fbpx
Wikipedia

Sexual selection in amphibians

Sexual selection in amphibians involves sexual selection processes in amphibians, including frogs, salamanders and newts. Prolonged breeders, the majority of frog species, have breeding seasons at regular intervals where male-male competition occurs with males arriving at the waters edge first in large number and producing a wide range of vocalizations, with variations in depth of calls the speed of calls[1] and other complex behaviours to attract mates. The fittest males will have the deepest croaks and the best territories, with females making their mate choices at least partly based on the males depth of croaking. This has led to sexual dimorphism, with females being larger than males in 90% of species, males in 10% and males fighting for groups of females.

Male Dendropsophus microcephalus calling

There is a direct competition between males to win the attention of the females in salamanders and newts, with elaborate courtship displays to keep the females attention long enough to get her interested in choosing him to mate with.[2] Some species store sperm through long breeding seasons, as the extra time may allow for interactions with rival sperm.[3]

Selection in salamanders edit

 
Smooth newt
 
Italian crested newt
 
Red-backed salamander

Important aspects of courtship displays include male alert posture, approach and pursuit, the marking of the substrate trail, and tail undulations.[2] Once a male has the attention of the female, he may release pheromones, which are used for individual recognition, mate recognition, and mate choice decisions,[4] and generally increase female receptivity towards the male's spermatophore and improve his chances of reproductive success with her. In the red-legged salamander (Plethodon shermani), males directly apply pheromones onto the female's nares.[5] In the aquatic smooth newt (Lissotriton vulgaris), the pheromones are delivered indirectly; the male wafts them towards the female with his tail.[6] In some species, it has been shown that the size a larger, more elaborate male's tail has played a role in the female's decision to mate.[7] In the newt species of the Italian crested newt (Triturus carnifex), males that possessed large mid-dorsal body crests were more favorable to the females looking to mate.[8]

Female choice is the final step in the males attempt to reproduce; if the female does not find the male's traits desirable, reproduction will not be successful. As it is in most species, body size is an important and most common desired trait amongst females. Body size is indicative of fitness and/or genetic quality; and because it is easily observable, it can be an indicator for how a female will pick her mate. A larger body size positively correlates to higher paternity; thus larger males are more successful in siring more offspring than smaller males.[9] Male salamanders also prefer larger females because they produce more ova than smaller females.[10]

In sexual coercion, a form of male sexual intimidation which includes forced copulations, repeated mating attempts with the same female, and punishing resisting females.[11] males forcibly mate with females; an example is the red-backed salamander (Plethodon cinereus). It has also been proposed as a third form of sexual selection, along with intersexual selection and intrasexual selection,[11] even though prevailing scientific opinion is that sexual coercion is a manifestation of sexual conflict, stemming out of the natural discord between males and females. In some species of salamanders, females that are territorial and aggressive also intimidate males. Plethodon cinereus females aggressively punish males who are exhibiting social polygyny.[12] Males have been shown to punish females that partake in mating polyandry, forcing them into mating monogamously.[13]

Populations of salamanders tend to be heavily male-biased, which results in a higher number of males vying for access to the lesser number of females.[14] Males will use different tactics in an effort to keep other males away from the female of interest and ensure their own paternity and reproductive success, such as sexual defense, which is used to steal other males spermatophore, which may have already deposited into the female, and sexual interference, when a male pushes the female away from another courting male,[10] such as sperm capping, whereby a competing male lays his spermatophore directly on top of those previously deposited by another male. Females also participate in sexual interference: a rival female places herself between the courting male and the female he is courting, and attempts to engage the male in their own courtship dance,[15] a tactic for avoiding sperm competition. The male removes the sperm from a female who had mated with a previous male: many species of salamanders will remove the spermatophore from the previous male and insert his own; thus, ensuring his paternity.[16][17]

Sperm storage is a method by which females can control the sperm that fertilizes their eggs. Female salamanders and newts, most notably Triturus vulgaris, possess sperm-storage glands called spermathecae in the walls of their cloacae.[18] Sperm storage has evolved independently to control reproduction in response to the environment. Females have the ability to store multiple spermatophores until they are needed for fertilization.[17] Sperm storage leads to the post-copulatory sexual selection process of cryptic female choice, which occurs when a female removes the deposited sperm or favors one set of spermatophore over another, during or after copulation.[19] Spermatophores are composed of a cap containing the spermatozoa and a gelatinous support arrangement which fastens the spermatophore to a substrate.[20] Spermatophores are very central to reproductive success; therefore, they are handled carefully. Mating success has a direct correlation to favorable phenotypic traits such as tail loss, adult body size, and age.[21]

Vocal signaling in frogs edit

 
Gray tree frog
 
Glass frog
 
Northern cricket frog
 
Boophis madagascariensis

The vocalizations that male frogs use to call for females are crucial for reproductive success in frogs.[22] Initially, male frogs produce advertisement calls, considered a pre-mating isolating mechanism because females use them to isolate conspecific males,[23] as cited in[24] which convey information about species, sexual readiness, and size,[24] with females moving towards the call they find most attractive. In Hyla versicolor, females favor long calls rather than short calls[25] while in Dendropsophus microcephalus they are attracted to short and high rate calling.[24]

isolated spring peeper call.

If a female shows interest male frogs will switch to courtship calls,[24] some of which are simply an increase in rate,[23] as cited by Wells and Schwartz,[24] which can contain fifty percent more notes than advertisement calls, and can become more complex when a male is at a specific oviposition site.[26] as cited by[24] The glass frog (Hyalinobatrachium fleischmanni) from Panama gives long calls when a female frog is first seen and will switch to multiple staccato chirps once a female indicates interest.[27] as cited by[24]

Since anurans are highly sensitive to motion, females use conspicuous movements to communicate interest.[28] A female frog may also send vocal signals of her own to males, motivating copulation and competitive behaviors.[29] Female Iberian midwife toads (Alytes cisternasii) use courtship calls to distinguish themselves from other male competitors. The African common platanna (Xenopus laevis) uses duet calling when males are not visible, allowing females to distinguish territorial males from satellite males.[30] The Emei music frog (Babina daunchina) female calling stimulates male movement towards sound or phonotaxis.

sophisticated call of Brazilian Torrent Frog.

Calling is an energetically expensive activity, requiring high levels of aerobic respiration. Selection favors the most efficient call production, a good quality call that does not require high levels of energy.[24] allowing a male frog to be able to call continuously for females. Dendropsophus microcephalus, a small tree frog, will produce 100 notes per minute for several hours, requiring up to 25 times more oxygen.[24] Some frogs produce less energetic calls and remain in chorus for longer periods of time with the ability to remain active longer being better for mating success than a higher energy vocal display.[25]

The morphological and biochemical processes of call production are influenced by sexual selection.[23] as cited by[24] Male frogs have developed vocal sacs that enable louder calls, with the larger tympanum increasing the radiation of the vocal signal.[31] as cited by[32] Males also have larger trunk muscles with highly aerobic muscle fibers, many mitochondria, and fat storage.[25]

The sensory exploitation hypothesis states that females have natural preexisting preferences for sexual signals that have been favored by sexual selection,[33] with males producing calls that are known to be favorable for females. A recent study of advertisement calls from 72 species of anurans has indicated that most call features in males show phylogenetic correlation, indicating that species-specific evolution of calls might be constrained by a lack of genetic variation or because female preferences for call features have diverged to only a limited extent.[34] Male anurans will often alter the rate of calling in order to intimidate other males though females tend to favor advertisement and courtship calls over aggressive ones, indicating that aggressive vocalizations are a form of male- male competition.[35] as cited by[24]

Advertisement and aggressive calls are usually similar in frequency but produced at different rates. Geocrinia victoriana, an eastern Australian smooth frog, has a long aggressive note to ward off competitors and shorter notes to attract females.[36] In Northern cricket frogs (Acris crepitans) male aggressive calls serve a dual purpose of attracting females and intimidating other male callers. The Madagascar bright eyed frog (Boophis madagascariensis) is able to produce 28 different types of aggressive calls by modifying pulse rate and notes.[37] as cited by[24]

Males can intercept vocal signals that were not meant for them in a tactic known as eavesdropping. The Hyla versicolor male increases calling rate and duration once a female is in vicinity. Eavesdroppers will intercept these increased signals and begin to call for the female in a more aggressive manner,[24] which generally leads to escalated conflicts.[38][39] Eavesdropping also allows male frogs to assess the body size of their competitors.[24] The frequency of vocal signals is dependent on the mass of the laryngeal apparatus, and a large one will produce lower frequency calls, positively correlated to overall body size.[39] Male frogs will use eavesdropping to decide whether or not to initiate aggressive conflicts. Male anurans are more likely to attack a frog with higher frequency calls.[39] It can also inform frogs when the competition has become too intense, where it can reduce or stop calling altogether and move to a different location outside of the chorus. In order to save energy they attempt to call females where there is less competition and a better chance to court a female.[24] Once a satellite male hears a change in vocalizations of other males he will begin courtship calling in an attempt to steal a female, with some males now invests more energy into a competitive courtship call which may sway the female's interest. Satellite males may contribute 20% of sexual reproduction in a populations.[40] as cited by[41]

Polygynandry in frogs edit

 
Allobates femoralis
 
Bibron's toadlet

Anuran mating systems are promiscuous: females and males breed with multiple mates and females exhibit sequential polyandry when one female is fertilized by more than one male at different times. Anurans are external fertilizers so there is no sperm competition.[42] Polyandry has benefits for females such as fertility insurance, paternal care insurance and dispersal of mortality risk. Female anurans have a high risk of mating with sperm depleted males due to frequent mating[42] and tend to select the same few males displaying fitness qualities. The requirement for successful copulation is that the male and female gametes have to be released at the same time and at the right orientation.[43] as cited by[42]

In anurans, 70% of polyandrous species exhibit paternal care. In Allobates femoralis the female's offspring survival depends on the male's carrying abilities, as they are responsible for carrying tadpoles from terrestrial to aquatic sites.[42] If a female only mates with one male and he is a poor parent, she may lose all of her offspring. Females reduce the risk of losing all of their offspring because it is likely that some of the males they mated with make good parents.[42] Sequential polyandry will allow females to have a multiple clutches in different nest sites, reducing the risk of abiotic factors, such as temperature, or biotic factors like predators affecting development of the offspring.[44] as cited by[42]

Some male behaviors that may have led to polyandry are displacement, where a male is gripped onto and mating with a female, another male may make an effort to mate as well and displace the male that was originally copulating; forced copulation, where there is a high density of aggressive males, and female abandonment, where a female but spots a better one to mate with and abandons the less favorable one. This can lead to sequential polyandry if the male leaves before the entire clutch is released and an additional male fertilizes these eggs as well.[42] In Bibron's toadlet (Pseudophryne bibronii) females spread out their eggs across the nests of two to eight males, with those who exhibited higher levels of polyandrous behavior having higher mean offspring survivorship, showing that insurance against nest failure and dispersal of mortality risk is related to the evolution of polyandry.[45]

Dimorphism and morphology edit

 
Small male and large female Fleay's barred frog (Mixophyes fleayi) in amplexus
 
Male tusked frog

Sexual dimorphism is seen as size difference between sexes in salamanders[46] and in 90% of anuran species.[47] Larger females is a result of selection for fecundity, including egg size or clutch size as they can store more energy and produce more offspring.[48] as cited by[41] Females are never more than twice the size of males.[41]

Size is important in male-male competition, with larger males having an advantage, due to the "male combat hypothesis"[47] which suggests that male combative behavior is positively related to male body size. Males in the tusked frog (Adelotus brevis) and the fanged frog (Limnonectes kuhlii) use fangs and tusks as weapons to defend their calling sites. Larger body size and larger weaponry leads to higher reproductive rates.[49] as cited by[41] However, data suggest that this male mechanism only operates in superfamily Dentrobatoidea,[41] possibly due to alternative mating techniques that reduce selection pressure on body size. Large African bullfrog (Pyxicephalus adspersus) males defend territories while small intimidated males move away and become satellite males. These smaller males use eavesdropping techniques and mate with females who are in range of their satellite location.[41]

Sexual role reversal in Dendrobates auratus edit

 
Poison dart frog Dendrobates auratus

Initial observation of mating in the green poison-dart frog Dendrobates auratus shows convincing evidence of sex-role reversal. Small female clutch size, female-female competition, and large parental care investment by males are characteristic of sexual role reversal.[50] as cited by[51] To test whether this sexual role reversal was true, two hypotheses were tested; the sexual role reversal hypothesis and the parental quality hypothesis.[52]

For the sexual role reversal hypothesis to be true, females would compete for as many males as possible, and this was not supported because male-male competition for mates was as common as female-female competition. In sexual role reversal males usually become more selective; however, in Dendrobates auratus females were more selective when choosing males, supporting the parental quality hypothesis, which predicted that females would compete for the parental care of particular males. Females were defensive of their mating partners and used courtship techniques to prevent males from mating with other females and destroyed rivals' eggs.[52] These observations support the parental quality hypothesis. Summers[52] weakens the sexual role reversal hypothesis and strengthens the case parental quality selection.

References edit

  1. ^ Tungara Frog Study’s Findings Counter Sexual Selection Theory Published 29 August 2015, by Matt Towns
  2. ^ a b Romano, Antonio; Giacomo Bruni (2011). "Courtship behaviour, mating season and male sexual interference in Salamandrina perspicillata". Amphibia-Reptilia. 32 (1): 63–76. doi:10.1163/017353710X541878.
  3. ^ Adams, Erika M.; Jones, A.G.; Arnold, S.J. (2005). "Multiple paternity in a natural population of a salamander with long-term sperm storage". Molecular Ecology. 14 (6): 1803–1810. doi:10.1111/j.1365-294X.2005.02539.x. PMID 15836651. S2CID 18899524.
  4. ^ Steinfartz, Sebastian; Barbara Caspers (2011). "Preference for the other sex: Olfactory sex recognition in terrestrial fire salamanders (Salamandra salamandra)". Amphibia-Reptilia. 32 (4): 503–508. doi:10.1163/156853811x603265.
  5. ^ M. Kiemnec-Tyburczy, Karen; Sarah K. Woodley; Pamela W. Feldhoff; Richard C. Feldhoff; Lynne D. Houck (2011). "Dermal application of courtship pheromones does not influence receptivity in female red-legged salamanders (Plethodon shermani)". Journal of Herpetology. 45 (2): 169–173. doi:10.1670/10-014.1. S2CID 83886115.
  6. ^ Promislow, Daniel E. L. (1987). "Courtship behavior of a plethodontid salamander, Desmognathus aeneus". Journal of Herpetology. 21 (4): 298–306. doi:10.2307/1563971. JSTOR 1563971.
  7. ^ Ficetola, Gentile Francesco; Anna Bonardi; Emiliano Colleon (2012). "Evolution of Sexual Dimorphism in the Number of Tail Vertebrae in Salamanders: Comparing Multiple Hypotheses". Evolutionary Biology. 40 (2): 220–227. doi:10.1007/s11692-012-9203-6. S2CID 13414859.
  8. ^ Wiens, J.J.; M. Sparreboom; J. W. Arntzen (2011). "Crest evolution in newts: implications for reconstruction methods, sexual selection, phenotypic plasticity and the origin of novelties". Journal of Evolutionary Biology. 24 (10): 2073–2086. doi:10.1111/j.1420-9101.2011.02340.x. PMID 21707814.
  9. ^ Chandler, C. H.; K. R. Zamudio (2008). "Reproductive success by large, closely related males facilitated by sperm storage in an aggregate breeding amphibian". Molecular Ecology. 17 (6): 1564–1576. doi:10.1111/j.1365-294X.2007.03614.x. PMID 18179431. S2CID 20341638.
  10. ^ a b Williams, Rod N.; J. Andrew DeWoody (2009). "Reproductive Success and Sexual Selection in Wild Eastern Tiger Salamanders (Ambystoma t. tigrinum)". Evolutionary Biology. 36 (2): 201–213. doi:10.1007/s11692-009-9058-7. S2CID 22305575.
  11. ^ a b Hucko, Jeffery A.; Jaeger, Robert G.; Prosen, Ethan D (2006). "Sexual coercion in the salamander Plethodon cinereus: is it merely a result of familiarity?". Herpetologica. 62 (1): 10–19. doi:10.1655/04-05.1. S2CID 85816435.
  12. ^ Prosen, Ethan D.; RG Jaeger; DR Lee (2004). "Sexual coercion in a territorial salamander: females punish socially polygynous male partners". Animal Behaviour. 67 (1): 85–92. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.02.005. S2CID 53193673.
  13. ^ Jaeger, Robert G.; Gillette, Jennifer R.; Cooper, Robert C (2002). "Sexual coercion in a territorial salamander: males punish socially polyandrous female partners". Animal Behaviour. 63 (5): 871–877. doi:10.1006/anbe.2001.1977. S2CID 53197139.
  14. ^ Liebgold, Eric B.; Brodie, Edmund D.; Cabe, Paul R. (2011). "Female philopatry and male-biased dispersal in a direct-developing salamander, Plethodon cinereus". Molecular Ecology. 20 (2): 249–257. doi:10.1111/j.1365-294X.2010.04946.x. PMID 21134012. S2CID 1828992.
  15. ^ Fei, Liang; Spareboom, Max; Xie, Feng (2001). "Reproductive behaviour of the Chinhai salamander (Echinotriton chinhaiensis) (Caudata: Salamandridae)". Amphibia-Reptilia. 22 (3): 309–320. doi:10.1163/156853801317050106.
  16. ^ Steinfartz, S.; Stemshorn, K.; Kuesters, D.; Tautz, D (2005). "Patterns of multiple paternity within and between annual reproduction cycles of the fire salamander (Salamandra salamandra) under natural conditions". Journal of Zoology. 268 (1): 1–8. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7998.2005.00001.x.
  17. ^ a b Kuehnel, Susanne; Kuper, A. (2012). "Sperm storage in caecilian amphibians". Frontiers in Zoology. 9 (1): 12. doi:10.1186/1742-9994-9-12. PMC 3418217. PMID 22672478.
  18. ^ Sever, David M.; Siegel, Dustin S. (2006). "Sperm aggregations in the spermatheca of the red back salamander (Plethodon cinereus)". Acta Zoologica. 87 (4): 331–340. doi:10.1111/j.1463-6395.2006.00249.x.
  19. ^ Eberhard, W. (2009). "Postcopulatory sexual selection: Darwin's omission and its consequences". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 106 (Suppl 1): 10025–10032. doi:10.1073/pnas.0901217106. PMC 2702800. PMID 19528642.
  20. ^ Doyle, Jacqueline M.; McCormick, CM.; DeWoody, J. (2011). "The quantification of spermatozoa by real-time quantitative PCR, spectrophotometry, and spermatophore cap size". Molecular Ecology Resources. 11 (1): 101–106. doi:10.1111/j.1755-0998.2010.02892.x. PMID 21429105. S2CID 23325666.
  21. ^ Houck, Lynne D.; Verrell, Paul A. (1993). "Studies of courtship behavior in plethodontid salamanders: a review". Herpetologica. 49 (2): 175–184. JSTOR 3892794.
  22. ^ Phelps, S.; Rand, A.; Ryan, M. (2006). "A cognitive framework for mate choice and species recognition". The American Naturalist. 167 (1): 28–42. doi:10.1086/498538. PMID 16475097. S2CID 15851718.
  23. ^ a b c Gerhardt, H. & Huber, F. Acoustic Communication in Insects and Anurans: Common Problems and Diverse Solutions. (Chicago: University of Chicago Press.: 2002).
  24. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o Wells, K. & Schwartz, J. The behavioral ecology of anuran communication. Hearing and sound communication in amphibians. 28, 44–86 (2007).
  25. ^ a b c Wells, K. D. & Ryan, M. "The energetics of calling in frogs." Anuran communication. 45–60 (2001).
  26. ^ Hoskin, J. (2004). "Australian microhylid frogs (Cophixalus and Austrochaperina): phylogeny, taxonomy, calls, distribution and breeding biology". Australian Journal of Zoology. 52 (3): 237–269. doi:10.1071/zo03056.
  27. ^ Greer, B.; Wells, K. (1980). "Territorial and reproductive behavior of the tropical American frog Centrolenella fleischmanni". Herpetologica. 36: 318–326.
  28. ^ Reichert, M (2013). "Visual cues elicit courtship signals in a nocturnal anuran". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 67 (2): 265–271. doi:10.1007/s00265-012-1446-9. S2CID 17704707.
  29. ^ Cui, J.; Wang, Y.; Brauth, S.; Tang, Y. (2010). "A novel female call incites male–female interaction and male–male competition in the Emei music frog, Babina daunchina". Animal Behaviour. 80 (2): 181–187. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.05.012. S2CID 53273985.
  30. ^ Emerson, S.; Boyd, S. (1999). "Mating vocalizations of female frogs: Control and evolutionary mechanisms". Brain, Behavior and Evolution. 53 (4): 187–197. doi:10.1159/000006594. PMID 10343085. S2CID 10147703.
  31. ^ Hetherington, T. E. (1994). "Sexual differences in the tympanic frequency responses of the American bullfrog (Rana catesbeiana)". Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 96 (2): 1186–1188. Bibcode:1994ASAJ...96.1186H. doi:10.1121/1.410326. PMID 7930061.
  32. ^ Schulte-Hostedde, A. I.; Kuula, S.; Martin, C.; Schank, C. C. M.; Lesbarrères, D. (2011). "Allometry and sexually dimorphic traits in male anurans". Journal of Evolutionary Biology. 24 (5): 1154–1159. doi:10.1111/j.1420-9101.2011.02256.x. PMID 21418118. S2CID 15212710.
  33. ^ Bee, M. A.; Schwartz, J. J.; Summers, K. (2013). "All's well that begins Wells : celebrating 60 years of Animal Behaviour and 36 years of research on anuran social behaviour". Animal Behaviour. 85: 5–18. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.10.031.
  34. ^ McLean, M.; Bishop, P.; Nakagawa, S. (2012). "Assessing the Patterns of Evolution in Anuran Vocal Sexual Signals". Evolutionary Biology. 40: 141–149. doi:10.1007/s11692-012-9197-0. S2CID 13427.
  35. ^ Schwartz, Joshua J.; Wells, Kentwood D. (1985). "Intra- and interspecific vocal behavior of the Neotropical treefrog Hyla microcephala". Copeia. 1985 (1): 27–38. doi:10.2307/1444787. JSTOR 1444787. S2CID 87856720.
  36. ^ Littlejohn, M.; Harrison, P. (1984). "The functional significance of the diphasic advertisement call of Geocrinia victoriana (Anura : Leptodactylidae)". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 16 (4): 363–373. doi:10.1007/bf00295550. S2CID 25811399.
  37. ^ Narins, P.; Lewis, E.; McClelland, B. (2000). "Hyperextended call note repertoire of the endemic Madagascar treefrog Boophis madagascariensis (Rhacophoridae)". Journal of Zoology. 250 (3): 283–298. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.576.5841. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7998.2000.tb00772.x.
  38. ^ Johnstone, R. A. (2001). "Eavesdropping and animal conflict". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 98 (16): 9177–9180. Bibcode:2001PNAS...98.9177J. doi:10.1073/pnas.161058798. PMC 55393. PMID 11459936.
  39. ^ a b c Bee, M (2002). "Territorial male bullfrogs (Rana catesbeiana) do not assess fighting ability based on size-related variation in acoustic signals". Behavioral Ecology. 13: 109–124. doi:10.1093/beheco/13.1.109.
  40. ^ Howard, R (1978). "The evolution of mating strategies in bullfrog, Rana catesbeiana". Evolution. 32 (4): 850–871. doi:10.2307/2407499. hdl:2027.42/137397. JSTOR 2407499. PMID 28567941.
  41. ^ a b c d e f Han, X.; Fu, J. (2013). "Does life history shape sexual size dimorphism in anurans? A comparative analysis". BMC Evolutionary Biology. 13: 27. doi:10.1186/1471-2148-13-27. PMC 3570426. PMID 23368708.
  42. ^ a b c d e f g Byrne, P. G.; Roberts, J. D. (2012). "Evolutionary causes and consequences of sequential polyandry in anuran amphibians". Biological Reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society. 87 (1): 209–28. doi:10.1111/j.1469-185x.2011.00191.x. PMID 21740503. S2CID 21449934.
  43. ^ Krupa, James J. (1988). "Fertilization efficiency in the Great Plains toad (Bufo cognatus)". Copeia. 1988 (3): 800–802. doi:10.2307/1445408. JSTOR 1445408.
  44. ^ Tsuji, H.; Lue, K. Y. (2000). "The reproductive ecology of female Rana (Limnonectes) kuhlii, a fanged frog of Taiwan, with particular emphasis on multiple clutches". Herpetologica. 56 (2): 153–165. JSTOR 3893267.
  45. ^ Byrne, P. G.; Keogh, J. S. (2009). "Extreme sequential polyandry insures against nest failure in a frog". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 276 (1654): 115–120. doi:10.1098/rspb.2008.0794. PMC 2614246. PMID 18782745.
  46. ^ Bruce, Richard C. (1993). "Sexual size dimorphism in desmognathine salamanders". Copeia. 1993 (2): 313–318. doi:10.2307/1447131. JSTOR 1447131.
  47. ^ a b Shine, R. (1979). "Sexual selection and sexual dimorphism in the Amphibia". Copeia. 1979 (2): 297–306. doi:10.2307/1443418. JSTOR 1443418.
  48. ^ Andersson, M. Sexual Selection. Princeton University Press (1994)
  49. ^ Tsuji, H.; Matsui, M. (2002). "Male-male combat and head morphology in a fanged frog (Rana kuhlii) from Taiwan". Journal of Herpetology. 36 (3): 520–526. doi:10.1670/0022-1511(2002)036[0520:mmcahm]2.0.co;2. S2CID 85329561.
  50. ^ Wells, K. D. (1978). "Courtship and parental behavior in a Panamanian poison-arrow frog (Dendrobates auratus)". Herpetologica. 34: 148–155.
  51. ^ Eens, M.; Pinxten, R. (2000). "Sex-role reversal in vertebrates: behavioural and endocrinological accounts". Behavioural Processes. 51 (1–3): 135–147. doi:10.1016/s0376-6357(00)00124-8. PMID 11074317. S2CID 20732874.
  52. ^ a b c Summers, K (1989). "Sexual selection and intra-female competition in the green poison-dart frog, Dendrobates auratus" (PDF). Animal Behaviour. 37: 797–805. doi:10.1016/0003-3472(89)90064-x. hdl:2027.42/27957. S2CID 34627111.

sexual, selection, amphibians, involves, sexual, selection, processes, amphibians, including, frogs, salamanders, newts, prolonged, breeders, majority, frog, species, have, breeding, seasons, regular, intervals, where, male, male, competition, occurs, with, ma. Sexual selection in amphibians involves sexual selection processes in amphibians including frogs salamanders and newts Prolonged breeders the majority of frog species have breeding seasons at regular intervals where male male competition occurs with males arriving at the waters edge first in large number and producing a wide range of vocalizations with variations in depth of calls the speed of calls 1 and other complex behaviours to attract mates The fittest males will have the deepest croaks and the best territories with females making their mate choices at least partly based on the males depth of croaking This has led to sexual dimorphism with females being larger than males in 90 of species males in 10 and males fighting for groups of females Male Dendropsophus microcephalus callingThere is a direct competition between males to win the attention of the females in salamanders and newts with elaborate courtship displays to keep the females attention long enough to get her interested in choosing him to mate with 2 Some species store sperm through long breeding seasons as the extra time may allow for interactions with rival sperm 3 Contents 1 Selection in salamanders 2 Vocal signaling in frogs 3 Polygynandry in frogs 4 Dimorphism and morphology 5 Sexual role reversal in Dendrobates auratus 6 ReferencesSelection in salamanders edit nbsp Smooth newt nbsp Italian crested newt nbsp Red backed salamanderImportant aspects of courtship displays include male alert posture approach and pursuit the marking of the substrate trail and tail undulations 2 Once a male has the attention of the female he may release pheromones which are used for individual recognition mate recognition and mate choice decisions 4 and generally increase female receptivity towards the male s spermatophore and improve his chances of reproductive success with her In the red legged salamander Plethodon shermani males directly apply pheromones onto the female s nares 5 In the aquatic smooth newt Lissotriton vulgaris the pheromones are delivered indirectly the male wafts them towards the female with his tail 6 In some species it has been shown that the size a larger more elaborate male s tail has played a role in the female s decision to mate 7 In the newt species of the Italian crested newt Triturus carnifex males that possessed large mid dorsal body crests were more favorable to the females looking to mate 8 Female choice is the final step in the males attempt to reproduce if the female does not find the male s traits desirable reproduction will not be successful As it is in most species body size is an important and most common desired trait amongst females Body size is indicative of fitness and or genetic quality and because it is easily observable it can be an indicator for how a female will pick her mate A larger body size positively correlates to higher paternity thus larger males are more successful in siring more offspring than smaller males 9 Male salamanders also prefer larger females because they produce more ova than smaller females 10 In sexual coercion a form of male sexual intimidation which includes forced copulations repeated mating attempts with the same female and punishing resisting females 11 males forcibly mate with females an example is the red backed salamander Plethodon cinereus It has also been proposed as a third form of sexual selection along with intersexual selection and intrasexual selection 11 even though prevailing scientific opinion is that sexual coercion is a manifestation of sexual conflict stemming out of the natural discord between males and females In some species of salamanders females that are territorial and aggressive also intimidate males Plethodon cinereus females aggressively punish males who are exhibiting social polygyny 12 Males have been shown to punish females that partake in mating polyandry forcing them into mating monogamously 13 Populations of salamanders tend to be heavily male biased which results in a higher number of males vying for access to the lesser number of females 14 Males will use different tactics in an effort to keep other males away from the female of interest and ensure their own paternity and reproductive success such as sexual defense which is used to steal other males spermatophore which may have already deposited into the female and sexual interference when a male pushes the female away from another courting male 10 such as sperm capping whereby a competing male lays his spermatophore directly on top of those previously deposited by another male Females also participate in sexual interference a rival female places herself between the courting male and the female he is courting and attempts to engage the male in their own courtship dance 15 a tactic for avoiding sperm competition The male removes the sperm from a female who had mated with a previous male many species of salamanders will remove the spermatophore from the previous male and insert his own thus ensuring his paternity 16 17 Sperm storage is a method by which females can control the sperm that fertilizes their eggs Female salamanders and newts most notably Triturus vulgaris possess sperm storage glands called spermathecae in the walls of their cloacae 18 Sperm storage has evolved independently to control reproduction in response to the environment Females have the ability to store multiple spermatophores until they are needed for fertilization 17 Sperm storage leads to the post copulatory sexual selection process of cryptic female choice which occurs when a female removes the deposited sperm or favors one set of spermatophore over another during or after copulation 19 Spermatophores are composed of a cap containing the spermatozoa and a gelatinous support arrangement which fastens the spermatophore to a substrate 20 Spermatophores are very central to reproductive success therefore they are handled carefully Mating success has a direct correlation to favorable phenotypic traits such as tail loss adult body size and age 21 Vocal signaling in frogs edit nbsp Gray tree frog nbsp Glass frog nbsp Northern cricket frog nbsp Boophis madagascariensisThe vocalizations that male frogs use to call for females are crucial for reproductive success in frogs 22 Initially male frogs produce advertisement calls considered a pre mating isolating mechanism because females use them to isolate conspecific males 23 as cited in 24 which convey information about species sexual readiness and size 24 with females moving towards the call they find most attractive In Hyla versicolor females favor long calls rather than short calls 25 while in Dendropsophus microcephalus they are attracted to short and high rate calling 24 source source track isolated spring peeper call If a female shows interest male frogs will switch to courtship calls 24 some of which are simply an increase in rate 23 as cited by Wells and Schwartz 24 which can contain fifty percent more notes than advertisement calls and can become more complex when a male is at a specific oviposition site 26 as cited by 24 The glass frog Hyalinobatrachium fleischmanni from Panama gives long calls when a female frog is first seen and will switch to multiple staccato chirps once a female indicates interest 27 as cited by 24 Since anurans are highly sensitive to motion females use conspicuous movements to communicate interest 28 A female frog may also send vocal signals of her own to males motivating copulation and competitive behaviors 29 Female Iberian midwife toads Alytes cisternasii use courtship calls to distinguish themselves from other male competitors The African common platanna Xenopus laevis uses duet calling when males are not visible allowing females to distinguish territorial males from satellite males 30 The Emei music frog Babina daunchina female calling stimulates male movement towards sound or phonotaxis source source sophisticated call of Brazilian Torrent Frog Calling is an energetically expensive activity requiring high levels of aerobic respiration Selection favors the most efficient call production a good quality call that does not require high levels of energy 24 allowing a male frog to be able to call continuously for females Dendropsophus microcephalus a small tree frog will produce 100 notes per minute for several hours requiring up to 25 times more oxygen 24 Some frogs produce less energetic calls and remain in chorus for longer periods of time with the ability to remain active longer being better for mating success than a higher energy vocal display 25 The morphological and biochemical processes of call production are influenced by sexual selection 23 as cited by 24 Male frogs have developed vocal sacs that enable louder calls with the larger tympanum increasing the radiation of the vocal signal 31 as cited by 32 Males also have larger trunk muscles with highly aerobic muscle fibers many mitochondria and fat storage 25 The sensory exploitation hypothesis states that females have natural preexisting preferences for sexual signals that have been favored by sexual selection 33 with males producing calls that are known to be favorable for females A recent study of advertisement calls from 72 species of anurans has indicated that most call features in males show phylogenetic correlation indicating that species specific evolution of calls might be constrained by a lack of genetic variation or because female preferences for call features have diverged to only a limited extent 34 Male anurans will often alter the rate of calling in order to intimidate other males though females tend to favor advertisement and courtship calls over aggressive ones indicating that aggressive vocalizations are a form of male male competition 35 as cited by 24 Advertisement and aggressive calls are usually similar in frequency but produced at different rates Geocrinia victoriana an eastern Australian smooth frog has a long aggressive note to ward off competitors and shorter notes to attract females 36 In Northern cricket frogs Acris crepitans male aggressive calls serve a dual purpose of attracting females and intimidating other male callers The Madagascar bright eyed frog Boophis madagascariensis is able to produce 28 different types of aggressive calls by modifying pulse rate and notes 37 as cited by 24 Males can intercept vocal signals that were not meant for them in a tactic known as eavesdropping The Hyla versicolor male increases calling rate and duration once a female is in vicinity Eavesdroppers will intercept these increased signals and begin to call for the female in a more aggressive manner 24 which generally leads to escalated conflicts 38 39 Eavesdropping also allows male frogs to assess the body size of their competitors 24 The frequency of vocal signals is dependent on the mass of the laryngeal apparatus and a large one will produce lower frequency calls positively correlated to overall body size 39 Male frogs will use eavesdropping to decide whether or not to initiate aggressive conflicts Male anurans are more likely to attack a frog with higher frequency calls 39 It can also inform frogs when the competition has become too intense where it can reduce or stop calling altogether and move to a different location outside of the chorus In order to save energy they attempt to call females where there is less competition and a better chance to court a female 24 Once a satellite male hears a change in vocalizations of other males he will begin courtship calling in an attempt to steal a female with some males now invests more energy into a competitive courtship call which may sway the female s interest Satellite males may contribute 20 of sexual reproduction in a populations 40 as cited by 41 Polygynandry in frogs edit nbsp Allobates femoralis nbsp Bibron s toadletAnuran mating systems are promiscuous females and males breed with multiple mates and females exhibit sequential polyandry when one female is fertilized by more than one male at different times Anurans are external fertilizers so there is no sperm competition 42 Polyandry has benefits for females such as fertility insurance paternal care insurance and dispersal of mortality risk Female anurans have a high risk of mating with sperm depleted males due to frequent mating 42 and tend to select the same few males displaying fitness qualities The requirement for successful copulation is that the male and female gametes have to be released at the same time and at the right orientation 43 as cited by 42 In anurans 70 of polyandrous species exhibit paternal care In Allobates femoralis the female s offspring survival depends on the male s carrying abilities as they are responsible for carrying tadpoles from terrestrial to aquatic sites 42 If a female only mates with one male and he is a poor parent she may lose all of her offspring Females reduce the risk of losing all of their offspring because it is likely that some of the males they mated with make good parents 42 Sequential polyandry will allow females to have a multiple clutches in different nest sites reducing the risk of abiotic factors such as temperature or biotic factors like predators affecting development of the offspring 44 as cited by 42 Some male behaviors that may have led to polyandry are displacement where a male is gripped onto and mating with a female another male may make an effort to mate as well and displace the male that was originally copulating forced copulation where there is a high density of aggressive males and female abandonment where a female but spots a better one to mate with and abandons the less favorable one This can lead to sequential polyandry if the male leaves before the entire clutch is released and an additional male fertilizes these eggs as well 42 In Bibron s toadlet Pseudophryne bibronii females spread out their eggs across the nests of two to eight males with those who exhibited higher levels of polyandrous behavior having higher mean offspring survivorship showing that insurance against nest failure and dispersal of mortality risk is related to the evolution of polyandry 45 Dimorphism and morphology edit nbsp Small male and large female Fleay s barred frog Mixophyes fleayi in amplexus nbsp Male tusked frogSexual dimorphism is seen as size difference between sexes in salamanders 46 and in 90 of anuran species 47 Larger females is a result of selection for fecundity including egg size or clutch size as they can store more energy and produce more offspring 48 as cited by 41 Females are never more than twice the size of males 41 Size is important in male male competition with larger males having an advantage due to the male combat hypothesis 47 which suggests that male combative behavior is positively related to male body size Males in the tusked frog Adelotus brevis and the fanged frog Limnonectes kuhlii use fangs and tusks as weapons to defend their calling sites Larger body size and larger weaponry leads to higher reproductive rates 49 as cited by 41 However data suggest that this male mechanism only operates in superfamily Dentrobatoidea 41 possibly due to alternative mating techniques that reduce selection pressure on body size Large African bullfrog Pyxicephalus adspersus males defend territories while small intimidated males move away and become satellite males These smaller males use eavesdropping techniques and mate with females who are in range of their satellite location 41 Sexual role reversal in Dendrobates auratus edit nbsp Poison dart frog Dendrobates auratusInitial observation of mating in the green poison dart frog Dendrobates auratus shows convincing evidence of sex role reversal Small female clutch size female female competition and large parental care investment by males are characteristic of sexual role reversal 50 as cited by 51 To test whether this sexual role reversal was true two hypotheses were tested the sexual role reversal hypothesis and the parental quality hypothesis 52 For the sexual role reversal hypothesis to be true females would compete for as many males as possible and this was not supported because male male competition for mates was as common as female female competition In sexual role reversal males usually become more selective however in Dendrobates auratus females were more selective when choosing males supporting the parental quality hypothesis which predicted that females would compete for the parental care of particular males Females were defensive of their mating partners and used courtship techniques to prevent males from mating with other females and destroyed rivals eggs 52 These observations support the parental quality hypothesis Summers 52 weakens the sexual role reversal hypothesis and strengthens the case parental quality selection References edit Tungara Frog Study s Findings Counter Sexual Selection Theory Published 29 August 2015 by Matt Towns a b Romano Antonio Giacomo Bruni 2011 Courtship behaviour mating season and male sexual interference in Salamandrina perspicillata Amphibia Reptilia 32 1 63 76 doi 10 1163 017353710X541878 Adams Erika M Jones A G Arnold S J 2005 Multiple paternity in a natural population of a salamander with long term sperm storage Molecular Ecology 14 6 1803 1810 doi 10 1111 j 1365 294X 2005 02539 x PMID 15836651 S2CID 18899524 Steinfartz Sebastian Barbara Caspers 2011 Preference for the other sex Olfactory sex recognition in terrestrial fire salamanders Salamandra salamandra Amphibia Reptilia 32 4 503 508 doi 10 1163 156853811x603265 M Kiemnec Tyburczy Karen Sarah K Woodley Pamela W Feldhoff Richard C Feldhoff Lynne D Houck 2011 Dermal application of courtship pheromones does not influence receptivity in female red legged salamanders Plethodon shermani Journal of Herpetology 45 2 169 173 doi 10 1670 10 014 1 S2CID 83886115 Promislow Daniel E L 1987 Courtship behavior of a plethodontid salamander Desmognathus aeneus Journal of Herpetology 21 4 298 306 doi 10 2307 1563971 JSTOR 1563971 Ficetola Gentile Francesco Anna Bonardi Emiliano Colleon 2012 Evolution of Sexual Dimorphism in the Number of Tail Vertebrae in Salamanders Comparing Multiple Hypotheses Evolutionary Biology 40 2 220 227 doi 10 1007 s11692 012 9203 6 S2CID 13414859 Wiens J J M Sparreboom J W Arntzen 2011 Crest evolution in newts implications for reconstruction methods sexual selection phenotypic plasticity and the origin of novelties Journal of Evolutionary Biology 24 10 2073 2086 doi 10 1111 j 1420 9101 2011 02340 x PMID 21707814 Chandler C H K R Zamudio 2008 Reproductive success by large closely related males facilitated by sperm storage in an aggregate breeding amphibian Molecular Ecology 17 6 1564 1576 doi 10 1111 j 1365 294X 2007 03614 x PMID 18179431 S2CID 20341638 a b Williams Rod N J Andrew DeWoody 2009 Reproductive Success and Sexual Selection in Wild Eastern Tiger Salamanders Ambystoma t tigrinum Evolutionary Biology 36 2 201 213 doi 10 1007 s11692 009 9058 7 S2CID 22305575 a b Hucko Jeffery A Jaeger Robert G Prosen Ethan D 2006 Sexual coercion in the salamander Plethodon cinereus is it merely a result of familiarity Herpetologica 62 1 10 19 doi 10 1655 04 05 1 S2CID 85816435 Prosen Ethan D RG Jaeger DR Lee 2004 Sexual coercion in a territorial salamander females punish socially polygynous male partners Animal Behaviour 67 1 85 92 doi 10 1016 j anbehav 2003 02 005 S2CID 53193673 Jaeger Robert G Gillette Jennifer R Cooper Robert C 2002 Sexual coercion in a territorial salamander males punish socially polyandrous female partners Animal Behaviour 63 5 871 877 doi 10 1006 anbe 2001 1977 S2CID 53197139 Liebgold Eric B Brodie Edmund D Cabe Paul R 2011 Female philopatry and male biased dispersal in a direct developing salamander Plethodon cinereus Molecular Ecology 20 2 249 257 doi 10 1111 j 1365 294X 2010 04946 x PMID 21134012 S2CID 1828992 Fei Liang Spareboom Max Xie Feng 2001 Reproductive behaviour of the Chinhai salamander Echinotriton chinhaiensis Caudata Salamandridae Amphibia Reptilia 22 3 309 320 doi 10 1163 156853801317050106 Steinfartz S Stemshorn K Kuesters D Tautz D 2005 Patterns of multiple paternity within and between annual reproduction cycles of the fire salamander Salamandra salamandra under natural conditions Journal of Zoology 268 1 1 8 doi 10 1111 j 1469 7998 2005 00001 x a b Kuehnel Susanne Kuper A 2012 Sperm storage in caecilian amphibians Frontiers in Zoology 9 1 12 doi 10 1186 1742 9994 9 12 PMC 3418217 PMID 22672478 Sever David M Siegel Dustin S 2006 Sperm aggregations in the spermatheca of the red back salamander Plethodon cinereus Acta Zoologica 87 4 331 340 doi 10 1111 j 1463 6395 2006 00249 x Eberhard W 2009 Postcopulatory sexual selection Darwin s omission and its consequences Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 106 Suppl 1 10025 10032 doi 10 1073 pnas 0901217106 PMC 2702800 PMID 19528642 Doyle Jacqueline M McCormick CM DeWoody J 2011 The quantification of spermatozoa by real time quantitative PCR spectrophotometry and spermatophore cap size Molecular Ecology Resources 11 1 101 106 doi 10 1111 j 1755 0998 2010 02892 x PMID 21429105 S2CID 23325666 Houck Lynne D Verrell Paul A 1993 Studies of courtship behavior in plethodontid salamanders a review Herpetologica 49 2 175 184 JSTOR 3892794 Phelps S Rand A Ryan M 2006 A cognitive framework for mate choice and species recognition The American Naturalist 167 1 28 42 doi 10 1086 498538 PMID 16475097 S2CID 15851718 a b c Gerhardt H amp Huber F Acoustic Communication in Insects and Anurans Common Problems and Diverse Solutions Chicago University of Chicago Press 2002 a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o Wells K amp Schwartz J The behavioral ecology of anuran communication Hearing and sound communication in amphibians 28 44 86 2007 a b c Wells K D amp Ryan M The energetics of calling in frogs Anuran communication 45 60 2001 Hoskin J 2004 Australian microhylid frogs Cophixalus and Austrochaperina phylogeny taxonomy calls distribution and breeding biology Australian Journal of Zoology 52 3 237 269 doi 10 1071 zo03056 Greer B Wells K 1980 Territorial and reproductive behavior of the tropical American frog Centrolenella fleischmanni Herpetologica 36 318 326 Reichert M 2013 Visual cues elicit courtship signals in a nocturnal anuran Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 67 2 265 271 doi 10 1007 s00265 012 1446 9 S2CID 17704707 Cui J Wang Y Brauth S Tang Y 2010 A novel female call incites male female interaction and male male competition in the Emei music frog Babina daunchina Animal Behaviour 80 2 181 187 doi 10 1016 j anbehav 2010 05 012 S2CID 53273985 Emerson S Boyd S 1999 Mating vocalizations of female frogs Control and evolutionary mechanisms Brain Behavior and Evolution 53 4 187 197 doi 10 1159 000006594 PMID 10343085 S2CID 10147703 Hetherington T E 1994 Sexual differences in the tympanic frequency responses of the American bullfrog Rana catesbeiana Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 96 2 1186 1188 Bibcode 1994ASAJ 96 1186H doi 10 1121 1 410326 PMID 7930061 Schulte Hostedde A I Kuula S Martin C Schank C C M Lesbarreres D 2011 Allometry and sexually dimorphic traits in male anurans Journal of Evolutionary Biology 24 5 1154 1159 doi 10 1111 j 1420 9101 2011 02256 x PMID 21418118 S2CID 15212710 Bee M A Schwartz J J Summers K 2013 All s well that begins Wells celebrating 60 years of Animal Behaviour and 36 years of research on anuran social behaviour Animal Behaviour 85 5 18 doi 10 1016 j anbehav 2012 10 031 McLean M Bishop P Nakagawa S 2012 Assessing the Patterns of Evolution in Anuran Vocal Sexual Signals Evolutionary Biology 40 141 149 doi 10 1007 s11692 012 9197 0 S2CID 13427 Schwartz Joshua J Wells Kentwood D 1985 Intra and interspecific vocal behavior of the Neotropical treefrog Hyla microcephala Copeia 1985 1 27 38 doi 10 2307 1444787 JSTOR 1444787 S2CID 87856720 Littlejohn M Harrison P 1984 The functional significance of the diphasic advertisement call of Geocrinia victoriana Anura Leptodactylidae Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 16 4 363 373 doi 10 1007 bf00295550 S2CID 25811399 Narins P Lewis E McClelland B 2000 Hyperextended call note repertoire of the endemic Madagascar treefrog Boophis madagascariensis Rhacophoridae Journal of Zoology 250 3 283 298 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 576 5841 doi 10 1111 j 1469 7998 2000 tb00772 x Johnstone R A 2001 Eavesdropping and animal conflict Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 98 16 9177 9180 Bibcode 2001PNAS 98 9177J doi 10 1073 pnas 161058798 PMC 55393 PMID 11459936 a b c Bee M 2002 Territorial male bullfrogs Rana catesbeiana do not assess fighting ability based on size related variation in acoustic signals Behavioral Ecology 13 109 124 doi 10 1093 beheco 13 1 109 Howard R 1978 The evolution of mating strategies in bullfrog Rana catesbeiana Evolution 32 4 850 871 doi 10 2307 2407499 hdl 2027 42 137397 JSTOR 2407499 PMID 28567941 a b c d e f Han X Fu J 2013 Does life history shape sexual size dimorphism in anurans A comparative analysis BMC Evolutionary Biology 13 27 doi 10 1186 1471 2148 13 27 PMC 3570426 PMID 23368708 a b c d e f g Byrne P G Roberts J D 2012 Evolutionary causes and consequences of sequential polyandry in anuran amphibians Biological Reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society 87 1 209 28 doi 10 1111 j 1469 185x 2011 00191 x PMID 21740503 S2CID 21449934 Krupa James J 1988 Fertilization efficiency in the Great Plains toad Bufo cognatus Copeia 1988 3 800 802 doi 10 2307 1445408 JSTOR 1445408 Tsuji H Lue K Y 2000 The reproductive ecology of female Rana Limnonectes kuhlii a fanged frog of Taiwan with particular emphasis on multiple clutches Herpetologica 56 2 153 165 JSTOR 3893267 Byrne P G Keogh J S 2009 Extreme sequential polyandry insures against nest failure in a frog Proceedings of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences 276 1654 115 120 doi 10 1098 rspb 2008 0794 PMC 2614246 PMID 18782745 Bruce Richard C 1993 Sexual size dimorphism in desmognathine salamanders Copeia 1993 2 313 318 doi 10 2307 1447131 JSTOR 1447131 a b Shine R 1979 Sexual selection and sexual dimorphism in the Amphibia Copeia 1979 2 297 306 doi 10 2307 1443418 JSTOR 1443418 Andersson M Sexual Selection Princeton University Press 1994 Tsuji H Matsui M 2002 Male male combat and head morphology in a fanged frog Rana kuhlii from Taiwan Journal of Herpetology 36 3 520 526 doi 10 1670 0022 1511 2002 036 0520 mmcahm 2 0 co 2 S2CID 85329561 Wells K D 1978 Courtship and parental behavior in a Panamanian poison arrow frog Dendrobates auratus Herpetologica 34 148 155 Eens M Pinxten R 2000 Sex role reversal in vertebrates behavioural and endocrinological accounts Behavioural Processes 51 1 3 135 147 doi 10 1016 s0376 6357 00 00124 8 PMID 11074317 S2CID 20732874 a b c Summers K 1989 Sexual selection and intra female competition in the green poison dart frog Dendrobates auratus PDF Animal Behaviour 37 797 805 doi 10 1016 0003 3472 89 90064 x hdl 2027 42 27957 S2CID 34627111 Portal nbsp Amphibians Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Sexual selection in amphibians amp oldid 1181894904, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.