fbpx
Wikipedia

Reward system

The reward system (the mesocorticolimbic circuit) is a group of neural structures responsible for incentive salience (i.e., "wanting"; desire or craving for a reward and motivation), associative learning (primarily positive reinforcement and classical conditioning), and positively-valenced emotions, particularly ones involving pleasure as a core component (e.g., joy, euphoria and ecstasy).[1][2] Reward is the attractive and motivational property of a stimulus that induces appetitive behavior, also known as approach behavior, and consummatory behavior.[1] A rewarding stimulus has been described as "any stimulus, object, event, activity, or situation that has the potential to make us approach and consume it is by definition a reward".[1] In operant conditioning, rewarding stimuli function as positive reinforcers;[3] however, the converse statement also holds true: positive reinforcers are rewarding.[3][4]

Examples of primary rewards.[3] Clockwise from top left: water, food, parental care, and sex.
Addiction and dependence glossary[5][6][7][8]
  • addiction – a biopsychosocial disorder characterized by persistent use of drugs (including alcohol) despite substantial harm and adverse consequences
  • addictive drug – psychoactive substances that with repeated use are associated with significantly higher rates of substance use disorders, due in large part to the drug's effect on brain reward systems
  • dependence – an adaptive state associated with a withdrawal syndrome upon cessation of repeated exposure to a stimulus (e.g., drug intake)
  • drug sensitization or reverse tolerance – the escalating effect of a drug resulting from repeated administration at a given dose
  • drug withdrawal – symptoms that occur upon cessation of repeated drug use
  • physical dependence – dependence that involves persistent physical–somatic withdrawal symptoms (e.g., fatigue and delirium tremens)
  • psychological dependence – dependence that involves emotional–motivational withdrawal symptoms (e.g., dysphoria and anhedonia)
  • reinforcing stimuli – stimuli that increase the probability of repeating behaviors paired with them
  • rewarding stimuli – stimuli that the brain interprets as intrinsically positive and desirable or as something to approach
  • sensitization – an amplified response to a stimulus resulting from repeated exposure to it
  • substance use disorder – a condition in which the use of substances leads to clinically and functionally significant impairment or distress
  • tolerance – the diminishing effect of a drug resulting from repeated administration at a given dose

The reward system motivates animals to approach stimuli or engage in behaviour that increases fitness (sex, energy-dense foods, etc.). Survival for most animal species depends upon maximizing contact with beneficial stimuli and minimizing contact with harmful stimuli. Reward cognition serves to increase the likelihood of survival and reproduction by causing associative learning, eliciting approach and consummatory behavior, and triggering positively-valenced emotions.[3] Thus, reward is a mechanism that evolved to help increase the adaptive fitness of animals.[9] In drug addiction, certain substances over-activate the reward circuit, leading to compulsive substance-seeking behavior resulting from synaptic plasticity in the circuit.[10]

Primary rewards are a class of rewarding stimuli which facilitate the survival of one's self and offspring, and they include homeostatic (e.g., palatable food) and reproductive (e.g., sexual contact and parental investment) rewards.[1][11] Intrinsic rewards are unconditioned rewards that are attractive and motivate behavior because they are inherently pleasurable.[1] Extrinsic rewards (e.g., money or seeing one's favorite sports team winning a game) are conditioned rewards that are attractive and motivate behavior but are not inherently pleasurable.[1][12] Extrinsic rewards derive their motivational value as a result of a learned association (i.e., conditioning) with intrinsic rewards.[1] Extrinsic rewards may also elicit pleasure (e.g., euphoria from winning a lot of money in a lottery) after being classically conditioned with intrinsic rewards.[1]

Definition

In neuroscience, the reward system is a collection of brain structures and neural pathways that are responsible for reward-related cognition, including associative learning (primarily classical conditioning and operant reinforcement), incentive salience (i.e., motivation and "wanting", desire, or craving for a reward), and positively-valenced emotions, particularly emotions that involve pleasure (i.e., hedonic "liking").[3][2]

Terms that are commonly used to describe behavior related to the "wanting" or desire component of reward include appetitive behavior, approach behavior, preparatory behavior, instrumental behavior, anticipatory behavior, and seeking.[13] Terms that are commonly used to describe behavior related to the "liking" or pleasure component of reward include consummatory behavior and taking behavior.[13]

The three primary functions of rewards are their capacity to:

  1. produce associative learning (i.e., classical conditioning and operant reinforcement);[3]
  2. affect decision-making and induce approach behavior (via the assignment of motivational salience to rewarding stimuli);[3]
  3. elicit positively-valenced emotions, particularly pleasure.[3]

Neuroanatomy

Overview

The brain structures that compose the reward system are located primarily within the cortico-basal ganglia-thalamo-cortical loop;[14] the basal ganglia portion of the loop drives activity within the reward system.[14] Most of the pathways that connect structures within the reward system are glutamatergic interneurons, GABAergic medium spiny neurons (MSNs), and dopaminergic projection neurons,[14][15] although other types of projection neurons contribute (e.g., orexinergic projection neurons). The reward system includes the ventral tegmental area, ventral striatum (i.e., the nucleus accumbens and olfactory tubercle), dorsal striatum (i.e., the caudate nucleus and putamen), substantia nigra (i.e., the pars compacta and pars reticulata), prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex, insular cortex, hippocampus, hypothalamus (particularly, the orexinergic nucleus in the lateral hypothalamus), thalamus (multiple nuclei), subthalamic nucleus, globus pallidus (both external and internal), ventral pallidum, parabrachial nucleus, amygdala, and the remainder of the extended amygdala.[2][14][16][17][18] The dorsal raphe nucleus and cerebellum appear to modulate some forms of reward-related cognition (i.e., associative learning, motivational salience, and positive emotions) and behaviors as well.[19][20][21] The laterodorsal tegmental nucleus (LTD), pedunculopontine nucleus (PPTg), and lateral habenula (LHb) (both directly and indirectly via the rostromedial tegmental nucleus) are also capable of inducing aversive salience and incentive salience through their projections to the ventral tegmental area (VTA).[22] The LDT and PPTg both send glutaminergic projections to the VTA that synapse on dopaminergic neurons, both of which can produce incentive salience. The LHb sends glutaminergic projections, the majority of which synapse on GABAergic RMTg neurons that in turn drive inhibition of dopaminergic VTA neurons, although some LHb projections terminate on VTA interneurons. These LHb projections are activated both by aversive stimuli and by the absence of an expected reward, and excitation of the LHb can induce aversion.[23][24][25]

Most of the dopamine pathways (i.e., neurons that use the neurotransmitter dopamine to communicate with other neurons) that project out of the ventral tegmental area are part of the reward system;[14] in these pathways, dopamine acts on D1-like receptors or D2-like receptors to either stimulate (D1-like) or inhibit (D2-like) the production of cAMP.[26] The GABAergic medium spiny neurons of the striatum are components of the reward system as well.[14] The glutamatergic projection nuclei in the subthalamic nucleus, prefrontal cortex, hippocampus, thalamus, and amygdala connect to other parts of the reward system via glutamate pathways.[14] The medial forebrain bundle, which is a set of many neural pathways that mediate brain stimulation reward (i.e., reward derived from direct electrochemical stimulation of the lateral hypothalamus), is also a component of the reward system.[27]

Two theories exist with regard to the activity of the nucleus accumbens and the generation liking and wanting. The inhibition (or hyper­polar­ization) hypothesis proposes that the nucleus accumbens exerts tonic inhibitory effects on downstream structures such as the ventral pallidum, hypothalamus or ventral tegmental area, and that in inhibiting MSNs in the nucleus accumbens (NAcc), these structures are excited, "releasing" reward related behavior. While GABA receptor agonists are capable of eliciting both "liking" and "wanting" reactions in the nucleus accumbens, glutaminergic inputs from the basolateral amygdala, ventral hippocampus, and medial prefrontal cortex can drive incentive salience. Furthermore, while most studies find that NAcc neurons reduce firing in response to reward, a number of studies find the opposite response. This had led to the proposal of the disinhibition (or depolarization) hypothesis, that proposes that excitation or NAcc neurons, or at least certain subsets, drives reward related behavior.[2][28][29]

After nearly 50 years of research on brain-stimulation reward, experts have certified that dozens of sites in the brain will maintain intracranial self-stimulation. Regions include the lateral hypothalamus and medial forebrain bundles, which are especially effective. Stimulation there activates fibers that form the ascending pathways; the ascending pathways include the mesolimbic dopamine pathway, which projects from the ventral tegmental area to the nucleus accumbens. There are several explanations as to why the mesolimbic dopamine pathway is central to circuits mediating reward. First, there is a marked increase in dopamine release from the mesolimbic pathway when animals engage in intracranial self-stimulation.[9] Second, experiments consistently indicate that brain-stimulation reward stimulates the reinforcement of pathways that are normally activated by natural rewards, and drug reward or intracranial self-stimulation can exert more powerful activation of central reward mechanisms because they activate the reward center directly rather than through the peripheral nerves.[9][30][31] Third, when animals are administered addictive drugs or engage in naturally rewarding behaviors, such as feeding or sexual activity, there is a marked release of dopamine within the nucleus accumbens.[9] However, dopamine is not the only reward compound in the brain.

Key pathway

 
Diagram showing some of the key components of the mesocorticolimbic ("reward") circuit.

Ventral tegmental area

  • The ventral tegmental area (VTA) is important in responding to stimuli and cues that indicate a reward is present. Rewarding stimuli (and all addictive drugs) act on the circuit by triggering the VTA to release dopamine signals to the nucleus accumbens, either directly or indirectly.[citation needed] The VTA has two important pathways: The mesolimbic pathway projecting to limbic (striatal) regions and underpinning the motivational behaviors and processes, and the mesocortical pathway projecting to the prefrontal cortex, underpinning cognitive functions, such as learning external cues, etc.[32]
  • Dopaminergic neurons in this region converts the amino acid tyrosine into DOPA using the enzyme tyrosine hydroxylase, which is then converted to dopamine using the enzyme dopa-decarboxylase.[33]

Striatum (Nucleus Accumbens)

  • The striatum is broadly involved in acquiring and eliciting learned behaviors in response to a rewarding cue. The VTA projects to the striatum, and activates the GABA-ergic Medium Spiny Neurons via D1 and D2 receptors within the ventral (Nucleus Accumbens) and dorsal striatum.[34]
  • The Ventral Striatum (the Nucleus Accumbens) is broadly involved in acquiring behavior when fed into by the VTA, and eliciting behavior when fed into by the PFC. The NAc shell projects to the pallidum and the VTA, regulating limbic and autonomic functions. This modulates the reinforcing properties of stimuli, and short term aspects of reward. The NAc Core projects to the substantia nigra and is involved in the development of reward-seeking behaviors and its expression. It is involved in spatial learning, conditional response, and impulsive choice; the long term elements of reward.[32]
  • The Dorsal Striatum is involved in learning, the Dorsal Medial Striatum in goal directed learning, and the Dorsal Lateral Striatum in stimulus-response learning foundational to Pavlovian response.[35] On repeated activation by a stimuli, the Nucleus Accumbens can activate the Dorsal Striatum via an intrastriatal loop. The transition of signals from the NAc to the DS allows reward associated cues to activate the DS without the reward itself being present. This can activate cravings and reward-seeking behaviors (and is responsible for triggering relapse during abstinence in addiction).[36]

Prefrontal Cortex

  • The VTA dopaminergic neurons project to the PFC, activating glutaminergic neurons that project to multiple other regions, including the Dorsal Striatum and NAc, ultimately allowing the PFC to mediate salience and conditional behaviors in response to stimuli.[36]
  • Notably, abstinence from addicting drugs activates the PFC, glutamatergic projection to the NAc, which leads to strong cravings, and modulates reinstatement of addiction behaviors resulting from abstinence. The PFC also interacts with the VTA through the mesocortical pathway, and helps associate environmental cues with the reward.[36]

Hippocampus

  • The Hippocampus has multiple functions, including in the creation and storage of memories . In the reward circuit, it serves to contextual memories and associated cues. It ultimately underpins the reinstatement of reward-seeking behaviors via cues, and contextual triggers.[37]

Amygdala

  • The AMY receives input from the VTA, and outputs to the NAc. The amygdala is important in creating powerful emotional flashbulb memories, and likely underpins the creation of strong cue-associated memories.[38] It also is important in mediating the anxiety effects of withdrawal, and increased drug intake in addiction.[39]

Pleasure centers

Pleasure is a component of reward, but not all rewards are pleasurable (e.g., money does not elicit pleasure unless this response is conditioned).[1] Stimuli that are naturally pleasurable, and therefore attractive, are known as intrinsic rewards, whereas stimuli that are attractive and motivate approach behavior, but are not inherently pleasurable, are termed extrinsic rewards.[1] Extrinsic rewards (e.g., money) are rewarding as a result of a learned association with an intrinsic reward.[1] In other words, extrinsic rewards function as motivational magnets that elicit "wanting", but not "liking" reactions once they have been acquired.[1]

The reward system contains pleasure centers or hedonic hotspots – i.e., brain structures that mediate pleasure or "liking" reactions from intrinsic rewards. As of October 2017, hedonic hotspots have been identified in subcompartments within the nucleus accumbens shell, ventral pallidum, parabrachial nucleus, orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), and insular cortex.[2][18][40] The hotspot within the nucleus accumbens shell is located in the rostrodorsal quadrant of the medial shell, while the hedonic coldspot is located in a more posterior region. The posterior ventral pallidum also contains a hedonic hotspot, while the anterior ventral pallidum contains a hedonic coldspot. In rats, microinjections of opioids, endocannabinoids, and orexin are capable of enhancing liking reactions in these hotspots.[2] The hedonic hotspots located in the anterior OFC and posterior insula have been demonstrated to respond to orexin and opioids in rats, as has the overlapping hedonic coldspot in the anterior insula and posterior OFC.[40] On the other hand, the parabrachial nucleus hotspot has only been demonstrated to respond to benzodiazepine receptor agonists.[2]

Hedonic hotspots are functionally linked, in that activation of one hotspot results in the recruitment of the others, as indexed by the induced expression of c-Fos, an immediate early gene. Furthermore, inhibition of one hotspot results in the blunting of the effects of activating another hotspot.[2][40] Therefore, the simultaneous activation of every hedonic hotspot within the reward system is believed to be necessary for generating the sensation of an intense euphoria.[41]

Wanting and liking

 
Tuning of appetitive and defensive reactions in the nucleus accumbens shell. (Above) AMPA blockade requires D1 function in order to produce motivated behaviors, regardless of valence, and D2 function to produce defensive behaviors. GABA agonism, on the other hand, does not requires dopamine receptor function.(Below)The expansion of the anatomical regions that produce defensive behaviors under stress, and appetitive behaviors in the home environment produced by AMPA antagonism. This flexibility is less evident with GABA agonism.[28]

Incentive salience is the "wanting" or "desire" attribute, which includes a motivational component, that is assigned to a rewarding stimulus by the nucleus accumbens shell (NAcc shell).[1][42][43] The degree of dopamine neurotransmission into the NAcc shell from the mesolimbic pathway is highly correlated with the magnitude of incentive salience for rewarding stimuli.[42]

Activation of the dorsorostral region of the nucleus accumbens correlates with increases in wanting without concurrent increases in liking.[44] However, dopaminergic neurotransmission into the nucleus accumbens shell is responsible not only for appetitive motivational salience (i.e., incentive salience) towards rewarding stimuli, but also for aversive motivational salience, which directs behavior away from undesirable stimuli.[13][45][46] In the dorsal striatum, activation of D1 expressing MSNs produces appetitive incentive salience, while activation of D2 expressing MSNs produces aversion. In the NAcc, such a dichotomy is not as clear cut, and activation of both D1 and D2 MSNs is sufficient to enhance motivation,[47][48] likely via disinhibiting the VTA through inhibiting the ventral pallidum.[49][50]

Robinson and Berridge's 1993 incentive-sensitization theory proposed that reward contains separable psychological components: wanting (incentive) and liking (pleasure). To explain increasing contact with a certain stimulus such as chocolate, there are two independent factors at work – our desire to have the chocolate (wanting) and the pleasure effect of the chocolate (liking). According to Robinson and Berridge, wanting and liking are two aspects of the same process, so rewards are usually wanted and liked to the same degree. However, wanting and liking also change independently under certain circumstances. For example, rats that do not eat after receiving dopamine (experiencing a loss of desire for food) act as though they still like food. In another example, activated self-stimulation electrodes in the lateral hypothalamus of rats increase appetite, but also cause more adverse reactions to tastes such as sugar and salt; apparently, the stimulation increases wanting but not liking. Such results demonstrate that the reward system of rats includes independent processes of wanting and liking. The wanting component is thought to be controlled by dopaminergic pathways, whereas the liking component is thought to be controlled by opiate-benzodiazepine systems.[9]

Anti-reward system

Koobs & Le Moal proposed that there exists a separate circuit responsible for the attenuation of reward-pursuing behavior, which they termed the anti-reward circuit. This component acts as brakes on the reward circuit, thus preventing the over pursuit of food, sex, etc. This circuit involves multiple parts of the amygdala (the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis, the central nucleus), the Nucleus Accumbens, and signal molecules including norepinephrine, corticotropin-releasing factor, and dynorphin.[51] This circuit is also hypothesized to mediate the unpleasant components of stress, and is thus thought to be involved in addiction and withdrawal. While the reward circuit mediates the initial positive reinforcement involved in the development of addiction, it is the anti-reward circuit that later dominates via negative reinforcement that motivates the pursuit of the rewarding stimuli.[52]

Learning

Rewarding stimuli can drive learning in both the form of classical conditioning (Pavlovian conditioning) and operant conditioning (instrumental conditioning). In classical conditioning, a reward can act as an unconditioned stimulus that, when associated with the conditioned stimulus, causes the conditioned stimulus to elicit both musculoskeletal (in the form of simple approach and avoidance behaviors) and vegetative responses. In operant conditioning, a reward may act as a reinforcer in that it increases or supports actions that lead to itself.[3] Learned behaviors may or may not be sensitive to the value of the outcomes they lead to; behaviors that are sensitive to the contingency of an outcome on the performance of an action as well as the outcome value are goal-directed, while elicited actions that are insensitive to contingency or value are called habits.[53] This distinction is thought to reflect two forms of learning, model free and model based. Model free learning involves the simple caching and updating of values. In contrast, model based learning involves the storage and construction of an internal model of events that allows inference and flexible prediction. Although pavlovian conditioning is generally assumed to be model-free, the incentive salience assigned to a conditioned stimulus is flexible with regard to changes in internal motivational states.[54]

Distinct neural systems are responsible for learning associations between stimuli and outcomes, actions and outcomes, and stimuli and responses. Although classical conditioning is not limited to the reward system, the enhancement of instrumental performance by stimuli (i.e., Pavlovian-instrumental transfer) requires the nucleus accumbens. Habitual and goal directed instrumental learning are dependent upon the lateral striatum and the medial striatum, respectively.[53]

During instrumental learning, opposing changes in the ratio of AMPA to NMDA receptors and phosphorylated ERK occurs in the D1-type and D2-type MSNs that constitute the direct and indirect pathways, respectively.[55][56] These changes in synaptic plasticity and the accompanying learning is dependent upon activation of striatal D1 and NMDA receptors. The intracellular cascade activated by D1 receptors involves the recruitment of protein kinase A, and through resulting phosphorylation of DARPP-32, the inhibition of phosphatases that deactivate ERK. NMDA receptors activate ERK through a different but interrelated Ras-Raf-MEK-ERK pathway. Alone NMDA mediated activation of ERK is self-limited, as NMDA activation also inhibits PKA mediated inhibition of ERK deactivating phosphatases. However, when D1 and NMDA cascades are co-activated, they work synergistically, and the resultant activation of ERK regulates synaptic plasticity in the form of spine restructuring, transport of AMPA receptors, regulation of CREB, and increasing cellular excitability via inhibiting Kv4.2[57][58][59]

Disorders

Addiction

ΔFosB (DeltaFosB) – a gene transcription factoroverexpression in the D1-type medium spiny neurons of the nucleus accumbens is the crucial common factor among virtually all forms of addiction (i.e., behavioral addictions and drug addictions) that induces addiction-related behavior and neural plasticity.[6][60][61][62] In particular, ΔFosB promotes self-administration, reward sensitization, and reward cross-sensitization effects among specific addictive drugs and behaviors.[6][60][61][63][64] Certain epigenetic modifications of histone protein tails (i.e., histone modifications) in specific regions of the brain are also known to play a crucial role in the molecular basis of addictions.[62][65][66][67]

Addictive drugs and behaviors are rewarding and reinforcing (i.e., are addictive) due to their effects on the dopamine reward pathway.[17][68]

The lateral hypothalamus and medial forebrain bundle has been the most-frequently-studied brain-stimulation reward site, particularly in studies of the effects of drugs on brain stimulation reward.[69] The neurotransmitter system that has been most-clearly identified with the habit-forming actions of drugs-of-abuse is the mesolimbic dopamine system, with its efferent targets in the nucleus accumbens and its local GABAergic afferents. The reward-relevant actions of amphetamine and cocaine are in the dopaminergic synapses of the nucleus accumbens and perhaps the medial prefrontal cortex. Rats also learn to lever-press for cocaine injections into the medial prefrontal cortex, which works by increasing dopamine turnover in the nucleus accumbens.[70][71] Nicotine infused directly into the nucleus accumbens also enhances local dopamine release, presumably by a presynaptic action on the dopaminergic terminals of this region. Nicotinic receptors localize to dopaminergic cell bodies and local nicotine injections increase dopaminergic cell firing that is critical for nicotinic reward.[72][73] Some additional habit-forming drugs are also likely to decrease the output of medium spiny neurons as a consequence, despite activating dopaminergic projections. For opiates, the lowest-threshold site for reward effects involves actions on GABAergic neurons in the ventral tegmental area, a secondary site of opiate-rewarding actions on medium spiny output neurons of the nucleus accumbens. Thus the following form the core of currently characterised drug-reward circuitry; GABAergic afferents to the mesolimbic dopamine neurons (primary substrate of opiate reward), the mesolimbic dopamine neurons themselves (primary substrate of psychomotor stimulant reward), and GABAergic efferents to the mesolimbic dopamine neurons (a secondary site of opiate reward).[69]

Motivation

Dysfunctional motivational salience appears in a number of psychiatric symptoms and disorders. Anhedonia, traditionally defined as a reduced capacity to feel pleasure, has been re-examined as reflecting blunted incentive salience, as most anhedonic populations exhibit intact “liking”.[74][75] On the other end of the spectrum, heightened incentive salience that is narrowed for specific stimuli is characteristic of behavioral and drug addictions. In the case of fear or paranoia, dysfunction may lie in elevated aversive salience.[76]

Neuroimaging studies across diagnoses associated with anhedonia have reported reduced activity in the OFC and ventral striatum.[77] One meta analysis reported anhedonia was associated with reduced neural response to reward anticipation in the caudate nucleus, putamen, nucleus accumbens and medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC).[78]

Mood disorders

Certain types of depression are associated with reduced motivation, as assessed by willingness to expend effort for reward. These abnormalities have been tentatively linked to reduced activity in areas of the striatum, and while dopaminergic abnormalities are hypothesized to play a role, most studies probing dopamine function in depression have reported inconsistent results.[79][80] Although postmortem and neuroimaging studies have found abnormalities in numerous regions of the reward system, few findings are consistently replicated. Some studies have reported reduced NAcc, hippocampus, medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), and orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) activity, as well as elevated basolateral amygdala and subgenual cingulate cortex (sgACC) activity during tasks related to reward or positive stimuli. These neuroimaging abnormalities are complemented by little post mortem research, but what little research has been done suggests reduced excitatory synapses in the mPFC.[81] Reduced activity in the mPFC during reward related tasks appears to be localized to more dorsal regions(i.e. the pregenual cingulate cortex), while the more ventral sgACC is hyperactive in depression.[82]

Attempts to investigate underlying neural circuitry in animal models has also yielded conflicting results. Two paradigms are commonly used to simulate depression, chronic social defeat (CSDS), and chronic mild stress (CMS), although many exist. CSDS produces reduced preference for sucrose, reduced social interactions, and increased immobility in the forced swim test. CMS similarly reduces sucrose preference, and behavioral despair as assessed by tail suspension and forced swim tests. Animals susceptible to CSDS exhibit increased phasic VTA firing, and inhibition of VTA-NAcc projections attenuates behavioral deficits induced by CSDS.[83] However, inhibition of VTA-mPFC projections exacerbates social withdrawal. On the other hand, CMS associated reductions in sucrose preference and immobility were attenuated and exacerbated by VTA excitation and inhibition, respectively.[84][85] Although these differences may be attributable to different stimulation protocols or poor translational paradigms, variable results may also lie in the heterogenous functionality of reward related regions.[86]

Optogenetic stimulation of the mPFC as a whole produces antidepressant effects. This effect appears localized to the rodent homologue of the pgACC (the prelimbic cortex), as stimulation of the rodent homologue of the sgACC (the infralimbic cortex) produces no behavioral effects. Furthermore, deep brain stimulation in the infralimbic cortex, which is thought to have an inhibitory effect, also produces an antidepressant effect. This finding is congruent with the observation that pharmacological inhibition of the infralimbic cortex attenuates depressive behaviors.[86]

Schizophrenia

Schizophrenia is associated with deficits in motivation, commonly grouped under other negative symptoms such as reduced spontaneous speech. The experience of “liking” is frequently reported to be intact,[87] both behaviorally and neurally, although results may be specific to certain stimuli, such as monetary rewards.[88] Furthermore, implicit learning and simple reward-related tasks are also intact in schizophrenia.[89] Rather, deficits in the reward system are apparent during reward-related tasks that are cognitively complex. These deficits are associated with both abnormal striatal and OFC activity, as well as abnormalities in regions associated with cognitive functions such as the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC).[90]

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder

In those with ADHD, core aspects of the reward system are underactive, making it challenging to derive reward from regular activities. Those with the disorder experience a boost of motivation after a high-stimulation behaviour triggers a release of dopamine. In the aftermath of that boost and reward, the return to baseline levels results in an immediate drop in motivation.[91]

Impairments of dopaminergic and serotonergic function are said to be key factors in ADHD.[92] These impairments can lead to executive dysfunction such as dysregulation of reward processing and motivational dysfunction, including anhedonia.[93]

History

 
Skinner box

The first clue to the presence of a reward system in the brain came with an accidental discovery by James Olds and Peter Milner in 1954. They discovered that rats would perform behaviors such as pressing a bar, to administer a brief burst of electrical stimulation to specific sites in their brains. This phenomenon is called intracranial self-stimulation or brain stimulation reward. Typically, rats will press a lever hundreds or thousands of times per hour to obtain this brain stimulation, stopping only when they are exhausted. While trying to teach rats how to solve problems and run mazes, stimulation of certain regions of the brain where the stimulation was found seemed to give pleasure to the animals. They tried the same thing with humans and the results were similar. The explanation to why animals engage in a behavior that has no value to the survival of either themselves or their species is that the brain stimulation is activating the system underlying reward.[94]

In a fundamental discovery made in 1954, researchers James Olds and Peter Milner found that low-voltage electrical stimulation of certain regions of the brain of the rat acted as a reward in teaching the animals to run mazes and solve problems.[95][failed verification][96] It seemed that stimulation of those parts of the brain gave the animals pleasure,[95] and in later work humans reported pleasurable sensations from such stimulation.[citation needed] When rats were tested in Skinner boxes where they could stimulate the reward system by pressing a lever, the rats pressed for hours.[96] Research in the next two decades established that dopamine is one of the main chemicals aiding neural signaling in these regions, and dopamine was suggested to be the brain's "pleasure chemical".[97]

Ivan Pavlov was a psychologist who used the reward system to study classical conditioning. Pavlov used the reward system by rewarding dogs with food after they had heard a bell or another stimulus. Pavlov was rewarding the dogs so that the dogs associated food, the reward, with the bell, the stimulus.[98] Edward L. Thorndike used the reward system to study operant conditioning. He began by putting cats in a puzzle box and placing food outside of the box so that the cat wanted to escape. The cats worked to get out of the puzzle box to get to the food. Although the cats ate the food after they escaped the box, Thorndike learned that the cats attempted to escape the box without the reward of food. Thorndike used the rewards of food and freedom to stimulate the reward system of the cats. Thorndike used this to see how the cats learned to escape the box.[99]

Other species

Animals quickly learn to press a bar to obtain an injection of opiates directly into the midbrain tegmentum or the nucleus accumbens. The same animals do not work to obtain the opiates if the dopaminergic neurons of the mesolimbic pathway are inactivated. In this perspective, animals, like humans, engage in behaviors that increase dopamine release.

Kent Berridge, a researcher in affective neuroscience, found that sweet (liked ) and bitter (disliked ) tastes produced distinct orofacial expressions, and these expressions were similarly displayed by human newborns, orangutans, and rats. This was evidence that pleasure (specifically, liking) has objective features and was essentially the same across various animal species. Most neuroscience studies have shown that the more dopamine released by the reward, the more effective the reward is. This is called the hedonic impact, which can be changed by the effort for the reward and the reward itself. Berridge discovered that blocking dopamine systems did not seem to change the positive reaction to something sweet (as measured by facial expression). In other words, the hedonic impact did not change based on the amount of sugar. This discounted the conventional assumption that dopamine mediates pleasure. Even with more-intense dopamine alterations, the data seemed to remain constant.[100] However, a clinical study from January 2019 that assessed the effect of a dopamine precursor (levodopa), antagonist (risperidone), and a placebo on reward responses to music – including the degree of pleasure experienced during musical chills, as measured by changes in electrodermal activity as well as subjective ratings – found that the manipulation of dopamine neurotransmission bidirectionally regulates pleasure cognition (specifically, the hedonic impact of music) in human subjects.[101][102] This research demonstrated that increased dopamine neurotransmission acts as a sine qua non condition for pleasurable hedonic reactions to music in humans.[101][102]

Berridge developed the incentive salience hypothesis to address the wanting aspect of rewards. It explains the compulsive use of drugs by drug addicts even when the drug no longer produces euphoria, and the cravings experienced even after the individual has finished going through withdrawal. Some addicts respond to certain stimuli involving neural changes caused by drugs. This sensitization in the brain is similar to the effect of dopamine because wanting and liking reactions occur. Human and animal brains and behaviors experience similar changes regarding reward systems because these systems are so prominent.[100]

See also

References

  1. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l m Schultz, Wolfram (July 2015). "Neuronal Reward and Decision Signals: From Theories to Data". Physiological Reviews. 95 (3): 853–951. doi:10.1152/physrev.00023.2014. PMC 4491543. PMID 26109341.
  2. ^ a b c d e f g h Berridge KC, Kringelbach ML (May 2015). "Pleasure systems in the brain". Neuron. 86 (3): 646–664. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2015.02.018. PMC 4425246. PMID 25950633. In the prefrontal cortex, recent evidence indicates that the [orbitofrontal cortex] OFC and insula cortex may each contain their own additional hot spots (D.C. Castro et al., Soc. Neurosci., abstract). In specific subregions of each area, either opioid-stimulating or orexin-stimulating microinjections appear to enhance the number of liking reactions elicited by sweetness, similar to the [nucleus accumbens] NAc and [ventral pallidum] VP hot spots. Successful confirmation of hedonic hot spots in the OFC or insula would be important and possibly relevant to the orbitofrontal mid-anterior site mentioned earlier that especially tracks the subjective pleasure of foods in humans (Georgiadis et al., 2012; Kringelbach, 2005; Kringelbach et al., 2003; Small et al., 2001; Veldhuizen et al., 2010). Finally, in the brainstem, a hindbrain site near the parabrachial nucleus of dorsal pons also appears able to contribute to hedonic gains of function (Söderpalm and Berridge, 2000). A brainstem mechanism for pleasure may seem more surprising than forebrain hot spots to anyone who views the brainstem as merely reflexive, but the pontine parabrachial nucleus contributes to taste, pain, and many visceral sensations from the body and has also been suggested to play an important role in motivation (Wu et al., 2012) and in human emotion (especially related to the somatic marker hypothesis) (Damasio, 2010).
  3. ^ a b c d e f g h i Schultz W (2015). "Neuronal reward and decision signals: from theories to data". Physiological Reviews. 95 (3): 853–951. doi:10.1152/physrev.00023.2014. PMC 4491543. PMID 26109341. Rewards in operant conditioning are positive reinforcers. ... Operant behavior gives a good definition for rewards. Anything that makes an individual come back for more is a positive reinforcer and therefore a reward. Although it provides a good definition, positive reinforcement is only one of several reward functions. ... Rewards are attractive. They are motivating and make us exert an effort. ... Rewards induce approach behavior, also called appetitive or preparatory behavior, sexual behavior, and consummatory behavior. ... Thus any stimulus, object, event, activity, or situation that has the potential to make us approach and consume it is by definition a reward. ... Rewarding stimuli, objects, events, situations, and activities consist of several major components. First, rewards have basic sensory components (visual, auditory, somatosensory, gustatory, and olfactory) ... Second, rewards are salient and thus elicit attention, which are manifested as orienting responses. The salience of rewards derives from three principal factors, namely, their physical intensity and impact (physical salience), their novelty and surprise (novelty/surprise salience), and their general motivational impact shared with punishers (motivational salience). A separate form not included in this scheme, incentive salience, primarily addresses dopamine function in addiction and refers only to approach behavior (as opposed to learning) ... Third, rewards have a value component that determines the positively motivating effects of rewards and is not contained in, nor explained by, the sensory and attentional components. This component reflects behavioral preferences and thus is subjective and only partially determined by physical parameters. Only this component constitutes what we understand as a reward. It mediates the specific behavioral reinforcing, approach generating, and emotional effects of rewards that are crucial for the organism's survival and reproduction, whereas all other components are only supportive of these functions. ... Rewards can also be intrinsic to behavior. They contrast with extrinsic rewards that provide motivation for behavior and constitute the essence of operant behavior in laboratory tests. Intrinsic rewards are activities that are pleasurable on their own and are undertaken for their own sake, without being the means for getting extrinsic rewards. ... Intrinsic rewards are genuine rewards in their own right, as they induce learning, approach, and pleasure, like perfectioning, playing, and enjoying the piano. Although they can serve to condition higher order rewards, they are not conditioned, higher order rewards, as attaining their reward properties does not require pairing with an unconditioned reward. ... These emotions are also called liking (for pleasure) and wanting (for desire) in addiction research and strongly support the learning and approach generating functions of reward.
  4. ^ Guo, Rong; Böhmer, Wendelin; Hebart, Martin; Chien, Samson; Sommer, Tobias; Obermayer, Klaus; Gläscher, Jan (14 December 2016). "Interaction of Instrumental and Goal-Directed Learning Modulates Prediction Error Representations in the Ventral Striatum". The Journal of Neuroscience. Society for Neuroscience. 36 (50): 12650–12660. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.1677-16.2016. ISSN 0270-6474. PMC 6705659. PMID 27974615.
  5. ^ Malenka RC, Nestler EJ, Hyman SE (2009). "Chapter 15: Reinforcement and Addictive Disorders". In Sydor A, Brown RY (eds.). Molecular Neuropharmacology: A Foundation for Clinical Neuroscience (2nd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill Medical. pp. 364–375. ISBN 9780071481274.
  6. ^ a b c Nestler EJ (December 2013). "Cellular basis of memory for addiction". Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience. 15 (4): 431–443. PMC 3898681. PMID 24459410. Despite the importance of numerous psychosocial factors, at its core, drug addiction involves a biological process: the ability of repeated exposure to a drug of abuse to induce changes in a vulnerable brain that drive the compulsive seeking and taking of drugs, and loss of control over drug use, that define a state of addiction. ... A large body of literature has demonstrated that such ΔFosB induction in D1-type [nucleus accumbens] neurons increases an animal's sensitivity to drug as well as natural rewards and promotes drug self-administration, presumably through a process of positive reinforcement ... Another ΔFosB target is cFos: as ΔFosB accumulates with repeated drug exposure it represses c-Fos and contributes to the molecular switch whereby ΔFosB is selectively induced in the chronic drug-treated state.41. ... Moreover, there is increasing evidence that, despite a range of genetic risks for addiction across the population, exposure to sufficiently high doses of a drug for long periods of time can transform someone who has relatively lower genetic loading into an addict.
  7. ^ "Glossary of Terms". Mount Sinai School of Medicine. Department of Neuroscience. Retrieved 9 February 2015.
  8. ^ Volkow ND, Koob GF, McLellan AT (January 2016). "Neurobiologic Advances from the Brain Disease Model of Addiction". New England Journal of Medicine. 374 (4): 363–371. doi:10.1056/NEJMra1511480. PMC 6135257. PMID 26816013. Substance-use disorder: A diagnostic term in the fifth edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5) referring to recurrent use of alcohol or other drugs that causes clinically and functionally significant impairment, such as health problems, disability, and failure to meet major responsibilities at work, school, or home. Depending on the level of severity, this disorder is classified as mild, moderate, or severe.
    Addiction: A term used to indicate the most severe, chronic stage of substance-use disorder, in which there is a substantial loss of self-control, as indicated by compulsive drug taking despite the desire to stop taking the drug. In the DSM-5, the term addiction is synonymous with the classification of severe substance-use disorder.
  9. ^ a b c d e Kolb, Bryan; Whishaw, Ian Q. (2001). An Introduction to Brain and Behavior (1st ed.). New York: Worth. pp. 438–441. ISBN 9780716751694.
  10. ^ Brain & Behavior Research Foundation (13 March 2019). "The Biology of Addiction". YouTube.{{cite web}}: CS1 maint: url-status (link)
  11. ^ . Medical News Today. 15 January 2008. Archived from the original on 23 September 2010. Retrieved 14 November 2010.
  12. ^ Duarte, Isabel C.; Afonso, Sónia; Jorge, Helena; Cayolla, Ricardo; Ferreira, Carlos; Castelo-Branco, Miguel (1 May 2017). "Tribal love: the neural correlates of passionate engagement in football fans". Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 12 (5): 718–728. doi:10.1093/scan/nsx003. PMC 5460049. PMID 28338882.
  13. ^ a b c Salamone, John D.; Correa, Mercè (November 2012). "The Mysterious Motivational Functions of Mesolimbic Dopamine". Neuron. 76 (3): 470–485. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2012.10.021. PMC 4450094. PMID 23141060.
  14. ^ a b c d e f g Yager LM, Garcia AF, Wunsch AM, Ferguson SM (August 2015). "The ins and outs of the striatum: Role in drug addiction". Neuroscience. 301: 529–541. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.06.033. PMC 4523218. PMID 26116518. [The striatum] receives dopaminergic inputs from the ventral tegmental area (VTA) and the substantia nigra (SNr) and glutamatergic inputs from several areas, including the cortex, hippocampus, amygdala, and thalamus (Swanson, 1982; Phillipson and Griffiths, 1985; Finch, 1996; Groenewegen et al., 1999; Britt et al., 2012). These glutamatergic inputs make contact on the heads of dendritic spines of the striatal GABAergic medium spiny projection neurons (MSNs) whereas dopaminergic inputs synapse onto the spine neck, allowing for an important and complex interaction between these two inputs in modulation of MSN activity ... It should also be noted that there is a small population of neurons in the [nucleus accumbens] NAc that coexpress both D1 and D2 receptors, though this is largely restricted to the NAc shell (Bertran- Gonzalez et al., 2008). ... Neurons in the NAc core and NAc shell subdivisions also differ functionally. The NAc core is involved in the processing of conditioned stimuli whereas the NAc shell is more important in the processing of unconditioned stimuli; Classically, these two striatal MSN populations are thought to have opposing effects on basal ganglia output. Activation of the dMSNs causes a net excitation of the thalamus resulting in a positive cortical feedback loop; thereby acting as a 'go' signal to initiate behavior. Activation of the iMSNs, however, causes a net inhibition of thalamic activity resulting in a negative cortical feedback loop and therefore serves as a 'brake' to inhibit behavior ... there is also mounting evidence that iMSNs play a role in motivation and addiction (Lobo and Nestler, 2011; Grueter et al., 2013). For example, optogenetic activation of NAc core and shell iMSNs suppressed the development of a cocaine CPP whereas selective ablation of NAc core and shell iMSNs ... enhanced the development and the persistence of an amphetamine CPP (Durieux et al., 2009; Lobo et al., 2010). These findings suggest that iMSNs can bidirectionally modulate drug reward. ... Together these data suggest that iMSNs normally act to restrain drug-taking behavior and recruitment of these neurons may in fact be protective against the development of compulsive drug use.
  15. ^ Taylor SB, Lewis CR, Olive MF (2013). "The neurocircuitry of illicit psychostimulant addiction: acute and chronic effects in humans". Subst Abuse Rehabil. 4: 29–43. doi:10.2147/SAR.S39684. PMC 3931688. PMID 24648786. Regions of the basal ganglia, which include the dorsal and ventral striatum, internal and external segments of the globus pallidus, subthalamic nucleus, and dopaminergic cell bodies in the substantia nigra, are highly implicated not only in fine motor control but also in [prefrontal cortex] PFC function.43 Of these regions, the [nucleus accumbens] NAc (described above) and the [dorsal striatum] DS (described below) are most frequently examined with respect to addiction. Thus, only a brief description of the modulatory role of the basal ganglia in addiction-relevant circuits will be mentioned here. The overall output of the basal ganglia is predominantly via the thalamus, which then projects back to the PFC to form cortico-striatal-thalamo-cortical (CSTC) loops. Three CSTC loops are proposed to modulate executive function, action selection, and behavioral inhibition. In the dorsolateral prefrontal circuit, the basal ganglia primarily modulate the identification and selection of goals, including rewards.44 The [orbitofrontal cortex] OFC circuit modulates decision-making and impulsivity, and the anterior cingulate circuit modulates the assessment of consequences.44 These circuits are modulated by dopaminergic inputs from the [ventral tegmental area] VTA to ultimately guide behaviors relevant to addiction, including the persistence and narrowing of the behavioral repertoire toward drug seeking, and continued drug use despite negative consequences.43–45
  16. ^ Grall-Bronnec M, Sauvaget A (2014). "The use of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation for modulating craving and addictive behaviours: a critical literature review of efficacy, technical and methodological considerations". Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 47: 592–613. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.10.013. PMID 25454360. Studies have shown that cravings are underpinned by activation of the reward and motivation circuits (McBride et al., 2006, Wang et al., 2007, Wing et al., 2012, Goldman et al., 2013, Jansen et al., 2013 and Volkow et al., 2013). According to these authors, the main neural structures involved are: the nucleus accumbens, dorsal striatum, orbitofrontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), amygdala, hippocampus and insula.
  17. ^ a b Malenka RC, Nestler EJ, Hyman SE (2009). Sydor A, Brown RY (eds.). Molecular Neuropharmacology: A Foundation for Clinical Neuroscience (2nd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill Medical. pp. 365–366, 376. ISBN 978-0-07-148127-4. The neural substrates that underlie the perception of reward and the phenomenon of positive reinforcement are a set of interconnected forebrain structures called brain reward pathways; these include the nucleus accumbens (NAc; the major component of the ventral striatum), the basal forebrain (components of which have been termed the extended amygdala, as discussed later in this chapter), hippocampus, hypothalamus, and frontal regions of cerebral cortex. These structures receive rich dopaminergic innervation from the ventral tegmental area (VTA) of the midbrain. Addictive drugs are rewarding and reinforcing because they act in brain reward pathways to enhance either dopamine release or the effects of dopamine in the NAc or related structures, or because they produce effects similar to dopamine. ... A macrostructure postulated to integrate many of the functions of this circuit is described by some investigators as the extended amygdala. The extended amygdala is said to comprise several basal forebrain structures that share similar morphology, immunocytochemical features, and connectivity and that are well suited to mediating aspects of reward function; these include the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis, the central medial amygdala, the shell of the NAc, and the sublenticular substantia innominata.
  18. ^ a b Richard JM, Castro DC, Difeliceantonio AG, Robinson MJ, Berridge KC (November 2013). "Mapping brain circuits of reward and motivation: in the footsteps of Ann Kelley". Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37 (9 Pt A): 1919–1931. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.12.008. PMC 3706488. PMID 23261404.
    Figure 3: Neural circuits underlying motivated 'wanting' and hedonic 'liking'.
  19. ^ Luo M, Zhou J, Liu Z (August 2015). "Reward processing by the dorsal raphe nucleus: 5-HT and beyond". Learn. Mem. 22 (9): 452–460. doi:10.1101/lm.037317.114. PMC 4561406. PMID 26286655.
  20. ^ Moulton EA, Elman I, Becerra LR, Goldstein RZ, Borsook D (May 2014). "The cerebellum and addiction: insights gained from neuroimaging research". Addiction Biology. 19 (3): 317–331. doi:10.1111/adb.12101. PMC 4031616. PMID 24851284.
  21. ^ Caligiore D, Pezzulo G, Baldassarre G, Bostan AC, Strick PL, Doya K, Helmich RC, Dirkx M, Houk J, Jörntell H, Lago-Rodriguez A, Galea JM, Miall RC, Popa T, Kishore A, Verschure PF, Zucca R, Herreros I (February 2017). "Consensus Paper: Towards a Systems-Level View of Cerebellar Function: the Interplay Between Cerebellum, Basal Ganglia, and Cortex". Cerebellum. 16 (1): 203–229. doi:10.1007/s12311-016-0763-3. PMC 5243918. PMID 26873754.
  22. ^ Ogawa, SK; Watabe-Uchida, M (2018). "Organization of dopamine and serotonin system: Anatomical and functional mapping of monosynaptic inputs using rabies virus". Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior. 174: 9–22. doi:10.1016/j.pbb.2017.05.001. PMID 28476484. S2CID 5089422.
  23. ^ Morales, M; Margolis, EB (February 2017). "Ventral tegmental area: cellular heterogeneity, connectivity and behaviour". Nature Reviews. Neuroscience. 18 (2): 73–85. doi:10.1038/nrn.2016.165. PMID 28053327. S2CID 10311562.
  24. ^ Lammel, S; Lim, BK; Malenka, RC (January 2014). "Reward and aversion in a heterogeneous midbrain dopamine system". Neuropharmacology. 76 Pt B (0): 351–9. doi:10.1016/j.neuropharm.2013.03.019. PMC 3778102. PMID 23578393.
  25. ^ Nieh, EH; Kim, SY; Namburi, P; Tye, KM (20 May 2013). "Optogenetic dissection of neural circuits underlying emotional valence and motivated behaviors". Brain Research. 1511: 73–92. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2012.11.001. hdl:1721.1/92890. PMC 4099056. PMID 23142759.
  26. ^ Trantham-Davidson H, Neely LC, Lavin A, Seamans JK (2004). "Mechanisms underlying differential D1 versus D2 dopamine receptor regulation of inhibition in prefrontal cortex". The Journal of Neuroscience. 24 (47): 10652–10659. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.3179-04.2004. PMC 5509068. PMID 15564581.
  27. ^ You ZB, Chen YQ, Wise RA (2001). "Dopamine and glutamate release in the nucleus accumbens and ventral tegmental area of rat following lateral hypothalamic self-stimulation". Neuroscience. 107 (4): 629–639. doi:10.1016/s0306-4522(01)00379-7. PMID 11720786. S2CID 33615497.
  28. ^ a b Castro, DC; Cole, SL; Berridge, KC (2015). "Lateral hypothalamus, nucleus accumbens, and ventral pallidum roles in eating and hunger: interactions between homeostatic and reward circuitry". Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience. 9: 90. doi:10.3389/fnsys.2015.00090. PMC 4466441. PMID 26124708.
  29. ^ Carlezon WA, Jr; Thomas, MJ (2009). "Biological substrates of reward and aversion: a nucleus accumbens activity hypothesis". Neuropharmacology. 56 Suppl 1 (Suppl 1): 122–32. doi:10.1016/j.neuropharm.2008.06.075. PMC 2635333. PMID 18675281.
  30. ^ Wise RA, Rompre PP (1989). "Brain dopamine and reward". Annual Review of Psychology. 40: 191–225. doi:10.1146/annurev.ps.40.020189.001203. PMID 2648975.
  31. ^ Wise RA (October 2002). "Brain reward circuitry: insights from unsensed incentives". Neuron. 36 (2): 229–240. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00965-0. PMID 12383779. S2CID 16547037.
  32. ^ a b Kokane, S. S., & Perrotti, L. I. (2020). Sex Differences and the Role of Estradiol in Mesolimbic Reward Circuits and Vulnerability to Cocaine and Opiate Addiction. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 14.
  33. ^ Becker, J. B., & Chartoff, E. (2019). Sex differences in neural mechanisms mediating reward and addiction. Neuropsychopharmacology, 44(1), 166-183.
  34. ^ Stoof, J. C., & Kebabian, J. W. (1984). Two dopamine receptors: biochemistry, physiology and pharmacology. Life sciences, 35(23), 2281-2296.
  35. ^ Yin, H. H., Knowlton, B. J., & Balleine, B. W. (2005). Blockade of NMDA receptors in the dorsomedial striatum prevents action–outcome learning in instrumental conditioning. European Journal of Neuroscience, 22(2), 505-512.
  36. ^ a b c Koob, G. F., & Volkow, N. D. (2016). Neurobiology of addiction: a neurocircuitry analysis. The Lancet Psychiatry, 3(8), 760-773.
  37. ^ Kutlu, M. G., & Gould, T. J. (2016). Effects of drugs of abuse on hippocampal plasticity and hippocampus-dependent learning and memory: contributions to development and maintenance of addiction. Learning & memory, 23(10), 515-533.
  38. ^ McGaugh, J. L. (July 2004). "The amygdala modulates the consolidation of memories of emotionally arousing experiences". Annual Review of Neuroscience. 27 (1): 1–28.
  39. ^ Koob G. F., Le Moal M. (2008). Addiction and the brain antireward system. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 59 29–53. 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093548 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar] Koob G. F., Sanna P. P., Bloom F. E. (1998). Neuroscience of addiction. Neuron 21 467–476.
  40. ^ a b c Castro, DC; Berridge, KC (24 October 2017). "Opioid and orexin hedonic hotspots in rat orbitofrontal cortex and insula". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 114 (43): E9125–E9134. Bibcode:2017PNAS..114E9125C. doi:10.1073/pnas.1705753114. PMC 5664503. PMID 29073109. Here, we show that opioid or orexin stimulations in orbitofrontal cortex and insula causally enhance hedonic "liking" reactions to sweetness and find a third cortical site where the same neurochemical stimulations reduce positive hedonic impact.
  41. ^ Kringelbach ML, Berridge KC (2012). (PDF). Scientific American. 307 (2): 44–45. Bibcode:2012SciAm.307b..40K. doi:10.1038/scientificamerican0812-40. PMID 22844850. Archived from the original (PDF) on 29 March 2017. Retrieved 17 January 2017. So it makes sense that the real pleasure centers in the brain – those directly responsible for generating pleasurable sensations – turn out to lie within some of the structures previously identified as part of the reward circuit. One of these so-called hedonic hotspots lies in a subregion of the nucleus accumbens called the medial shell. A second is found within the ventral pallidum, a deep-seated structure near the base of the forebrain that receives most of its signals from the nucleus accumbens. ...
         On the other hand, intense euphoria is harder to come by than everyday pleasures. The reason may be that strong enhancement of pleasure – like the chemically induced pleasure bump we produced in lab animals – seems to require activation of the entire network at once. Defection of any single component dampens the high.
         Whether the pleasure circuit – and in particular, the ventral pallidum – works the same way in humans is unclear.
  42. ^ a b Berridge KC (April 2012). "From prediction error to incentive salience: mesolimbic computation of reward motivation". Eur. J. Neurosci. 35 (7): 1124–1143. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2012.07990.x. PMC 3325516. PMID 22487042. Here I discuss how mesocorticolimbic mechanisms generate the motivation component of incentive salience. Incentive salience takes Pavlovian learning and memory as one input and as an equally important input takes neurobiological state factors (e.g. drug states, appetite states, satiety states) that can vary independently of learning. Neurobiological state changes can produce unlearned fluctuations or even reversals in the ability of a previously learned reward cue to trigger motivation. Such fluctuations in cue-triggered motivation can dramatically depart from all previously learned values about the associated reward outcome. ... Associative learning and prediction are important contributors to motivation for rewards. Learning gives incentive value to arbitrary cues such as a Pavlovian conditioned stimulus (CS) that is associated with a reward (unconditioned stimulus or UCS). Learned cues for reward are often potent triggers of desires. For example, learned cues can trigger normal appetites in everyone, and can sometimes trigger compulsive urges and relapse in addicts.
    Cue-triggered 'wanting' for the UCS
    A brief CS encounter (or brief UCS encounter) often primes a pulse of elevated motivation to obtain and consume more reward UCS. This is a signature feature of incentive salience.
    Cue as attractive motivational magnets
    When a Pavlovian CS+ is attributed with incentive salience it not only triggers 'wanting' for its UCS, but often the cue itself becomes highly attractive – even to an irrational degree. This cue attraction is another signature feature of incentive salience ... Two recognizable features of incentive salience are often visible that can be used in neuroscience experiments: (i) UCS-directed 'wanting' – CS-triggered pulses of intensified 'wanting' for the UCS reward; and (ii) CS-directed 'wanting' – motivated attraction to the Pavlovian cue, which makes the arbitrary CS stimulus into a motivational magnet.
  43. ^ Malenka RC, Nestler EJ, Hyman SE (2009). Sydor A, Brown RY (eds.). Molecular Neuropharmacology: A Foundation for Clinical Neuroscience (2nd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill Medical. pp. 147–148, 367, 376. ISBN 978-0-07-148127-4. VTA DA neurons play a critical role in motivation, reward-related behavior (Chapter 15), attention, and multiple forms of memory. This organization of the DA system, wide projection from a limited number of cell bodies, permits coordinated responses to potent new rewards. Thus, acting in diverse terminal fields, dopamine confers motivational salience ("wanting") on the reward itself or associated cues (nucleus accumbens shell region), updates the value placed on different goals in light of this new experience (orbital prefrontal cortex), helps consolidate multiple forms of memory (amygdala and hippocampus), and encodes new motor programs that will facilitate obtaining this reward in the future (nucleus accumbens core region and dorsal striatum). In this example, dopamine modulates the processing of sensorimotor information in diverse neural circuits to maximize the ability of the organism to obtain future rewards. ...
    The brain reward circuitry that is targeted by addictive drugs normally mediates the pleasure and strengthening of behaviors associated with natural reinforcers, such as food, water, and sexual contact. Dopamine neurons in the VTA are activated by food and water, and dopamine release in the NAc is stimulated by the presence of natural reinforcers, such as food, water, or a sexual partner. ...
    The NAc and VTA are central components of the circuitry underlying reward and memory of reward. As previously mentioned, the activity of dopaminergic neurons in the VTA appears to be linked to reward prediction. The NAc is involved in learning associated with reinforcement and the modulation of motoric responses to stimuli that satisfy internal homeostatic needs. The shell of the NAc appears to be particularly important to initial drug actions within reward circuitry; addictive drugs appear to have a greater effect on dopamine release in the shell than in the core of the NAc.
  44. ^ Berridge KC, Kringelbach ML (1 June 2013). "Neuroscience of affect: brain mechanisms of pleasure and displeasure". Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 23 (3): 294–303. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2013.01.017. PMC 3644539. PMID 23375169. For instance, mesolimbic dopamine, probably the most popular brain neurotransmitter candidate for pleasure two decades ago, turns out not to cause pleasure or liking at all. Rather dopamine more selectively mediates a motivational process of incentive salience, which is a mechanism for wanting rewards but not for liking them .... Rather opioid stimulation has the special capacity to enhance liking only if the stimulation occurs within an anatomical hotspot
  45. ^ Calipari, Erin S.; Bagot, Rosemary C.; Purushothaman, Immanuel; Davidson, Thomas J.; Yorgason, Jordan T.; Peña, Catherine J.; Walker, Deena M.; Pirpinias, Stephen T.; Guise, Kevin G.; Ramakrishnan, Charu; Deisseroth, Karl; Nestler, Eric J. (8 March 2016). "In vivo imaging identifies temporal signature of D1 and D2 medium spiny neurons in cocaine reward". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 113 (10): 2726–2731. Bibcode:2016PNAS..113.2726C. doi:10.1073/pnas.1521238113. PMC 4791010. PMID 26831103.
  46. ^ Baliki, M. N.; Mansour, A.; Baria, A. T.; Huang, L.; Berger, S. E.; Fields, H. L.; Apkarian, A. V. (9 October 2013). "Parceling Human Accumbens into Putative Core and Shell Dissociates Encoding of Values for Reward and Pain". Journal of Neuroscience. 33 (41): 16383–16393. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1731-13.2013. PMC 3792469. PMID 24107968.
  47. ^ Soares-Cunha, Carina; Coimbra, Barbara; Sousa, Nuno; Rodrigues, Ana J. (September 2016). "Reappraising striatal D1- and D2-neurons in reward and aversion". Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 68: 370–386. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.05.021. hdl:1822/47044. PMID 27235078. S2CID 207092810.
  48. ^ Bamford, Nigel S.; Wightman, R. Mark; Sulzer, David (February 2018). "Dopamine's Effects on Corticostriatal Synapses during Reward-Based Behaviors". Neuron. 97 (3): 494–510. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2018.01.006. PMC 5808590. PMID 29420932.
  49. ^ Soares-Cunha, Carina; Coimbra, Barbara; David-Pereira, Ana; Borges, Sonia; Pinto, Luisa; Costa, Patricio; Sousa, Nuno; Rodrigues, Ana J. (September 2016). "Activation of D2 dopamine receptor-expressing neurons in the nucleus accumbens increases motivation". Nature Communications. 7 (1): 11829. Bibcode:2016NatCo...711829S. doi:10.1038/ncomms11829. PMC 4931006. PMID 27337658.
  50. ^ Soares-Cunha, Carina; Coimbra, Bárbara; Domingues, Ana Verónica; Vasconcelos, Nivaldo; Sousa, Nuno; Rodrigues, Ana João (March 2018). "Nucleus Accumbens Microcircuit Underlying D2-MSN-Driven Increase in Motivation". eNeuro. 5 (2): ENEURO.0386–18.2018. doi:10.1523/ENEURO.0386-18.2018. PMC 5957524. PMID 29780881.
  51. ^ Koob G. F., Le Moal M. (2008). Addiction and the brain antireward system. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 59 29–53. 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093548 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar] Koob G. F., Sanna P. P., Bloom F. E. (1998). Neuroscience of addiction. Neuron 21 467–476
  52. ^ Meyer, J. S., & Quenzer, L. F. (2013). Psychopharmacology: Drugs, the brain, and behavior. Sinauer Associates.
  53. ^ a b Yin, HH; Ostlund, SB; Balleine, BW (October 2008). "Reward-guided learning beyond dopamine in the nucleus accumbens: the integrative functions of cortico-basal ganglia networks". The European Journal of Neuroscience. 28 (8): 1437–48. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06422.x. PMC 2756656. PMID 18793321.
  54. ^ Dayan, P; Berridge, KC (June 2014). "Model-based and model-free Pavlovian reward learning: revaluation, revision, and revelation". Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience. 14 (2): 473–92. doi:10.3758/s13415-014-0277-8. PMC 4074442. PMID 24647659.
  55. ^ Balleine, BW; Morris, RW; Leung, BK (2 December 2015). "Thalamocortical integration of instrumental learning and performance and their disintegration in addiction". Brain Research. 1628 (Pt A): 104–16. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2014.12.023. PMID 25514336. S2CID 11776683. Importantly, we found evidence of increased activity in the direct pathway; both intracellular changes in the expression of the plasticity marker pERK and AMPA/NMDA ratios evoked by stimulating cortical afferents were increased in the D1-direct pathway neurons. In contrast, D2 neurons showed an opposing change in plasticity; stimulation of cortical afferents reduced AMPA/NMDA ratios on those neurons (Shan et al., 2014).
  56. ^ Nakanishi, S; Hikida, T; Yawata, S (12 December 2014). "Distinct dopaminergic control of the direct and indirect pathways in reward-based and avoidance learning behaviors". Neuroscience. 282: 49–59. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.04.026. PMID 24769227. S2CID 21652525.
  57. ^ Shiflett, MW; Balleine, BW (15 September 2011). "Molecular substrates of action control in cortico-striatal circuits". Progress in Neurobiology. 95 (1): 1–13. doi:10.1016/j.pneurobio.2011.05.007. PMC 3175490. PMID 21704115.
  58. ^ Schultz, W (April 2013). "Updating dopamine reward signals". Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 23 (2): 229–38. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2012.11.012. PMC 3866681. PMID 23267662.
  59. ^ Shiflett, MW; Balleine, BW (17 March 2011). "Contributions of ERK signaling in the striatum to instrumental learning and performance". Behavioural Brain Research. 218 (1): 240–7. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2010.12.010. PMC 3022085. PMID 21147168.
  60. ^ a b Ruffle JK (November 2014). "Molecular neurobiology of addiction: what's all the (Δ)FosB about?". Am. J. Drug Alcohol Abuse. 40 (6): 428–437. doi:10.3109/00952990.2014.933840. PMID 25083822. S2CID 19157711.
    The strong correlation between chronic drug exposure and ΔFosB provides novel opportunities for targeted therapies in addiction (118), and suggests methods to analyze their efficacy (119). Over the past two decades, research has progressed from identifying ΔFosB induction to investigating its subsequent action (38). It is likely that ΔFosB research will now progress into a new era – the use of ΔFosB as a biomarker. ...
    Conclusions
    ΔFosB is an essential transcription factor implicated in the molecular and behavioral pathways of addiction following repeated drug exposure. The formation of ΔFosB in multiple brain regions, and the molecular pathway leading to the formation of AP-1 complexes is well understood. The establishment of a functional purpose for ΔFosB has allowed further determination as to some of the key aspects of its molecular cascades, involving effectors such as GluR2 (87,88), Cdk5 (93) and NFkB (100). Moreover, many of these molecular changes identified are now directly linked to the structural, physiological and behavioral changes observed following chronic drug exposure (60,95,97,102). New frontiers of research investigating the molecular roles of ΔFosB have been opened by epigenetic studies, and recent advances have illustrated the role of ΔFosB acting on DNA and histones, truly as a molecular switch (34). As a consequence of our improved understanding of ΔFosB in addiction, it is possible to evaluate the addictive potential of current medications (119), as well as use it as a biomarker for assessing the efficacy of therapeutic interventions (121,122,124). Some of these proposed interventions have limitations (125) or are in their infancy (75). However, it is hoped that some of these preliminary findings may lead to innovative treatments, which are much needed in addiction.
  61. ^ a b Olsen CM (December 2011). "Natural rewards, neuroplasticity, and non-drug addictions". Neuropharmacology. 61 (7): 1109–1122. doi:10.1016/j.neuropharm.2011.03.010. PMC 3139704. PMID 21459101. Functional neuroimaging studies in humans have shown that gambling (Breiter et al, 2001), shopping (Knutson et al, 2007), orgasm (Komisaruk et al, 2004), playing video games (Koepp et al, 1998; Hoeft et al, 2008) and the sight of appetizing food (Wang et al, 2004a) activate many of the same brain regions (i.e., the mesocorticolimbic system and extended amygdala) as drugs of abuse (Volkow et al, 2004). ... Cross-sensitization is also bidirectional, as a history of amphetamine administration facilitates sexual behavior and enhances the associated increase in NAc DA ... As described for food reward, sexual experience can also lead to activation of plasticity-related signaling cascades. The transcription factor delta FosB is increased in the NAc, PFC, dorsal striatum, and VTA following repeated sexual behavior (Wallace et al., 2008; Pitchers et al., 2010b). This natural increase in delta FosB or viral overexpression of delta FosB within the NAc modulates sexual performance, and NAc blockade of delta FosB attenuates this behavior (Hedges et al, 2009; Pitchers et al., 2010b). Further, viral overexpression of delta FosB enhances the conditioned place preference for an environment paired with sexual experience (Hedges et al., 2009). ... In some people, there is a transition from "normal" to compulsive engagement in natural rewards (such as food or sex), a condition that some have termed behavioral or non-drug addictions (Holden, 2001; Grant et al., 2006a). ... In humans, the role of dopamine signaling in incentive-sensitization processes has recently been highlighted by the observation of a dopamine dysregulation syndrome in some patients taking dopaminergic drugs. This syndrome is characterized by a medication-induced increase in (or compulsive) engagement in non-drug rewards such as gambling, shopping, or sex (Evans et al, 2006; Aiken, 2007; Lader, 2008)."
    Table 1: Summary of plasticity observed following exposure to drug or natural reinforcers"
  62. ^ a b Biliński P, Wojtyła A, Kapka-Skrzypczak L, Chwedorowicz R, Cyranka M, Studziński T (2012). "Epigenetic regulation in drug addiction". Ann. Agric. Environ. Med. 19 (3): 491–496. PMID 23020045. For these reasons, ΔFosB is considered a primary and causative transcription factor in creating new neural connections in the reward centre, prefrontal cortex, and other regions of the limbic system. This is reflected in the increased, stable and long-lasting level of sensitivity to cocaine and other drugs, and tendency to relapse even after long periods of abstinence. These newly constructed networks function very efficiently via new pathways as soon as drugs of abuse are further taken ... In this way, the induction of CDK5 gene expression occurs together with suppression of the G9A gene coding for dimethyltransferase acting on the histone H3. A feedback mechanism can be observed in the regulation of these 2 crucial factors that determine the adaptive epigenetic response to cocaine. This depends on ΔFosB inhibiting G9a gene expression, i.e. H3K9me2 synthesis which in turn inhibits transcription factors for ΔFosB. For this reason, the observed hyper-expression of G9a, which ensures high levels of the dimethylated form of histone H3, eliminates the neuronal structural and plasticity effects caused by cocaine by means of this feedback which blocks ΔFosB transcription
  63. ^ Pitchers KK, Vialou V, Nestler EJ, Laviolette SR, Lehman MN, Coolen LM (February 2013). "Natural and drug rewards act on common neural plasticity mechanisms with ΔFosB as a key mediator". The Journal of Neuroscience. 33 (8): 3434–3442. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4881-12.2013. PMC 3865508. PMID 23426671. Drugs of abuse induce neuroplasticity in the natural reward pathway, specifically the nucleus accumbens (NAc), thereby causing development and expression of addictive behavior. ... Together, these findings demonstrate that drugs of abuse and natural reward behaviors act on common molecular and cellular mechanisms of plasticity that control vulnerability to drug addiction, and that this increased vulnerability is mediated by ΔFosB and its downstream transcriptional targets. ... Sexual behavior is highly rewarding (Tenk et al., 2009), and sexual experience causes sensitized drug-related behaviors, including cross-sensitization to amphetamine (Amph)-induced locomotor activity (Bradley and Meisel, 2001; Pitchers et al., 2010a) and enhanced Amph reward (Pitchers et al., 2010a). Moreover, sexual experience induces neural plasticity in the NAc similar to that induced by psychostimulant exposure, including increased dendritic spine density (Meisel and Mullins, 2006; Pitchers et al., 2010a), altered glutamate receptor trafficking, and decreased synaptic strength in prefrontal cortex-responding NAc shell neurons (Pitchers et al., 2012). Finally, periods of abstinence from sexual experience were found to be critical for enhanced Amph reward, NAc spinogenesis (Pitchers et al., 2010a), and glutamate receptor trafficking (Pitchers et al., 2012). These findings suggest that natural and drug reward experiences share common mechanisms of neural plasticity
  64. ^ Beloate LN, Weems PW, Casey GR, Webb IC, Coolen LM (February 2016). "Nucleus accumbens NMDA receptor activation regulates amphetamine cross-sensitization and deltaFosB expression following sexual experience in male rats". Neuropharmacology. 101: 154–164. doi:10.1016/j.neuropharm.2015.09.023. PMID 26391065. S2CID 25317397.
  65. ^ Robison AJ, Nestler EJ (November 2011). "Transcriptional and epigenetic mechanisms of addiction". Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 12 (11): 623–637. doi:10.1038/nrn3111. PMC 3272277. PMID 21989194. ΔFosB serves as one of the master control proteins governing this structural plasticity. ... ΔFosB also represses G9a expression, leading to reduced repressive histone methylation at the cdk5 gene. The net result is gene activation and increased CDK5 expression. ... In contrast, ΔFosB binds to the c-fos gene and recruits several co-repressors, including HDAC1 (histone deacetylase 1) and SIRT 1 (sirtuin 1). ... The net result is c-fos gene repression.
    Figure 4: Epigenetic basis of drug regulation of gene expression
  66. ^ Hitchcock LN, Lattal KM (2014). "Histone-mediated epigenetics in addiction". Epigenetics and Neuroplasticity—Evidence and Debate. Prog Mol Biol Transl Sci. Progress in Molecular Biology and Translational Science. Vol. 128. pp. 51–87. doi:10.1016/B978-0-12-800977-2.00003-6. ISBN 9780128009772. PMC 5914502. PMID 25410541.
  67. ^ Walker DM, Nestler EJ (2018). "Neuroepigenetics and addiction". Neurogenetics, Part II. Handbook of Clinical Neurology. Vol. 148. pp. 747–765. doi:10.1016/B978-0-444-64076-5.00048-X. ISBN 9780444640765. PMC 5868351. PMID 29478612.
  68. ^ Rang HP (2003). Pharmacology. Edinburgh: Churchill Livingstone. p. 596. ISBN 978-0-443-07145-4.
  69. ^ a b Roy A. Wise, Drug-activation of brain reward pathways, Drug and Alcohol Dependence 1998; 51 13–22.
  70. ^ Goeders N.E., Smith J.E. (1983). "Cortical dopaminergic involvement in cocaine reinforcement". Science. 221 (4612): 773–775. Bibcode:1983Sci...221..773G. doi:10.1126/science.6879176. PMID 6879176.
  71. ^ Goeders N.E., Smith J.E. (1993). "Intracranial cocaine self-administration into the medial prefrontal cortex increases dopamine turnover in the nucleus accumbens". J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 265 (2): 592–600. PMID 8496810.
  72. ^ Clarke, Hommer D.W.; Pert A.; Skirboll L.R. (1985). "Electrophysiological actions of nicotine on substantia nigra single units". Br. J. Pharmacol. 85 (4): 827–835. doi:10.1111/j.1476-5381.1985.tb11081.x. PMC 1916681. PMID 4041681.
  73. ^ Westfall, Thomas C.; Grant, Heather; Perry, Holly (January 1983). "Release of dopamine and 5-hydroxytryptamine from rat striatal slices following activation of nicotinic cholinergic receptors". General Pharmacology: The Vascular System. 14 (3): 321–325. doi:10.1016/0306-3623(83)90037-x. PMID 6135645.
  74. ^ Rømer Thomsen, K; Whybrow, PC; Kringelbach, ML (2015). "Reconceptualizing anhedonia: novel perspectives on balancing the pleasure networks in the human brain". Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 9: 49. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00049. PMC 4356228. PMID 25814941.
  75. ^ Thomsen, KR (2015). "Measuring anhedonia: impaired ability to pursue, experience, and learn about reward". Frontiers in Psychology. 6: 1409. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01409. PMC 4585007. PMID 26441781.
  76. ^ Olney, JJ; Warlow, SM; Naffziger, EE; Berridge, KC (August 2018). "Current perspectives on incentive salience and applications to clinical disorders". Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences. 22: 59–69. doi:10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.01.007. PMC 5831552. PMID 29503841.
  77. ^ Preda, Adrian (2014). "Brain Imaging Correlates of Anhedonia". In Ritsner, Michael (ed.). Anhedonia : a comprehensive handbook. Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands. ISBN 978-94-017-8590-7.
  78. ^ Zhang, B; Lin, P; Shi, H; Öngür, D; Auerbach, RP; Wang, X; Yao, S; Wang, X (September 2016). "Mapping anhedonia-specific dysfunction in a transdiagnostic approach: an ALE meta-analysis". Brain Imaging and Behavior. 10 (3): 920–39. doi:10.1007/s11682-015-9457-6. PMC 4838562. PMID 26487590.
  79. ^ Treadway, Michael, T. (2016). "The Neurobiology of Motivational Deficits in Depression— An Update on Candidate Pathomechanisms". In Simpson, Eleanor H.; Balsam, Peter D. (eds.). Behavioral Neuroscience of Motivation (Current Topics in Behavioral Neurosciences) (1st ed.). Switzerland: Springer International Publishing. p. 343. ISBN 978-3-319-26933-7. In a relatively recent literature, studies of motivation and reinforcement in depression have been largely consistent in detecting differences as compared to healthy controls (Whitton et al. 2015). In several studies using the effort expenditure for reward task (EEfRT), patients with MDD expended less effort for rewards when compared with controls (Treadway et al. 2012; Yang et al. 2014)
  80. ^ Salamone, JD; Yohn, SE; López-Cruz, L; San Miguel, N; Correa, M (May 2016). "Activational and effort-related aspects of motivation: neural mechanisms and implications for psychopathology". Brain: A Journal of Neurology. 139 (Pt 5): 1325–47. doi:10.1093/brain/aww050. PMC 5839596. PMID 27189581.
  81. ^ Russo, SJ; Nestler, EJ (September 2013). "The brain reward circuitry in mood disorders". Nature Reviews. Neuroscience. 14 (9): 609–25. doi:10.1038/nrn3381. PMC 3867253. PMID 23942470.
  82. ^ Treadway, MT; Zald, DH (January 2011). "Reconsidering anhedonia in depression: lessons from translational neuroscience". Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 35 (3): 537–55. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.06.006. PMC 3005986. PMID 20603146.
  83. ^ Walsh, JJ; Han, MH (12 December 2014). "The heterogeneity of ventral tegmental area neurons: Projection functions in a mood-related context". Neuroscience. 282: 101–8. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.06.006. PMC 4339667. PMID 24931766.
  84. ^ Lammel, S; Lim, BK; Malenka, RC (January 2014). "Reward and aversion in a heterogeneous midbrain dopamine system". Neuropharmacology. 76 Pt B (0): 351–9. doi:10.1016/j.neuropharm.2013.03.019. PMC 3778102. PMID 23578393.
  85. ^ Knowland, D; Lim, BK (5 January 2018). "Circuit-based frameworks of depressive behaviors: The role of reward circuitry and beyond". Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior. 174: 42–52. doi:10.1016/j.pbb.2017.12.010. PMC 6340396. PMID 29309799.
  86. ^ a b Lammel, S; Tye, KM; Warden, MR (January 2014). "Progress in understanding mood disorders: optogenetic dissection of neural circuits". Genes, Brain and Behavior. 13 (1): 38–51. doi:10.1111/gbb.12049. PMID 23682971. S2CID 18542868.
  87. ^ Bucci, P; Galderisi, S (May 2017). "Categorizing and assessing negative symptoms". Current Opinion in Psychiatry. 30 (3): 201–208. doi:10.1097/YCO.0000000000000322. PMID 28212174. S2CID 9923114. They also provide a separate assessment of the consummatory anhedonia (reduced experience of pleasure derived from ongoing enjoyable activities) and anticipatory anhedonia (reduced ability to anticipate future pleasure). In fact, the former one seems to be relatively intact in schizophrenia, whereas the latter one seems to be impaired [32 – 34]. However, discrepant data have also been reported [35].
  88. ^ Young, Anticevic & Barch 2018, p. 215a,"Several recent reviews (e.g., Cohen and Minor, 2010) have found that individuals with schizophrenia show relatively intact self-reported emotional responses to affect-eliciting stimuli as well as other indicators of intact response...A more mixed picture arises from functional neuroimaging studies examining brain responses to other types of pleasurable stimuli in schizophrenia (Paradiso et al., 2003)"
  89. ^ Young, Anticevic & Barch 2018, p. 215b,"As such it is surprising that behavioral studies have suggested that reinforcement learning is intact in schizophrenia when learning is relatively implicit (though, see Siegert et al., 2008 for evidence of impaired Serial Reaction Time task learning), but more impaired when explicit representations of stimulus-reward contingencies are needed (see Gold et al., 2008). This pattern has given rise to the theory that the striatally mediated gradual reinforcement learning system may be intact in schizophrenia, while more rapid, on-line, cortically mediated learning systems are impaired."
  90. ^ Young, Anticevic & Barch 2018, p. 216, "We have recently shown that individuals with schizophrenia can show improved cognitive control performance when information about rewards are externally presented but not when they must be internally maintained (Mann et al., 2013), with some evidence for impairments in DLPFC and striatal activation during internal maintenance of reward information being associated with individuals’ differences in motivation (Chung and Barch, 2016)."
  91. ^ Littman, Ph.D., Ellen (February 2017). "Never Enough? Why ADHD Brains Crave Stimulation". ADDditude Magazine. New Hope Media LLC. Retrieved 27 May 2021.
  92. ^ Chandler DJ, Waterhouse BD, Gao WJ (May 2014). "New perspectives on catecholaminergic regulation of executive circuits: evidence for independent modulation of prefrontal functions by midbrain dopaminergic and noradrenergic neurons". Frontiers in Neural Circuits. 8: 53. doi:10.3389/fncir.2014.00053. PMC 4033238. PMID 24904299.
  93. ^ Blum, Kenneth; Chen, Amanda Lih-Chuan; Braverman, Eric R; Comings, David E; Chen, Thomas JH; Arcuri, Vanessa; Blum, Seth H; Downs, Bernard W; Waite, Roger L; Notaro, Alison; Lubar, Joel; Williams, Lonna; Prihoda, Thomas J; Palomo, Tomas; Oscar-Berman, Marlene (October 2008). "Attention-deficit-hyperactivity disorder and reward deficiency syndrome". Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment. 4 (5): 893–918. doi:10.2147/ndt.s2627. ISSN 1176-6328. PMC 2626918. PMID 19183781.
  94. ^ Wise RA (1996). "Addictive drugs and brain stimulation reward". Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 19: 319–340. doi:10.1146/annurev.ne.19.030196.001535. PMID 8833446.
  95. ^ a b "human nervous system | Description, Development, Anatomy, & Function". Encyclopedia Britannica.
  96. ^ a b James Olds and Peter Milner (December 1954). . Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology. 47 (6): 419–427. doi:10.1037/h0058775. PMID 13233369. Archived from the original on 5 February 2012. Retrieved 26 April 2011.
  97. ^ Kringelbach, Morten L.; Berridge, Kent C. (25 June 2010). "The Functional Neuroanatomy of Pleasure and Happiness". Discovery Medicine. 9 (49): 579–587. PMC 3008353. PMID 20587348 – via www.discoverymedicine.com.
  98. ^ Ivan Petrovich Pavlov; G. V. Anrep (2003). Conditioned Reflexes. Courier Corporation. pp. 1–. ISBN 978-0-486-43093-5.
  99. ^ Fridlund, Alan and James Kalat. Mind and Brain, the Science of Psychology. California: Cengage Learning, 2014. Print.
  100. ^ a b Berridge, Kent C.; Kringelbach, Morten L. (August 2008). "Affective neuroscience of pleasure: reward in humans and animals". Psychopharmacology. 199 (3): 457–480. doi:10.1007/s00213-008-1099-6. PMC 3004012. PMID 18311558.
  101. ^ a b Ferreri L, Mas-Herrero E, Zatorre RJ, Ripollés P, Gomez-Andres A, Alicart H, Olivé G, Marco-Pallarés J, Antonijoan RM, Valle M, Riba J, Rodriguez-Fornells A (January 2019). "Dopamine modulates the reward experiences elicited by music". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 116 (9): 3793–3798. Bibcode:2019PNAS..116.3793F. doi:10.1073/pnas.1811878116. PMC 6397525. PMID 30670642. Listening to pleasurable music is often accompanied by measurable bodily reactions such as goose bumps or shivers down the spine, commonly called 'chills' or 'frissons.' ... Overall, our results straightforwardly revealed that pharmacological interventions bidirectionally modulated the reward responses elicited by music. In particular, we found that risperidone impaired participants' ability to experience musical pleasure, whereas levodopa enhanced it. ... Here, in contrast, studying responses to abstract rewards in human subjects, we show that manipulation of dopaminergic transmission affects both the pleasure (i.e., amount of time reporting chills and emotional arousal measured by EDA) and the motivational components of musical reward (money willing to spend). These findings suggest that dopaminergic signaling is a sine qua non condition not only for motivational responses, as has been shown with primary and secondary rewards, but also for hedonic reactions to music. This result supports recent findings showing that dopamine also mediates the perceived pleasantness attained by other types of abstract rewards and challenges previous findings in animal models on primary rewards, such as food.
  102. ^ a b Goupil L, Aucouturier JJ (February 2019). "Musical pleasure and musical emotions". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 116 (9): 3364–3366. Bibcode:2019PNAS..116.3364G. doi:10.1073/pnas.1900369116. PMC 6397567. PMID 30770455. In a pharmacological study published in PNAS, Ferreri et al. (1) present evidence that enhancing or inhibiting dopamine signaling using levodopa or risperidone modulates the pleasure experienced while listening to music. ... In a final salvo to establish not only the correlational but also the causal implication of dopamine in musical pleasure, the authors have turned to directly manipulating dopaminergic signaling in the striatum, first by applying excitatory and inhibitory transcranial magnetic stimulation over their participants' left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, a region known to modulate striatal function (5), and finally, in the current study, by administrating pharmaceutical agents able to alter dopamine synaptic availability (1), both of which influenced perceived pleasure, physiological measures of arousal, and the monetary value assigned to music in the predicted direction. ... While the question of the musical expression of emotion has a long history of investigation, including in PNAS (6), and the 1990s psychophysiological strand of research had already established that musical pleasure could activate the autonomic nervous system (7), the authors' demonstration of the implication of the reward system in musical emotions was taken as inaugural proof that these were veridical emotions whose study has full legitimacy to inform the neurobiology of our everyday cognitive, social, and affective functions (8). Incidentally, this line of work, culminating in the article by Ferreri et al. (1), has plausibly done more to attract research funding for the field of music sciences than any other in this community. The evidence of Ferreri et al. (1) provides the latest support for a compelling neurobiological model in which musical pleasure arises from the interaction of ancient reward/valuation systems (striatal–limbic–paralimbic) with more phylogenetically advanced perception/predictions systems (temporofrontal).
  • Young, Jared W.; Anticevic, Alan; Barch, Deanna M. (2018). "Cognitive and Motivational Neuroscience of Psychotic Disorders". In Charney, Dennis S.; Sklar, Pamela; Buxbaum, Joseph D.; Nestler, Eric J. (eds.). Charney & Nestler's Neurobiology of Mental Illness (5th ed.). New York: Oxford University Press. ISBN 9780190681425.

External links

  • Scholarpedia Reward
  • Scholarpedia Reward signals

reward, system, reward, system, mesocorticolimbic, circuit, group, neural, structures, responsible, incentive, salience, wanting, desire, craving, reward, motivation, associative, learning, primarily, positive, reinforcement, classical, conditioning, positivel. The reward system the mesocorticolimbic circuit is a group of neural structures responsible for incentive salience i e wanting desire or craving for a reward and motivation associative learning primarily positive reinforcement and classical conditioning and positively valenced emotions particularly ones involving pleasure as a core component e g joy euphoria and ecstasy 1 2 Reward is the attractive and motivational property of a stimulus that induces appetitive behavior also known as approach behavior and consummatory behavior 1 A rewarding stimulus has been described as any stimulus object event activity or situation that has the potential to make us approach and consume it is by definition a reward 1 In operant conditioning rewarding stimuli function as positive reinforcers 3 however the converse statement also holds true positive reinforcers are rewarding 3 4 Examples of primary rewards 3 Clockwise from top left water food parental care and sex Addiction and dependence glossary 5 6 7 8 addiction a biopsychosocial disorder characterized by persistent use of drugs including alcohol despite substantial harm and adverse consequences addictive drug psychoactive substances that with repeated use are associated with significantly higher rates of substance use disorders due in large part to the drug s effect on brain reward systems dependence an adaptive state associated with a withdrawal syndrome upon cessation of repeated exposure to a stimulus e g drug intake drug sensitization or reverse tolerance the escalating effect of a drug resulting from repeated administration at a given dose drug withdrawal symptoms that occur upon cessation of repeated drug use physical dependence dependence that involves persistent physical somatic withdrawal symptoms e g fatigue and delirium tremens psychological dependence dependence that involves emotional motivational withdrawal symptoms e g dysphoria and anhedonia reinforcing stimuli stimuli that increase the probability of repeating behaviors paired with them rewarding stimuli stimuli that the brain interprets as intrinsically positive and desirable or as something to approach sensitization an amplified response to a stimulus resulting from repeated exposure to it substance use disorder a condition in which the use of substances leads to clinically and functionally significant impairment or distress tolerance the diminishing effect of a drug resulting from repeated administration at a given dosevteThe reward system motivates animals to approach stimuli or engage in behaviour that increases fitness sex energy dense foods etc Survival for most animal species depends upon maximizing contact with beneficial stimuli and minimizing contact with harmful stimuli Reward cognition serves to increase the likelihood of survival and reproduction by causing associative learning eliciting approach and consummatory behavior and triggering positively valenced emotions 3 Thus reward is a mechanism that evolved to help increase the adaptive fitness of animals 9 In drug addiction certain substances over activate the reward circuit leading to compulsive substance seeking behavior resulting from synaptic plasticity in the circuit 10 Primary rewards are a class of rewarding stimuli which facilitate the survival of one s self and offspring and they include homeostatic e g palatable food and reproductive e g sexual contact and parental investment rewards 1 11 Intrinsic rewards are unconditioned rewards that are attractive and motivate behavior because they are inherently pleasurable 1 Extrinsic rewards e g money or seeing one s favorite sports team winning a game are conditioned rewards that are attractive and motivate behavior but are not inherently pleasurable 1 12 Extrinsic rewards derive their motivational value as a result of a learned association i e conditioning with intrinsic rewards 1 Extrinsic rewards may also elicit pleasure e g euphoria from winning a lot of money in a lottery after being classically conditioned with intrinsic rewards 1 Contents 1 Definition 2 Neuroanatomy 2 1 Overview 2 2 Key pathway 2 3 Pleasure centers 3 Wanting and liking 4 Anti reward system 5 Learning 6 Disorders 6 1 Addiction 6 2 Motivation 6 2 1 Mood disorders 6 2 2 Schizophrenia 6 2 3 Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder 7 History 8 Other species 9 See also 10 References 11 External linksDefinition EditIn neuroscience the reward system is a collection of brain structures and neural pathways that are responsible for reward related cognition including associative learning primarily classical conditioning and operant reinforcement incentive salience i e motivation and wanting desire or craving for a reward and positively valenced emotions particularly emotions that involve pleasure i e hedonic liking 3 2 Terms that are commonly used to describe behavior related to the wanting or desire component of reward include appetitive behavior approach behavior preparatory behavior instrumental behavior anticipatory behavior and seeking 13 Terms that are commonly used to describe behavior related to the liking or pleasure component of reward include consummatory behavior and taking behavior 13 The three primary functions of rewards are their capacity to produce associative learning i e classical conditioning and operant reinforcement 3 affect decision making and induce approach behavior via the assignment of motivational salience to rewarding stimuli 3 elicit positively valenced emotions particularly pleasure 3 Neuroanatomy EditOverview Edit The brain structures that compose the reward system are located primarily within the cortico basal ganglia thalamo cortical loop 14 the basal ganglia portion of the loop drives activity within the reward system 14 Most of the pathways that connect structures within the reward system are glutamatergic interneurons GABAergic medium spiny neurons MSNs and dopaminergic projection neurons 14 15 although other types of projection neurons contribute e g orexinergic projection neurons The reward system includes the ventral tegmental area ventral striatum i e the nucleus accumbens and olfactory tubercle dorsal striatum i e the caudate nucleus and putamen substantia nigra i e the pars compacta and pars reticulata prefrontal cortex anterior cingulate cortex insular cortex hippocampus hypothalamus particularly the orexinergic nucleus in the lateral hypothalamus thalamus multiple nuclei subthalamic nucleus globus pallidus both external and internal ventral pallidum parabrachial nucleus amygdala and the remainder of the extended amygdala 2 14 16 17 18 The dorsal raphe nucleus and cerebellum appear to modulate some forms of reward related cognition i e associative learning motivational salience and positive emotions and behaviors as well 19 20 21 The laterodorsal tegmental nucleus LTD pedunculopontine nucleus PPTg and lateral habenula LHb both directly and indirectly via the rostromedial tegmental nucleus are also capable of inducing aversive salience and incentive salience through their projections to the ventral tegmental area VTA 22 The LDT and PPTg both send glutaminergic projections to the VTA that synapse on dopaminergic neurons both of which can produce incentive salience The LHb sends glutaminergic projections the majority of which synapse on GABAergic RMTg neurons that in turn drive inhibition of dopaminergic VTA neurons although some LHb projections terminate on VTA interneurons These LHb projections are activated both by aversive stimuli and by the absence of an expected reward and excitation of the LHb can induce aversion 23 24 25 Most of the dopamine pathways i e neurons that use the neurotransmitter dopamine to communicate with other neurons that project out of the ventral tegmental area are part of the reward system 14 in these pathways dopamine acts on D1 like receptors or D2 like receptors to either stimulate D1 like or inhibit D2 like the production of cAMP 26 The GABAergic medium spiny neurons of the striatum are components of the reward system as well 14 The glutamatergic projection nuclei in the subthalamic nucleus prefrontal cortex hippocampus thalamus and amygdala connect to other parts of the reward system via glutamate pathways 14 The medial forebrain bundle which is a set of many neural pathways that mediate brain stimulation reward i e reward derived from direct electrochemical stimulation of the lateral hypothalamus is also a component of the reward system 27 Two theories exist with regard to the activity of the nucleus accumbens and the generation liking and wanting The inhibition or hyper polar ization hypothesis proposes that the nucleus accumbens exerts tonic inhibitory effects on downstream structures such as the ventral pallidum hypothalamus or ventral tegmental area and that in inhibiting MSNs in the nucleus accumbens NAcc these structures are excited releasing reward related behavior While GABA receptor agonists are capable of eliciting both liking and wanting reactions in the nucleus accumbens glutaminergic inputs from the basolateral amygdala ventral hippocampus and medial prefrontal cortex can drive incentive salience Furthermore while most studies find that NAcc neurons reduce firing in response to reward a number of studies find the opposite response This had led to the proposal of the disinhibition or depolarization hypothesis that proposes that excitation or NAcc neurons or at least certain subsets drives reward related behavior 2 28 29 After nearly 50 years of research on brain stimulation reward experts have certified that dozens of sites in the brain will maintain intracranial self stimulation Regions include the lateral hypothalamus and medial forebrain bundles which are especially effective Stimulation there activates fibers that form the ascending pathways the ascending pathways include the mesolimbic dopamine pathway which projects from the ventral tegmental area to the nucleus accumbens There are several explanations as to why the mesolimbic dopamine pathway is central to circuits mediating reward First there is a marked increase in dopamine release from the mesolimbic pathway when animals engage in intracranial self stimulation 9 Second experiments consistently indicate that brain stimulation reward stimulates the reinforcement of pathways that are normally activated by natural rewards and drug reward or intracranial self stimulation can exert more powerful activation of central reward mechanisms because they activate the reward center directly rather than through the peripheral nerves 9 30 31 Third when animals are administered addictive drugs or engage in naturally rewarding behaviors such as feeding or sexual activity there is a marked release of dopamine within the nucleus accumbens 9 However dopamine is not the only reward compound in the brain Key pathway Edit Diagram showing some of the key components of the mesocorticolimbic reward circuit Ventral tegmental area The ventral tegmental area VTA is important in responding to stimuli and cues that indicate a reward is present Rewarding stimuli and all addictive drugs act on the circuit by triggering the VTA to release dopamine signals to the nucleus accumbens either directly or indirectly citation needed The VTA has two important pathways The mesolimbic pathway projecting to limbic striatal regions and underpinning the motivational behaviors and processes and the mesocortical pathway projecting to the prefrontal cortex underpinning cognitive functions such as learning external cues etc 32 Dopaminergic neurons in this region converts the amino acid tyrosine into DOPA using the enzyme tyrosine hydroxylase which is then converted to dopamine using the enzyme dopa decarboxylase 33 Striatum Nucleus Accumbens The striatum is broadly involved in acquiring and eliciting learned behaviors in response to a rewarding cue The VTA projects to the striatum and activates the GABA ergic Medium Spiny Neurons via D1 and D2 receptors within the ventral Nucleus Accumbens and dorsal striatum 34 The Ventral Striatum the Nucleus Accumbens is broadly involved in acquiring behavior when fed into by the VTA and eliciting behavior when fed into by the PFC The NAc shell projects to the pallidum and the VTA regulating limbic and autonomic functions This modulates the reinforcing properties of stimuli and short term aspects of reward The NAc Core projects to the substantia nigra and is involved in the development of reward seeking behaviors and its expression It is involved in spatial learning conditional response and impulsive choice the long term elements of reward 32 The Dorsal Striatum is involved in learning the Dorsal Medial Striatum in goal directed learning and the Dorsal Lateral Striatum in stimulus response learning foundational to Pavlovian response 35 On repeated activation by a stimuli the Nucleus Accumbens can activate the Dorsal Striatum via an intrastriatal loop The transition of signals from the NAc to the DS allows reward associated cues to activate the DS without the reward itself being present This can activate cravings and reward seeking behaviors and is responsible for triggering relapse during abstinence in addiction 36 Prefrontal Cortex The VTA dopaminergic neurons project to the PFC activating glutaminergic neurons that project to multiple other regions including the Dorsal Striatum and NAc ultimately allowing the PFC to mediate salience and conditional behaviors in response to stimuli 36 Notably abstinence from addicting drugs activates the PFC glutamatergic projection to the NAc which leads to strong cravings and modulates reinstatement of addiction behaviors resulting from abstinence The PFC also interacts with the VTA through the mesocortical pathway and helps associate environmental cues with the reward 36 Hippocampus The Hippocampus has multiple functions including in the creation and storage of memories In the reward circuit it serves to contextual memories and associated cues It ultimately underpins the reinstatement of reward seeking behaviors via cues and contextual triggers 37 Amygdala The AMY receives input from the VTA and outputs to the NAc The amygdala is important in creating powerful emotional flashbulb memories and likely underpins the creation of strong cue associated memories 38 It also is important in mediating the anxiety effects of withdrawal and increased drug intake in addiction 39 Pleasure centers Edit Pleasure is a component of reward but not all rewards are pleasurable e g money does not elicit pleasure unless this response is conditioned 1 Stimuli that are naturally pleasurable and therefore attractive are known as intrinsic rewards whereas stimuli that are attractive and motivate approach behavior but are not inherently pleasurable are termed extrinsic rewards 1 Extrinsic rewards e g money are rewarding as a result of a learned association with an intrinsic reward 1 In other words extrinsic rewards function as motivational magnets that elicit wanting but not liking reactions once they have been acquired 1 The reward system contains pleasure centers or hedonic hotspots i e brain structures that mediate pleasure or liking reactions from intrinsic rewards As of October 2017 update hedonic hotspots have been identified in subcompartments within the nucleus accumbens shell ventral pallidum parabrachial nucleus orbitofrontal cortex OFC and insular cortex 2 18 40 The hotspot within the nucleus accumbens shell is located in the rostrodorsal quadrant of the medial shell while the hedonic coldspot is located in a more posterior region The posterior ventral pallidum also contains a hedonic hotspot while the anterior ventral pallidum contains a hedonic coldspot In rats microinjections of opioids endocannabinoids and orexin are capable of enhancing liking reactions in these hotspots 2 The hedonic hotspots located in the anterior OFC and posterior insula have been demonstrated to respond to orexin and opioids in rats as has the overlapping hedonic coldspot in the anterior insula and posterior OFC 40 On the other hand the parabrachial nucleus hotspot has only been demonstrated to respond to benzodiazepine receptor agonists 2 Hedonic hotspots are functionally linked in that activation of one hotspot results in the recruitment of the others as indexed by the induced expression of c Fos an immediate early gene Furthermore inhibition of one hotspot results in the blunting of the effects of activating another hotspot 2 40 Therefore the simultaneous activation of every hedonic hotspot within the reward system is believed to be necessary for generating the sensation of an intense euphoria 41 Wanting and liking EditMain article Incentive salience Tuning of appetitive and defensive reactions in the nucleus accumbens shell Above AMPA blockade requires D1 function in order to produce motivated behaviors regardless of valence and D2 function to produce defensive behaviors GABA agonism on the other hand does not requires dopamine receptor function Below The expansion of the anatomical regions that produce defensive behaviors under stress and appetitive behaviors in the home environment produced by AMPA antagonism This flexibility is less evident with GABA agonism 28 Incentive salience is the wanting or desire attribute which includes a motivational component that is assigned to a rewarding stimulus by the nucleus accumbens shell NAcc shell 1 42 43 The degree of dopamine neurotransmission into the NAcc shell from the mesolimbic pathway is highly correlated with the magnitude of incentive salience for rewarding stimuli 42 Activation of the dorsorostral region of the nucleus accumbens correlates with increases in wanting without concurrent increases in liking 44 However dopaminergic neurotransmission into the nucleus accumbens shell is responsible not only for appetitive motivational salience i e incentive salience towards rewarding stimuli but also for aversive motivational salience which directs behavior away from undesirable stimuli 13 45 46 In the dorsal striatum activation of D1 expressing MSNs produces appetitive incentive salience while activation of D2 expressing MSNs produces aversion In the NAcc such a dichotomy is not as clear cut and activation of both D1 and D2 MSNs is sufficient to enhance motivation 47 48 likely via disinhibiting the VTA through inhibiting the ventral pallidum 49 50 Robinson and Berridge s 1993 incentive sensitization theory proposed that reward contains separable psychological components wanting incentive and liking pleasure To explain increasing contact with a certain stimulus such as chocolate there are two independent factors at work our desire to have the chocolate wanting and the pleasure effect of the chocolate liking According to Robinson and Berridge wanting and liking are two aspects of the same process so rewards are usually wanted and liked to the same degree However wanting and liking also change independently under certain circumstances For example rats that do not eat after receiving dopamine experiencing a loss of desire for food act as though they still like food In another example activated self stimulation electrodes in the lateral hypothalamus of rats increase appetite but also cause more adverse reactions to tastes such as sugar and salt apparently the stimulation increases wanting but not liking Such results demonstrate that the reward system of rats includes independent processes of wanting and liking The wanting component is thought to be controlled by dopaminergic pathways whereas the liking component is thought to be controlled by opiate benzodiazepine systems 9 Anti reward system EditKoobs amp Le Moal proposed that there exists a separate circuit responsible for the attenuation of reward pursuing behavior which they termed the anti reward circuit This component acts as brakes on the reward circuit thus preventing the over pursuit of food sex etc This circuit involves multiple parts of the amygdala the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis the central nucleus the Nucleus Accumbens and signal molecules including norepinephrine corticotropin releasing factor and dynorphin 51 This circuit is also hypothesized to mediate the unpleasant components of stress and is thus thought to be involved in addiction and withdrawal While the reward circuit mediates the initial positive reinforcement involved in the development of addiction it is the anti reward circuit that later dominates via negative reinforcement that motivates the pursuit of the rewarding stimuli 52 Learning EditFurther information Associative learning Rewarding stimuli can drive learning in both the form of classical conditioning Pavlovian conditioning and operant conditioning instrumental conditioning In classical conditioning a reward can act as an unconditioned stimulus that when associated with the conditioned stimulus causes the conditioned stimulus to elicit both musculoskeletal in the form of simple approach and avoidance behaviors and vegetative responses In operant conditioning a reward may act as a reinforcer in that it increases or supports actions that lead to itself 3 Learned behaviors may or may not be sensitive to the value of the outcomes they lead to behaviors that are sensitive to the contingency of an outcome on the performance of an action as well as the outcome value are goal directed while elicited actions that are insensitive to contingency or value are called habits 53 This distinction is thought to reflect two forms of learning model free and model based Model free learning involves the simple caching and updating of values In contrast model based learning involves the storage and construction of an internal model of events that allows inference and flexible prediction Although pavlovian conditioning is generally assumed to be model free the incentive salience assigned to a conditioned stimulus is flexible with regard to changes in internal motivational states 54 Distinct neural systems are responsible for learning associations between stimuli and outcomes actions and outcomes and stimuli and responses Although classical conditioning is not limited to the reward system the enhancement of instrumental performance by stimuli i e Pavlovian instrumental transfer requires the nucleus accumbens Habitual and goal directed instrumental learning are dependent upon the lateral striatum and the medial striatum respectively 53 During instrumental learning opposing changes in the ratio of AMPA to NMDA receptors and phosphorylated ERK occurs in the D1 type and D2 type MSNs that constitute the direct and indirect pathways respectively 55 56 These changes in synaptic plasticity and the accompanying learning is dependent upon activation of striatal D1 and NMDA receptors The intracellular cascade activated by D1 receptors involves the recruitment of protein kinase A and through resulting phosphorylation of DARPP 32 the inhibition of phosphatases that deactivate ERK NMDA receptors activate ERK through a different but interrelated Ras Raf MEK ERK pathway Alone NMDA mediated activation of ERK is self limited as NMDA activation also inhibits PKA mediated inhibition of ERK deactivating phosphatases However when D1 and NMDA cascades are co activated they work synergistically and the resultant activation of ERK regulates synaptic plasticity in the form of spine restructuring transport of AMPA receptors regulation of CREB and increasing cellular excitability via inhibiting Kv4 2 57 58 59 Disorders EditAddiction Edit Main article Addiction DFosB DeltaFosB a gene transcription factor overexpression in the D1 type medium spiny neurons of the nucleus accumbens is the crucial common factor among virtually all forms of addiction i e behavioral addictions and drug addictions that induces addiction related behavior and neural plasticity 6 60 61 62 In particular DFosB promotes self administration reward sensitization and reward cross sensitization effects among specific addictive drugs and behaviors 6 60 61 63 64 Certain epigenetic modifications of histone protein tails i e histone modifications in specific regions of the brain are also known to play a crucial role in the molecular basis of addictions 62 65 66 67 Addictive drugs and behaviors are rewarding and reinforcing i e are addictive due to their effects on the dopamine reward pathway 17 68 The lateral hypothalamus and medial forebrain bundle has been the most frequently studied brain stimulation reward site particularly in studies of the effects of drugs on brain stimulation reward 69 The neurotransmitter system that has been most clearly identified with the habit forming actions of drugs of abuse is the mesolimbic dopamine system with its efferent targets in the nucleus accumbens and its local GABAergic afferents The reward relevant actions of amphetamine and cocaine are in the dopaminergic synapses of the nucleus accumbens and perhaps the medial prefrontal cortex Rats also learn to lever press for cocaine injections into the medial prefrontal cortex which works by increasing dopamine turnover in the nucleus accumbens 70 71 Nicotine infused directly into the nucleus accumbens also enhances local dopamine release presumably by a presynaptic action on the dopaminergic terminals of this region Nicotinic receptors localize to dopaminergic cell bodies and local nicotine injections increase dopaminergic cell firing that is critical for nicotinic reward 72 73 Some additional habit forming drugs are also likely to decrease the output of medium spiny neurons as a consequence despite activating dopaminergic projections For opiates the lowest threshold site for reward effects involves actions on GABAergic neurons in the ventral tegmental area a secondary site of opiate rewarding actions on medium spiny output neurons of the nucleus accumbens Thus the following form the core of currently characterised drug reward circuitry GABAergic afferents to the mesolimbic dopamine neurons primary substrate of opiate reward the mesolimbic dopamine neurons themselves primary substrate of psychomotor stimulant reward and GABAergic efferents to the mesolimbic dopamine neurons a secondary site of opiate reward 69 Motivation Edit Main article Motivational salience Dysfunctional motivational salience appears in a number of psychiatric symptoms and disorders Anhedonia traditionally defined as a reduced capacity to feel pleasure has been re examined as reflecting blunted incentive salience as most anhedonic populations exhibit intact liking 74 75 On the other end of the spectrum heightened incentive salience that is narrowed for specific stimuli is characteristic of behavioral and drug addictions In the case of fear or paranoia dysfunction may lie in elevated aversive salience 76 Neuroimaging studies across diagnoses associated with anhedonia have reported reduced activity in the OFC and ventral striatum 77 One meta analysis reported anhedonia was associated with reduced neural response to reward anticipation in the caudate nucleus putamen nucleus accumbens and medial prefrontal cortex mPFC 78 Mood disorders Edit Certain types of depression are associated with reduced motivation as assessed by willingness to expend effort for reward These abnormalities have been tentatively linked to reduced activity in areas of the striatum and while dopaminergic abnormalities are hypothesized to play a role most studies probing dopamine function in depression have reported inconsistent results 79 80 Although postmortem and neuroimaging studies have found abnormalities in numerous regions of the reward system few findings are consistently replicated Some studies have reported reduced NAcc hippocampus medial prefrontal cortex mPFC and orbitofrontal cortex OFC activity as well as elevated basolateral amygdala and subgenual cingulate cortex sgACC activity during tasks related to reward or positive stimuli These neuroimaging abnormalities are complemented by little post mortem research but what little research has been done suggests reduced excitatory synapses in the mPFC 81 Reduced activity in the mPFC during reward related tasks appears to be localized to more dorsal regions i e the pregenual cingulate cortex while the more ventral sgACC is hyperactive in depression 82 Attempts to investigate underlying neural circuitry in animal models has also yielded conflicting results Two paradigms are commonly used to simulate depression chronic social defeat CSDS and chronic mild stress CMS although many exist CSDS produces reduced preference for sucrose reduced social interactions and increased immobility in the forced swim test CMS similarly reduces sucrose preference and behavioral despair as assessed by tail suspension and forced swim tests Animals susceptible to CSDS exhibit increased phasic VTA firing and inhibition of VTA NAcc projections attenuates behavioral deficits induced by CSDS 83 However inhibition of VTA mPFC projections exacerbates social withdrawal On the other hand CMS associated reductions in sucrose preference and immobility were attenuated and exacerbated by VTA excitation and inhibition respectively 84 85 Although these differences may be attributable to different stimulation protocols or poor translational paradigms variable results may also lie in the heterogenous functionality of reward related regions 86 Optogenetic stimulation of the mPFC as a whole produces antidepressant effects This effect appears localized to the rodent homologue of the pgACC the prelimbic cortex as stimulation of the rodent homologue of the sgACC the infralimbic cortex produces no behavioral effects Furthermore deep brain stimulation in the infralimbic cortex which is thought to have an inhibitory effect also produces an antidepressant effect This finding is congruent with the observation that pharmacological inhibition of the infralimbic cortex attenuates depressive behaviors 86 Schizophrenia Edit Schizophrenia is associated with deficits in motivation commonly grouped under other negative symptoms such as reduced spontaneous speech The experience of liking is frequently reported to be intact 87 both behaviorally and neurally although results may be specific to certain stimuli such as monetary rewards 88 Furthermore implicit learning and simple reward related tasks are also intact in schizophrenia 89 Rather deficits in the reward system are apparent during reward related tasks that are cognitively complex These deficits are associated with both abnormal striatal and OFC activity as well as abnormalities in regions associated with cognitive functions such as the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex DLPFC 90 Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder Edit In those with ADHD core aspects of the reward system are underactive making it challenging to derive reward from regular activities Those with the disorder experience a boost of motivation after a high stimulation behaviour triggers a release of dopamine In the aftermath of that boost and reward the return to baseline levels results in an immediate drop in motivation 91 Impairments of dopaminergic and serotonergic function are said to be key factors in ADHD 92 These impairments can lead to executive dysfunction such as dysregulation of reward processing and motivational dysfunction including anhedonia 93 History Edit Skinner box The first clue to the presence of a reward system in the brain came with an accidental discovery by James Olds and Peter Milner in 1954 They discovered that rats would perform behaviors such as pressing a bar to administer a brief burst of electrical stimulation to specific sites in their brains This phenomenon is called intracranial self stimulation or brain stimulation reward Typically rats will press a lever hundreds or thousands of times per hour to obtain this brain stimulation stopping only when they are exhausted While trying to teach rats how to solve problems and run mazes stimulation of certain regions of the brain where the stimulation was found seemed to give pleasure to the animals They tried the same thing with humans and the results were similar The explanation to why animals engage in a behavior that has no value to the survival of either themselves or their species is that the brain stimulation is activating the system underlying reward 94 In a fundamental discovery made in 1954 researchers James Olds and Peter Milner found that low voltage electrical stimulation of certain regions of the brain of the rat acted as a reward in teaching the animals to run mazes and solve problems 95 failed verification 96 It seemed that stimulation of those parts of the brain gave the animals pleasure 95 and in later work humans reported pleasurable sensations from such stimulation citation needed When rats were tested in Skinner boxes where they could stimulate the reward system by pressing a lever the rats pressed for hours 96 Research in the next two decades established that dopamine is one of the main chemicals aiding neural signaling in these regions and dopamine was suggested to be the brain s pleasure chemical 97 Ivan Pavlov was a psychologist who used the reward system to study classical conditioning Pavlov used the reward system by rewarding dogs with food after they had heard a bell or another stimulus Pavlov was rewarding the dogs so that the dogs associated food the reward with the bell the stimulus 98 Edward L Thorndike used the reward system to study operant conditioning He began by putting cats in a puzzle box and placing food outside of the box so that the cat wanted to escape The cats worked to get out of the puzzle box to get to the food Although the cats ate the food after they escaped the box Thorndike learned that the cats attempted to escape the box without the reward of food Thorndike used the rewards of food and freedom to stimulate the reward system of the cats Thorndike used this to see how the cats learned to escape the box 99 Other species EditAnimals quickly learn to press a bar to obtain an injection of opiates directly into the midbrain tegmentum or the nucleus accumbens The same animals do not work to obtain the opiates if the dopaminergic neurons of the mesolimbic pathway are inactivated In this perspective animals like humans engage in behaviors that increase dopamine release Kent Berridge a researcher in affective neuroscience found that sweet liked and bitter disliked tastes produced distinct orofacial expressions and these expressions were similarly displayed by human newborns orangutans and rats This was evidence that pleasure specifically liking has objective features and was essentially the same across various animal species Most neuroscience studies have shown that the more dopamine released by the reward the more effective the reward is This is called the hedonic impact which can be changed by the effort for the reward and the reward itself Berridge discovered that blocking dopamine systems did not seem to change the positive reaction to something sweet as measured by facial expression In other words the hedonic impact did not change based on the amount of sugar This discounted the conventional assumption that dopamine mediates pleasure Even with more intense dopamine alterations the data seemed to remain constant 100 However a clinical study from January 2019 that assessed the effect of a dopamine precursor levodopa antagonist risperidone and a placebo on reward responses to music including the degree of pleasure experienced during musical chills as measured by changes in electrodermal activity as well as subjective ratings found that the manipulation of dopamine neurotransmission bidirectionally regulates pleasure cognition specifically the hedonic impact of music in human subjects 101 102 This research demonstrated that increased dopamine neurotransmission acts as a sine qua non condition for pleasurable hedonic reactions to music in humans 101 102 Berridge developed the incentive salience hypothesis to address the wanting aspect of rewards It explains the compulsive use of drugs by drug addicts even when the drug no longer produces euphoria and the cravings experienced even after the individual has finished going through withdrawal Some addicts respond to certain stimuli involving neural changes caused by drugs This sensitization in the brain is similar to the effect of dopamine because wanting and liking reactions occur Human and animal brains and behaviors experience similar changes regarding reward systems because these systems are so prominent 100 See also EditCarrot and stick Compliance psychology Frisson Motivation Norm of reciprocityReferences Edit a b c d e f g h i j k l m Schultz Wolfram July 2015 Neuronal Reward and Decision Signals From Theories to Data Physiological Reviews 95 3 853 951 doi 10 1152 physrev 00023 2014 PMC 4491543 PMID 26109341 a b c d e f g h Berridge KC Kringelbach ML May 2015 Pleasure systems in the brain Neuron 86 3 646 664 doi 10 1016 j neuron 2015 02 018 PMC 4425246 PMID 25950633 In the prefrontal cortex recent evidence indicates that the orbitofrontal cortex OFC and insula cortex may each contain their own additional hot spots D C Castro et al Soc Neurosci abstract In specific subregions of each area either opioid stimulating or orexin stimulating microinjections appear to enhance the number of liking reactions elicited by sweetness similar to the nucleus accumbens NAc and ventral pallidum VP hot spots Successful confirmation of hedonic hot spots in the OFC or insula would be important and possibly relevant to the orbitofrontal mid anterior site mentioned earlier that especially tracks the subjective pleasure of foods in humans Georgiadis et al 2012 Kringelbach 2005 Kringelbach et al 2003 Small et al 2001 Veldhuizen et al 2010 Finally in the brainstem a hindbrain site near the parabrachial nucleus of dorsal pons also appears able to contribute to hedonic gains of function Soderpalm and Berridge 2000 A brainstem mechanism for pleasure may seem more surprising than forebrain hot spots to anyone who views the brainstem as merely reflexive but the pontine parabrachial nucleus contributes to taste pain and many visceral sensations from the body and has also been suggested to play an important role in motivation Wu et al 2012 and in human emotion especially related to the somatic marker hypothesis Damasio 2010 a b c d e f g h i Schultz W 2015 Neuronal reward and decision signals from theories to data Physiological Reviews 95 3 853 951 doi 10 1152 physrev 00023 2014 PMC 4491543 PMID 26109341 Rewards in operant conditioning are positive reinforcers Operant behavior gives a good definition for rewards Anything that makes an individual come back for more is a positive reinforcer and therefore a reward Although it provides a good definition positive reinforcement is only one of several reward functions Rewards are attractive They are motivating and make us exert an effort Rewards induce approach behavior also called appetitive or preparatory behavior sexual behavior and consummatory behavior Thus any stimulus object event activity or situation that has the potential to make us approach and consume it is by definition a reward Rewarding stimuli objects events situations and activities consist of several major components First rewards have basic sensory components visual auditory somatosensory gustatory and olfactory Second rewards are salient and thus elicit attention which are manifested as orienting responses The salience of rewards derives from three principal factors namely their physical intensity and impact physical salience their novelty and surprise novelty surprise salience and their general motivational impact shared with punishers motivational salience A separate form not included in this scheme incentive salience primarily addresses dopamine function in addiction and refers only to approach behavior as opposed to learning Third rewards have a value component that determines the positively motivating effects of rewards and is not contained in nor explained by the sensory and attentional components This component reflects behavioral preferences and thus is subjective and only partially determined by physical parameters Only this component constitutes what we understand as a reward It mediates the specific behavioral reinforcing approach generating and emotional effects of rewards that are crucial for the organism s survival and reproduction whereas all other components are only supportive of these functions Rewards can also be intrinsic to behavior They contrast with extrinsic rewards that provide motivation for behavior and constitute the essence of operant behavior in laboratory tests Intrinsic rewards are activities that are pleasurable on their own and are undertaken for their own sake without being the means for getting extrinsic rewards Intrinsic rewards are genuine rewards in their own right as they induce learning approach and pleasure like perfectioning playing and enjoying the piano Although they can serve to condition higher order rewards they are not conditioned higher order rewards as attaining their reward properties does not require pairing with an unconditioned reward These emotions are also called liking for pleasure and wanting for desire in addiction research and strongly support the learning and approach generating functions of reward Guo Rong Bohmer Wendelin Hebart Martin Chien Samson Sommer Tobias Obermayer Klaus Glascher Jan 14 December 2016 Interaction of Instrumental and Goal Directed Learning Modulates Prediction Error Representations in the Ventral Striatum The Journal of Neuroscience Society for Neuroscience 36 50 12650 12660 doi 10 1523 jneurosci 1677 16 2016 ISSN 0270 6474 PMC 6705659 PMID 27974615 Malenka RC Nestler EJ Hyman SE 2009 Chapter 15 Reinforcement and Addictive Disorders In Sydor A Brown RY eds Molecular Neuropharmacology A Foundation for Clinical Neuroscience 2nd ed New York McGraw Hill Medical pp 364 375 ISBN 9780071481274 a b c Nestler EJ December 2013 Cellular basis of memory for addiction Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience 15 4 431 443 PMC 3898681 PMID 24459410 Despite the importance of numerous psychosocial factors at its core drug addiction involves a biological process the ability of repeated exposure to a drug of abuse to induce changes in a vulnerable brain that drive the compulsive seeking and taking of drugs and loss of control over drug use that define a state of addiction A large body of literature has demonstrated that such DFosB induction in D1 type nucleus accumbens neurons increases an animal s sensitivity to drug as well as natural rewards and promotes drug self administration presumably through a process of positive reinforcement Another DFosB target is cFos as DFosB accumulates with repeated drug exposure it represses c Fos and contributes to the molecular switch whereby DFosB is selectively induced in the chronic drug treated state 41 Moreover there is increasing evidence that despite a range of genetic risks for addiction across the population exposure to sufficiently high doses of a drug for long periods of time can transform someone who has relatively lower genetic loading into an addict Glossary of Terms Mount Sinai School of Medicine Department of Neuroscience Retrieved 9 February 2015 Volkow ND Koob GF McLellan AT January 2016 Neurobiologic Advances from the Brain Disease Model of Addiction New England Journal of Medicine 374 4 363 371 doi 10 1056 NEJMra1511480 PMC 6135257 PMID 26816013 Substance use disorder A diagnostic term in the fifth edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders DSM 5 referring to recurrent use of alcohol or other drugs that causes clinically and functionally significant impairment such as health problems disability and failure to meet major responsibilities at work school or home Depending on the level of severity this disorder is classified as mild moderate or severe Addiction A term used to indicate the most severe chronic stage of substance use disorder in which there is a substantial loss of self control as indicated by compulsive drug taking despite the desire to stop taking the drug In the DSM 5 the term addiction is synonymous with the classification of severe substance use disorder a b c d e Kolb Bryan Whishaw Ian Q 2001 An Introduction to Brain and Behavior 1st ed New York Worth pp 438 441 ISBN 9780716751694 Brain amp Behavior Research Foundation 13 March 2019 The Biology of Addiction YouTube a href Template Cite web html title Template Cite web cite web a CS1 maint url status link Dopamine Involved In Aggression Medical News Today 15 January 2008 Archived from the original on 23 September 2010 Retrieved 14 November 2010 Duarte Isabel C Afonso Sonia Jorge Helena Cayolla Ricardo Ferreira Carlos Castelo Branco Miguel 1 May 2017 Tribal love the neural correlates of passionate engagement in football fans Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 12 5 718 728 doi 10 1093 scan nsx003 PMC 5460049 PMID 28338882 a b c Salamone John D Correa Merce November 2012 The Mysterious Motivational Functions of Mesolimbic Dopamine Neuron 76 3 470 485 doi 10 1016 j neuron 2012 10 021 PMC 4450094 PMID 23141060 a b c d e f g Yager LM Garcia AF Wunsch AM Ferguson SM August 2015 The ins and outs of the striatum Role in drug addiction Neuroscience 301 529 541 doi 10 1016 j neuroscience 2015 06 033 PMC 4523218 PMID 26116518 The striatum receives dopaminergic inputs from the ventral tegmental area VTA and the substantia nigra SNr and glutamatergic inputs from several areas including the cortex hippocampus amygdala and thalamus Swanson 1982 Phillipson and Griffiths 1985 Finch 1996 Groenewegen et al 1999 Britt et al 2012 These glutamatergic inputs make contact on the heads of dendritic spines of the striatal GABAergic medium spiny projection neurons MSNs whereas dopaminergic inputs synapse onto the spine neck allowing for an important and complex interaction between these two inputs in modulation of MSN activity It should also be noted that there is a small population of neurons in the nucleus accumbens NAc that coexpress both D1 and D2 receptors though this is largely restricted to the NAc shell Bertran Gonzalez et al 2008 Neurons in the NAc core and NAc shell subdivisions also differ functionally The NAc core is involved in the processing of conditioned stimuli whereas the NAc shell is more important in the processing of unconditioned stimuli Classically these two striatal MSN populations are thought to have opposing effects on basal ganglia output Activation of the dMSNs causes a net excitation of the thalamus resulting in a positive cortical feedback loop thereby acting as a go signal to initiate behavior Activation of the iMSNs however causes a net inhibition of thalamic activity resulting in a negative cortical feedback loop and therefore serves as a brake to inhibit behavior there is also mounting evidence that iMSNs play a role in motivation and addiction Lobo and Nestler 2011 Grueter et al 2013 For example optogenetic activation of NAc core and shell iMSNs suppressed the development of a cocaine CPP whereas selective ablation of NAc core and shell iMSNs enhanced the development and the persistence of an amphetamine CPP Durieux et al 2009 Lobo et al 2010 These findings suggest that iMSNs can bidirectionally modulate drug reward Together these data suggest that iMSNs normally act to restrain drug taking behavior and recruitment of these neurons may in fact be protective against the development of compulsive drug use Taylor SB Lewis CR Olive MF 2013 The neurocircuitry of illicit psychostimulant addiction acute and chronic effects in humans Subst Abuse Rehabil 4 29 43 doi 10 2147 SAR S39684 PMC 3931688 PMID 24648786 Regions of the basal ganglia which include the dorsal and ventral striatum internal and external segments of the globus pallidus subthalamic nucleus and dopaminergic cell bodies in the substantia nigra are highly implicated not only in fine motor control but also in prefrontal cortex PFC function 43 Of these regions the nucleus accumbens NAc described above and the dorsal striatum DS described below are most frequently examined with respect to addiction Thus only a brief description of the modulatory role of the basal ganglia in addiction relevant circuits will be mentioned here The overall output of the basal ganglia is predominantly via the thalamus which then projects back to the PFC to form cortico striatal thalamo cortical CSTC loops Three CSTC loops are proposed to modulate executive function action selection and behavioral inhibition In the dorsolateral prefrontal circuit the basal ganglia primarily modulate the identification and selection of goals including rewards 44 The orbitofrontal cortex OFC circuit modulates decision making and impulsivity and the anterior cingulate circuit modulates the assessment of consequences 44 These circuits are modulated by dopaminergic inputs from the ventral tegmental area VTA to ultimately guide behaviors relevant to addiction including the persistence and narrowing of the behavioral repertoire toward drug seeking and continued drug use despite negative consequences 43 45 Grall Bronnec M Sauvaget A 2014 The use of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation for modulating craving and addictive behaviours a critical literature review of efficacy technical and methodological considerations Neurosci Biobehav Rev 47 592 613 doi 10 1016 j neubiorev 2014 10 013 PMID 25454360 Studies have shown that cravings are underpinned by activation of the reward and motivation circuits McBride et al 2006 Wang et al 2007 Wing et al 2012 Goldman et al 2013 Jansen et al 2013 and Volkow et al 2013 According to these authors the main neural structures involved are the nucleus accumbens dorsal striatum orbitofrontal cortex anterior cingulate cortex dorsolateral prefrontal cortex DLPFC amygdala hippocampus and insula a b Malenka RC Nestler EJ Hyman SE 2009 Sydor A Brown RY eds Molecular Neuropharmacology A Foundation for Clinical Neuroscience 2nd ed New York McGraw Hill Medical pp 365 366 376 ISBN 978 0 07 148127 4 The neural substrates that underlie the perception of reward and the phenomenon of positive reinforcement are a set of interconnected forebrain structures called brain reward pathways these include the nucleus accumbens NAc the major component of the ventral striatum the basal forebrain components of which have been termed the extended amygdala as discussed later in this chapter hippocampus hypothalamus and frontal regions of cerebral cortex These structures receive rich dopaminergic innervation from the ventral tegmental area VTA of the midbrain Addictive drugs are rewarding and reinforcing because they act in brain reward pathways to enhance either dopamine release or the effects of dopamine in the NAc or related structures or because they produce effects similar to dopamine A macrostructure postulated to integrate many of the functions of this circuit is described by some investigators as the extended amygdala The extended amygdala is said to comprise several basal forebrain structures that share similar morphology immunocytochemical features and connectivity and that are well suited to mediating aspects of reward function these include the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis the central medial amygdala the shell of the NAc and the sublenticular substantia innominata a b Richard JM Castro DC Difeliceantonio AG Robinson MJ Berridge KC November 2013 Mapping brain circuits of reward and motivation in the footsteps of Ann Kelley Neurosci Biobehav Rev 37 9 Pt A 1919 1931 doi 10 1016 j neubiorev 2012 12 008 PMC 3706488 PMID 23261404 Figure 3 Neural circuits underlying motivated wanting and hedonic liking Luo M Zhou J Liu Z August 2015 Reward processing by the dorsal raphe nucleus 5 HT and beyond Learn Mem 22 9 452 460 doi 10 1101 lm 037317 114 PMC 4561406 PMID 26286655 Moulton EA Elman I Becerra LR Goldstein RZ Borsook D May 2014 The cerebellum and addiction insights gained from neuroimaging research Addiction Biology 19 3 317 331 doi 10 1111 adb 12101 PMC 4031616 PMID 24851284 Caligiore D Pezzulo G Baldassarre G Bostan AC Strick PL Doya K Helmich RC Dirkx M Houk J Jorntell H Lago Rodriguez A Galea JM Miall RC Popa T Kishore A Verschure PF Zucca R Herreros I February 2017 Consensus Paper Towards a Systems Level View of Cerebellar Function the Interplay Between Cerebellum Basal Ganglia and Cortex Cerebellum 16 1 203 229 doi 10 1007 s12311 016 0763 3 PMC 5243918 PMID 26873754 Ogawa SK Watabe Uchida M 2018 Organization of dopamine and serotonin system Anatomical and functional mapping of monosynaptic inputs using rabies virus Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 174 9 22 doi 10 1016 j pbb 2017 05 001 PMID 28476484 S2CID 5089422 Morales M Margolis EB February 2017 Ventral tegmental area cellular heterogeneity connectivity and behaviour Nature Reviews Neuroscience 18 2 73 85 doi 10 1038 nrn 2016 165 PMID 28053327 S2CID 10311562 Lammel S Lim BK Malenka RC January 2014 Reward and aversion in a heterogeneous midbrain dopamine system Neuropharmacology 76 Pt B 0 351 9 doi 10 1016 j neuropharm 2013 03 019 PMC 3778102 PMID 23578393 Nieh EH Kim SY Namburi P Tye KM 20 May 2013 Optogenetic dissection of neural circuits underlying emotional valence and motivated behaviors Brain Research 1511 73 92 doi 10 1016 j brainres 2012 11 001 hdl 1721 1 92890 PMC 4099056 PMID 23142759 Trantham Davidson H Neely LC Lavin A Seamans JK 2004 Mechanisms underlying differential D1 versus D2 dopamine receptor regulation of inhibition in prefrontal cortex The Journal of Neuroscience 24 47 10652 10659 doi 10 1523 jneurosci 3179 04 2004 PMC 5509068 PMID 15564581 You ZB Chen YQ Wise RA 2001 Dopamine and glutamate release in the nucleus accumbens and ventral tegmental area of rat following lateral hypothalamic self stimulation Neuroscience 107 4 629 639 doi 10 1016 s0306 4522 01 00379 7 PMID 11720786 S2CID 33615497 a b Castro DC Cole SL Berridge KC 2015 Lateral hypothalamus nucleus accumbens and ventral pallidum roles in eating and hunger interactions between homeostatic and reward circuitry Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience 9 90 doi 10 3389 fnsys 2015 00090 PMC 4466441 PMID 26124708 Carlezon WA Jr Thomas MJ 2009 Biological substrates of reward and aversion a nucleus accumbens activity hypothesis Neuropharmacology 56 Suppl 1 Suppl 1 122 32 doi 10 1016 j neuropharm 2008 06 075 PMC 2635333 PMID 18675281 Wise RA Rompre PP 1989 Brain dopamine and reward Annual Review of Psychology 40 191 225 doi 10 1146 annurev ps 40 020189 001203 PMID 2648975 Wise RA October 2002 Brain reward circuitry insights from unsensed incentives Neuron 36 2 229 240 doi 10 1016 S0896 6273 02 00965 0 PMID 12383779 S2CID 16547037 a b Kokane S S amp Perrotti L I 2020 Sex Differences and the Role of Estradiol in Mesolimbic Reward Circuits and Vulnerability to Cocaine and Opiate Addiction Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 14 Becker J B amp Chartoff E 2019 Sex differences in neural mechanisms mediating reward and addiction Neuropsychopharmacology 44 1 166 183 Stoof J C amp Kebabian J W 1984 Two dopamine receptors biochemistry physiology and pharmacology Life sciences 35 23 2281 2296 Yin H H Knowlton B J amp Balleine B W 2005 Blockade of NMDA receptors in the dorsomedial striatum prevents action outcome learning in instrumental conditioning European Journal of Neuroscience 22 2 505 512 a b c Koob G F amp Volkow N D 2016 Neurobiology of addiction a neurocircuitry analysis The Lancet Psychiatry 3 8 760 773 Kutlu M G amp Gould T J 2016 Effects of drugs of abuse on hippocampal plasticity and hippocampus dependent learning and memory contributions to development and maintenance of addiction Learning amp memory 23 10 515 533 McGaugh J L July 2004 The amygdala modulates the consolidation of memories of emotionally arousing experiences Annual Review of Neuroscience 27 1 1 28 Koob G F Le Moal M 2008 Addiction and the brain antireward system Annu Rev Psychol 59 29 53 10 1146 annurev psych 59 103006 093548 PubMed CrossRef Google Scholar Koob G F Sanna P P Bloom F E 1998 Neuroscience of addiction Neuron 21 467 476 a b c Castro DC Berridge KC 24 October 2017 Opioid and orexin hedonic hotspots in rat orbitofrontal cortex and insula Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 114 43 E9125 E9134 Bibcode 2017PNAS 114E9125C doi 10 1073 pnas 1705753114 PMC 5664503 PMID 29073109 Here we show that opioid or orexin stimulations in orbitofrontal cortex and insula causally enhance hedonic liking reactions to sweetness and find a third cortical site where the same neurochemical stimulations reduce positive hedonic impact Kringelbach ML Berridge KC 2012 The Joyful Mind PDF Scientific American 307 2 44 45 Bibcode 2012SciAm 307b 40K doi 10 1038 scientificamerican0812 40 PMID 22844850 Archived from the original PDF on 29 March 2017 Retrieved 17 January 2017 So it makes sense that the real pleasure centers in the brain those directly responsible for generating pleasurable sensations turn out to lie within some of the structures previously identified as part of the reward circuit One of these so called hedonic hotspots lies in a subregion of the nucleus accumbens called the medial shell A second is found within the ventral pallidum a deep seated structure near the base of the forebrain that receives most of its signals from the nucleus accumbens On the other hand intense euphoria is harder to come by than everyday pleasures The reason may be that strong enhancement of pleasure like the chemically induced pleasure bump we produced in lab animals seems to require activation of the entire network at once Defection of any single component dampens the high Whether the pleasure circuit and in particular the ventral pallidum works the same way in humans is unclear a b Berridge KC April 2012 From prediction error to incentive salience mesolimbic computation of reward motivation Eur J Neurosci 35 7 1124 1143 doi 10 1111 j 1460 9568 2012 07990 x PMC 3325516 PMID 22487042 Here I discuss how mesocorticolimbic mechanisms generate the motivation component of incentive salience Incentive salience takes Pavlovian learning and memory as one input and as an equally important input takes neurobiological state factors e g drug states appetite states satiety states that can vary independently of learning Neurobiological state changes can produce unlearned fluctuations or even reversals in the ability of a previously learned reward cue to trigger motivation Such fluctuations in cue triggered motivation can dramatically depart from all previously learned values about the associated reward outcome Associative learning and prediction are important contributors to motivation for rewards Learning gives incentive value to arbitrary cues such as a Pavlovian conditioned stimulus CS that is associated with a reward unconditioned stimulus or UCS Learned cues for reward are often potent triggers of desires For example learned cues can trigger normal appetites in everyone and can sometimes trigger compulsive urges and relapse in addicts Cue triggered wanting for the UCSA brief CS encounter or brief UCS encounter often primes a pulse of elevated motivation to obtain and consume more reward UCS This is a signature feature of incentive salience Cue as attractive motivational magnetsWhen a Pavlovian CS is attributed with incentive salience it not only triggers wanting for its UCS but often the cue itself becomes highly attractive even to an irrational degree This cue attraction is another signature feature of incentive salience Two recognizable features of incentive salience are often visible that can be used in neuroscience experiments i UCS directed wanting CS triggered pulses of intensified wanting for the UCS reward and ii CS directed wanting motivated attraction to the Pavlovian cue which makes the arbitrary CS stimulus into a motivational magnet Malenka RC Nestler EJ Hyman SE 2009 Sydor A Brown RY eds Molecular Neuropharmacology A Foundation for Clinical Neuroscience 2nd ed New York McGraw Hill Medical pp 147 148 367 376 ISBN 978 0 07 148127 4 VTA DA neurons play a critical role in motivation reward related behavior Chapter 15 attention and multiple forms of memory This organization of the DA system wide projection from a limited number of cell bodies permits coordinated responses to potent new rewards Thus acting in diverse terminal fields dopamine confers motivational salience wanting on the reward itself or associated cues nucleus accumbens shell region updates the value placed on different goals in light of this new experience orbital prefrontal cortex helps consolidate multiple forms of memory amygdala and hippocampus and encodes new motor programs that will facilitate obtaining this reward in the future nucleus accumbens core region and dorsal striatum In this example dopamine modulates the processing of sensorimotor information in diverse neural circuits to maximize the ability of the organism to obtain future rewards The brain reward circuitry that is targeted by addictive drugs normally mediates the pleasure and strengthening of behaviors associated with natural reinforcers such as food water and sexual contact Dopamine neurons in the VTA are activated by food and water and dopamine release in the NAc is stimulated by the presence of natural reinforcers such as food water or a sexual partner The NAc and VTA are central components of the circuitry underlying reward and memory of reward As previously mentioned the activity of dopaminergic neurons in the VTA appears to be linked to reward prediction The NAc is involved in learning associated with reinforcement and the modulation of motoric responses to stimuli that satisfy internal homeostatic needs The shell of the NAc appears to be particularly important to initial drug actions within reward circuitry addictive drugs appear to have a greater effect on dopamine release in the shell than in the core of the NAc Berridge KC Kringelbach ML 1 June 2013 Neuroscience of affect brain mechanisms of pleasure and displeasure Current Opinion in Neurobiology 23 3 294 303 doi 10 1016 j conb 2013 01 017 PMC 3644539 PMID 23375169 For instance mesolimbic dopamine probably the most popular brain neurotransmitter candidate for pleasure two decades ago turns out not to cause pleasure or liking at all Rather dopamine more selectively mediates a motivational process of incentive salience which is a mechanism for wanting rewards but not for liking them Rather opioid stimulation has the special capacity to enhance liking only if the stimulation occurs within an anatomical hotspot Calipari Erin S Bagot Rosemary C Purushothaman Immanuel Davidson Thomas J Yorgason Jordan T Pena Catherine J Walker Deena M Pirpinias Stephen T Guise Kevin G Ramakrishnan Charu Deisseroth Karl Nestler Eric J 8 March 2016 In vivo imaging identifies temporal signature of D1 and D2 medium spiny neurons in cocaine reward Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 113 10 2726 2731 Bibcode 2016PNAS 113 2726C doi 10 1073 pnas 1521238113 PMC 4791010 PMID 26831103 Baliki M N Mansour A Baria A T Huang L Berger S E Fields H L Apkarian A V 9 October 2013 Parceling Human Accumbens into Putative Core and Shell Dissociates Encoding of Values for Reward and Pain Journal of Neuroscience 33 41 16383 16393 doi 10 1523 JNEUROSCI 1731 13 2013 PMC 3792469 PMID 24107968 Soares Cunha Carina Coimbra Barbara Sousa Nuno Rodrigues Ana J September 2016 Reappraising striatal D1 and D2 neurons in reward and aversion Neuroscience amp Biobehavioral Reviews 68 370 386 doi 10 1016 j neubiorev 2016 05 021 hdl 1822 47044 PMID 27235078 S2CID 207092810 Bamford Nigel S Wightman R Mark Sulzer David February 2018 Dopamine s Effects on Corticostriatal Synapses during Reward Based Behaviors Neuron 97 3 494 510 doi 10 1016 j neuron 2018 01 006 PMC 5808590 PMID 29420932 Soares Cunha Carina Coimbra Barbara David Pereira Ana Borges Sonia Pinto Luisa Costa Patricio Sousa Nuno Rodrigues Ana J September 2016 Activation of D2 dopamine receptor expressing neurons in the nucleus accumbens increases motivation Nature Communications 7 1 11829 Bibcode 2016NatCo 711829S doi 10 1038 ncomms11829 PMC 4931006 PMID 27337658 Soares Cunha Carina Coimbra Barbara Domingues Ana Veronica Vasconcelos Nivaldo Sousa Nuno Rodrigues Ana Joao March 2018 Nucleus Accumbens Microcircuit Underlying D2 MSN Driven Increase in Motivation eNeuro 5 2 ENEURO 0386 18 2018 doi 10 1523 ENEURO 0386 18 2018 PMC 5957524 PMID 29780881 Koob G F Le Moal M 2008 Addiction and the brain antireward system Annu Rev Psychol 59 29 53 10 1146 annurev psych 59 103006 093548 PubMed CrossRef Google Scholar Koob G F Sanna P P Bloom F E 1998 Neuroscience of addiction Neuron 21 467 476 Meyer J S amp Quenzer L F 2013 Psychopharmacology Drugs the brain and behavior Sinauer Associates a b Yin HH Ostlund SB Balleine BW October 2008 Reward guided learning beyond dopamine in the nucleus accumbens the integrative functions of cortico basal ganglia networks The European Journal of Neuroscience 28 8 1437 48 doi 10 1111 j 1460 9568 2008 06422 x PMC 2756656 PMID 18793321 Dayan P Berridge KC June 2014 Model based and model free Pavlovian reward learning revaluation revision and revelation Cognitive Affective amp Behavioral Neuroscience 14 2 473 92 doi 10 3758 s13415 014 0277 8 PMC 4074442 PMID 24647659 Balleine BW Morris RW Leung BK 2 December 2015 Thalamocortical integration of instrumental learning and performance and their disintegration in addiction Brain Research 1628 Pt A 104 16 doi 10 1016 j brainres 2014 12 023 PMID 25514336 S2CID 11776683 Importantly we found evidence of increased activity in the direct pathway both intracellular changes in the expression of the plasticity marker pERK and AMPA NMDA ratios evoked by stimulating cortical afferents were increased in the D1 direct pathway neurons In contrast D2 neurons showed an opposing change in plasticity stimulation of cortical afferents reduced AMPA NMDA ratios on those neurons Shan et al 2014 Nakanishi S Hikida T Yawata S 12 December 2014 Distinct dopaminergic control of the direct and indirect pathways in reward based and avoidance learning behaviors Neuroscience 282 49 59 doi 10 1016 j neuroscience 2014 04 026 PMID 24769227 S2CID 21652525 Shiflett MW Balleine BW 15 September 2011 Molecular substrates of action control in cortico striatal circuits Progress in Neurobiology 95 1 1 13 doi 10 1016 j pneurobio 2011 05 007 PMC 3175490 PMID 21704115 Schultz W April 2013 Updating dopamine reward signals Current Opinion in Neurobiology 23 2 229 38 doi 10 1016 j conb 2012 11 012 PMC 3866681 PMID 23267662 Shiflett MW Balleine BW 17 March 2011 Contributions of ERK signaling in the striatum to instrumental learning and performance Behavioural Brain Research 218 1 240 7 doi 10 1016 j bbr 2010 12 010 PMC 3022085 PMID 21147168 a b Ruffle JK November 2014 Molecular neurobiology of addiction what s all the D FosB about Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse 40 6 428 437 doi 10 3109 00952990 2014 933840 PMID 25083822 S2CID 19157711 The strong correlation between chronic drug exposure and DFosB provides novel opportunities for targeted therapies in addiction 118 and suggests methods to analyze their efficacy 119 Over the past two decades research has progressed from identifying DFosB induction to investigating its subsequent action 38 It is likely that DFosB research will now progress into a new era the use of DFosB as a biomarker ConclusionsDFosB is an essential transcription factor implicated in the molecular and behavioral pathways of addiction following repeated drug exposure The formation of DFosB in multiple brain regions and the molecular pathway leading to the formation of AP 1 complexes is well understood The establishment of a functional purpose for DFosB has allowed further determination as to some of the key aspects of its molecular cascades involving effectors such as GluR2 87 88 Cdk5 93 and NFkB 100 Moreover many of these molecular changes identified are now directly linked to the structural physiological and behavioral changes observed following chronic drug exposure 60 95 97 102 New frontiers of research investigating the molecular roles of DFosB have been opened by epigenetic studies and recent advances have illustrated the role of DFosB acting on DNA and histones truly as a molecular switch 34 As a consequence of our improved understanding of DFosB in addiction it is possible to evaluate the addictive potential of current medications 119 as well as use it as a biomarker for assessing the efficacy of therapeutic interventions 121 122 124 Some of these proposed interventions have limitations 125 or are in their infancy 75 However it is hoped that some of these preliminary findings may lead to innovative treatments which are much needed in addiction a b Olsen CM December 2011 Natural rewards neuroplasticity and non drug addictions Neuropharmacology 61 7 1109 1122 doi 10 1016 j neuropharm 2011 03 010 PMC 3139704 PMID 21459101 Functional neuroimaging studies in humans have shown that gambling Breiter et al 2001 shopping Knutson et al 2007 orgasm Komisaruk et al 2004 playing video games Koepp et al 1998 Hoeft et al 2008 and the sight of appetizing food Wang et al 2004a activate many of the same brain regions i e the mesocorticolimbic system and extended amygdala as drugs of abuse Volkow et al 2004 Cross sensitization is also bidirectional as a history of amphetamine administration facilitates sexual behavior and enhances the associated increase in NAc DA As described for food reward sexual experience can also lead to activation of plasticity related signaling cascades The transcription factor delta FosB is increased in the NAc PFC dorsal striatum and VTA following repeated sexual behavior Wallace et al 2008 Pitchers et al 2010b This natural increase in delta FosB or viral overexpression of delta FosB within the NAc modulates sexual performance and NAc blockade of delta FosB attenuates this behavior Hedges et al 2009 Pitchers et al 2010b Further viral overexpression of delta FosB enhances the conditioned place preference for an environment paired with sexual experience Hedges et al 2009 In some people there is a transition from normal to compulsive engagement in natural rewards such as food or sex a condition that some have termed behavioral or non drug addictions Holden 2001 Grant et al 2006a In humans the role of dopamine signaling in incentive sensitization processes has recently been highlighted by the observation of a dopamine dysregulation syndrome in some patients taking dopaminergic drugs This syndrome is characterized by a medication induced increase in or compulsive engagement in non drug rewards such as gambling shopping or sex Evans et al 2006 Aiken 2007 Lader 2008 Table 1 Summary of plasticity observed following exposure to drug or natural reinforcers a b Bilinski P Wojtyla A Kapka Skrzypczak L Chwedorowicz R Cyranka M Studzinski T 2012 Epigenetic regulation in drug addiction Ann Agric Environ Med 19 3 491 496 PMID 23020045 For these reasons DFosB is considered a primary and causative transcription factor in creating new neural connections in the reward centre prefrontal cortex and other regions of the limbic system This is reflected in the increased stable and long lasting level of sensitivity to cocaine and other drugs and tendency to relapse even after long periods of abstinence These newly constructed networks function very efficiently via new pathways as soon as drugs of abuse are further taken In this way the induction of CDK5 gene expression occurs together with suppression of the G9A gene coding for dimethyltransferase acting on the histone H3 A feedback mechanism can be observed in the regulation of these 2 crucial factors that determine the adaptive epigenetic response to cocaine This depends on DFosB inhibiting G9a gene expression i e H3K9me2 synthesis which in turn inhibits transcription factors for DFosB For this reason the observed hyper expression of G9a which ensures high levels of the dimethylated form of histone H3 eliminates the neuronal structural and plasticity effects caused by cocaine by means of this feedback which blocks DFosB transcription Pitchers KK Vialou V Nestler EJ Laviolette SR Lehman MN Coolen LM February 2013 Natural and drug rewards act on common neural plasticity mechanisms with DFosB as a key mediator The Journal of Neuroscience 33 8 3434 3442 doi 10 1523 JNEUROSCI 4881 12 2013 PMC 3865508 PMID 23426671 Drugs of abuse induce neuroplasticity in the natural reward pathway specifically the nucleus accumbens NAc thereby causing development and expression of addictive behavior Together these findings demonstrate that drugs of abuse and natural reward behaviors act on common molecular and cellular mechanisms of plasticity that control vulnerability to drug addiction and that this increased vulnerability is mediated by DFosB and its downstream transcriptional targets Sexual behavior is highly rewarding Tenk et al 2009 and sexual experience causes sensitized drug related behaviors including cross sensitization to amphetamine Amph induced locomotor activity Bradley and Meisel 2001 Pitchers et al 2010a and enhanced Amph reward Pitchers et al 2010a Moreover sexual experience induces neural plasticity in the NAc similar to that induced by psychostimulant exposure including increased dendritic spine density Meisel and Mullins 2006 Pitchers et al 2010a altered glutamate receptor trafficking and decreased synaptic strength in prefrontal cortex responding NAc shell neurons Pitchers et al 2012 Finally periods of abstinence from sexual experience were found to be critical for enhanced Amph reward NAc spinogenesis Pitchers et al 2010a and glutamate receptor trafficking Pitchers et al 2012 These findings suggest that natural and drug reward experiences share common mechanisms of neural plasticity Beloate LN Weems PW Casey GR Webb IC Coolen LM February 2016 Nucleus accumbens NMDA receptor activation regulates amphetamine cross sensitization and deltaFosB expression following sexual experience in male rats Neuropharmacology 101 154 164 doi 10 1016 j neuropharm 2015 09 023 PMID 26391065 S2CID 25317397 Robison AJ Nestler EJ November 2011 Transcriptional and epigenetic mechanisms of addiction Nat Rev Neurosci 12 11 623 637 doi 10 1038 nrn3111 PMC 3272277 PMID 21989194 DFosB serves as one of the master control proteins governing this structural plasticity DFosB also represses G9a expression leading to reduced repressive histone methylation at the cdk5 gene The net result is gene activation and increased CDK5 expression In contrast DFosB binds to the c fos gene and recruits several co repressors including HDAC1 histone deacetylase 1 and SIRT 1 sirtuin 1 The net result is c fos gene repression Figure 4 Epigenetic basis of drug regulation of gene expression Hitchcock LN Lattal KM 2014 Histone mediated epigenetics in addiction Epigenetics and Neuroplasticity Evidence and Debate Prog Mol Biol Transl Sci Progress in Molecular Biology and Translational Science Vol 128 pp 51 87 doi 10 1016 B978 0 12 800977 2 00003 6 ISBN 9780128009772 PMC 5914502 PMID 25410541 Walker DM Nestler EJ 2018 Neuroepigenetics and addiction Neurogenetics Part II Handbook of Clinical Neurology Vol 148 pp 747 765 doi 10 1016 B978 0 444 64076 5 00048 X ISBN 9780444640765 PMC 5868351 PMID 29478612 Rang HP 2003 Pharmacology Edinburgh Churchill Livingstone p 596 ISBN 978 0 443 07145 4 a b Roy A Wise Drug activation of brain reward pathways Drug and Alcohol Dependence 1998 51 13 22 Goeders N E Smith J E 1983 Cortical dopaminergic involvement in cocaine reinforcement Science 221 4612 773 775 Bibcode 1983Sci 221 773G doi 10 1126 science 6879176 PMID 6879176 Goeders N E Smith J E 1993 Intracranial cocaine self administration into the medial prefrontal cortex increases dopamine turnover in the nucleus accumbens J Pharmacol Exp Ther 265 2 592 600 PMID 8496810 Clarke Hommer D W Pert A Skirboll L R 1985 Electrophysiological actions of nicotine on substantia nigra single units Br J Pharmacol 85 4 827 835 doi 10 1111 j 1476 5381 1985 tb11081 x PMC 1916681 PMID 4041681 Westfall Thomas C Grant Heather Perry Holly January 1983 Release of dopamine and 5 hydroxytryptamine from rat striatal slices following activation of nicotinic cholinergic receptors General Pharmacology The Vascular System 14 3 321 325 doi 10 1016 0306 3623 83 90037 x PMID 6135645 Romer Thomsen K Whybrow PC Kringelbach ML 2015 Reconceptualizing anhedonia novel perspectives on balancing the pleasure networks in the human brain Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 9 49 doi 10 3389 fnbeh 2015 00049 PMC 4356228 PMID 25814941 Thomsen KR 2015 Measuring anhedonia impaired ability to pursue experience and learn about reward Frontiers in Psychology 6 1409 doi 10 3389 fpsyg 2015 01409 PMC 4585007 PMID 26441781 Olney JJ Warlow SM Naffziger EE Berridge KC August 2018 Current perspectives on incentive salience and applications to clinical disorders Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 22 59 69 doi 10 1016 j cobeha 2018 01 007 PMC 5831552 PMID 29503841 Preda Adrian 2014 Brain Imaging Correlates of Anhedonia In Ritsner Michael ed Anhedonia a comprehensive handbook Dordrecht Springer Netherlands ISBN 978 94 017 8590 7 Zhang B Lin P Shi H Ongur D Auerbach RP Wang X Yao S Wang X September 2016 Mapping anhedonia specific dysfunction in a transdiagnostic approach an ALE meta analysis Brain Imaging and Behavior 10 3 920 39 doi 10 1007 s11682 015 9457 6 PMC 4838562 PMID 26487590 Treadway Michael T 2016 The Neurobiology of Motivational Deficits in Depression An Update on Candidate Pathomechanisms In Simpson Eleanor H Balsam Peter D eds Behavioral Neuroscience of Motivation Current Topics in Behavioral Neurosciences 1st ed Switzerland Springer International Publishing p 343 ISBN 978 3 319 26933 7 In a relatively recent literature studies of motivation and reinforcement in depression have been largely consistent in detecting differences as compared to healthy controls Whitton et al 2015 In several studies using the effort expenditure for reward task EEfRT patients with MDD expended less effort for rewards when compared with controls Treadway et al 2012 Yang et al 2014 Salamone JD Yohn SE Lopez Cruz L San Miguel N Correa M May 2016 Activational and effort related aspects of motivation neural mechanisms and implications for psychopathology Brain A Journal of Neurology 139 Pt 5 1325 47 doi 10 1093 brain aww050 PMC 5839596 PMID 27189581 Russo SJ Nestler EJ September 2013 The brain reward circuitry in mood disorders Nature Reviews Neuroscience 14 9 609 25 doi 10 1038 nrn3381 PMC 3867253 PMID 23942470 Treadway MT Zald DH January 2011 Reconsidering anhedonia in depression lessons from translational neuroscience Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 35 3 537 55 doi 10 1016 j neubiorev 2010 06 006 PMC 3005986 PMID 20603146 Walsh JJ Han MH 12 December 2014 The heterogeneity of ventral tegmental area neurons Projection functions in a mood related context Neuroscience 282 101 8 doi 10 1016 j neuroscience 2014 06 006 PMC 4339667 PMID 24931766 Lammel S Lim BK Malenka RC January 2014 Reward and aversion in a heterogeneous midbrain dopamine system Neuropharmacology 76 Pt B 0 351 9 doi 10 1016 j neuropharm 2013 03 019 PMC 3778102 PMID 23578393 Knowland D Lim BK 5 January 2018 Circuit based frameworks of depressive behaviors The role of reward circuitry and beyond Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 174 42 52 doi 10 1016 j pbb 2017 12 010 PMC 6340396 PMID 29309799 a b Lammel S Tye KM Warden MR January 2014 Progress in understanding mood disorders optogenetic dissection of neural circuits Genes Brain and Behavior 13 1 38 51 doi 10 1111 gbb 12049 PMID 23682971 S2CID 18542868 Bucci P Galderisi S May 2017 Categorizing and assessing negative symptoms Current Opinion in Psychiatry 30 3 201 208 doi 10 1097 YCO 0000000000000322 PMID 28212174 S2CID 9923114 They also provide a separate assessment of the consummatory anhedonia reduced experience of pleasure derived from ongoing enjoyable activities and anticipatory anhedonia reduced ability to anticipate future pleasure In fact the former one seems to be relatively intact in schizophrenia whereas the latter one seems to be impaired 32 34 However discrepant data have also been reported 35 Young Anticevic amp Barch 2018 p 215a Several recent reviews e g Cohen and Minor 2010 have found that individuals with schizophrenia show relatively intact self reported emotional responses to affect eliciting stimuli as well as other indicators of intact response A more mixed picture arises from functional neuroimaging studies examining brain responses to other types of pleasurable stimuli in schizophrenia Paradiso et al 2003 Young Anticevic amp Barch 2018 p 215b As such it is surprising that behavioral studies have suggested that reinforcement learning is intact in schizophrenia when learning is relatively implicit though see Siegert et al 2008 for evidence of impaired Serial Reaction Time task learning but more impaired when explicit representations of stimulus reward contingencies are needed see Gold et al 2008 This pattern has given rise to the theory that the striatally mediated gradual reinforcement learning system may be intact in schizophrenia while more rapid on line cortically mediated learning systems are impaired Young Anticevic amp Barch 2018 p 216 We have recently shown that individuals with schizophrenia can show improved cognitive control performance when information about rewards are externally presented but not when they must be internally maintained Mann et al 2013 with some evidence for impairments in DLPFC and striatal activation during internal maintenance of reward information being associated with individuals differences in motivation Chung and Barch 2016 Littman Ph D Ellen February 2017 Never Enough Why ADHD Brains Crave Stimulation ADDditude Magazine New Hope Media LLC Retrieved 27 May 2021 Chandler DJ Waterhouse BD Gao WJ May 2014 New perspectives on catecholaminergic regulation of executive circuits evidence for independent modulation of prefrontal functions by midbrain dopaminergic and noradrenergic neurons Frontiers in Neural Circuits 8 53 doi 10 3389 fncir 2014 00053 PMC 4033238 PMID 24904299 Blum Kenneth Chen Amanda Lih Chuan Braverman Eric R Comings David E Chen Thomas JH Arcuri Vanessa Blum Seth H Downs Bernard W Waite Roger L Notaro Alison Lubar Joel Williams Lonna Prihoda Thomas J Palomo Tomas Oscar Berman Marlene October 2008 Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and reward deficiency syndrome Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment 4 5 893 918 doi 10 2147 ndt s2627 ISSN 1176 6328 PMC 2626918 PMID 19183781 Wise RA 1996 Addictive drugs and brain stimulation reward Annu Rev Neurosci 19 319 340 doi 10 1146 annurev ne 19 030196 001535 PMID 8833446 a b human nervous system Description Development Anatomy amp Function Encyclopedia Britannica a b James Olds and Peter Milner December 1954 Positive reinforcement produced by electrical stimulation of the septal area and other regions of rat brain Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 47 6 419 427 doi 10 1037 h0058775 PMID 13233369 Archived from the original on 5 February 2012 Retrieved 26 April 2011 Kringelbach Morten L Berridge Kent C 25 June 2010 The Functional Neuroanatomy of Pleasure and Happiness Discovery Medicine 9 49 579 587 PMC 3008353 PMID 20587348 via www discoverymedicine com Ivan Petrovich Pavlov G V Anrep 2003 Conditioned Reflexes Courier Corporation pp 1 ISBN 978 0 486 43093 5 Fridlund Alan and James Kalat Mind and Brain the Science of Psychology California Cengage Learning 2014 Print a b Berridge Kent C Kringelbach Morten L August 2008 Affective neuroscience of pleasure reward in humans and animals Psychopharmacology 199 3 457 480 doi 10 1007 s00213 008 1099 6 PMC 3004012 PMID 18311558 a b Ferreri L Mas Herrero E Zatorre RJ Ripolles P Gomez Andres A Alicart H Olive G Marco Pallares J Antonijoan RM Valle M Riba J Rodriguez Fornells A January 2019 Dopamine modulates the reward experiences elicited by music Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 116 9 3793 3798 Bibcode 2019PNAS 116 3793F doi 10 1073 pnas 1811878116 PMC 6397525 PMID 30670642 Listening to pleasurable music is often accompanied by measurable bodily reactions such as goose bumps or shivers down the spine commonly called chills or frissons Overall our results straightforwardly revealed that pharmacological interventions bidirectionally modulated the reward responses elicited by music In particular we found that risperidone impaired participants ability to experience musical pleasure whereas levodopa enhanced it Here in contrast studying responses to abstract rewards in human subjects we show that manipulation of dopaminergic transmission affects both the pleasure i e amount of time reporting chills and emotional arousal measured by EDA and the motivational components of musical reward money willing to spend These findings suggest that dopaminergic signaling is a sine qua non condition not only for motivational responses as has been shown with primary and secondary rewards but also for hedonic reactions to music This result supports recent findings showing that dopamine also mediates the perceived pleasantness attained by other types of abstract rewards and challenges previous findings in animal models on primary rewards such as food a b Goupil L Aucouturier JJ February 2019 Musical pleasure and musical emotions Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 116 9 3364 3366 Bibcode 2019PNAS 116 3364G doi 10 1073 pnas 1900369116 PMC 6397567 PMID 30770455 In a pharmacological study published in PNAS Ferreri et al 1 present evidence that enhancing or inhibiting dopamine signaling using levodopa or risperidone modulates the pleasure experienced while listening to music In a final salvo to establish not only the correlational but also the causal implication of dopamine in musical pleasure the authors have turned to directly manipulating dopaminergic signaling in the striatum first by applying excitatory and inhibitory transcranial magnetic stimulation over their participants left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex a region known to modulate striatal function 5 and finally in the current study by administrating pharmaceutical agents able to alter dopamine synaptic availability 1 both of which influenced perceived pleasure physiological measures of arousal and the monetary value assigned to music in the predicted direction While the question of the musical expression of emotion has a long history of investigation including in PNAS 6 and the 1990s psychophysiological strand of research had already established that musical pleasure could activate the autonomic nervous system 7 the authors demonstration of the implication of the reward system in musical emotions was taken as inaugural proof that these were veridical emotions whose study has full legitimacy to inform the neurobiology of our everyday cognitive social and affective functions 8 Incidentally this line of work culminating in the article by Ferreri et al 1 has plausibly done more to attract research funding for the field of music sciences than any other in this community The evidence of Ferreri et al 1 provides the latest support for a compelling neurobiological model in which musical pleasure arises from the interaction of ancient reward valuation systems striatal limbic paralimbic with more phylogenetically advanced perception predictions systems temporofrontal Young Jared W Anticevic Alan Barch Deanna M 2018 Cognitive and Motivational Neuroscience of Psychotic Disorders In Charney Dennis S Sklar Pamela Buxbaum Joseph D Nestler Eric J eds Charney amp Nestler s Neurobiology of Mental Illness 5th ed New York Oxford University Press ISBN 9780190681425 External links Edit Wikimedia Commons has media related to Reward system Scholarpedia Reward Scholarpedia Reward signals Portals Psychology Biology Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Reward system amp oldid 1148453788 Pleasure centers, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.