fbpx
Wikipedia

Dependency grammar

Dependency grammar (DG) is a class of modern grammatical theories that are all based on the dependency relation (as opposed to the constituency relation of phrase structure) and that can be traced back primarily to the work of Lucien Tesnière. Dependency is the notion that linguistic units, e.g. words, are connected to each other by directed links. The (finite) verb is taken to be the structural center of clause structure. All other syntactic units (words) are either directly or indirectly connected to the verb in terms of the directed links, which are called dependencies. Dependency grammar differs from phrase structure grammar in that while it can identify phrases it tends to overlook phrasal nodes. A dependency structure is determined by the relation between a word (a head) and its dependents. Dependency structures are flatter than phrase structures in part because they lack a finite verb phrase constituent, and they are thus well suited for the analysis of languages with free word order, such as Czech or Warlpiri.

History edit

The notion of dependencies between grammatical units has existed since the earliest recorded grammars, e.g. Pāṇini, and the dependency concept therefore arguably predates that of phrase structure by many centuries.[1] Ibn Maḍāʾ, a 12th-century linguist from Córdoba, Andalusia, may have been the first grammarian to use the term dependency in the grammatical sense that we use it today. In early modern times, the dependency concept seems to have coexisted side by side with that of phrase structure, the latter having entered Latin, French, English and other grammars from the widespread study of term logic of antiquity.[2] Dependency is also concretely present in the works of Sámuel Brassai (1800–1897), a Hungarian linguist, Franz Kern (1830–1894), a German philologist, and of Heimann Hariton Tiktin (1850–1936), a Romanian linguist.[3]

Modern dependency grammars, however, begin primarily with the work of Lucien Tesnière. Tesnière was a Frenchman, a polyglot, and a professor of linguistics at the universities in Strasbourg and Montpellier. His major work Éléments de syntaxe structurale was published posthumously in 1959 – he died in 1954. The basic approach to syntax he developed seems to have been seized upon independently by others in the 1960s[4] and a number of other dependency-based grammars have gained prominence since those early works.[5] DG has generated a lot of interest in Germany[6] in both theoretical syntax and language pedagogy. In recent years, the great development surrounding dependency-based theories has come from computational linguistics and is due, in part, to the influential work that David Hays did in machine translation at the RAND Corporation in the 1950s and 1960s. Dependency-based systems are increasingly being used to parse natural language and generate tree banks. Interest in dependency grammar is growing at present, international conferences on dependency linguistics being a relatively recent development (Depling 2011, Depling 2013, Depling 2015, Depling 2017, Depling 2019 2019-03-06 at the Wayback Machine).

Dependency vs. phrase structure edit

Dependency is a one-to-one correspondence: for every element (e.g. word or morph) in the sentence, there is exactly one node in the structure of that sentence that corresponds to that element. The result of this one-to-one correspondence is that dependency grammars are word (or morph) grammars. All that exist are the elements and the dependencies that connect the elements into a structure. This situation should be compared with phrase structure. Phrase structure is a one-to-one-or-more correspondence, which means that, for every element in a sentence, there is one or more nodes in the structure that correspond to that element. The result of this difference is that dependency structures are minimal[7] compared to their phrase structure counterparts, since they tend to contain many fewer nodes.

 

These trees illustrate two possible ways to render the dependency and phrase structure relations (see below). This dependency tree is an "ordered" tree, i.e. it reflects actual word order. Many dependency trees abstract away from linear order and focus just on hierarchical order, which means they do not show actual word order. This constituency (= phrase structure) tree follows the conventions of bare phrase structure (BPS), whereby the words themselves are employed as the node labels.

The distinction between dependency and phrase structure grammars derives in large part from the initial division of the clause. The phrase structure relation derives from an initial binary division, whereby the clause is split into a subject noun phrase (NP) and a predicate verb phrase (VP). This division is certainly present in the basic analysis of the clause that we find in the works of, for instance, Leonard Bloomfield and Noam Chomsky. Tesnière, however, argued vehemently against this binary division, preferring instead to position the verb as the root of all clause structure. Tesnière's stance was that the subject-predicate division stems from term logic and has no place in linguistics.[8] The importance of this distinction is that if one acknowledges the initial subject-predicate division in syntax is real, then one is likely to go down the path of phrase structure grammar, while if one rejects this division, then one must consider the verb as the root of all structure, and so go down the path of dependency grammar.

Dependency grammars edit

The following frameworks are dependency-based:

 
Hybrid constituency/dependency tree from the Quranic Arabic Corpus

Link grammar is similar to dependency grammar, but link grammar does not include directionality between the linked words, and thus does not describe head-dependent relationships. Hybrid dependency/phrase structure grammar uses dependencies between words, but also includes dependencies between phrasal nodes – see for example the Quranic Arabic Dependency Treebank. The derivation trees of tree-adjoining grammar are dependency structures, although the full trees of TAG rendered in terms of phrase structure, so in this regard, it is not clear whether TAG should be viewed more as a dependency or phrase structure grammar.

There are major differences between the grammars just listed. In this regard, the dependency relation is compatible with other major tenets of theories of grammar. Thus like phrase structure grammars, dependency grammars can be mono- or multistratal, representational or derivational, construction- or rule-based.

Representing dependencies edit

There are various conventions that DGs employ to represent dependencies. The following schemata (in addition to the tree above and the trees further below) illustrate some of these conventions:

 

The representations in (a–d) are trees, whereby the specific conventions employed in each tree vary. Solid lines are dependency edges and lightly dotted lines are projection lines. The only difference between tree (a) and tree (b) is that tree (a) employs the category class to label the nodes whereas tree (b) employs the words themselves as the node labels.[9] Tree (c) is a reduced tree insofar as the string of words below and projection lines are deemed unnecessary and are hence omitted. Tree (d) abstracts away from linear order and reflects just hierarchical order.[10] The arrow arcs in (e) are an alternative convention used to show dependencies and are favored by Word Grammar.[11] The brackets in (f) are seldom used, but are nevertheless quite capable of reflecting the dependency hierarchy; dependents appear enclosed in more brackets than their heads. And finally, the indentations like those in (g) are another convention that is sometimes employed to indicate the hierarchy of words.[12] Dependents are placed underneath their heads and indented. Like tree (d), the indentations in (g) abstract away from linear order.

The point to these conventions is that they are just that, namely conventions. They do not influence the basic commitment to dependency as the relation that is grouping syntactic units.

Types of dependencies edit

The dependency representations above (and further below) show syntactic dependencies. Indeed, most work in dependency grammar focuses on syntactic dependencies. Syntactic dependencies are, however, just one of three or four types of dependencies. Meaning–text theory, for instance, emphasizes the role of semantic and morphological dependencies in addition to syntactic dependencies.[13] A fourth type, prosodic dependencies, can also be acknowledged. Distinguishing between these types of dependencies can be important, in part because if one fails to do so, the likelihood that semantic, morphological, and/or prosodic dependencies will be mistaken for syntactic dependencies is great. The following four subsections briefly sketch each of these dependency types. During the discussion, the existence of syntactic dependencies is taken for granted and used as an orientation point for establishing the nature of the other three dependency types.

Semantic dependencies edit

Semantic dependencies are understood in terms of predicates and their arguments.[14] The arguments of a predicate are semantically dependent on that predicate. Often, semantic dependencies overlap with and point in the same direction as syntactic dependencies. At times, however, semantic dependencies can point in the opposite direction of syntactic dependencies, or they can be entirely independent of syntactic dependencies. The hierarchy of words in the following examples show standard syntactic dependencies, whereas the arrows indicate semantic dependencies:

 

The two arguments Sam and Sally in tree (a) are dependent on the predicate likes, whereby these arguments are also syntactically dependent on likes. What this means is that the semantic and syntactic dependencies overlap and point in the same direction (down the tree). Attributive adjectives, however, are predicates that take their head noun as their argument, hence big is a predicate in tree (b) that takes bones as its one argument; the semantic dependency points up the tree and therefore runs counter to the syntactic dependency. A similar situation obtains in (c), where the preposition predicate on takes the two arguments the picture and the wall; one of these semantic dependencies points up the syntactic hierarchy, whereas the other points down it. Finally, the predicate to help in (d) takes the one argument Jim but is not directly connected to Jim in the syntactic hierarchy, which means that semantic dependency is entirely independent of the syntactic dependencies.

Morphological dependencies edit

Morphological dependencies obtain between words or parts of words.[15] When a given word or part of a word influences the form of another word, then the latter is morphologically dependent on the former. Agreement and concord are therefore manifestations of morphological dependencies. Like semantic dependencies, morphological dependencies can overlap with and point in the same direction as syntactic dependencies, overlap with and point in the opposite direction of syntactic dependencies, or be entirely independent of syntactic dependencies. The arrows are now used to indicate morphological dependencies.

 

The plural houses in (a) demands the plural of the demonstrative determiner, hence these appears, not this, which means there is a morphological dependency that points down the hierarchy from houses to these. The situation is reversed in (b), where the singular subject Sam demands the appearance of the agreement suffix -s on the finite verb works, which means there is a morphological dependency pointing up the hierarchy from Sam to works. The type of determiner in the German examples (c) and (d) influences the inflectional suffix that appears on the adjective alt. When the indefinite article ein is used, the strong masculine ending -er appears on the adjective. When the definite article der is used, in contrast, the weak ending -e appears on the adjective. Thus since the choice of determiner impacts the morphological form of the adjective, there is a morphological dependency pointing from the determiner to the adjective, whereby this morphological dependency is entirely independent of the syntactic dependencies. Consider further the following French sentences:

 

The masculine subject le chien in (a) demands the masculine form of the predicative adjective blanc, whereas the feminine subject la maison demands the feminine form of this adjective. A morphological dependency that is entirely independent of the syntactic dependencies therefore points again across the syntactic hierarchy.

Morphological dependencies play an important role in typological studies. Languages are classified as mostly head-marking (Sam work-s) or mostly dependent-marking (these houses), whereby most if not all languages contain at least some minor measure of both head and dependent marking.[16]

Prosodic dependencies edit

Prosodic dependencies are acknowledged in order to accommodate the behavior of clitics.[17] A clitic is a syntactically autonomous element that is prosodically dependent on a host. A clitic is therefore integrated into the prosody of its host, meaning that it forms a single word with its host. Prosodic dependencies exist entirely in the linear dimension (horizontal dimension), whereas standard syntactic dependencies exist in the hierarchical dimension (vertical dimension). Classic examples of clitics in English are reduced auxiliaries (e.g. -ll, -s, -ve) and the possessive marker -s. The prosodic dependencies in the following examples are indicated with the hyphen and the lack of a vertical projection line:

 

The hyphens and lack of projection lines indicate prosodic dependencies. A hyphen that appears on the left of the clitic indicates that the clitic is prosodically dependent on the word immediately to its left (He'll, There's), whereas a hyphen that appears on the right side of the clitic (not shown here) indicates that the clitic is prosodically dependent on the word that appears immediately to its right. A given clitic is often prosodically dependent on its syntactic dependent (He'll, There's) or on its head (would've). At other times, it can depend prosodically on a word that is neither its head nor its immediate dependent (Florida's).

Syntactic dependencies edit

Syntactic dependencies are the focus of most work in DG, as stated above. How the presence and the direction of syntactic dependencies are determined is of course often open to debate. In this regard, it must be acknowledged that the validity of syntactic dependencies in the trees throughout this article is being taken for granted. However, these hierarchies are such that many DGs can largely support them, although there will certainly be points of disagreement. The basic question about how syntactic dependencies are discerned has proven difficult to answer definitively. One should acknowledge in this area, however, that the basic task of identifying and discerning the presence and direction of the syntactic dependencies of DGs is no easier or harder than determining the constituent groupings of phrase structure grammars. A variety of heuristics are employed to this end, basic tests for constituents being useful tools; the syntactic dependencies assumed in the trees in this article are grouping words together in a manner that most closely matches the results of standard permutation, substitution, and ellipsis tests for constituents. Etymological considerations also provide helpful clues about the direction of dependencies. A promising principle upon which to base the existence of syntactic dependencies is distribution.[18] When one is striving to identify the root of a given phrase, the word that is most responsible for determining the distribution of that phrase as a whole is its root.

Linear order and discontinuities edit

Traditionally, DGs have had a different approach to linear order (word order) than phrase structure grammars. Dependency structures are minimal compared to their phrase structure counterparts, and these minimal structures allow one to focus intently on the two ordering dimensions.[19] Separating the vertical dimension (hierarchical order) from the horizontal dimension (linear order) is easily accomplished. This aspect of dependency structures has allowed DGs, starting with Tesnière (1959), to focus on hierarchical order in a manner that is hardly possible for phrase structure grammars. For Tesnière, linear order was secondary to hierarchical order insofar as hierarchical order preceded linear order in the mind of a speaker. The stemmas (trees) that Tesnière produced reflected this view; they abstracted away from linear order to focus almost entirely on hierarchical order. Many DGs that followed Tesnière adopted this practice, that is, they produced tree structures that reflect hierarchical order alone, e.g.

 

The traditional focus on hierarchical order generated the impression that DGs have little to say about linear order, and it has contributed to the view that DGs are particularly well-suited to examine languages with free word order. A negative result of this focus on hierarchical order, however, is that there is a dearth of DG explorations of particular word order phenomena, such as of standard discontinuities. Comprehensive dependency grammar accounts of topicalization, wh-fronting, scrambling, and extraposition are mostly absent from many established DG frameworks. This situation can be contrasted with phrase structure grammars, which have devoted tremendous effort to exploring these phenomena.

The nature of the dependency relation does not, however, prevent one from focusing on linear order. Dependency structures are as capable of exploring word order phenomena as phrase structures. The following trees illustrate this point; they represent one way of exploring discontinuities using dependency structures. The trees suggest the manner in which common discontinuities can be addressed. An example from German is used to illustrate a scrambling discontinuity:

 

The a-trees on the left show projectivity violations (= crossing lines), and the b-trees on the right demonstrate one means of addressing these violations. The displaced constituent takes on a word as its head that is not its governor. The words in red mark the catena (=chain) of words that extends from the root of the displaced constituent to the governor of that constituent.[20] Discontinuities are then explored in terms of these catenae. The limitations on topicalization, wh-fronting, scrambling, and extraposition can be explored and identified by examining the nature of the catenae involved.

Syntactic functions edit

Traditionally, DGs have treated the syntactic functions (= grammatical functions, grammatical relations) as primitive. They posit an inventory of functions (e.g. subject, object, oblique, determiner, attribute, predicative, etc.). These functions can appear as labels on the dependencies in the tree structures, e.g.[21]

 

The syntactic functions in this tree are shown in green: ATTR (attribute), COMP-P (complement of preposition), COMP-TO (complement of to), DET (determiner), P-ATTR (prepositional attribute), PRED (predicative), SUBJ (subject), TO-COMP (to complement). The functions chosen and abbreviations used in the tree here are merely representative of the general stance of DGs toward the syntactic functions. The actual inventory of functions and designations employed vary from DG to DG.

As a primitive of the theory, the status of these functions is very different from that in some phrase structure grammars. Traditionally, phrase structure grammars derive the syntactic functions from the constellation. For instance, the object is identified as the NP appearing inside finite VP, and the subject as the NP appearing outside of finite VP. Since DGs reject the existence of a finite VP constituent, they were never presented with the option to view the syntactic functions in this manner. The issue is a question of what comes first: traditionally, DGs take the syntactic functions to be primitive and they then derive the constellation from these functions, whereas phrase structure grammars traditionally take the constellation to be primitive and they then derive the syntactic functions from the constellation.

This question about what comes first (the functions or the constellation) is not an inflexible matter. The stances of both grammar types (dependency and phrase structure) are not narrowly limited to the traditional views. Dependency and phrase structure are both fully compatible with both approaches to the syntactic functions. Indeed, monostratal systems, that are solely based on dependency or phrase structure, will likely reject the notion that the functions are derived from the constellation or that the constellation is derived from the functions. They will take both to be primitive, which means neither can be derived from the other.

See also edit

Notes edit

  1. ^ Concerning the history of the dependency concept, see Percival (1990).
  2. ^ Concerning the influence of term logic on the theory of grammar, see Percival (1976).
  3. ^ Concerning dependency in the works of Brassai, see Imrényi (2013). Concerning dependency in the works of Kern, see Kern's essays (e.g. Kern 1883, 1884). Concerning dependency in the works of Tiktin, see Coseriu (1980).
  4. ^ Concerning early dependency grammars that may have developed independently of Tesnière's work, see for instance Hays (1960), Gaifman (1965), and Robinson (1970).
  5. ^ Some prominent dependency grammars that were well established by the 1980s are from Hudson (1984), Sgall, Hajičová et Panevova (1986), Mel’čuk (1988), and Starosta (1988).
  6. ^ Some prominent dependency grammars from the German schools are from Heringer (1996), Engel (1994), Eroms (2000), and Ágel et al. (2003/6) is a massive two volume collection of essays on dependency grammar and valency theory from more than 100 authors.
  7. ^ The minimality of dependency structures is emphasized, for instance, by Ninio (2006), Hudson 2007: 117, and by Osborne et al. (2011).
  8. ^ Concerning Tesnière's rejection of the subject-predicate division of the clause, see Tesnière (1959:103–105), and for discussion of empirical considerations that support Tesnière's point, see Matthews (2007:17ff.), Miller (2011:54ff.), and Osborne et al. (2011:323f.).
  9. ^ The conventions illustrated with trees (a) and (b) are preferred by Osborne et al. (2011, 2013).
  10. ^ Unordered trees like (d) are associated above all with Tesnière's stemmas and with the syntactic strata of Mel’čuk's Meaning-Text Theory.
  11. ^ Three major works on Word Grammar are Hudson (1984, 1990, 2007).
  12. ^ Lobin (2003) makes heavy use of these indentations.
  13. ^ For a discussion of semantic, morphological, and syntactic dependencies in Meaning-Text Theory, see Melʹc̆uk (2003:191ff.) and Osborne 2019: Ch. 5).
  14. ^ Concerning semantic dependencies, see Melʹc̆uk (2003:192f.).
  15. ^ Concerning morphological dependencies, see Melʹc̆uk (2003:193ff.).
  16. ^ The distinction between head- and dependent-marking was established by Nichols (1986). Nichols was using a dependency-based understanding of these distinctions.
  17. ^ Concerning prosodic dependencies and the analysis of clitics, see Groß (2011).
  18. ^ Distribution is primary principle used by Owens (1984:36), Schubert (1988:40), and Melʹc̆uk (2003:200) for discerning syntactic dependencies.
  19. ^ Concerning the importance of the two ordering dimensions, see Tesnière (1959:16ff).
  20. ^ See Osborne et al. (2012) concerning catenae.
  21. ^ For discussion and examples of the labels for syntactic functions that are attached to dependency edges and arcs, see for instance Mel'cuk (1988:22, 69) and van Valin (2001:102ff.).

References edit

  • Ágel, Vilmos; Eichinger, Ludwig M.; Eroms, Hans Werner; Hellwig, Peter; Heringer, Hans Jürgen; Lobin, Henning, eds. (2003). Dependenz und Valenz:Ein internationales Handbuch der zeitgenössischen Forschung [Dependency and Valency:An International Handbook of Contemporary Research] (in German). Berlin: de Gruyter. ISBN 978-3110141900. Retrieved 24 August 2012.
  • Coseriu, E. 1980. Un précurseur méconnu de la syntaxe structurale: H. Tiktin. In Recherches de Linguistique : Hommage à Maurice Leroy. Éditions de l’Université de Bruxelles, 48–62.
  • Engel, U. 1994. Syntax der deutschen Sprache, 3rd edition. Berlin: Erich Schmidt Verlag.
  • Eroms, Hans-Werner (2000). Syntax der deutschen Sprache. Berlin [u.a.]: de Gruyter. doi:10.1515/9783110808124. ISBN 978-3110156669. Retrieved 24 August 2012.
  • Groß, T. 2011. Clitics in dependency morphology. Depling 2011 Proceedings, 58–68.
  • Helbig, Gerhard; Buscha, Joachim (2007). Deutsche Grammatik: ein Handbuch für den Ausländerunterricht [German Grammar: A Guide for Foreigners Teaching] (6. [Dr.]. ed.). Berlin: Langenscheidt. ISBN 978-3-468-49493-2. Retrieved 24 August 2012.
  • Heringer, H. 1996. Deutsche Syntax dependentiell. Tübingen: Stauffenburg.
  • Hays, D. 1960. Grouping and dependency theories. P-1910, RAND Corporation.
  • Hays, D. 1964. Dependency theory: A formalism and some observations. Language, 40: 511-525. Reprinted in Syntactic Theory 1, Structuralist, edited by Fred W. Householder. Penguin, 1972.
  • Hudson, Richard (1984). Word grammar (1. publ. ed.). Oxford, OX, England: B. Blackwell. ISBN 978-0631131861.
  • Hudson, R. 1990. English Word Grammar. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
  • Hudson, R. 2007. Language Networks: The New Word Grammar. Oxford University Press.
  • Imrényi, A. 2013. Constituency or dependency? Notes on Sámuel Brassai's syntactic model of Hungarian. In Szigetvári, Péter (ed.), VLlxx. Papers Presented to László Varga on his 70th Birthday. Budapest: Tinta. 167–182.
  • Kern, F. 1883. Zur Methodik des deutschen Unterrichts. Berlin: Nicolaische Verlags-Buchhandlung.
  • Kern, F. 1884. Grundriss der Deutschen Satzlehre. Berlin: Nicolaische Verlags-Buchhandlung.
  • Liu, H. 2009. Dependency Grammar: from Theory to Practice. Beijing: Science Press.
  • Lobin, H. 2003. Koordinationssyntax als prozedurales Phänomen. Tübingen: Gunter Narr-Verlag.
  • Matthews, P. H. (2007). Syntactic Relations: a critical survey (1. publ. ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 9780521608299. Retrieved 24 August 2012.
  • Melʹc̆uk, Igor A. (1987). Dependency syntax : theory and practice. Albany: State University Press of New York. ISBN 978-0-88706-450-0. Retrieved 24 August 2012.
  • Melʹc̆uk, I. 2003. Levels of dependency in linguistic description: Concepts and problems. In Ágel et al., 170–187.
  • Miller, J. 2011. A critical introduction to syntax. London: continuum.
  • Nichols, J. 1986. Head-marking and dependent-marking languages. Language 62, 56–119.
  • Ninio, A. 2006. Language and the learning curve: A new theory of syntactic development. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Osborne, T. 2019. A Dependency Grammar of English: An Introduction and Beyond. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. https://doi.org/10.1075/z.224
  • Osborne, T., M. Putnam, and T. Groß 2011. Bare phrase structure, label-less trees, and specifier-less syntax: Is Minimalism becoming a dependency grammar? The Linguistic Review 28, 315–364.
  • Osborne, T., M. Putnam, and T. Groß 2012. Catenae: Introducing a novel unit of syntactic analysis. Syntax 15, 4, 354–396.
  • Owens, J. 1984. On getting a head: A problem in dependency grammar. Lingua 62, 25–42.
  • Percival, K. 1976. On the historical source of immediate-constituent analysis. In: Notes from the linguistic underground, James McCawley (ed.), Syntax and Semantics 7, 229–242. New York: Academic Press.
  • Percival, K. 1990. Reflections on the history of dependency notions in linguistics. Historiographia Linguistica, 17, 29–47.
  • Robinson, J. 1970. Dependency structures and transformational rules. Language 46, 259–285.
  • Schubert, K. 1988. Metataxis: Contrastive dependency syntax for machine translation. Dordrecht: Foris.
  • Sgall, P., E. Hajičová, and J. Panevová 1986. The meaning of the sentence in its semantic and pragmatic aspects. Dordrecht: D. Reidel Publishing Company.
  • Starosta, S. 1988. The case for lexicase. London: Pinter Publishers.
  • Tesnière, L. 1959. Éléments de syntaxe structurale. Paris: Klincksieck.
  • Tesnière, L. 1966. Éléments de syntaxe structurale, 2nd edition. Paris: Klincksieck.
  • Tesnière, L. 2015. Elements of structural syntax [English translation of Tesnière 1966]. John Benjamins, Amsterdam.
  • van Valin, R. 2001. An introduction to syntax. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

External links edit

  • Universal Dependencies – a set of treebanks in a harmonized dependency grammar representation

dependency, grammar, class, modern, grammatical, theories, that, based, dependency, relation, opposed, constituency, relation, phrase, structure, that, traced, back, primarily, work, lucien, tesnière, dependency, notion, that, linguistic, units, words, connect. Dependency grammar DG is a class of modern grammatical theories that are all based on the dependency relation as opposed to the constituency relation of phrase structure and that can be traced back primarily to the work of Lucien Tesniere Dependency is the notion that linguistic units e g words are connected to each other by directed links The finite verb is taken to be the structural center of clause structure All other syntactic units words are either directly or indirectly connected to the verb in terms of the directed links which are called dependencies Dependency grammar differs from phrase structure grammar in that while it can identify phrases it tends to overlook phrasal nodes A dependency structure is determined by the relation between a word a head and its dependents Dependency structures are flatter than phrase structures in part because they lack a finite verb phrase constituent and they are thus well suited for the analysis of languages with free word order such as Czech or Warlpiri Contents 1 History 2 Dependency vs phrase structure 3 Dependency grammars 4 Representing dependencies 5 Types of dependencies 5 1 Semantic dependencies 5 2 Morphological dependencies 5 3 Prosodic dependencies 5 4 Syntactic dependencies 6 Linear order and discontinuities 7 Syntactic functions 8 See also 9 Notes 10 References 11 External linksHistory editThe notion of dependencies between grammatical units has existed since the earliest recorded grammars e g Paṇini and the dependency concept therefore arguably predates that of phrase structure by many centuries 1 Ibn Maḍaʾ a 12th century linguist from Cordoba Andalusia may have been the first grammarian to use the term dependency in the grammatical sense that we use it today In early modern times the dependency concept seems to have coexisted side by side with that of phrase structure the latter having entered Latin French English and other grammars from the widespread study of term logic of antiquity 2 Dependency is also concretely present in the works of Samuel Brassai 1800 1897 a Hungarian linguist Franz Kern 1830 1894 a German philologist and of Heimann Hariton Tiktin 1850 1936 a Romanian linguist 3 Modern dependency grammars however begin primarily with the work of Lucien Tesniere Tesniere was a Frenchman a polyglot and a professor of linguistics at the universities in Strasbourg and Montpellier His major work Elements de syntaxe structurale was published posthumously in 1959 he died in 1954 The basic approach to syntax he developed seems to have been seized upon independently by others in the 1960s 4 and a number of other dependency based grammars have gained prominence since those early works 5 DG has generated a lot of interest in Germany 6 in both theoretical syntax and language pedagogy In recent years the great development surrounding dependency based theories has come from computational linguistics and is due in part to the influential work that David Hays did in machine translation at the RAND Corporation in the 1950s and 1960s Dependency based systems are increasingly being used to parse natural language and generate tree banks Interest in dependency grammar is growing at present international conferences on dependency linguistics being a relatively recent development Depling 2011 Depling 2013 Depling 2015 Depling 2017 Depling 2019 Archived 2019 03 06 at the Wayback Machine Dependency vs phrase structure editDependency is a one to one correspondence for every element e g word or morph in the sentence there is exactly one node in the structure of that sentence that corresponds to that element The result of this one to one correspondence is that dependency grammars are word or morph grammars All that exist are the elements and the dependencies that connect the elements into a structure This situation should be compared with phrase structure Phrase structure is a one to one or more correspondence which means that for every element in a sentence there is one or more nodes in the structure that correspond to that element The result of this difference is that dependency structures are minimal 7 compared to their phrase structure counterparts since they tend to contain many fewer nodes nbsp dd dd dd These trees illustrate two possible ways to render the dependency and phrase structure relations see below This dependency tree is an ordered tree i e it reflects actual word order Many dependency trees abstract away from linear order and focus just on hierarchical order which means they do not show actual word order This constituency phrase structure tree follows the conventions of bare phrase structure BPS whereby the words themselves are employed as the node labels The distinction between dependency and phrase structure grammars derives in large part from the initial division of the clause The phrase structure relation derives from an initial binary division whereby the clause is split into a subject noun phrase NP and a predicate verb phrase VP This division is certainly present in the basic analysis of the clause that we find in the works of for instance Leonard Bloomfield and Noam Chomsky Tesniere however argued vehemently against this binary division preferring instead to position the verb as the root of all clause structure Tesniere s stance was that the subject predicate division stems from term logic and has no place in linguistics 8 The importance of this distinction is that if one acknowledges the initial subject predicate division in syntax is real then one is likely to go down the path of phrase structure grammar while if one rejects this division then one must consider the verb as the root of all structure and so go down the path of dependency grammar Dependency grammars editThe following frameworks are dependency based nbsp Hybrid constituency dependency tree from the Quranic Arabic Corpus Algebraic syntax Operator grammar Link grammar Functional generative description Lexicase Meaning text theory Word grammar Extensible dependency grammar Universal Dependencies Link grammar is similar to dependency grammar but link grammar does not include directionality between the linked words and thus does not describe head dependent relationships Hybrid dependency phrase structure grammar uses dependencies between words but also includes dependencies between phrasal nodes see for example the Quranic Arabic Dependency Treebank The derivation trees of tree adjoining grammar are dependency structures although the full trees of TAG rendered in terms of phrase structure so in this regard it is not clear whether TAG should be viewed more as a dependency or phrase structure grammar There are major differences between the grammars just listed In this regard the dependency relation is compatible with other major tenets of theories of grammar Thus like phrase structure grammars dependency grammars can be mono or multistratal representational or derivational construction or rule based Representing dependencies editThere are various conventions that DGs employ to represent dependencies The following schemata in addition to the tree above and the trees further below illustrate some of these conventions nbsp dd The representations in a d are trees whereby the specific conventions employed in each tree vary Solid lines are dependency edges and lightly dotted lines are projection lines The only difference between tree a and tree b is that tree a employs the category class to label the nodes whereas tree b employs the words themselves as the node labels 9 Tree c is a reduced tree insofar as the string of words below and projection lines are deemed unnecessary and are hence omitted Tree d abstracts away from linear order and reflects just hierarchical order 10 The arrow arcs in e are an alternative convention used to show dependencies and are favored by Word Grammar 11 The brackets in f are seldom used but are nevertheless quite capable of reflecting the dependency hierarchy dependents appear enclosed in more brackets than their heads And finally the indentations like those in g are another convention that is sometimes employed to indicate the hierarchy of words 12 Dependents are placed underneath their heads and indented Like tree d the indentations in g abstract away from linear order The point to these conventions is that they are just that namely conventions They do not influence the basic commitment to dependency as the relation that is grouping syntactic units Types of dependencies editThe dependency representations above and further below show syntactic dependencies Indeed most work in dependency grammar focuses on syntactic dependencies Syntactic dependencies are however just one of three or four types of dependencies Meaning text theory for instance emphasizes the role of semantic and morphological dependencies in addition to syntactic dependencies 13 A fourth type prosodic dependencies can also be acknowledged Distinguishing between these types of dependencies can be important in part because if one fails to do so the likelihood that semantic morphological and or prosodic dependencies will be mistaken for syntactic dependencies is great The following four subsections briefly sketch each of these dependency types During the discussion the existence of syntactic dependencies is taken for granted and used as an orientation point for establishing the nature of the other three dependency types Semantic dependencies edit Semantic dependencies are understood in terms of predicates and their arguments 14 The arguments of a predicate are semantically dependent on that predicate Often semantic dependencies overlap with and point in the same direction as syntactic dependencies At times however semantic dependencies can point in the opposite direction of syntactic dependencies or they can be entirely independent of syntactic dependencies The hierarchy of words in the following examples show standard syntactic dependencies whereas the arrows indicate semantic dependencies nbsp dd The two arguments Sam and Sally in tree a are dependent on the predicate likes whereby these arguments are also syntactically dependent on likes What this means is that the semantic and syntactic dependencies overlap and point in the same direction down the tree Attributive adjectives however are predicates that take their head noun as their argument hence big is a predicate in tree b that takes bones as its one argument the semantic dependency points up the tree and therefore runs counter to the syntactic dependency A similar situation obtains in c where the preposition predicate on takes the two arguments the picture and the wall one of these semantic dependencies points up the syntactic hierarchy whereas the other points down it Finally the predicate to help in d takes the one argument Jim but is not directly connected to Jim in the syntactic hierarchy which means that semantic dependency is entirely independent of the syntactic dependencies Morphological dependencies edit Morphological dependencies obtain between words or parts of words 15 When a given word or part of a word influences the form of another word then the latter is morphologically dependent on the former Agreement and concord are therefore manifestations of morphological dependencies Like semantic dependencies morphological dependencies can overlap with and point in the same direction as syntactic dependencies overlap with and point in the opposite direction of syntactic dependencies or be entirely independent of syntactic dependencies The arrows are now used to indicate morphological dependencies nbsp The plural houses in a demands the plural of the demonstrative determiner hence these appears not this which means there is a morphological dependency that points down the hierarchy from houses to these The situation is reversed in b where the singular subject Sam demands the appearance of the agreement suffix s on the finite verb works which means there is a morphological dependency pointing up the hierarchy from Sam to works The type of determiner in the German examples c and d influences the inflectional suffix that appears on the adjective alt When the indefinite article ein is used the strong masculine ending er appears on the adjective When the definite article der is used in contrast the weak ending e appears on the adjective Thus since the choice of determiner impacts the morphological form of the adjective there is a morphological dependency pointing from the determiner to the adjective whereby this morphological dependency is entirely independent of the syntactic dependencies Consider further the following French sentences nbsp dd The masculine subject le chien in a demands the masculine form of the predicative adjective blanc whereas the feminine subject la maison demands the feminine form of this adjective A morphological dependency that is entirely independent of the syntactic dependencies therefore points again across the syntactic hierarchy Morphological dependencies play an important role in typological studies Languages are classified as mostly head marking Sam work s or mostly dependent marking these houses whereby most if not all languages contain at least some minor measure of both head and dependent marking 16 Prosodic dependencies edit Prosodic dependencies are acknowledged in order to accommodate the behavior of clitics 17 A clitic is a syntactically autonomous element that is prosodically dependent on a host A clitic is therefore integrated into the prosody of its host meaning that it forms a single word with its host Prosodic dependencies exist entirely in the linear dimension horizontal dimension whereas standard syntactic dependencies exist in the hierarchical dimension vertical dimension Classic examples of clitics in English are reduced auxiliaries e g ll s ve and the possessive marker s The prosodic dependencies in the following examples are indicated with the hyphen and the lack of a vertical projection line nbsp The hyphens and lack of projection lines indicate prosodic dependencies A hyphen that appears on the left of the clitic indicates that the clitic is prosodically dependent on the word immediately to its left He ll There s whereas a hyphen that appears on the right side of the clitic not shown here indicates that the clitic is prosodically dependent on the word that appears immediately to its right A given clitic is often prosodically dependent on its syntactic dependent He ll There s or on its head would ve At other times it can depend prosodically on a word that is neither its head nor its immediate dependent Florida s Syntactic dependencies edit Syntactic dependencies are the focus of most work in DG as stated above How the presence and the direction of syntactic dependencies are determined is of course often open to debate In this regard it must be acknowledged that the validity of syntactic dependencies in the trees throughout this article is being taken for granted However these hierarchies are such that many DGs can largely support them although there will certainly be points of disagreement The basic question about how syntactic dependencies are discerned has proven difficult to answer definitively One should acknowledge in this area however that the basic task of identifying and discerning the presence and direction of the syntactic dependencies of DGs is no easier or harder than determining the constituent groupings of phrase structure grammars A variety of heuristics are employed to this end basic tests for constituents being useful tools the syntactic dependencies assumed in the trees in this article are grouping words together in a manner that most closely matches the results of standard permutation substitution and ellipsis tests for constituents Etymological considerations also provide helpful clues about the direction of dependencies A promising principle upon which to base the existence of syntactic dependencies is distribution 18 When one is striving to identify the root of a given phrase the word that is most responsible for determining the distribution of that phrase as a whole is its root Linear order and discontinuities editTraditionally DGs have had a different approach to linear order word order than phrase structure grammars Dependency structures are minimal compared to their phrase structure counterparts and these minimal structures allow one to focus intently on the two ordering dimensions 19 Separating the vertical dimension hierarchical order from the horizontal dimension linear order is easily accomplished This aspect of dependency structures has allowed DGs starting with Tesniere 1959 to focus on hierarchical order in a manner that is hardly possible for phrase structure grammars For Tesniere linear order was secondary to hierarchical order insofar as hierarchical order preceded linear order in the mind of a speaker The stemmas trees that Tesniere produced reflected this view they abstracted away from linear order to focus almost entirely on hierarchical order Many DGs that followed Tesniere adopted this practice that is they produced tree structures that reflect hierarchical order alone e g nbsp dd The traditional focus on hierarchical order generated the impression that DGs have little to say about linear order and it has contributed to the view that DGs are particularly well suited to examine languages with free word order A negative result of this focus on hierarchical order however is that there is a dearth of DG explorations of particular word order phenomena such as of standard discontinuities Comprehensive dependency grammar accounts of topicalization wh fronting scrambling and extraposition are mostly absent from many established DG frameworks This situation can be contrasted with phrase structure grammars which have devoted tremendous effort to exploring these phenomena The nature of the dependency relation does not however prevent one from focusing on linear order Dependency structures are as capable of exploring word order phenomena as phrase structures The following trees illustrate this point they represent one way of exploring discontinuities using dependency structures The trees suggest the manner in which common discontinuities can be addressed An example from German is used to illustrate a scrambling discontinuity nbsp dd The a trees on the left show projectivity violations crossing lines and the b trees on the right demonstrate one means of addressing these violations The displaced constituent takes on a word as its head that is not its governor The words in red mark the catena chain of words that extends from the root of the displaced constituent to the governor of that constituent 20 Discontinuities are then explored in terms of these catenae The limitations on topicalization wh fronting scrambling and extraposition can be explored and identified by examining the nature of the catenae involved Syntactic functions editTraditionally DGs have treated the syntactic functions grammatical functions grammatical relations as primitive They posit an inventory of functions e g subject object oblique determiner attribute predicative etc These functions can appear as labels on the dependencies in the tree structures e g 21 nbsp dd The syntactic functions in this tree are shown in green ATTR attribute COMP P complement of preposition COMP TO complement of to DET determiner P ATTR prepositional attribute PRED predicative SUBJ subject TO COMP to complement The functions chosen and abbreviations used in the tree here are merely representative of the general stance of DGs toward the syntactic functions The actual inventory of functions and designations employed vary from DG to DG As a primitive of the theory the status of these functions is very different from that in some phrase structure grammars Traditionally phrase structure grammars derive the syntactic functions from the constellation For instance the object is identified as the NP appearing inside finite VP and the subject as the NP appearing outside of finite VP Since DGs reject the existence of a finite VP constituent they were never presented with the option to view the syntactic functions in this manner The issue is a question of what comes first traditionally DGs take the syntactic functions to be primitive and they then derive the constellation from these functions whereas phrase structure grammars traditionally take the constellation to be primitive and they then derive the syntactic functions from the constellation This question about what comes first the functions or the constellation is not an inflexible matter The stances of both grammar types dependency and phrase structure are not narrowly limited to the traditional views Dependency and phrase structure are both fully compatible with both approaches to the syntactic functions Indeed monostratal systems that are solely based on dependency or phrase structure will likely reject the notion that the functions are derived from the constellation or that the constellation is derived from the functions They will take both to be primitive which means neither can be derived from the other See also editDependency relation in mathematics Head directionality parameter Igor Mel cuk Parse tree Recursive categorical syntaxNotes edit Concerning the history of the dependency concept see Percival 1990 Concerning the influence of term logic on the theory of grammar see Percival 1976 Concerning dependency in the works of Brassai see Imrenyi 2013 Concerning dependency in the works of Kern see Kern s essays e g Kern 1883 1884 Concerning dependency in the works of Tiktin see Coseriu 1980 Concerning early dependency grammars that may have developed independently of Tesniere s work see for instance Hays 1960 Gaifman 1965 and Robinson 1970 Some prominent dependency grammars that were well established by the 1980s are from Hudson 1984 Sgall Hajicova et Panevova 1986 Mel cuk 1988 and Starosta 1988 Some prominent dependency grammars from the German schools are from Heringer 1996 Engel 1994 Eroms 2000 and Agel et al 2003 6 is a massive two volume collection of essays on dependency grammar and valency theory from more than 100 authors The minimality of dependency structures is emphasized for instance by Ninio 2006 Hudson 2007 117 and by Osborne et al 2011 Concerning Tesniere s rejection of the subject predicate division of the clause see Tesniere 1959 103 105 and for discussion of empirical considerations that support Tesniere s point see Matthews 2007 17ff Miller 2011 54ff and Osborne et al 2011 323f The conventions illustrated with trees a and b are preferred by Osborne et al 2011 2013 Unordered trees like d are associated above all with Tesniere s stemmas and with the syntactic strata of Mel cuk s Meaning Text Theory Three major works on Word Grammar are Hudson 1984 1990 2007 Lobin 2003 makes heavy use of these indentations For a discussion of semantic morphological and syntactic dependencies in Meaning Text Theory see Melʹc uk 2003 191ff and Osborne 2019 Ch 5 Concerning semantic dependencies see Melʹc uk 2003 192f Concerning morphological dependencies see Melʹc uk 2003 193ff The distinction between head and dependent marking was established by Nichols 1986 Nichols was using a dependency based understanding of these distinctions Concerning prosodic dependencies and the analysis of clitics see Gross 2011 Distribution is primary principle used by Owens 1984 36 Schubert 1988 40 and Melʹc uk 2003 200 for discerning syntactic dependencies Concerning the importance of the two ordering dimensions see Tesniere 1959 16ff See Osborne et al 2012 concerning catenae For discussion and examples of the labels for syntactic functions that are attached to dependency edges and arcs see for instance Mel cuk 1988 22 69 and van Valin 2001 102ff References editAgel Vilmos Eichinger Ludwig M Eroms Hans Werner Hellwig Peter Heringer Hans Jurgen Lobin Henning eds 2003 Dependenz und Valenz Ein internationales Handbuch der zeitgenossischen Forschung Dependency and Valency An International Handbook of Contemporary Research in German Berlin de Gruyter ISBN 978 3110141900 Retrieved 24 August 2012 Coseriu E 1980 Un precurseur meconnu de la syntaxe structurale H Tiktin In Recherches de Linguistique Hommage a Maurice Leroy Editions de l Universite de Bruxelles 48 62 Engel U 1994 Syntax der deutschen Sprache 3rd edition Berlin Erich Schmidt Verlag Eroms Hans Werner 2000 Syntax der deutschen Sprache Berlin u a de Gruyter doi 10 1515 9783110808124 ISBN 978 3110156669 Retrieved 24 August 2012 Gross T 2011 Clitics in dependency morphology Depling 2011 Proceedings 58 68 Helbig Gerhard Buscha Joachim 2007 Deutsche Grammatik ein Handbuch fur den Auslanderunterricht German Grammar A Guide for Foreigners Teaching 6 Dr ed Berlin Langenscheidt ISBN 978 3 468 49493 2 Retrieved 24 August 2012 Heringer H 1996 Deutsche Syntax dependentiell Tubingen Stauffenburg Hays D 1960 Grouping and dependency theories P 1910 RAND Corporation Hays D 1964 Dependency theory A formalism and some observations Language 40 511 525 Reprinted in Syntactic Theory 1 Structuralist edited by Fred W Householder Penguin 1972 Hudson Richard 1984 Word grammar 1 publ ed Oxford OX England B Blackwell ISBN 978 0631131861 Hudson R 1990 English Word Grammar Oxford Basil Blackwell Hudson R 2007 Language Networks The New Word Grammar Oxford University Press Imrenyi A 2013 Constituency or dependency Notes on Samuel Brassai s syntactic model of Hungarian In Szigetvari Peter ed VLlxx Papers Presented to Laszlo Varga on his 70th Birthday Budapest Tinta 167 182 Kern F 1883 Zur Methodik des deutschen Unterrichts Berlin Nicolaische Verlags Buchhandlung Kern F 1884 Grundriss der Deutschen Satzlehre Berlin Nicolaische Verlags Buchhandlung Liu H 2009 Dependency Grammar from Theory to Practice Beijing Science Press Lobin H 2003 Koordinationssyntax als prozedurales Phanomen Tubingen Gunter Narr Verlag Matthews P H 2007 Syntactic Relations a critical survey 1 publ ed Cambridge Cambridge University Press ISBN 9780521608299 Retrieved 24 August 2012 Melʹc uk Igor A 1987 Dependency syntax theory and practice Albany State University Press of New York ISBN 978 0 88706 450 0 Retrieved 24 August 2012 Melʹc uk I 2003 Levels of dependency in linguistic description Concepts and problems In Agel et al 170 187 Miller J 2011 A critical introduction to syntax London continuum Nichols J 1986 Head marking and dependent marking languages Language 62 56 119 Ninio A 2006 Language and the learning curve A new theory of syntactic development Oxford Oxford University Press Osborne T 2019 A Dependency Grammar of English An Introduction and Beyond Amsterdam John Benjamins https doi org 10 1075 z 224 Osborne T M Putnam and T Gross 2011 Bare phrase structure label less trees and specifier less syntax Is Minimalism becoming a dependency grammar The Linguistic Review 28 315 364 Osborne T M Putnam and T Gross 2012 Catenae Introducing a novel unit of syntactic analysis Syntax 15 4 354 396 Owens J 1984 On getting a head A problem in dependency grammar Lingua 62 25 42 Percival K 1976 On the historical source of immediate constituent analysis In Notes from the linguistic underground James McCawley ed Syntax and Semantics 7 229 242 New York Academic Press Percival K 1990 Reflections on the history of dependency notions in linguistics Historiographia Linguistica 17 29 47 Robinson J 1970 Dependency structures and transformational rules Language 46 259 285 Schubert K 1988 Metataxis Contrastive dependency syntax for machine translation Dordrecht Foris Sgall P E Hajicova and J Panevova 1986 The meaning of the sentence in its semantic and pragmatic aspects Dordrecht D Reidel Publishing Company Starosta S 1988 The case for lexicase London Pinter Publishers Tesniere L 1959 Elements de syntaxe structurale Paris Klincksieck Tesniere L 1966 Elements de syntaxe structurale 2nd edition Paris Klincksieck Tesniere L 2015 Elements of structural syntax English translation of Tesniere 1966 John Benjamins Amsterdam van Valin R 2001 An introduction to syntax Cambridge UK Cambridge University Press External links editUniversal Dependencies a set of treebanks in a harmonized dependency grammar representation Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Dependency grammar amp oldid 1199671285, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.