fbpx
Wikipedia

Ernest Hilgard

Ernest Ropiequet "Jack" Hilgard (July 25, 1904 – October 22, 2001) was an American psychologist and professor at Stanford University. He became famous in the 1950s for his research on hypnosis, especially with regard to pain control. Along with André Muller Weitzenhoffer, Hilgard developed the Stanford Hypnotic Susceptibility Scales. A Review of General Psychology survey, published in 2002, ranked Hilgard as the 29th most cited psychologist of the 20th century.[1]

Ernest Ropiequet Hilgard
Born(1904-07-25)July 25, 1904
DiedOctober 22, 2001(2001-10-22) (aged 97)
NationalityAmerican
Alma materUniversity of Illinois
Yale University
Known forHypnosis, Atkinson & Hilgard's Introduction to Psychology
SpouseJosephine R. Hilgard
Children2
AwardsNAS Award for Scientific Reviewing (1984)
Scientific career
FieldsPsychology
InstitutionsStanford University
Doctoral advisorRaymond Dodge
Doctoral studentsLloyd Humphreys
Wayne H. Holtzman
Angus Campbell
Charles Tart

Biography edit

Born in Belleville, Illinois, Ernest Ropiequet Hilgard was the son of a physician, Dr. George Engelmann Hilgard, and Laura Ropiequet Hilgard. Hilgard was initially drawn to engineering; he received a bachelor's degree in chemical engineering from the University of Illinois in 1924. He then studied psychology, receiving a Ph.D. from Yale University in 1930. He was elected a Fellow of the American Academy of Arts and Sciences in 1958.[2] In 1969, he was elected to the American Philosophical Society.[3] In 1984 Hilgard was awarded the NAS Award for Scientific Reviewing from the National Academy of Sciences, of which he was also a member.[4][5]

Hilgard met fellow psychologist Josephine Rohrs at Yale; they married in 1931 and had two children, Henry (born 1936) and Elizabeth Ann (born 1944).[6][7] Hilgard died in 2001 in Palo Alto, California, at the age of 97.[7]

Hypnosis edit

Hilgard is specifically known for his theory that a so-called "hidden observer" is created in the mind while hypnosis is taking place. His research on the hidden observer during hypnotic pain management was intended to provide support for his neodissociationist theory. This theory held that a person undergoing hypnosis can still observe his or her own pain without consciously experiencing any suffering. The phenomenon of the "hidden observer" was controversial and critics claimed it could be manufactured by suggestions, indicating that it was possibly no more than an artifact of the instructions given to the research participants.[citation needed] Writing in the late 1970s (Hilgard, E. (1977). Divided consciousness: Multiple controls in human thought and action. New York, NY: Wiley), Ernest Hilgard became convinced that we all have another being sharing our lives. Hilgard termed this entity the hidden observer.

In one of his books, Hilgard described a classic test demonstrating how this hidden entity is part of our consciousness. He wrote of a blind student who was hypnotized and, while in a trance state, was told that he would become deaf. The suggestion was so strong that he failed to react to any form of noise, even large sounds next to his ear. Of course, he also failed to respond to any questions he was asked while in his trance state. The hypnotist was keen to discover if anybody else was able to hear. He quietly said to the student, Perhaps there is some part of you that is hearing my voice and processing the information. If there is, I should like the index finger of your right hand to rise as a sign that this is the case(Hilgard, 1977, p. 186). The finger rose. At this, the student requested that he be brought out of the hypnotically induced period of deafness. On being awakened, the student said that he had requested to come out of the trance state because I felt my finger rise in a way that was not a spontaneous twitch, so you must have done something to make it rise, and I want to know what you did (p. 186). The hypnotist then asked him what he remembered. Because the trance was light, the student never actually lost consciousness; all that occurred was that his hearing had ceased. In order to deal with the boredom of being deprived of both sight and sound, he had decided to work on some statistical problems in his head. It was while he was doing this that he suddenly felt his finger lift. This was obviously strange to him, because under normal circumstances he was, like all of us, the person who decides on how the body moves. In this case he was not. Not only that, but somebody else in his head was responding to an external request that he had not heard. As far as Hilgard was concerned, the person who responded was the hidden observer.

One of Hilgard's subjects made the following interesting statement about what she experienced, making particular reference to what she sensed was her higher self: The hidden observer is cognizant of everything that is going on ... The hidden observer sees more, he questions more, he's aware of what is going on all of the time but getting in touch is totally unnecessary ... He's like a guardian angel that guards you from doing anything that will mess you up ... The hidden observer is looking through the tunnel, and sees everything in the tunnel ... Unless someone tells me to get in touch with the hidden observer I'm not in contact. It's just there. (Hilgard, 1977, p. 210) The hidden observer protects us from doing anything in hypnosis that we would not do under any circumstance consciously, such as causing someone else physical harm.

Divided consciousness edit

Divided consciousness is a term coined by Hilgard to define a psychological state in which one's consciousness is split into distinct components, possibly during hypnosis. The theory of a division of consciousness was touched upon by Carl Jung in 1935 when he stated, "The so-called unity of consciousness is an illusion ... we like to think that we are one but we are not."[8] Ernest Hilgard believed that hypnosis causes a split in awareness and a vivid form of everyday mind splits.[9] Drawing themes from Pierre Janet, Hilgard viewed hypnosis from this perspective as a willingness to divide the main systems of consciousness into different sectors. He argued that this split in consciousness can not only help define the state of mind reached during hypnosis, but can also help to define a vast range of psychological issues such as dissociative identity disorder.

In Hilgard's Divided Consciousness Reconsidered, he offers many examples of "dissociated" human behavior. With regard to theory, he does state that it is useful to assign two modes of consciousness, a receptive mode and an active mode—that is, a bimodal consciousness. In other places he mentions the concept of coconsciousness, wherein two or more states of consciousness may be equally receptive or active, as, for example, in some types of dissociative personalities.[10]

Many psychological studies assume a unity of consciousness. Doubt is cast on this assumption by psychophysical studies in normal subjects and those with blindsight showing the simultaneous dissociation of different modes of reporting of a sensation, and by clinical studies of anosognosic patients showing dissociations of awareness of their own states.[citation needed] These and other phenomena are interpreted to imply two kinds of division of consciousness: the separation of phenomenal experience from reflexive consciousness and the non-unity of reflexive consciousness. Reflexive consciousness is taken to be necessary for report and is associated with ‘the self’ as the subject of experience and its own agent of reporting. Reflexive consciousness is operative only when we attend to our own states. When we are involved in the world, reflexivity intervenes less and our consciousness is more unified.[11]

Some experimental work, such as one performed on 169 undergraduate students, some of whom performed tasks in selective attention and divided attention conditions being correlated with scores on the Harvard Group Scale of Hypnotic Susceptibility—refute Hilgard's findings.[12]

The organizing principles that constitute human consciousness and other mental phenomena may be described by analysis and reconstruction of the underlying dynamics of psychophysiological measures.[13]

Duality of personality edit

This idea of the basic duality of human personality is culturally and historically almost universal. The ancient Chinese called these two independent consciousnesses hun and po, the ancient Egyptians the ka and the ba, and the ancient Greeks the Daemon and the Eidolon. In each case, the two entities shared their senses and perceptions of the external world but interpreted those perceptions with regard to their own history, knowledge, and personality.

For the Greeks, the relationship was an unequal one. The higher self, the Daemon, acted as a form of guardian angel or higher self over its lower self, the Eidolon. The Stoic philosopher Epictetus wrote: God has placed at every man's side a guardian, the Daemon of each man, who is charged to watch over him; a Daemon that cannot sleep, nor be deceived. To what greater and more watchful guardian could He have entrusted each of us? So, when you have shut the doors, and made darkness in the house, remember, never to say that you are alone; for you are not alone. But God is there, and your Daemon is there (Epictetus, 1998/2nd century, 14:11) The belief was that the Daemon had foreknowledge of future circumstances and events and as such could warn its Eidolon of the dangers. It was as if in some way the Daemon had already lived the life of its Eidolon.

Textbooks edit

Hilgard was also the author of three hugely influential textbooks on topics other than hypnosis. The first, "Conditioning and Learning", jointly authored with Donald Marquis, was very widely cited up until the 1960s. When Gregory Kimble updated a second edition in 1961, Hilgard and Marquis's names were made part of the title, a distinction, as Hilgard himself noted, usually reserved for deceased authors.

A second text, "Theories of Learning" (1948), was also widely cited, and lasted for five editions (through 1981); the last three editions involved Hilgard's Stanford colleague Gordon H. Bower.

The third textbook was the well written and wide-ranging "Introduction to Psychology" (1953), which was, according to his biography on the website of the American Psychological Association, "for a long period, the most widely used introductory psychology text in the world." Several editions were co-authored by Rita L. Atkinson or Richard C. Atkinson, another colleague at Stanford and later chancellor of the University of California at San Diego and then president and regent of the University of California. The 15th edition, published in 2009, is called "Atkinson and Hilgard's Introduction to Psychology".

Publications edit

  • Hilgard E.R. and Marquis D.G 1940. Conditioning and learning. New York: Appleton-Century.
    • Hilgard E.R. and Marquis D.G. 1961. Conditioning and learning. 2nd ed, Prentice-Hall. ISBN 978-0-13-388876-8
  • Hilgard E.R. 1948. Theories of learning. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
    • Hilgard E.R. and Bower G.H. 1966. Theories of learning. 3rd ed, New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
  • Hilgard E.R. 1965. Susceptibility to hypnosis. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.
  • Hilgard E.R. 1953, 1970. Introduction to psychology. Harcourt. ISBN 0-15-543646-5
    • Hilgard E.R., Atkinson R.L. and Atkinson R.C. 1975. Introduction to psychology. Harcourt Brace Jovanovich. ISBN 0-15-543657-0
  • Hilgard E.R. 1977. Divided consciousness: multiple controls in human thought and action. New York, NY: Wiley. ISBN 978-0-471-39602-4
    • Hilgard E.R. 1986. Divided consciousness: multiple controls in human thought and action (expanded edition). New York, NY: Wiley. ISBN 0-471-80572-6
  • Hilgard E.R. 1987. Psychology in America: a historical survey. San Diego: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.
  • Hilgard E.R. and J. Hilgard. 1994. Hypnosis in the relief of pain. Revised ed. Philadelphia: Brunner/Mazel.

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ Haggbloom, Steven J.; Warnick, Jason E.; Jones, Vinessa K.; Yarbrough, Gary L.; Russell, Tenea M.; Borecky, Chris M.; McGahhey, Reagan; Powell, John L. III; et al. (2002). "The 100 most eminent psychologists of the 20th century". Review of General Psychology. 6 (2): 139–152. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.6.2.139. S2CID 145668721.
  2. ^ "Book of Members, 1780–2010: Chapter H" (PDF). American Academy of Arts and Sciences. Retrieved 10 April 2011.
  3. ^ "APS Member History". search.amphilsoc.org. Retrieved 2022-09-13.
  4. ^ . National Academy of Sciences. Archived from the original on 18 March 2011. Retrieved 27 February 2011.
  5. ^ "E. R. Hilgard". www.nasonline.org. Retrieved 2022-09-13.
  6. ^ Bowers, Kenneth S. (1990). "Josephine R. Hilgard (1906–1989): Obituary". American Psychologist. 45 (12): 1382. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.45.12.1382. ISSN 1935-990X.
  7. ^ a b Saxon, Wolfgang (2001-11-03). "Ernest R. Hilgard, Leader in Study of Hypnosis, Dies at 97". The New York Times. ISSN 0362-4331. Retrieved 2020-01-01.
  8. ^ (Review: Dissociationism Revived, Matthew Hugh Erdelyi, Science, New Series, Vol. 200, No. 4342 (May 12, 1978), pp. 654–655; Published by: American Association for the Advancement of Science)
  9. ^ (Myers, David G. Psychology: Eighth Edition in Modules. New York, NY: Worth Publishers, 2007)
  10. ^ Huebner, B. (1979). Distributing cognition: A defense of collective mentality, Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 8 (6),591; Retrieved from [1][dead link]
  11. ^ Hebb, D, Juzyck, P, Klein R.,(1983). The Nature of Thought, Medical Research Council, Applied Psychology Unit, Cambridge, UK. Retrieved from https://books.google.com/books?id=0cprqzSe6BkC&dq=By+Donald+Olding+Hebb,+Peter+W.+Jusczyk,+Raymond+M.+Klein&pg=PA32
  12. ^ Some operationalizations of the neodissociation concept and their relationship to hypnotic susceptibility. Stava, Lawrence J.; Jaffa, Melvyn. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. Vol 54(6), Jun 1988, 989-996.)
  13. ^ Chaos, brain and divided consciousness. Petr Bob, Acta Univ Carol Med Monogr. 2007;153 :9-80 17867519 (P, S, G, E, B)

Further reading edit

  • Mestre M.V., Tortosa F., Samper P., and Nácher M.J. 2002. Psychology's evolution through its texts: analysis of E R. Hilgard's Introduction to Psychology. Psicothema, 14, 810–815. ISSN 0214-9915

External links edit

ernest, hilgard, ernest, ropiequet, jack, hilgard, july, 1904, october, 2001, american, psychologist, professor, stanford, university, became, famous, 1950s, research, hypnosis, especially, with, regard, pain, control, along, with, andré, muller, weitzenhoffer. Ernest Ropiequet Jack Hilgard July 25 1904 October 22 2001 was an American psychologist and professor at Stanford University He became famous in the 1950s for his research on hypnosis especially with regard to pain control Along with Andre Muller Weitzenhoffer Hilgard developed the Stanford Hypnotic Susceptibility Scales A Review of General Psychology survey published in 2002 ranked Hilgard as the 29th most cited psychologist of the 20th century 1 Ernest Ropiequet HilgardBorn 1904 07 25 July 25 1904Belleville IllinoisDiedOctober 22 2001 2001 10 22 aged 97 Palo Alto CaliforniaNationalityAmericanAlma materUniversity of IllinoisYale UniversityKnown forHypnosis Atkinson amp Hilgard s Introduction to PsychologySpouseJosephine R HilgardChildren2AwardsNAS Award for Scientific Reviewing 1984 Scientific careerFieldsPsychologyInstitutionsStanford UniversityDoctoral advisorRaymond DodgeDoctoral studentsLloyd HumphreysWayne H HoltzmanAngus CampbellCharles Tart Contents 1 Biography 2 Hypnosis 3 Divided consciousness 4 Duality of personality 5 Textbooks 6 Publications 7 See also 8 References 9 Further reading 10 External linksBiography editBorn in Belleville Illinois Ernest Ropiequet Hilgard was the son of a physician Dr George Engelmann Hilgard and Laura Ropiequet Hilgard Hilgard was initially drawn to engineering he received a bachelor s degree in chemical engineering from the University of Illinois in 1924 He then studied psychology receiving a Ph D from Yale University in 1930 He was elected a Fellow of the American Academy of Arts and Sciences in 1958 2 In 1969 he was elected to the American Philosophical Society 3 In 1984 Hilgard was awarded the NAS Award for Scientific Reviewing from the National Academy of Sciences of which he was also a member 4 5 Hilgard met fellow psychologist Josephine Rohrs at Yale they married in 1931 and had two children Henry born 1936 and Elizabeth Ann born 1944 6 7 Hilgard died in 2001 in Palo Alto California at the age of 97 7 Hypnosis editHilgard is specifically known for his theory that a so called hidden observer is created in the mind while hypnosis is taking place His research on the hidden observer during hypnotic pain management was intended to provide support for his neodissociationist theory This theory held that a person undergoing hypnosis can still observe his or her own pain without consciously experiencing any suffering The phenomenon of the hidden observer was controversial and critics claimed it could be manufactured by suggestions indicating that it was possibly no more than an artifact of the instructions given to the research participants citation needed Writing in the late 1970s Hilgard E 1977 Divided consciousness Multiple controls in human thought and action New York NY Wiley Ernest Hilgard became convinced that we all have another being sharing our lives Hilgard termed this entity the hidden observer In one of his books Hilgard described a classic test demonstrating how this hidden entity is part of our consciousness He wrote of a blind student who was hypnotized and while in a trance state was told that he would become deaf The suggestion was so strong that he failed to react to any form of noise even large sounds next to his ear Of course he also failed to respond to any questions he was asked while in his trance state The hypnotist was keen to discover if anybody else was able to hear He quietly said to the student Perhaps there is some part of you that is hearing my voice and processing the information If there is I should like the index finger of your right hand to rise as a sign that this is the case Hilgard 1977 p 186 The finger rose At this the student requested that he be brought out of the hypnotically induced period of deafness On being awakened the student said that he had requested to come out of the trance state because I felt my finger rise in a way that was not a spontaneous twitch so you must have done something to make it rise and I want to know what you did p 186 The hypnotist then asked him what he remembered Because the trance was light the student never actually lost consciousness all that occurred was that his hearing had ceased In order to deal with the boredom of being deprived of both sight and sound he had decided to work on some statistical problems in his head It was while he was doing this that he suddenly felt his finger lift This was obviously strange to him because under normal circumstances he was like all of us the person who decides on how the body moves In this case he was not Not only that but somebody else in his head was responding to an external request that he had not heard As far as Hilgard was concerned the person who responded was the hidden observer One of Hilgard s subjects made the following interesting statement about what she experienced making particular reference to what she sensed was her higher self The hidden observer is cognizant of everything that is going on The hidden observer sees more he questions more he s aware of what is going on all of the time but getting in touch is totally unnecessary He s like a guardian angel that guards you from doing anything that will mess you up The hidden observer is looking through the tunnel and sees everything in the tunnel Unless someone tells me to get in touch with the hidden observer I m not in contact It s just there Hilgard 1977 p 210 The hidden observer protects us from doing anything in hypnosis that we would not do under any circumstance consciously such as causing someone else physical harm Divided consciousness editDivided consciousness is a term coined by Hilgard to define a psychological state in which one s consciousness is split into distinct components possibly during hypnosis The theory of a division of consciousness was touched upon by Carl Jung in 1935 when he stated The so called unity of consciousness is an illusion we like to think that we are one but we are not 8 Ernest Hilgard believed that hypnosis causes a split in awareness and a vivid form of everyday mind splits 9 Drawing themes from Pierre Janet Hilgard viewed hypnosis from this perspective as a willingness to divide the main systems of consciousness into different sectors He argued that this split in consciousness can not only help define the state of mind reached during hypnosis but can also help to define a vast range of psychological issues such as dissociative identity disorder In Hilgard s Divided Consciousness Reconsidered he offers many examples of dissociated human behavior With regard to theory he does state that it is useful to assign two modes of consciousness a receptive mode and an active mode that is a bimodal consciousness In other places he mentions the concept of coconsciousness wherein two or more states of consciousness may be equally receptive or active as for example in some types of dissociative personalities 10 Many psychological studies assume a unity of consciousness Doubt is cast on this assumption by psychophysical studies in normal subjects and those with blindsight showing the simultaneous dissociation of different modes of reporting of a sensation and by clinical studies of anosognosic patients showing dissociations of awareness of their own states citation needed These and other phenomena are interpreted to imply two kinds of division of consciousness the separation of phenomenal experience from reflexive consciousness and the non unity of reflexive consciousness Reflexive consciousness is taken to be necessary for report and is associated with the self as the subject of experience and its own agent of reporting Reflexive consciousness is operative only when we attend to our own states When we are involved in the world reflexivity intervenes less and our consciousness is more unified 11 Some experimental work such as one performed on 169 undergraduate students some of whom performed tasks in selective attention and divided attention conditions being correlated with scores on the Harvard Group Scale of Hypnotic Susceptibility refute Hilgard s findings 12 The organizing principles that constitute human consciousness and other mental phenomena may be described by analysis and reconstruction of the underlying dynamics of psychophysiological measures 13 Duality of personality editThis idea of the basic duality of human personality is culturally and historically almost universal The ancient Chinese called these two independent consciousnesses hun and po the ancient Egyptians the ka and the ba and the ancient Greeks the Daemon and the Eidolon In each case the two entities shared their senses and perceptions of the external world but interpreted those perceptions with regard to their own history knowledge and personality For the Greeks the relationship was an unequal one The higher self the Daemon acted as a form of guardian angel or higher self over its lower self the Eidolon The Stoic philosopher Epictetus wrote God has placed at every man s side a guardian the Daemon of each man who is charged to watch over him a Daemon that cannot sleep nor be deceived To what greater and more watchful guardian could He have entrusted each of us So when you have shut the doors and made darkness in the house remember never to say that you are alone for you are not alone But God is there and your Daemon is there Epictetus 1998 2nd century 14 11 The belief was that the Daemon had foreknowledge of future circumstances and events and as such could warn its Eidolon of the dangers It was as if in some way the Daemon had already lived the life of its Eidolon Textbooks editHilgard was also the author of three hugely influential textbooks on topics other than hypnosis The first Conditioning and Learning jointly authored with Donald Marquis was very widely cited up until the 1960s When Gregory Kimble updated a second edition in 1961 Hilgard and Marquis s names were made part of the title a distinction as Hilgard himself noted usually reserved for deceased authors A second text Theories of Learning 1948 was also widely cited and lasted for five editions through 1981 the last three editions involved Hilgard s Stanford colleague Gordon H Bower The third textbook was the well written and wide ranging Introduction to Psychology 1953 which was according to his biography on the website of the American Psychological Association for a long period the most widely used introductory psychology text in the world Several editions were co authored by Rita L Atkinson or Richard C Atkinson another colleague at Stanford and later chancellor of the University of California at San Diego and then president and regent of the University of California The 15th edition published in 2009 is called Atkinson and Hilgard s Introduction to Psychology Publications editHilgard E R and Marquis D G 1940 Conditioning and learning New York Appleton Century Hilgard E R and Marquis D G 1961 Conditioning and learning 2nd ed Prentice Hall ISBN 978 0 13 388876 8 Hilgard E R 1948 Theories of learning New York Appleton Century Crofts Hilgard E R and Bower G H 1966 Theories of learning 3rd ed New York Appleton Century Crofts Hilgard E R 1965 Susceptibility to hypnosis New York Harcourt Brace Jovanovich Hilgard E R 1953 1970 Introduction to psychology Harcourt ISBN 0 15 543646 5 Hilgard E R Atkinson R L and Atkinson R C 1975 Introduction to psychology Harcourt Brace Jovanovich ISBN 0 15 543657 0 Hilgard E R 1977 Divided consciousness multiple controls in human thought and action New York NY Wiley ISBN 978 0 471 39602 4 Hilgard E R 1986 Divided consciousness multiple controls in human thought and action expanded edition New York NY Wiley ISBN 0 471 80572 6 Hilgard E R 1987 Psychology in America a historical survey San Diego Harcourt Brace Jovanovich Hilgard E R and J Hilgard 1994 Hypnosis in the relief of pain Revised ed Philadelphia Brunner Mazel See also editAlien hand syndrome Bicameral mentality Brain asymmetry Dual consciousness Divided consciousness Cognitive Neuroscience Folk psychology Ideomotor phenomenon Julian Jaynes Laterality Lateralization of brain function Left brain interpreter Mind body problem Parallel computing Philosophy of mind Society of Mind Split brain Theory of mindReferences edit Haggbloom Steven J Warnick Jason E Jones Vinessa K Yarbrough Gary L Russell Tenea M Borecky Chris M McGahhey Reagan Powell John L III et al 2002 The 100 most eminent psychologists of the 20th century Review of General Psychology 6 2 139 152 doi 10 1037 1089 2680 6 2 139 S2CID 145668721 Book of Members 1780 2010 Chapter H PDF American Academy of Arts and Sciences Retrieved 10 April 2011 APS Member History search amphilsoc org Retrieved 2022 09 13 NAS Award for Scientific Reviewing National Academy of Sciences Archived from the original on 18 March 2011 Retrieved 27 February 2011 E R Hilgard www nasonline org Retrieved 2022 09 13 Bowers Kenneth S 1990 Josephine R Hilgard 1906 1989 Obituary American Psychologist 45 12 1382 doi 10 1037 0003 066X 45 12 1382 ISSN 1935 990X a b Saxon Wolfgang 2001 11 03 Ernest R Hilgard Leader in Study of Hypnosis Dies at 97 The New York Times ISSN 0362 4331 Retrieved 2020 01 01 Review Dissociationism Revived Matthew Hugh Erdelyi Science New Series Vol 200 No 4342 May 12 1978 pp 654 655 Published by American Association for the Advancement of Science Myers David G Psychology Eighth Edition in Modules New York NY Worth Publishers 2007 Huebner B 1979 Distributing cognition A defense of collective mentality Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 8 6 591 Retrieved from 1 dead link Hebb D Juzyck P Klein R 1983 The Nature of Thought Medical Research Council Applied Psychology Unit Cambridge UK Retrieved from https books google com books id 0cprqzSe6BkC amp dq By Donald Olding Hebb Peter W Jusczyk Raymond M Klein amp pg PA32 Some operationalizations of the neodissociation concept and their relationship to hypnotic susceptibility Stava Lawrence J Jaffa Melvyn Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Vol 54 6 Jun 1988 989 996 Chaos brain and divided consciousness Petr Bob Acta Univ Carol Med Monogr 2007 153 9 80 17867519 P S G E B Further reading editMestre M V Tortosa F Samper P and Nacher M J 2002 Psychology s evolution through its texts analysis of E R Hilgard s Introduction to Psychology Psicothema 14 810 815 ISSN 0214 9915External links editThis article uses bare URLs which are uninformative and vulnerable to link rot Please consider converting them to full citations to ensure the article remains verifiable and maintains a consistent citation style Several templates and tools are available to assist in formatting such as reFill documentation and Citation bot documentation March 2024 Learn how and when to remove this template message https web archive org web 20051111062701 http slick org deathwatch mailarchive msg00383 html http findarticles com p articles mi g2699 is 0004 ai 2699000497 http garfield library upenn edu classics1988 A1988M802100001 pdf http www garfield library upenn edu classics1984 A1984SW51700001 pdf http www apa org about archives presidents bio ernest hilgard aspx National Academy of Sciences Biographical Memoir Ernest Ropiequet Hilgard Papers Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Ernest Hilgard amp oldid 1213020432 Divided consciousness, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.