fbpx
Wikipedia

B. F. Skinner

Burrhus Frederic Skinner (March 20, 1904 – August 18, 1990) was an American psychologist, behaviorist, inventor, and social philosopher.[2][3][4][5] He was the Edgar Pierce Professor of Psychology at Harvard University from 1958 until his retirement in 1974.[6]

B. F. Skinner
Skinner, c. 1950
Born
Burrhus Frederic Skinner

(1904-03-20)March 20, 1904
DiedAugust 18, 1990(1990-08-18) (aged 86)
Alma materHamilton College (AB)
Harvard University (PhD)
Known forBehavior analysis
Operant conditioning
Radical behaviorism
Verbal Behavior
Spouse
Yvonne (Eve) Blue
(m. 1936)
[1]
ChildrenJulie and Deborah
AwardsNational Medal of Science (1968)
Scientific career
FieldsPsychology, linguistics, philosophy
InstitutionsUniversity of Minnesota
Indiana University
Harvard University
Signature

Considering free will to be an illusion, Skinner saw human action as dependent on consequences of previous actions, a theory he would articulate as the principle of reinforcement: If the consequences to an action are bad, there is a high chance the action will not be repeated; if the consequences are good, the probability of the action being repeated becomes stronger.[7]

Skinner developed behavior analysis, especially the philosophy of radical behaviorism,[8] and founded the experimental analysis of behavior, a school of experimental research psychology. He also used operant conditioning to strengthen behavior, considering the rate of response to be the most effective measure of response strength. To study operant conditioning, he invented the operant conditioning chamber (aka the Skinner box),[7] and to measure rate he invented the cumulative recorder. Using these tools, he and Charles Ferster produced Skinner's most influential experimental work, outlined in their 1957 book Schedules of Reinforcement.[9][10]

Skinner was a prolific author, publishing 21 books and 180 articles.[11] He imagined the application of his ideas to the design of a human community in his 1948 utopian novel, Walden Two,[3] while his analysis of human behavior culminated in his 1958 work, Verbal Behavior.[12]

Skinner, John B. Watson and Ivan Pavlov, are considered to be the pioneers of modern behaviorism. Accordingly, a June 2002 survey listed Skinner as the most influential psychologist of the 20th century.[13]

Biography edit

Skinner was born in Susquehanna, Pennsylvania, to Grace and William Skinner, the latter of whom was a lawyer. Skinner became an atheist after a Christian teacher tried to assuage his fear of the hell that his grandmother described.[14] His brother Edward, two and a half years younger, died at age 16 of a cerebral hemorrhage.[15]

Skinner's closest friend as a young boy was Raphael Miller, whom he called Doc because his father was a doctor. Doc and Skinner became friends due to their parents' religiousness and both had an interest in contraptions and gadgets. They had set up a telegraph line between their houses to send messages to each other, although they had to call each other on the telephone due to the confusing messages sent back and forth. During one summer, Doc and Skinner started an elderberry business to gather berries and sell them door to door. They found that when they picked the ripe berries, the unripe ones came off the branches too, so they built a device that was able to separate them. The device was a bent piece of metal to form a trough. They would pour water down the trough into a bucket, and the ripe berries would sink into the bucket and the unripe ones would be pushed over the edge to be thrown away.[16]

Education edit

Skinner attended Hamilton College in Clinton, New York, with the intention of becoming a writer. He found himself at a social disadvantage at the college because of his intellectual attitude.[further explanation needed][17] He was a member of Lambda Chi Alpha fraternity.[16]

He wrote for the school paper, but, as an atheist, he was critical of the traditional mores of his college. After receiving his Bachelor of Arts in English literature in 1926, he attended Harvard University, where he would later research and teach. While attending Harvard, a fellow student, Fred S. Keller, convinced Skinner that he could make an experimental science of the study of behavior. This led Skinner to invent a prototype for the Skinner box and to join Keller in the creation of other tools for small experiments.[17]

After graduation, Skinner unsuccessfully tried to write a novel while he lived with his parents, a period that he later called the "Dark Years".[17] He became disillusioned with his literary skills despite encouragement from the renowned poet Robert Frost, concluding that he had little world experience and no strong personal perspective from which to write. His encounter with John B. Watson's behaviorism led him into graduate study in psychology and to the development of his own version of behaviorism.[17]

Later life edit

 
The gravestone of B. F. Skinner and his wife Eve at Mount Auburn Cemetery

Skinner received a PhD from Harvard in 1931, and remained there as a researcher for some years. In 1936, he went to the University of Minnesota in Minneapolis to teach.[18] In 1945, he moved to Indiana University,[19] where he was chair of the psychology department from 1946 to 1947, before returning to Harvard as a tenured professor in 1948. He remained at Harvard for the rest of his life. In 1973, Skinner was one of the signers of the Humanist Manifesto II.[20]

In 1936, Skinner married Yvonne "Eve" Blue. The couple had two daughters, Julie (later Vargas) and Deborah (later Buzan; married Barry Buzan).[21][22] Yvonne died in 1997,[23] and is buried in Mount Auburn Cemetery, Cambridge, Massachusetts.[17]

Skinner's public exposure had increased in the 1970s, he remained active even after his retirement in 1974, until his death. In 1989, Skinner was diagnosed with leukemia and died on August 18, 1990, in Cambridge, Massachusetts. Ten days before his death, he was given the lifetime achievement award by the American Psychological Association and gave a talk concerning his work.[24]

Contributions to psychology edit

Behaviorism edit

Skinner referred to his approach to the study of behavior as radical behaviorism,[25] which originated in the early 1900s as a reaction to depth psychology and other traditional forms of psychology, which often had difficulty making predictions that could be tested experimentally. This philosophy of behavioral science assumes that behavior is a consequence of environmental histories of reinforcement (see applied behavior analysis). In his words:

The position can be stated as follows: what is felt or introspectively observed is not some nonphysical world of consciousness, mind, or mental life but the observer's own body. This does not mean, as I shall show later, that introspection is a kind of psychological research, nor does it mean (and this is the heart of the argument) that what are felt or introspectively observed are the causes of the behavior. An organism behaves as it does because of its current structure, but most of this is out of reach of introspection. At the moment we must content ourselves, as the methodological behaviorist insists, with a person's genetic and environment histories. What are introspectively observed are certain collateral products of those histories.... In this way we repair the major damage wrought by mentalism. When what a person does [is] attributed to what is going on inside him, investigation is brought to an end. Why explain the explanation? For twenty-five hundred years people have been preoccupied with feelings and mental life, but only recently has any interest been shown in a more precise analysis of the role of the environment. Ignorance of that role led in the first place to mental fictions, and it has been perpetuated by the explanatory practices to which they gave rise.[25]

Foundations of Skinner's behaviorism edit

Skinner's ideas about behaviorism were largely set forth in his first book, The Behavior of Organisms (1938).[9] Here, he gives a systematic description of the manner in which environmental variables control behavior. He distinguished two sorts of behavior which are controlled in different ways:

  • Respondent behaviors are elicited by stimuli, and may be modified through respondent conditioning, often called classical (or pavlovian) conditioning, in which a neutral stimulus is paired with an eliciting stimulus. Such behaviors may be measured by their latency or strength.
  • Operant behaviors are 'emitted', meaning that initially they are not induced by any particular stimulus. They are strengthened through operant conditioning (aka instrumental conditioning), in which the occurrence of a response yields a reinforcer. Such behaviors may be measured by their rate.

Both of these sorts of behavior had already been studied experimentally, most notably: respondents, by Ivan Pavlov;[26] and operants, by Edward Thorndike.[27] Skinner's account differed in some ways from earlier ones,[28] and was one of the first accounts to bring them under one roof.

The idea that behavior is strengthened or weakened by its consequences raises several questions. Among the most commonly asked are these:

  1. Operant responses are strengthened by reinforcement, but where do they come from in the first place?
  2. Once it is in the organism's repertoire, how is a response directed or controlled?
  3. How can very complex and seemingly novel behaviors be explained?

1. Origin of operant behavior edit

Skinner's answer to the first question was very much like Darwin's answer to the question of the origin of a 'new' bodily structure, namely, variation and selection. Similarly, the behavior of an individual varies from moment to moment; a variation that is followed by reinforcement is strengthened and becomes prominent in that individual's behavioral repertoire. Shaping was Skinner's term for the gradual modification of behavior by the reinforcement of desired variations. Skinner believed that 'superstitious' behavior can arise when a response happens to be followed by reinforcement to which it is actually unrelated.[clarification needed]

2. Control of operant behavior edit

The second question, "how is operant behavior controlled?" arises because, to begin with, the behavior is "emitted" without reference to any particular stimulus. Skinner answered this question by saying that a stimulus comes to control an operant if it is present when the response is reinforced and absent when it is not. For example, if lever-pressing only brings food when a light is on, a rat, or a child, will learn to press the lever only when the light is on. Skinner summarized this relationship by saying that a discriminative stimulus (e.g. light or sound) sets the occasion for the reinforcement (food) of the operant (lever-press). This three-term contingency (stimulus-response-reinforcer) is one of Skinner's most important concepts, and sets his theory apart from theories that use only pair-wise associations.[28]

3. Explaining complex behavior edit

Most behavior of humans cannot easily be described in terms of individual responses reinforced one by one, and Skinner devoted a great deal of effort to the problem of behavioral complexity. Some complex behavior can be seen as a sequence of relatively simple responses, and here Skinner invoked the idea of "chaining". Chaining is based on the fact, experimentally demonstrated, that a discriminative stimulus not only sets the occasion for subsequent behavior, but it can also reinforce a behavior that precedes it. That is, a discriminative stimulus is also a "conditioned reinforcer". For example, the light that sets the occasion for lever pressing may also be used to reinforce "turning around" in the presence of a noise. This results in the sequence "noise – turn-around – light – press lever – food." Much longer chains can be built by adding more stimuli and responses.

However, Skinner recognized that a great deal of behavior, especially human behavior, cannot be accounted for by gradual shaping or the construction of response sequences.[29] Complex behavior often appears suddenly in its final form, as when a person first finds his way to the elevator by following instructions given at the front desk. To account for such behavior, Skinner introduced the concept of rule-governed behavior. First, relatively simple behaviors come under the control of verbal stimuli: the child learns to "jump," "open the book," and so on. After a large number of responses come under such verbal control, a sequence of verbal stimuli can evoke an almost unlimited variety of complex responses.[29]

Reinforcement edit

Reinforcement, a key concept of behaviorism, is the primary process that shapes and controls behavior, and occurs in two ways: positive and negative. In The Behavior of Organisms (1938), Skinner defines negative reinforcement to be synonymous with punishment, i.e. the presentation of an aversive stimulus. This definition would subsequently be re-defined in Science and Human Behavior (1953).

In what has now become the standard set of definitions, positive reinforcement is the strengthening of behavior by the occurrence of some event (e.g., praise after some behavior is performed), whereas negative reinforcement is the strengthening of behavior by the removal or avoidance of some aversive event (e.g., opening and raising an umbrella over your head on a rainy day is reinforced by the cessation of rain falling on you).

Both types of reinforcement strengthen behavior, or increase the probability of a behavior reoccurring; the difference being in whether the reinforcing event is something applied (positive reinforcement) or something removed or avoided (negative reinforcement). Punishment can be the application of an aversive stimulus/event (positive punishment or punishment by contingent stimulation) or the removal of a desirable stimulus (negative punishment or punishment by contingent withdrawal). Though punishment is often used to suppress behavior, Skinner argued that this suppression is temporary and has a number of other, often unwanted, consequences.[30] Extinction is the absence of a rewarding stimulus, which weakens behavior.

Writing in 1981, Skinner pointed out that Darwinian natural selection is, like reinforced behavior, "selection by consequences." Though, as he said, natural selection has now "made its case," he regretted that essentially the same process, "reinforcement", was less widely accepted as underlying human behavior.[31]

Schedules of reinforcement edit

Skinner recognized that behavior is typically reinforced more than once, and, together with Charles Ferster, he did an extensive analysis of the various ways in which reinforcements could be arranged over time, calling it the schedules of reinforcement.[10]

The most notable schedules of reinforcement studied by Skinner were continuous, interval (fixed or variable), and ratio (fixed or variable). All are methods used in operant conditioning.

  • Continuous reinforcement (CRF): each time a specific action is performed the subject receives a reinforcement. This method is effective when teaching a new behavior because it quickly establishes an association between the target behavior and the reinforcer.[32]
  • Interval schedule: based on the time intervals between reinforcements.[7]
    • Fixed interval schedule (FI): A procedure in which reinforcements are presented at fixed time periods, provided that the appropriate response is made. This schedule yields a response rate that is low just after reinforcement and becomes rapid just before the next reinforcement is scheduled.
    • Variable interval schedule (VI): A procedure in which behavior is reinforced after scheduled but unpredictable time durations following the previous reinforcement. This schedule yields the most stable rate of responding, with the average frequency of reinforcement determining the frequency of response.
  • Ratio schedules: based on the ratio of responses to reinforcements.[7]
    • Fixed ratio schedule (FR): A procedure in which reinforcement is delivered after a specific number of responses have been made.
    • Variable ratio schedule (VR):[7] A procedure in which reinforcement comes after a number of responses that is randomized from one reinforcement to the next (e.g. slot machines). The lower the number of responses required, the higher the response rate tends to be. Variable ratio schedules tend to produce very rapid and steady responding rates in contrast with fixed ratio schedules where the frequency of response usually drops after the reinforcement occurs.

Token economy edit

"Skinnerian" principles have been used to create token economies in a number of institutions, such as psychiatric hospitals. When participants behave in desirable ways, their behavior is reinforced with tokens that can be changed for such items as candy, cigarettes, coffee, or the exclusive use of a radio or television set.[33]

Verbal Behavior edit

Challenged by Alfred North Whitehead during a casual discussion while at Harvard to provide an account of a randomly provided piece of verbal behavior,[34] Skinner set about attempting to extend his then-new functional, inductive approach to the complexity of human verbal behavior.[35] Developed over two decades, his work appeared in the book Verbal Behavior. Although Noam Chomsky was highly critical of Verbal Behavior, he conceded that Skinner's "S-R psychology" was worth a review.[36] (behavior analysts reject the "S-R" characterization: operant conditioning involves the emission of a response which then becomes more or less likely depending upon its consequence.)[36]

Verbal Behavior had an uncharacteristically cool reception, partly as a result of Chomsky's review, partly because of Skinner's failure to address or rebut any of Chomsky's criticisms.[37] Skinner's peers may have been slow to adopt the ideas presented in Verbal Behavior because of the absence of experimental evidence—unlike the empirical density that marked Skinner's experimental work.[38]

Scientific inventions edit

Operant conditioning chamber edit

An operant conditioning chamber (also known as a "Skinner box") is a laboratory apparatus used in the experimental analysis of animal behavior. It was invented by Skinner while he was a graduate student at Harvard University. As used by Skinner, the box had a lever (for rats), or a disk in one wall (for pigeons). A press on this "manipulandum" could deliver food to the animal through an opening in the wall, and responses reinforced in this way increased in frequency. By controlling this reinforcement together with discriminative stimuli such as lights and tones, or punishments such as electric shocks, experimenters have used the operant box to study a wide variety of topics, including schedules of reinforcement, discriminative control, delayed response ("memory"), punishment, and so on. By channeling research in these directions, the operant conditioning chamber has had a huge influence on course of research in animal learning and its applications. It enabled great progress on problems that could be studied by measuring the rate, probability, or force of a simple, repeatable response. However, it discouraged the study of behavioral processes not easily conceptualized in such terms—spatial learning, in particular, which is now studied in quite different ways, for example, by the use of the water maze.[28]

Cumulative recorder edit

The cumulative recorder makes a pen-and-ink record of simple repeated responses. Skinner designed it for use with the operant chamber as a convenient way to record and view the rate of responses such as a lever press or a key peck. In this device, a sheet of paper gradually unrolls over a cylinder. Each response steps a small pen across the paper, starting at one edge; when the pen reaches the other edge, it quickly resets to the initial side. The slope of the resulting ink line graphically displays the rate of the response; for example, rapid responses yield a steeply sloping line on the paper, slow responding yields a line of low slope. The cumulative recorder was a key tool used by Skinner in his analysis of behavior, and it was very widely adopted by other experimenters, gradually falling out of use with the advent of the laboratory computer and use of line graphs.[39] Skinner's major experimental exploration of response rates, presented in his book with Charles Ferster, Schedules of Reinforcement, is full of cumulative records produced by this device.[10]

Air crib edit

The air crib is an easily cleaned, temperature- and humidity-controlled box-bed intended to replace the standard infant crib. After raising one baby, Skinner felt that he could simplify the process for parents and improve the experience for children. He primarily thought of the idea to help his wife cope with the day-to-day tasks of child rearing. Skinner had some specific concerns about raising a baby in the rough environment where he lived in Minnesota. Keeping the child warm was a central priority (Faye, 2007).[40] Though this was the main goal, it also was designed to reduce laundry, diaper rash, and cradle cap, while still allowing the baby to be more mobile and comfortable, and less prone to cry. Reportedly it had some success in these goals and was used with an estimate of 300 children who were raised in the air crib.   The notorious crib was advertised commercially. Psychology Today tracked down 50 children and ran a short piece on the effects of the air crib. The reports came back positive and that these children and parents enjoyed using the crib (Epstein, 2080).[41] One of these air cribs resides in the gallery at the Center for the History of Psychology in Akron, Ohio (Faye, -3000).[40]

The air crib was designed with three solid walls and a safety-glass panel at the front which could be lowered to move the baby in and out of the crib. The floor was stretched canvas. Sheets were intended to be used over the canvas and were easily rolled off when soiled. Addressing Skinners' concern for temperature, a control box on top of the crib regulated temperature and humidity. Filtered air flowed through the crib from below. This crib was higher than most standard cribs, allowing easier access to the child without the need to bend over (Faye, 2010[40]).

The air crib was a controversial invention. It was popularly characterized as a cruel pen, and it was often compared to Skinner's operant conditioning chamber (or "Skinner box"). An article, titled "Baby in a Box," caught the eye of many and contributed to skepticism about the device (Bjork, 1997).[42] A picture published with the article showed the Skinners' daughter, Deborah, peering out of the crib with her hands and face pressed upon the glass. Skinner also used the term "experiment" when describing the crib, and this association with laboratory animal experimentation discouraged the crib's commercial success, although several companies attempted to produce and sell it.

In 2004, therapist Lauren Slater repeated a claim that Skinner may have used his baby daughter in some of his experiments. His outraged daughter publicly accused Slater of not making a good-faith effort to check her facts before publishing. Debora was quoted by the Guardian saying "According to Opening Skinner's Box: Great Psychological Experiments of the Twentieth Century, my father, who was a psychologist based at Harvard from the 1950s to the 90s, "used his infant daughter, Deborah, to prove his theories by putting her for a few hours a day in a laboratory box . . . in which all her needs were controlled and shaped". But it's not true. My father did nothing of the sort."[43]

Teaching machine edit

 
The teaching machine, a mechanical invention to automate the task of programmed learning

The teaching machine was a mechanical device whose purpose was to administer a curriculum of programmed learning. The machine embodies key elements of Skinner's theory of learning and had important implications for education in general and classroom instruction in particular.[44]

In one incarnation, the machine was a box that housed a list of questions that could be viewed one at a time through a small window. (see picture.) There was also a mechanism through which the learner could respond to each question. Upon delivering a correct answer, the learner would be rewarded.[45]

Skinner advocated the use of teaching machines for a broad range of students (e.g., preschool aged to adult) and instructional purposes (e.g., reading and music). For example, one machine that he envisioned could teach rhythm. He wrote:[46]

A relatively simple device supplies the necessary contingencies. The student taps a rhythmic pattern in unison with the device. "Unison" is specified very loosely at first (the student can be a little early or late at each tap) but the specifications are slowly sharpened. The process is repeated for various speeds and patterns. In another arrangement, the student echoes rhythmic patterns sounded by the machine, though not in unison, and again the specifications for an accurate reproduction are progressively sharpened. Rhythmic patterns can also be brought under the control of a printed score.

The instructional potential of the teaching machine stemmed from several factors: it provided automatic, immediate and regular reinforcement without the use of aversive control; the material presented was coherent, yet varied and novel; the pace of learning could be adjusted to suit the individual. As a result, students were interested, attentive, and learned efficiently by producing the desired behavior, "learning by doing."[47]

Teaching machines, though perhaps rudimentary, were not rigid instruments of instruction. They could be adjusted and improved based upon the students' performance. For example, if a student made many incorrect responses, the machine could be reprogrammed to provide less advanced prompts or questions—the idea being that students acquire behaviors most efficiently if they make few errors. Multiple-choice formats were not well-suited for teaching machines because they tended to increase student mistakes, and the contingencies of reinforcement were relatively uncontrolled.

Not only useful in teaching explicit skills, machines could also promote the development of a repertoire of behaviors that Skinner called self-management. Effective self-management means attending to stimuli appropriate to a task, avoiding distractions, reducing the opportunity of reward for competing behaviors, and so on. For example, machines encourage students to pay attention before receiving a reward. Skinner contrasted this with the common classroom practice of initially capturing students' attention (e.g., with a lively video) and delivering a reward (e.g., entertainment) before the students have actually performed any relevant behavior. This practice fails to reinforce correct behavior and actually counters the development of self-management.

Skinner pioneered the use of teaching machines in the classroom, especially at the primary level. Today computers run software that performs similar teaching tasks, and there has been a resurgence of interest in the topic related to the development of adaptive learning systems.[48]

Pigeon-guided missile edit

During World War II, the US Navy required a weapon effective against surface ships, such as the German Bismarck class battleships. Although missile and TV technology existed, the size of the primitive guidance systems available rendered automatic guidance impractical. To solve this problem, Skinner initiated Project Pigeon, which was intended to provide a simple and effective guidance system. Skinner trained pigeons through operant conditioning to peck a camera obscura screen showing incoming targets on individual screens (Schultz-Figueroa, 2019).[49] This system divided the nose cone of a missile into three compartments, with a pigeon placed in each. Within the ship, the three lenses projected an image of distant objects onto a screen in front of each bird. Thus, when the missile was launched from an aircraft within sight of an enemy ship, an image of the ship would appear on the screen. The screen was hinged, which connected the screens to the bomb's guidance system. This was done through four small rubber pneumatic tubes that were attached to each side of the frame, which directed a constant airflow to a pneumatic pickup system that controlled the thrusters of the bomb. Resulting in the missile being guided towards the targeted ship, through just the peck coming from the pigeon (Schultz-Figueroa, 2019).[49]

Despite an effective demonstration, the project was abandoned, and eventually more conventional solutions, such as those based on radar, became available. Skinner complained that "our problem was no one would take us seriously." Before the project was completely abandoned it was tested extensively in the laboratory. After the United States Army ultimately denied it the United States Naval Research Laboratory picked up Skinner's Research and renamed it Project ORCON, which was a contraction of "organic" and "control". Skinner worked closely with the US Naval Research Laboratory continuously testing the pigeon's tracking capacity for guiding missiles to their intended targets. In the end, the pigeons' performance and accuracy relied on so many uncontrollable factors that Project ORCON, like Project Pigeon before it, was again discontinued. It was never used in the field.[49]

Verbal summator edit

Early in his career Skinner became interested in "latent speech" and experimented with a device he called the verbal summator.[50] This device can be thought of as an auditory version of the Rorschach inkblots.[50] When using the device, human participants listened to incomprehensible auditory "garbage" but often read meaning into what they heard. Thus, as with the Rorschach blots, the device was intended to yield overt behavior that projected subconscious thoughts. Skinner's interest in projective testing was brief, but he later used observations with the summator in creating his theory of verbal behavior. The device also led other researchers to invent new tests such as the tautophone test, the auditory apperception test, and the Azzageddi[when defined as?] test.[51]

Influence on teaching edit

Along with psychology, education has also been influenced by Skinner's views, which are extensively presented in his book The Technology of Teaching, as well as reflected in Fred S. Keller's Personalized System of Instruction and Ogden R. Lindsley's Precision Teaching.

Skinner argued that education has two major purposes:

  1. to teach repertoires of both verbal and nonverbal behavior; and
  2. to interest students in learning.

He recommended bringing students' behavior under appropriate control by providing reinforcement only in the presence of stimuli relevant to the learning task. Because he believed that human behavior can be affected by small consequences, something as simple as "the opportunity to move forward after completing one stage of an activity" can be an effective reinforcer. Skinner was convinced that, to learn, a student must engage in behavior, and not just passively receive information.[44]: 389 

Skinner believed that effective teaching must be based on positive reinforcement which is, he argued, more effective at changing and establishing behavior than punishment. He suggested that the main thing people learn from being punished is how to avoid punishment. For example, if a child is forced to practice playing an instrument, the child comes to associate practicing with punishment and thus develops feelings of dreadfulness and wishes to avoid practicing the instrument. This view had obvious implications for the then widespread practice of rote learning and punitive discipline in education. The use of educational activities as punishment may induce rebellious behavior such as vandalism or absence.[52]

Because teachers are primarily responsible for modifying student behavior, Skinner argued that teachers must learn effective ways of teaching. In The Technology of Teaching (1968), Skinner has a chapter on why teachers fail:[53]: 93–113  He says that teachers have not been given an in-depth understanding of teaching and learning. Without knowing the science underpinning teaching, teachers fall back on procedures that work poorly or not at all, such as:

  • using aversive techniques (which produce escape and avoidance and undesirable emotional effects);
  • relying on telling and explaining ("Unfortunately, a student does not learn simply when he is shown or told.");[53]: 103 
  • failing to adapt learning tasks to the student's current level; and
  • failing to provide positive reinforcement frequently enough.

Skinner suggests that any age-appropriate skill can be taught. The steps are

  1. Clearly specify the action or performance the student is to learn.
  2. Break down the task into small achievable steps, going from simple to complex.
  3. Let the student perform each step, reinforcing correct actions.
  4. Adjust so that the student is always successful until finally the goal is reached.
  5. Shift to intermittent reinforcement to maintain the student's performance.

Contributions to social theory edit

Skinner is popularly known mainly for his books Walden Two (1948) and Beyond Freedom and Dignity, (for which he made the cover of Time magazine).[54] The former describes a fictional "experimental community"[55] in 1940s United States. The productivity and happiness of citizens in this community is far greater than in the outside world because the residents practice scientific social planning and use operant conditioning in raising their children.

Walden Two, like Thoreau's Walden, champions a lifestyle that does not support war, or foster competition and social strife. It encourages a lifestyle of minimal consumption, rich social relationships, personal happiness, satisfying work, and leisure.[56] In 1967, Kat Kinkade and others founded the Twin Oaks Community, using Walden Two as a blueprint. The community still exists and continues to use the Planner-Manager system and other aspects of the community described in Skinner's book, though behavior modification is not a community practice.[57]

In Beyond Freedom and Dignity, Skinner suggests that a technology of behavior could help to make a better society. We would, however, have to accept that an autonomous agent is not the driving force of our actions. Skinner offers alternatives to punishment, and challenges his readers to use science and modern technology to construct a better society.

Political views edit

Skinner's political writings emphasized his hopes that an effective and human science of behavioral control – a technology of human behavior – could help with problems as yet unsolved and often aggravated by advances in technology such as the atomic bomb. Indeed, one of Skinner's goals was to prevent humanity from destroying itself.[58] He saw political activity as the use of aversive or non-aversive means to control a population. Skinner favored the use of positive reinforcement as a means of control, citing Jean-Jacques Rousseau's novel Emile: or, On Education as an example of literature that "did not fear the power of positive reinforcement."[3]

Skinner's book, Walden Two, presents a vision of a decentralized, localized society, which applies a practical, scientific approach and behavioral expertise to deal peacefully with social problems. (For example, his views led him to oppose corporal punishment in schools, and he wrote a letter to the California Senate that helped lead it to a ban on spanking.[59]) Skinner's utopia is both a thought experiment and a rhetorical piece. In Walden Two, Skinner answers the problem that exists in many utopian novels – "What is the Good Life?" The book's answer is a life of friendship, health, art, a healthy balance between work and leisure, a minimum of unpleasantness, and a feeling that one has made worthwhile contributions to a society in which resources are ensured, in part, by minimizing consumption.

If the world is to save any part of its resources for the future, it must reduce not only consumption but the number of consumers.

— B. F. Skinner, Walden Two (1948), p. xi

Skinner described his novel as "my New Atlantis", in reference to Bacon's utopia.[60]

When Milton's Satan falls from heaven, he ends in hell. And what does he say to reassure himself? 'Here, at least, we shall be free.' And that, I think, is the fate of the old-fashioned liberal. He's going to be free, but he's going to find himself in hell.

— B. F. Skinner, from William F. Buckley Jr, On the Firing Line, p. 87.

"'Superstition' in the Pigeon" experiment edit

One of Skinner's experiments examined the formation of superstition in one of his favorite experimental animals, the pigeon. Skinner placed a series of hungry pigeons in a cage attached to an automatic mechanism that delivered food to the pigeon "at regular intervals with no reference whatsoever to the bird's behavior."[61] He discovered that the pigeons associated the delivery of the food with whatever chance actions they had been performing as it was delivered, and that they subsequently continued to perform these same actions.[61]

One bird was conditioned to turn counter-clockwise about the cage, making two or three turns between reinforcements. Another repeatedly thrust its head into one of the upper corners of the cage. A third developed a 'tossing' response, as if placing its head beneath an invisible bar and lifting it repeatedly. Two birds developed a pendulum motion of the head and body, in which the head was extended forward and swung from right to left with a sharp movement followed by a somewhat slower return.

Skinner suggested that the pigeons behaved as if they were influencing the automatic mechanism with their "rituals", and that this experiment shed light on human behavior:[61]

The experiment might be said to demonstrate a sort of superstition. The bird behaves as if there were a causal relation between its behavior and the presentation of food, although such a relation is lacking. There are many analogies in human behavior. Rituals for changing one's fortune at cards are good examples. A few accidental connections between a ritual and favorable consequences suffice to set up and maintain the behavior in spite of many unreinforced instances. The bowler who has released a ball down the alley but continues to behave as if she were controlling it by twisting and turning her arm and shoulder is another case in point. These behaviors have, of course, no real effect upon one's luck or upon a ball half way down an alley, just as in the present case the food would appear as often if the pigeon did nothing—or, more strictly speaking, did something else.

Modern behavioral psychologists have disputed Skinner's "superstition" explanation for the behaviors he recorded. Subsequent research (e.g. Staddon and Simmelhag, 1971), while finding similar behavior, failed to find support for Skinner's "adventitious reinforcement" explanation for it. By looking at the timing of different behaviors within the interval, Staddon and Simmelhag were able to distinguish two classes of behavior: the terminal response, which occurred in anticipation of food, and interim responses, that occurred earlier in the interfood interval and were rarely contiguous with food. Terminal responses seem to reflect classical (as opposed to operant) conditioning, rather than adventitious reinforcement, guided by a process like that observed in 1968 by Brown and Jenkins in their "autoshaping" procedures. The causation of interim activities (such as the schedule-induced polydipsia seen in a similar situation with rats) also cannot be traced to adventitious reinforcement and its details are still obscure (Staddon, 1977).[62]

Criticism edit

Noam Chomsky edit

American linguist Noam Chomsky published a review of Skinner's Verbal Behavior in the linguistics journal Language in 1959.[63] Chomsky argued that Skinner's attempt to use behaviorism to explain human language amounted to little more than word games. Conditioned responses could not account for a child's ability to create or understand an infinite variety of novel sentences. Chomsky's review has been credited with launching the cognitive revolution in psychology and other disciplines. Skinner, who rarely responded directly to critics, never formally replied to Chomsky's critique, but endorsed Kenneth MacCorquodale's 1972 reply.[64]

I read half a dozen pages, saw that it missed the point of my book, and went no further. [...] My reasons, I am afraid, show a lack of character. In the first place I should have had to read the review, and I found its tone distasteful. It was not really a review of my book but of what Chomsky took, erroneously, to be my position.[65]

Many academics in the 1960s believed that Skinner's silence on the question meant Chomsky's criticism had been justified. But MacCorquodale wrote that Chomsky's criticism did not focus on Skinner's Verbal Behavior, but rather attacked a confusion of ideas from behavioral psychology. MacCorquodale also regretted Chomsky's aggressive tone.[64] Furthermore, Chomsky had aimed at delivering a definitive refutation of Skinner by citing dozens of animal instinct and animal learning studies. On the one hand, he argued that the studies on animal instinct proved that animal behavior is innate, and therefore Skinner was mistaken. On the other, Chomsky's opinion of the studies on learning was that one cannot draw an analogy from animal studies to human behavior—or, that research on animal instinct refutes research on animal learning.[63][66]

Chomsky also reviewed Skinner's Beyond Freedom and Dignity, using the same basic motives as his Verbal Behavior review. Among Chomsky's criticisms were that Skinner's laboratory work could not be extended to humans, that when it was extended to humans it represented "scientistic" behavior attempting to emulate science but which was not scientific, that Skinner was not a scientist because he rejected the hypothetico-deductive model of theory testing, and that Skinner had no science of behavior.[67]

Psychodynamic psychology edit

Skinner has been repeatedly criticized for his supposed animosity towards Sigmund Freud, psychoanalysis, and psychodynamic psychology. Some have argued, however, that Skinner shared several of Freud's assumptions, and that he was influenced by Freudian points of view in more than one field, among them the analysis of defense mechanisms, such as repression.[68][69] To study such phenomena, Skinner even designed his own projective test, the "verbal summator" described above.[70]

J. E. R. Staddon edit

As understood by Skinner, ascribing dignity to individuals involves giving them credit for their actions. To say "Skinner is brilliant" means that Skinner is an originating force. If Skinner's determinist theory is right, he is merely the focus of his environment. He is not an originating force and he had no choice in saying the things he said or doing the things he did. Skinner's environment and genetics both allowed and compelled him to write his book. Similarly, the environment and genetic potentials of the advocates of freedom and dignity cause them to resist the reality that their own activities are deterministically grounded. J. E. R. Staddon has argued the compatibilist position;[71] Skinner's determinism is not in any way contradictory to traditional notions of reward and punishment, as he believed.[72][73]

Professional career edit

Roles edit

Awards edit

Honorary degrees edit

Skinner received honorary degrees from:

Honorary societies edit

Skinner was inducted to the following honorary societies:

Bibliography edit

  • 1938. The Behavior of Organisms: An Experimental Analysis, 1938. ISBN 1-58390-007-1, ISBN 0-87411-487-X.
  • 1948. Walden Two. ISBN 0-87220-779-X (revised 1976 ed.).
  • 1953. Science and Human Behavior. ISBN 0-02-929040-6.[i]
  • 1957. Schedules of Reinforcement, with C. B. Ferster. ISBN 0-13-792309-0.
  • 1957. Verbal Behavior. ISBN 1-58390-021-7.
  • 1961. The Analysis of Behavior: A Program for Self Instruction, with James G. Holland. ISBN 0-07-029565-4.
  • 1968.The Technology of Teaching. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. LCCN 68--12340 ISBN 0-13-902163-9.
  • 1969. Contingencies of Reinforcement: A Theoretical Analysis. ISBN 0-390-81280-3.
  • 1971. Beyond Freedom and Dignity. ISBN 0-394-42555-3.
  • 1974. About Behaviorism. ISBN 0-394-71618-3.
  • 1976. Particulars of My Life: Part One of an Autobiography. ISBN 0-394-40071-2.
  • 1978. Reflections on Behaviorism and Society. ISBN 0-13-770057-1.
  • 1979. The Shaping of a Behaviorist: Part Two of an Autobiography. ISBN 0-394-50581-6.
  • 1980. Notebooks, edited by Robert Epstein. ISBN 0-13-624106-9.
  • 1982. Skinner for the Classroom, edited by R. Epstein. ISBN 0-87822-261-8.
  • 1983. Enjoy Old Age: A Program of Self-Management, with M. E. Vaughan. ISBN 0-393-01805-9.
  • 1983. A Matter of Consequences: Part Three of an Autobiography. ISBN 0-394-53226-0, ISBN 0-8147-7845-3.
  • 1987. Upon Further Reflection. ISBN 0-13-938986-5.
  • 1989. Recent Issues in the Analysis of Behavior. ISBN 0-675-20674-X.
  • Cumulative Record: A Selection of Papers, 1959, 1961, 1972 and 1999 as Cumulative Record: Definitive Edition. ISBN 0-87411-969-3 (paperback)
    • Includes reprint: Skinner, B. F. 1945. "Baby in a Box." Ladies' Home Journal. — Skinner's original, personal account of the much-misrepresented "Baby in a box" device.

See also edit

References edit

Notes edit

  1. ^ A free copy of this book (in a 1.6 MB .pdf file) may be downloaded at the B. F. Skinner Foundation web site BFSkinner.org.

Citations edit

  1. ^ Sobel, Dava (August 20, 1990). . The New York Times. Archived from the original on August 6, 2010. Retrieved August 30, 2015.
  2. ^ Smith, L. D.; Woodward, W. R. (1996). B. F. Skinner and Behaviorism in American Culture. Bethlehem, Pennsylvania: Lehigh University Press. ISBN 978-0-934223-40-9.
  3. ^ a b c Skinner, B. F. (1948). Walden Two. New York: Macmillan Publishers. ISBN 0-87220-779-X. The science of human behavior is used to eliminate poverty, sexual expression, government as we know it, create a lifestyle without that such as war.
  4. ^ Skinner, B. F. (1972). Beyond Freedom and Dignity. Vintage Books. ISBN 978-0-553-14372-0. OCLC 34263003.
  5. ^ "Skinner, Burrhus Frederic". History of Behavior Analysis. Retrieved July 29, 2021.
  6. ^ Swenson, Christa (May 1999). . History of Psychology Archives. Archived from the original on April 4, 2007.
  7. ^ a b c d e Schacter, Daniel L.; Gilbert, Daniel T.; Wagner, Daniel M. (2011). Psychology (2nd ed.). New York: Worth Publishers. p. 17. ISBN 978-1-4292-3719-2.
  8. ^ Skinner, B. F. (1974). About Behaviorism. Random House. ISBN 0-394-71618-3.
  9. ^ a b Skinner, B. F. (1938). The Behavior of Organisms. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. ISBN 1-58390-007-1.
  10. ^ a b c Ferster, Charles B.; Skinner, B. F. (1957). Schedules of Reinforcement. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. ISBN 0-13-792309-0.
  11. ^ Smith, Nathaniel G.; Morris, Edward K. (2021). "Full Bibliography". B. F. Skinner Foundation. Retrieved July 29, 2021. Also available as a .
  12. ^ Skinner, B. F. (1958). Verbal Behavior. Acton, Massachusetts: Copley Publishing Group. ISBN 1-58390-021-7.
  13. ^ Haggbloom, Steven J.; Warnick, Renee; Warnick, Jason E.; Jones, Vinessa K.; et al. (June 1, 2002). "The 100 most eminent psychologists of the 20th century". Review of General Psychology. 6 (2): 139–52. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.586.1913. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.6.2.139. S2CID 145668721.
  14. ^ Skinner, B. F. (1967). "B. F. Skinner". In Boring, E. G.; Lindzey, G. (eds.). A History of Psychology in Autobiography. Vol. 5. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. pp. 387–413. doi:10.1037/11579-014. Within a year I had gone to Miss Graves to tell her that I no longer believed in God. 'I know,' she said, 'I have been through that myself.' But her strategy misfired: I never went through it.
  15. ^ Mahoney, Michael J. (October 1991). "B. F. Skinner: A Collective Tribute". Canadian Psychology. 32 (4): 628–635. doi:10.1037/h0084641.
  16. ^ a b Skinner, B. F (1976). Particulars of My Life (1st ed.). New York: Knopf. ISBN 978-0-394-40071-6.
  17. ^ a b c d e Bjork, Daniel W. (2013). B. F. Skinner: A Life. American Psychological Association. ISBN 978-1-55798-416-6.
  18. ^ "Establishment History". University of Minnesota. Retrieved December 16, 2020.
  19. ^ Vargas, Julie. "Biographical Information". B. F. Skinner Foundation. Retrieved December 16, 2020.
  20. ^ . American Humanist Association. Archived from the original on October 20, 2012. Retrieved October 9, 2012.
  21. ^ Skinner, Deborah. . Horses by Skinner. Archived from the original on May 30, 2015. Retrieved September 4, 2014.
  22. ^ Buzan, Deborah Skinner (March 12, 2004). "I was not a lab rat". The Guardian. Retrieved September 4, 2014.
  23. ^ . Harvard University Library. Archived from the original on July 3, 2018. Retrieved July 30, 2021.
  24. ^ The Famous People. (2017). B. F. Skinner biography
  25. ^ a b Skinner, B. F. 1974. "Causes of Behavior." Pp. 16−18 in About Behaviorism. ISBN 0-394-71618-3. section 3, "Radical Behaviorism." https://archive.org/stream/aboutbehaviorism00skin#page/16/mode/2up
  26. ^ Pavlov, Ivan (1927). Conditioned Reflexes. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  27. ^ Thorndike, Edward L. (1911). Animal Intelligence: Experimental Studies. New York: Macmillan.
  28. ^ a b c Jenkins, H. M. 1979. "Animal Learning & Behavior." Ch. 5 in The First Century of Experimental Psychology, edited by E. Hearst. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  29. ^ a b Skinner, B. F. 1966. Contingencies of Reinforcement. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
  30. ^ Skinner, B. F. 1953. Science and Human Behavior. New York: Macmillan.
  31. ^ Skinner, B. F. (1981). (PDF). Science. 213 (4507): 501–04. Bibcode:1981Sci...213..501S. doi:10.1126/science.7244649. PMID 7244649. Archived from the original (PDF) on July 2, 2010. Retrieved August 14, 2010.
  32. ^ "Different Types of Reinforcement Schedules" (PDF). autismpdc.fpg.unc.edu. National Professional Development Center for Autism Spectrum Disorders. Archived (PDF) from the original on October 9, 2022. Retrieved February 14, 2015.
  33. ^ Hergenhahn, B. R. (2009). An Introduction to the History of Psychology. United States: Wadsworth Cengage Learning. p. 449. ISBN 978-0-495-50621-8.
  34. ^ B. F. Skinner, (1957) Verbal Behavior. The account in the appendix is that he asked Skinner to explain why he said "No black scorpion, Carter is falling upon this table."
  35. ^ "Skinner, Burrhus Frederick(1904 - 1990)". Credo Reference, Gale. Retrieved October 1, 2013.
  36. ^ a b Chomsky, Noam (1967). "A Review of B. F. Skinner's Verbal Behavior" (PDF). In Jakobovits, L. A.; Miron, M. S. (eds.). Readings in the Psychology of Language. Prentice-Hall. pp. 48–63. Archived (PDF) from the original on October 9, 2022. Retrieved July 29, 2021.
  37. ^ Richelle, M. 1993. B. F. Skinner: A Reappraisal. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  38. ^ Michael, J. (1984). "Verbal Behavior". Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 42 (3): 363–376. doi:10.1901/jeab.1984.42-363. PMC 1348108. PMID 16812395.
  39. ^ Kubina, Richard M.; Kostewicz, Douglas E.; Brennan, Kaitlyn M.; King, Seth A. (September 2017). "A Critical Review of Line Graphs in Behavior Analytic Journals". Educational Psychology Review. 29 (3): 583–598. doi:10.1007/s10648-015-9339-x. ISSN 1040-726X. S2CID 142317036.
  40. ^ a b c Joyce, Nick & Faye, Cathy (September 1, 2010). "Skinner Air Crib". 23. {{cite journal}}: Cite journal requires |journal= (help)
  41. ^ Epstein, Robert (November 1, 1995). "Babies in Boxes". Psychology Today.
  42. ^ Bjork. "B. F. Skinner: A life". Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
  43. ^ Buzan, Deborah Skinner (March 12, 2004). "I was not a lab rat". the Guardian. Retrieved January 21, 2023.
  44. ^ a b Skinner, B. F. (1961). "Why we need teaching machines". Harvard Educational Review. 31: 377–398.
  45. ^ . College of Education, University of Houston. Archived from the original on June 1, 2019. Retrieved September 24, 2012.
  46. ^ Skinner, B.F. (1961). "Teaching machines". Scientific American. 205 (3): 90–112. doi:10.2307/1926170. JSTOR 1926170. PMID 13913636.
  47. ^ Skinner, B. F., and J. Holland. 1961. The Analysis of Behavior: A Program for Self Instruction. p. 387.
  48. ^ "Rebirth of the Teaching Machine through the Seduction of Data Analytics: This Time It's Personal". Philip McRae, Ph.D. April 14, 2013.
  49. ^ a b c Schultz-Figueroa. "Project Pigeon: Rendering the War Animal through Optical Technology". JCMS: Journal of Cinema and Media Studies.
  50. ^ a b Skinner, B. F. (1936). "The Verbal Summator and a Method for the Study of Latent Speech". Journal of Psychology. 2 (1): 71–107. doi:10.1080/00223980.1936.9917445. hdl:11858/00-001M-0000-002D-7E05-E. S2CID 144303708.
  51. ^ Rutherford, A. 2003. "B. F. Skinner and the auditory inkblot: The rise and fall of the verbal summator as a projective technique." History of Psychology 4:362–78.
  52. ^ Holland, J. 1992. "B. F Skinner." American Psychologist.
  53. ^ a b Skinner, B. F. 1968. The Technology of Teaching. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. LCCN 68--12340.
  54. ^ . Time. Archived from the original on September 30, 2007.
  55. ^ Skinner, B. F. 1968. "The Design of Experimental Communities." Pp. 271-75 in International Encyclopedia of the Social Sciences 16, edited by S. Darity. New York.
  56. ^ Ramsey, Richard David. 1979. "Morning Star: The Values-Communication of Skinner's 'Walden Two'" (Ph.D. dissertation). Troy, NY: Rensselaer Polytechnic Institute. – via University Microfilms, Ann Arbor, MI. (Ramsey attempts to analyze Walden Two, Beyond Freedom and Dignity, and other Skinner works in the context of Skinner's life; lists over 500 sources.)
  57. ^ Kuhlman, Hilke (October 1, 2010). Living Walden Two: B. F. Skinner's Behaviorist Utopia and Experimental Communities. University of Illinois Press. p. 87.
  58. ^ see Beyond Freedom and Dignity, 1974 for example
  59. ^ Asimov, Nanette (January 30, 1996). "Spanking Debate Hits Assembly". SFGate. San Francisco Chronicle. Retrieved March 2, 2008.
  60. ^ A matter of Consequences, p. 412.
  61. ^ a b c Skinner, B. F. (1948). "'Superstition' in the Pigeon". Journal of Experimental Psychology. 38 (2): 168–172. doi:10.1037/h0055873. PMID 18913665. S2CID 22577459.
  62. ^ Timberlake, W; Lucas, G A (November 1, 1985). "The basis of superstitious behavior: chance contingency, stimulus substitution, or appetitive behavior?". J Exp Anal Behav. 44 (3): 279–299. doi:10.1901/jeab.1985.44-279. PMC 1348192. PMID 4086972.
  63. ^ a b Chomsky, Noam (1959). . Language. 35 (1): 26–58. doi:10.2307/411334. JSTOR 411334. Archived from the original on September 29, 2015. Retrieved May 20, 2007.
  64. ^ a b MacCorquodale, Kenneth (January 1, 1970). "On Chomsky's review of Skinner's Verbal Behavior". Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 13 (1): 83–99. doi:10.1901/jeab.1970.13-83. ISSN 1938-3711. PMC 1333660.
  65. ^ Skinner, B. F. (1972). "A Lecture on 'Having' a Poem". In Skinner, B. F. (ed.). (PDF) (3rd ed.). Appleton-Century-Crofts. pp. 345–355. ISBN 978-0-9899839-9-0. Archived from the original (PDF) on August 7, 2021. Retrieved August 7, 2021.
  66. ^ Palmer, David C. (2006). "On Chomsky's appraisal of Skinner's Verbal Behavior: a half century of misunderstanding". The Behavior Analyst. 29 (2): 253–267. doi:10.1007/BF03392134. PMC 2223153. PMID 22478467.
  67. ^ Chomsky, Noam (1971). "The Case Against B. F. Skinner". New York Review of Books.
  68. ^ Toates, F. (2009). Burrhus F. Skinner: The shaping of behavior. Houndmills, Basingstoke, England: Palgrave Macmillan.
  69. ^ Overskeid, Geir (September 2007). "Looking for Skinner and Finding Freud". American Psychologist. 62 (6): 590–595. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.321.6288. doi:10.1037/0003-066x.62.6.590. PMID 17874899. S2CID 4610708.
  70. ^ Rutherford, A. (2003). "B. F. Skinner and the auditory inkblot: The rise and fall of the verbal summator as a projective technique". History of Psychology. 6 (4): 362–378. doi:10.1037/1093-4510.6.4.362. PMID 14735913.
  71. ^ Staddon, J. E. R. 2014. The New Behaviorism (2nd ed.).
  72. ^ Staddon, J. E. R. 1995. "On Responsibility and Punishment." The Atlantic Monthly 1995(2):88−94.
  73. ^ Staddon, J. E. R. 1999. "On Responsibility in Science and Law." Social Philosophy and Policy 16:146−74. reprint: 1999. Pp. 146−74 in Responsibility, edited by E. F. Paul, F. D. Miller, and J. Paul.Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  74. ^ "The Pantheon of Skeptics". CSI. Committee for Skeptical Inquiry. from the original on January 31, 2017. Retrieved April 30, 2017.
  75. ^ "APS Member History". search.amphilsoc.org. Retrieved February 27, 2023.
  76. ^ "Burrhus Frederic Skinner". American Academy of Arts & Sciences. February 9, 2023. Retrieved February 27, 2023.
  77. ^ "B. F. Skinner". www.nasonline.org. Retrieved February 27, 2023.

Further reading edit

  • Chiesa, M. (2004). Radical Behaviorism: The Philosophy and the Science.
  • Epstein, Robert (1997). "." Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis 30:545–69. Retrieved 2 June 2005 – via ENVMED.rochester.edu
  • Pauly, Philip Joseph (1987). Controlling Life: Jacques Loeb and the Engineering Ideal in Biology. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-504244-3. Retrieved August 14, 2010.
  • Sundberg, M. L. (2008) The VB-MAPP: The Verbal Behavior Milestones Assessment and Placement Program
  • Basil-Curzon, L. (2004) Teaching in Further Education: A outline of Principles and Practice
  • Hardin, C.J. (2004) Effective Classroom Management
  • Kaufhold, J. A. (2002) The Psychology of Learning and the Art of Teaching
  • Bjork, D. W. (1993) B. F. Skinner: A Life
  • Dews, P. B., ed. (1970) Festschrift For B. F. Skinner.New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
  • Evans, R. I. (1968) B. F. Skinner: the man and his ideas
  • Nye, Robert D. (1979) What Is B. F. Skinner Really Saying? Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
  • Rutherford, A. (2009) Beyond the box: B. F. Skinner's technology of behavior from laboratory to life, 1950s-1970s.. Toronto: University of Toronto Press.
  • Sagal, P. T. (1981) Skinner's Philosophy. Washington, DC: University Press of America.
  • Smith, D. L. (2002). On Prediction and Control. B. F. Skinner and the Technological Ideal of Science. In W. E. Pickren & D. A. Dewsbury, (Eds.), Evolving Perspectives on the History of Psychology, Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association.
  • Swirski, Peter (2011) "How I Stopped Worrying and Loved Behavioural Engineering or Communal Life, Adaptations, and B.F. Skinner's Walden Two". American Utopia and Social Engineering in Literature, Social Thought, and Political History. New York, Routledge.
  • Wiener, D. N. (1996) B. F. Skinner: benign anarchist
  • Wolfgang, C.H. and Glickman, Carl D. (1986) Solving Discipline Problems Allyn and Bacon, Inc

External links edit

  • B. F. Skinner Foundation homepage
  • National Academy of Sciences biography
  • Works by or about B. F. Skinner at Internet Archive
  • Works by B. F. Skinner at LibriVox (public domain audiobooks)  
  • I was not a lab rat, response by Skinner's daughter about the "baby box"
  • Society for Experimental Analysis of Behavior
  • Skinner and Teaching Machine on YouTube
  • Reprint of "the Minotaur of the Behaviorist Maze: Surviving Stanford's Learning House in the 1970s: Journal of Humanistic Psychology, Vol. 51, Number 3, July 2011. 266–272.

skinner, burrhus, frederic, skinner, march, 1904, august, 1990, american, psychologist, behaviorist, inventor, social, philosopher, edgar, pierce, professor, psychology, harvard, university, from, 1958, until, retirement, 1974, skinner, 1950bornburrhus, freder. Burrhus Frederic Skinner March 20 1904 August 18 1990 was an American psychologist behaviorist inventor and social philosopher 2 3 4 5 He was the Edgar Pierce Professor of Psychology at Harvard University from 1958 until his retirement in 1974 6 B F SkinnerSkinner c 1950BornBurrhus Frederic Skinner 1904 03 20 March 20 1904Susquehanna Pennsylvania U S DiedAugust 18 1990 1990 08 18 aged 86 Cambridge Massachusetts U S Alma materHamilton College AB Harvard University PhD Known forBehavior analysisOperant conditioningRadical behaviorismVerbal BehaviorSpouseYvonne Eve Blue m 1936 wbr 1 ChildrenJulie and DeborahAwardsNational Medal of Science 1968 Scientific careerFieldsPsychology linguistics philosophyInstitutionsUniversity of MinnesotaIndiana UniversityHarvard UniversitySignatureConsidering free will to be an illusion Skinner saw human action as dependent on consequences of previous actions a theory he would articulate as the principle of reinforcement If the consequences to an action are bad there is a high chance the action will not be repeated if the consequences are good the probability of the action being repeated becomes stronger 7 Skinner developed behavior analysis especially the philosophy of radical behaviorism 8 and founded the experimental analysis of behavior a school of experimental research psychology He also used operant conditioning to strengthen behavior considering the rate of response to be the most effective measure of response strength To study operant conditioning he invented the operant conditioning chamber aka the Skinner box 7 and to measure rate he invented the cumulative recorder Using these tools he and Charles Ferster produced Skinner s most influential experimental work outlined in their 1957 book Schedules of Reinforcement 9 10 Skinner was a prolific author publishing 21 books and 180 articles 11 He imagined the application of his ideas to the design of a human community in his 1948 utopian novel Walden Two 3 while his analysis of human behavior culminated in his 1958 work Verbal Behavior 12 Skinner John B Watson and Ivan Pavlov are considered to be the pioneers of modern behaviorism Accordingly a June 2002 survey listed Skinner as the most influential psychologist of the 20th century 13 Contents 1 Biography 1 1 Education 1 2 Later life 2 Contributions to psychology 2 1 Behaviorism 2 2 Foundations of Skinner s behaviorism 2 2 1 1 Origin of operant behavior 2 2 2 2 Control of operant behavior 2 2 3 3 Explaining complex behavior 2 3 Reinforcement 2 3 1 Schedules of reinforcement 2 3 2 Token economy 2 4 Verbal Behavior 3 Scientific inventions 3 1 Operant conditioning chamber 3 2 Cumulative recorder 3 3 Air crib 3 4 Teaching machine 3 5 Pigeon guided missile 3 6 Verbal summator 4 Influence on teaching 5 Contributions to social theory 6 Political views 7 Superstition in the Pigeon experiment 8 Criticism 8 1 Noam Chomsky 8 2 Psychodynamic psychology 8 3 J E R Staddon 9 Professional career 9 1 Roles 9 2 Awards 9 3 Honorary degrees 9 4 Honorary societies 10 Bibliography 11 See also 12 References 12 1 Notes 12 2 Citations 13 Further reading 14 External linksBiography editSkinner was born in Susquehanna Pennsylvania to Grace and William Skinner the latter of whom was a lawyer Skinner became an atheist after a Christian teacher tried to assuage his fear of the hell that his grandmother described 14 His brother Edward two and a half years younger died at age 16 of a cerebral hemorrhage 15 Skinner s closest friend as a young boy was Raphael Miller whom he called Doc because his father was a doctor Doc and Skinner became friends due to their parents religiousness and both had an interest in contraptions and gadgets They had set up a telegraph line between their houses to send messages to each other although they had to call each other on the telephone due to the confusing messages sent back and forth During one summer Doc and Skinner started an elderberry business to gather berries and sell them door to door They found that when they picked the ripe berries the unripe ones came off the branches too so they built a device that was able to separate them The device was a bent piece of metal to form a trough They would pour water down the trough into a bucket and the ripe berries would sink into the bucket and the unripe ones would be pushed over the edge to be thrown away 16 Education edit Skinner attended Hamilton College in Clinton New York with the intention of becoming a writer He found himself at a social disadvantage at the college because of his intellectual attitude further explanation needed 17 He was a member of Lambda Chi Alpha fraternity 16 He wrote for the school paper but as an atheist he was critical of the traditional mores of his college After receiving his Bachelor of Arts in English literature in 1926 he attended Harvard University where he would later research and teach While attending Harvard a fellow student Fred S Keller convinced Skinner that he could make an experimental science of the study of behavior This led Skinner to invent a prototype for the Skinner box and to join Keller in the creation of other tools for small experiments 17 After graduation Skinner unsuccessfully tried to write a novel while he lived with his parents a period that he later called the Dark Years 17 He became disillusioned with his literary skills despite encouragement from the renowned poet Robert Frost concluding that he had little world experience and no strong personal perspective from which to write His encounter with John B Watson s behaviorism led him into graduate study in psychology and to the development of his own version of behaviorism 17 Later life edit nbsp The gravestone of B F Skinner and his wife Eve at Mount Auburn CemeterySkinner received a PhD from Harvard in 1931 and remained there as a researcher for some years In 1936 he went to the University of Minnesota in Minneapolis to teach 18 In 1945 he moved to Indiana University 19 where he was chair of the psychology department from 1946 to 1947 before returning to Harvard as a tenured professor in 1948 He remained at Harvard for the rest of his life In 1973 Skinner was one of the signers of the Humanist Manifesto II 20 In 1936 Skinner married Yvonne Eve Blue The couple had two daughters Julie later Vargas and Deborah later Buzan married Barry Buzan 21 22 Yvonne died in 1997 23 and is buried in Mount Auburn Cemetery Cambridge Massachusetts 17 Skinner s public exposure had increased in the 1970s he remained active even after his retirement in 1974 until his death In 1989 Skinner was diagnosed with leukemia and died on August 18 1990 in Cambridge Massachusetts Ten days before his death he was given the lifetime achievement award by the American Psychological Association and gave a talk concerning his work 24 Contributions to psychology editBehaviorism edit Main articles Behaviorism and Radical behaviorism Skinner referred to his approach to the study of behavior as radical behaviorism 25 which originated in the early 1900s as a reaction to depth psychology and other traditional forms of psychology which often had difficulty making predictions that could be tested experimentally This philosophy of behavioral science assumes that behavior is a consequence of environmental histories of reinforcement see applied behavior analysis In his words The position can be stated as follows what is felt or introspectively observed is not some nonphysical world of consciousness mind or mental life but the observer s own body This does not mean as I shall show later that introspection is a kind of psychological research nor does it mean and this is the heart of the argument that what are felt or introspectively observed are the causes of the behavior An organism behaves as it does because of its current structure but most of this is out of reach of introspection At the moment we must content ourselves as the methodological behaviorist insists with a person s genetic and environment histories What are introspectively observed are certain collateral products of those histories In this way we repair the major damage wrought by mentalism When what a person does is attributed to what is going on inside him investigation is brought to an end Why explain the explanation For twenty five hundred years people have been preoccupied with feelings and mental life but only recently has any interest been shown in a more precise analysis of the role of the environment Ignorance of that role led in the first place to mental fictions and it has been perpetuated by the explanatory practices to which they gave rise 25 Foundations of Skinner s behaviorism edit Skinner s ideas about behaviorism were largely set forth in his first book The Behavior of Organisms 1938 9 Here he gives a systematic description of the manner in which environmental variables control behavior He distinguished two sorts of behavior which are controlled in different ways Respondent behaviors are elicited by stimuli and may be modified through respondent conditioning often called classical or pavlovian conditioning in which a neutral stimulus is paired with an eliciting stimulus Such behaviors may be measured by their latency or strength Operant behaviors are emitted meaning that initially they are not induced by any particular stimulus They are strengthened through operant conditioning aka instrumental conditioning in which the occurrence of a response yields a reinforcer Such behaviors may be measured by their rate Both of these sorts of behavior had already been studied experimentally most notably respondents by Ivan Pavlov 26 and operants by Edward Thorndike 27 Skinner s account differed in some ways from earlier ones 28 and was one of the first accounts to bring them under one roof The idea that behavior is strengthened or weakened by its consequences raises several questions Among the most commonly asked are these Operant responses are strengthened by reinforcement but where do they come from in the first place Once it is in the organism s repertoire how is a response directed or controlled How can very complex and seemingly novel behaviors be explained 1 Origin of operant behavior edit Skinner s answer to the first question was very much like Darwin s answer to the question of the origin of a new bodily structure namely variation and selection Similarly the behavior of an individual varies from moment to moment a variation that is followed by reinforcement is strengthened and becomes prominent in that individual s behavioral repertoire Shaping was Skinner s term for the gradual modification of behavior by the reinforcement of desired variations Skinner believed that superstitious behavior can arise when a response happens to be followed by reinforcement to which it is actually unrelated clarification needed 2 Control of operant behavior edit The second question how is operant behavior controlled arises because to begin with the behavior is emitted without reference to any particular stimulus Skinner answered this question by saying that a stimulus comes to control an operant if it is present when the response is reinforced and absent when it is not For example if lever pressing only brings food when a light is on a rat or a child will learn to press the lever only when the light is on Skinner summarized this relationship by saying that a discriminative stimulus e g light or sound sets the occasion for the reinforcement food of the operant lever press This three term contingency stimulus response reinforcer is one of Skinner s most important concepts and sets his theory apart from theories that use only pair wise associations 28 3 Explaining complex behavior edit Most behavior of humans cannot easily be described in terms of individual responses reinforced one by one and Skinner devoted a great deal of effort to the problem of behavioral complexity Some complex behavior can be seen as a sequence of relatively simple responses and here Skinner invoked the idea of chaining Chaining is based on the fact experimentally demonstrated that a discriminative stimulus not only sets the occasion for subsequent behavior but it can also reinforce a behavior that precedes it That is a discriminative stimulus is also a conditioned reinforcer For example the light that sets the occasion for lever pressing may also be used to reinforce turning around in the presence of a noise This results in the sequence noise turn around light press lever food Much longer chains can be built by adding more stimuli and responses However Skinner recognized that a great deal of behavior especially human behavior cannot be accounted for by gradual shaping or the construction of response sequences 29 Complex behavior often appears suddenly in its final form as when a person first finds his way to the elevator by following instructions given at the front desk To account for such behavior Skinner introduced the concept of rule governed behavior First relatively simple behaviors come under the control of verbal stimuli the child learns to jump open the book and so on After a large number of responses come under such verbal control a sequence of verbal stimuli can evoke an almost unlimited variety of complex responses 29 Reinforcement edit Main article Reinforcement Reinforcement a key concept of behaviorism is the primary process that shapes and controls behavior and occurs in two ways positive and negative In The Behavior of Organisms 1938 Skinner defines negative reinforcement to be synonymous with punishment i e the presentation of an aversive stimulus This definition would subsequently be re defined in Science and Human Behavior 1953 In what has now become the standard set of definitions positive reinforcement is the strengthening of behavior by the occurrence of some event e g praise after some behavior is performed whereas negative reinforcement is the strengthening of behavior by the removal or avoidance of some aversive event e g opening and raising an umbrella over your head on a rainy day is reinforced by the cessation of rain falling on you Both types of reinforcement strengthen behavior or increase the probability of a behavior reoccurring the difference being in whether the reinforcing event is something applied positive reinforcement or something removed or avoided negative reinforcement Punishment can be the application of an aversive stimulus event positive punishment or punishment by contingent stimulation or the removal of a desirable stimulus negative punishment or punishment by contingent withdrawal Though punishment is often used to suppress behavior Skinner argued that this suppression is temporary and has a number of other often unwanted consequences 30 Extinction is the absence of a rewarding stimulus which weakens behavior Writing in 1981 Skinner pointed out that Darwinian natural selection is like reinforced behavior selection by consequences Though as he said natural selection has now made its case he regretted that essentially the same process reinforcement was less widely accepted as underlying human behavior 31 Schedules of reinforcement edit Main article Schedules of reinforcement Skinner recognized that behavior is typically reinforced more than once and together with Charles Ferster he did an extensive analysis of the various ways in which reinforcements could be arranged over time calling it the schedules of reinforcement 10 The most notable schedules of reinforcement studied by Skinner were continuous interval fixed or variable and ratio fixed or variable All are methods used in operant conditioning Continuous reinforcement CRF each time a specific action is performed the subject receives a reinforcement This method is effective when teaching a new behavior because it quickly establishes an association between the target behavior and the reinforcer 32 Interval schedule based on the time intervals between reinforcements 7 Fixed interval schedule FI A procedure in which reinforcements are presented at fixed time periods provided that the appropriate response is made This schedule yields a response rate that is low just after reinforcement and becomes rapid just before the next reinforcement is scheduled Variable interval schedule VI A procedure in which behavior is reinforced after scheduled but unpredictable time durations following the previous reinforcement This schedule yields the most stable rate of responding with the average frequency of reinforcement determining the frequency of response Ratio schedules based on the ratio of responses to reinforcements 7 Fixed ratio schedule FR A procedure in which reinforcement is delivered after a specific number of responses have been made Variable ratio schedule VR 7 A procedure in which reinforcement comes after a number of responses that is randomized from one reinforcement to the next e g slot machines The lower the number of responses required the higher the response rate tends to be Variable ratio schedules tend to produce very rapid and steady responding rates in contrast with fixed ratio schedules where the frequency of response usually drops after the reinforcement occurs Token economy edit Skinnerian principles have been used to create token economies in a number of institutions such as psychiatric hospitals When participants behave in desirable ways their behavior is reinforced with tokens that can be changed for such items as candy cigarettes coffee or the exclusive use of a radio or television set 33 Verbal Behavior edit Main article Verbal Behavior Challenged by Alfred North Whitehead during a casual discussion while at Harvard to provide an account of a randomly provided piece of verbal behavior 34 Skinner set about attempting to extend his then new functional inductive approach to the complexity of human verbal behavior 35 Developed over two decades his work appeared in the book Verbal Behavior Although Noam Chomsky was highly critical of Verbal Behavior he conceded that Skinner s S R psychology was worth a review 36 behavior analysts reject the S R characterization operant conditioning involves the emission of a response which then becomes more or less likely depending upon its consequence 36 Verbal Behavior had an uncharacteristically cool reception partly as a result of Chomsky s review partly because of Skinner s failure to address or rebut any of Chomsky s criticisms 37 Skinner s peers may have been slow to adopt the ideas presented in Verbal Behavior because of the absence of experimental evidence unlike the empirical density that marked Skinner s experimental work 38 Scientific inventions editOperant conditioning chamber edit Main article Operant conditioning chamber An operant conditioning chamber also known as a Skinner box is a laboratory apparatus used in the experimental analysis of animal behavior It was invented by Skinner while he was a graduate student at Harvard University As used by Skinner the box had a lever for rats or a disk in one wall for pigeons A press on this manipulandum could deliver food to the animal through an opening in the wall and responses reinforced in this way increased in frequency By controlling this reinforcement together with discriminative stimuli such as lights and tones or punishments such as electric shocks experimenters have used the operant box to study a wide variety of topics including schedules of reinforcement discriminative control delayed response memory punishment and so on By channeling research in these directions the operant conditioning chamber has had a huge influence on course of research in animal learning and its applications It enabled great progress on problems that could be studied by measuring the rate probability or force of a simple repeatable response However it discouraged the study of behavioral processes not easily conceptualized in such terms spatial learning in particular which is now studied in quite different ways for example by the use of the water maze 28 Cumulative recorder edit The cumulative recorder makes a pen and ink record of simple repeated responses Skinner designed it for use with the operant chamber as a convenient way to record and view the rate of responses such as a lever press or a key peck In this device a sheet of paper gradually unrolls over a cylinder Each response steps a small pen across the paper starting at one edge when the pen reaches the other edge it quickly resets to the initial side The slope of the resulting ink line graphically displays the rate of the response for example rapid responses yield a steeply sloping line on the paper slow responding yields a line of low slope The cumulative recorder was a key tool used by Skinner in his analysis of behavior and it was very widely adopted by other experimenters gradually falling out of use with the advent of the laboratory computer and use of line graphs 39 Skinner s major experimental exploration of response rates presented in his book with Charles Ferster Schedules of Reinforcement is full of cumulative records produced by this device 10 Air crib edit The air crib is an easily cleaned temperature and humidity controlled box bed intended to replace the standard infant crib After raising one baby Skinner felt that he could simplify the process for parents and improve the experience for children He primarily thought of the idea to help his wife cope with the day to day tasks of child rearing Skinner had some specific concerns about raising a baby in the rough environment where he lived in Minnesota Keeping the child warm was a central priority Faye 2007 40 Though this was the main goal it also was designed to reduce laundry diaper rash and cradle cap while still allowing the baby to be more mobile and comfortable and less prone to cry Reportedly it had some success in these goals and was used with an estimate of 300 children who were raised in the air crib The notorious crib was advertised commercially Psychology Today tracked down 50 children and ran a short piece on the effects of the air crib The reports came back positive and that these children and parents enjoyed using the crib Epstein 2080 41 One of these air cribs resides in the gallery at the Center for the History of Psychology in Akron Ohio Faye 3000 40 The air crib was designed with three solid walls and a safety glass panel at the front which could be lowered to move the baby in and out of the crib The floor was stretched canvas Sheets were intended to be used over the canvas and were easily rolled off when soiled Addressing Skinners concern for temperature a control box on top of the crib regulated temperature and humidity Filtered air flowed through the crib from below This crib was higher than most standard cribs allowing easier access to the child without the need to bend over Faye 2010 40 The air crib was a controversial invention It was popularly characterized as a cruel pen and it was often compared to Skinner s operant conditioning chamber or Skinner box An article titled Baby in a Box caught the eye of many and contributed to skepticism about the device Bjork 1997 42 A picture published with the article showed the Skinners daughter Deborah peering out of the crib with her hands and face pressed upon the glass Skinner also used the term experiment when describing the crib and this association with laboratory animal experimentation discouraged the crib s commercial success although several companies attempted to produce and sell it In 2004 therapist Lauren Slater repeated a claim that Skinner may have used his baby daughter in some of his experiments His outraged daughter publicly accused Slater of not making a good faith effort to check her facts before publishing Debora was quoted by the Guardian saying According to Opening Skinner s Box Great Psychological Experiments of the Twentieth Century my father who was a psychologist based at Harvard from the 1950s to the 90s used his infant daughter Deborah to prove his theories by putting her for a few hours a day in a laboratory box in which all her needs were controlled and shaped But it s not true My father did nothing of the sort 43 Teaching machine edit nbsp The teaching machine a mechanical invention to automate the task of programmed learningThe teaching machine was a mechanical device whose purpose was to administer a curriculum of programmed learning The machine embodies key elements of Skinner s theory of learning and had important implications for education in general and classroom instruction in particular 44 In one incarnation the machine was a box that housed a list of questions that could be viewed one at a time through a small window see picture There was also a mechanism through which the learner could respond to each question Upon delivering a correct answer the learner would be rewarded 45 Skinner advocated the use of teaching machines for a broad range of students e g preschool aged to adult and instructional purposes e g reading and music For example one machine that he envisioned could teach rhythm He wrote 46 A relatively simple device supplies the necessary contingencies The student taps a rhythmic pattern in unison with the device Unison is specified very loosely at first the student can be a little early or late at each tap but the specifications are slowly sharpened The process is repeated for various speeds and patterns In another arrangement the student echoes rhythmic patterns sounded by the machine though not in unison and again the specifications for an accurate reproduction are progressively sharpened Rhythmic patterns can also be brought under the control of a printed score The instructional potential of the teaching machine stemmed from several factors it provided automatic immediate and regular reinforcement without the use of aversive control the material presented was coherent yet varied and novel the pace of learning could be adjusted to suit the individual As a result students were interested attentive and learned efficiently by producing the desired behavior learning by doing 47 Teaching machines though perhaps rudimentary were not rigid instruments of instruction They could be adjusted and improved based upon the students performance For example if a student made many incorrect responses the machine could be reprogrammed to provide less advanced prompts or questions the idea being that students acquire behaviors most efficiently if they make few errors Multiple choice formats were not well suited for teaching machines because they tended to increase student mistakes and the contingencies of reinforcement were relatively uncontrolled Not only useful in teaching explicit skills machines could also promote the development of a repertoire of behaviors that Skinner called self management Effective self management means attending to stimuli appropriate to a task avoiding distractions reducing the opportunity of reward for competing behaviors and so on For example machines encourage students to pay attention before receiving a reward Skinner contrasted this with the common classroom practice of initially capturing students attention e g with a lively video and delivering a reward e g entertainment before the students have actually performed any relevant behavior This practice fails to reinforce correct behavior and actually counters the development of self management Skinner pioneered the use of teaching machines in the classroom especially at the primary level Today computers run software that performs similar teaching tasks and there has been a resurgence of interest in the topic related to the development of adaptive learning systems 48 Pigeon guided missile edit Main article Project Pigeon During World War II the US Navy required a weapon effective against surface ships such as the German Bismarck class battleships Although missile and TV technology existed the size of the primitive guidance systems available rendered automatic guidance impractical To solve this problem Skinner initiated Project Pigeon which was intended to provide a simple and effective guidance system Skinner trained pigeons through operant conditioning to peck a camera obscura screen showing incoming targets on individual screens Schultz Figueroa 2019 49 This system divided the nose cone of a missile into three compartments with a pigeon placed in each Within the ship the three lenses projected an image of distant objects onto a screen in front of each bird Thus when the missile was launched from an aircraft within sight of an enemy ship an image of the ship would appear on the screen The screen was hinged which connected the screens to the bomb s guidance system This was done through four small rubber pneumatic tubes that were attached to each side of the frame which directed a constant airflow to a pneumatic pickup system that controlled the thrusters of the bomb Resulting in the missile being guided towards the targeted ship through just the peck coming from the pigeon Schultz Figueroa 2019 49 Despite an effective demonstration the project was abandoned and eventually more conventional solutions such as those based on radar became available Skinner complained that our problem was no one would take us seriously Before the project was completely abandoned it was tested extensively in the laboratory After the United States Army ultimately denied it the United States Naval Research Laboratory picked up Skinner s Research and renamed it Project ORCON which was a contraction of organic and control Skinner worked closely with the US Naval Research Laboratory continuously testing the pigeon s tracking capacity for guiding missiles to their intended targets In the end the pigeons performance and accuracy relied on so many uncontrollable factors that Project ORCON like Project Pigeon before it was again discontinued It was never used in the field 49 Verbal summator edit Early in his career Skinner became interested in latent speech and experimented with a device he called the verbal summator 50 This device can be thought of as an auditory version of the Rorschach inkblots 50 When using the device human participants listened to incomprehensible auditory garbage but often read meaning into what they heard Thus as with the Rorschach blots the device was intended to yield overt behavior that projected subconscious thoughts Skinner s interest in projective testing was brief but he later used observations with the summator in creating his theory of verbal behavior The device also led other researchers to invent new tests such as the tautophone test the auditory apperception test and the Azzageddi when defined as test 51 Influence on teaching editAlong with psychology education has also been influenced by Skinner s views which are extensively presented in his book The Technology of Teaching as well as reflected in Fred S Keller s Personalized System of Instruction and Ogden R Lindsley s Precision Teaching Skinner argued that education has two major purposes to teach repertoires of both verbal and nonverbal behavior and to interest students in learning He recommended bringing students behavior under appropriate control by providing reinforcement only in the presence of stimuli relevant to the learning task Because he believed that human behavior can be affected by small consequences something as simple as the opportunity to move forward after completing one stage of an activity can be an effective reinforcer Skinner was convinced that to learn a student must engage in behavior and not just passively receive information 44 389 Skinner believed that effective teaching must be based on positive reinforcement which is he argued more effective at changing and establishing behavior than punishment He suggested that the main thing people learn from being punished is how to avoid punishment For example if a child is forced to practice playing an instrument the child comes to associate practicing with punishment and thus develops feelings of dreadfulness and wishes to avoid practicing the instrument This view had obvious implications for the then widespread practice of rote learning and punitive discipline in education The use of educational activities as punishment may induce rebellious behavior such as vandalism or absence 52 Because teachers are primarily responsible for modifying student behavior Skinner argued that teachers must learn effective ways of teaching In The Technology of Teaching 1968 Skinner has a chapter on why teachers fail 53 93 113 He says that teachers have not been given an in depth understanding of teaching and learning Without knowing the science underpinning teaching teachers fall back on procedures that work poorly or not at all such as using aversive techniques which produce escape and avoidance and undesirable emotional effects relying on telling and explaining Unfortunately a student does not learn simply when he is shown or told 53 103 failing to adapt learning tasks to the student s current level and failing to provide positive reinforcement frequently enough Skinner suggests that any age appropriate skill can be taught The steps are Clearly specify the action or performance the student is to learn Break down the task into small achievable steps going from simple to complex Let the student perform each step reinforcing correct actions Adjust so that the student is always successful until finally the goal is reached Shift to intermittent reinforcement to maintain the student s performance Contributions to social theory editSkinner is popularly known mainly for his books Walden Two 1948 and Beyond Freedom and Dignity for which he made the cover of Time magazine 54 The former describes a fictional experimental community 55 in 1940s United States The productivity and happiness of citizens in this community is far greater than in the outside world because the residents practice scientific social planning and use operant conditioning in raising their children Walden Two like Thoreau s Walden champions a lifestyle that does not support war or foster competition and social strife It encourages a lifestyle of minimal consumption rich social relationships personal happiness satisfying work and leisure 56 In 1967 Kat Kinkade and others founded the Twin Oaks Community usingWalden Two as a blueprint The community still exists and continues to use the Planner Manager system and other aspects of the community described in Skinner s book though behavior modification is not a community practice 57 In Beyond Freedom and Dignity Skinner suggests that a technology of behavior could help to make a better society We would however have to accept that an autonomous agent is not the driving force of our actions Skinner offers alternatives to punishment and challenges his readers to use science and modern technology to construct a better society Political views editSkinner s political writings emphasized his hopes that an effective and human science of behavioral control a technology of human behavior could help with problems as yet unsolved and often aggravated by advances in technology such as the atomic bomb Indeed one of Skinner s goals was to prevent humanity from destroying itself 58 He saw political activity as the use of aversive or non aversive means to control a population Skinner favored the use of positive reinforcement as a means of control citing Jean Jacques Rousseau s novel Emile or On Education as an example of literature that did not fear the power of positive reinforcement 3 Skinner s book Walden Two presents a vision of a decentralized localized society which applies a practical scientific approach and behavioral expertise to deal peacefully with social problems For example his views led him to oppose corporal punishment in schools and he wrote a letter to the California Senate that helped lead it to a ban on spanking 59 Skinner s utopia is both a thought experiment and a rhetorical piece In Walden Two Skinner answers the problem that exists in many utopian novels What is the Good Life The book s answer is a life of friendship health art a healthy balance between work and leisure a minimum of unpleasantness and a feeling that one has made worthwhile contributions to a society in which resources are ensured in part by minimizing consumption If the world is to save any part of its resources for the future it must reduce not only consumption but the number of consumers B F Skinner Walden Two 1948 p xiSkinner described his novel as my New Atlantis in reference to Bacon s utopia 60 When Milton s Satan falls from heaven he ends in hell And what does he say to reassure himself Here at least we shall be free And that I think is the fate of the old fashioned liberal He s going to be free but he s going to find himself in hell B F Skinner from William F Buckley Jr On the Firing Line p 87 Superstition in the Pigeon experiment editOne of Skinner s experiments examined the formation of superstition in one of his favorite experimental animals the pigeon Skinner placed a series of hungry pigeons in a cage attached to an automatic mechanism that delivered food to the pigeon at regular intervals with no reference whatsoever to the bird s behavior 61 He discovered that the pigeons associated the delivery of the food with whatever chance actions they had been performing as it was delivered and that they subsequently continued to perform these same actions 61 One bird was conditioned to turn counter clockwise about the cage making two or three turns between reinforcements Another repeatedly thrust its head into one of the upper corners of the cage A third developed a tossing response as if placing its head beneath an invisible bar and lifting it repeatedly Two birds developed a pendulum motion of the head and body in which the head was extended forward and swung from right to left with a sharp movement followed by a somewhat slower return Skinner suggested that the pigeons behaved as if they were influencing the automatic mechanism with their rituals and that this experiment shed light on human behavior 61 The experiment might be said to demonstrate a sort of superstition The bird behaves as if there were a causal relation between its behavior and the presentation of food although such a relation is lacking There are many analogies in human behavior Rituals for changing one s fortune at cards are good examples A few accidental connections between a ritual and favorable consequences suffice to set up and maintain the behavior in spite of many unreinforced instances The bowler who has released a ball down the alley but continues to behave as if she were controlling it by twisting and turning her arm and shoulder is another case in point These behaviors have of course no real effect upon one s luck or upon a ball half way down an alley just as in the present case the food would appear as often if the pigeon did nothing or more strictly speaking did something else Modern behavioral psychologists have disputed Skinner s superstition explanation for the behaviors he recorded Subsequent research e g Staddon and Simmelhag 1971 while finding similar behavior failed to find support for Skinner s adventitious reinforcement explanation for it By looking at the timing of different behaviors within the interval Staddon and Simmelhag were able to distinguish two classes of behavior the terminal response which occurred in anticipation of food and interim responses that occurred earlier in the interfood interval and were rarely contiguous with food Terminal responses seem to reflect classical as opposed to operant conditioning rather than adventitious reinforcement guided by a process like that observed in 1968 by Brown and Jenkins in their autoshaping procedures The causation of interim activities such as the schedule induced polydipsia seen in a similar situation with rats also cannot be traced to adventitious reinforcement and its details are still obscure Staddon 1977 62 Criticism editNoam Chomsky edit American linguist Noam Chomsky published a review of Skinner s Verbal Behavior in the linguistics journal Language in 1959 63 Chomsky argued that Skinner s attempt to use behaviorism to explain human language amounted to little more than word games Conditioned responses could not account for a child s ability to create or understand an infinite variety of novel sentences Chomsky s review has been credited with launching the cognitive revolution in psychology and other disciplines Skinner who rarely responded directly to critics never formally replied to Chomsky s critique but endorsed Kenneth MacCorquodale s 1972 reply 64 I read half a dozen pages saw that it missed the point of my book and went no further My reasons I am afraid show a lack of character In the first place I should have had to read the review and I found its tone distasteful It was not really a review of my book but of what Chomsky took erroneously to be my position 65 Many academics in the 1960s believed that Skinner s silence on the question meant Chomsky s criticism had been justified But MacCorquodale wrote that Chomsky s criticism did not focus on Skinner s Verbal Behavior but rather attacked a confusion of ideas from behavioral psychology MacCorquodale also regretted Chomsky s aggressive tone 64 Furthermore Chomsky had aimed at delivering a definitive refutation of Skinner by citing dozens of animal instinct and animal learning studies On the one hand he argued that the studies on animal instinct proved that animal behavior is innate and therefore Skinner was mistaken On the other Chomsky s opinion of the studies on learning was that one cannot draw an analogy from animal studies to human behavior or that research on animal instinct refutes research on animal learning 63 66 Chomsky also reviewed Skinner s Beyond Freedom and Dignity using the same basic motives as his Verbal Behavior review Among Chomsky s criticisms were that Skinner s laboratory work could not be extended to humans that when it was extended to humans it represented scientistic behavior attempting to emulate science but which was not scientific that Skinner was not a scientist because he rejected the hypothetico deductive model of theory testing and that Skinner had no science of behavior 67 Psychodynamic psychology edit Skinner has been repeatedly criticized for his supposed animosity towards Sigmund Freud psychoanalysis and psychodynamic psychology Some have argued however that Skinner shared several of Freud s assumptions and that he was influenced by Freudian points of view in more than one field among them the analysis of defense mechanisms such as repression 68 69 To study such phenomena Skinner even designed his own projective test the verbal summator described above 70 J E R Staddon edit As understood by Skinner ascribing dignity to individuals involves giving them credit for their actions To say Skinner is brilliant means that Skinner is an originating force If Skinner s determinist theory is right he is merely the focus of his environment He is not an originating force and he had no choice in saying the things he said or doing the things he did Skinner s environment and genetics both allowed and compelled him to write his book Similarly the environment and genetic potentials of the advocates of freedom and dignity cause them to resist the reality that their own activities are deterministically grounded J E R Staddon has argued the compatibilist position 71 Skinner s determinism is not in any way contradictory to traditional notions of reward and punishment as he believed 72 73 Professional career editRoles edit 1936 1937 Instructor University of Minnesota 1937 1939 Assistant Professor University of Minnesota 1939 1945 Associate Professor University of Minnesota 1945 1948 Professor and chair Indiana University 1947 1948 William James Lecturer Harvard University 1948 1958 Professor Harvard University 1958 1974 Professor of Psychology Harvard University 1949 1950 President Midwestern Psychological Association 1954 1955 President Eastern Psychological Association 1966 1967 President Pavlovian Society of North America 1974 1990 Professor of Psychology and Social Relations Emeritus Harvard UniversityAwards edit 1926 AB Hamilton College 1930 MA Harvard University 1930 1931 Thayer Fellowship 1931 PhD Harvard University 1931 1932 Walker Fellowship 1931 1933 National Research Council Fellowship 1933 1936 Junior Fellowship Harvard Society of Fellows 1942 Guggenheim Fellowship postponed until 1944 1945 1942 Howard Crosby Warren Medal Society of Experimental Psychologists 1958 Distinguished Scientific Contribution Award American Psychological Association 1958 1974 Edgar Pierce Professor of Psychology Harvard University 1964 1974 Career Award National Institute of Mental Health 1966 Edward Lee Thorndike Award American Psychological Association 1968 National Medal of Science National Science Foundation 1969 Overseas Fellow in Churchill College Cambridge 1971 Gold Medal Award American Psychological Foundation 1971 Joseph P Kennedy Jr Foundation for Mental Retardation International award 1972 Humanist of the Year American Humanist Association 1972 Creative Leadership in Education Award New York University 1972 Career Contribution Award Massachusetts Psychological Association 1978 Distinguished Contributions to Educational Research Award and Development American Educational Research Association 1978 National Association for Retarded Citizens Award 1985 Award for Excellence in Psychiatry Albert Einstein School of Medicine 1985 President s Award New York Academy of Science 1990 William James Fellow Award American Psychological Society 1990 Lifetime Achievement Award American Psychological Association 1991 Outstanding Member and Distinguished Professional Achievement Award Society for Performance Improvement 1997 Scholar Hall of Fame Award Academy of Resource and Development 2011 Committee for Skeptical Inquiry Pantheon of Skeptics Inducted 74 Honorary degrees edit Skinner received honorary degrees from Alfred University Ball State University Dickinson College Hamilton College Harvard University Hobart and William Smith Colleges Johns Hopkins University Keio University Long Island University C W Post Campus McGill University North Carolina State University Ohio Wesleyan University Ripon College Rockford College Tufts University University of Chicago University of Exeter University of Missouri University of North Texas Western Michigan University University of Maryland Baltimore County Honorary societies edit Skinner was inducted to the following honorary societies PSI CHI International Honor Society in Psychology American Philosophical Society 75 American Academy of Arts and Sciences 76 United States National Academy of Sciences 77 Bibliography edit1938 The Behavior of Organisms An Experimental Analysis 1938 ISBN 1 58390 007 1 ISBN 0 87411 487 X 1948 Walden Two ISBN 0 87220 779 X revised 1976 ed 1953 Science and Human Behavior ISBN 0 02 929040 6 i 1957 Schedules of Reinforcement with C B Ferster ISBN 0 13 792309 0 1957 Verbal Behavior ISBN 1 58390 021 7 1961 The Analysis of Behavior A Program for Self Instruction with James G Holland ISBN 0 07 029565 4 1968 The Technology of Teaching New York Appleton Century Crofts LCCN 68 12340 ISBN 0 13 902163 9 1969 Contingencies of Reinforcement A Theoretical Analysis ISBN 0 390 81280 3 1971 Beyond Freedom and Dignity ISBN 0 394 42555 3 1974 About Behaviorism ISBN 0 394 71618 3 1976 Particulars of My Life Part One of an Autobiography ISBN 0 394 40071 2 1978 Reflections on Behaviorism and Society ISBN 0 13 770057 1 1979 The Shaping of a Behaviorist Part Two of an Autobiography ISBN 0 394 50581 6 1980 Notebooks edited by Robert Epstein ISBN 0 13 624106 9 1982 Skinner for the Classroom edited by R Epstein ISBN 0 87822 261 8 1983 Enjoy Old Age A Program of Self Management with M E Vaughan ISBN 0 393 01805 9 1983 A Matter of Consequences Part Three of an Autobiography ISBN 0 394 53226 0 ISBN 0 8147 7845 3 1987 Upon Further Reflection ISBN 0 13 938986 5 1989 Recent Issues in the Analysis of Behavior ISBN 0 675 20674 X Cumulative Record A Selection of Papers 1959 1961 1972 and 1999 as Cumulative Record Definitive Edition ISBN 0 87411 969 3 paperback Includes reprint Skinner B F 1945 Baby in a Box Ladies Home Journal Skinner s original personal account of the much misrepresented Baby in a box device See also editApplied behavior analysis Back to Freedom and DignityReferences editNotes edit A free copy of this book in a 1 6 MB pdf file may be downloaded at the B F Skinner Foundation web site BFSkinner org Citations edit Sobel Dava August 20 1990 B F Skinner the Champion Of Behaviorism Is Dead at 86 The New York Times Archived from the original on August 6 2010 Retrieved August 30 2015 Smith L D Woodward W R 1996 B F Skinner and Behaviorism in American Culture Bethlehem Pennsylvania Lehigh University Press ISBN 978 0 934223 40 9 a b c Skinner B F 1948 Walden Two New York Macmillan Publishers ISBN 0 87220 779 X The science of human behavior is used to eliminate poverty sexual expression government as we know it create a lifestyle without that such as war Skinner B F 1972 Beyond Freedom and Dignity Vintage Books ISBN 978 0 553 14372 0 OCLC 34263003 Skinner Burrhus Frederic History of Behavior Analysis Retrieved July 29 2021 Swenson Christa May 1999 Burrhus Frederick Skinner History of Psychology Archives Archived from the original on April 4 2007 a b c d e Schacter Daniel L Gilbert Daniel T Wagner Daniel M 2011 Psychology 2nd ed New York Worth Publishers p 17 ISBN 978 1 4292 3719 2 Skinner B F 1974 About Behaviorism Random House ISBN 0 394 71618 3 a b Skinner B F 1938 The Behavior of Organisms New York Appleton Century Crofts ISBN 1 58390 007 1 a b c Ferster Charles B Skinner B F 1957 Schedules of Reinforcement New York Appleton Century Crofts ISBN 0 13 792309 0 Smith Nathaniel G Morris Edward K 2021 Full Bibliography B F Skinner Foundation Retrieved July 29 2021 Also available as a PDF Skinner B F 1958 Verbal Behavior Acton Massachusetts Copley Publishing Group ISBN 1 58390 021 7 Haggbloom Steven J Warnick Renee Warnick Jason E Jones Vinessa K et al June 1 2002 The 100 most eminent psychologists of the 20th century Review of General Psychology 6 2 139 52 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 586 1913 doi 10 1037 1089 2680 6 2 139 S2CID 145668721 Skinner B F 1967 B F Skinner In Boring E G Lindzey G eds A History of Psychology in Autobiography Vol 5 New York Appleton Century Crofts pp 387 413 doi 10 1037 11579 014 Within a year I had gone to Miss Graves to tell her that I no longer believed in God I know she said I have been through that myself But her strategy misfired I never went through it Mahoney Michael J October 1991 B F Skinner A Collective Tribute Canadian Psychology 32 4 628 635 doi 10 1037 h0084641 a b Skinner B F 1976 Particulars of My Life 1st ed New York Knopf ISBN 978 0 394 40071 6 a b c d e Bjork Daniel W 2013 B F Skinner A Life American Psychological Association ISBN 978 1 55798 416 6 Establishment History University of Minnesota Retrieved December 16 2020 Vargas Julie Biographical Information B F Skinner Foundation Retrieved December 16 2020 Humanist Manifesto II American Humanist Association Archived from the original on October 20 2012 Retrieved October 9 2012 Skinner Deborah About Skinner Horses by Skinner Archived from the original on May 30 2015 Retrieved September 4 2014 Buzan Deborah Skinner March 12 2004 I was not a lab rat The Guardian Retrieved September 4 2014 Skinner Yvonne 1911 1997 Papers of Yvonne Skinner ca 1916 1977 A Finding Aid Harvard University Library Archived from the original on July 3 2018 Retrieved July 30 2021 The Famous People 2017 B F Skinner biography a b Skinner B F 1974 Causes of Behavior Pp 16 18 in About Behaviorism ISBN 0 394 71618 3 section 3 Radical Behaviorism https archive org stream aboutbehaviorism00skin page 16 mode 2up Pavlov Ivan 1927 Conditioned Reflexes Oxford Oxford University Press Thorndike Edward L 1911 Animal Intelligence Experimental Studies New York Macmillan a b c Jenkins H M 1979 Animal Learning amp Behavior Ch 5 in The First Century of Experimental Psychology edited by E Hearst Hillsdale NJ Erlbaum a b Skinner B F 1966 Contingencies of Reinforcement New York Appleton Century Crofts Skinner B F 1953 Science and Human Behavior New York Macmillan Skinner B F 1981 Selection by Consequences PDF Science 213 4507 501 04 Bibcode 1981Sci 213 501S doi 10 1126 science 7244649 PMID 7244649 Archived from the original PDF on July 2 2010 Retrieved August 14 2010 Different Types of Reinforcement Schedules PDF autismpdc fpg unc edu National Professional Development Center for Autism Spectrum Disorders Archived PDF from the original on October 9 2022 Retrieved February 14 2015 Hergenhahn B R 2009 An Introduction to the History of Psychology United States Wadsworth Cengage Learning p 449 ISBN 978 0 495 50621 8 B F Skinner 1957 Verbal Behavior The account in the appendix is that he asked Skinner to explain why he said No black scorpion Carter is falling upon this table Skinner Burrhus Frederick 1904 1990 Credo Reference Gale Retrieved October 1 2013 a b Chomsky Noam 1967 A Review of B F Skinner s Verbal Behavior PDF In Jakobovits L A Miron M S eds Readings in the Psychology of Language Prentice Hall pp 48 63 Archived PDF from the original on October 9 2022 Retrieved July 29 2021 Richelle M 1993 B F Skinner A Reappraisal Hillsdale Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Michael J 1984 Verbal Behavior Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 42 3 363 376 doi 10 1901 jeab 1984 42 363 PMC 1348108 PMID 16812395 Kubina Richard M Kostewicz Douglas E Brennan Kaitlyn M King Seth A September 2017 A Critical Review of Line Graphs in Behavior Analytic Journals Educational Psychology Review 29 3 583 598 doi 10 1007 s10648 015 9339 x ISSN 1040 726X S2CID 142317036 a b c Joyce Nick amp Faye Cathy September 1 2010 Skinner Air Crib 23 a href Template Cite journal html title Template Cite journal cite journal a Cite journal requires journal help Epstein Robert November 1 1995 Babies in Boxes Psychology Today Bjork B F Skinner A life Washington DC American Psychological Association Buzan Deborah Skinner March 12 2004 I was not a lab rat the Guardian Retrieved January 21 2023 a b Skinner B F 1961 Why we need teaching machines Harvard Educational Review 31 377 398 Programmed Instruction and Task Analysis College of Education University of Houston Archived from the original on June 1 2019 Retrieved September 24 2012 Skinner B F 1961 Teaching machines Scientific American 205 3 90 112 doi 10 2307 1926170 JSTOR 1926170 PMID 13913636 Skinner B F and J Holland 1961 The Analysis of Behavior A Program for Self Instruction p 387 Rebirth of the Teaching Machine through the Seduction of Data Analytics This Time It s Personal Philip McRae Ph D April 14 2013 a b c Schultz Figueroa Project Pigeon Rendering the War Animal through Optical Technology JCMS Journal of Cinema and Media Studies a b Skinner B F 1936 The Verbal Summator and a Method for the Study of Latent Speech Journal of Psychology 2 1 71 107 doi 10 1080 00223980 1936 9917445 hdl 11858 00 001M 0000 002D 7E05 E S2CID 144303708 Rutherford A 2003 B F Skinner and the auditory inkblot The rise and fall of the verbal summator as a projective technique History of Psychology 4 362 78 Holland J 1992 B F Skinner American Psychologist a b Skinner B F 1968 The Technology of Teaching New York Appleton Century Crofts LCCN 68 12340 B F Skinner Sep 20 1971 Time Archived from the original on September 30 2007 Skinner B F 1968 The Design of Experimental Communities Pp 271 75 in International Encyclopedia of the Social Sciences 16 edited by S Darity New York Ramsey Richard David 1979 Morning Star The Values Communication of Skinner s Walden Two Ph D dissertation Troy NY Rensselaer Polytechnic Institute via University Microfilms Ann Arbor MI Ramsey attempts to analyze Walden Two Beyond Freedom and Dignity and other Skinner works in the context of Skinner s life lists over 500 sources Kuhlman Hilke October 1 2010 Living Walden Two B F Skinner s Behaviorist Utopia and Experimental Communities University of Illinois Press p 87 see Beyond Freedom and Dignity 1974 for example Asimov Nanette January 30 1996 Spanking Debate Hits Assembly SFGate San Francisco Chronicle Retrieved March 2 2008 A matter of Consequences p 412 a b c Skinner B F 1948 Superstition in the Pigeon Journal of Experimental Psychology 38 2 168 172 doi 10 1037 h0055873 PMID 18913665 S2CID 22577459 Timberlake W Lucas G A November 1 1985 The basis of superstitious behavior chance contingency stimulus substitution or appetitive behavior J Exp Anal Behav 44 3 279 299 doi 10 1901 jeab 1985 44 279 PMC 1348192 PMID 4086972 a b Chomsky Noam 1959 Reviews Verbal Behavior by B F Skinner Language 35 1 26 58 doi 10 2307 411334 JSTOR 411334 Archived from the original on September 29 2015 Retrieved May 20 2007 a b MacCorquodale Kenneth January 1 1970 On Chomsky s review of Skinner s Verbal Behavior Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 13 1 83 99 doi 10 1901 jeab 1970 13 83 ISSN 1938 3711 PMC 1333660 Skinner B F 1972 A Lecture on Having a Poem In Skinner B F ed Cumulative Record PDF 3rd ed Appleton Century Crofts pp 345 355 ISBN 978 0 9899839 9 0 Archived from the original PDF on August 7 2021 Retrieved August 7 2021 Palmer David C 2006 On Chomsky s appraisal of Skinner s Verbal Behavior a half century of misunderstanding The Behavior Analyst 29 2 253 267 doi 10 1007 BF03392134 PMC 2223153 PMID 22478467 Chomsky Noam 1971 The Case Against B F Skinner New York Review of Books Toates F 2009 Burrhus F Skinner The shaping of behavior Houndmills Basingstoke England Palgrave Macmillan Overskeid Geir September 2007 Looking for Skinner and Finding Freud American Psychologist 62 6 590 595 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 321 6288 doi 10 1037 0003 066x 62 6 590 PMID 17874899 S2CID 4610708 Rutherford A 2003 B F Skinner and the auditory inkblot The rise and fall of the verbal summator as a projective technique History of Psychology 6 4 362 378 doi 10 1037 1093 4510 6 4 362 PMID 14735913 Staddon J E R 2014 The New Behaviorism 2nd ed Staddon J E R 1995 On Responsibility and Punishment The Atlantic Monthly 1995 2 88 94 Staddon J E R 1999 On Responsibility in Science and Law Social Philosophy and Policy 16 146 74 reprint 1999 Pp 146 74 in Responsibility edited by E F Paul F D Miller and J Paul Cambridge Cambridge University Press The Pantheon of Skeptics CSI Committee for Skeptical Inquiry Archived from the original on January 31 2017 Retrieved April 30 2017 APS Member History search amphilsoc org Retrieved February 27 2023 Burrhus Frederic Skinner American Academy of Arts amp Sciences February 9 2023 Retrieved February 27 2023 B F Skinner www nasonline org Retrieved February 27 2023 Further reading editChiesa M 2004 Radical Behaviorism The Philosophy and the Science Epstein Robert 1997 Skinner as self manager Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis 30 545 69 Retrieved 2 June 2005 via ENVMED rochester edu Pauly Philip Joseph 1987 Controlling Life Jacques Loeb and the Engineering Ideal in Biology Oxford UK Oxford University Press ISBN 978 0 19 504244 3 Retrieved August 14 2010 Sundberg M L 2008 The VB MAPP The Verbal Behavior Milestones Assessment and Placement Program Basil Curzon L 2004 Teaching in Further Education A outline of Principles and Practice Hardin C J 2004 Effective Classroom Management Kaufhold J A 2002 The Psychology of Learning and the Art of Teaching Bjork D W 1993 B F Skinner A Life Dews P B ed 1970 Festschrift For B F Skinner New York Appleton Century Crofts Evans R I 1968 B F Skinner the man and his ideas Nye Robert D 1979 What Is B F Skinner Really Saying Englewood Cliffs NJ Prentice Hall Rutherford A 2009 Beyond the box B F Skinner s technology of behavior from laboratory to life 1950s 1970s Toronto University of Toronto Press Sagal P T 1981 Skinner s Philosophy Washington DC University Press of America Smith D L 2002 On Prediction and Control B F Skinner and the Technological Ideal of Science In W E Pickren amp D A Dewsbury Eds Evolving Perspectives on the History of Psychology Washington D C American Psychological Association Swirski Peter 2011 How I Stopped Worrying and Loved Behavioural Engineering or Communal Life Adaptations and B F Skinner s Walden Two American Utopia and Social Engineering in Literature Social Thought and Political History New York Routledge Wiener D N 1996 B F Skinner benign anarchist Wolfgang C H and Glickman Carl D 1986 Solving Discipline Problems Allyn and Bacon IncExternal links edit nbsp Wikiquote has quotations related to B F Skinner nbsp Wikimedia Commons has media related to B F Skinner Library resources about B F Skinner Resources in your library Resources in other libraries By B F Skinner Resources in your library Resources in other libraries B F Skinner Foundation homepage National Academy of Sciences biography Works by or about B F Skinner at Internet Archive Works by B F Skinner at LibriVox public domain audiobooks nbsp I was not a lab rat response by Skinner s daughter about the baby box Audio Recordings Society for Experimental Analysis of Behavior Skinner and Teaching Machine on YouTube Reprint of the Minotaur of the Behaviorist Maze Surviving Stanford s Learning House in the 1970s Journal of Humanistic Psychology Vol 51 Number 3 July 2011 266 272 Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title B F Skinner amp oldid 1216650793, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.