fbpx
Wikipedia

Mirror neuron

A mirror neuron is a neuron that fires both when an organism acts and when the organism observes the same action performed by another.[1][2][3] Thus, the neuron "mirrors" the behavior of the other, as though the observer were itself acting. Mirror neurons are not always physiologically distinct from other types of neurons in the brain; their main differentiating factor is their response patterns.[4] By this definition, such neurons have been directly observed in humans[5] and primate species,[6] and in birds.[7]

Mirror system
Identifiers
MeSHD059167
Anatomical terms of neuroanatomy
[edit on Wikidata]

In humans, brain activity consistent with that of mirror neurons has been found in the premotor cortex, the supplementary motor area, the primary somatosensory cortex, and the inferior parietal cortex.[8] The function of the mirror system in humans is a subject of much speculation. Birds have been shown to have imitative resonance behaviors and neurological evidence suggests the presence of some form of mirroring system.[6][9] To date, no widely accepted neural or computational models have been put forward to describe how mirror neuron activity supports cognitive functions.[10][11][12]

The subject of mirror neurons continues to generate intense debate. In 2014, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B published a special issue entirely devoted to mirror neuron research.[13] Some researchers speculate that mirror systems may simulate observed actions, and thus contribute to theory of mind skills,[14][15] while others relate mirror neurons to language abilities.[16] Neuroscientists such as Marco Iacoboni have argued that mirror neuron systems in the human brain help humans understand the actions and intentions of other people. In addition, Iacoboni has argued that mirror neurons are the neural basis of the human capacity for emotions such as empathy.[17]

Discovery edit

In the 1980s and 1990s, neurophysiologists Giacomo Rizzolatti, Giuseppe Di Pellegrino, Luciano Fadiga, Leonardo Fogassi, and Vittorio Gallese at the University of Parma placed electrodes in the ventral premotor cortex of the macaque monkey to study neurons specialized in the control of hand and mouth actions; for example, taking hold of an object and manipulating it. During each experiment, the researchers allowed the monkey to reach for pieces of food, and recorded from single neurons in the monkey's brain, thus measuring the neuron's response to certain movements.[18][19] They found that some neurons responded when the monkey observed a person picking up a piece of food, and also when the monkey itself picked up the food. The discovery was initially submitted to Nature, but was rejected for its "lack of general interest" before being published in a less competitive journal.[20]

A few years later, the same group published another empirical paper, discussing the role of the mirror-neuron system in action recognition, and proposing that the human Broca's area was the homologue region of the monkey ventral premotor cortex.[21] While these papers reported the presence of mirror neurons responding to hand actions, a subsequent study by Pier Francesco Ferrari and colleagues[22] described the presence of mirror neurons responding to mouth actions and facial gestures.

Further experiments confirmed that about 10% of neurons in the monkey inferior frontal and inferior parietal cortex have "mirror" properties and give similar responses to performed hand actions and observed actions. In 2002 Christian Keysers and colleagues reported that, in both humans and monkeys, the mirror system also responds to the sound of actions.[3][23][24]

Reports on mirror neurons have been widely published[21] and confirmed[25] with mirror neurons found in both inferior frontal and inferior parietal regions of the brain. Recently, evidence from functional neuroimaging strongly suggests that humans have similar mirror neurons systems: researchers have identified brain regions which respond during both action and observation of action. Not surprisingly, these brain regions include those found in the macaque monkey.[1] However, functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) can examine the entire brain at once and suggests that a much wider network of brain areas shows mirror properties in humans than previously thought. These additional areas include the somatosensory cortex and are thought to make the observer feel what it feels like to move in the observed way.[26][27]

Origin edit

Many implicitly assume that the mirrorness of mirror neurons is due primarily to heritable genetic factors and that the genetic predisposition to develop mirror neurons evolved because they facilitate action understanding.[28] In contrast, a number of theoretical accounts argue that mirror neurons could simply emerge due to learned associations, including the Hebbian Theory,[29] the Associative Learning Theory,[28] and Canalization.[30]

In monkeys edit

 
Neonatal (newborn) macaque imitating facial expressions

The first animal in which researchers have studied mirror neurons individually is the macaque monkey. In these monkeys, mirror neurons are found in the inferior frontal gyrus (region F5) and the inferior parietal lobule.[1]

Mirror neurons are believed to mediate the understanding of other animals' behaviour. For example, a mirror neuron which fires when the monkey rips a piece of paper would also fire when the monkey sees a person rip paper, or hears paper ripping (without visual cues). These properties have led researchers to believe that mirror neurons encode abstract concepts of actions like 'ripping paper', whether the action is performed by the monkey or another animal.[1]

The function of mirror neurons in macaques remains unknown. Adult macaques do not seem to learn by imitation. Recent experiments by Ferrari and colleagues suggest that infant macaques can imitate a human's face movements, though only as neonates and during a limited temporal window.[31] Even if it has not yet been empirically demonstrated, it has been proposed that mirror neurons cause this behaviour and other imitative phenomena.[32] Indeed, there is limited understanding of the degree to which monkeys show imitative behaviour.[10]

In adult monkeys, mirror neurons may enable the monkey to understand what another monkey is doing, or to recognize the other monkey's action.[33]

In rodents edit

A number of studies have shown that rats and mice show signs of distress while witnessing another rodent receive footshocks.[34] The group of Christian Keysers's recorded from neurons while rats experienced pain or witnessed the pain of others, and has revealed the presence of pain mirror neurons in the rat's anterior cingulate cortex, i.e. neurons that respond both while an animal experiences pain and while witnessing the pain of others.[35] Deactivating this region of the cingulate cortex led to reduced emotional contagion in the rats, so that observer rats showed reduced distress while witnessing another rat experience pain.[35] The homologous part of the anterior cingulate cortex has been associated with empathy for pain in humans,[36] suggesting a homology between the systems involved in emotional contagion in rodents and empathy/emotional contagion for pain in humans.

In humans edit

 
Diagram of the brain, showing the locations of the frontal and parietal lobes of the cerebrum, viewed from the left. The inferior frontal lobe is the lower part of the blue area, and the superior parietal lobe is the upper part of the yellow area.

It is not normally possible to study single neurons in the human brain, so most evidence for mirror neurons in humans is indirect. Brain imaging experiments using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) have shown that the human inferior frontal cortex and superior parietal lobe are active when the person performs an action and also when the person sees another individual performing an action. It has been suggested that these brain regions contain mirror neurons, and they have been defined as the human mirror neuron system.[37] More recent experiments have shown that even at the level of single participants, scanned using fMRI, large areas containing multiple fMRI voxels increase their activity both during the observation and execution of actions.[26]

Neuropsychological studies looking at lesion areas that cause action knowledge, pantomime interpretation, and biological motion perception deficits have pointed to a causal link between the integrity of the inferior frontal gyrus and these behaviours.[38][39][40] Transcranial magnetic stimulation studies have confirmed this as well.[41][42] These results indicate the activation in mirror neuron related areas are unlikely to be just epiphenomenal.

A study published in April 2010 reports recordings from single neurons with mirror properties in the human brain.[43] Mukamel et al. (Current Biology, 2010) recorded from the brains of 21 patients who were being treated at Ronald Reagan UCLA Medical Center for intractable epilepsy. The patients had been implanted with intracranial depth electrodes to identify seizure foci for potential surgical treatment. Electrode location was based solely on clinical criteria; the researchers, with the patients' consent, used the same electrodes to "piggyback" their research. The researchers found a small number of neurons that fired or showed their greatest activity both when the individual performed a task and when they observed a task. Other neurons had anti-mirror properties: they responded when the participant performed an action, but were inhibited when the participant saw that action.

The mirror neurons found were located in the supplementary motor area and medial temporal cortex (other brain regions were not sampled). For purely practical reasons, these regions are not the same as those in which mirror neurons had been recorded from in the monkey: researchers in Parma were studying the ventral premotor cortex and the associated inferior parietal lobe, two regions in which epilepsy rarely occurs, and hence, single cell recordings in these regions are not usually done in humans. On the other hand, no one has to date looked for mirror neurons in the supplementary motor area or the medial temporal lobe in the monkey. Together, this therefore does not suggest that humans and monkeys have mirror neurons in different locations, but rather that they may have mirror neurons both in the ventral premotor cortex and inferior parietal lobe, where they have been recorded in the monkey, and in the supplementary motor areas and medial temporal lobe, where they have been recorded from in human – especially because detailed human fMRI analyses suggest activity compatible with the presence of mirror neurons in all these regions.[26]

Another study has suggested that human beings do not necessarily have more mirror neurons than monkeys, but instead that there is a core set of mirror neurons used in action observation and execution. However, for other proposed functions of mirror neurons the mirror system may have the ability to recruit other areas of the brain when doing its auditory, somatosensory, and affective components.[44]

Development edit

Human infant data using eye-tracking measures suggest that the mirror neuron system develops before 12 months of age and that this system may help human infants understand other people's actions.[45] A critical question concerns how mirror neurons acquire mirror properties. Two closely related models postulate that mirror neurons are trained through Hebbian[46] or Associative learning[47][48][12] (see Associative Sequence Learning). However, if premotor neurons need to be trained by action in order to acquire mirror properties, it is unclear how newborn babies are able to mimic the facial gestures of another person (imitation of unseen actions), as suggested by the work of Meltzoff and Moore. One possibility is that the sight of tongue protrusion recruits an innate releasing mechanism in neonates. Careful analysis suggests that 'imitation' of this single gesture may account for almost all reports of facial mimicry by new-born infants.[49]

Possible functions edit

Understanding intentions edit

Many studies link mirror neurons to understanding goals and intentions. Fogassi et al. (2005)[25] recorded the activity of 41 mirror neurons in the inferior parietal lobe (IPL) of two rhesus macaques. The IPL has long been recognized as an association cortex that integrates sensory information. The monkeys watched an experimenter either grasp an apple and bring it to his mouth or grasp an object and place it in a cup.

  • In total, 15 mirror neurons fired vigorously when the monkey observed the "grasp-to-eat" motion, but registered no activity while exposed to the "grasp-to-place" condition.
  • For 4 other mirror neurons, the reverse held true: they activated in response to the experimenter eventually placing the apple in the cup but not to eating it.

Only the type of action, and not the kinematic force with which models manipulated objects, determined neuron activity. It was also significant that neurons fired before the monkey observed the human model starting the second motor act (bringing the object to the mouth or placing it in a cup). Therefore, IPL neurons "code the same act (grasping) in a different way according to the final goal of the action in which the act is embedded."[25] They may furnish a neural basis for predicting another individual's subsequent actions and inferring intention.[25]

Learning facilitation edit

Another possible function of mirror neurons would be facilitation of learning. The mirror neurons code the concrete representation of the action, i.e., the representation that would be activated if the observer acted. This would allow us to simulate (to repeat internally) the observed action implicitly (in the brain) to collect our own motor programs of observed actions and to get ready to reproduce the actions later. It is implicit training. Due to this, the observer will produce the action explicitly (in his/her behavior) with agility and finesse. This happens due to associative learning processes. The more frequently a synaptic connection is activated, the stronger it becomes.[50]

Empathy edit

Stephanie Preston and Frans de Waal,[51] Jean Decety,[52][53] and Vittorio Gallese[54][55] and Christian Keysers[3] have independently argued that the mirror neuron system is involved in empathy. A large number of experiments using fMRI, electroencephalography (EEG) and magnetoencephalography (MEG) have shown that certain brain regions (in particular the anterior insula, anterior cingulate cortex, and inferior frontal cortex) are active when people experience an emotion (disgust, happiness, pain, etc.) and when they see another person experiencing an emotion.[56][57][58][59][60][61][62] David Freedberg and Vittorio Gallese have also put forward the idea that this function of the mirror neuron system is crucial for aesthetic experiences.[63] Nevertheless, an experiment aimed at investigating the activity of mirror neurons in empathy conducted by Soukayna Bekkali and Peter Enticott at the University of Deakin yielded a different result. After analyzing the report's data, they came up with two conclusions about motor empathy and emotional empathy. First, there is no relationship between motor empathy and the activity of mirror neurons. Second, there is only weak evidence of these neurons' activity in the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), and no evidence of emotional empathy associated with mirror neurons in key brain regions (inferior parietal lobule: IPL). In other words, there has not been an exact conclusion about the role of mirror neurons in empathy and if they are essential for human empathy.[64] However, these brain regions are not quite the same as the ones which mirror hand actions, and mirror neurons for emotional states or empathy have not yet been described in monkeys.

In a recent study, done in 2022, sixteen hand actions were given for each assignment. The assignment pictured both an activity word phase and the intended word phase. The hand actions were selected in "trails" each introduced twice. One of the times was with a matching phase and the other time was with a misleading word phase. The action words were depicted in two to three words with each beginning with the word "to". For instance, "to point" (action) or "to spin" (intention).

Participants were expected to answer whether the correct word phase matched the corresponding action or intention word. The word phase had to be answered within 3000 ms, with a 1000 ms black screen between each image. The black screens purpose was for an adequate amount of time in between responses. Participants pressed on the keyboard "x" or "m" to indicate their responses in a yes/no format.[65]

Christian Keysers at the Social Brain Lab and colleagues have shown that people who are more empathic according to self-report questionnaires have stronger activations both in the mirror system for hand actions[66] and the mirror system for emotions,[61] providing more direct support for the idea that the mirror system is linked to empathy. Some researchers observed that the human mirror system does not passively respond to the observation of actions but is influenced by the mindset of the observer.[67] Researchers observed the link of the mirror neurons during empathetic engagement in patient care.[68]

Studies in rats have shown that the anterior cingulate cortex contains mirror neurons for pain, i.e. neurons responding both during the first-hand experience of pain and while witnessing the pain of others,[35] and inhibition of this region leads to reduced emotional contagion in rats[35] and mice,[34] and reduced aversion towards harming others.[69] This provides causal evidence for a link between pain mirror neurons, and emotional contagion and prosocial behavior, two phenomena associated with empathy, in rodents. That brain activity in the homologous brain region is associated with individual variability in empathy in humans[36] suggests that a similar mechanism may be at play across mammals.

Human self awareness edit

V. S. Ramachandran has speculated that mirror neurons may provide the neurological basis of human self-awareness.[70] In an essay written for the Edge Foundation in 2009 Ramachandran gave the following explanation of his theory: "... I also speculated that these neurons can not only help simulate other people's behavior but can be turned 'inward'—as it were—to create second-order representations or meta-representations of your own earlier brain processes. This could be the neural basis of introspection, and of the reciprocity of self awareness and other awareness. There is obviously a chicken-or-egg question here as to which evolved first, but... The main point is that the two co-evolved, mutually enriching each other to create the mature representation of self that characterizes modern humans."[71]

Language edit

In humans, functional MRI studies have reported finding areas homologous to the monkey mirror neuron system in the inferior frontal cortex, close to Broca's area, one of the hypothesized language regions of the brain. This has led to suggestions that human language evolved from a gesture performance/understanding system implemented in mirror neurons. Mirror neurons have been said to have the potential to provide a mechanism for action-understanding, imitation-learning, and the simulation of other people's behaviour.[72] This hypothesis is supported by some cytoarchitectonic homologies between monkey premotor area F5 and human Broca's area.[73] Rates of vocabulary expansion link to the ability of children to vocally mirror non-words and so to acquire the new word pronunciations. Such speech repetition occurs automatically, fast[74] and separately in the brain to speech perception.[75][76] Moreover, such vocal imitation can occur without comprehension such as in speech shadowing[77] and echolalia.[78]

Further evidence for this link comes from a recent study in which the brain activity of two participants was measured using fMRI while they were gesturing words to each other using hand gestures with a game of charades – a modality that some have suggested might represent the evolutionary precursor of human language. Analysis of the data using Granger Causality revealed that the mirror-neuron system of the observer indeed reflects the pattern of activity in the motor system of the sender, supporting the idea that the motor concept associated with the words is indeed transmitted from one brain to another using the mirror system[79]

The mirror neuron system seems to be inherently inadequate to play any role in syntax, given that this definitory property of human languages which is implemented in hierarchical recursive structure is flattened into linear sequences of phonemes making the recursive structure not accessible to sensory detection[80]

Automatic imitation edit

The term is commonly used to refer to cases in which an individual, having observed a body movement, unintentionally performs a similar body movement or alters the way that a body movement is performed. Automatic imitation rarely involves overt execution of matching responses. Instead the effects typically consist of reaction time, rather than accuracy, differences between compatible and incompatible trials. Research reveals that the existence of automatic imitation, which is a covert form of imitation, is distinct from spatial compatibility. It also indicates that, although automatic imitation is subject to input modulation by attentional processes, and output modulation by inhibitory processes, it is mediated by learned, long-term sensorimotor associations that cannot be altered directly by intentional processes. Many researchers believe that automatic imitation is mediated by the mirror neuron system.[81] Additionally, there are data that demonstrate that our postural control is impaired when people listen to sentences about other actions. For example, if the task is to maintain posture, people do it worse when they listen to sentences like this: "I get up, put on my slippers, go to the bathroom." This phenomenon may be due to the fact that during action perception there is similar motor cortex activation as if a human being performed the same action (mirror neurons system).[82]

Motor mimicry edit

In contrast with automatic imitation, motor mimicry is observed in (1) naturalistic social situations and (2) via measures of action frequency within a session rather than measures of speed and/or accuracy within trials.[83]

The integration of research on motor mimicry and automatic imitation could reveal plausible indications that these phenomena depend on the same psychological and neural processes. Preliminary evidence however comes from studies showing that social priming has similar effects on motor mimicry.[84][85]

Nevertheless, the similarities between automatic imitation, mirror effects, and motor mimicry have led some researchers to propose that automatic imitation is mediated by the mirror neuron system and that it is a tightly controlled laboratory equivalent of the motor mimicry observed in naturalistic social contexts. If true, then automatic imitation can be used as a tool to investigate how the mirror neuron system contributes to cognitive functioning and how motor mimicry promotes prosocial attitudes and behavior.[86][87]

Meta-analysis of imitation studies in humans suggest that there is enough evidence of mirror system activation during imitation that mirror neuron involvement is likely, even though no published studies have recorded the activities of singular neurons. However, it is likely insufficient for motor imitation. Studies show that regions of the frontal and parietal lobes that extend beyond the classical mirror system are equally activated during imitation. This suggests that other areas, along with the mirror system are crucial to imitation behaviors.[8]

Autism edit

It has also been proposed that problems with the mirror neuron system may underlie cognitive disorders, particularly autism.[88][89] However the connection between mirror neuron dysfunction and autism is tentative and it remains to be demonstrated how mirror neurons are related to many of the important characteristics of autism.[10]

Some researchers claim there is a link between mirror neuron deficiency and autism. EEG recordings from motor areas are suppressed when someone watches another person move, a signal that may relate to mirror neuron system. This suppression was less in children with autism.[88] Although these findings have been replicated by several groups,[90][91] other studies have not found evidence of a dysfunctional mirror neuron system in autism.[10] In 2008, Oberman et al. published a research paper that presented conflicting EEG evidence. Oberman and Ramachandran found typical mu-suppression for familiar stimuli, but not for unfamiliar stimuli, leading them to conclude that the mirror neuron system of children with ASD (Autism Spectrum Disorder) was functional, but less sensitive than that of typical children.[92] Based on the conflicting evidence presented by mu-wave suppression experiments, Patricia Churchland has cautioned that mu-wave suppression results cannot be used as a valid index for measuring the performance of mirror neuron systems.[93] Recent research indicates that mirror neurons do not play a role in autism:

...no clear cut evidence emerges for a fundamental mirror system deficit in autism. Behavioural studies have shown that people with autism have a good understanding of action goals. Furthermore, two independent neuroimaging studies have reported that the parietal component of the mirror system is functioning typically in individuals with autism.[94]

Some anatomical differences have been found in the mirror neuron related brain areas in adults with autism spectrum disorders, compared to non-autistic adults. All these cortical areas were thinner and the degree of thinning was correlated with autism symptom severity, a correlation nearly restricted to these brain regions.[95] Based on these results, some researchers claim that autism is caused by impairments in the mirror neuron system, leading to disabilities in social skills, imitation, empathy and theory of mind.[who?]

Many researchers have pointed out that the "broken mirrors" theory of autism is overly simplistic, and mirror neurons alone cannot explain the differences found in individuals with autism. First of all, as noted above, none of these studies were direct measures of mirror neuron activity - in other words fMRI activity or EEG rhythm suppression do not unequivocally index mirror neurons. Dinstein and colleagues found normal mirror neuron activity in people with autism using fMRI.[96] In individuals with autism, deficits in intention understanding, action understanding and biological motion perception (the key functions of mirror neurons) are not always found,[97][98] or are task dependent.[99][100] Today, very few people believe an all-or-nothing problem with the mirror system can underlie autism. Instead, "additional research needs to be done, and more caution should be used when reaching out to the media."[101]

Research from 2010[96] concluded that autistic individuals do not exhibit mirror neuron dysfunction, although the small sample size limits the extent to which these results can be generalized. A more recent review argued there was not enough neurological evidence to support this “broken-mirror theory” of autism.[102]

Theory of mind edit

In Philosophy of mind, mirror neurons have become the primary rallying call of simulation theorists concerning our "theory of mind." "Theory of mind" refers to our ability to infer another person's mental state (i.e., beliefs and desires) from experiences or their behaviour.

There are several competing models which attempt to account for our theory of mind; the most notable in relation to mirror neurons is simulation theory. According to simulation theory, theory of mind is available because we subconsciously empathize with the person we're observing and, accounting for relevant differences, imagine what we would desire and believe in that scenario.[103][104] Mirror neurons have been interpreted as the mechanism by which we simulate others in order to better understand them, and therefore their discovery has been taken by some as a validation of simulation theory (which appeared a decade before the discovery of mirror neurons).[54] More recently, Theory of Mind and Simulation have been seen as complementary systems, with different developmental time courses.[105][106][107]

At the neuronal-level, in a 2015 study by Keren Haroush and Ziv Williams using jointly interacting primates performing an iterated prisoner's dilemma game, the authors identified neurons in the anterior cingulate cortex that selectively predicted an opponent's yet unknown decisions or covert state of mind. These "other-predictive neurons" differentiated between self and other decisions and were uniquely sensitive to social context, but they did not encode the opponent's observed actions or receipt of reward. These cingulate cells may therefore importantly complement the function of mirror neurons by providing additional information about other social agents that is not immediately observable or known.[108]

Sex differences edit

A series of recent studies conducted by Yawei Cheng, using a variety of neurophysiological measures, including MEG,[109] spinal reflex excitability,[110] electroencephalography,[111][112] have documented the presence of a gender difference in the human mirror neuron system, with female participants exhibiting stronger motor resonance than male participants.

In another study, sex-based differences among mirror neuron mechanisms was reinforced in that the data showed enhanced empathetic ability in females relative to males[citation needed]. During an emotional social interaction, females showed a greater ability in emotional perspective taking[clarification needed] than did males when interacting with another person face-to-face. However, in the study, data showed that when it came to recognizing the emotions of others, all participants' abilities were very similar and there was no key difference between the male and female subjects.[113]

Sleep paralysis edit

Baland Jalal and V. S. Ramachandran have hypothesized that the mirror neuron system is important in giving rise to the intruder hallucination and out-of-body experiences during sleep paralysis.[114] According to this theory, sleep paralysis leads to disinhibition of the mirror neuron system, paving the way for hallucinations of human-like shadowy beings. The deafferentation of sensory information during sleep paralysis is proposed as the mechanism for such mirror neuron disinhibition.[114] The authors suggest that their hypothesis on the role of the mirror neuron system could be tested:

"These ideas could be explored using neuroimaging, to examine the selective activation of brain regions associated with mirror neuron activity, when the individual is hallucinating an intruder or having an out-of-body experience during sleep paralysis ."[114]

Mirror neuron function, psychosis, and empathy in schizophrenia edit

Recent research, which measured mu-wave suppression, suggests that mirror neuron activity is positively correlated with psychotic symptoms (i.e., greater mu suppression/mirror neuron activity was highest among subjects with the greater severity of psychotic symptoms). Researchers concluded that "higher mirror neuron activity may be the underpinning of schizophrenia sensory gating deficits and may contribute to sensory misattributions particularly in response to socially relevant stimuli, and be a putative mechanism for delusions and hallucinations."[115]

Doubts concerning mirror neurons edit

Although some in the scientific community have expressed excitement about the discovery of mirror neurons, there are scientists who have expressed doubts about both the existence and role of mirror neurons in humans. The consensus today seems to be that the importance of so-called mirror neurons is widely overblown. According to scientists such as Hickok, Pascolo, and Dinstein, it is not clear whether mirror neurons really form a distinct class of cells (as opposed to an occasional phenomenon seen in cells that have other functions),[116] and whether mirror activity is a distinct type of response or simply an artifact of an overall facilitation of the motor system.[11]

In 2008, Ilan Dinstein et al. argued that the original analyses were unconvincing because they were based on qualitative descriptions of individual cell properties, and did not take into account the small number of strongly mirror-selective neurons in motor areas.[10] Other scientists have argued that the measurements of neuron fire delay seem not to be compatible with standard reaction times,[116] and pointed out that nobody has reported that an interruption of the motor areas in F5 would produce a decrease in action recognition.[11] (Critics of this argument have replied that these authors have missed human neuropsychological and TMS studies reporting disruption of these areas do indeed cause action deficits[39][41] without affecting other kinds of perception.)[40]

In 2009, Lingnau et al. carried out an experiment in which they compared motor acts that were first observed and then executed to motor acts that were first executed and then observed. They concluded that there was a significant asymmetry between the two processes that indicated that mirror neurons do not exist in humans. They stated "Crucially, we found no signs of adaptation for motor acts that were first executed and then observed. Failure to find cross-modal adaptation for executed and observed motor acts is not compatible with the core assumption of mirror neuron theory, which holds that action recognition and understanding are based on motor simulation."[117] However, in the same year, Kilner et al. showed that if goal directed actions are used as stimuli, both IPL and premotor regions show the repetition suppression between observation and execution that is predicted by mirror neurons.[118]

In 2009, Greg Hickok published an extensive argument against the claim that mirror neurons are involved in action-understanding: "Eight Problems for the Mirror Neuron Theory of Action Understanding in Monkeys and Humans." He concluded that "The early hypothesis that these cells underlie action understanding is likewise an interesting and prima facie reasonable idea. However, despite its widespread acceptance, the proposal has never been adequately tested in monkeys, and in humans there is strong empirical evidence, in the form of physiological and neuropsychological (double-) dissociations, against the claim."[11]

 
The mirror neurons can be activated only after the goal of the observed action has been attributed by other brain structures.

Vladimir Kosonogov sees another contradiction. The proponents of mirror neuron theory of action understanding postulate that the mirror neurons code the goals of others' actions because they are activated if the observed action is goal-directed. However, the mirror neurons are activated only when the observed action is goal-directed (object-directed action or a communicative gesture, which certainly has a goal too). How do they "know" that the definite action is goal-directed? At what stage of their activation do they detect a goal of the movement or its absence? In his opinion, the mirror neuron system can be activated only after the goal of the observed action is attributed by some other brain structures.[50]

Neurophilosophers such as Patricia Churchland have expressed both scientific and philosophical objections to the theory that mirror neurons are responsible for understanding the intentions of others. In chapter 5 of her 2011 book, Braintrust, Churchland points out that the claim that mirror neurons are involved in understanding intentions (through simulating observed actions) is based on assumptions that are clouded by unresolved philosophical issues. She makes the argument that intentions are understood (coded) at a more complex level of neural activity than that of individual neurons. Churchland states that "A neuron, though computationally complex, is just a neuron. It is not an intelligent homunculus. If a neural network represents something complex, such as an intention [to insult], it must have the right input and be in the right place in the neural circuitry to do that."[119]

Cecilia Heyes has advanced the theory that mirror neurons are the byproduct of associative learning as opposed to evolutionary adaptation. She argues that mirror neurons in humans are the product of social interaction and not an evolutionary adaptation for action-understanding. In particular, Heyes rejects the theory advanced by V.S. Ramachandran that mirror neurons have been "the driving force behind the great leap forward in human evolution."[12][120]

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ a b c d Rizzolatti G, Craighero L (2004). "The mirror-neuron system". Annual Review of Neuroscience. 27 (1): 169–192. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144230. PMID 15217330. S2CID 1729870.
  2. ^ Keysers C (November 2009). "Mirror neurons". Current Biology. 19 (21): R971–R973. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2009.08.026. hdl:20.500.11755/c62977db-8a4e-4cc9-b82a-5013b52a0bc6. PMID 19922849. S2CID 12668046.
  3. ^ a b c Keysers C (2011-06-23). The Empathic Brain. Kindle.
  4. ^ Acharya, Sourya; Shukla, Samarth (2012). "Mirror neurons: Enigma of the metaphysical modular brain". Journal of Natural Science, Biology and Medicine. 3 (2): 118–124. doi:10.4103/0976-9668.101878. PMC 3510904. PMID 23225972.
  5. ^ Mukamel R, Ekstrom AD, Kaplan J, Iacoboni M, Fried I (April 2010). "Single-neuron responses in humans during execution and observation of actions". Current Biology. 20 (8): 750–756. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2010.02.045. PMC 2904852. PMID 20381353.
  6. ^ a b Rizzolatti G, Fadiga L, Fogassi L, Gallese V (May 1999). "Resonance behaviors and mirror neurons". Archives Italiennes de Biologie. 137 (2–3): 85–100. PMID 10349488.
  7. ^ Prather JF, Peters S, Nowicki S, Mooney R (January 2008). "Precise auditory-vocal mirroring in neurons for learned vocal communication". Nature. 451 (7176): 305–310. Bibcode:2008Natur.451..305P. doi:10.1038/nature06492. PMID 18202651. S2CID 4344150.
  8. ^ a b Molenberghs P, Cunnington R, Mattingley JB (July 2009). "Is the mirror neuron system involved in imitation? A short review and meta-analysis". Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 33 (7): 975–980. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.03.010. PMID 19580913. S2CID 25620637.
  9. ^ Akins CK, Klein ED, Zentall TR (August 2002). "Imitative learning in Japanese quail (Coturnix japonica) using the bidirectional control procedure". Animal Learning & Behavior. 30 (3): 275–281. doi:10.3758/bf03192836. PMID 12391793.
  10. ^ a b c d e Dinstein I, Thomas C, Behrmann M, Heeger DJ (January 2008). "A mirror up to nature". Current Biology. 18 (1): R13–R18. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2007.11.004. PMC 2517574. PMID 18177704.
  11. ^ a b c d Hickok G (July 2009). "Eight problems for the mirror neuron theory of action understanding in monkeys and humans". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 21 (7): 1229–1243. doi:10.1162/jocn.2009.21189. PMC 2773693. PMID 19199415.
  12. ^ a b c Heyes, Cecilia (March 2010). "Where do mirror neurons come from?". Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 34 (4): 575–583. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.11.007. PMID 19914284. S2CID 2578537.
  13. ^ Ferrari PF, Rizzolatti G (2014). "Mirror neuron research: the past and the future". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences. 369 (1644): 20130169. doi:10.1098/rstb.2013.0169. PMC 4006175. PMID 24778369.
  14. ^ Keysers C, Gazzola V (2006). "Towards a unifying neural theory of social cognition". Understanding Emotions. Progress in Brain Research. Vol. 156. pp. 379–401. doi:10.1016/S0079-6123(06)56021-2. ISBN 9780444521828. PMID 17015092.
  15. ^ Arbib MA (April 2012). How the brain got language: The mirror system hypothesis. Oxford University Press.
  16. ^ Théoret H, Pascual-Leone A (October 2002). "Language acquisition: do as you hear". Current Biology. 12 (21): R736–R737. doi:10.1016/S0960-9822(02)01251-4. PMID 12419204.
  17. ^ Blakeslee S (January 10, 2006). "Cells That Read Minds". New York Times | Science.
  18. ^ di Pellegrino G, Fadiga L, Fogassi L, Gallese V, Rizzolatti G (1992). "Understanding motor events: a neurophysiological study". Experimental Brain Research. 91 (1): 176–180. doi:10.1007/bf00230027. PMID 1301372. S2CID 206772150.
  19. ^ Rizzolatti G, Fadiga L, Gallese V, Fogassi L (March 1996). "Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions". Brain Research. Cognitive Brain Research. 3 (2): 131–141. doi:10.1016/0926-6410(95)00038-0. PMID 8713554.
  20. ^ Rizzolatti G, Fabbri-Destro M (January 2010). "Mirror neurons: from discovery to autism". Experimental Brain Research. 200 (3–4): 223–237. doi:10.1007/s00221-009-2002-3. PMID 19760408. S2CID 3342808.
  21. ^ a b Gallese V, Fadiga L, Fogassi L, Rizzolatti G (April 1996). "Action recognition in the premotor cortex". Brain. 119 ( Pt 2) (2): 593–609. doi:10.1093/brain/119.2.593. PMID 8800951.
  22. ^ Ferrari PF, Gallese V, Rizzolatti G, Fogassi L (April 2003). "Mirror neurons responding to the observation of ingestive and communicative mouth actions in the monkey ventral premotor cortex". The European Journal of Neuroscience. 17 (8): 1703–1714. doi:10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02601.x. PMID 12752388. S2CID 1915143.
  23. ^ Kohler E, Keysers C, Umiltà MA, Fogassi L, Gallese V, Rizzolatti G (August 2002). "Hearing sounds, understanding actions: action representation in mirror neurons". Science. 297 (5582). New York, N.Y.: 846–8. Bibcode:2002Sci...297..846K. doi:10.1126/science.1070311. PMID 12161656. S2CID 16923101.
  24. ^ Gazzola V, Aziz-Zadeh L, Keysers C (September 2006). "Empathy and the somatotopic auditory mirror system in humans". Current Biology. 16 (18): 1824–9. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2006.07.072. PMID 16979560. S2CID 5223812.
  25. ^ a b c d Fogassi L, Ferrari PF, Gesierich B, Rozzi S, Chersi F, Rizzolatti G (April 2005). "Parietal lobe: from action organization to intention understanding". Science. 308 (5722). New York, N.Y.: 662–7. Bibcode:2005Sci...308..662F. doi:10.1126/science.1106138. PMID 15860620. S2CID 5720234.
  26. ^ a b c Gazzola V, Keysers C (June 2009). "The observation and execution of actions share motor and somatosensory voxels in all tested subjects: single-subject analyses of unsmoothed fMRI data". Cerebral Cortex. 19 (6): 1239–1255. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhn181. PMC 2677653. PMID 19020203.
  27. ^ Keysers C, Kaas JH, Gazzola V (June 2010). "Somatosensation in social perception". Nature Reviews. Neuroscience. 11 (6): 417–428. doi:10.1038/nrn2833. PMID 20445542. S2CID 12221575.
  28. ^ a b Cook R, Bird G, Catmur C, Press C, Heyes C (April 2014). "Mirror neurons: from origin to function" (PDF). The Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 37 (2): 177–192. doi:10.1017/S0140525X13000903. PMID 24775147. S2CID 52873194.
  29. ^ Keysers C, Gazzola V (2014). "Hebbian learning and predictive mirror neurons for actions, sensations and emotions". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences. 369 (1644): 20130175. doi:10.1098/rstb.2013.0175. PMC 4006178. PMID 24778372.
  30. ^ Del Giudice M, Manera V, Keysers C (March 2009). "Programmed to learn? The ontogeny of mirror neurons". Developmental Science. 12 (2): 350–363. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00783.x. hdl:2318/133096. PMID 19143807. S2CID 15093476.
  31. ^ Ferrari PF, Visalberghi E, Paukner A, Fogassi L, Ruggiero A, Suomi SJ (September 2006). "Neonatal imitation in rhesus macaques". PLOS Biology. 4 (9): e302. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0040302. PMC 1560174. PMID 16953662.
  32. ^ Ferrari PF, Bonini L, Fogassi L (August 2009). "From monkey mirror neurons to primate behaviours: possible 'direct' and 'indirect' pathways". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences. 364 (1528): 2311–23. doi:10.1098/rstb.2009.0062. PMC 2865083. PMID 19620103.
  33. ^ Rizzolatti G, Arbib MA (May 1998). "Language within our grasp". Trends in Neurosciences. 21 (5): 188–194. doi:10.1016/S0166-2236(98)01260-0. hdl:11858/00-001M-0000-002C-4B59-6. PMID 9610880. S2CID 679023.
  34. ^ a b Keum S, Shin HS (October 2019). "Neural Basis of Observational Fear Learning: A Potential Model of Affective Empathy". Neuron. 104 (1): 78–86. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2019.09.013. PMID 31600517. S2CID 203932127.
  35. ^ a b c d Carrillo M, Han Y, Migliorati F, Liu M, Gazzola V, Keysers C (April 2019). "Emotional Mirror Neurons in the Rat's Anterior Cingulate Cortex". Current Biology. 29 (8): 1301–1312.e6. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2019.03.024. PMC 6488290. PMID 30982647.
  36. ^ a b Lamm C, Decety J, Singer T (February 2011). "Meta-analytic evidence for common and distinct neural networks associated with directly experienced pain and empathy for pain". NeuroImage. 54 (3): 2492–2502. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.014. PMID 20946964. S2CID 6021487.
  37. ^ Iacoboni M, Woods RP, Brass M, Bekkering H, Mazziotta JC, Rizzolatti G (December 1999). "Cortical mechanisms of human imitation". Science. 286 (5449): 2526–2528. doi:10.1126/science.286.5449.2526. PMID 10617472.
  38. ^ Saygin AP, Wilson SM, Dronkers NF, Bates E (2004). "Action comprehension in aphasia: linguistic and non-linguistic deficits and their lesion correlates". Neuropsychologia. 42 (13): 1788–1804. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.544.9071. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.04.016. PMID 15351628. S2CID 11622224.
  39. ^ a b Tranel D, Kemmerer D, Adolphs R, Damasio H, Damasio AR (May 2003). "Neural correlates of conceptual knowledge for actions". Cognitive Neuropsychology. 20 (3): 409–432. doi:10.1080/02643290244000248. PMID 20957578. S2CID 16131.
  40. ^ a b Saygin AP (September 2007). "Superior temporal and premotor brain areas necessary for biological motion perception". Brain. 130 (Pt 9): 2452–2461. doi:10.1093/brain/awm162. PMID 17660183.
  41. ^ a b Pobric G, Hamilton AF (March 2006). "Action understanding requires the left inferior frontal cortex". Current Biology. 16 (5): 524–529. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2006.01.033. PMID 16527749.
  42. ^ Candidi M, Urgesi C, Ionta S, Aglioti SM (2008). "Virtual lesion of ventral premotor cortex impairs visual perception of biomechanically possible but not impossible actions". Social Neuroscience. 3 (3–4): 388–400. doi:10.1080/17470910701676269. PMID 18979387. S2CID 37390465.
  43. ^ Keysers C, Gazzola V (April 2010). "Social neuroscience: mirror neurons recorded in humans". Current Biology. 20 (8): R353–R354. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2010.03.013. hdl:20.500.11755/351f0172-b06b-41de-a013-852c64e197fa. PMID 21749952. S2CID 3609747.
  44. ^ Molenberghs P, Cunnington R, Mattingley JB (January 2012). "Brain regions with mirror properties: a meta-analysis of 125 human fMRI studies". Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 36 (1): 341–349. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.07.004. PMID 21782846. S2CID 37871374.
  45. ^ Falck-Ytter T, Gredebäck G, von Hofsten C (July 2006). "Infants predict other people's action goals". Nature Neuroscience. 9 (7): 878–9. doi:10.1038/nn1729. PMID 16783366. S2CID 2409686.
  46. ^ Keysers C, Perrett DI (November 2004). "Demystifying social cognition: a Hebbian perspective". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 8 (11): 501–507. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2004.09.005. PMID 15491904. S2CID 8039741.
  47. ^ Heyes C (June 2001). "Causes and consequences of imitation". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 5 (6): 253–261. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01661-2. PMID 11390296. S2CID 15602731.
  48. ^ Brass M, Heyes C (October 2005). "Imitation: is cognitive neuroscience solving the correspondence problem?". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 9 (10): 489–95. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.08.007. PMID 16126449. S2CID 1594505.
  49. ^ Anisfeld M (1996). "Only tongue protruding modeling is matched by neonates". Developmental Review. 16 (2): 149–161. doi:10.1006/drev.1996.0006.
  50. ^ a b Kosonogov, V. (December 2012). "Why the Mirror Neurons Cannot Support Action Understanding". Neurophysiology. 44 (6): 499–502. doi:10.1007/s11062-012-9327-4. S2CID 254867235.
  51. ^ Preston SD, de Waal FB (February 2002). "Empathy: Its ultimate and proximate bases". The Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 25 (1): 1–72. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.554.2794. doi:10.1017/s0140525x02000018. PMID 12625087.
  52. ^ Decety, J (2002). "Naturaliser l'empathie" [Empathy naturalized]. L' Encéphale (in French). 28 (1): 9–20. OCLC 110778688. INIST 13554627.
  53. ^ Decety J, Jackson PL (June 2004). "The functional architecture of human empathy". Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews. 3 (2): 71–100. doi:10.1177/1534582304267187. PMID 15537986. S2CID 145310279.
  54. ^ a b Gallese V, Goldman A (December 1998). "Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind-reading". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2 (12): 493–501. doi:10.1016/s1364-6613(98)01262-5. PMID 21227300. S2CID 10108122.
  55. ^ Gallese V (2001). "The "Shared Manifold" hypothesis: from mirror neurons to empathy". Journal of Consciousness Studies. 8: 33–50.
  56. ^ Botvinick M, Jha AP, Bylsma LM, Fabian SA, Solomon PE, Prkachin KM (March 2005). "Viewing facial expressions of pain engages cortical areas involved in the direct experience of pain". NeuroImage. 25 (1): 312–319. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.11.043. PMID 15734365. S2CID 24988672.
  57. ^ Cheng Y, Yang CY, Lin CP, Lee PL, Decety J (May 2008). "The perception of pain in others suppresses somatosensory oscillations: a magnetoencephalography study". NeuroImage. 40 (4): 1833–1840. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.01.064. PMID 18353686. S2CID 1827514.
  58. ^ Morrison I, Lloyd D, di Pellegrino G, Roberts N (June 2004). "Vicarious responses to pain in anterior cingulate cortex: is empathy a multisensory issue?". Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience. 4 (2): 270–278. doi:10.3758/CABN.4.2.270. PMID 15460933.
  59. ^ Wicker B, Keysers C, Plailly J, Royet JP, Gallese V, Rizzolatti G (October 2003). "Both of us disgusted in My insula: the common neural basis of seeing and feeling disgust". Neuron. 40 (3): 655–64. doi:10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00679-2. PMID 14642287. S2CID 766157.
  60. ^ Singer T, Seymour B, O'Doherty J, Kaube H, Dolan RJ, Frith CD (February 2004). "Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain". Science. 303 (5661). New York, N.Y.: 1157–1162. Bibcode:2004Sci...303.1157S. doi:10.1126/science.1093535. hdl:21.11116/0000-0001-A020-5. PMID 14976305. S2CID 14727944.
  61. ^ a b Jabbi M, Swart M, Keysers C (February 2007). "Empathy for positive and negative emotions in the gustatory cortex". NeuroImage. 34 (4): 1744–1753. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.10.032. PMID 17175173. S2CID 13988152.
  62. ^ Lamm C, Batson CD, Decety J (January 2007). "The neural substrate of human empathy: effects of perspective-taking and cognitive appraisal". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 19 (1): 42–58. doi:10.1162/jocn.2007.19.1.42. PMID 17214562. S2CID 2828843.
  63. ^ Freedberg D, Gallese V (May 2007). "Motion, emotion and empathy in esthetic experience". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 11 (5): 197–203. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2007.02.003. PMID 17347026. S2CID 1996468.
  64. ^ Jarrett C (March 25, 2019). "There is Only Weak Evidence that Mirror Neurons Underlie Human Empathy- New Review and Meta-Analysis". Research Digest.[dead link]
  65. ^ Thompson EL, Bird G, Catmur C (November 2022). "Mirror neuron brain regions contribute to identifying actions, but not intentions". Human Brain Mapping. 43 (16): 4901–4913. doi:10.1002/hbm.26036. PMC 9582378. PMID 35906896.
  66. ^ Gazzola, Aziz-Zadeh and Keysers (2006). (PDF). Archived from the original (PDF) on 2007-06-30.
  67. ^ Molenberghs P, Hayward L, Mattingley JB, Cunnington R (January 2012). "Activation patterns during action observation are modulated by context in mirror system areas". NeuroImage. 59 (1): 608–615. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.07.080. PMID 21840404. S2CID 13951700.
  68. ^ Hojat M, Louis DZ, Maio V, Gonnella JS (2013). "Empathy and health care quality". American Journal of Medical Quality. 28 (1): 6–7. doi:10.1177/1062860612464731. PMID 23288854. S2CID 12645544.
  69. ^ Hernandez-Lallement J, Attah AT, Soyman E, Pinhal CM, Gazzola V, Keysers C (March 2020). "Harm to Others Acts as a Negative Reinforcer in Rats". Current Biology. 30 (6): 949–961.e7. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2020.01.017. hdl:20.500.11755/ee7ae8ac-7393-4276-84ce-1bad1b8e5e0d. PMID 32142701. S2CID 212424287.
  70. ^ Oberman L, Ramachandran VS (2009). "Reflections on the Mirror Neuron System: Their Evolutionary Functions Beyond Motor Representation". In Pineda JA (ed.). Mirror Neuron Systems: The Role of Mirroring Processes in Social Cognition. Humana Press. pp. 39–62. ISBN 978-1-934115-34-3.
  71. ^ Ramachandran VS (January 1, 2009). "Self Awareness: The Last Frontier, Edge Foundation web essay". Retrieved July 26, 2011.
  72. ^ Skoyles JR (2000). "Gesture, language origins, and right handedness" (PDF). Psycoloquy. 11 (24).
  73. ^ Petrides M, Cadoret G, Mackey S (June 2005). "Orofacial somatomotor responses in the macaque monkey homologue of Broca's area". Nature. 435 (7046): 1235–1238. Bibcode:2005Natur.435.1235P. doi:10.1038/nature03628. PMID 15988526. S2CID 4397762.
  74. ^ Porter RJ, Lubker JF (September 1980). "Rapid reproduction of vowel-vowel sequences: evidence for a fast and direct acoustic-motoric linkage in speech". Journal of Speech and Hearing Research. 23 (3): 593–602. doi:10.1044/jshr.2303.593. PMID 7421161.
  75. ^ McCarthy R, Warrington EK (June 1984). "A two-route model of speech production. Evidence from aphasia". Brain. 107 ( Pt 2) (2): 463–485. doi:10.1093/brain/107.2.463. PMID 6722512.
  76. ^ McCarthy RA, Warrington EK (2001). "Repeating without semantics: surface dysphasia?". Neurocase. 7 (1): 77–87. doi:10.1093/neucas/7.1.77. PMID 11239078. S2CID 12988855.
  77. ^ Marslen-Wilson W (August 1973). "Linguistic structure and speech shadowing at very short latencies". Nature. 244 (5417): 522–523. Bibcode:1973Natur.244..522M. doi:10.1038/244522a0. PMID 4621131. S2CID 4220775.
  78. ^ Fay WH, Coleman RO (July 1977). "A human sound transducer/reproducer: temporal capabilities of a profoundly echolalic child". Brain and Language. 4 (3): 396–402. doi:10.1016/0093-934X(77)90034-7. PMID 907878. S2CID 29492873.
  79. ^ Schippers MB, Roebroeck A, Renken R, Nanetti L, Keysers C (May 2010). "Mapping the information flow from one brain to another during gestural communication". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 107 (20): 9388–9393. Bibcode:2010PNAS..107.9388S. doi:10.1073/pnas.1001791107. PMC 2889063. PMID 20439736.
  80. ^ Moro A (2008). The Boundaries of Babel. The Brain and the Enigma of Impossible Languages. MIT Press. p. 257. ISBN 978-0-262-13498-9.
  81. ^ Longo MR, Kosobud A, Bertenthal BI (April 2008). "Automatic imitation of biomechanically possible and impossible actions: effects of priming movements versus goals" (PDF). Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 34 (2): 489–501. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.34.2.489. PMID 18377184.
  82. ^ Kosonogov V (October 2011). "Listening to action-related sentences impairs postural control". Journal of Electromyography and Kinesiology. 21 (5): 742–745. doi:10.1016/j.jelekin.2011.05.007. PMID 21705230.
  83. ^ Chartrand TL, Bargh JA (June 1999). "The chameleon effect: the perception-behavior link and social interaction". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 76 (6): 893–910. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.76.6.893. PMID 10402679. S2CID 11818459.
  84. ^ Lakin JL, Chartrand TL (July 2003). "Using nonconscious behavioral mimicry to create affiliation and rapport". Psychological Science. 14 (4): 334–339. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.14481. PMID 12807406. S2CID 8458849.
  85. ^ van Baaren RB, Maddux WW, Chartrand TL, de Bouter C, van Knippenberg A (May 2003). "It takes two to mimic: behavioral consequences of self-construals". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 84 (5): 1093–1102. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.84.5.1093. hdl:2066/63130. PMID 12757151. S2CID 729948.
  86. ^ Heyes C (May 2011). "Automatic imitation". Psychological Bulletin. 137 (3): 463–483. doi:10.1037/a0022288. PMID 21280938. S2CID 6975248.
  87. ^ Paukner A, Suomi SJ, Visalberghi E, Ferrari PF (August 2009). "Capuchin monkeys display affiliation toward humans who imitate them". Science. 325 (5942): 880–883. Bibcode:2009Sci...325..880P. doi:10.1126/science.1176269. PMC 2764469. PMID 19679816.
  88. ^ a b Oberman LM, Hubbard EM, McCleery JP, Altschuler EL, Ramachandran VS, Pineda JA (July 2005). "EEG evidence for mirror neuron dysfunction in autism spectrum disorders". Brain Research. Cognitive Brain Research. 24 (2): 190–198. doi:10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.01.014. PMID 15993757.
  89. ^ Dapretto M, Davies MS, Pfeifer JH, Scott AA, Sigman M, Bookheimer SY, Iacoboni M (January 2006). "Understanding emotions in others: mirror neuron dysfunction in children with autism spectrum disorders". Nature Neuroscience. 9 (1): 28–30. doi:10.1038/nn1611. PMC 3713227. PMID 16327784.
  90. ^ Dapretto M, Davies MS, Pfeifer JH, Scott AA, Sigman M, Bookheimer SY, Iacoboni M (January 2006). "Understanding emotions in others: mirror neuron dysfunction in children with autism spectrum disorders". Nature Neuroscience. 9 (1): 28–30. doi:10.1038/nn1611. PMC 3713227. PMID 16327784.
  91. ^ Perkins T, Stokes M, McGillivray J, Bittar R (October 2010). "Mirror neuron dysfunction in autism spectrum disorders". Journal of Clinical Neuroscience. 17 (10): 1239–1243. doi:10.1016/j.jocn.2010.01.026. PMID 20598548. S2CID 15141982.
  92. ^ Oberman LM, Ramachandran VS, Pineda JA (April 2008). "Modulation of mu suppression in children with autism spectrum disorders in response to familiar or unfamiliar stimuli: the mirror neuron hypothesis". Neuropsychologia. 46 (5): 1558–1565. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.01.010. PMID 18304590. S2CID 14280719.
  93. ^ Churcland PS (2011). "6". Braintrust. Princeton University Press. p. 156.
  94. ^ Hamilton A, Marsh L (August 2013). "Two systems for action comprehension in autism: mirroring and mentalizing." (PDF). In Baron-Cohen S, Lombardo M, Tager-Flusberg H (eds.). Understanding Other Minds: Perspectives from developmental social neuroscience. Oxford: OUP. pp. 380–396. ISBN 978-0-19-166880-7.
  95. ^ Hadjikhani N, Joseph RM, Snyder J, Tager-Flusberg H (September 2006). "Anatomical differences in the mirror neuron system and social cognition network in autism". Cerebral Cortex. 16 (9): 1276–1282. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhj069. PMID 16306324.
  96. ^ a b Callaway E (12 May 2010). "Mirror neurons seen behaving normally in autism". New Scientist.
  97. ^ Hamilton AF (August 2009). "Goals, intentions and mental states: challenges for theories of autism". Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines. 50 (8): 881–892. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.621.6275. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02098.x. PMID 19508497.
  98. ^ Murphy P, Brady N, Fitzgerald M, Troje NF (December 2009). "No evidence for impaired perception of biological motion in adults with autistic spectrum disorders". Neuropsychologia. 47 (14): 3225–3235. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.026. PMID 19666038. S2CID 12495492.
  99. ^ Saygin AP, Cook J, Blakemore SJ (October 2010). Baker CI (ed.). "Unaffected perceptual thresholds for biological and non-biological form-from-motion perception in autism spectrum conditions". PLOS ONE. 5 (10): e13491. Bibcode:2010PLoSO...513491S. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0013491. PMC 2956672. PMID 20976151.
  100. ^ Cook J, Saygin AP, Swain R, Blakemore SJ (December 2009). "Reduced sensitivity to minimum-jerk biological motion in autism spectrum conditions". Neuropsychologia. 47 (14): 3275–3278. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.010. PMC 2779370. PMID 19632248.
  101. ^ Fan YT, Decety J, Yang CY, Liu JL, Cheng Y (September 2010). "Unbroken mirror neurons in autism spectrum disorders". Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines. 51 (9): 981–988. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02269.x. PMID 20524939.
  102. ^ Heyes C, Catmur C (January 2022). "What Happened to Mirror Neurons?". Perspectives on Psychological Science. 17 (1): 153–168. doi:10.1177/1745691621990638. PMC 8785302. PMID 34241539.
  103. ^ Gordon R (1986). "Folk psychology as simulation". Mind and Language. 1 (2): 158–171. doi:10.1111/j.1468-0017.1986.tb00324.x.
  104. ^ Goldman A (1989). "Interpretation psychologized". Mind and Language. 4 (3): 161–185. doi:10.1111/j.1468-0017.1989.tb00249.x.
  105. ^ Meltzoff AN, Decety J (March 2003). "What imitation tells us about social cognition: a rapprochement between developmental psychology and cognitive neuroscience". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences. 358 (1431): 491–500. doi:10.1098/rstb.2002.1261. PMC 1351349. PMID 12689375.
  106. ^ Sommerville JA, Decety J (April 2006). "Weaving the fabric of social interaction: articulating developmental psychology and cognitive neuroscience in the domain of motor cognition". Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 13 (2): 179–200. doi:10.3758/BF03193831. PMID 16892982. S2CID 14689479.
  107. ^ Keysers C, Gazzola V (May 2007). "Integrating simulation and theory of mind: from self to social cognition". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 11 (5): 194–196. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2007.02.002. PMID 17344090. S2CID 18930071.
  108. ^ Haroush K, Williams ZM (March 2015). "Neuronal prediction of opponent's behavior during cooperative social interchange in primates". Cell. 160 (6): 1233–1245. doi:10.1016/j.cell.2015.01.045. PMC 4364450. PMID 25728667.
  109. ^ Cheng YW, Tzeng OJ, Decety J, Imada T, Hsieh JC (July 2006). "Gender differences in the human mirror system: a magnetoencephalography study". NeuroReport. 17 (11): 1115–1119. doi:10.1097/01.wnr.0000223393.59328.21. PMID 16837838. S2CID 18811017.
  110. ^ Cheng Y, Decety J, Lin CP, Hsieh JC, Hung D, Tzeng OJ (June 2007). "Sex differences in spinal excitability during observation of bipedal locomotion". NeuroReport. 18 (9): 887–890. doi:10.1097/WNR.0b013e3280ebb486. PMID 17515795. S2CID 16295878.
  111. ^ Yang CY, Decety J, Lee S, Chen C, Cheng Y (January 2009). "Gender differences in the mu rhythm during empathy for pain: an electroencephalographic study". Brain Research. 1251: 176–184. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2008.11.062. PMID 19083993. S2CID 40145972.
  112. ^ Cheng Y, Lee PL, Yang CY, Lin CP, Hung D, Decety J (May 2008). "Gender differences in the mu rhythm of the human mirror-neuron system". PLOS ONE. 3 (5): e2113. Bibcode:2008PLoSO...3.2113C. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0002113. PMC 2361218. PMID 18461176.
  113. ^ Schulte-Rüther M, Markowitsch HJ, Shah NJ, Fink GR, Piefke M (August 2008). "Gender differences in brain networks supporting empathy". NeuroImage. 42 (1): 393–403. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.04.180. PMID 18514546. S2CID 10461927.
  114. ^ a b c Jalal B, Ramachandran VS (2017). "Sleep Paralysis, "The Ghostly Bedroom Intruder" and Out-of-Body Experiences: The Role of Mirror Neurons". Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 11: 92. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2017.00092. PMC 5329044. PMID 28293186.
  115. ^ McCormick LM, Brumm MC, Beadle JN, Paradiso S, Yamada T, Andreasen N (March 2012). "Mirror neuron function, psychosis, and empathy in schizophrenia". Psychiatry Research. 201 (3): 233–239. doi:10.1016/j.pscychresns.2012.01.004. PMC 3545445. PMID 22510432.
  116. ^ a b Pascolo PB, Ragogna R, Rossi R (2009). "The Mirror-Neuron System Paradigm and its consistency". Gait & Posture. 30 (Suppl. 1): 65. doi:10.1016/j.gaitpost.2009.07.064.
  117. ^ Lingnau A, Gesierich B, Caramazza A (June 2009). "Asymmetric fMRI adaptation reveals no evidence for mirror neurons in humans". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 106 (24): 9925–9930. Bibcode:2009PNAS..106.9925L. doi:10.1073/pnas.0902262106. PMC 2701024. PMID 19497880.
  118. ^ Kilner JM, Neal A, Weiskopf N, Friston KJ, Frith CD (August 2009). "Evidence of mirror neurons in human inferior frontal gyrus". The Journal of Neuroscience. 29 (32): 10153–10159. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.2668-09.2009. PMC 2788150. PMID 19675249.
  119. ^ Churchland, Patricia, Braintrust (2011), Chapter 6, page 142
  120. ^ Ramachandran VS (2000). "Mirror neurons and imitation learning as the driving force behind "the great leap forward" in human evolution". Edge. Retrieved 13 April 2013.

Further reading edit

  • Carey B (12 February 2008). "You remind me of me". The New York Times.
  • Hickok G (2014). The Myth of Mirror Neurons. National Geographic Books. ISBN 978-0-393-08961-5.
  • Keysers C (2011). The Empathic Brain. CreateSpace Independent Publishing Platform. ISBN 978-1-4637-6906-2.
  • Rizzolatti G, Sinigaglia C (2008). Mirrors in the Brain. How We Share our Actions and Emotions. Oxford (UK): Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-921798-4.
  • Morsella E, Bargh JA, Gollwitzer PM, eds. (2009). Oxford Handbook of Human Action. New York: Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-530998-0.
  • Gallese V, Gernsbacher MA, Heyes C, Hickok G, Iacoboni M (July 2011). "Mirror Neuron Forum". Perspectives on Psychological Science. 6 (4): 369–407. doi:10.1177/1745691611413392. PMC 4266473. PMID 25520744.
  • Iacoboni M, Mazziotta JC (September 2007). "Mirror neuron system: basic findings and clinical applications". Annals of Neurology. 62 (3): 213–218. doi:10.1002/ana.21198. PMID 17721988. S2CID 3225339.
  • Keysers C, Gazzola V (2006). "Towards a unifying neural theory of social cognition". Understanding Emotions. Progress in Brain Research. Vol. 156. pp. 379–401. doi:10.1016/S0079-6123(06)56021-2. ISBN 9780444521828. PMID 17015092.
  • Pascolo B (2013). "Mirror neurons: still an open question?" (PDF). Progress in Neuroscience. 1 (1–4): 25–82.
  • Preston SD, de Waal FB (February 2002). "Empathy: Its ultimate and proximate bases". The Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 25 (1): 1–72. doi:10.1017/s0140525x02000018. PMID 12625087.
  • Rizzolatti G, Fabbri-Destro M, Cattaneo L (January 2009). "Mirror neurons and their clinical relevance". Nature Clinical Practice. Neurology. 5 (1): 24–34. doi:10.1038/ncpneuro0990. PMID 19129788. S2CID 2979216.

External links edit

  • Hickok G, Poeppel D. "Talking Brains". News And Views On the Neural Organization of Language
  • Ramachandran VS. . TED talks. Archived from the original on 10 February 2012.
  • Thomas B (2012). "What's So Special about Mirror Neurons?". Scientific American Guest Blog. (an overview of prominent research approaches based on interviews with Iacoboni, Hickok, Heyes and Gallese)
  • . NOVA scienceNOW. January 2005. Archived from the original on 11 January 2006.

mirror, neuron, mirror, system, redirects, here, band, mirror, system, this, article, needs, updated, please, help, update, this, article, reflect, recent, events, newly, available, information, april, 2020, mirror, neuron, neuron, that, fires, both, when, org. Mirror system redirects here For the band see Mirror System This article needs to be updated Please help update this article to reflect recent events or newly available information April 2020 A mirror neuron is a neuron that fires both when an organism acts and when the organism observes the same action performed by another 1 2 3 Thus the neuron mirrors the behavior of the other as though the observer were itself acting Mirror neurons are not always physiologically distinct from other types of neurons in the brain their main differentiating factor is their response patterns 4 By this definition such neurons have been directly observed in humans 5 and primate species 6 and in birds 7 Mirror systemIdentifiersMeSHD059167Anatomical terms of neuroanatomy edit on Wikidata In humans brain activity consistent with that of mirror neurons has been found in the premotor cortex the supplementary motor area the primary somatosensory cortex and the inferior parietal cortex 8 The function of the mirror system in humans is a subject of much speculation Birds have been shown to have imitative resonance behaviors and neurological evidence suggests the presence of some form of mirroring system 6 9 To date no widely accepted neural or computational models have been put forward to describe how mirror neuron activity supports cognitive functions 10 11 12 The subject of mirror neurons continues to generate intense debate In 2014 Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B published a special issue entirely devoted to mirror neuron research 13 Some researchers speculate that mirror systems may simulate observed actions and thus contribute to theory of mind skills 14 15 while others relate mirror neurons to language abilities 16 Neuroscientists such as Marco Iacoboni have argued that mirror neuron systems in the human brain help humans understand the actions and intentions of other people In addition Iacoboni has argued that mirror neurons are the neural basis of the human capacity for emotions such as empathy 17 Contents 1 Discovery 2 Origin 3 In monkeys 4 In rodents 5 In humans 5 1 Development 6 Possible functions 6 1 Understanding intentions 6 2 Learning facilitation 6 3 Empathy 6 4 Human self awareness 6 5 Language 6 6 Automatic imitation 6 7 Motor mimicry 6 8 Autism 6 9 Theory of mind 6 10 Sex differences 6 11 Sleep paralysis 6 12 Mirror neuron function psychosis and empathy in schizophrenia 7 Doubts concerning mirror neurons 8 See also 9 References 10 Further reading 11 External linksDiscovery editIn the 1980s and 1990s neurophysiologists Giacomo Rizzolatti Giuseppe Di Pellegrino Luciano Fadiga Leonardo Fogassi and Vittorio Gallese at the University of Parma placed electrodes in the ventral premotor cortex of the macaque monkey to study neurons specialized in the control of hand and mouth actions for example taking hold of an object and manipulating it During each experiment the researchers allowed the monkey to reach for pieces of food and recorded from single neurons in the monkey s brain thus measuring the neuron s response to certain movements 18 19 They found that some neurons responded when the monkey observed a person picking up a piece of food and also when the monkey itself picked up the food The discovery was initially submitted to Nature but was rejected for its lack of general interest before being published in a less competitive journal 20 A few years later the same group published another empirical paper discussing the role of the mirror neuron system in action recognition and proposing that the human Broca s area was the homologue region of the monkey ventral premotor cortex 21 While these papers reported the presence of mirror neurons responding to hand actions a subsequent study by Pier Francesco Ferrari and colleagues 22 described the presence of mirror neurons responding to mouth actions and facial gestures Further experiments confirmed that about 10 of neurons in the monkey inferior frontal and inferior parietal cortex have mirror properties and give similar responses to performed hand actions and observed actions In 2002 Christian Keysers and colleagues reported that in both humans and monkeys the mirror system also responds to the sound of actions 3 23 24 Reports on mirror neurons have been widely published 21 and confirmed 25 with mirror neurons found in both inferior frontal and inferior parietal regions of the brain Recently evidence from functional neuroimaging strongly suggests that humans have similar mirror neurons systems researchers have identified brain regions which respond during both action and observation of action Not surprisingly these brain regions include those found in the macaque monkey 1 However functional magnetic resonance imaging fMRI can examine the entire brain at once and suggests that a much wider network of brain areas shows mirror properties in humans than previously thought These additional areas include the somatosensory cortex and are thought to make the observer feel what it feels like to move in the observed way 26 27 Origin editMany implicitly assume that the mirrorness of mirror neurons is due primarily to heritable genetic factors and that the genetic predisposition to develop mirror neurons evolved because they facilitate action understanding 28 In contrast a number of theoretical accounts argue that mirror neurons could simply emerge due to learned associations including the Hebbian Theory 29 the Associative Learning Theory 28 and Canalization 30 In monkeys edit nbsp Neonatal newborn macaque imitating facial expressions The first animal in which researchers have studied mirror neurons individually is the macaque monkey In these monkeys mirror neurons are found in the inferior frontal gyrus region F5 and the inferior parietal lobule 1 Mirror neurons are believed to mediate the understanding of other animals behaviour For example a mirror neuron which fires when the monkey rips a piece of paper would also fire when the monkey sees a person rip paper or hears paper ripping without visual cues These properties have led researchers to believe that mirror neurons encode abstract concepts of actions like ripping paper whether the action is performed by the monkey or another animal 1 The function of mirror neurons in macaques remains unknown Adult macaques do not seem to learn by imitation Recent experiments by Ferrari and colleagues suggest that infant macaques can imitate a human s face movements though only as neonates and during a limited temporal window 31 Even if it has not yet been empirically demonstrated it has been proposed that mirror neurons cause this behaviour and other imitative phenomena 32 Indeed there is limited understanding of the degree to which monkeys show imitative behaviour 10 In adult monkeys mirror neurons may enable the monkey to understand what another monkey is doing or to recognize the other monkey s action 33 In rodents editA number of studies have shown that rats and mice show signs of distress while witnessing another rodent receive footshocks 34 The group of Christian Keysers s recorded from neurons while rats experienced pain or witnessed the pain of others and has revealed the presence of pain mirror neurons in the rat s anterior cingulate cortex i e neurons that respond both while an animal experiences pain and while witnessing the pain of others 35 Deactivating this region of the cingulate cortex led to reduced emotional contagion in the rats so that observer rats showed reduced distress while witnessing another rat experience pain 35 The homologous part of the anterior cingulate cortex has been associated with empathy for pain in humans 36 suggesting a homology between the systems involved in emotional contagion in rodents and empathy emotional contagion for pain in humans In humans edit nbsp Diagram of the brain showing the locations of the frontal and parietal lobes of the cerebrum viewed from the left The inferior frontal lobe is the lower part of the blue area and the superior parietal lobe is the upper part of the yellow area It is not normally possible to study single neurons in the human brain so most evidence for mirror neurons in humans is indirect Brain imaging experiments using functional magnetic resonance imaging fMRI have shown that the human inferior frontal cortex and superior parietal lobe are active when the person performs an action and also when the person sees another individual performing an action It has been suggested that these brain regions contain mirror neurons and they have been defined as the human mirror neuron system 37 More recent experiments have shown that even at the level of single participants scanned using fMRI large areas containing multiple fMRI voxels increase their activity both during the observation and execution of actions 26 Neuropsychological studies looking at lesion areas that cause action knowledge pantomime interpretation and biological motion perception deficits have pointed to a causal link between the integrity of the inferior frontal gyrus and these behaviours 38 39 40 Transcranial magnetic stimulation studies have confirmed this as well 41 42 These results indicate the activation in mirror neuron related areas are unlikely to be just epiphenomenal A study published in April 2010 reports recordings from single neurons with mirror properties in the human brain 43 Mukamel et al Current Biology 2010 recorded from the brains of 21 patients who were being treated at Ronald Reagan UCLA Medical Center for intractable epilepsy The patients had been implanted with intracranial depth electrodes to identify seizure foci for potential surgical treatment Electrode location was based solely on clinical criteria the researchers with the patients consent used the same electrodes to piggyback their research The researchers found a small number of neurons that fired or showed their greatest activity both when the individual performed a task and when they observed a task Other neurons had anti mirror properties they responded when the participant performed an action but were inhibited when the participant saw that action The mirror neurons found were located in the supplementary motor area and medial temporal cortex other brain regions were not sampled For purely practical reasons these regions are not the same as those in which mirror neurons had been recorded from in the monkey researchers in Parma were studying the ventral premotor cortex and the associated inferior parietal lobe two regions in which epilepsy rarely occurs and hence single cell recordings in these regions are not usually done in humans On the other hand no one has to date looked for mirror neurons in the supplementary motor area or the medial temporal lobe in the monkey Together this therefore does not suggest that humans and monkeys have mirror neurons in different locations but rather that they may have mirror neurons both in the ventral premotor cortex and inferior parietal lobe where they have been recorded in the monkey and in the supplementary motor areas and medial temporal lobe where they have been recorded from in human especially because detailed human fMRI analyses suggest activity compatible with the presence of mirror neurons in all these regions 26 Another study has suggested that human beings do not necessarily have more mirror neurons than monkeys but instead that there is a core set of mirror neurons used in action observation and execution However for other proposed functions of mirror neurons the mirror system may have the ability to recruit other areas of the brain when doing its auditory somatosensory and affective components 44 Development edit Human infant data using eye tracking measures suggest that the mirror neuron system develops before 12 months of age and that this system may help human infants understand other people s actions 45 A critical question concerns how mirror neurons acquire mirror properties Two closely related models postulate that mirror neurons are trained through Hebbian 46 or Associative learning 47 48 12 see Associative Sequence Learning However if premotor neurons need to be trained by action in order to acquire mirror properties it is unclear how newborn babies are able to mimic the facial gestures of another person imitation of unseen actions as suggested by the work of Meltzoff and Moore One possibility is that the sight of tongue protrusion recruits an innate releasing mechanism in neonates Careful analysis suggests that imitation of this single gesture may account for almost all reports of facial mimicry by new born infants 49 Possible functions editUnderstanding intentions edit Many studies link mirror neurons to understanding goals and intentions Fogassi et al 2005 25 recorded the activity of 41 mirror neurons in the inferior parietal lobe IPL of two rhesus macaques The IPL has long been recognized as an association cortex that integrates sensory information The monkeys watched an experimenter either grasp an apple and bring it to his mouth or grasp an object and place it in a cup In total 15 mirror neurons fired vigorously when the monkey observed the grasp to eat motion but registered no activity while exposed to the grasp to place condition For 4 other mirror neurons the reverse held true they activated in response to the experimenter eventually placing the apple in the cup but not to eating it Only the type of action and not the kinematic force with which models manipulated objects determined neuron activity It was also significant that neurons fired before the monkey observed the human model starting the second motor act bringing the object to the mouth or placing it in a cup Therefore IPL neurons code the same act grasping in a different way according to the final goal of the action in which the act is embedded 25 They may furnish a neural basis for predicting another individual s subsequent actions and inferring intention 25 Learning facilitation edit Another possible function of mirror neurons would be facilitation of learning The mirror neurons code the concrete representation of the action i e the representation that would be activated if the observer acted This would allow us to simulate to repeat internally the observed action implicitly in the brain to collect our own motor programs of observed actions and to get ready to reproduce the actions later It is implicit training Due to this the observer will produce the action explicitly in his her behavior with agility and finesse This happens due to associative learning processes The more frequently a synaptic connection is activated the stronger it becomes 50 Empathy edit Stephanie Preston and Frans de Waal 51 Jean Decety 52 53 and Vittorio Gallese 54 55 and Christian Keysers 3 have independently argued that the mirror neuron system is involved in empathy A large number of experiments using fMRI electroencephalography EEG and magnetoencephalography MEG have shown that certain brain regions in particular the anterior insula anterior cingulate cortex and inferior frontal cortex are active when people experience an emotion disgust happiness pain etc and when they see another person experiencing an emotion 56 57 58 59 60 61 62 David Freedberg and Vittorio Gallese have also put forward the idea that this function of the mirror neuron system is crucial for aesthetic experiences 63 Nevertheless an experiment aimed at investigating the activity of mirror neurons in empathy conducted by Soukayna Bekkali and Peter Enticott at the University of Deakin yielded a different result After analyzing the report s data they came up with two conclusions about motor empathy and emotional empathy First there is no relationship between motor empathy and the activity of mirror neurons Second there is only weak evidence of these neurons activity in the inferior frontal gyrus IFG and no evidence of emotional empathy associated with mirror neurons in key brain regions inferior parietal lobule IPL In other words there has not been an exact conclusion about the role of mirror neurons in empathy and if they are essential for human empathy 64 However these brain regions are not quite the same as the ones which mirror hand actions and mirror neurons for emotional states or empathy have not yet been described in monkeys In a recent study done in 2022 sixteen hand actions were given for each assignment The assignment pictured both an activity word phase and the intended word phase The hand actions were selected in trails each introduced twice One of the times was with a matching phase and the other time was with a misleading word phase The action words were depicted in two to three words with each beginning with the word to For instance to point action or to spin intention Participants were expected to answer whether the correct word phase matched the corresponding action or intention word The word phase had to be answered within 3000 ms with a 1000 ms black screen between each image The black screens purpose was for an adequate amount of time in between responses Participants pressed on the keyboard x or m to indicate their responses in a yes no format 65 Christian Keysers at the Social Brain Lab and colleagues have shown that people who are more empathic according to self report questionnaires have stronger activations both in the mirror system for hand actions 66 and the mirror system for emotions 61 providing more direct support for the idea that the mirror system is linked to empathy Some researchers observed that the human mirror system does not passively respond to the observation of actions but is influenced by the mindset of the observer 67 Researchers observed the link of the mirror neurons during empathetic engagement in patient care 68 Studies in rats have shown that the anterior cingulate cortex contains mirror neurons for pain i e neurons responding both during the first hand experience of pain and while witnessing the pain of others 35 and inhibition of this region leads to reduced emotional contagion in rats 35 and mice 34 and reduced aversion towards harming others 69 This provides causal evidence for a link between pain mirror neurons and emotional contagion and prosocial behavior two phenomena associated with empathy in rodents That brain activity in the homologous brain region is associated with individual variability in empathy in humans 36 suggests that a similar mechanism may be at play across mammals Human self awareness edit V S Ramachandran has speculated that mirror neurons may provide the neurological basis of human self awareness 70 In an essay written for the Edge Foundation in 2009 Ramachandran gave the following explanation of his theory I also speculated that these neurons can not only help simulate other people s behavior but can be turned inward as it were to create second order representations or meta representations of your own earlier brain processes This could be the neural basis of introspection and of the reciprocity of self awareness and other awareness There is obviously a chicken or egg question here as to which evolved first but The main point is that the two co evolved mutually enriching each other to create the mature representation of self that characterizes modern humans 71 Language edit In humans functional MRI studies have reported finding areas homologous to the monkey mirror neuron system in the inferior frontal cortex close to Broca s area one of the hypothesized language regions of the brain This has led to suggestions that human language evolved from a gesture performance understanding system implemented in mirror neurons Mirror neurons have been said to have the potential to provide a mechanism for action understanding imitation learning and the simulation of other people s behaviour 72 This hypothesis is supported by some cytoarchitectonic homologies between monkey premotor area F5 and human Broca s area 73 Rates of vocabulary expansion link to the ability of children to vocally mirror non words and so to acquire the new word pronunciations Such speech repetition occurs automatically fast 74 and separately in the brain to speech perception 75 76 Moreover such vocal imitation can occur without comprehension such as in speech shadowing 77 and echolalia 78 Further evidence for this link comes from a recent study in which the brain activity of two participants was measured using fMRI while they were gesturing words to each other using hand gestures with a game of charades a modality that some have suggested might represent the evolutionary precursor of human language Analysis of the data using Granger Causality revealed that the mirror neuron system of the observer indeed reflects the pattern of activity in the motor system of the sender supporting the idea that the motor concept associated with the words is indeed transmitted from one brain to another using the mirror system 79 The mirror neuron system seems to be inherently inadequate to play any role in syntax given that this definitory property of human languages which is implemented in hierarchical recursive structure is flattened into linear sequences of phonemes making the recursive structure not accessible to sensory detection 80 Automatic imitation edit The term is commonly used to refer to cases in which an individual having observed a body movement unintentionally performs a similar body movement or alters the way that a body movement is performed Automatic imitation rarely involves overt execution of matching responses Instead the effects typically consist of reaction time rather than accuracy differences between compatible and incompatible trials Research reveals that the existence of automatic imitation which is a covert form of imitation is distinct from spatial compatibility It also indicates that although automatic imitation is subject to input modulation by attentional processes and output modulation by inhibitory processes it is mediated by learned long term sensorimotor associations that cannot be altered directly by intentional processes Many researchers believe that automatic imitation is mediated by the mirror neuron system 81 Additionally there are data that demonstrate that our postural control is impaired when people listen to sentences about other actions For example if the task is to maintain posture people do it worse when they listen to sentences like this I get up put on my slippers go to the bathroom This phenomenon may be due to the fact that during action perception there is similar motor cortex activation as if a human being performed the same action mirror neurons system 82 Motor mimicry edit In contrast with automatic imitation motor mimicry is observed in 1 naturalistic social situations and 2 via measures of action frequency within a session rather than measures of speed and or accuracy within trials 83 The integration of research on motor mimicry and automatic imitation could reveal plausible indications that these phenomena depend on the same psychological and neural processes Preliminary evidence however comes from studies showing that social priming has similar effects on motor mimicry 84 85 Nevertheless the similarities between automatic imitation mirror effects and motor mimicry have led some researchers to propose that automatic imitation is mediated by the mirror neuron system and that it is a tightly controlled laboratory equivalent of the motor mimicry observed in naturalistic social contexts If true then automatic imitation can be used as a tool to investigate how the mirror neuron system contributes to cognitive functioning and how motor mimicry promotes prosocial attitudes and behavior 86 87 Meta analysis of imitation studies in humans suggest that there is enough evidence of mirror system activation during imitation that mirror neuron involvement is likely even though no published studies have recorded the activities of singular neurons However it is likely insufficient for motor imitation Studies show that regions of the frontal and parietal lobes that extend beyond the classical mirror system are equally activated during imitation This suggests that other areas along with the mirror system are crucial to imitation behaviors 8 Autism edit It has also been proposed that problems with the mirror neuron system may underlie cognitive disorders particularly autism 88 89 However the connection between mirror neuron dysfunction and autism is tentative and it remains to be demonstrated how mirror neurons are related to many of the important characteristics of autism 10 Some researchers claim there is a link between mirror neuron deficiency and autism EEG recordings from motor areas are suppressed when someone watches another person move a signal that may relate to mirror neuron system This suppression was less in children with autism 88 Although these findings have been replicated by several groups 90 91 other studies have not found evidence of a dysfunctional mirror neuron system in autism 10 In 2008 Oberman et al published a research paper that presented conflicting EEG evidence Oberman and Ramachandran found typical mu suppression for familiar stimuli but not for unfamiliar stimuli leading them to conclude that the mirror neuron system of children with ASD Autism Spectrum Disorder was functional but less sensitive than that of typical children 92 Based on the conflicting evidence presented by mu wave suppression experiments Patricia Churchland has cautioned that mu wave suppression results cannot be used as a valid index for measuring the performance of mirror neuron systems 93 Recent research indicates that mirror neurons do not play a role in autism no clear cut evidence emerges for a fundamental mirror system deficit in autism Behavioural studies have shown that people with autism have a good understanding of action goals Furthermore two independent neuroimaging studies have reported that the parietal component of the mirror system is functioning typically in individuals with autism 94 Some anatomical differences have been found in the mirror neuron related brain areas in adults with autism spectrum disorders compared to non autistic adults All these cortical areas were thinner and the degree of thinning was correlated with autism symptom severity a correlation nearly restricted to these brain regions 95 Based on these results some researchers claim that autism is caused by impairments in the mirror neuron system leading to disabilities in social skills imitation empathy and theory of mind who Many researchers have pointed out that the broken mirrors theory of autism is overly simplistic and mirror neurons alone cannot explain the differences found in individuals with autism First of all as noted above none of these studies were direct measures of mirror neuron activity in other words fMRI activity or EEG rhythm suppression do not unequivocally index mirror neurons Dinstein and colleagues found normal mirror neuron activity in people with autism using fMRI 96 In individuals with autism deficits in intention understanding action understanding and biological motion perception the key functions of mirror neurons are not always found 97 98 or are task dependent 99 100 Today very few people believe an all or nothing problem with the mirror system can underlie autism Instead additional research needs to be done and more caution should be used when reaching out to the media 101 Research from 2010 96 concluded that autistic individuals do not exhibit mirror neuron dysfunction although the small sample size limits the extent to which these results can be generalized A more recent review argued there was not enough neurological evidence to support this broken mirror theory of autism 102 Theory of mind edit In Philosophy of mind mirror neurons have become the primary rallying call of simulation theorists concerning our theory of mind Theory of mind refers to our ability to infer another person s mental state i e beliefs and desires from experiences or their behaviour There are several competing models which attempt to account for our theory of mind the most notable in relation to mirror neurons is simulation theory According to simulation theory theory of mind is available because we subconsciously empathize with the person we re observing and accounting for relevant differences imagine what we would desire and believe in that scenario 103 104 Mirror neurons have been interpreted as the mechanism by which we simulate others in order to better understand them and therefore their discovery has been taken by some as a validation of simulation theory which appeared a decade before the discovery of mirror neurons 54 More recently Theory of Mind and Simulation have been seen as complementary systems with different developmental time courses 105 106 107 At the neuronal level in a 2015 study by Keren Haroush and Ziv Williams using jointly interacting primates performing an iterated prisoner s dilemma game the authors identified neurons in the anterior cingulate cortex that selectively predicted an opponent s yet unknown decisions or covert state of mind These other predictive neurons differentiated between self and other decisions and were uniquely sensitive to social context but they did not encode the opponent s observed actions or receipt of reward These cingulate cells may therefore importantly complement the function of mirror neurons by providing additional information about other social agents that is not immediately observable or known 108 Sex differences edit A series of recent studies conducted by Yawei Cheng using a variety of neurophysiological measures including MEG 109 spinal reflex excitability 110 electroencephalography 111 112 have documented the presence of a gender difference in the human mirror neuron system with female participants exhibiting stronger motor resonance than male participants In another study sex based differences among mirror neuron mechanisms was reinforced in that the data showed enhanced empathetic ability in females relative to males citation needed During an emotional social interaction females showed a greater ability in emotional perspective taking clarification needed than did males when interacting with another person face to face However in the study data showed that when it came to recognizing the emotions of others all participants abilities were very similar and there was no key difference between the male and female subjects 113 Sleep paralysis editBaland Jalal and V S Ramachandran have hypothesized that the mirror neuron system is important in giving rise to the intruder hallucination and out of body experiences during sleep paralysis 114 According to this theory sleep paralysis leads to disinhibition of the mirror neuron system paving the way for hallucinations of human like shadowy beings The deafferentation of sensory information during sleep paralysis is proposed as the mechanism for such mirror neuron disinhibition 114 The authors suggest that their hypothesis on the role of the mirror neuron system could be tested These ideas could be explored using neuroimaging to examine the selective activation of brain regions associated with mirror neuron activity when the individual is hallucinating an intruder or having an out of body experience during sleep paralysis 114 Mirror neuron function psychosis and empathy in schizophrenia edit Recent research which measured mu wave suppression suggests that mirror neuron activity is positively correlated with psychotic symptoms i e greater mu suppression mirror neuron activity was highest among subjects with the greater severity of psychotic symptoms Researchers concluded that higher mirror neuron activity may be the underpinning of schizophrenia sensory gating deficits and may contribute to sensory misattributions particularly in response to socially relevant stimuli and be a putative mechanism for delusions and hallucinations 115 Doubts concerning mirror neurons editAlthough some in the scientific community have expressed excitement about the discovery of mirror neurons there are scientists who have expressed doubts about both the existence and role of mirror neurons in humans The consensus today seems to be that the importance of so called mirror neurons is widely overblown According to scientists such as Hickok Pascolo and Dinstein it is not clear whether mirror neurons really form a distinct class of cells as opposed to an occasional phenomenon seen in cells that have other functions 116 and whether mirror activity is a distinct type of response or simply an artifact of an overall facilitation of the motor system 11 In 2008 Ilan Dinstein et al argued that the original analyses were unconvincing because they were based on qualitative descriptions of individual cell properties and did not take into account the small number of strongly mirror selective neurons in motor areas 10 Other scientists have argued that the measurements of neuron fire delay seem not to be compatible with standard reaction times 116 and pointed out that nobody has reported that an interruption of the motor areas in F5 would produce a decrease in action recognition 11 Critics of this argument have replied that these authors have missed human neuropsychological and TMS studies reporting disruption of these areas do indeed cause action deficits 39 41 without affecting other kinds of perception 40 In 2009 Lingnau et al carried out an experiment in which they compared motor acts that were first observed and then executed to motor acts that were first executed and then observed They concluded that there was a significant asymmetry between the two processes that indicated that mirror neurons do not exist in humans They stated Crucially we found no signs of adaptation for motor acts that were first executed and then observed Failure to find cross modal adaptation for executed and observed motor acts is not compatible with the core assumption of mirror neuron theory which holds that action recognition and understanding are based on motor simulation 117 However in the same year Kilner et al showed that if goal directed actions are used as stimuli both IPL and premotor regions show the repetition suppression between observation and execution that is predicted by mirror neurons 118 In 2009 Greg Hickok published an extensive argument against the claim that mirror neurons are involved in action understanding Eight Problems for the Mirror Neuron Theory of Action Understanding in Monkeys and Humans He concluded that The early hypothesis that these cells underlie action understanding is likewise an interesting and prima facie reasonable idea However despite its widespread acceptance the proposal has never been adequately tested in monkeys and in humans there is strong empirical evidence in the form of physiological and neuropsychological double dissociations against the claim 11 nbsp The mirror neurons can be activated only after the goal of the observed action has been attributed by other brain structures Vladimir Kosonogov sees another contradiction The proponents of mirror neuron theory of action understanding postulate that the mirror neurons code the goals of others actions because they are activated if the observed action is goal directed However the mirror neurons are activated only when the observed action is goal directed object directed action or a communicative gesture which certainly has a goal too How do they know that the definite action is goal directed At what stage of their activation do they detect a goal of the movement or its absence In his opinion the mirror neuron system can be activated only after the goal of the observed action is attributed by some other brain structures 50 Neurophilosophers such as Patricia Churchland have expressed both scientific and philosophical objections to the theory that mirror neurons are responsible for understanding the intentions of others In chapter 5 of her 2011 book Braintrust Churchland points out that the claim that mirror neurons are involved in understanding intentions through simulating observed actions is based on assumptions that are clouded by unresolved philosophical issues She makes the argument that intentions are understood coded at a more complex level of neural activity than that of individual neurons Churchland states that A neuron though computationally complex is just a neuron It is not an intelligent homunculus If a neural network represents something complex such as an intention to insult it must have the right input and be in the right place in the neural circuitry to do that 119 Cecilia Heyes has advanced the theory that mirror neurons are the byproduct of associative learning as opposed to evolutionary adaptation She argues that mirror neurons in humans are the product of social interaction and not an evolutionary adaptation for action understanding In particular Heyes rejects the theory advanced by V S Ramachandran that mirror neurons have been the driving force behind the great leap forward in human evolution 12 120 See also editAssociative sequence learning Common coding theory Emotional contagion Empathy Mirror touch synesthesia Mirroring psychology Motor cognition Motor theory of speech perception On Intelligence Positron emission tomography Simulation theory of empathy Speech repetition Spindle neuronReferences edit a b c d Rizzolatti G Craighero L 2004 The mirror neuron system Annual Review of Neuroscience 27 1 169 192 doi 10 1146 annurev neuro 27 070203 144230 PMID 15217330 S2CID 1729870 Keysers C November 2009 Mirror neurons Current Biology 19 21 R971 R973 doi 10 1016 j cub 2009 08 026 hdl 20 500 11755 c62977db 8a4e 4cc9 b82a 5013b52a0bc6 PMID 19922849 S2CID 12668046 a b c Keysers C 2011 06 23 The Empathic Brain Kindle Acharya Sourya Shukla Samarth 2012 Mirror neurons Enigma of the metaphysical modular brain Journal of Natural Science Biology and Medicine 3 2 118 124 doi 10 4103 0976 9668 101878 PMC 3510904 PMID 23225972 Mukamel R Ekstrom AD Kaplan J Iacoboni M Fried I April 2010 Single neuron responses in humans during execution and observation of actions Current Biology 20 8 750 756 doi 10 1016 j cub 2010 02 045 PMC 2904852 PMID 20381353 a b Rizzolatti G Fadiga L Fogassi L Gallese V May 1999 Resonance behaviors and mirror neurons Archives Italiennes de Biologie 137 2 3 85 100 PMID 10349488 Prather JF Peters S Nowicki S Mooney R January 2008 Precise auditory vocal mirroring in neurons for learned vocal communication Nature 451 7176 305 310 Bibcode 2008Natur 451 305P doi 10 1038 nature06492 PMID 18202651 S2CID 4344150 a b Molenberghs P Cunnington R Mattingley JB July 2009 Is the mirror neuron system involved in imitation A short review and meta analysis Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 33 7 975 980 doi 10 1016 j neubiorev 2009 03 010 PMID 19580913 S2CID 25620637 Akins CK Klein ED Zentall TR August 2002 Imitative learning in Japanese quail Coturnix japonica using the bidirectional control procedure Animal Learning amp Behavior 30 3 275 281 doi 10 3758 bf03192836 PMID 12391793 a b c d e Dinstein I Thomas C Behrmann M Heeger DJ January 2008 A mirror up to nature Current Biology 18 1 R13 R18 doi 10 1016 j cub 2007 11 004 PMC 2517574 PMID 18177704 a b c d Hickok G July 2009 Eight problems for the mirror neuron theory of action understanding in monkeys and humans Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 21 7 1229 1243 doi 10 1162 jocn 2009 21189 PMC 2773693 PMID 19199415 a b c Heyes Cecilia March 2010 Where do mirror neurons come from Neuroscience amp Biobehavioral Reviews 34 4 575 583 doi 10 1016 j neubiorev 2009 11 007 PMID 19914284 S2CID 2578537 Ferrari PF Rizzolatti G 2014 Mirror neuron research the past and the future Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B Biological Sciences 369 1644 20130169 doi 10 1098 rstb 2013 0169 PMC 4006175 PMID 24778369 Keysers C Gazzola V 2006 Towards a unifying neural theory of social cognition Understanding Emotions Progress in Brain Research Vol 156 pp 379 401 doi 10 1016 S0079 6123 06 56021 2 ISBN 9780444521828 PMID 17015092 Arbib MA April 2012 How the brain got language The mirror system hypothesis Oxford University Press Theoret H Pascual Leone A October 2002 Language acquisition do as you hear Current Biology 12 21 R736 R737 doi 10 1016 S0960 9822 02 01251 4 PMID 12419204 Blakeslee S January 10 2006 Cells That Read Minds New York Times Science di Pellegrino G Fadiga L Fogassi L Gallese V Rizzolatti G 1992 Understanding motor events a neurophysiological study Experimental Brain Research 91 1 176 180 doi 10 1007 bf00230027 PMID 1301372 S2CID 206772150 Rizzolatti G Fadiga L Gallese V Fogassi L March 1996 Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions Brain Research Cognitive Brain Research 3 2 131 141 doi 10 1016 0926 6410 95 00038 0 PMID 8713554 Rizzolatti G Fabbri Destro M January 2010 Mirror neurons from discovery to autism Experimental Brain Research 200 3 4 223 237 doi 10 1007 s00221 009 2002 3 PMID 19760408 S2CID 3342808 a b Gallese V Fadiga L Fogassi L Rizzolatti G April 1996 Action recognition in the premotor cortex Brain 119 Pt 2 2 593 609 doi 10 1093 brain 119 2 593 PMID 8800951 Ferrari PF Gallese V Rizzolatti G Fogassi L April 2003 Mirror neurons responding to the observation of ingestive and communicative mouth actions in the monkey ventral premotor cortex The European Journal of Neuroscience 17 8 1703 1714 doi 10 1046 j 1460 9568 2003 02601 x PMID 12752388 S2CID 1915143 Kohler E Keysers C Umilta MA Fogassi L Gallese V Rizzolatti G August 2002 Hearing sounds understanding actions action representation in mirror neurons Science 297 5582 New York N Y 846 8 Bibcode 2002Sci 297 846K doi 10 1126 science 1070311 PMID 12161656 S2CID 16923101 Gazzola V Aziz Zadeh L Keysers C September 2006 Empathy and the somatotopic auditory mirror system in humans Current Biology 16 18 1824 9 doi 10 1016 j cub 2006 07 072 PMID 16979560 S2CID 5223812 a b c d Fogassi L Ferrari PF Gesierich B Rozzi S Chersi F Rizzolatti G April 2005 Parietal lobe from action organization to intention understanding Science 308 5722 New York N Y 662 7 Bibcode 2005Sci 308 662F doi 10 1126 science 1106138 PMID 15860620 S2CID 5720234 a b c Gazzola V Keysers C June 2009 The observation and execution of actions share motor and somatosensory voxels in all tested subjects single subject analyses of unsmoothed fMRI data Cerebral Cortex 19 6 1239 1255 doi 10 1093 cercor bhn181 PMC 2677653 PMID 19020203 Keysers C Kaas JH Gazzola V June 2010 Somatosensation in social perception Nature Reviews Neuroscience 11 6 417 428 doi 10 1038 nrn2833 PMID 20445542 S2CID 12221575 a b Cook R Bird G Catmur C Press C Heyes C April 2014 Mirror neurons from origin to function PDF The Behavioral and Brain Sciences 37 2 177 192 doi 10 1017 S0140525X13000903 PMID 24775147 S2CID 52873194 Keysers C Gazzola V 2014 Hebbian learning and predictive mirror neurons for actions sensations and emotions Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B Biological Sciences 369 1644 20130175 doi 10 1098 rstb 2013 0175 PMC 4006178 PMID 24778372 Del Giudice M Manera V Keysers C March 2009 Programmed to learn The ontogeny of mirror neurons Developmental Science 12 2 350 363 doi 10 1111 j 1467 7687 2008 00783 x hdl 2318 133096 PMID 19143807 S2CID 15093476 Ferrari PF Visalberghi E Paukner A Fogassi L Ruggiero A Suomi SJ September 2006 Neonatal imitation in rhesus macaques PLOS Biology 4 9 e302 doi 10 1371 journal pbio 0040302 PMC 1560174 PMID 16953662 Ferrari PF Bonini L Fogassi L August 2009 From monkey mirror neurons to primate behaviours possible direct and indirect pathways Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B Biological Sciences 364 1528 2311 23 doi 10 1098 rstb 2009 0062 PMC 2865083 PMID 19620103 Rizzolatti G Arbib MA May 1998 Language within our grasp Trends in Neurosciences 21 5 188 194 doi 10 1016 S0166 2236 98 01260 0 hdl 11858 00 001M 0000 002C 4B59 6 PMID 9610880 S2CID 679023 a b Keum S Shin HS October 2019 Neural Basis of Observational Fear Learning A Potential Model of Affective Empathy Neuron 104 1 78 86 doi 10 1016 j neuron 2019 09 013 PMID 31600517 S2CID 203932127 a b c d Carrillo M Han Y Migliorati F Liu M Gazzola V Keysers C April 2019 Emotional Mirror Neurons in the Rat s Anterior Cingulate Cortex Current Biology 29 8 1301 1312 e6 doi 10 1016 j cub 2019 03 024 PMC 6488290 PMID 30982647 a b Lamm C Decety J Singer T February 2011 Meta analytic evidence for common and distinct neural networks associated with directly experienced pain and empathy for pain NeuroImage 54 3 2492 2502 doi 10 1016 j neuroimage 2010 10 014 PMID 20946964 S2CID 6021487 Iacoboni M Woods RP Brass M Bekkering H Mazziotta JC Rizzolatti G December 1999 Cortical mechanisms of human imitation Science 286 5449 2526 2528 doi 10 1126 science 286 5449 2526 PMID 10617472 Saygin AP Wilson SM Dronkers NF Bates E 2004 Action comprehension in aphasia linguistic and non linguistic deficits and their lesion correlates Neuropsychologia 42 13 1788 1804 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 544 9071 doi 10 1016 j neuropsychologia 2004 04 016 PMID 15351628 S2CID 11622224 a b Tranel D Kemmerer D Adolphs R Damasio H Damasio AR May 2003 Neural correlates of conceptual knowledge for actions Cognitive Neuropsychology 20 3 409 432 doi 10 1080 02643290244000248 PMID 20957578 S2CID 16131 a b Saygin AP September 2007 Superior temporal and premotor brain areas necessary for biological motion perception Brain 130 Pt 9 2452 2461 doi 10 1093 brain awm162 PMID 17660183 a b Pobric G Hamilton AF March 2006 Action understanding requires the left inferior frontal cortex Current Biology 16 5 524 529 doi 10 1016 j cub 2006 01 033 PMID 16527749 Candidi M Urgesi C Ionta S Aglioti SM 2008 Virtual lesion of ventral premotor cortex impairs visual perception of biomechanically possible but not impossible actions Social Neuroscience 3 3 4 388 400 doi 10 1080 17470910701676269 PMID 18979387 S2CID 37390465 Keysers C Gazzola V April 2010 Social neuroscience mirror neurons recorded in humans Current Biology 20 8 R353 R354 doi 10 1016 j cub 2010 03 013 hdl 20 500 11755 351f0172 b06b 41de a013 852c64e197fa PMID 21749952 S2CID 3609747 Molenberghs P Cunnington R Mattingley JB January 2012 Brain regions with mirror properties a meta analysis of 125 human fMRI studies Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 36 1 341 349 doi 10 1016 j neubiorev 2011 07 004 PMID 21782846 S2CID 37871374 Falck Ytter T Gredeback G von Hofsten C July 2006 Infants predict other people s action goals Nature Neuroscience 9 7 878 9 doi 10 1038 nn1729 PMID 16783366 S2CID 2409686 Keysers C Perrett DI November 2004 Demystifying social cognition a Hebbian perspective Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8 11 501 507 doi 10 1016 j tics 2004 09 005 PMID 15491904 S2CID 8039741 Heyes C June 2001 Causes and consequences of imitation Trends in Cognitive Sciences 5 6 253 261 doi 10 1016 S1364 6613 00 01661 2 PMID 11390296 S2CID 15602731 Brass M Heyes C October 2005 Imitation is cognitive neuroscience solving the correspondence problem Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9 10 489 95 doi 10 1016 j tics 2005 08 007 PMID 16126449 S2CID 1594505 Anisfeld M 1996 Only tongue protruding modeling is matched by neonates Developmental Review 16 2 149 161 doi 10 1006 drev 1996 0006 a b Kosonogov V December 2012 Why the Mirror Neurons Cannot Support Action Understanding Neurophysiology 44 6 499 502 doi 10 1007 s11062 012 9327 4 S2CID 254867235 Preston SD de Waal FB February 2002 Empathy Its ultimate and proximate bases The Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25 1 1 72 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 554 2794 doi 10 1017 s0140525x02000018 PMID 12625087 Decety J 2002 Naturaliser l empathie Empathy naturalized L Encephale in French 28 1 9 20 OCLC 110778688 INIST 13554627 Decety J Jackson PL June 2004 The functional architecture of human empathy Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews 3 2 71 100 doi 10 1177 1534582304267187 PMID 15537986 S2CID 145310279 a b Gallese V Goldman A December 1998 Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind reading Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2 12 493 501 doi 10 1016 s1364 6613 98 01262 5 PMID 21227300 S2CID 10108122 Gallese V 2001 The Shared Manifold hypothesis from mirror neurons to empathy Journal of Consciousness Studies 8 33 50 Botvinick M Jha AP Bylsma LM Fabian SA Solomon PE Prkachin KM March 2005 Viewing facial expressions of pain engages cortical areas involved in the direct experience of pain NeuroImage 25 1 312 319 doi 10 1016 j neuroimage 2004 11 043 PMID 15734365 S2CID 24988672 Cheng Y Yang CY Lin CP Lee PL Decety J May 2008 The perception of pain in others suppresses somatosensory oscillations a magnetoencephalography study NeuroImage 40 4 1833 1840 doi 10 1016 j neuroimage 2008 01 064 PMID 18353686 S2CID 1827514 Morrison I Lloyd D di Pellegrino G Roberts N June 2004 Vicarious responses to pain in anterior cingulate cortex is empathy a multisensory issue Cognitive Affective amp Behavioral Neuroscience 4 2 270 278 doi 10 3758 CABN 4 2 270 PMID 15460933 Wicker B Keysers C Plailly J Royet JP Gallese V Rizzolatti G October 2003 Both of us disgusted in My insula the common neural basis of seeing and feeling disgust Neuron 40 3 655 64 doi 10 1016 s0896 6273 03 00679 2 PMID 14642287 S2CID 766157 Singer T Seymour B O Doherty J Kaube H Dolan RJ Frith CD February 2004 Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain Science 303 5661 New York N Y 1157 1162 Bibcode 2004Sci 303 1157S doi 10 1126 science 1093535 hdl 21 11116 0000 0001 A020 5 PMID 14976305 S2CID 14727944 a b Jabbi M Swart M Keysers C February 2007 Empathy for positive and negative emotions in the gustatory cortex NeuroImage 34 4 1744 1753 doi 10 1016 j neuroimage 2006 10 032 PMID 17175173 S2CID 13988152 Lamm C Batson CD Decety J January 2007 The neural substrate of human empathy effects of perspective taking and cognitive appraisal Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 19 1 42 58 doi 10 1162 jocn 2007 19 1 42 PMID 17214562 S2CID 2828843 Freedberg D Gallese V May 2007 Motion emotion and empathy in esthetic experience Trends in Cognitive Sciences 11 5 197 203 doi 10 1016 j tics 2007 02 003 PMID 17347026 S2CID 1996468 Jarrett C March 25 2019 There is Only Weak Evidence that Mirror Neurons Underlie Human Empathy New Review and Meta Analysis Research Digest dead link Thompson EL Bird G Catmur C November 2022 Mirror neuron brain regions contribute to identifying actions but not intentions Human Brain Mapping 43 16 4901 4913 doi 10 1002 hbm 26036 PMC 9582378 PMID 35906896 Gazzola Aziz Zadeh and Keysers 2006 Current Biology PDF Archived from the original PDF on 2007 06 30 Molenberghs P Hayward L Mattingley JB Cunnington R January 2012 Activation patterns during action observation are modulated by context in mirror system areas NeuroImage 59 1 608 615 doi 10 1016 j neuroimage 2011 07 080 PMID 21840404 S2CID 13951700 Hojat M Louis DZ Maio V Gonnella JS 2013 Empathy and health care quality American Journal of Medical Quality 28 1 6 7 doi 10 1177 1062860612464731 PMID 23288854 S2CID 12645544 Hernandez Lallement J Attah AT Soyman E Pinhal CM Gazzola V Keysers C March 2020 Harm to Others Acts as a Negative Reinforcer in Rats Current Biology 30 6 949 961 e7 doi 10 1016 j cub 2020 01 017 hdl 20 500 11755 ee7ae8ac 7393 4276 84ce 1bad1b8e5e0d PMID 32142701 S2CID 212424287 Oberman L Ramachandran VS 2009 Reflections on the Mirror Neuron System Their Evolutionary Functions Beyond Motor Representation In Pineda JA ed Mirror Neuron Systems The Role of Mirroring Processes in Social Cognition Humana Press pp 39 62 ISBN 978 1 934115 34 3 Ramachandran VS January 1 2009 Self Awareness The Last Frontier Edge Foundation web essay Retrieved July 26 2011 Skoyles JR 2000 Gesture language origins and right handedness PDF Psycoloquy 11 24 Petrides M Cadoret G Mackey S June 2005 Orofacial somatomotor responses in the macaque monkey homologue of Broca s area Nature 435 7046 1235 1238 Bibcode 2005Natur 435 1235P doi 10 1038 nature03628 PMID 15988526 S2CID 4397762 Porter RJ Lubker JF September 1980 Rapid reproduction of vowel vowel sequences evidence for a fast and direct acoustic motoric linkage in speech Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 23 3 593 602 doi 10 1044 jshr 2303 593 PMID 7421161 McCarthy R Warrington EK June 1984 A two route model of speech production Evidence from aphasia Brain 107 Pt 2 2 463 485 doi 10 1093 brain 107 2 463 PMID 6722512 McCarthy RA Warrington EK 2001 Repeating without semantics surface dysphasia Neurocase 7 1 77 87 doi 10 1093 neucas 7 1 77 PMID 11239078 S2CID 12988855 Marslen Wilson W August 1973 Linguistic structure and speech shadowing at very short latencies Nature 244 5417 522 523 Bibcode 1973Natur 244 522M doi 10 1038 244522a0 PMID 4621131 S2CID 4220775 Fay WH Coleman RO July 1977 A human sound transducer reproducer temporal capabilities of a profoundly echolalic child Brain and Language 4 3 396 402 doi 10 1016 0093 934X 77 90034 7 PMID 907878 S2CID 29492873 Schippers MB Roebroeck A Renken R Nanetti L Keysers C May 2010 Mapping the information flow from one brain to another during gestural communication Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 107 20 9388 9393 Bibcode 2010PNAS 107 9388S doi 10 1073 pnas 1001791107 PMC 2889063 PMID 20439736 Moro A 2008 The Boundaries of Babel The Brain and the Enigma of Impossible Languages MIT Press p 257 ISBN 978 0 262 13498 9 Longo MR Kosobud A Bertenthal BI April 2008 Automatic imitation of biomechanically possible and impossible actions effects of priming movements versus goals PDF Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance 34 2 489 501 doi 10 1037 0096 1523 34 2 489 PMID 18377184 Kosonogov V October 2011 Listening to action related sentences impairs postural control Journal of Electromyography and Kinesiology 21 5 742 745 doi 10 1016 j jelekin 2011 05 007 PMID 21705230 Chartrand TL Bargh JA June 1999 The chameleon effect the perception behavior link and social interaction Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 76 6 893 910 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 76 6 893 PMID 10402679 S2CID 11818459 Lakin JL Chartrand TL July 2003 Using nonconscious behavioral mimicry to create affiliation and rapport Psychological Science 14 4 334 339 doi 10 1111 1467 9280 14481 PMID 12807406 S2CID 8458849 van Baaren RB Maddux WW Chartrand TL de Bouter C van Knippenberg A May 2003 It takes two to mimic behavioral consequences of self construals Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 84 5 1093 1102 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 84 5 1093 hdl 2066 63130 PMID 12757151 S2CID 729948 Heyes C May 2011 Automatic imitation Psychological Bulletin 137 3 463 483 doi 10 1037 a0022288 PMID 21280938 S2CID 6975248 Paukner A Suomi SJ Visalberghi E Ferrari PF August 2009 Capuchin monkeys display affiliation toward humans who imitate them Science 325 5942 880 883 Bibcode 2009Sci 325 880P doi 10 1126 science 1176269 PMC 2764469 PMID 19679816 a b Oberman LM Hubbard EM McCleery JP Altschuler EL Ramachandran VS Pineda JA July 2005 EEG evidence for mirror neuron dysfunction in autism spectrum disorders Brain Research Cognitive Brain Research 24 2 190 198 doi 10 1016 j cogbrainres 2005 01 014 PMID 15993757 Dapretto M Davies MS Pfeifer JH Scott AA Sigman M Bookheimer SY Iacoboni M January 2006 Understanding emotions in others mirror neuron dysfunction in children with autism spectrum disorders Nature Neuroscience 9 1 28 30 doi 10 1038 nn1611 PMC 3713227 PMID 16327784 Dapretto M Davies MS Pfeifer JH Scott AA Sigman M Bookheimer SY Iacoboni M January 2006 Understanding emotions in others mirror neuron dysfunction in children with autism spectrum disorders Nature Neuroscience 9 1 28 30 doi 10 1038 nn1611 PMC 3713227 PMID 16327784 Perkins T Stokes M McGillivray J Bittar R October 2010 Mirror neuron dysfunction in autism spectrum disorders Journal of Clinical Neuroscience 17 10 1239 1243 doi 10 1016 j jocn 2010 01 026 PMID 20598548 S2CID 15141982 Oberman LM Ramachandran VS Pineda JA April 2008 Modulation of mu suppression in children with autism spectrum disorders in response to familiar or unfamiliar stimuli the mirror neuron hypothesis Neuropsychologia 46 5 1558 1565 doi 10 1016 j neuropsychologia 2008 01 010 PMID 18304590 S2CID 14280719 Churcland PS 2011 6 Braintrust Princeton University Press p 156 Hamilton A Marsh L August 2013 Two systems for action comprehension in autism mirroring and mentalizing PDF In Baron Cohen S Lombardo M Tager Flusberg H eds Understanding Other Minds Perspectives from developmental social neuroscience Oxford OUP pp 380 396 ISBN 978 0 19 166880 7 Hadjikhani N Joseph RM Snyder J Tager Flusberg H September 2006 Anatomical differences in the mirror neuron system and social cognition network in autism Cerebral Cortex 16 9 1276 1282 doi 10 1093 cercor bhj069 PMID 16306324 a b Callaway E 12 May 2010 Mirror neurons seen behaving normally in autism New Scientist Hamilton AF August 2009 Goals intentions and mental states challenges for theories of autism Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines 50 8 881 892 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 621 6275 doi 10 1111 j 1469 7610 2009 02098 x PMID 19508497 Murphy P Brady N Fitzgerald M Troje NF December 2009 No evidence for impaired perception of biological motion in adults with autistic spectrum disorders Neuropsychologia 47 14 3225 3235 doi 10 1016 j neuropsychologia 2009 07 026 PMID 19666038 S2CID 12495492 Saygin AP Cook J Blakemore SJ October 2010 Baker CI ed Unaffected perceptual thresholds for biological and non biological form from motion perception in autism spectrum conditions PLOS ONE 5 10 e13491 Bibcode 2010PLoSO 513491S doi 10 1371 journal pone 0013491 PMC 2956672 PMID 20976151 Cook J Saygin AP Swain R Blakemore SJ December 2009 Reduced sensitivity to minimum jerk biological motion in autism spectrum conditions Neuropsychologia 47 14 3275 3278 doi 10 1016 j neuropsychologia 2009 07 010 PMC 2779370 PMID 19632248 Fan YT Decety J Yang CY Liu JL Cheng Y September 2010 Unbroken mirror neurons in autism spectrum disorders Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines 51 9 981 988 doi 10 1111 j 1469 7610 2010 02269 x PMID 20524939 Heyes C Catmur C January 2022 What Happened to Mirror Neurons Perspectives on Psychological Science 17 1 153 168 doi 10 1177 1745691621990638 PMC 8785302 PMID 34241539 Gordon R 1986 Folk psychology as simulation Mind and Language 1 2 158 171 doi 10 1111 j 1468 0017 1986 tb00324 x Goldman A 1989 Interpretation psychologized Mind and Language 4 3 161 185 doi 10 1111 j 1468 0017 1989 tb00249 x Meltzoff AN Decety J March 2003 What imitation tells us about social cognition a rapprochement between developmental psychology and cognitive neuroscience Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B Biological Sciences 358 1431 491 500 doi 10 1098 rstb 2002 1261 PMC 1351349 PMID 12689375 Sommerville JA Decety J April 2006 Weaving the fabric of social interaction articulating developmental psychology and cognitive neuroscience in the domain of motor cognition Psychonomic Bulletin amp Review 13 2 179 200 doi 10 3758 BF03193831 PMID 16892982 S2CID 14689479 Keysers C Gazzola V May 2007 Integrating simulation and theory of mind from self to social cognition Trends in Cognitive Sciences 11 5 194 196 doi 10 1016 j tics 2007 02 002 PMID 17344090 S2CID 18930071 Haroush K Williams ZM March 2015 Neuronal prediction of opponent s behavior during cooperative social interchange in primates Cell 160 6 1233 1245 doi 10 1016 j cell 2015 01 045 PMC 4364450 PMID 25728667 Cheng YW Tzeng OJ Decety J Imada T Hsieh JC July 2006 Gender differences in the human mirror system a magnetoencephalography study NeuroReport 17 11 1115 1119 doi 10 1097 01 wnr 0000223393 59328 21 PMID 16837838 S2CID 18811017 Cheng Y Decety J Lin CP Hsieh JC Hung D Tzeng OJ June 2007 Sex differences in spinal excitability during observation of bipedal locomotion NeuroReport 18 9 887 890 doi 10 1097 WNR 0b013e3280ebb486 PMID 17515795 S2CID 16295878 Yang CY Decety J Lee S Chen C Cheng Y January 2009 Gender differences in the mu rhythm during empathy for pain an electroencephalographic study Brain Research 1251 176 184 doi 10 1016 j brainres 2008 11 062 PMID 19083993 S2CID 40145972 Cheng Y Lee PL Yang CY Lin CP Hung D Decety J May 2008 Gender differences in the mu rhythm of the human mirror neuron system PLOS ONE 3 5 e2113 Bibcode 2008PLoSO 3 2113C doi 10 1371 journal pone 0002113 PMC 2361218 PMID 18461176 Schulte Ruther M Markowitsch HJ Shah NJ Fink GR Piefke M August 2008 Gender differences in brain networks supporting empathy NeuroImage 42 1 393 403 doi 10 1016 j neuroimage 2008 04 180 PMID 18514546 S2CID 10461927 a b c Jalal B Ramachandran VS 2017 Sleep Paralysis The Ghostly Bedroom Intruder and Out of Body Experiences The Role of Mirror Neurons Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 11 92 doi 10 3389 fnhum 2017 00092 PMC 5329044 PMID 28293186 McCormick LM Brumm MC Beadle JN Paradiso S Yamada T Andreasen N March 2012 Mirror neuron function psychosis and empathy in schizophrenia Psychiatry Research 201 3 233 239 doi 10 1016 j pscychresns 2012 01 004 PMC 3545445 PMID 22510432 a b Pascolo PB Ragogna R Rossi R 2009 The Mirror Neuron System Paradigm and its consistency Gait amp Posture 30 Suppl 1 65 doi 10 1016 j gaitpost 2009 07 064 Lingnau A Gesierich B Caramazza A June 2009 Asymmetric fMRI adaptation reveals no evidence for mirror neurons in humans Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 106 24 9925 9930 Bibcode 2009PNAS 106 9925L doi 10 1073 pnas 0902262106 PMC 2701024 PMID 19497880 Kilner JM Neal A Weiskopf N Friston KJ Frith CD August 2009 Evidence of mirror neurons in human inferior frontal gyrus The Journal of Neuroscience 29 32 10153 10159 doi 10 1523 jneurosci 2668 09 2009 PMC 2788150 PMID 19675249 Churchland Patricia Braintrust 2011 Chapter 6 page 142 Ramachandran VS 2000 Mirror neurons and imitation learning as the driving force behind the great leap forward in human evolution Edge Retrieved 13 April 2013 Further reading editCarey B 12 February 2008 You remind me of me The New York Times Hickok G 2014 The Myth of Mirror Neurons National Geographic Books ISBN 978 0 393 08961 5 Keysers C 2011 The Empathic Brain CreateSpace Independent Publishing Platform ISBN 978 1 4637 6906 2 Rizzolatti G Sinigaglia C 2008 Mirrors in the Brain How We Share our Actions and Emotions Oxford UK Oxford University Press ISBN 978 0 19 921798 4 Morsella E Bargh JA Gollwitzer PM eds 2009 Oxford Handbook of Human Action New York Oxford University Press ISBN 978 0 19 530998 0 Gallese V Gernsbacher MA Heyes C Hickok G Iacoboni M July 2011 Mirror Neuron Forum Perspectives on Psychological Science 6 4 369 407 doi 10 1177 1745691611413392 PMC 4266473 PMID 25520744 Iacoboni M Mazziotta JC September 2007 Mirror neuron system basic findings and clinical applications Annals of Neurology 62 3 213 218 doi 10 1002 ana 21198 PMID 17721988 S2CID 3225339 Keysers C Gazzola V 2006 Towards a unifying neural theory of social cognition Understanding Emotions Progress in Brain Research Vol 156 pp 379 401 doi 10 1016 S0079 6123 06 56021 2 ISBN 9780444521828 PMID 17015092 Pascolo B 2013 Mirror neurons still an open question PDF Progress in Neuroscience 1 1 4 25 82 Preston SD de Waal FB February 2002 Empathy Its ultimate and proximate bases The Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25 1 1 72 doi 10 1017 s0140525x02000018 PMID 12625087 Rizzolatti G Fabbri Destro M Cattaneo L January 2009 Mirror neurons and their clinical relevance Nature Clinical Practice Neurology 5 1 24 34 doi 10 1038 ncpneuro0990 PMID 19129788 S2CID 2979216 External links edit nbsp Wikimedia Commons has media related to Mirror neurons Hickok G Poeppel D Talking Brains News And Views On the Neural Organization of Language Ramachandran VS The neurons that shaped civilization TED talks Archived from the original on 10 February 2012 Thomas B 2012 What s So Special about Mirror Neurons Scientific American Guest Blog an overview of prominent research approaches based on interviews with Iacoboni Hickok Heyes and Gallese Mirror Neurons NOVA scienceNOW January 2005 Archived from the original on 11 January 2006 Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Mirror neuron amp oldid 1208845996, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.