fbpx
Wikipedia

Eurasiatic languages

Eurasiatic[1] is a proposed language macrofamily that would include many language families historically spoken in northern, western, and southern Eurasia.

Eurasiatic
Geographic
distribution
Before the 16th century, most of Siberia and Western Eurasia; today worldwide
Linguistic classificationNostratic (?)
  • Eurasiatic
Proto-languageProto-Eurasiatic
Subdivisions
GlottologNone
The worldwide distribution of the Eurasiatic macrofamily of languages according to Pagel et al.

The idea of a Eurasiatic superfamily dates back more than 100 years. Joseph Greenberg's proposal, dating to the 1990s, is the most widely discussed version. In 2013, Mark Pagel and three colleagues published what they believe to be statistical evidence for a Eurasiatic language family.

The branches of Eurasiatic vary between proposals, but typically include Altaic (Mongolic, Tungusic and Turkic), Chukchi-Kamchatkan, Eskimo–Aleut, Indo-European, and Uralic—although Greenberg uses the controversial Uralic-Yukaghir classification instead. Other branches sometimes included are the Kartvelian and Dravidian families, as proposed by Pagel et al., in addition to the language isolates Nivkh, Etruscan and Greenberg's "Korean–Japanese–Ainu". Some proposals group Eurasiatic with even larger macrofamilies, such as Nostratic; again, many other professional linguists regard the methods used as invalid.

History of the concept edit

In 1994 Merritt Ruhlen claimed Eurasiatic is supported by the existence of a grammatical pattern "whereby plurals of nouns are formed by suffixing -t to the noun root ... whereas duals of nouns are formed by suffixing -k." Rasmus Rask noted this grammatical pattern in the groups now called Uralic and Eskimo–Aleut as early as 1818, but it can also be found in Tungusic, Nivkh (also called Gilyak) and Chukchi–Kamchatkan—all of which Greenberg placed in Eurasiatic. According to Ruhlen, this pattern is not found in language families or languages outside Eurasiatic.[2]

In 1998, Joseph Greenberg extended his work in mass comparison, a methodology he first proposed in the 1950s to categorize the languages of Africa, to suggest a Eurasiatic language.[3] In 2000, he expanded his argument for Eurasiatic into a full-length book, Indo-European and Its Closest Relatives: The Eurasiatic Language Family, in which he outlines both phonetic and grammatical evidence that he feels demonstrate the validity of language family. The heart of his argument is 72 morphological features that he judges as common across the various language families he examines.[4] Of the many variant proposals, Greenberg's has attracted the most academic attention.[3]

Greenberg's Eurasiatic hypothesis has been dismissed by many linguists, often on the ground that his research on mass comparison is unreliable. The primary criticism of comparative methods is that cognates are assumed to have a common origin on the basis of similar sounds and word meanings. It is generally assumed that semantic and phonetic corruption destroys any trace of original sound and meaning within 5,000 to 9,000 years making the application of comparative methods to ancient superfamilies highly questionable. Additionally, apparent cognates can arise by chance or from loan words. Without the existence of statistical estimates of chance collisions, conclusions based on comparison alone are thus viewed as doubtful.[5]

Stefan Georg and Alexander Vovin, who, unlike many of their colleagues, do not stipulate a priori that attempts to find ancient relationships are bound to fail, examined Greenberg's claims in detail.[6] They state that Greenberg's morphological arguments are the correct approach to determining families, but doubt his conclusions. They write "[Greenberg's] 72 morphemes look like massive evidence in favour of Eurasiatic at first glance. If valid, few linguists would have the right to doubt that a point has been made  [...] However, closer inspection  [...] shows too many misinterpretations, errors and wrong analyses  [...] these allow no other judgement than that [Greenberg's] attempt to demonstrate the validity of his Eurasiatic has failed."[7]

In the 1980s, Russian linguist Nikolai Dmitrievich Andreev [ru]'s Boreal languages [ru] hypothesis (Russian: Бореальный язык) linking the Indo-European, Uralic, and Altaic (including Korean in his later papers) language families. Andreev also proposed 203 lexical roots for his hypothesized Boreal macrofamily. After Andreev's death in 1997, the Boreal hypothesis was further expanded by Sorin Paliga (2003, 2007).[8][9]

Pagel et al. edit

In 2013, Mark Pagel, Quentin D. Atkinson, Andreea S. Calude, and Andrew Meade published statistical evidence that attempts to overcome these objections. According to their earlier work, most words exhibit a "half-life" of between 2,000 and 4,000 years, consistent with existing theories of linguistic replacement. However, they also identified some words – numerals, pronouns, and certain adverbs – that exhibit a much slower rate of replacement with half-lives of 10,000 to 20,000 or more years. Drawing from research in a diverse group of modern languages, the authors were able to show the same slow replacement rates for key words regardless of current pronunciation. They conclude that a stable core of largely unchanging words is a common feature of all human discourse, and model replacement as inversely proportional to usage frequency.[5]

Pagel et al. used hypothesized reconstructions of proto-words from seven language families listed in the Languages of the World Etymological Database (LWED).[5] They limited their search to the 200 most common words as described by the Swadesh fundamental vocabulary list. Twelve words were excluded because proto-words had been proposed for two or fewer language families. The remaining 188 words yielded 3804 different reconstructions (sometimes with multiple constructions for a given family). In contrast to traditional comparative linguistics, the researchers did not attempt to "prove" any given pairing as cognates (based on similar sounds), but rather treated each pairing as a binary random variable subject to error. The set of possible cognate pairings was then analyzed as a whole for predictable regularities.[10]

Words were separated into groupings based on how many language families appeared to be cognate for the word. Among the 188 words, cognate groups ranged from 1 (no cognates) to 7 (all languages cognate) with a mean of 2.3 ± 1.1. The distribution of cognate class size was positively skewed − many more small groups than large ones − as predicted by their hypothesis of variant decay rates.[10] Words were then grouped by their generalized worldwide frequency of use, part of speech, and previously estimated rate of replacement. Cognate class size was positively correlated with estimated replacement rate (r=0.43, p<0.001). Generalized frequency combined with part of speech was also a strong predictor of class size (r=0.48, p<0.001). Pagel et al. conclude "This result suggests that, consistent with their short estimated half-lives, infrequently used words typically do not exist long enough to be deeply ancestral, but that above the threshold frequency words gain greater stability, which then translates into larger cognate class sizes."[11]

Twenty-three word meanings[A] had cognate class sizes of four or more.[11] Words used more than once per 1,000 spoken words (χ2=24.29, P<0.001), pronouns (χ2=26.1, P<0.0001), and adverbs (χ2=14.5, P=0.003) were over-representing among those 23 words. Frequently used words, controlled for part of speech, were 7.5 times more likely (P<0.001) than infrequently used words to be judged as cognate. These findings matched their a priori predictions about word classes more likes to retain sound and meaning over long periods of time.[clarification needed] [12] The authors write "Our ability to predict these words independently of their sound correspondences dilutes the usual criticisms leveled at such long-range linguistic reconstructions, that proto-words are unreliable or inaccurate, or that apparent phonetic similarities among them reflect chance sound resemblances." On the first point, they argue that inaccurate reconstructions should weaken, not enhance, the signals. On the second, they argue that chance resemblances should be equally common across all word usage frequencies, in contrast to what the data shows.[13]

The team then created a Markov chain Monte Carlo simulation to estimate and date the phylogenetic trees of the seven language families under examination. Five separate runs produced the same (unrooted) tree, with three sets of language families: an eastern grouping of Altaic, Inuit–Yupik, and Chukchi–Kamchatkan; a central and southern Asia grouping of Kartvelian and Dravidian; and a northern and western European grouping of Indo-European and Uralic.[12] Two rootings were considered, using established age estimates for Proto-Indo-European and Proto-Chukchi–Kamchatkan as calibration.[14] The first roots the tree to the midpoint of the branch leading to proto-Dravidian and yields an estimated origin for Eurasiatic of 14450 ± 1750 years ago. The second roots the tree to the proto-Kartvelian branch and yields 15610 ± 2290 years ago. Internal nodes have less certainty, but exceed chance expectations, and do not affect the top-level age estimate. The authors conclude "All inferred ages must be treated with caution but our estimates are consistent with proposals linking the near concomitant spread of the language families that comprise this group to the retreat of glaciers in Eurasia at the end of the last ice age ~15,000 years ago."[12]

Many academics specializing in historical linguistics via the comparative method are skeptical of the conclusions of the paper, and critical of its assumptions and methodology.[15] Writing on University of Pennsylvania blog Language Log, Sarah Thomason questions the accuracy of the LWED data on which the paper was based. She notes that LWED lists multiple possible proto-word reconstructions for most words, increasing the possibility of chance matches.[16] Pagel et al.. anticipated this criticism and state that since infrequently used words generally have more proposed reconstructions, such errors should "produce a bias in the opposite direction" of what the statistics actual shows (i.e. that infrequently used words should have larger cognate groups if chance alone was the source).[17] Thomason also argues that since the LWED is contributed to primarily by believers in Nostratic, a proposed superfamily even broader than Eurasiatic, the data is likely to be biased towards proto-words that can be judged cognate.[16] Pagel et al.. admit they "cannot rule out this bias" but say they think it is unlikely bias has systematically impacted their results. They argue certain word types generally believed to be long lived (e.g. numbers) do not appear on their 23 word list, while other words of relatively low importance in modern society, but important to ancient people do appear on the list (e.g. bark and ashes), thus casting doubt on bias being the cause of the apparent cognates.[13] Thomason says she is "unqualified" to comment on the statistics themselves, but says any model that uses bad data as input cannot provide reliable results.[16]

Asya Pereltsvaig takes a different approach to her critique of the paper. Outlining the history (in English) of several of the words on the Pagel list, she concludes it is impossible that such words could have retained any sound and meaning pairings from 15,000 years ago given how much they have changed in the 1,500 or so year attested history of English. She also states that the authors are "looking in the wrong place" to begin with since "grammatical properties are more reliable than words as indicators of familial relationships".[18]

Pagel et al. also examined two other possible objections to their conclusions. They rule out linguistic borrowing as a significant factor in the results on the basis that for a word to appear cognate in many language families solely because of borrowing would require frequent swapping back and forth. This is deemed unlikely because of the large geographical area covered by the language groups and because frequently used words are the least likely to be borrowed in modern times.[13] Finally, they state that leaving aside closed class words with simple phonologies (e.g. I and we) does not affect their conclusions.[19]

Classification edit

According to Greenberg, the language family that Eurasiatic is most closely connected to is Amerind. He states that "the Eurasiatic-Amerind family represents a relatively recent expansion (circa 15,000 years ago) into territory opened up by the melting of the Arctic ice cap".[20] In contrast, "Eurasiatic-Amerind stands apart from the other families of the Old World, among which the differences are much greater and represent deeper chronological groupings". Like Eurasiatic, Amerind is not a generally accepted proposal.[21]

Eurasiatic and another proposed macrofamily, Nostratic, often include many of the same language families. Vladislav Illich-Svitych's Nostratic dictionary did not include the smaller Siberian language families listed in Eurasiatic, but this was only because protolanguages had not been reconstructed for them; Nostraticists have not attempted to exclude these languages from Nostratic. Many Nostratic theorists have accepted Eurasiatic as a subgroup within Nostratic alongside Afroasiatic, Kartvelian, and Dravidian.[22] LWED likewise views Eurasiatic as a subfamily of Nostratic.[3] The Nostratic family is not endorsed by the mainstream of comparative linguistics.

Harold C. Fleming includes Eurasiatic as a subgroup of the hypothetical Borean family.[23]

Subdivisions edit

 
Eurasiatic family tree in order of first attestation

The subdivisioning of Eurasiatic varies by proposal, but usually includes Turkic, Tungusic, Mongolic, Chukchi-Kamchatkan, Eskimo–Aleut, Indo-European, and Uralic.

Greenberg enumerates eight branches of Eurasiatic, as follows: Turkic, Tungusic, Mongolic, Chukchi-Kamchatkan, Eskimo–Aleut, Etruscan, Indo-European, "Korean-Japanese-Ainu", Nivkh, and Uralic–Yukaghir.[24] He then breaks these families into smaller sub-groups, some of which are themselves not widely accepted as phylogenetic groupings.

Pagel et al. use a slightly different branching, listing seven language families: Mongolic, Tungusic, Turkic, Chukchi-Kamchatkan, Dravidian, "Inuit-Yupik"—which is a name giving to LWED grouping of Inuit (Eskimo) languages that does not include Aleut[clarification needed] —Indo-European, Kartvelian, and Uralic.[5]

Murray Gell-Mann, Ilia Peiros, and Georgiy Starostin group Chukotko-Kamchatkan and Nivkh with Almosan instead of Eurasiatic.[25]

Regardless of version, these lists cover the languages spoken in most of Europe, Central and Northern Asia and (in the case of Eskimo-Aleut) on either side of the Bering Strait.

The branching of Eurasiatic is roughly (following Greenberg):

  • Eurasiatic
    • Indo-European (unity undisputed)
    • Uralic (unity undisputed)
    • Yukaghir (sometimes grouped under Paleosiberian, linked to Uralic by Greenberg)
    • Paleosiberian (sub-grouping almost universally considered a term of convenience; members are considered Eurasiatic regardless of subgroup's validity)
    • Macro-Altaic (controversial; Roy Andrew Miller 1971, Gustaf John Ramstedt 1952, Matthias Castrén 1844; "Macro-Altaic" has less scholarly acceptance than "Micro-Altaic" or Altaic proper, uniting only Turkic, Mongolic and Tungusic)
    • Ainu (considered an isolate by almost all save Greenberg)
    • Tyrsenian (grouping of three closely related extinct languages; their affiliation with Eurasiatic, based primarily on "mi" first person singular, is highly speculative given lack of attestation)


Jäger (2015) edit

A computational phylogenetic analysis Jäger (2015) provided the following phylogeny of language families in Eurasia:[26]

Geographical distribution edit

Merritt Ruhlen suggests that the geographical distribution of Eurasiatic shows that it and the Dené–Caucasian family are the result of separate migrations. Dené–Caucasian is the older of the two groups, with the emergence of Eurasiatic being more recent. The Eurasiatic expansion overwhelmed Dené–Caucasian, leaving speakers of the latter restricted mainly to isolated pockets (the Basques in the Pyrenees Mountains, Caucasian peoples in the Caucasus Mountains, and the Burushaski in the Hindu Kush Mountains) surrounded by Eurasiatic speakers. Dené–Caucasian survived in these areas because they were difficult to access and therefore easy to defend; the reasons for its survival elsewhere are unclear. Ruhlen argues that Eurasiatic is supported by stronger and clearer evidence than Dené–Caucasian, and that this also indicates that the spread of Dené–Caucasian occurred before that of Eurasiatic.[27]

The existence of a Dené–Caucasian family is disputed or rejected by some linguists, including Lyle Campbell,[28] Ives Goddard,[29] and Larry Trask.[30]

The last common ancestor of the family[vague] was estimated by phylogenetic analysis of ultraconserved words at roughly 15,000 years old, suggesting that these languages spread from a "refuge" area at the Last Glacial Maximum.[12]

See also edit

Notes edit

^A The 23 words are (listed in order of cognate class size): Thou (7 cognates), I (6), Not, That, To give, We, Who (5), Ashes, Bark, Black, Fire, Hand, Male/man, Mother, Old, This, To flow, To hear, To pull, To spit, What, Worm, Ye (4)[12]

  1. ^ Evolution of Human Languages (March 2014).
  2. ^ Ruhlen, p. 70
  3. ^ a b c Pagel et al.. (SI), p. 1
  4. ^ Georg and Vovin, p. 335
  5. ^ a b c d Pagel et al., p. 1
  6. ^ Georg and Vovin, p. 334
  7. ^ Georg and Vovin, p. 336
  8. ^ Paliga, Sorin (2003). N. D. Andreev's Proto-Boreal Theory and Its Implications in Understanding the Central-East and Southeast European Ethnogenesis: Slavic, Baltic and Thracian. Romanoslavica 38: 93–104. Papers and articles for the 13th International Congress of Slavicists, Ljubljana, August 15–21, 2003.
  9. ^ Paliga, Sorin (2007). Lexicon Proto-Borealicum et alia lexica etymologica minora. Evenimentul. doi:10.13140/2.1.4932.0009. ISBN 978-973-87920-3-6.
  10. ^ a b Pagel et al., p. 2
  11. ^ a b Pagel et al., p. 3
  12. ^ a b c d e Pagel et al., p. 4
  13. ^ a b c Pagel et al., p. 5
  14. ^ Pagel et al.. (SI), pp. 2-3
  15. ^ Heggarty, P. (5 August 2013). "Ultraconserved words and Eurasiatic? The "faces in the fire" of language prehistory". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 110 (35): E3254. Bibcode:2013PNAS..110E3254H. doi:10.1073/pnas.1309114110. PMC 3761561. PMID 23918403.
  16. ^ a b c Thomason
  17. ^ Pagel et al.. (SI), pp. 3-4
  18. ^ Pereltsvaig
  19. ^ Pagel et al., p. 6
  20. ^ Greenberg (2002), p. 2
  21. ^ Campbell; Goddard; Mithun
  22. ^ Greenberg (2005), p. 331
  23. ^ Fleming
  24. ^ Greenberg (2000), p. 279-81
  25. ^ Gell-Mann et al., pp. 13–30
  26. ^ Jäger, Gerhard (2015). "Statistical support for linguistic macrofamilies". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 112 (41): 12752–12757. Bibcode:2015PNAS..11212752J. doi:10.1073/pnas.1500331112. PMC 4611657. PMID 26403857.
  27. ^ Ruhlen
  28. ^ Campbell, pp. 286-288
  29. ^ Goddard, p. 318
  30. ^ Trask, p. 85

References edit

  • Campbell, Lyle (1997). American Indian Languages: The Historical Linguistics of Native America. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Gell-Mann, Murray; Ilia Peiros; George Starostin (2009). "Distant Language Relationship: The Current Perspective" (PDF). Journal of Language Relationship (1).
  • Harold Fleming. . Archived from the original on 2007-06-09. Retrieved 2010-12-20.
  • Georg, Stefan; Alexander Vovin (2003). "From mass comparison to mess comparison: Greenberg's 'Eurasiatic' theory". Diachronica. 20 (2). doi:10.1075/dia.20.2.06geo.
  • Goddard, Ives (1996). ""The Classification of the Native Languages of North America"". In William Sturtevant (ed.). Languages (Handbook of North American Indians Vol. 17). Washington, D.C.: Smithsonian Institution.
  • Greenberg, Joseph H. 1957. Essays in Linguistics. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
  • Greenberg, Joseph H. 2000. Indo-European and Its Closest Relatives: The Eurasiatic Language Family. Volume 1, Grammar. Stanford: Stanford University Press.
  • Greenberg, Joseph H. 2002. Indo-European and Its Closest Relatives: The Eurasiatic Language Family. Volume 2, Lexicon. Stanford: Stanford University Press.
  • Greenberg, Joseph H. 2005. Genetic Linguistics: Essays on Theory and Method, edited by William Croft. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Mithun, Marianne. 1999. The Languages of Native North America. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Nichols, Johanna. 1992. Linguistic Diversity in Space and Time. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
  • Pagel, M.; Atkinson, Q. D.; S. Calude, A.; Meade, A. (6 May 2013). "Ultraconserved words point to deep language ancestry across Eurasia". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 110 (21): 8471–8476. Bibcode:2013PNAS..110.8471P. doi:10.1073/pnas.1218726110. PMC 3666749. PMID 23650390.
  • Pagel, M.; Atkinson, Q. D.; S. Calude, A.; Meade, A. (6 May 2013). "Ultraconserved words point to deep language ancestry across Eurasia". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 110 (21): 8471–8476. Bibcode:2013PNAS..110.8471P. doi:10.1073/pnas.1218726110. PMC 3666749. PMID 23650390.
  • Ruhlen, Merritt (1994). The Origin of Language: Tracing the Evolution of the Mother Tongue. New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
  • Pereltsvaig, Asya (May 10, 2013). . GeoCurrents. Archived from the original on June 7, 2013. Retrieved May 11, 2013.
  • Thomason, Sally (May 8, 2013). "Ultraconserved words? Really??". Language Log. Retrieved May 8, 2013.
  • Trask, Larry (2000). The Dictionary of Historical and Comparative Linguistics. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.

Further reading edit

  • Bancel, Pierre J.; de l'Etang, Alain Matthey. "The millennial persistence of Indo-European and Eurasiatic pronouns and the origin of nominals". In: In Hot Pursuit of Language in Prehistory: Essays in the four fields of anthropology. In honor of Harold Crane Fleming. Edited by John D. Bengtson. John Benjamins Publishing Company, 2008. pp. 439–464. https://doi.org/10.1075/z.145.32ban

External links edit

  • NorthEuraLex
  • "Indo-Uralic and Altaic" by Frederik Kortlandt (2006)
  • Regions Based on Social Structure: A Reconsideration

eurasiatic, languages, eurasiatic, proposed, language, macrofamily, that, would, include, many, language, families, historically, spoken, northern, western, southern, eurasia, eurasiaticgeographicdistributionbefore, 16th, century, most, siberia, western, euras. Eurasiatic 1 is a proposed language macrofamily that would include many language families historically spoken in northern western and southern Eurasia EurasiaticGeographicdistributionBefore the 16th century most of Siberia and Western Eurasia today worldwideLinguistic classificationNostratic EurasiaticProto languageProto EurasiaticSubdivisionsIndo European Uralic Yukaghir Altaic Turkic Mongolic and Tungusic Chukotko Kamchatkan Eskimo Aleut Korean Japanese Ainu sometimes included Nivkh sometimes included Etruscan sometimes included Algic sometimes included Wakashan rarely included GlottologNoneThe worldwide distribution of the Eurasiatic macrofamily of languages according to Pagel et al The idea of a Eurasiatic superfamily dates back more than 100 years Joseph Greenberg s proposal dating to the 1990s is the most widely discussed version In 2013 Mark Pagel and three colleagues published what they believe to be statistical evidence for a Eurasiatic language family The branches of Eurasiatic vary between proposals but typically include Altaic Mongolic Tungusic and Turkic Chukchi Kamchatkan Eskimo Aleut Indo European and Uralic although Greenberg uses the controversial Uralic Yukaghir classification instead Other branches sometimes included are the Kartvelian and Dravidian families as proposed by Pagel et al in addition to the language isolates Nivkh Etruscan and Greenberg s Korean Japanese Ainu Some proposals group Eurasiatic with even larger macrofamilies such as Nostratic again many other professional linguists regard the methods used as invalid Contents 1 History of the concept 1 1 Pagel et al 2 Classification 2 1 Subdivisions 2 2 Jager 2015 3 Geographical distribution 4 See also 5 Notes 6 References 7 Further reading 8 External linksHistory of the concept editIn 1994 Merritt Ruhlen claimed Eurasiatic is supported by the existence of a grammatical pattern whereby plurals of nouns are formed by suffixing t to the noun root whereas duals of nouns are formed by suffixing k Rasmus Rask noted this grammatical pattern in the groups now called Uralic and Eskimo Aleut as early as 1818 but it can also be found in Tungusic Nivkh also called Gilyak and Chukchi Kamchatkan all of which Greenberg placed in Eurasiatic According to Ruhlen this pattern is not found in language families or languages outside Eurasiatic 2 In 1998 Joseph Greenberg extended his work in mass comparison a methodology he first proposed in the 1950s to categorize the languages of Africa to suggest a Eurasiatic language 3 In 2000 he expanded his argument for Eurasiatic into a full length book Indo European and Its Closest Relatives The Eurasiatic Language Family in which he outlines both phonetic and grammatical evidence that he feels demonstrate the validity of language family The heart of his argument is 72 morphological features that he judges as common across the various language families he examines 4 Of the many variant proposals Greenberg s has attracted the most academic attention 3 Greenberg s Eurasiatic hypothesis has been dismissed by many linguists often on the ground that his research on mass comparison is unreliable The primary criticism of comparative methods is that cognates are assumed to have a common origin on the basis of similar sounds and word meanings It is generally assumed that semantic and phonetic corruption destroys any trace of original sound and meaning within 5 000 to 9 000 years making the application of comparative methods to ancient superfamilies highly questionable Additionally apparent cognates can arise by chance or from loan words Without the existence of statistical estimates of chance collisions conclusions based on comparison alone are thus viewed as doubtful 5 Stefan Georg and Alexander Vovin who unlike many of their colleagues do not stipulate a priori that attempts to find ancient relationships are bound to fail examined Greenberg s claims in detail 6 They state that Greenberg s morphological arguments are the correct approach to determining families but doubt his conclusions They write Greenberg s 72 morphemes look like massive evidence in favour of Eurasiatic at first glance If valid few linguists would have the right to doubt that a point has been made However closer inspection shows too many misinterpretations errors and wrong analyses these allow no other judgement than that Greenberg s attempt to demonstrate the validity of his Eurasiatic has failed 7 In the 1980s Russian linguist Nikolai Dmitrievich Andreev ru s Boreal languages ru hypothesis Russian Borealnyj yazyk linking the Indo European Uralic and Altaic including Korean in his later papers language families Andreev also proposed 203 lexical roots for his hypothesized Boreal macrofamily After Andreev s death in 1997 the Boreal hypothesis was further expanded by Sorin Paliga 2003 2007 8 9 Pagel et al edit In 2013 Mark Pagel Quentin D Atkinson Andreea S Calude and Andrew Meade published statistical evidence that attempts to overcome these objections According to their earlier work most words exhibit a half life of between 2 000 and 4 000 years consistent with existing theories of linguistic replacement However they also identified some words numerals pronouns and certain adverbs that exhibit a much slower rate of replacement with half lives of 10 000 to 20 000 or more years Drawing from research in a diverse group of modern languages the authors were able to show the same slow replacement rates for key words regardless of current pronunciation They conclude that a stable core of largely unchanging words is a common feature of all human discourse and model replacement as inversely proportional to usage frequency 5 Pagel et al used hypothesized reconstructions of proto words from seven language families listed in the Languages of the World Etymological Database LWED 5 They limited their search to the 200 most common words as described by the Swadesh fundamental vocabulary list Twelve words were excluded because proto words had been proposed for two or fewer language families The remaining 188 words yielded 3804 different reconstructions sometimes with multiple constructions for a given family In contrast to traditional comparative linguistics the researchers did not attempt to prove any given pairing as cognates based on similar sounds but rather treated each pairing as a binary random variable subject to error The set of possible cognate pairings was then analyzed as a whole for predictable regularities 10 Words were separated into groupings based on how many language families appeared to be cognate for the word Among the 188 words cognate groups ranged from 1 no cognates to 7 all languages cognate with a mean of 2 3 1 1 The distribution of cognate class size was positively skewed many more small groups than large ones as predicted by their hypothesis of variant decay rates 10 Words were then grouped by their generalized worldwide frequency of use part of speech and previously estimated rate of replacement Cognate class size was positively correlated with estimated replacement rate r 0 43 p lt 0 001 Generalized frequency combined with part of speech was also a strong predictor of class size r 0 48 p lt 0 001 Pagel et al conclude This result suggests that consistent with their short estimated half lives infrequently used words typically do not exist long enough to be deeply ancestral but that above the threshold frequency words gain greater stability which then translates into larger cognate class sizes 11 Twenty three word meanings A had cognate class sizes of four or more 11 Words used more than once per 1 000 spoken words x2 24 29 P lt 0 001 pronouns x2 26 1 P lt 0 0001 and adverbs x2 14 5 P 0 003 were over representing among those 23 words Frequently used words controlled for part of speech were 7 5 times more likely P lt 0 001 than infrequently used words to be judged as cognate These findings matched their a priori predictions about word classes more likes to retain sound and meaning over long periods of time clarification needed 12 The authors write Our ability to predict these words independently of their sound correspondences dilutes the usual criticisms leveled at such long range linguistic reconstructions that proto words are unreliable or inaccurate or that apparent phonetic similarities among them reflect chance sound resemblances On the first point they argue that inaccurate reconstructions should weaken not enhance the signals On the second they argue that chance resemblances should be equally common across all word usage frequencies in contrast to what the data shows 13 The team then created a Markov chain Monte Carlo simulation to estimate and date the phylogenetic trees of the seven language families under examination Five separate runs produced the same unrooted tree with three sets of language families an eastern grouping of Altaic Inuit Yupik and Chukchi Kamchatkan a central and southern Asia grouping of Kartvelian and Dravidian and a northern and western European grouping of Indo European and Uralic 12 Two rootings were considered using established age estimates for Proto Indo European and Proto Chukchi Kamchatkan as calibration 14 The first roots the tree to the midpoint of the branch leading to proto Dravidian and yields an estimated origin for Eurasiatic of 14450 1750 years ago The second roots the tree to the proto Kartvelian branch and yields 15610 2290 years ago Internal nodes have less certainty but exceed chance expectations and do not affect the top level age estimate The authors conclude All inferred ages must be treated with caution but our estimates are consistent with proposals linking the near concomitant spread of the language families that comprise this group to the retreat of glaciers in Eurasia at the end of the last ice age 15 000 years ago 12 Many academics specializing in historical linguistics via the comparative method are skeptical of the conclusions of the paper and critical of its assumptions and methodology 15 Writing on University of Pennsylvania blog Language Log Sarah Thomason questions the accuracy of the LWED data on which the paper was based She notes that LWED lists multiple possible proto word reconstructions for most words increasing the possibility of chance matches 16 Pagel et al anticipated this criticism and state that since infrequently used words generally have more proposed reconstructions such errors should produce a bias in the opposite direction of what the statistics actual shows i e that infrequently used words should have larger cognate groups if chance alone was the source 17 Thomason also argues that since the LWED is contributed to primarily by believers in Nostratic a proposed superfamily even broader than Eurasiatic the data is likely to be biased towards proto words that can be judged cognate 16 Pagel et al admit they cannot rule out this bias but say they think it is unlikely bias has systematically impacted their results They argue certain word types generally believed to be long lived e g numbers do not appear on their 23 word list while other words of relatively low importance in modern society but important to ancient people do appear on the list e g bark and ashes thus casting doubt on bias being the cause of the apparent cognates 13 Thomason says she is unqualified to comment on the statistics themselves but says any model that uses bad data as input cannot provide reliable results 16 Asya Pereltsvaig takes a different approach to her critique of the paper Outlining the history in English of several of the words on the Pagel list she concludes it is impossible that such words could have retained any sound and meaning pairings from 15 000 years ago given how much they have changed in the 1 500 or so year attested history of English She also states that the authors are looking in the wrong place to begin with since grammatical properties are more reliable than words as indicators of familial relationships 18 Pagel et al also examined two other possible objections to their conclusions They rule out linguistic borrowing as a significant factor in the results on the basis that for a word to appear cognate in many language families solely because of borrowing would require frequent swapping back and forth This is deemed unlikely because of the large geographical area covered by the language groups and because frequently used words are the least likely to be borrowed in modern times 13 Finally they state that leaving aside closed class words with simple phonologies e g I and we does not affect their conclusions 19 Classification editAccording to Greenberg the language family that Eurasiatic is most closely connected to is Amerind He states that the Eurasiatic Amerind family represents a relatively recent expansion circa 15 000 years ago into territory opened up by the melting of the Arctic ice cap 20 In contrast Eurasiatic Amerind stands apart from the other families of the Old World among which the differences are much greater and represent deeper chronological groupings Like Eurasiatic Amerind is not a generally accepted proposal 21 Eurasiatic and another proposed macrofamily Nostratic often include many of the same language families Vladislav Illich Svitych s Nostratic dictionary did not include the smaller Siberian language families listed in Eurasiatic but this was only because protolanguages had not been reconstructed for them Nostraticists have not attempted to exclude these languages from Nostratic Many Nostratic theorists have accepted Eurasiatic as a subgroup within Nostratic alongside Afroasiatic Kartvelian and Dravidian 22 LWED likewise views Eurasiatic as a subfamily of Nostratic 3 The Nostratic family is not endorsed by the mainstream of comparative linguistics Harold C Fleming includes Eurasiatic as a subgroup of the hypothetical Borean family 23 Subdivisions edit nbsp Eurasiatic family tree in order of first attestationThe subdivisioning of Eurasiatic varies by proposal but usually includes Turkic Tungusic Mongolic Chukchi Kamchatkan Eskimo Aleut Indo European and Uralic Greenberg enumerates eight branches of Eurasiatic as follows Turkic Tungusic Mongolic Chukchi Kamchatkan Eskimo Aleut Etruscan Indo European Korean Japanese Ainu Nivkh and Uralic Yukaghir 24 He then breaks these families into smaller sub groups some of which are themselves not widely accepted as phylogenetic groupings Pagel et al use a slightly different branching listing seven language families Mongolic Tungusic Turkic Chukchi Kamchatkan Dravidian Inuit Yupik which is a name giving to LWED grouping of Inuit Eskimo languages that does not include Aleut clarification needed Indo European Kartvelian and Uralic 5 Murray Gell Mann Ilia Peiros and Georgiy Starostin group Chukotko Kamchatkan and Nivkh with Almosan instead of Eurasiatic 25 Regardless of version these lists cover the languages spoken in most of Europe Central and Northern Asia and in the case of Eskimo Aleut on either side of the Bering Strait The branching of Eurasiatic is roughly following Greenberg Eurasiatic Indo European unity undisputed Uralic unity undisputed Yukaghir sometimes grouped under Paleosiberian linked to Uralic by Greenberg Paleosiberian sub grouping almost universally considered a term of convenience members are considered Eurasiatic regardless of subgroup s validity Nivkh a language isolate placed under Palaeo Siberian by Starostin not always included even within Nostratic Eskimo Aleut unity undisputed Chukotko Kamchatkan Chukotian unity undisputed Macro Altaic controversial Roy Andrew Miller 1971 Gustaf John Ramstedt 1952 Matthias Castren 1844 Macro Altaic has less scholarly acceptance than Micro Altaic or Altaic proper uniting only Turkic Mongolic and Tungusic Micro Altaic unity disputed Turkic unity undisputed Mongolic unity undisputed Tungusic unity undisputed Japonic unity undisputed Japanese Ryukyuan Korean language isolate Ainu considered an isolate by almost all save Greenberg Tyrsenian grouping of three closely related extinct languages their affiliation with Eurasiatic based primarily on mi first person singular is highly speculative given lack of attestation Etruscan extinct Raetic extinct Lemnian extinct Jager 2015 edit A computational phylogenetic analysis Jager 2015 provided the following phylogeny of language families in Eurasia 26 YeniseianDravidianNakh DaghestanianAustroasiaticJaponicAinuSino TibetanHmong MienAustro Tai AustronesianTai Kadai Eurasiatic TungusicMongolicTurkicYukaghirNivkhUralicChukotko KamchatkanIndo EuropeanGeographical distribution editMerritt Ruhlen suggests that the geographical distribution of Eurasiatic shows that it and the Dene Caucasian family are the result of separate migrations Dene Caucasian is the older of the two groups with the emergence of Eurasiatic being more recent The Eurasiatic expansion overwhelmed Dene Caucasian leaving speakers of the latter restricted mainly to isolated pockets the Basques in the Pyrenees Mountains Caucasian peoples in the Caucasus Mountains and the Burushaski in the Hindu Kush Mountains surrounded by Eurasiatic speakers Dene Caucasian survived in these areas because they were difficult to access and therefore easy to defend the reasons for its survival elsewhere are unclear Ruhlen argues that Eurasiatic is supported by stronger and clearer evidence than Dene Caucasian and that this also indicates that the spread of Dene Caucasian occurred before that of Eurasiatic 27 The existence of a Dene Caucasian family is disputed or rejected by some linguists including Lyle Campbell 28 Ives Goddard 29 and Larry Trask 30 The last common ancestor of the family vague was estimated by phylogenetic analysis of ultraconserved words at roughly 15 000 years old suggesting that these languages spread from a refuge area at the Last Glacial Maximum 12 See also editAncient North Eurasian Comparative mythology Indo Semitic languages Indo Uralic languages Nostratic languages Proto Human language Ural Altaic languages Uralo Siberian languagesNotes edit A The 23 words are listed in order of cognate class size Thou 7 cognates I 6 Not That To give We Who 5 Ashes Bark Black Fire Hand Male man Mother Old This To flow To hear To pull To spit What Worm Ye 4 12 Evolution of Human Languages March 2014 Ruhlen p 70 a b c Pagel et al SI p 1 Georg and Vovin p 335 a b c d Pagel et al p 1 Georg and Vovin p 334 Georg and Vovin p 336 Paliga Sorin 2003 N D Andreev s Proto Boreal Theory and Its Implications in Understanding the Central East and Southeast European Ethnogenesis Slavic Baltic and Thracian Romanoslavica 38 93 104 Papers and articles for the 13th International Congress of Slavicists Ljubljana August 15 21 2003 Paliga Sorin 2007 Lexicon Proto Borealicum et alia lexica etymologica minora Evenimentul doi 10 13140 2 1 4932 0009 ISBN 978 973 87920 3 6 a b Pagel et al p 2 a b Pagel et al p 3 a b c d e Pagel et al p 4 a b c Pagel et al p 5 Pagel et al SI pp 2 3 Heggarty P 5 August 2013 Ultraconserved words and Eurasiatic The faces in the fire of language prehistory Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 110 35 E3254 Bibcode 2013PNAS 110E3254H doi 10 1073 pnas 1309114110 PMC 3761561 PMID 23918403 a b c Thomason Pagel et al SI pp 3 4 Pereltsvaig Pagel et al p 6 Greenberg 2002 p 2 Campbell Goddard Mithun Greenberg 2005 p 331 Fleming Greenberg 2000 p 279 81 Gell Mann et al pp 13 30 Jager Gerhard 2015 Statistical support for linguistic macrofamilies Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 112 41 12752 12757 Bibcode 2015PNAS 11212752J doi 10 1073 pnas 1500331112 PMC 4611657 PMID 26403857 Ruhlen Campbell pp 286 288 Goddard p 318 Trask p 85References editCampbell Lyle 1997 American Indian Languages The Historical Linguistics of Native America Oxford Oxford University Press Gell Mann Murray Ilia Peiros George Starostin 2009 Distant Language Relationship The Current Perspective PDF Journal of Language Relationship 1 Harold Fleming Afrasian and Its Closest Relatives the Borean Hypothesis Archived from the original on 2007 06 09 Retrieved 2010 12 20 Georg Stefan Alexander Vovin 2003 From mass comparison to mess comparison Greenberg s Eurasiatic theory Diachronica 20 2 doi 10 1075 dia 20 2 06geo Goddard Ives 1996 The Classification of the Native Languages of North America In William Sturtevant ed Languages Handbook of North American Indians Vol 17 Washington D C Smithsonian Institution Greenberg Joseph H 1957 Essays in Linguistics Chicago University of Chicago Press Greenberg Joseph H 2000 Indo European and Its Closest Relatives The Eurasiatic Language Family Volume 1 Grammar Stanford Stanford University Press Greenberg Joseph H 2002 Indo European and Its Closest Relatives The Eurasiatic Language Family Volume 2 Lexicon Stanford Stanford University Press Greenberg Joseph H 2005 Genetic Linguistics Essays on Theory and Method edited by William Croft Oxford Oxford University Press Mithun Marianne 1999 The Languages of Native North America Cambridge Cambridge University Press Nichols Johanna 1992 Linguistic Diversity in Space and Time Chicago University of Chicago Press Pagel M Atkinson Q D S Calude A Meade A 6 May 2013 Ultraconserved words point to deep language ancestry across Eurasia Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 110 21 8471 8476 Bibcode 2013PNAS 110 8471P doi 10 1073 pnas 1218726110 PMC 3666749 PMID 23650390 Pagel M Atkinson Q D S Calude A Meade A 6 May 2013 Ultraconserved words point to deep language ancestry across Eurasia Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 110 21 8471 8476 Bibcode 2013PNAS 110 8471P doi 10 1073 pnas 1218726110 PMC 3666749 PMID 23650390 Ruhlen Merritt 1994 The Origin of Language Tracing the Evolution of the Mother Tongue New York John Wiley amp Sons Inc Pereltsvaig Asya May 10 2013 Do Ultraconserved Words Reveal Linguistic Macro Families GeoCurrents Archived from the original on June 7 2013 Retrieved May 11 2013 Thomason Sally May 8 2013 Ultraconserved words Really Language Log Retrieved May 8 2013 Trask Larry 2000 The Dictionary of Historical and Comparative Linguistics Edinburgh Edinburgh University Press Further reading editBancel Pierre J de l Etang Alain Matthey The millennial persistence of Indo European and Eurasiatic pronouns and the origin of nominals In In Hot Pursuit of Language in Prehistory Essays in the four fields of anthropology In honor of Harold Crane Fleming Edited by John D Bengtson John Benjamins Publishing Company 2008 pp 439 464 https doi org 10 1075 z 145 32banExternal links editNorthEuraLex Indo Uralic and Altaic by Frederik Kortlandt 2006 Regions Based on Social Structure A Reconsideration Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Eurasiatic languages amp oldid 1193806965, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.