fbpx
Wikipedia

Nawat language (typological overview)

This rather technical article provides a typological sketch of the Pipil language (also known as Nawat). Another related article outlines Pipil grammar in fuller detail. The distinctive purpose of the present article is to single out those specific features of Nawat linguistic structure that are relevant to this language's general typological classification and characterization, answering the question: What major features make this language similar to or different from other languages? Most of the assertions in this article are generalizations from information found in the Pipil grammar article.

This article is likely to be of most interest and use to readers interested in general linguistics, language typology, and related areas such as areal typology, and especially (though not exclusively) to professional linguists. Another possible use of this article is as a thumbnail sketch of the language, a checklist of its most salient points, serving as a brief guide to the fuller language description in the Pipil grammar article.

Phonology edit

Phoneme inventory edit

The Nawat phoneme inventory is smaller than that of most languages in the area. Phonemically relevant voice distinctions are generally absent: plosives are normally voiceless (though there exist some voiced allophones), as are fricatives and affricates; liquids, nasals and semivowels are normally voiced (though there exist voiceless allophones).

Other aspects edit

Syllables consist of a vowel nucleus preceded and followed by a maximum of one consonant: (C)V(C). Word stress is normally phonologically determined, and rarely distinctive.

Morphology edit

Inflectional and derivational morphology are of moderate complexity, with a fairly balanced mix of prefixing and suffixing mechanisms.

Nominal morphology edit

In the nominal morphology there is no inflection for case or definiteness, the morphological categories being number, state (absolute vs. construct) and person (of the possessor, with construct state).

Verbal morphology edit

Morphological verb categories
  • tense
  • mood
  • person/number of subject
  • person/number of object
  • valency decrease (unaccusative or unergative)
  • valency increase (causative, applicative)
  • direction (towards the speaker)

In the verbal morphology, tense, mood and number (of the subject) are marked by suffixes, person (of the subject, and of the object with transitive verbs) by prefixes. Prefixes can also reduce a transitive verb's argument valency, making it either unaccusative (ta-) or unergative (mu-). A further prefix ((w)al-) adds a directional meaning component (roughly 'towards the speaker') to some verbs, though no longer productively. Derivational suffixes can add to a verb's valency, acting as causatives (which add a new subject as agent) or applicatives (which add a new object as recipient). A few cases of paradigmatic suppletion occur (witz 'comes' : walaj 'came'; ki-uni 'he drinks it (transitive)' : ati 'he drinks (unaccusative)'.

Syntax edit

Word order edit

Word order
  • flexible VO
  • determiner + head
  • quantifier + head
  • adjective + head / head + adjective
  • head + possessor
  • head + relative clause
  • preposition + head

Sentence word order shows considerable flexibility. Pronominal arguments (which may represent subject or object) usually precede the verb, and are only used for emphasis: compare Naja nikita 'I see him/her/it' and Naja nechita 'He/she/it sees me' (naja 'I, me').

Within the noun phrase, determiners and quantifiers precede the head. Adjectives may either precede or follow the head noun. Possessors follow possessed, and relative clauses follow their head. There are prepositional structures.

Head or dependent marking edit

HEAD-marking constructions
  • VERB + subject / subject + VERB
  • VERB + object
  • POSSESSED + possessor
  • RELATIONAL + noun (see below)

There is pro-drop for both subject and object (i.e. subject and object pronouns are omitted in unmarked sentences). One or two arguments of the verb are indexed for person and number in the verb (one when intransitive, two when transitive). A maximum of one object index is possible. If a transitive verb has both a patient ('direct object' in English grammar) and a recipient ('indirect object'), it is the latter which is indexed as object of the Nawat verb, e.g. Nechmaka at 'He gives me water', where nech- indexes a first-person-singular object. There is no marking on the arguments to indicate the various semantic relations so far mentioned. This is a head-marking structure, meaning that relations between a head and dependent are marked on the head (here, the verb) and not on the dependent (here, the subject or object). This is also illustrated by the above examples Naja nikita 'I see him' vs. Naja nechita 'He sees me', where the 'case' of the pronoun naja is determined by the indexing on the verb.

Head-marking patterns are found in other parts of Nawat syntax too, so the language shows consistency for this typological feature. The canonical expression of possessive or genitive relations is parallel: the head (i.e. the possessed) is marked with indices indicating the person of the dependent (the possessor), the noun phrase expressing which is either omitted normally if pronominal (a pro-drop phenomenon) or occurs as a noun phrase following the head and unmarked for the possessive relation, e.g. nu-nan 'my mother', i-nan 'his mother', i-nan ne kunet 'the child's mother'.

Argument valency restrictions edit

Another kind of typological consistency between verbal and nominal constructions is seen in the fact that just as verbs may be classified as intransitives, which do not require an object, and transitives, which require one, so some nouns in Nawat need not have a possessor while others must have one.

Some nouns change their form depending on whether they have a possessor or not, alternating between absolute and construct states, such as absolute kunet, construct -kunew 'child'; this is reminiscent of how verbs may change their forms depending on their transitivity (i.e. whether they take an object or not), e.g. intransitive waki, transitive -watza 'dry', transitive miki 'die', transitive -miktia 'kill', etc. Once again, it is changes in the head that determine the nature of the grammatical relation between the head and its dependent(s).

Adpositions edit

Other relations between a verb and its noun phrase complements or adjuncts are expressed by means of a small number of prepositions or relational constructions. The relational construction itself, e.g. nu-wan 'with me', i-wan 'with him/her/it/', i-wan ne kunet 'with the child', is internally just like possessive constructions, with wan playing the same role as a construct noun.

Predication and sentence types edit

There is no general copula; instead, a nominal (or other non-verbal) predicate with no verbal component may be made a clause's grammatical nucleus. Some of these take subject indices just like verbal predicates, but tense can only be expressed periphrastically in such sentences.

Negation is achieved by placing a negative particle in front of the predicate. Yes–no questions have no special grammatical marking, while wh-questions are identified by the presence of a question word, which usually precedes the verb (or other predicate).

Subordinate clauses are either introduced by a subordinator in clause-initial position or else are juxtaposed with no subordinating conjunction.

See also edit

Bibliography edit

  • Campbell, Lyle (1985). The Pipil language of El Salvador. Mouton Grammar Library (No. 1). Berlin: Mouton Publishers. ISBN 0-89925-040-8 (U.S.), ISBN 3-11-010344-3.
  • Campbell, Lyle, Terrence Kaufman and Thomas C. Smith-Stark (1986). "Meso-America as a Linguistic Area." Language 62:3, p. 530–570.

nawat, language, typological, overview, this, article, multiple, issues, please, help, improve, discuss, these, issues, talk, page, learn, when, remove, these, template, messages, this, article, needs, additional, citations, verification, please, help, improve. This article has multiple issues Please help improve it or discuss these issues on the talk page Learn how and when to remove these template messages This article needs additional citations for verification Please help improve this article by adding citations to reliable sources Unsourced material may be challenged and removed Find sources Nawat language typological overview news newspapers books scholar JSTOR January 2008 Learn how and when to remove this template message This article may be too technical for most readers to understand Please help improve it to make it understandable to non experts without removing the technical details May 2022 Learn how and when to remove this template message Learn how and when to remove this template message This rather technical article provides a typological sketch of the Pipil language also known as Nawat Another related article outlines Pipil grammar in fuller detail The distinctive purpose of the present article is to single out those specific features of Nawat linguistic structure that are relevant to this language s general typological classification and characterization answering the question What major features make this language similar to or different from other languages Most of the assertions in this article are generalizations from information found in the Pipil grammar article This article is likely to be of most interest and use to readers interested in general linguistics language typology and related areas such as areal typology and especially though not exclusively to professional linguists Another possible use of this article is as a thumbnail sketch of the language a checklist of its most salient points serving as a brief guide to the fuller language description in the Pipil grammar article Contents 1 Phonology 1 1 Phoneme inventory 1 2 Other aspects 2 Morphology 2 1 Nominal morphology 2 2 Verbal morphology 3 Syntax 3 1 Word order 3 2 Head or dependent marking 3 3 Argument valency restrictions 3 4 Adpositions 3 5 Predication and sentence types 4 See also 5 BibliographyPhonology editPhoneme inventory edit The Nawat phoneme inventory is smaller than that of most languages in the area Phonemically relevant voice distinctions are generally absent plosives are normally voiceless though there exist some voiced allophones as are fricatives and affricates liquids nasals and semivowels are normally voiced though there exist voiceless allophones Other aspects edit Syllables consist of a vowel nucleus preceded and followed by a maximum of one consonant C V C Word stress is normally phonologically determined and rarely distinctive Morphology editInflectional and derivational morphology are of moderate complexity with a fairly balanced mix of prefixing and suffixing mechanisms Nominal morphology edit In the nominal morphology there is no inflection for case or definiteness the morphological categories being number state absolute vs construct and person of the possessor with construct state Verbal morphology edit Morphological verb categoriestense mood person number of subject person number of object valency decrease unaccusative or unergative valency increase causative applicative direction towards the speaker In the verbal morphology tense mood and number of the subject are marked by suffixes person of the subject and of the object with transitive verbs by prefixes Prefixes can also reduce a transitive verb s argument valency making it either unaccusative ta or unergative mu A further prefix w al adds a directional meaning component roughly towards the speaker to some verbs though no longer productively Derivational suffixes can add to a verb s valency acting as causatives which add a new subject as agent or applicatives which add a new object as recipient A few cases of paradigmatic suppletion occur witz comes walaj came ki uni he drinks it transitive ati he drinks unaccusative Syntax editWord order edit Word orderflexible VO determiner head quantifier head adjective head head adjective head possessor head relative clause preposition headSentence word order shows considerable flexibility Pronominal arguments which may represent subject or object usually precede the verb and are only used for emphasis compare Naja nikita I see him her it and Naja nechita He she it sees me naja I me Within the noun phrase determiners and quantifiers precede the head Adjectives may either precede or follow the head noun Possessors follow possessed and relative clauses follow their head There are prepositional structures Head or dependent marking edit HEAD marking constructionsVERB subject subject VERB VERB object POSSESSED possessor RELATIONAL noun see below There is pro drop for both subject and object i e subject and object pronouns are omitted in unmarked sentences One or two arguments of the verb are indexed for person and number in the verb one when intransitive two when transitive A maximum of one object index is possible If a transitive verb has both a patient direct object in English grammar and a recipient indirect object it is the latter which is indexed as object of the Nawat verb e g Nechmaka at He gives me water where nech indexes a first person singular object There is no marking on the arguments to indicate the various semantic relations so far mentioned This is a head marking structure meaning that relations between a head and dependent are marked on the head here the verb and not on the dependent here the subject or object This is also illustrated by the above examples Naja nikita I see him vs Naja nechita He sees me where the case of the pronoun naja is determined by the indexing on the verb Head marking patterns are found in other parts of Nawat syntax too so the language shows consistency for this typological feature The canonical expression of possessive or genitive relations is parallel the head i e the possessed is marked with indices indicating the person of the dependent the possessor the noun phrase expressing which is either omitted normally if pronominal a pro drop phenomenon or occurs as a noun phrase following the head and unmarked for the possessive relation e g nu nan my mother i nan his mother i nan ne kunet the child s mother Argument valency restrictions edit Another kind of typological consistency between verbal and nominal constructions is seen in the fact that just as verbs may be classified as intransitives which do not require an object and transitives which require one so some nouns in Nawat need not have a possessor while others must have one Some nouns change their form depending on whether they have a possessor or not alternating between absolute and construct states such as absolute kunet construct kunew child this is reminiscent of how verbs may change their forms depending on their transitivity i e whether they take an object or not e g intransitive waki transitive watza dry transitive miki die transitive miktia kill etc Once again it is changes in the head that determine the nature of the grammatical relation between the head and its dependent s Adpositions edit Other relations between a verb and its noun phrase complements or adjuncts are expressed by means of a small number of prepositions or relational constructions The relational construction itself e g nu wan with me i wan with him her it i wan ne kunet with the child is internally just like possessive constructions with wan playing the same role as a construct noun Predication and sentence types edit There is no general copula instead a nominal or other non verbal predicate with no verbal component may be made a clause s grammatical nucleus Some of these take subject indices just like verbal predicates but tense can only be expressed periphrastically in such sentences Negation is achieved by placing a negative particle in front of the predicate Yes no questions have no special grammatical marking while wh questions are identified by the presence of a question word which usually precedes the verb or other predicate Subordinate clauses are either introduced by a subordinator in clause initial position or else are juxtaposed with no subordinating conjunction See also editPipil language Pipil grammarBibliography editCampbell Lyle 1985 The Pipil language of El Salvador Mouton Grammar Library No 1 Berlin Mouton Publishers ISBN 0 89925 040 8 U S ISBN 3 11 010344 3 Campbell Lyle Terrence Kaufman and Thomas C Smith Stark 1986 Meso America as a Linguistic Area Language 62 3 p 530 570 Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Nawat language typological overview amp oldid 1165519873, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.