-2019-0030. hdl:21.11116/0000-0004-EBB1-B. S2CID 209962593.
  13. ^ Köhler, Wolfgang (1970). Gestalt psychology; an introduction to new concepts in modern psychology. New York: Liveright. ISBN 0871402181.
  14. ^ Ramachandran, V.S.; Hubbard, E.M. (1 December 2001). "Synaesthesia -- A window into perception, thought and language". Journal of Consciousness Studies. 8 (12): 3–34.
  15. ^ Bremner, Andrew J.; Caparos, Serge; Davidoff, Jules; de Fockert, Jan; Linnell, Karina J.; Spence, Charles (February 2013). ""Bouba" and "Kiki" in Namibia? A remote culture make similar shape–sound matches, but different shape–taste matches to Westerners". Cognition. 126 (2): 165–172. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2012.09.007. PMID 23121711. S2CID 27805778.
  16. ^ Ćwiek, Aleksandra; Fuchs, Susanne; Draxler, Christoph; Asu, Eva Liina; Dediu, Dan; Hiovain, Katri; Kawahara, Shigeto; Koutalidis, Sofia; Krifka, Manfred; Lippus, Pärtel; Lupyan, Gary; Oh, Grace E.; Paul, Jing; Petrone, Caterina; Ridouane, Rachid; Reiter, Sabine; Schümchen, Nathalie; Szalontai, Ádám; Ünal-Logacev, Özlem; Zeller, Jochen; Perlman, Marcus; Winter, Bodo (3 January 2022). "The bouba/kiki effect is robust across cultures and writing systems". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 377 (1841): 20200390. doi:10.1098/rstb.2020.0390. ISSN 0962-8436. PMC 8591387. PMID 34775818. S2CID 244103844.
  17. ^ Sakamoto, Maki; Watanabe, Junji (12 March 2018). "Bouba/Kiki in Touch: Associations Between Tactile Perceptual Qualities and Japanese Phonemes". Frontiers in Psychology. 9: 295. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00295. PMC 5857596. PMID 29593602.
  18. ^ Kumagai, Gakuji (31 December 2020). "The pluripotentiality of bilabial consonants: The images of softness and cuteness in Japanese and English". Open Linguistics. 6 (1): 693–707. doi:10.1515/opli-2020-0040. S2CID 231628731.
  19. ^ Winter, Bodo; Sóskuthy, Márton; Perlman, Marcus; Dingemanse, Mark (20 January 2022). "Trilled /r/ is associated with roughness, linking sound and touch across spoken languages". Scientific Reports. 12 (1): 1035. Bibcode:2022NatSR..12.1035W. doi:10.1038/s41598-021-04311-7. ISSN 2045-2322. PMC 8776840. PMID 35058475.
  20. ^ Klink, Richard R. (1 February 2000). "Creating Brand Names With Meaning: The Use of Sound Symbolism". Marketing Letters. 11 (1): 5–20. doi:10.1023/A:1008184423824. ISSN 1573-059X. S2CID 166893362.
  21. ^ Klink, Richard R. (2003). "Creating Meaningful Brands: The Relationship Between Brand Name and Brand Mark". Marketing Letters. 14 (3): 143–157. doi:10.1023/A:1027476132607. S2CID 114904260.

sound, symbolism, linguistics, sound, symbolism, perception, speech, sounds, concept, meanings, form, linguistic, iconicity, example, english, word, ding, sound, similar, actual, sound, bell, linguistic, sound, perceived, similar, only, sounds, also, other, se. In linguistics sound symbolism is the perception of speech sounds as concept meanings It is a form of linguistic iconicity For example the English word ding may sound similar to the actual sound of a bell Linguistic sound may be perceived as similar to not only sounds but also to other sensory properties such as size vision touch or smell or abstract domains such as emotion or value judgment Such correspondence between linguistic sound and meaning may significantly affect the form of spoken languages Contents 1 History 1 1 Plato and the Cratylus Dialogue 1 2 Upanishads 1 3 Shingon Buddhism 1 4 Early Western phonosemantics 1 5 Modern linguistics 2 Types 2 1 Onomatopoeia 2 2 Ideophone 2 3 Phonaesthemes 2 4 Sound symbolism in basic vocabulary 2 5 Magnitude symbolism 2 6 Deictic symbolism 2 7 Pronominal symbolism 2 8 Bouba Kiki effect 2 9 Tactile sound symbolism 3 Use in commerce 4 See also 5 ReferencesHistory EditPlato and the Cratylus Dialogue Edit In Cratylus Plato has Socrates commenting on the origins and correctness of various names and words When Hermogenes asks if he can provide another hypothesis on how signs come into being his own is simply convention Socrates initially suggests that they fit their referents in virtue of the sounds they are made of Now the letter rho as I was saying appeared to the imposer of names an excellent instrument for the expression of motion and he frequently uses the letter for this purpose for example in the actual words rein and roe he represents motion by rho also in the words tromos trembling trachus rugged and again in words such as krouein strike thrauein crush ereikein bruise thruptein break kermatixein crumble rumbein whirl of all these sorts of movements he generally finds an expression in the letter R because as I imagine he had observed that the tongue was most agitated and least at rest in the pronunciation of this letter which he therefore used in order to express motion Cratylus 1 However faced by an overwhelming number of counterexamples given by Hermogenes Socrates has to admit that my first notions of original names are truly wild and ridiculous Upanishads Edit The Upanishads and Vyakaraṇa contain a lot of material about sound symbolism for instance The mute consonants represent the earth the sibilants the sky the vowels heaven The mute consonants represent fire the sibilants air the vowels the sun The mute consonants represent the eye the sibilants the ear the vowels the mind Aitareya Aranyaka III 2 6 2 2 The concept of Sphota and Nirukta is also based on this Shingon Buddhism Edit Kukai the founder of Shingon wrote his Sound word reality in the 9th century which relates all sounds to the voice of the Dharmakaya Buddha Early Western phonosemantics Edit The idea of phonosemantics was sporadically discussed during the Middle Ages and the Renaissance In 1690 Locke wrote against the idea in An Essay Concerning Human Understanding His argument was that if there were any connection between sounds and ideas then we would all be speaking the same language but this is an over generalisation Leibniz s book New Essays on Human Understanding published in 1765 contains a point by point critique of Locke s essay Leibniz picks up on the generalization used by Locke and adopts a less rigid approach clearly there is no perfect correspondence between words and things but neither is the relationship completely arbitrary although he seems vague about what that relationship might be 3 Modern linguistics Edit Modern linguistics has been seen as opposing sound symbolism beginning with Ferdinand de Saussure 1857 1913 considered the founder of modern scientific linguistics Central to what de Saussure says about words are two related statements First he says that the sign is arbitrary He considers the words that we use to indicate things and concepts could be any words they are essentially just a consensus agreed upon by the speakers of a language and have no discernible pattern or relationship to the thing Second he says that because words are arbitrary they have meaning only in relation to other words A dog is a dog because it is not a cat or a mouse or a horse etc These ideas have permeated the study of words since the 19th century citation needed Types EditOnomatopoeia Edit See also Onomatopoeia Onomatopoeia refers to words that imitate sounds Some examples in English are bow wow or meow each representing the sound of a dog or a cat Ideophone Edit See also Ideophones An ideophone is a member of an open lexical class of marked words that depict sensory imagery 4 Unlike onomatopoeia an ideophone refers to words that depict any sensory domain such as vision or touch Examples are Korean mallang mallang 말랑말랑 soft and Japanese kira kira キラキラ shiny Ideophones are heavily present in many African and East Southeast Asian languages such as Japanese Thai and Xhosa Their form is very often reduplicated but not necessarily so Phonaesthemes Edit See also Phonaestheme A phonaestheme is a sub morphemic sequence of sounds that are associated to a certain range of meanings A well known example is English gl which is present in many words related to light or vision such as gleam glow or glare Since it is submorphemic gl itself is not a morpheme and it does not form compounds with other morphemes eam ow and are have no meaning of their own Phonaesthemes however are not necessarily iconic as they may be language specific and may not iconically resemble the meaning they are associated to Sound symbolism in basic vocabulary Edit Blasi et al 2016 5 Joo 2020 6 and Johansson et al 2020 7 demonstrated that in the languages around the world certain concepts in the basic vocabulary such as the Swadesh list or the Leipzig Jakarta list tend to be represented by words containing certain sounds Below are some of the phonosemantic associations confirmed by the three studies Concept SoundBreast Nasal sounds e g m Knee Rounded vowels e g o Tongue Lateral consonants e g l Magnitude symbolism Edit High front vowels such as i are known to be perceptually associated to small size whereas low and or back vowels such as u or a are usually associated with big size This phenomenon is known as magnitude symbolism Sapir 1929 8 showed that when asked which of the two tables named mil and mal is bigger than the other many people choose mal to be bigger than mil This phenomenon is not only observable in pseudowords but present throughout English vocabulary as well 9 Deictic symbolism Edit In many languages the proximal demonstrative pronoun this tends to have high front vowels such as i whereas the distal demonstrative pronoun that tends to have low and or back vowels such as u 10 Examples include English this and that French ceci and cela and Indonesian ini and itu Pronominal symbolism Edit First person pronouns me and second person pronouns you tend to contain a nasal sound 11 Joo 2020 12 suggests that this may be related to the infant s tendency of using the nasal sound to seek the attention of the caretaker Bouba Kiki effect Edit See also Bouba kiki effect nbsp Which of the two shapes is bouba and which one is kiki Kohler 1929 1947 1970 13 introduced what is known as the Takete Maluma phenomenon When presented two shapes one being curvy and another being spiky and asked which one is called Takete and which one is called Maluma participants are more likely to associate the name Takete to the spiky shape and the name Maluma to the curvy shape Following Ramachandran and Hubbard 2001 14 this phenomenon is now more commonly known as the Bouba kiki effect and has been demonstrated to be valid across different cultures and languages 15 16 Tactile sound symbolism Edit Bilabial consonants have been demonstrated to be linked to the perception of softness arguably due to the soft texture of human lips 17 18 The trilled R is frequent in words for rough while infrequent in words for smooth 19 Use in commerce EditSound symbolism is used in commerce for the names of products and even companies themselves 20 For example a car company may be interested in how to name their car to make it sound faster or stronger Furthermore sound symbolism can be used to create a meaningful relationship between a company s brand name and the brand mark itself Sound symbolism can relate to the color shade shape and size of the brand mark 21 See also EditIdeasthesia Ideophone Imitation of natural sounds in various cultures Onomatopoeia Phono semantic matching Phonestheme Phonaesthetics Sign linguistics ZaumReferences Edit This is an open source translation available at Internet Classics Archive 1 The Upanishads translated by Max Muller 1879 adapted from a literature review by Margaret Magnus Dingemanse Mark 15 May 2019 Chapter 1 Ideophone as a comparative concept Iconicity in Language and Literature 16 13 33 doi 10 1075 ill 16 02din hdl 21 11116 0000 0002 6949 7 ISBN 978 90 272 0311 3 S2CID 171541216 Blasi Damian E Wichmann Soren Hammarstrom Harald Stadler Peter F Christiansen Morten H 27 September 2016 Sound meaning association biases evidenced across thousands of languages Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 113 39 10818 10823 Bibcode 2016PNAS 11310818B doi 10 1073 pnas 1605782113 PMC 5047153 PMID 27621455 Joo Ian 27 May 2020 Phonosemantic biases found in Leipzig Jakarta lists of 66 languages Linguistic Typology 24 1 1 12 doi 10 1515 lingty 2019 0030 hdl 21 11116 0000 0004 EBB1 B S2CID 209962593 Erben Johansson Niklas Anikin Andrey Carling Gerd Holmer Arthur 27 August 2020 The typology of sound symbolism Defining macro concepts via their semantic and phonetic features Linguistic Typology 24 2 253 310 doi 10 1515 lingty 2020 2034 S2CID 209913202 Sapir E 1929 A study in phonetic symbolism Journal of Experimental Psychology 12 3 225 239 doi 10 1037 h0070931 hdl 11858 00 001M 0000 002C 4324 D S2CID 37090694 Winter Bodo Perlman Marcus 28 June 2021 Size sound symbolism in the English lexicon Glossa A Journal of General Linguistics 6 1 doi 10 5334 gjgl 1646 S2CID 238832121 Johansson Niklas Zlatev Jordan 1 January 1970 Motivations for Sound Symbolism in Spatial Deixis A Typological Study of 101 Languages Public Journal of Semiotics 5 1 3 20 doi 10 37693 pjos 2013 5 9668 Nichols Johanna Peterson David A June 1996 The Amerind Personal Pronouns Language 72 2 336 doi 10 2307 416653 JSTOR 416653 Joo Ian 27 May 2020 Phonosemantic biases found in Leipzig Jakarta lists of 66 languages Linguistic Typology 24 1 1 12 doi 10 1515 lingty 2019 0030 hdl 21 11116 0000 0004 EBB1 B S2CID 209962593 Kohler Wolfgang 1970 Gestalt psychology an introduction to new concepts in modern psychology New York Liveright ISBN 0871402181 Ramachandran V S Hubbard E M 1 December 2001 Synaesthesia A window into perception thought and language Journal of Consciousness Studies 8 12 3 34 Bremner Andrew J Caparos Serge Davidoff Jules de Fockert Jan Linnell Karina J Spence Charles February 2013 Bouba and Kiki in Namibia A remote culture make similar shape sound matches but different shape taste matches to Westerners Cognition 126 2 165 172 doi 10 1016 j cognition 2012 09 007 PMID 23121711 S2CID 27805778 Cwiek Aleksandra Fuchs Susanne Draxler Christoph Asu Eva Liina Dediu Dan Hiovain Katri Kawahara Shigeto Koutalidis Sofia Krifka Manfred Lippus Partel Lupyan Gary Oh Grace E Paul Jing Petrone Caterina Ridouane Rachid Reiter Sabine Schumchen Nathalie Szalontai Adam Unal Logacev Ozlem Zeller Jochen Perlman Marcus Winter Bodo 3 January 2022 The bouba kiki effect is robust across cultures and writing systems Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences 377 1841 20200390 doi 10 1098 rstb 2020 0390 ISSN 0962 8436 PMC 8591387 PMID 34775818 S2CID 244103844 Sakamoto Maki Watanabe Junji 12 March 2018 Bouba Kiki in Touch Associations Between Tactile Perceptual Qualities and Japanese Phonemes Frontiers in Psychology 9 295 doi 10 3389 fpsyg 2018 00295 PMC 5857596 PMID 29593602 Kumagai Gakuji 31 December 2020 The pluripotentiality of bilabial consonants The images of softness and cuteness in Japanese and English Open Linguistics 6 1 693 707 doi 10 1515 opli 2020 0040 S2CID 231628731 Winter Bodo Soskuthy Marton Perlman Marcus Dingemanse Mark 20 January 2022 Trilled r is associated with roughness linking sound and touch across spoken languages Scientific Reports 12 1 1035 Bibcode 2022NatSR 12 1035W doi 10 1038 s41598 021 04311 7 ISSN 2045 2322 PMC 8776840 PMID 35058475 Klink Richard R 1 February 2000 Creating Brand Names With Meaning The Use of Sound Symbolism Marketing Letters 11 1 5 20 doi 10 1023 A 1008184423824 ISSN 1573 059X S2CID 166893362 Klink Richard R 2003 Creating Meaningful Brands The Relationship Between Brand Name and Brand Mark Marketing Letters 14 3 143 157 doi 10 1023 A 1027476132607 S2CID 114904260 Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Sound symbolism amp oldid 1179459160, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.