fbpx
Wikipedia

Memory consolidation

Memory consolidation is a category of processes that stabilize a memory trace after its initial acquisition.[1] A memory trace is a change in the nervous system caused by memorizing something. Consolidation is distinguished into two specific processes. The first, synaptic consolidation, which is thought to correspond to late-phase long-term potentiation,[2] occurs on a small scale in the synaptic connections and neural circuits within the first few hours after learning. The second process is systems consolidation, occurring on a much larger scale in the brain, rendering hippocampus-dependent memories independent of the hippocampus over a period of weeks to years. Recently, a third process has become the focus of research, reconsolidation, in which previously consolidated memories can be made labile again through reactivation of the memory trace.[3][4]

The line processes to make information memory

History edit

Memory consolidation was first referred to in the writings of the renowned Roman teacher of rhetoric Quintillian. He noted the "curious fact... that the interval of a single night will greatly increase the strength of the memory," and presented the possibility that "... the power of recollection .. undergoes a process of ripening and maturing during the time which intervenes." The process of consolidation was later proposed based on clinical data illustrated in 1882 by Ribot's Law of Regression, "progressive destruction advances progressively from the unstable to the stable". This idea was elaborated on by William H. Burnham a few years later in a paper on amnesia integrating findings from experimental psychology and neurology. Coining of the term "consolidation" is credited to the German researchers Müller and Alfons Pilzecker who rediscovered the concept that memory takes time to fixate or undergo "Konsolidierung" in their studies conducted between 1892 and 1900.[1] The two proposed the perseveration-consolidation hypothesis after they found that new information learned could disrupt information previously learnt if not enough time had passed to allow the old information to be consolidated.[5] This led to the suggestion that new memories are fragile in nature but as time passes they become solidified.[5]

 
Lateral view of the hippocampus which is located in the medial temporal lobe

Systematic studies of anterograde amnesia started to emerge in the 1960s and 1970s. The case of Henry Molaison, formerly known as patient H.M., became a landmark in studies of memory as it relates to amnesia and the removal of the hippocampal zone and sparked massive interest in the study of brain lesions and their effect on memory. After Molaison underwent a bilateral medial temporal lobe resection to alleviate epileptic symptoms the patient began to suffer from memory impairments. Molaison lost the ability to encode and consolidate newly learned information leading researchers to conclude the medial temporal lobe (MTL) was an important structure involved in this process.[6] Molaison also showed signs of retrograde amnesia spanning a period of about 3 years prior to the surgery suggesting that recently acquired memories of as long as a couple years could remain in the MTL prior to consolidation into other brain areas.[7] Research into other patients with resections of the MTL have shown a positive relationship between the degree of memory impairment and the extent of MTL removal which points to a temporal gradient in the consolidating nature of the MTL.[6]

These studies were accompanied by the creation of animal models of human amnesia in an effort to identify brain substrates critical for slow consolidation. Meanwhile, neuropharmacological studies of selected brain areas began to shed light on the molecules possibly responsible for fast consolidation.[1] In recent decades, advancements in cellular preparations, molecular biology, and neurogenetics have revolutionized the study of consolidation. Providing additional support is the study of functional brain activity in humans which has revealed that the activity of brain regions changes over time after a new memory is acquired.[6] This change can occur as quickly as a couple hours after the memory has been encoded suggesting that there is a temporal dimension to the reorganization of the memory as it is represented in the brain.[5]

Synaptic consolidation edit

Synaptic consolidation is one form of memory consolidation seen across all species and long-term memory tasks. Long-term memory, when discussed in the context of synaptic consolidation, is conventionally said to be memory that lasts for at least 24 hours.[1] Synaptic consolidation is achieved faster than systems consolidation (which is assumed to take weeks, months, or even to years to be accomplished[8]). There is evidence to suggest that synaptic consolidation takes place within minutes to hours of memory encoding or learning (shown, for example, in goldfish),[1] and as such it is considered the 'fast' type of consolidation. It is also referred to as 'initial consolidation'.[9] As soon as six hours after training, memories become impervious to interferences that disrupt synaptic consolidation and the formation of long-term memory. Late-phase LTP,[2] the long-lasting form of one of the best understood forms of synaptic plasticity, is thought to be the cellular process underlying synaptic consolidation.

Standard model edit

The standard model of synaptic consolidation suggests that alterations of synaptic protein synthesis and changes in membrane potential are achieved through activating intracellular transduction cascades. These molecular cascades trigger transcription factors that lead to changes in gene expression. The result of the gene expression is the lasting alteration of synaptic proteins, as well as synaptic remodeling and growth. In a short time-frame immediately following learning, the molecular cascade, expression and process of both transcription factors and immediate early genes, are susceptible to disruptions. Disruptions caused by specific drugs, antibodies and gross physical trauma can block the effects of synaptic consolidation.[1]

Long-term potentiation edit

LTP can be thought of as the prolonged strengthening of synaptic transmission,[10] and is known to produce increases in the neurotransmitter production and receptor sensitivity, lasting minutes to even days. The process of LTP is regarded as a contributing factor to synaptic plasticity and in the growth of synaptic strength, which are suggested to underlie memory formation. LTP is also considered to be an important mechanism in terms of maintaining memories within brain regions,[11] and therefore is thought to be involved in learning.[10] There is compelling evidence that LTP is critical for Pavlovian fear conditioning in rats suggesting that it mediates learning and memory in mammals. Specifically, NMDA-receptor antagonists appear to block the induction of both LTP and fear conditioning and that fear conditioning increases amygdaloidal synaptic transmission that would result in LTP.[12]

Spacing effect edit

Distributed learning has been found to enhance memory consolidation, specifically for relational memory. Experimental results suggest that distributing learning over the course of 24 hours decreases the rate of forgetting compared to massed learning, and enhances relational memory consolidation. When interpreted in the context of synaptic consolidation, mechanisms of synaptic strengthening may depend on the spacing of memory reactivation to allow sufficient time for protein synthesis to occur, and thereby strengthen long-term memory.[13]

One study that demonstrates this effect was conducted in 1984 by Smith and Rothkopf.[14] In this experiment, subjects were sorted into three groups to test retention and learning. "Each group was taught the same 8 hour statistics class, but one group was taught the class in one day, the next over the course of four days in one room, and the last was taught the class over the course of four days in different rooms. The subjects were tested five days later in a completely new setting. The results of the experiment were that taking the class over a period of four days was much more effective than taking it in one single mass. Interestingly, the group that took the course over four days and in different rooms performed the best in the final retention test out of all the groups."[14] This shows that spacing out study sessions and studying in different environments helps with retention as it provides time for the brain to consolidate the information without being interrupted by new information. The benefits of spacing were also demonstrated in an earlier study by Reder and Anderson (1982) which yielded similar results, confirming the spacing effect’s relevance and effects on learning.[15][16]

Protein synthesis edit

Protein synthesis plays an important role in the formation of new memories. Studies have shown that protein synthesis inhibitors administered after learning, weaken memory, suggesting that protein synthesis is required for memory consolidation. Additionally, reports have suggested that the effects of protein synthesis inhibitors also inhibit LTP.[17] However, other results have shown that protein synthesis may not in fact be necessary for memory consolidation, as it has been found that the formation of memories can withstand vast amounts of protein synthesis inhibition, suggesting that this criterion of protein synthesis as necessary for memory consolidation is not unconditional.[17]

Systems consolidation edit

Systems consolidation is the second form of memory consolidation. It is a reorganization process in which memories from the hippocampal region, where memories are first encoded, are moved to the neo-cortex in a more permanent form of storage.[8][18] Systems consolidation is a slow dynamic process that can take anywhere from one to two decades to be fully formed in humans, unlike synaptic consolidation that only takes minutes to hours for new information to stabilize into memories.[18]

Standard model edit

The standard model of systems consolidation has been summarized by Squire and Alvarez (1995);[19] it states that when novel information is originally encoded and registered, memory of these new stimuli becomes retained in both the hippocampus and cortical regions.[20] Later the hippocampus' representations of this information become active in explicit (conscious) recall or implicit (unconscious) recall like in sleep and 'offline' processes.[1]

Memory is retained in the hippocampus for up to one week after initial learning, representing the hippocampus-dependent stage.[20] During this stage the hippocampus is 'teaching' the cortex more and more about the information and when the information is recalled it strengthens the cortico-cortical connection thus making the memory hippocampus-independent.[1] Therefore, from one week and beyond the initial training experience, the memory is slowly transferred to the neo-cortex where it becomes permanently stored.[1] In this view the hippocampus can perform the task of storing memories temporarily because the synapses are able to change quickly whereas the neocortical synapses change over time.[19] Consolidation is thus the process whereby the hippocampus activates the neocortex continually leading to strong connections between the two. Since the hippocampus can only support memories temporarily the remaining activation will be seen only in the neocortex which is able to support memory indefinitely. Squire and Alvarez took the temporally graded nature of patients with retrograde amnesia as support for the notion that once a connection has been established within the neocortex the hippocampus is no longer required, but this process is dynamic and extends for several years.

Squire and Alvarez also proposed the idea that MTL structures play a role in the consolidation of memories within the neocortex by providing a binding area for multiple cortical regions involved in the initial encoding of the memory.[19] In this sense the MTL would act as a relay station for the various perceptual input that make up a memory and stores it as a whole event. After this has occurred the MTL directs information towards the neocortex to provide a permanent representation of the memory.

Multiple trace theory edit

Multiple trace theory (MTT) builds on the distinction between semantic memory and episodic memory and addresses perceived shortcomings of the standard model with respect to the dependency of the hippocampus. MTT argues that the hippocampus is always involved in the retrieval and storage of episodic memories.[21] It is thought that semantic memories, including basic information encoded during the storage of episodic memories, can be established in structures apart from the hippocampal system such as the neo-cortex in the process of consolidation.[21] Hence, while proper hippocampal functioning is necessary for the retention and retrieval of episodic memories, it is less necessary during the encoding and use of semantic memories. As memories age there are long-term interactions between the hippocampus and neo-cortex and this leads to the establishment of aspects of memory within structures aside from the hippocampus.[21] MTT thus states that both episodic and semantic memories rely on the hippocampus and the latter becomes somewhat independent of the hippocampus during consolidation.[21] An important distinction between MTT and the standard model is that the standard model proposes that all memories become independent of the hippocampus after several years. However, Nadel and Moscovitch have shown that the hippocampus was involved in memory recall for all remote autobiographical memories no matter of their age.[21] An important point they make while interpreting the results is that activation in the hippocampus was equally as strong regardless of the fact that the memories recalled were as old as 45 years prior to the date of the experiment.[21] This is complicated by the fact that the hippocampus is constantly involved in the encoding of new events and activation due to this fact is hard to separate using baseline measures.[21] Because of this, activation of the hippocampus during retrieval of distant memories may simply be a by-product of the subject encoding the study as an event.[21]

Criticisms edit

Haist, Gore, and Mao, sought to examine the temporal nature of consolidation within the hippocampus to test MTT against the standard view.[22] They found that the hippocampus does not substantially contribute to the recollection of remote memories after a period of a few years. They claim that advances in the functional magnetic resonance imaging have allowed them to improve their distinction between the hippocampus and the entorhinal cortex which they claim is more enduring in its activation from remote memory retrieval.[22] They also criticize the use of memories during testing which cannot be confirmed as accurate.[22] Finally, they state that the initial interview in the scanner acted as an encoding event as such differences between recent and remote memories would be obscured.[22]

Semantic vs. episodic memory edit

Nadel and Moscovitch argued that when studying the structures and systems involved in memory consolidation, semantic memory and episodic memory need to be distinguished as relying on two different memory systems. When episodic information is encoded there are semantic aspects of the memory that are encoded as well and this is proposed as an explanation of the varying gradients of memory loss seen in amnesic patients.[21] Amnesic patients with hippocampal damage show traces of memories and this has been used as support for the standard model because it suggests that memories are retained apart from the hippocampal system.[21] Nadel and Moscovitch argue that these retained memories have lost the richness of experience and exist as depersonalized events that have been semanticized over time.[21] They suggest that this instead provides support for their notion that episodic memories rely significantly on the hippocampal system but semantic memories can be established elsewhere in the brain and survive hippocampal damage.[21]

Declarative vs. procedural knowledge consolidation edit

Learning can be distinguished by two forms of knowledge: declarative and procedural. Declarative information includes the conscious recall of facts, episodes, and lists, and its storage typically connected with the mediotemporal lobe and the hippocampal systems as it includes the encoding of both semantic and episodic information of events. Procedural knowledge however has been said to function separate from this system as it relies primarily on motor areas of the brain.[23] The implicit nature of procedural knowledge allows it to exist absent from the conscious awareness that the information is there. Amnesic patients have shown retained ability to be trained on tasks and exhibit learning without the subject being aware that the training had ever taken place.[23] This introduces a dissociation between the two forms of memory and the fact that one form can exist absent the other suggests separate mechanisms are involved in consolidation. Squire has proposed the procedural knowledge is consolidated in some cases by the extrapyramidal motor system.[23] Squire demonstrated that intact learning of certain motor, perceptual, and cognitive skills can be retained in patients with amnesia.[23] They also retain the ability to be influenced by priming effects without the patients being able to consciously recall any training session occurring.[23]

Emotional and stressful memory consolidation edit

The amygdala, specifically the basolateral region (BLA) is involved in the encoding of significant experiences and has been directly linked to memorable events.[5] Extensive evidence suggests that stress hormones such as epinephrine play a critical role in consolidating new memories and this is why stressful memories are recalled vividly.[24] Studies by Gold and van Buskirk provided initial evidence for this relationship when they showed that injections of epinephrine into subjects following a training period resulted in greater long-term retention of task related memories.[25][26] This study also provided evidence that the level of epinephrine injected was related to the level of retention suggesting that the level of stress or emotionality of the memory plays a role on the level of retention. It is suggested that epinephrine affects memory consolidation by activating the amygdala and studies have shown that antagonism of beta-andrenoreceptors prior to injection of epinephrine will block the retention of memory effects seen previously.[27][28] This is supported by the fact that beta-adrenoreceptor agonists have the opposite effect on the enhancement of memory consolidation.[27][28] The BLA is thought to be actively involved in memory consolidation and is influenced strongly by stress hormones resulting in increased activation and as such increased memory retention.[24] The BLA then projects to the hippocampus resulting in a strengthened memory.[5] This relationship was studied by Packard and Chen who found that when glutamate was administered to the hippocampus, enhanced consolidation was seen during food-rewarded maze tasks.[29] The opposite effect was also seen when the amygdala was inactivated using lidocaine.[29] Studies appear to suggest that the amygdala effects the consolidation of memories through its influence with stress hormones and the projections to other brain areas implicated in memory consolidation.[5]

Sleep consolidation edit

Rapid eye movement (REM) sleep has been thought of to be an important concept in the overnight learning in humans by establishing information in the hippocampal and cortical regions of the brain.[30] REM sleep elicits an increase in neuronal activity following an enriched or novel waking experience, thus increasing neuronal plasticity and therefore playing an essential role in the consolidation of memories.[31] This has come into question in recent years however and studies on sleep deprivation have shown that animals and humans who are denied REM sleep do not show deficits in task learning. It has been proposed that since the brain is in a non-memory encoding state during sleep, consolidation would be unlikely to occur.[32]

More recent studies, however, have looked at the relationship between slow-wave sleep and memory consolidation, rather than REM sleep. One study found that low levels of acetylcholine found in the central nervous system, which are present during slow-wave sleep, aid in the consolidation of memories and therefore assist in the learning process.[33][34]

Recent studies have examined the relationship between REM sleep and procedural learning consolidation. In particular studies have been done on sensory and motor related tasks. In one study testing finger-tapping, people were split into two groups and tested post-training with or without intervening sleep; results concluded that sleep post-training increases both speed and accuracy in this particular task, while increasing the activation of both cortical and hippocampal regions; whereas the post-training awake group had no such improvements.[30] It has been theorized that this may be related more-so to a process of synaptic consolidation rather than systems consolidation because of the short-term nature of the process involved.[32] Researchers examining the effect of sleep on motor learning have noted that while consolidation occurs over a period of 4–6 hours during sleep, this is also true during waking hours, which may negate any role of sleep in learning.[32] In this sense sleep would serve no special purpose to enhance consolidation of memories because it occurs independently of sleep. Other studies have examined the process of replay which has been described as a reactivation of patterns that were stimulated during a learning phase. Replay has been demonstrated in the hippocampus and this has lent support to the notion that it serves a consolidation purpose.[32] However, replay is not specific to sleep and both rats and primates show signs during restful-awake periods.[32] Also, replay may simply be residual activation in areas that were involved previously in the learning phase and may have no actual effect on consolidation.[32] This reactivation of the memory traces has also been seen in non-REM sleep specifically for hippocampus-dependant memories.[35] Researchers have noted strong reactivation of the hippocampus during sleep immediately after a learning task. This reactivation led to enhanced performance on the learned task.[35] One such experiment had participants learn word pair associations (declarative memories) before either retention periods of sleep or periods of wakefulness. Researchers found that retrieval expectancy played a role as to whether participants were able to retain the information, as the participants that had been told about the delayed retrieval test performed better. However, their research showed that sleep was more likely to benefit the consolidation of memories if the information was relevant to future events or behaviors.[36] Researchers following this line of work have come to assume that dreams are a by-product of the reactivation of the brain areas and this can explain why dreams may be unrelated to the information being consolidated.[35] The dream experience itself is not what enhances memory performance but rather it is the reactivation of the neural circuits that causes this. Other researchers have looked at the role growth hormones play in the consolidation of memories, particularly those of procedural and declarative memories. They found that although growth hormones support general brain systems and memory functioning, it is still unclear if growth hormones play a role in the formation and processing of particular memories during sleep periods.[33]

Cortical slow oscillation- and spindle-complexes edit

Memory consolidation during sleep via reactivation of prior experiences and information is associated with sleep signatures of cortical "slow oscillations" and sleep spindles that are involved in the information flow between relevant brain areas. A more complete understanding of these mechanics may possibly allow purposely enabling or strengthening this reactivation.[37][38]

Zif268 & REM sleep edit

Zif268 is an Immediate early gene (IEG) thought to be involved in neuroplasticity by an up-regulation of the transcription factor during REM sleep after pre-exposure to an enriched environment.[31] Results from studies testing the effects of zif268 on mice brains postmortem, suggest that a waking experience prior to sleep can have an enduring effect in the brain, due to an increase of neuroplasticity.[31]

Reconsolidation edit

Memory reconsolidation is the process of previously consolidated memories being recalled and actively consolidated.[10] It is a distinct process that serves to maintain, strengthen and modify memories that are already stored in the long-term memory. Once memories undergo the process of consolidation and become part of long-term memory, they are thought of as stable. However, the retrieval of a memory trace can cause another labile phase that then requires an active process to make the memory stable after retrieval is complete.[10] It is believed that post-retrieval stabilization is different and distinct from consolidation, despite its overlap in function (e.g. storage) and its mechanisms (e.g. protein synthesis). Memory modification needs to be demonstrated in the retrieval in order for this independent process to be valid.[10]

History edit

The theory of reconsolidation has been debated for many years and is still controversial. Reconsolidation was first conceptualized in light of the discovery that phobias could often be eliminated by means of electroconvulsive shock therapy (ECT).[39] This seemed to indicate the involvement of a re-consolidation process for excited memories, and that the operation active in ECT was the disruption of that process; here, of the reconsolidation of retrieved fear memories by shock administration.

Further studies investigated the concept,[10] using ECT to test for reconsolidation; ECT was already known as an amnesic agent (leads to memory loss). These studies found it to be effective on retrieved memories when administered directly after the retrieval of a memory.[1]

Later research, wherein fear memories had been established in rats through Pavlovian fear conditioning, found that a consolidated fear memory can be brought to a labile state, by means of immediate amygdala infusions of the protein synthesis inhibitor anisomycin, but not by infusions made six hours afterwards.[3] It was concluded that consolidated fear memory, when reactivated, enters a changeable state that requires de novo protein synthesis for new consolidation, i.e., re-consolidation of the old memory.[3] Nader, Schafe, and Le Doux (2000) demonstrated that the reconsolidation process may make memories more malleable than previously believed.[40][16] Nader and his colleagues trained rats to be afraid of a tone by pairing the tone with a small shock. Groups of rats were then injected with anisomycin, an antibiotic that restricts protein synthesis, at different points in time. The rats that were injected with anisomycin after consolidation had taken place, retained the fear reaction to the tone. However, the rats that were injected before consolidation and reconsolidation could take place, did not retain the fear response when they heard the tone again later. It seems that interference that is made before memories are consolidated affect the way they are remembered later.

Brunet and colleagues (2008) studied patients that had been diagnosed with Post Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD). [41] Following the same method that Nader and his associates used, Brunet induced anxiety responses in the patients by having them listen to a 30 second recording describing the circumstances of their traumatic experiences. The patients were shortly thereafter injected with propranolol, a drug that blocks stress hormone receptors in the amygdala which is implicated in neurologically representing the emotional content of memories. These patients experienced a significant reduction in PTSD symptoms months after treatment. These findings were confirmed in later studies done in 2009 by Kindt and colleagues and in 2010 by Schiller and colleagues. [42][43]

These studies done by Nader and others seem to suggest that as memories are being remembered, they are fragile, as if experiencing them for the first time.

In addition to fear memories, appetitive memories are also prone to reconsolidation episodes, which can likewise be disrupted; namely, after local administration of a protein activity inhibitor.[44]

Since those breakthrough studies were done, there have been several others to probe the theory of memory reconsolidation. Subjects in these studies, along with humans, have included crabs, chicks, honeybees, medaka fish, lymnaea, and various rodents.[10] Further studies have demonstrated an analogue of memory reconsolidation in spinal cord pain processing pathways, suggesting a general role for reconsolidation in the central nervous system.[45]

Criticisms edit

Some studies have supported this theory, while others have failed to demonstrate disruption of consolidated memory after retrieval. It is important to note that negative results may be examples of conditions where memories are not susceptible to a permanent disruption, thus a determining factor of reconsolidation.[10] After much debate and a detailed review of this field it had been concluded that reconsolidation was a real phenomenon.[4] Tronson and Taylor compiled a lengthy summary of multiple reconsolidation studies, noting a number of studies were unable to show memory impairments due to blocked reconsolidation. However the need for standardized methods was underscored as in some learning tasks such as fear conditioning, certain forms of memory reactivation could actually represent new extinction learning rather than activation of an old memory trace. Under this possibility, traditional disruptions of reconsolidation might actually maintain the original memory trace but preventing the consolidation of extinction learning.[10] Recent work has suggested that epigenetic modifications may also prevent reconsolidation in some cases.[46] The removal of these epigenetic modifications with inhibitors of histone deacetylase enabled the erasure of remote memories after recall.

Reconsolidation experiments are more difficult to run than typical consolidation experiments as disruption of a previously consolidated memory must be shown to be specific to the reactivation of the original memory trace. Furthermore, it is important to demonstrate that the vulnerability of reactivation occurs in a limited time frame, which can be assessed by delaying infusion till six hours after reactivation. It is also useful to show that the behavioral measure used to assess disruption of memory is not just due to task impairment caused by the procedure, which can be demonstrated by testing control groups in absence of the original learning. Finally, it is important to rule out alternative explanations, such as extinction learning by lengthening the reactivation phase.[10] There have also been concerns about the use of reconsolidation research to justify psychotherapy treatments, and the generalizability of basic reconsolidation research into the therapy room[47]

Distinctions from consolidation edit

Questions arose if reconsolidation was a unique process or merely another phase of consolidation. Both consolidation and reconsolidation can be disrupted by pharmacological agents (e.g. the protein synthesis inhibitor anisomycin) and both require the transcription factor CREB. However, recent amygdala research suggests that BDNF is required for consolidation (but not reconsolidation) whereas the transcription factor and immediate early gene Zif268 is required for reconsolidation but not consolidation.[48] A similar double dissociation between Zif268 for reconsolidation and BDNF for consolidation was found in the hippocampus for fear conditioning.[49] However, not all memory tasks show this double dissociation, such as object recognition memory.[50]

Reconsolidation in psychotherapy edit

In the decade between 2005 and 2015, at least five groups argued the notion that memory reconsolidation can be used to treat psychological problems.[51][52][53][54][55] Three of these groups have proposed that the wide variety of different psychotherapies produce permanent change in clients to the extent that they manage to activate this same neurobiological mechanism of reconsolidation in a way that leads to deconsolidation.[53][54][56] One example of this is the Lefkoe Method, created in 1985 by Morty Lefkoe, president and founder of the Lefkoe Institute.[57][58][59] Memory reconsolidation may be a common factor in many forms of psychotherapy.[56]

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ a b c d e f g h i j Dudai, Y. (2004). "The Neurobiology of Consolidations, Or, How Stable is the Engram?". Annual Review of Psychology. 55: 51–86. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.142050. PMID 14744210.
  2. ^ a b Bramham, C. R.; Messaoudi, E. (2005). "BDNF function in adult synaptic plasticity: The synaptic consolidation hypothesis". Progress in Neurobiology. 76 (2): 99–125. doi:10.1016/j.pneurobio.2005.06.003. PMID 16099088. S2CID 22770640.
  3. ^ a b c Nader, K.; Schafe, G. E.; LeDoux, J. E. (2000). "Fear memories require protein synthesis in the amygdala for reconsolidation after retrieval". Nature. 406 (6797): 722–726. Bibcode:2000Natur.406..722N. doi:10.1038/35021052. PMID 10963596. S2CID 4420637.
  4. ^ a b Sara, SJ (2000). "Retrieval and reconsolidation: toward a neurobiology of remembering". Learn. Mem. 7 (2): 73–84. doi:10.1101/lm.7.2.73. PMID 10753974.
  5. ^ a b c d e f McGaugh, J. L. (2000). "Memory--a Century of Consolidation". Science. 287 (5451): 248–251. Bibcode:2000Sci...287..248M. doi:10.1126/science.287.5451.248. PMID 10634773.
  6. ^ a b c Scoville, W. B.; Milner, B. (1957). "Loss of Recent Memory After Bilateral Hippocampal Lesions". Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry. 20 (1): 11–21. doi:10.1136/jnnp.20.1.11. PMC 497229. PMID 13406589.
  7. ^ Milner, B.; Corkin, S.; Teuber, H. -L. (1968). "Further analysis of the hippocampal amnesic syndrome: 14-year follow-up study of H.M". Neuropsychologia. 6 (3): 215–234. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(68)90021-3.
  8. ^ a b Dudai, Y.; Karni, A.; Born, J. (2015). "The consolidation and transformation of memory". Neuron. 88 (1): 20–32. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.004. PMID 26447570.
  9. ^ Okuda, K.; Højgaard, K.; Privitera, R.; Bayraktar, G.; Takeuchi, T. (2020). "Initial memory consolidation and the synaptic tagging and capture hypothesis". European Journal of Neuroscience. 54 (8): 6826–6849. doi:10.1111/ejn.14902. PMID 32649022. S2CID 220469574.
  10. ^ a b c d e f g h i j Tronson, N. C.; Taylor, J. R. (2007). "Molecular mechanisms of memory reconsolidation". Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 8 (4): 262–275. doi:10.1038/nrn2090. PMID 17342174. S2CID 1835412.
  11. ^ Spencer, J. P. E. (2008). "Food for thought: The role of dietary flavonoids in enhancing human memory, learning and neuro-cognitive performance". Proceedings of the Nutrition Society. 67 (2): 238–252. doi:10.1017/S0029665108007088. PMID 18412998.
  12. ^ Maren, S. (1999). "Long-term potentiation in the amygdala: A mechanism for emotional learning and memory" (PDF). Trends in Neurosciences. 22 (12): 561–567. doi:10.1016/S0166-2236(99)01465-4. hdl:2027.42/56238. PMID 10542437. S2CID 18787168.
  13. ^ Litman, L.; Davachi, L. (2008). "Distributed learning enhances relational memory consolidation". Learn. Mem. 15 (9): 711–716. doi:10.1101/lm.1132008. PMID 18772260.
  14. ^ a b Smith, Steven M.; Rothkopf, Ernst Z. (1984). "Contextual Enrichment and Distribution of Practice in the Classroom". Cognition and Instruction. Informa UK Limited. 1 (3): 341–358. doi:10.1207/s1532690xci0103_4. ISSN 0737-0008.
  15. ^ Reder, Lynne M.; Anderson, John R. (1982). "Effects of spacing and embellishment on memory for the main points of a text". Memory & Cognition. Springer Science and Business Media LLC. 10 (2): 97–102. doi:10.3758/bf03209210. ISSN 0090-502X. PMID 7087784. S2CID 46236858.
  16. ^ a b Goldstein, E.B. (2014). Cognitive Psychology: Connecting Mind, Research and Everyday Experience (4th ed.). Cengage Learning. ISBN 978-1-305-17699-7.
  17. ^ a b Gold, P. E. (2008). "Protein synthesis inhibition and memory: Formation vs amnesia". Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 89 (3): 201–211. doi:10.1016/j.nlm.2007.10.006. PMC 2346577. PMID 18054504.
  18. ^ a b Roediger, H.L.; Dudai, Y.; Fitzpatrick, S.M. (2007). Science of Memory: Concepts. Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-972751-3.
  19. ^ a b c Squire, L. R.; Alvarez, P. (1995). "Retrograde amnesia and memory consolidation: A neurobiological perspective". Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 5 (2): 169–177. doi:10.1016/0959-4388(95)80023-9. PMID 7620304. S2CID 9080102.
  20. ^ a b Frankland, P. W.; Bontempi, B. (2005). "The organization of recent and remote memories". Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 6 (2): 119–130. doi:10.1038/nrn1607. PMID 15685217. S2CID 1115019.
  21. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l Nadel, L.; Moscovitch, M. (1997). "Memory consolidation, retrograde amnesia and the hippocampal complex". Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 7 (2): 217–227. doi:10.1016/S0959-4388(97)80010-4. PMID 9142752. S2CID 4802179.
  22. ^ a b c d Haist, F.; Bowden Gore, J. B.; Mao, H. (2001). "Consolidation of human memory over decades revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging". Nature Neuroscience. 4 (11): 1139–1145. doi:10.1038/nn739. PMID 11600889. S2CID 24890684.
  23. ^ a b c d e Squire, L. R. (1986). "Mechanisms of memory". Science. 232 (4758): 1612–1619. Bibcode:1986Sci...232.1612S. doi:10.1126/science.3086978. PMID 3086978.
  24. ^ a b McGaugh, J. L.; Roozendaal, B. (2002). "Role of adrenal stress hormones in forming lasting memories in the brain". Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 12 (2): 205–210. doi:10.1016/S0959-4388(02)00306-9. PMID 12015238. S2CID 10480860.
  25. ^ Gold, Paul E.; Van Buskirk, Roderick B. (February 1975). "Facilitation of time-dependent memory processes with posttrial epinephrine injections". Behavioral Biology. 13 (2): 145–153. doi:10.1016/S0091-6773(75)91784-8. PMID 1122202.
  26. ^ Gold, P. E.; McIntyre, C.; McNay, E.; Stefani, M.; Korol, D. L. (2001). "Neurochemical referees of dueling memory systems". Memory consolidation: Essays in honor of James L. McGaugh. pp. 219. doi:10.1037/10413-012. ISBN 978-1-55798-783-9.
  27. ^ a b Liang, KC; Juler, RG; McGaugh, JL (March 1986). "Modulating effects of posttraining epinephrine on memory: involvement of the amygdala noradrenergic system" (PDF). Brain Res. 368 (1): 125–33. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(86)91049-8. PMID 3955350. S2CID 12361503.
  28. ^ a b Liang, KC; McGaugh, JL; Yao, HY (February 1990). "Involvement of amygdala pathways in the influence of post-training intra-amygdala norepinephrine and peripheral epinephrine on memory storage" (PDF). Brain Res. 508 (2): 225–33. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(90)90400-6. PMID 2306613. S2CID 8665059.
  29. ^ a b Packard, Mark G; Chen, Scott A (September 1999). "The basolateral amygdala is a cofactor in memory enhancement produced by intrahippocampal glutamate injections". Psychobiology. 27 (3): 377–385. doi:10.3758/BF03332131. S2CID 143135085.
  30. ^ a b Walker, M.P.; Stickgold, R.; Alsop, D.; Gaab, N.; Schlaug, G. (2005). "Sleep-dependent motor memory plasticity in the human brain". Neuroscience. 133 (4): 911–917. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.471.2164. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.04.007. PMID 15964485. S2CID 3167159.
  31. ^ a b c Ribeiro, S. (1999). "Brain Gene Expression During REM Sleep Depends on Prior Waking Experience". Learning & Memory. 6 (5): 500–510. doi:10.1101/lm.6.5.500. PMC 311304. PMID 10541470.
  32. ^ a b c d e f Vertes, R. P. (2004). "Memory Consolidation in Sleep". Neuron. 44 (1): 135–148. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2004.08.034. PMID 15450166. S2CID 919482.
  33. ^ a b Gais, Steffen; Hüllemann, Philipp; Hallschmid, Manfred; Born, Jan (2006). "Sleep-dependent surges in growth hormone do not contribute to sleep-dependent memory consolidation". Psychoneuroendocrinology. Elsevier BV. 31 (6): 786–791. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2006.02.009. ISSN 0306-4530. PMID 16621327. S2CID 14639873.
  34. ^ Rasch, Björn H.; Born, Jan; Gais, Steffen (2006-05-01). "Combined Blockade of Cholinergic Receptors Shifts the Brain from Stimulus Encoding to Memory Consolidation". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 18 (5): 793–802. doi:10.1162/jocn.2006.18.5.793. ISSN 0898-929X. PMID 16768378. S2CID 7584537.
  35. ^ a b c Wamsley, E. J.; Tucker, M.; Payne, J. D.; Benavides, J. A.; Stickgold, R. (2010). "Dreaming of a Learning Task is Associated with Enhanced Sleep-Dependent Memory Consolidation". Current Biology. 20 (9): 850–855. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2010.03.027. PMC 2869395. PMID 20417102.
  36. ^ Wilhelm, Ines; Diekelmann, Susanne; Molzow, Ina; Ayoub, Amr; Mölle, Matthias; Born, Jan (2011-02-02). "Sleep Selectively Enhances Memory Expected to Be of Future Relevance". Journal of Neuroscience. 31 (5): 1563–1569. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3575-10.2011. ISSN 0270-6474. PMC 6623736. PMID 21289163.
  37. ^ "Press (re)play to remember: How the brain strengthens memories during sleep". medicalxpress.com. Retrieved 14 June 2021.
  38. ^ Schreiner, Thomas; Petzka, Marit; Staudigl, Tobias; Staresina, Bernhard P. (2021-05-25). "Endogenous memory reactivation during sleep in humans is clocked by slow oscillation-spindle complexes". Nature Communications. 12 (1): 3112. Bibcode:2021NatCo..12.3112S. doi:10.1038/s41467-021-23520-2. ISSN 2041-1723. PMC 8149676. PMID 34035303.   Available under CC BY 4.0.
  39. ^ Solyom, L.; Kenny, F.; Ledwidge, B. (1969). "Psychotherapy: Evaluation of a New Treatment Paradigm for Phobias". Canadian Psychiatric Association Journal. 14 (1): 3–9. doi:10.1177/070674376901400102. ISSN 0008-4824. PMID 4388484.
  40. ^ Nader, Karim; Schafe, Glenn E.; LeDoux, Joseph E. (2000). "The labile nature of consolidation theory". Nature Reviews Neuroscience. Springer Science and Business Media LLC. 1 (3): 216–219. doi:10.1038/35044580. ISSN 1471-003X. PMID 11257912. S2CID 5765968.
  41. ^ Brunet, Alain; Orr, Scott P.; Tremblay, Jacques; Robertson, Kate; Nader, Karim; Pitman, Roger K. (2008). "Effect of post-retrieval propranolol on psychophysiologic responding during subsequent script-driven traumatic imagery in post-traumatic stress disorder". Journal of Psychiatric Research. Elsevier BV. 42 (6): 503–506. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychires.2007.05.006. ISSN 0022-3956. PMID 17588604.
  42. ^ Kindt, Merel; Soeter, Marieke; Vervliet, Bram (2009-02-15). "Beyond extinction: erasing human fear responses and preventing the return of fear". Nature Neuroscience. Springer Science and Business Media LLC. 12 (3): 256–258. doi:10.1038/nn.2271. ISSN 1097-6256. PMID 19219038. S2CID 5948908.
  43. ^ Schiller, Daniela; Monfils, Marie-H.; Raio, Candace M.; Johnson, David C.; LeDoux, Joseph E.; Phelps, Elizabeth A. (2009-12-09). "Preventing the return of fear in humans using reconsolidation update mechanisms". Nature. Springer Science and Business Media LLC. 463 (7277): 49–53. doi:10.1038/nature08637. ISSN 0028-0836. PMC 3640262. PMID 20010606.
  44. ^ Crespo, J.A.; Stöckl, P.; Ueberall, F.; Marcel, J.; Saria, A.; Zernig, G. (February 2012). "Activation of PKCzeta and PKMzeta in the nucleus accumbens core is necessary for the retrieval, consolidation and reconsolidation of the drug memory". PLOS ONE. 7 (2): e30502. Bibcode:2012PLoSO...730502C. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0030502. PMC 3277594. PMID 22348011.
  45. ^ Bonin R. P. & De Koninck Y. (2014). "A spinal analogue of memory reconsolidation enables the erasure of hyperalgesia". Nat Neurosci. 17 (8): 1043–1045. doi:10.1038/nn.3758. PMC 4978538. PMID 24997764.
  46. ^ Gräff J; Joseph NF; Horn ME; Samiei A; Meng J; Seo J; Rei D; Bero AW; Phan TX; Wagner F; Holson E; Xu J; Sun J; Neve RL; Mach RH; Haggarty SJ; Tsai LH. (Jan 2014). "Epigenetic priming of memory updating during reconsolidation to attenuate remote fear memories". Cell. 156 (1–2): 261–276. doi:10.1016/j.cell.2013.12.020. PMC 3986862. PMID 24439381.
  47. ^ Patihis, L. (2015). "Let's be skeptical about reconsolidation and emotional arousal in therapy". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 38: e21. doi:10.1017/S0140525X14000272. PMID 26050685. SSRN 2677025.
  48. ^ Debiec, J.; Doyere, V.; Nader, K.; LeDoux, J.E. (2006). "Directly reactivated, but not indirectly reactivated, memories undergo reconsolidation in the amygdala". PNAS. 103 (9): 3428–3433. Bibcode:2006PNAS..103.3428D. doi:10.1073/pnas.0507168103. PMC 1413871. PMID 16492789.
  49. ^ Lee, J. L.; Everitt, B. J.; Thomas, K. L. (2004). "Independent cellular processes for hippocampal memory consolidation and reconsolidation". Science. 304 (5672): 839–843. Bibcode:2004Sci...304..839L. doi:10.1126/science.1095760. PMID 15073322. S2CID 24194409.
  50. ^ Bozon, B.; Davis, S.; Laroche, S. (2003). "A requirement for the immediate early gene zif268 in reconsolidation of recognition memory after retrieval". Neuron. 40 (4): 695–701. doi:10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00674-3. PMID 14622575. S2CID 17160003.
  51. ^ Centonze, Diego; Siracusano, Alberto; Calabresi, Paolo; Bernardi, Giorgio (October 2005). "Removing pathogenic memories: a neurobiology of psychotherapy". Molecular Neurobiology. 32 (2): 123–132. doi:10.1385/MN:32:2:123. PMID 16215277. S2CID 20176022.
  52. ^ Ecker, Bruce (September 2008). "Unlocking the emotional brain: finding the neural key to transformation". Psychotherapy Networker. 32 (5).
  53. ^ a b Welling, Hans (June 2012). "Transformative emotional sequence: towards a common principle of change" (PDF). Journal of Psychotherapy Integration. 22 (2): 109–136. doi:10.1037/a0027786.
  54. ^ a b Lane, Richard D.; Ryan, Lee; Nadel, Lynn; Greenberg, Leslie S. (2015). "Memory reconsolidation, emotional arousal and the process of change in psychotherapy: new insights from brain science" (PDF). Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 38: e1. doi:10.1017/S0140525X14000041. PMID 24827452.
  55. ^ Schiller, Daniela; Monfils, Marie-H.; Raio, Candace M.; Johnson, David C.; LeDoux, Joseph E.; Phelps, Elizabeth A. (January 2010). "Preventing the return of fear in humans using reconsolidation update mechanisms". Nature. 463 (7277): 49–53. Bibcode:2010Natur.463...49S. doi:10.1038/nature08637. PMC 3640262. PMID 20010606.
  56. ^ a b Ecker, Bruce; Ticic, Robin; Hulley, Laurel (2012). Unlocking the Emotional Brain: Eliminating Symptoms at Their Roots Using Memory Reconsolidation. New York: Routledge. ISBN 9780415897167. OCLC 772112300. But for a more hesitant view of the role of memory reconsolidation in psychotherapy that criticizes some of the claims of Ecker et al., see: Alberini, Cristina M. (April 2015). "Commentary on Tuch". Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association. 63 (2): 317–330. doi:10.1177/0003065115579720. PMID 25922379. S2CID 207597244.
  57. ^ "How the Lefkoe Belief Process works, Part 1". The Lefkoe Institute. 2010-02-03. Retrieved 2020-10-23.
  58. ^ "Eliminate Core Beliefs - Morty Lefkoe on the Today Show". YouTube. Archived from the original on 2021-12-15.
  59. ^ "About Morty Lefkoe". The Lefkoe Institute. 18 January 2008. Retrieved 2020-10-23.

Further reading edit

  • Carr, M. F.; Jadhav, S. P.; Frank, L. M. (2011). "Hippocampal replay in the awake state: A potential substrate for memory consolidation and retrieval". Nature Neuroscience. 14 (2): 147–153. doi:10.1038/nn.2732. PMC 3215304. PMID 21270783.
  • Tse, D.; Langston, R. F.; Kakeyama, M.; Bethus, I.; Spooner, P. A.; Wood, E. R.; Witter, M. P.; Morris, R. G. M. (2007). "Schemas and Memory Consolidation". Science. 316 (5821): 76–82. Bibcode:2007Sci...316...76T. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.385.8987. doi:10.1126/science.1135935. PMID 17412951. S2CID 11943298.
  • Wamsley, E. J.; Tucker, M.; Payne, J. D.; Benavides, J. A.; Stickgold, R. (2010). "Dreaming of a Learning Task is Associated with Enhanced Sleep-Dependent Memory Consolidation". Current Biology. 20 (9): 850–855. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2010.03.027. PMC 2869395. PMID 20417102.
  • McGaugh, J. L. (2002). "Memory consolidation and the amygdala: A systems perspective". Trends in Neurosciences. 25 (9): 456–461. doi:10.1016/S0166-2236(02)02211-7. PMID 12183206. S2CID 32408870.
  • McIntyre, C. K.; Power, A. N. E.; Roozendaal, B.; McGaugh, J. L. (2006). "Role of the Basolateral Amygdala in Memory Consolidation". Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 985: 273–293. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2003.tb07088.x. PMID 12724165. S2CID 9879141.
  • Nadel, L; Samsonovich, A; Ryan, L; Moscovitch, M (2000). "Multiple trace theory of human memory: Computational, neuroimaging, and neuropsychological results". Hippocampus. 10 (4): 352–68. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.90.9696. doi:10.1002/1098-1063(2000)10:4<352::AID-HIPO2>3.0.CO;2-D. PMID 10985275. S2CID 16979212.

memory, consolidation, category, processes, that, stabilize, memory, trace, after, initial, acquisition, memory, trace, change, nervous, system, caused, memorizing, something, consolidation, distinguished, into, specific, processes, first, synaptic, consolidat. Memory consolidation is a category of processes that stabilize a memory trace after its initial acquisition 1 A memory trace is a change in the nervous system caused by memorizing something Consolidation is distinguished into two specific processes The first synaptic consolidation which is thought to correspond to late phase long term potentiation 2 occurs on a small scale in the synaptic connections and neural circuits within the first few hours after learning The second process is systems consolidation occurring on a much larger scale in the brain rendering hippocampus dependent memories independent of the hippocampus over a period of weeks to years Recently a third process has become the focus of research reconsolidation in which previously consolidated memories can be made labile again through reactivation of the memory trace 3 4 The line processes to make information memoryContents 1 History 2 Synaptic consolidation 2 1 Standard model 2 2 Long term potentiation 2 3 Spacing effect 2 4 Protein synthesis 3 Systems consolidation 3 1 Standard model 3 2 Multiple trace theory 3 2 1 Criticisms 3 3 Semantic vs episodic memory 3 4 Declarative vs procedural knowledge consolidation 3 5 Emotional and stressful memory consolidation 3 6 Sleep consolidation 3 6 1 Cortical slow oscillation and spindle complexes 3 6 2 Zif268 amp REM sleep 4 Reconsolidation 4 1 History 4 2 Criticisms 4 3 Distinctions from consolidation 4 4 Reconsolidation in psychotherapy 5 See also 6 References 7 Further readingHistory editMemory consolidation was first referred to in the writings of the renowned Roman teacher of rhetoric Quintillian He noted the curious fact that the interval of a single night will greatly increase the strength of the memory and presented the possibility that the power of recollection undergoes a process of ripening and maturing during the time which intervenes The process of consolidation was later proposed based on clinical data illustrated in 1882 by Ribot s Law of Regression progressive destruction advances progressively from the unstable to the stable This idea was elaborated on by William H Burnham a few years later in a paper on amnesia integrating findings from experimental psychology and neurology Coining of the term consolidation is credited to the German researchers Muller and Alfons Pilzecker who rediscovered the concept that memory takes time to fixate or undergo Konsolidierung in their studies conducted between 1892 and 1900 1 The two proposed the perseveration consolidation hypothesis after they found that new information learned could disrupt information previously learnt if not enough time had passed to allow the old information to be consolidated 5 This led to the suggestion that new memories are fragile in nature but as time passes they become solidified 5 nbsp Lateral view of the hippocampus which is located in the medial temporal lobeSystematic studies of anterograde amnesia started to emerge in the 1960s and 1970s The case of Henry Molaison formerly known as patient H M became a landmark in studies of memory as it relates to amnesia and the removal of the hippocampal zone and sparked massive interest in the study of brain lesions and their effect on memory After Molaison underwent a bilateral medial temporal lobe resection to alleviate epileptic symptoms the patient began to suffer from memory impairments Molaison lost the ability to encode and consolidate newly learned information leading researchers to conclude the medial temporal lobe MTL was an important structure involved in this process 6 Molaison also showed signs of retrograde amnesia spanning a period of about 3 years prior to the surgery suggesting that recently acquired memories of as long as a couple years could remain in the MTL prior to consolidation into other brain areas 7 Research into other patients with resections of the MTL have shown a positive relationship between the degree of memory impairment and the extent of MTL removal which points to a temporal gradient in the consolidating nature of the MTL 6 These studies were accompanied by the creation of animal models of human amnesia in an effort to identify brain substrates critical for slow consolidation Meanwhile neuropharmacological studies of selected brain areas began to shed light on the molecules possibly responsible for fast consolidation 1 In recent decades advancements in cellular preparations molecular biology and neurogenetics have revolutionized the study of consolidation Providing additional support is the study of functional brain activity in humans which has revealed that the activity of brain regions changes over time after a new memory is acquired 6 This change can occur as quickly as a couple hours after the memory has been encoded suggesting that there is a temporal dimension to the reorganization of the memory as it is represented in the brain 5 Synaptic consolidation editSynaptic consolidation is one form of memory consolidation seen across all species and long term memory tasks Long term memory when discussed in the context of synaptic consolidation is conventionally said to be memory that lasts for at least 24 hours 1 Synaptic consolidation is achieved faster than systems consolidation which is assumed to take weeks months or even to years to be accomplished 8 There is evidence to suggest that synaptic consolidation takes place within minutes to hours of memory encoding or learning shown for example in goldfish 1 and as such it is considered the fast type of consolidation It is also referred to as initial consolidation 9 As soon as six hours after training memories become impervious to interferences that disrupt synaptic consolidation and the formation of long term memory Late phase LTP 2 the long lasting form of one of the best understood forms of synaptic plasticity is thought to be the cellular process underlying synaptic consolidation Standard model edit The standard model of synaptic consolidation suggests that alterations of synaptic protein synthesis and changes in membrane potential are achieved through activating intracellular transduction cascades These molecular cascades trigger transcription factors that lead to changes in gene expression The result of the gene expression is the lasting alteration of synaptic proteins as well as synaptic remodeling and growth In a short time frame immediately following learning the molecular cascade expression and process of both transcription factors and immediate early genes are susceptible to disruptions Disruptions caused by specific drugs antibodies and gross physical trauma can block the effects of synaptic consolidation 1 Long term potentiation edit Main article Long term potentiation LTP can be thought of as the prolonged strengthening of synaptic transmission 10 and is known to produce increases in the neurotransmitter production and receptor sensitivity lasting minutes to even days The process of LTP is regarded as a contributing factor to synaptic plasticity and in the growth of synaptic strength which are suggested to underlie memory formation LTP is also considered to be an important mechanism in terms of maintaining memories within brain regions 11 and therefore is thought to be involved in learning 10 There is compelling evidence that LTP is critical for Pavlovian fear conditioning in rats suggesting that it mediates learning and memory in mammals Specifically NMDA receptor antagonists appear to block the induction of both LTP and fear conditioning and that fear conditioning increases amygdaloidal synaptic transmission that would result in LTP 12 Spacing effect edit Main article Spacing effect Distributed learning has been found to enhance memory consolidation specifically for relational memory Experimental results suggest that distributing learning over the course of 24 hours decreases the rate of forgetting compared to massed learning and enhances relational memory consolidation When interpreted in the context of synaptic consolidation mechanisms of synaptic strengthening may depend on the spacing of memory reactivation to allow sufficient time for protein synthesis to occur and thereby strengthen long term memory 13 One study that demonstrates this effect was conducted in 1984 by Smith and Rothkopf 14 In this experiment subjects were sorted into three groups to test retention and learning Each group was taught the same 8 hour statistics class but one group was taught the class in one day the next over the course of four days in one room and the last was taught the class over the course of four days in different rooms The subjects were tested five days later in a completely new setting The results of the experiment were that taking the class over a period of four days was much more effective than taking it in one single mass Interestingly the group that took the course over four days and in different rooms performed the best in the final retention test out of all the groups 14 This shows that spacing out study sessions and studying in different environments helps with retention as it provides time for the brain to consolidate the information without being interrupted by new information The benefits of spacing were also demonstrated in an earlier study by Reder and Anderson 1982 which yielded similar results confirming the spacing effect s relevance and effects on learning 15 16 Protein synthesis edit Protein synthesis plays an important role in the formation of new memories Studies have shown that protein synthesis inhibitors administered after learning weaken memory suggesting that protein synthesis is required for memory consolidation Additionally reports have suggested that the effects of protein synthesis inhibitors also inhibit LTP 17 However other results have shown that protein synthesis may not in fact be necessary for memory consolidation as it has been found that the formation of memories can withstand vast amounts of protein synthesis inhibition suggesting that this criterion of protein synthesis as necessary for memory consolidation is not unconditional 17 Systems consolidation editSystems consolidation is the second form of memory consolidation It is a reorganization process in which memories from the hippocampal region where memories are first encoded are moved to the neo cortex in a more permanent form of storage 8 18 Systems consolidation is a slow dynamic process that can take anywhere from one to two decades to be fully formed in humans unlike synaptic consolidation that only takes minutes to hours for new information to stabilize into memories 18 Standard model edit The standard model of systems consolidation has been summarized by Squire and Alvarez 1995 19 it states that when novel information is originally encoded and registered memory of these new stimuli becomes retained in both the hippocampus and cortical regions 20 Later the hippocampus representations of this information become active in explicit conscious recall or implicit unconscious recall like in sleep and offline processes 1 Memory is retained in the hippocampus for up to one week after initial learning representing the hippocampus dependent stage 20 During this stage the hippocampus is teaching the cortex more and more about the information and when the information is recalled it strengthens the cortico cortical connection thus making the memory hippocampus independent 1 Therefore from one week and beyond the initial training experience the memory is slowly transferred to the neo cortex where it becomes permanently stored 1 In this view the hippocampus can perform the task of storing memories temporarily because the synapses are able to change quickly whereas the neocortical synapses change over time 19 Consolidation is thus the process whereby the hippocampus activates the neocortex continually leading to strong connections between the two Since the hippocampus can only support memories temporarily the remaining activation will be seen only in the neocortex which is able to support memory indefinitely Squire and Alvarez took the temporally graded nature of patients with retrograde amnesia as support for the notion that once a connection has been established within the neocortex the hippocampus is no longer required but this process is dynamic and extends for several years Squire and Alvarez also proposed the idea that MTL structures play a role in the consolidation of memories within the neocortex by providing a binding area for multiple cortical regions involved in the initial encoding of the memory 19 In this sense the MTL would act as a relay station for the various perceptual input that make up a memory and stores it as a whole event After this has occurred the MTL directs information towards the neocortex to provide a permanent representation of the memory Multiple trace theory edit Multiple trace theory MTT builds on the distinction between semantic memory and episodic memory and addresses perceived shortcomings of the standard model with respect to the dependency of the hippocampus MTT argues that the hippocampus is always involved in the retrieval and storage of episodic memories 21 It is thought that semantic memories including basic information encoded during the storage of episodic memories can be established in structures apart from the hippocampal system such as the neo cortex in the process of consolidation 21 Hence while proper hippocampal functioning is necessary for the retention and retrieval of episodic memories it is less necessary during the encoding and use of semantic memories As memories age there are long term interactions between the hippocampus and neo cortex and this leads to the establishment of aspects of memory within structures aside from the hippocampus 21 MTT thus states that both episodic and semantic memories rely on the hippocampus and the latter becomes somewhat independent of the hippocampus during consolidation 21 An important distinction between MTT and the standard model is that the standard model proposes that all memories become independent of the hippocampus after several years However Nadel and Moscovitch have shown that the hippocampus was involved in memory recall for all remote autobiographical memories no matter of their age 21 An important point they make while interpreting the results is that activation in the hippocampus was equally as strong regardless of the fact that the memories recalled were as old as 45 years prior to the date of the experiment 21 This is complicated by the fact that the hippocampus is constantly involved in the encoding of new events and activation due to this fact is hard to separate using baseline measures 21 Because of this activation of the hippocampus during retrieval of distant memories may simply be a by product of the subject encoding the study as an event 21 Criticisms edit Haist Gore and Mao sought to examine the temporal nature of consolidation within the hippocampus to test MTT against the standard view 22 They found that the hippocampus does not substantially contribute to the recollection of remote memories after a period of a few years They claim that advances in the functional magnetic resonance imaging have allowed them to improve their distinction between the hippocampus and the entorhinal cortex which they claim is more enduring in its activation from remote memory retrieval 22 They also criticize the use of memories during testing which cannot be confirmed as accurate 22 Finally they state that the initial interview in the scanner acted as an encoding event as such differences between recent and remote memories would be obscured 22 Semantic vs episodic memory edit Nadel and Moscovitch argued that when studying the structures and systems involved in memory consolidation semantic memory and episodic memory need to be distinguished as relying on two different memory systems When episodic information is encoded there are semantic aspects of the memory that are encoded as well and this is proposed as an explanation of the varying gradients of memory loss seen in amnesic patients 21 Amnesic patients with hippocampal damage show traces of memories and this has been used as support for the standard model because it suggests that memories are retained apart from the hippocampal system 21 Nadel and Moscovitch argue that these retained memories have lost the richness of experience and exist as depersonalized events that have been semanticized over time 21 They suggest that this instead provides support for their notion that episodic memories rely significantly on the hippocampal system but semantic memories can be established elsewhere in the brain and survive hippocampal damage 21 Declarative vs procedural knowledge consolidation edit Learning can be distinguished by two forms of knowledge declarative and procedural Declarative information includes the conscious recall of facts episodes and lists and its storage typically connected with the mediotemporal lobe and the hippocampal systems as it includes the encoding of both semantic and episodic information of events Procedural knowledge however has been said to function separate from this system as it relies primarily on motor areas of the brain 23 The implicit nature of procedural knowledge allows it to exist absent from the conscious awareness that the information is there Amnesic patients have shown retained ability to be trained on tasks and exhibit learning without the subject being aware that the training had ever taken place 23 This introduces a dissociation between the two forms of memory and the fact that one form can exist absent the other suggests separate mechanisms are involved in consolidation Squire has proposed the procedural knowledge is consolidated in some cases by the extrapyramidal motor system 23 Squire demonstrated that intact learning of certain motor perceptual and cognitive skills can be retained in patients with amnesia 23 They also retain the ability to be influenced by priming effects without the patients being able to consciously recall any training session occurring 23 Emotional and stressful memory consolidation edit The amygdala specifically the basolateral region BLA is involved in the encoding of significant experiences and has been directly linked to memorable events 5 Extensive evidence suggests that stress hormones such as epinephrine play a critical role in consolidating new memories and this is why stressful memories are recalled vividly 24 Studies by Gold and van Buskirk provided initial evidence for this relationship when they showed that injections of epinephrine into subjects following a training period resulted in greater long term retention of task related memories 25 26 This study also provided evidence that the level of epinephrine injected was related to the level of retention suggesting that the level of stress or emotionality of the memory plays a role on the level of retention It is suggested that epinephrine affects memory consolidation by activating the amygdala and studies have shown that antagonism of beta andrenoreceptors prior to injection of epinephrine will block the retention of memory effects seen previously 27 28 This is supported by the fact that beta adrenoreceptor agonists have the opposite effect on the enhancement of memory consolidation 27 28 The BLA is thought to be actively involved in memory consolidation and is influenced strongly by stress hormones resulting in increased activation and as such increased memory retention 24 The BLA then projects to the hippocampus resulting in a strengthened memory 5 This relationship was studied by Packard and Chen who found that when glutamate was administered to the hippocampus enhanced consolidation was seen during food rewarded maze tasks 29 The opposite effect was also seen when the amygdala was inactivated using lidocaine 29 Studies appear to suggest that the amygdala effects the consolidation of memories through its influence with stress hormones and the projections to other brain areas implicated in memory consolidation 5 Sleep consolidation edit See also Sleep and memory Rapid eye movement REM sleep has been thought of to be an important concept in the overnight learning in humans by establishing information in the hippocampal and cortical regions of the brain 30 REM sleep elicits an increase in neuronal activity following an enriched or novel waking experience thus increasing neuronal plasticity and therefore playing an essential role in the consolidation of memories 31 This has come into question in recent years however and studies on sleep deprivation have shown that animals and humans who are denied REM sleep do not show deficits in task learning It has been proposed that since the brain is in a non memory encoding state during sleep consolidation would be unlikely to occur 32 More recent studies however have looked at the relationship between slow wave sleep and memory consolidation rather than REM sleep One study found that low levels of acetylcholine found in the central nervous system which are present during slow wave sleep aid in the consolidation of memories and therefore assist in the learning process 33 34 Recent studies have examined the relationship between REM sleep and procedural learning consolidation In particular studies have been done on sensory and motor related tasks In one study testing finger tapping people were split into two groups and tested post training with or without intervening sleep results concluded that sleep post training increases both speed and accuracy in this particular task while increasing the activation of both cortical and hippocampal regions whereas the post training awake group had no such improvements 30 It has been theorized that this may be related more so to a process of synaptic consolidation rather than systems consolidation because of the short term nature of the process involved 32 Researchers examining the effect of sleep on motor learning have noted that while consolidation occurs over a period of 4 6 hours during sleep this is also true during waking hours which may negate any role of sleep in learning 32 In this sense sleep would serve no special purpose to enhance consolidation of memories because it occurs independently of sleep Other studies have examined the process of replay which has been described as a reactivation of patterns that were stimulated during a learning phase Replay has been demonstrated in the hippocampus and this has lent support to the notion that it serves a consolidation purpose 32 However replay is not specific to sleep and both rats and primates show signs during restful awake periods 32 Also replay may simply be residual activation in areas that were involved previously in the learning phase and may have no actual effect on consolidation 32 This reactivation of the memory traces has also been seen in non REM sleep specifically for hippocampus dependant memories 35 Researchers have noted strong reactivation of the hippocampus during sleep immediately after a learning task This reactivation led to enhanced performance on the learned task 35 One such experiment had participants learn word pair associations declarative memories before either retention periods of sleep or periods of wakefulness Researchers found that retrieval expectancy played a role as to whether participants were able to retain the information as the participants that had been told about the delayed retrieval test performed better However their research showed that sleep was more likely to benefit the consolidation of memories if the information was relevant to future events or behaviors 36 Researchers following this line of work have come to assume that dreams are a by product of the reactivation of the brain areas and this can explain why dreams may be unrelated to the information being consolidated 35 The dream experience itself is not what enhances memory performance but rather it is the reactivation of the neural circuits that causes this Other researchers have looked at the role growth hormones play in the consolidation of memories particularly those of procedural and declarative memories They found that although growth hormones support general brain systems and memory functioning it is still unclear if growth hormones play a role in the formation and processing of particular memories during sleep periods 33 Cortical slow oscillation and spindle complexes edit Memory consolidation during sleep via reactivation of prior experiences and information is associated with sleep signatures of cortical slow oscillations and sleep spindles that are involved in the information flow between relevant brain areas A more complete understanding of these mechanics may possibly allow purposely enabling or strengthening this reactivation 37 38 Zif268 amp REM sleep edit Zif268 is an Immediate early gene IEG thought to be involved in neuroplasticity by an up regulation of the transcription factor during REM sleep after pre exposure to an enriched environment 31 Results from studies testing the effects of zif268 on mice brains postmortem suggest that a waking experience prior to sleep can have an enduring effect in the brain due to an increase of neuroplasticity 31 Reconsolidation editMemory reconsolidation is the process of previously consolidated memories being recalled and actively consolidated 10 It is a distinct process that serves to maintain strengthen and modify memories that are already stored in the long term memory Once memories undergo the process of consolidation and become part of long term memory they are thought of as stable However the retrieval of a memory trace can cause another labile phase that then requires an active process to make the memory stable after retrieval is complete 10 It is believed that post retrieval stabilization is different and distinct from consolidation despite its overlap in function e g storage and its mechanisms e g protein synthesis Memory modification needs to be demonstrated in the retrieval in order for this independent process to be valid 10 History edit The theory of reconsolidation has been debated for many years and is still controversial Reconsolidation was first conceptualized in light of the discovery that phobias could often be eliminated by means of electroconvulsive shock therapy ECT 39 This seemed to indicate the involvement of a re consolidation process for excited memories and that the operation active in ECT was the disruption of that process here of the reconsolidation of retrieved fear memories by shock administration Further studies investigated the concept 10 using ECT to test for reconsolidation ECT was already known as an amnesic agent leads to memory loss These studies found it to be effective on retrieved memories when administered directly after the retrieval of a memory 1 Later research wherein fear memories had been established in rats through Pavlovian fear conditioning found that a consolidated fear memory can be brought to a labile state by means of immediate amygdala infusions of the protein synthesis inhibitor anisomycin but not by infusions made six hours afterwards 3 It was concluded that consolidated fear memory when reactivated enters a changeable state that requires de novo protein synthesis for new consolidation i e re consolidation of the old memory 3 Nader Schafe and Le Doux 2000 demonstrated that the reconsolidation process may make memories more malleable than previously believed 40 16 Nader and his colleagues trained rats to be afraid of a tone by pairing the tone with a small shock Groups of rats were then injected with anisomycin an antibiotic that restricts protein synthesis at different points in time The rats that were injected with anisomycin after consolidation had taken place retained the fear reaction to the tone However the rats that were injected before consolidation and reconsolidation could take place did not retain the fear response when they heard the tone again later It seems that interference that is made before memories are consolidated affect the way they are remembered later Brunet and colleagues 2008 studied patients that had been diagnosed with Post Traumatic Stress Disorder PTSD 41 Following the same method that Nader and his associates used Brunet induced anxiety responses in the patients by having them listen to a 30 second recording describing the circumstances of their traumatic experiences The patients were shortly thereafter injected with propranolol a drug that blocks stress hormone receptors in the amygdala which is implicated in neurologically representing the emotional content of memories These patients experienced a significant reduction in PTSD symptoms months after treatment These findings were confirmed in later studies done in 2009 by Kindt and colleagues and in 2010 by Schiller and colleagues 42 43 These studies done by Nader and others seem to suggest that as memories are being remembered they are fragile as if experiencing them for the first time In addition to fear memories appetitive memories are also prone to reconsolidation episodes which can likewise be disrupted namely after local administration of a protein activity inhibitor 44 Since those breakthrough studies were done there have been several others to probe the theory of memory reconsolidation Subjects in these studies along with humans have included crabs chicks honeybees medaka fish lymnaea and various rodents 10 Further studies have demonstrated an analogue of memory reconsolidation in spinal cord pain processing pathways suggesting a general role for reconsolidation in the central nervous system 45 Criticisms edit Some studies have supported this theory while others have failed to demonstrate disruption of consolidated memory after retrieval It is important to note that negative results may be examples of conditions where memories are not susceptible to a permanent disruption thus a determining factor of reconsolidation 10 After much debate and a detailed review of this field it had been concluded that reconsolidation was a real phenomenon 4 Tronson and Taylor compiled a lengthy summary of multiple reconsolidation studies noting a number of studies were unable to show memory impairments due to blocked reconsolidation However the need for standardized methods was underscored as in some learning tasks such as fear conditioning certain forms of memory reactivation could actually represent new extinction learning rather than activation of an old memory trace Under this possibility traditional disruptions of reconsolidation might actually maintain the original memory trace but preventing the consolidation of extinction learning 10 Recent work has suggested that epigenetic modifications may also prevent reconsolidation in some cases 46 The removal of these epigenetic modifications with inhibitors of histone deacetylase enabled the erasure of remote memories after recall Reconsolidation experiments are more difficult to run than typical consolidation experiments as disruption of a previously consolidated memory must be shown to be specific to the reactivation of the original memory trace Furthermore it is important to demonstrate that the vulnerability of reactivation occurs in a limited time frame which can be assessed by delaying infusion till six hours after reactivation It is also useful to show that the behavioral measure used to assess disruption of memory is not just due to task impairment caused by the procedure which can be demonstrated by testing control groups in absence of the original learning Finally it is important to rule out alternative explanations such as extinction learning by lengthening the reactivation phase 10 There have also been concerns about the use of reconsolidation research to justify psychotherapy treatments and the generalizability of basic reconsolidation research into the therapy room 47 Distinctions from consolidation edit Questions arose if reconsolidation was a unique process or merely another phase of consolidation Both consolidation and reconsolidation can be disrupted by pharmacological agents e g the protein synthesis inhibitor anisomycin and both require the transcription factor CREB However recent amygdala research suggests that BDNF is required for consolidation but not reconsolidation whereas the transcription factor and immediate early gene Zif268 is required for reconsolidation but not consolidation 48 A similar double dissociation between Zif268 for reconsolidation and BDNF for consolidation was found in the hippocampus for fear conditioning 49 However not all memory tasks show this double dissociation such as object recognition memory 50 Reconsolidation in psychotherapy edit In the decade between 2005 and 2015 at least five groups argued the notion that memory reconsolidation can be used to treat psychological problems 51 52 53 54 55 Three of these groups have proposed that the wide variety of different psychotherapies produce permanent change in clients to the extent that they manage to activate this same neurobiological mechanism of reconsolidation in a way that leads to deconsolidation 53 54 56 One example of this is the Lefkoe Method created in 1985 by Morty Lefkoe president and founder of the Lefkoe Institute 57 58 59 Memory reconsolidation may be a common factor in many forms of psychotherapy 56 See also editAtkinson Shiffrin memory model Coherence therapy Engram Patient HM Sharp wave ripple complexesReferences edit a b c d e f g h i j Dudai Y 2004 The Neurobiology of Consolidations Or How Stable is the Engram Annual Review of Psychology 55 51 86 doi 10 1146 annurev psych 55 090902 142050 PMID 14744210 a b Bramham C R Messaoudi E 2005 BDNF function in adult synaptic plasticity The synaptic consolidation hypothesis Progress in Neurobiology 76 2 99 125 doi 10 1016 j pneurobio 2005 06 003 PMID 16099088 S2CID 22770640 a b c Nader K Schafe G E LeDoux J E 2000 Fear memories require protein synthesis in the amygdala for reconsolidation after retrieval Nature 406 6797 722 726 Bibcode 2000Natur 406 722N doi 10 1038 35021052 PMID 10963596 S2CID 4420637 a b Sara SJ 2000 Retrieval and reconsolidation toward a neurobiology of remembering Learn Mem 7 2 73 84 doi 10 1101 lm 7 2 73 PMID 10753974 a b c d e f McGaugh J L 2000 Memory a Century of Consolidation Science 287 5451 248 251 Bibcode 2000Sci 287 248M doi 10 1126 science 287 5451 248 PMID 10634773 a b c Scoville W B Milner B 1957 Loss of Recent Memory After Bilateral Hippocampal Lesions Journal of Neurology Neurosurgery amp Psychiatry 20 1 11 21 doi 10 1136 jnnp 20 1 11 PMC 497229 PMID 13406589 Milner B Corkin S Teuber H L 1968 Further analysis of the hippocampal amnesic syndrome 14 year follow up study of H M Neuropsychologia 6 3 215 234 doi 10 1016 0028 3932 68 90021 3 a b Dudai Y Karni A Born J 2015 The consolidation and transformation of memory Neuron 88 1 20 32 doi 10 1016 j neuron 2015 09 004 PMID 26447570 Okuda K Hojgaard K Privitera R Bayraktar G Takeuchi T 2020 Initial memory consolidation and the synaptic tagging and capture hypothesis European Journal of Neuroscience 54 8 6826 6849 doi 10 1111 ejn 14902 PMID 32649022 S2CID 220469574 a b c d e f g h i j Tronson N C Taylor J R 2007 Molecular mechanisms of memory reconsolidation Nature Reviews Neuroscience 8 4 262 275 doi 10 1038 nrn2090 PMID 17342174 S2CID 1835412 Spencer J P E 2008 Food for thought The role of dietary flavonoids in enhancing human memory learning and neuro cognitive performance Proceedings of the Nutrition Society 67 2 238 252 doi 10 1017 S0029665108007088 PMID 18412998 Maren S 1999 Long term potentiation in the amygdala A mechanism for emotional learning and memory PDF Trends in Neurosciences 22 12 561 567 doi 10 1016 S0166 2236 99 01465 4 hdl 2027 42 56238 PMID 10542437 S2CID 18787168 Litman L Davachi L 2008 Distributed learning enhances relational memory consolidation Learn Mem 15 9 711 716 doi 10 1101 lm 1132008 PMID 18772260 a b Smith Steven M Rothkopf Ernst Z 1984 Contextual Enrichment and Distribution of Practice in the Classroom Cognition and Instruction Informa UK Limited 1 3 341 358 doi 10 1207 s1532690xci0103 4 ISSN 0737 0008 Reder Lynne M Anderson John R 1982 Effects of spacing and embellishment on memory for the main points of a text Memory amp Cognition Springer Science and Business Media LLC 10 2 97 102 doi 10 3758 bf03209210 ISSN 0090 502X PMID 7087784 S2CID 46236858 a b Goldstein E B 2014 Cognitive Psychology Connecting Mind Research and Everyday Experience 4th ed Cengage Learning ISBN 978 1 305 17699 7 a b Gold P E 2008 Protein synthesis inhibition and memory Formation vs amnesia Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 89 3 201 211 doi 10 1016 j nlm 2007 10 006 PMC 2346577 PMID 18054504 a b Roediger H L Dudai Y Fitzpatrick S M 2007 Science of Memory Concepts Oxford University Press ISBN 978 0 19 972751 3 a b c Squire L R Alvarez P 1995 Retrograde amnesia and memory consolidation A neurobiological perspective Current Opinion in Neurobiology 5 2 169 177 doi 10 1016 0959 4388 95 80023 9 PMID 7620304 S2CID 9080102 a b Frankland P W Bontempi B 2005 The organization of recent and remote memories Nature Reviews Neuroscience 6 2 119 130 doi 10 1038 nrn1607 PMID 15685217 S2CID 1115019 a b c d e f g h i j k l Nadel L Moscovitch M 1997 Memory consolidation retrograde amnesia and the hippocampal complex Current Opinion in Neurobiology 7 2 217 227 doi 10 1016 S0959 4388 97 80010 4 PMID 9142752 S2CID 4802179 a b c d Haist F Bowden Gore J B Mao H 2001 Consolidation of human memory over decades revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging Nature Neuroscience 4 11 1139 1145 doi 10 1038 nn739 PMID 11600889 S2CID 24890684 a b c d e Squire L R 1986 Mechanisms of memory Science 232 4758 1612 1619 Bibcode 1986Sci 232 1612S doi 10 1126 science 3086978 PMID 3086978 a b McGaugh J L Roozendaal B 2002 Role of adrenal stress hormones in forming lasting memories in the brain Current Opinion in Neurobiology 12 2 205 210 doi 10 1016 S0959 4388 02 00306 9 PMID 12015238 S2CID 10480860 Gold Paul E Van Buskirk Roderick B February 1975 Facilitation of time dependent memory processes with posttrial epinephrine injections Behavioral Biology 13 2 145 153 doi 10 1016 S0091 6773 75 91784 8 PMID 1122202 Gold P E McIntyre C McNay E Stefani M Korol D L 2001 Neurochemical referees of dueling memory systems Memory consolidation Essays in honor of James L McGaugh pp 219 doi 10 1037 10413 012 ISBN 978 1 55798 783 9 a b Liang KC Juler RG McGaugh JL March 1986 Modulating effects of posttraining epinephrine on memory involvement of the amygdala noradrenergic system PDF Brain Res 368 1 125 33 doi 10 1016 0006 8993 86 91049 8 PMID 3955350 S2CID 12361503 a b Liang KC McGaugh JL Yao HY February 1990 Involvement of amygdala pathways in the influence of post training intra amygdala norepinephrine and peripheral epinephrine on memory storage PDF Brain Res 508 2 225 33 doi 10 1016 0006 8993 90 90400 6 PMID 2306613 S2CID 8665059 a b Packard Mark G Chen Scott A September 1999 The basolateral amygdala is a cofactor in memory enhancement produced by intrahippocampal glutamate injections Psychobiology 27 3 377 385 doi 10 3758 BF03332131 S2CID 143135085 a b Walker M P Stickgold R Alsop D Gaab N Schlaug G 2005 Sleep dependent motor memory plasticity in the human brain Neuroscience 133 4 911 917 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 471 2164 doi 10 1016 j neuroscience 2005 04 007 PMID 15964485 S2CID 3167159 a b c Ribeiro S 1999 Brain Gene Expression During REM Sleep Depends on Prior Waking Experience Learning amp Memory 6 5 500 510 doi 10 1101 lm 6 5 500 PMC 311304 PMID 10541470 a b c d e f Vertes R P 2004 Memory Consolidation in Sleep Neuron 44 1 135 148 doi 10 1016 j neuron 2004 08 034 PMID 15450166 S2CID 919482 a b Gais Steffen Hullemann Philipp Hallschmid Manfred Born Jan 2006 Sleep dependent surges in growth hormone do not contribute to sleep dependent memory consolidation Psychoneuroendocrinology Elsevier BV 31 6 786 791 doi 10 1016 j psyneuen 2006 02 009 ISSN 0306 4530 PMID 16621327 S2CID 14639873 Rasch Bjorn H Born Jan Gais Steffen 2006 05 01 Combined Blockade of Cholinergic Receptors Shifts the Brain from Stimulus Encoding to Memory Consolidation Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 18 5 793 802 doi 10 1162 jocn 2006 18 5 793 ISSN 0898 929X PMID 16768378 S2CID 7584537 a b c Wamsley E J Tucker M Payne J D Benavides J A Stickgold R 2010 Dreaming of a Learning Task is Associated with Enhanced Sleep Dependent Memory Consolidation Current Biology 20 9 850 855 doi 10 1016 j cub 2010 03 027 PMC 2869395 PMID 20417102 Wilhelm Ines Diekelmann Susanne Molzow Ina Ayoub Amr Molle Matthias Born Jan 2011 02 02 Sleep Selectively Enhances Memory Expected to Be of Future Relevance Journal of Neuroscience 31 5 1563 1569 doi 10 1523 JNEUROSCI 3575 10 2011 ISSN 0270 6474 PMC 6623736 PMID 21289163 Press re play to remember How the brain strengthens memories during sleep medicalxpress com Retrieved 14 June 2021 Schreiner Thomas Petzka Marit Staudigl Tobias Staresina Bernhard P 2021 05 25 Endogenous memory reactivation during sleep in humans is clocked by slow oscillation spindle complexes Nature Communications 12 1 3112 Bibcode 2021NatCo 12 3112S doi 10 1038 s41467 021 23520 2 ISSN 2041 1723 PMC 8149676 PMID 34035303 nbsp Available under CC BY 4 0 Solyom L Kenny F Ledwidge B 1969 Psychotherapy Evaluation of a New Treatment Paradigm for Phobias Canadian Psychiatric Association Journal 14 1 3 9 doi 10 1177 070674376901400102 ISSN 0008 4824 PMID 4388484 Nader Karim Schafe Glenn E LeDoux Joseph E 2000 The labile nature of consolidation theory Nature Reviews Neuroscience Springer Science and Business Media LLC 1 3 216 219 doi 10 1038 35044580 ISSN 1471 003X PMID 11257912 S2CID 5765968 Brunet Alain Orr Scott P Tremblay Jacques Robertson Kate Nader Karim Pitman Roger K 2008 Effect of post retrieval propranolol on psychophysiologic responding during subsequent script driven traumatic imagery in post traumatic stress disorder Journal of Psychiatric Research Elsevier BV 42 6 503 506 doi 10 1016 j jpsychires 2007 05 006 ISSN 0022 3956 PMID 17588604 Kindt Merel Soeter Marieke Vervliet Bram 2009 02 15 Beyond extinction erasing human fear responses and preventing the return of fear Nature Neuroscience Springer Science and Business Media LLC 12 3 256 258 doi 10 1038 nn 2271 ISSN 1097 6256 PMID 19219038 S2CID 5948908 Schiller Daniela Monfils Marie H Raio Candace M Johnson David C LeDoux Joseph E Phelps Elizabeth A 2009 12 09 Preventing the return of fear in humans using reconsolidation update mechanisms Nature Springer Science and Business Media LLC 463 7277 49 53 doi 10 1038 nature08637 ISSN 0028 0836 PMC 3640262 PMID 20010606 Crespo J A Stockl P Ueberall F Marcel J Saria A Zernig G February 2012 Activation of PKCzeta and PKMzeta in the nucleus accumbens core is necessary for the retrieval consolidation and reconsolidation of the drug memory PLOS ONE 7 2 e30502 Bibcode 2012PLoSO 730502C doi 10 1371 journal pone 0030502 PMC 3277594 PMID 22348011 Bonin R P amp De Koninck Y 2014 A spinal analogue of memory reconsolidation enables the erasure of hyperalgesia Nat Neurosci 17 8 1043 1045 doi 10 1038 nn 3758 PMC 4978538 PMID 24997764 Graff J Joseph NF Horn ME Samiei A Meng J Seo J Rei D Bero AW Phan TX Wagner F Holson E Xu J Sun J Neve RL Mach RH Haggarty SJ Tsai LH Jan 2014 Epigenetic priming of memory updating during reconsolidation to attenuate remote fear memories Cell 156 1 2 261 276 doi 10 1016 j cell 2013 12 020 PMC 3986862 PMID 24439381 Patihis L 2015 Let s be skeptical about reconsolidation and emotional arousal in therapy Behavioral and Brain Sciences 38 e21 doi 10 1017 S0140525X14000272 PMID 26050685 SSRN 2677025 Debiec J Doyere V Nader K LeDoux J E 2006 Directly reactivated but not indirectly reactivated memories undergo reconsolidation in the amygdala PNAS 103 9 3428 3433 Bibcode 2006PNAS 103 3428D doi 10 1073 pnas 0507168103 PMC 1413871 PMID 16492789 Lee J L Everitt B J Thomas K L 2004 Independent cellular processes for hippocampal memory consolidation and reconsolidation Science 304 5672 839 843 Bibcode 2004Sci 304 839L doi 10 1126 science 1095760 PMID 15073322 S2CID 24194409 Bozon B Davis S Laroche S 2003 A requirement for the immediate early gene zif268 in reconsolidation of recognition memory after retrieval Neuron 40 4 695 701 doi 10 1016 s0896 6273 03 00674 3 PMID 14622575 S2CID 17160003 Centonze Diego Siracusano Alberto Calabresi Paolo Bernardi Giorgio October 2005 Removing pathogenic memories a neurobiology of psychotherapy Molecular Neurobiology 32 2 123 132 doi 10 1385 MN 32 2 123 PMID 16215277 S2CID 20176022 Ecker Bruce September 2008 Unlocking the emotional brain finding the neural key to transformation Psychotherapy Networker 32 5 a b Welling Hans June 2012 Transformative emotional sequence towards a common principle of change PDF Journal of Psychotherapy Integration 22 2 109 136 doi 10 1037 a0027786 a b Lane Richard D Ryan Lee Nadel Lynn Greenberg Leslie S 2015 Memory reconsolidation emotional arousal and the process of change in psychotherapy new insights from brain science PDF Behavioral and Brain Sciences 38 e1 doi 10 1017 S0140525X14000041 PMID 24827452 Schiller Daniela Monfils Marie H Raio Candace M Johnson David C LeDoux Joseph E Phelps Elizabeth A January 2010 Preventing the return of fear in humans using reconsolidation update mechanisms Nature 463 7277 49 53 Bibcode 2010Natur 463 49S doi 10 1038 nature08637 PMC 3640262 PMID 20010606 a b Ecker Bruce Ticic Robin Hulley Laurel 2012 Unlocking the Emotional Brain Eliminating Symptoms at Their Roots Using Memory Reconsolidation New York Routledge ISBN 9780415897167 OCLC 772112300 But for a more hesitant view of the role of memory reconsolidation in psychotherapy that criticizes some of the claims of Ecker et al see Alberini Cristina M April 2015 Commentary on Tuch Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association 63 2 317 330 doi 10 1177 0003065115579720 PMID 25922379 S2CID 207597244 How the Lefkoe Belief Process works Part 1 The Lefkoe Institute 2010 02 03 Retrieved 2020 10 23 Eliminate Core Beliefs Morty Lefkoe on the Today Show YouTube Archived from the original on 2021 12 15 About Morty Lefkoe The Lefkoe Institute 18 January 2008 Retrieved 2020 10 23 Further reading editCarr M F Jadhav S P Frank L M 2011 Hippocampal replay in the awake state A potential substrate for memory consolidation and retrieval Nature Neuroscience 14 2 147 153 doi 10 1038 nn 2732 PMC 3215304 PMID 21270783 Tse D Langston R F Kakeyama M Bethus I Spooner P A Wood E R Witter M P Morris R G M 2007 Schemas and Memory Consolidation Science 316 5821 76 82 Bibcode 2007Sci 316 76T CiteSeerX 10 1 1 385 8987 doi 10 1126 science 1135935 PMID 17412951 S2CID 11943298 Wamsley E J Tucker M Payne J D Benavides J A Stickgold R 2010 Dreaming of a Learning Task is Associated with Enhanced Sleep Dependent Memory Consolidation Current Biology 20 9 850 855 doi 10 1016 j cub 2010 03 027 PMC 2869395 PMID 20417102 McGaugh J L 2002 Memory consolidation and the amygdala A systems perspective Trends in Neurosciences 25 9 456 461 doi 10 1016 S0166 2236 02 02211 7 PMID 12183206 S2CID 32408870 McIntyre C K Power A N E Roozendaal B McGaugh J L 2006 Role of the Basolateral Amygdala in Memory Consolidation Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 985 273 293 doi 10 1111 j 1749 6632 2003 tb07088 x PMID 12724165 S2CID 9879141 Nadel L Samsonovich A Ryan L Moscovitch M 2000 Multiple trace theory of human memory Computational neuroimaging and neuropsychological results Hippocampus 10 4 352 68 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 90 9696 doi 10 1002 1098 1063 2000 10 4 lt 352 AID HIPO2 gt 3 0 CO 2 D PMID 10985275 S2CID 16979212 Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Memory consolidation amp oldid 1183895487, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.