fbpx
Wikipedia

Children's use of information

Children's use of information is an issue in ethics and child development. Information is learned from many different sources and source monitoring (see also source-monitoring error) is important in understanding how people use information and decide which information is credible.

Consider the example of a parent whose child has been diagnosed with hyperactivity; the parent searches the internet for information, reads books, participates in an online chat room with other parents in the same situation, and consults various medical professionals. Some of these sources will be credible (contain reliable information), and others will not. To be well-informed, the parent must filter information according to the reliability of the source. Children learn about the world in much the same way. They are told things by numerous people (e.g., teachers, parents, siblings, and friends), see things on the television or internet, and read information in books. Can children be effective consumers of information? At what age are they able to do this? How do they deal with ambiguous resources? This page will detail answers to those questions (and others) by drawing on peer-reviewed scientific research.

The development of source monitoring edit

Young children have more difficulty with understanding and recalling the sources of information than adults do.[1] Although episodic memory improves throughout childhood, development in the area of source monitoring tends to occur between the ages of 3 and 8 years.[2] At 3 years, children who are able to immediately recognize the source of the information they obtain have difficulty recalling this information after a short delay.[3] The development of source monitoring is gradual, and children achieve and display competency in certain aspects of source monitoring before others.[2] The developmental trajectory of source monitoring provides insight into what cognitive factors are necessary prerequisites. While there is no generally accepted unified theory for the development of source monitoring, five major theories contribute ideas about how source monitoring develops in children: source monitoring theory, fuzzy-trace theory, schema theory, person-based perspective, and the mental-state reasoning model.

Source monitoring theory edit

According to source monitoring theory, the source of information is attributed through a decision-making process, where source is inferred based on various characteristics inherent in the memory itself.[1] This means that the sources are not directly encoded, but rather reconstructed, when information is recalled. This decision-making process can either be through automatic, unconscious processing, or through heightened demanding and systematic processing that may require reasoning, and the retrieval of supporting memories. This theory implicates the development of episodic memory and memory strategies in the development of source monitoring more generally.

Fuzzy-trace theory edit

Fuzzy-trace theory hypothesizes that the source-monitoring errors that children make are caused by problems with memory storage and retrieval.[4] Memories are simultaneously stored in two different formats: the "gist" level (extracted from the experience), and the "verbatim" representation (information in exact detail). It is proposed that the source of information is encoded in memory as a verbatim detail. Memories for verbatim details decay more quickly over time than gist representations, and young children demonstrate faster decay of verbatim information than older children or adults. Younger children are more likely to experience memory intrusions due to weaker memory traces, which leads to a susceptibility to misleading information replacing memory traces from a previous event.[5] For this reason, developmental changes in episodic memory performance are viewed as the driving factor in source monitoring development.

Schema theory edit

Schema theory, as a derivative of script theory, states that after repeated exposure to similar events, individuals form a general representation of what typically happens.[6] Some details are the same at each instance of repeated events, and others can vary from instance to instance. In the script for what usually happens, there is a "slot" for each variable detail and the detail for a particular time is chosen from a list of possible variations. If source information is encoded as a slot, errors in source monitoring can be the result of incorrect retrieval of a specific detail. This framework allows for the storage of a large amount of detailed information about specific events, however is very cognitively demanding for children. Because of their limited cognitive resources, young children require more experience with repeated events in order to generate a schema. Details that vary from instance to instance can be lost, and children rely instead on the generalized event representation when attempting to recall a particular instance. The main assumption is that the development of source monitoring depends on an increase in cognitive processing capacity.

Person-based perspective edit

Person-based perspective emphasizes prospective processes. Prospective processes relate actions to one another through operations such as sequencing and planning.[7] These processes are affected by the perspective taken (self vs. other) or the goals and meaning to the individual. The meaning attached to the goal of an action can interfere with source monitoring by removing attention from the source details.[8] The person-based perspective is social-cognitive in nature (more so than other theories of source monitoring) and assumes that development is reliant on socialization and theory of mind development.

Mental-state reasoning model edit

The mental-state reasoning model highlights possible mechanisms behind young children's suggestibility.[9] Children who have difficulty with reasoning about conflicting mental representations are likely to overwrite their original memories with misinformation because they cannot reconcile two contradicting views of what actually occurred.[10] Source monitoring and the understanding of knowledge states, play a key role in resisting suggestions. The development of source monitoring is presumed to be based on better understanding of knowledge states, metacognition and theory of mind.

The origins of knowledge edit

Being aware of how we have acquired information is particularly difficult for young children (specifically 3 to 5 year-olds). After feeling a soft ball, 3 and 4 year-olds can correctly identify whether they know the ball is soft or hard, but cannot always say how they know. Perhaps it is because they felt the ball, saw the ball, or were told it was soft. The ability to recognize the origin of their knowledge requires the understanding of how knowledge is acquired.[11]

Knowledge and justification edit

When asked knowledge questions ("Do you know what is in the box?") then justification questions ("How do [or why don't] you know what is in the box?"), children who can correctly answer are able to reflect on knowledge that they have gained from a particular source and should be able to identify the source of information.[11][12]

3 and 4 year-olds are better at answering these questions when the questions refer to themselves in comparison to referring to another person. When these children are shown a hidden object they can correctly report their perceptual access (responding correctly to "Did you look into the box?") and their knowledge access of what is in the box (responding correctly to "Do you know [not know] what is in the box?").[12] Despite their ability to answer correctly when referring to the self, 3 and 4 year-olds have difficulty responding to the "other's" knowledge, where they deny the "other" has the knowledge. 3-4 year-olds can correctly identify that the other person has in fact looked in a box, but when asked "Does [other] know what is in the box?" the child will deny that the other person knows what is in the box.[12] The key assumption is then that although children are aware that perceptual access is needed, they are unable to acknowledge that the knowledge was gained from perceptual access.[12][13]

Gaining knowledge through looking, touching, and asking edit

Gaining knowledge is knowing how to acquire new information. The ability to recognize how specific knowledge can be gained by perceptual access (looking, feeling or smelling) is the understanding of Aspectuality. Aspectuality understanding is the awareness that an object is made up of many different properties (colour, weight, odour), which can be determined by a specific perceptual action (looking, feeling, smelling).[11][14] Identifying the colour of a car outside the window could involve asking someone for the information or looking out the window. In most cases, adults would acquire this knowledge from simply looking out the window. However, not all information can be gained in this manner. If you were asked to find out how old the driver of the car is, simply looking would not provide accurate information; you would have to ask. These two situations are dependent on whether the information being sought out is visible or invisible (respectively).[15] Knowledge development depends on children's ability to efficiently pursue their informational goal. Children do not always make the most effective or efficient decision when acquiring new information.[15]

Age differences edit

6 year-olds are able to distinguish when gaining knowledge requires looking (the information is visible) or when gaining knowledge requires asking (the information is invisible). However, 4 year-olds do not perform as consistently. Even with an expert present, 4 year-olds will overestimate the knowledge they can gain through looking. However, when the information to be gained is regarding a group of friends and the expert is a friend of the group, 4 year-olds tend to overestimate knowledge acquisition through asking.[15]

When given access to pairs of objects, which could either be identified by seeing (identical objects: different in colour) or by touch/feeling (identical objects: 1 soft, 1 hard), children perform relatively well, generally recognizing when they have adequate information and when they have inadequate information (i.e., knowing the object is blue after seeing it or knowing the object is soft after feeling it).[11][16] But, when young children are not given access (of seeing or feeling), they have difficulty predicting and identifying which mode of access would allow them to identify the object.[16] In this case, 3 and 4 year-olds overestimate the knowledge that could be gained through feeling the object.[11] 3 to 4 year-olds may not understand the perceptual access needed to acquire the specific knowledge.

Language and question phrasing edit

Waters and Beck (2012) state "understanding the link between perceptual access and consequent information (knowledge access) is a crucial component in the development of theory of mind". In a typical adult population, change in the phrasing of a particular question would not affect the understanding of what action needs to occur. "What colour is the bike that is outside?" or "Is the bike red?" should not change the action of looking to gain the needed information. However, children's performance is susceptible to this type of question phrasing.[14][17][18] There are language effects on knowledge access through 3 different question types:[14]

  1. Identity: "Find out which one is in the tunnel",
  2. Dimension: "Find out what colour the one in the tunnel is"
  3. Aspect: "Find out if the one in the tunnel is the red one or the blue one".

4 and 5 year-olds are more likely to respond correctly to aspect and dimension questions in comparison to identity questions.[14] Aspect and dimension questions are more explicit, which may be why performance is better in these categories. Identity questions require more cognitive effort as one has to remember how the objects were similar and how they differed.

Judging the credibility of sources and understanding of trust edit

Much of people's knowledge about the world is not obtained from direct experience, but indirectly from what other people or sources tell them. With widespread use of the internet people have access to nearly unlimited sources of information. Some of that information might be conflicting, and different sources of information vary in their accuracy and credibility. People can also deliberately deceive, be misunderstood, or be mistaken.[19] It is important that people develop the necessary skills to assess the accuracy of what they are being told.

Adults' credibility judgments edit

Adults make credibility judgments based on two factors: expertise and trustworthiness. For example, we might trust information from published research articles more than information from blogs because we know that anyone can write a blog, but scientists (experts) who write peer-reviewed articles are highly trained. With regards to trustworthiness, adults are less likely to believe someone who they think is trying to deceive them, and they take into account the intentions of the information-provider. For example, if someone's intention is to sell you something, you might be more skeptical of the accuracy of the information they provide because their motives cause you to question their honesty. Adults can effectively use these cues to make judgments about the credibility of different sources, but whether children can also do so is an important area for research.

Children's ability to make credibility judgments edit

Even very young children show an early sensitivity to issues of source credibility. By the age of 4, children show similar patterns to adults in a preference for perception over testimony; that is, they would rather see something with their own eyes than be told about it. Children also feel more confident in their knowledge when they have directly perceived it than if they have been told by someone else, even if the speaker is well informed.[20] However, children are not always able to directly perceive information, and they learn much of what they know from others. Some people are more credible sources of information than others, so children must actively evaluate information and decide whether or not to believe it. There are many factors or cues that children, by the age of 4, take into consideration when making judgements about whether or not to trust what a person says. For instance, knowledge and experience, traits, motivations, age, and reasoning or support.

Credibility judgments based on knowledge and experience edit

Young children have a reputation for credulity, or believing something without having any proof that it is true. Young children often trust what adults tell them, especially when they have no prior knowledge or expectations about the topic of the testimony.[21] Because children tend to interact with adults who are more knowledgeable than themselves, if they have no reason to believe otherwise, they will trust what adults tell them. In particular, 3 year-olds tendency to believe others is based on a selective bias to trust what people tell them. This selective trust is adaptive in the early years as they are learning language and their way around the world.[22]

Children have difficulty disregarding information provided to them, even if the source has been unreliable in the past. 3 year-olds will often continue to believe what a person tells them even after being repeatedly deceived by that person, but 4 year-olds are far better able to disregard this unreliable information.[23] Three-year-olds are better at making trust judgments when they are able to choose between two sources of knowledge, rather than deciding whether or not to believe a single person, and in this situation they are often able to choose the more reliable of two speakers.[24] By age 4, young children take an informant's knowledge, expertise, and reliability into account in order to avoid learning from unreliable or problematic sources.[25] They believe statements made by knowledgeable speakers more than ignorant speakers, before they can explicitly answer questions about who has access to knowledge.[26] They also prefer to seek information from sources who have been knowledgeable in the past. 4 year-olds can spontaneously use others' past performance to guide their learning.[27]

Credibility judgments based on traits and motivation edit

Almost all human institutions (e.g., family, community, business, government) are built on a foundation of trust.[28] There are many factors that influence children's trust in people and one of the most important is honesty.[29] There are various schools of philosophical thought that posits honesty to be morally right and lying to be morally wrong. On one end of the continuum, philosophers like Bok,[30] Kant,[31] and St. Augustine[32] hold a deontological view that focuses on intrinsic duties relating to the treatment of others. In other words, telling the truth is intrinsically right and lying is intrinsically wrong. On the other end of the continuum is the utilitarian view that emphasizes the greater good, specifically with respect to the outcome of one's act. Therefore, lying and its moral implications are context dependent.[33] In some situations, such as when being polite to spare another person's feelings, making a "prosocial lie" or deliberate false statement are endorsed.

Children consider motivation, as well as traits, when deciding whom to seek information from and whom to believe. In both Eastern and Western cultures, both adults and children adhere to the utilitarian perspective when giving moral evaluations of truths and lies in different social situations.[34] In terms of people's characteristics, children tend to place trust in people who are honest, smart, or kind over people who are dishonest, not smart, or not kind.[24] However, they also consider a person's intent or motivation. From age 7, children consider both honesty and benevolence when making trust judgments about other people, and older children are more likely to trust people who tell prosocial lies (to avoid hurting another person's feelings or to help another person) than young children.[28] For younger children, honesty is more important than a person's intention. As children get older, they increasingly attend to motivation as a key factor. The relationship between telling the truth and trusting a person is stable, but when it comes to lying, children consider the motivation of the speaker when deciding whether or not to trust them.

Credibility judgments based on age and reliability edit

Children evaluate the credibility of a speaker by evaluating how reliable that person has been in the past and can make predictions about a speaker based on their age.[35] Children as young as 3 years-old prefer to trust an unfamiliar adult rather than an unfamiliar child.[36] When considering both age and reliability, age is often the primary cue used to determine another's credibility. For example, 3 and 4 year-olds found adults to be more trustworthy than peers, unless the peer demonstrated greater reliability (i.e., adult incorrectly mislabelled objects, whereas peer correctly labelled them).[35] Children also consider both the prior history of accuracy and the level of perceptual access the speaker has when they provide information. Young children spontaneously keep track of the prior history of a person's accuracy or inaccuracy (reliability) and prefer to learn from someone with a good track record.[25] Children commonly interpret the speaker's history of inaccuracy as a lasting trait and so the speaker is considered an unreliable informant, at least within the domain they have been wrong about. However, under certain conditions, children may excuse a person's past inaccuracy and later trust that person for information. If a speaker has limited information (e.g., lack of perceptual access) in making a claim – for example, inaccurately identifying a toy while blindfolded – then children as young as 3 years-old appropriately excuse their past inaccuracy especially when they are later well-informed.[19] On the other hand, if a speaker has full access to information while making an inaccurate claim, children continue to regard him/her as unreliable.

Credibility judgments based on reasoning or support edit

Young children appreciate that people generally think and act sensibly when reasons are present, and they can evaluate a claim by assessing the quality of the reasoning provided.[37] Thus, children create an epistemic profile of a person based on the quality of the reasons they offer when making a claim.[38] As young as 3 years-old, children understand the difference between weak versus strong reasoning to support a statement. Children are more likely to trust someone when strong support is provided through: reliable testimony ("My teacher told me there's a book in the bag. I think that there's a book"), looking ("Before I came here, I looked and saw a ball in the bag. I think there's a ball in there"), and inference ("It's a backpack. Backpack holds books. I think there's a book in there").[38] On the other hand, desire ("I like crayons. I want there to be crayons in the bag. I think that there are crayons in there"), pretense ("I like to pretend. I'm going to pretend that there's a sandwich in the bag. I think there's a sandwich in there"), and guessing ("I don't know. I'm going to guess that there's a toy in the bag") are not viewed as strong support for a claim.[38] Children recognize that the mental states that they and others may hold are not always reliable means for drawing specific conclusions. 3 and 4 year-olds can also choose the more reasonable of two people and continue to seek, as well as, accept new information from the more credible person (the one who had better reasons in the past).[38]

Children's development of distrust edit

People are not always reliable in their provision of information and it is important to be able to differentiate between good sources and bad sources. Assessing someone's reliability is based not only on the knowledgeability of the speaker, but their motives/intentions as well. People may not always be motivated to tell the truth; instead, they may potentially lie to promote their own interest,[39] or the interest of others.[40] At about the age they begin preschool, children become better at distinguishing between helpful and deceptive people. 3 year-olds are not able to identify who is trying to help or trick another person and accept advice from both helpers and trickers.[41] On the other hand, 4 year-olds are more sceptical and could differentiate between helpers and trickers, but have no preference in choosing whom to accept advice from.[41] There may be a mismatch between knowledge and behaviour among 4 year-olds, in which they do not understand the implications of their knowledge or how to successfully apply it to their behaviour. 5 year-olds systematically preferred advice from helpers.[41] Ultimately theory of mind, or children's understanding of mental states, is related to selective trust in helpers (versus trickers). Beginning at 5 years-old, children use a person's prior history of deception to make reliability judgments about that person.[41]

Metacognition edit

Metacognition is an ability that allows people to think about their own thinking process, and how to use their thinking process to help themselves learn better. Metacognition includes two separate abilities: (1) knowledge of cognition and (2) regulation of cognition.[42][43] Knowledge of one's thinking process is not enough to regulate an individual's behaviour, and are required to use specific strategies to help them regulate their behaviour.[42][43]

Knowing you don't know edit

An important skill children need to learn is being able to identify when a message is incomplete and they don't have enough information to make an accurate interpretation. Being aware that an ambiguous situation has arisen is difficult for young children. Children accurately "know when they know", but often overestimate when they don't know.[44][45] Children's behaviour does not seem to match their verbal ability to acknowledge their "lack of knowledge". Despite incorrectly stating that they "know" something, children are still capable of changing their response upon hearing contradicting information to an initial interpretation and/or event.[44][46] Language plays an important role in children's accuracy in assessing their own knowledge. For children to accurately "know what they know" it is important for them to understand the various meanings of the word "know" as well as language used to describe certainty and uncertainty.[47][48]

Children's processing of ignorant and ambiguous statements edit

Ambiguous information is "a piece of information (word, message, or view) with multiple interpretations".[49] Adults not only have the awareness to spot ambiguity, but they also have ways to deal with it. But young children cannot do the same. Typically, children cannot successfully deal with ambiguous statements until they are 6 or 7 years old. [44][50] However, it seems in certain contexts with certain tasks, younger children also can somewhat deal with unclear or ambiguous statements. [47]

The role of language and prior experience edit

Language can affect whether one can correctly deal with ambiguous statements. One can "know" someone in many different ways, for example, seeing them, talking to them, having a prior history with them, having a relationship with them, etc. This makes questions like "Do you know?" very complex and hard to answer. Clearer definitions of "to know" seem to help children to check their own knowledge better. Children as young as 4 years could make far more accurate statements about their actual knowledge when a question was phrased "Have you heard of" rather than "Do you know".[51] By the age of 6, children typically could accurately check their knowledge with very little impact on their future answers regardless of the language used. 4-5 year-old's, on the other hand, were so changeable that the phrase used affected their future answers. 4-5 year-old's were also less likely to overestimate their knowledge of a target person if the starting question was phrased "Have you heard of" rather than "Do you know".[51] Not only can responses from children be altered by the question’s phrasing, but they can also change their response when the question suggests they are not familiar with the person. In a study where children were asked if they really knew who a specific person was (between 2 pictures of people they had never seen before), they were more accurate in assessing that they didn't know when it was suggested that the target person had never been to their before.[51] While experience is important, children tend not to over-rely on prior experiences and only use it when they've had enough experience that can help assess their knowledge.[51]

Partial information edit

Sometimes when confronted with ambiguous information, more than one piece of information is required to make and accurate interpretation. For example, in a study where children had to pick one of four pictures presented to them after hearing ambiguous information, they were capable of making tentative interpretations and then correctly changing their interpretations upon hearing contradicting, clarifying information.[44] This strategy uses multiple pieces of information and has been seen in children as young as 15 months old.[52] In situations where only partial information is available, young children make the best interpretation possible with the information given and go on to change this interpretation only when contradicting information emerges.[45]

Delaying interpretation edit

Clarifying (Disambiguating)information is not always instantly available. In these situations, adults seem to delay interpretation and seek clarifying information when appropriate. Children find it harder to understand what actions they need to take to clarify than to identify an ambiguous situation that pops up. There are 2 different types of delay in interpreting ambiguous information: one that is intentional and one that is instinctual.[49] The decision to delay interpretation and seek further information is a difficult one as it involves being aware that the information given at that point are not sufficient and knowing how to ask for the ones necessary for clarification. Children are typically failures in this process until the age of 7.[44] However, when this explicit decision to delay is simplified, children aged 5 and up showed some ability in successfully choosing to delay their response.[49] While the ability to intentionally delay interpretation seems to be difficult, the ability to delay interpretation instinctively seems to be easier for young children. In one example, children were asked to stamp the correct snowman once they knew which one it was.[49] Their knowledge of the correct snowman was based on a researcher slowly revealing the target snowman. The children's cards contained snowmen that differed by some feature visible on the 2nd half of the snowman only and therefore the children were required to wait until the 2nd half of the snowman was revealed in order to accurately assess which snowman was the correct one. In this scenario focusing on instinct, children as young as 5 were able to accurately delay interpretation.[49] Therefore, while it is difficult for children to explicitly demonstrate their awareness of an ambiguous situation and how to resolve it, they are implicitly able to handle situations in which delaying interpretation may be beneficial.

Referents and meaning edit

It is difficult for younger children to grasp the idea that objects can be referred to in different ways and that people can have partial knowledge of the different references (i.e. a "bouncy ball" might also be referred to as a "rubber sphere"). A child might know by looking that a toy is a toy truck, but they may not be aware that the toy truck is also a present. Referential opacity is the concept of whether or not referring to an object changes its meaning. If something is referentially transparent (substitution insensitive), altering the referent term does not alter the meaning, and something that is referentially opaque (substitution sensitive) means that altering the referent term would alter the meaning. An example of this is a study with a puppet named Heinz.[53] There is a ball in a box and children are told that Heinz knows that there is a ball in the box but does not know that the ball is a present. Children are then asked substitution-insensitive questions (i.e. Does Heinz know the ball is a present? – asking, "Does Heinz know the rubber sphere is a present?" does not alter the meaning of the question) and substitution sensitive questions (i.e. Does Heinz know there's a present in the box? – asking "Does Heinz know there is a rubber sphere in the box" does alter the meaning of the question). Regardless of age, substitution-insensitive questions seem to be easier than substitution-sensitive questions. The ability to correctly answer substitution-sensitive questions improves with age.[53] The ability to answer these types of questions is closely related to effectively evaluating ambiguous messages. Success on substitution-insensitive questions is necessary but not sufficient for success on evaluation ambiguous messages. Alternatively, success on substitution-sensitive questions is necessary and sufficient for success at evaluating ambiguous messages.[53]

Physical and epistemic uncertainty edit

Children can change their interpretation of information based on the context in which the interpretation is made.[54] Robinson and colleagues (2006) studied children's interpretation of information in two different: physical and epistemic uncertainty. Physical uncertainty occurs when an event has not yet happened, and therefore the outcome of that event has not been determined (i.e. the dice has not yet been rolled). Epistemic uncertainty occurs when an event has already occurred, but the child is not aware of the outcome of the event (i.e. the dice have been rolled, but the dice are hidden from the participant). 4 to 8 year old children have the ability to realize multiple possibilities for an event that has not yet occurred (physical uncertainty), however they do not seem to acknowledge that there are exactly the same possibilities for an event that has already happened when they don't know the outcome (epistemic uncertainty). Under the conditions of epistemic uncertainty, children simply guess one of the possibilities. Beck and colleagues (2011) propose that this happens because it is much easier to imagine the outcome during epistemic uncertainty, basically knowing that there is only one outcome.[55] Similarly, adults also prefer to make predictions or guess in epistemic uncertainty.[56]

Feeling-of-knowing (FOK) edit

Feeling-of-knowing occurs when people are unable to easily recall a memory or a fact, but they know that they learned it and are able to recognize it, such as in a multiple-choice test.[57] Adults' accuracy of feeling-of-knowing judgments is well above chance but not nearly perfect.[58] It seems to be the same for children as well. Lockl and Schneider (2002) did not find any developmental trends in the accuracy of feeling-of-knowing judgments. Instead, similar to adults, children's accuracy of feeling-of knowing judgments was low, but still above chance for all age groups studied (grade 1 through grade 4).[59] Getting a child to attend to this feeling-of-knowing (through language or prompting of prior experience) is one way to assist them in more accurately assessing their actual knowledge, allowing them to handle ambiguous situations at a much younger age.[51]

Forensic applications edit

Children are highly susceptible to a "suggestibility effect", producing "false memories" and/or "incorrect, post-event information" (see misinformation effect) when asked to engage in memory recall.[5] This has important implications for forensic interviewing and child witness testimony.[60] Consider the prominent case of Kelly Michael's (see Wee Care Nursery School abuse trial), where improper interviewing techniques lead to a miscarriage of justice. In order to develop reliable and age-appropriate instruments for interviewing children, it is imperative to consider their cognitive development, verbal and mental abilities.[5][60]

Forensic interviews and techniques edit

Factors that can impact disclosure edit

It is critical for a forensic interviewer to recognize their power during an interview, especially with a child witness. An interviewer can impact the course of a child's testimony in numerous ways, including:

  • Eliciting "false allegations" through leading and suggestive questions[61]
  • Determining the amount of detail a child may disclose[61]
  • Prevent a child from disclosing[61]
  • Interviewer's gender may also impact the quality and details of disclosure. Children between the ages of 4 and 6 are more likely to disclose to an interviewer of the same gender.[61]

The age of a child is also an important factor during interviewing. Younger children are more likely to provide shorter, less detailed accounts of an event in comparison to older children.[61] Preschool children are more likely to disclose information in an "accidental way" through triggers and obvious cues, whereas older school children are more likely to make "intentional disclosures" based on the nature of the question they are asked.[61]

Improper interviewing edit

There are varying techniques and extraneous factors that can influence the way a child discloses an event during child witness testimony (e.g., experiences of abuse by a parent or caregiver). There are two major types of barriers in forensic interviews: (1) improper interviewing and (2) clumsy interviewing[62][61]

Improper interviewing includes forensic techniques that are considered to be "risky and ineffective".[61] Each of the following techniques can create critically negative consequences in witness testimony and result in false allegations or the potential for a reduced conviction:[61]

  1. Use of reinforcement: If an interviewer utilizes rewards and punishments to coerce disclosure[61]
  2. Social influence: If an interviewer informs a child of another child's disclosure or recollection[61]
  3. Asking suggestive or leading questions: This can occur when an interviewer provides a child with information that has not been previously known[61]
  4. Removing the child from direct experience: For example, asking a child "what might have happened"[61]

Clumsy interviewing involves interviews that fail to include best practices and recommended techniques.[61] Interviewers who are not properly trained in forensic techniques can fail to follow structured interview guidelines and impact the outcome of a child's testimony and/or responses to questioning. This type of interviewing most often occurs when an interviewer lacks skill, forgets important procedures, and when there is a lack of necessary supervision.[61]

In order to prevent improper forensic interviewing, numerous methods to reduce suggestibility and the misinformation effect have been proven effective, including: taping interviews, recording transcripts, ensuring supervision by a qualified professional, experience in working with children, training in forensic interviewing, and maintaining a comfortable, safe environment.[61]

Types of interviewing edit

The cognitive interview utilizes cognitive theory as a focal point in its interview methodology. The cognitive interview, first developed in 1992 by researchers Fisher and Geiselman, was originally developed for adults and later modified for children. It utilizes two major perspectives from cognitive theory, including the "encoding specificity principle" and a "multi-component view of memory traces".[60]

Specifically, this method utilizes four major techniques:[60]

  1. Report everything: i.e., "tell me everything you remember"[60]
  2. Context reinstatement: i.e., "think back to where you were at the time"[60]
  3. Reverse order: i.e., "now that you told me what happened, try to remember it again but this time starting at the end and recounting it in reverse chronological order"[60]
  4. Change perspective: i.e., "what would the perpetrator have seen and heard?"[60]

The National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD) Protocol developed an investigative protocol in 2000 to create a structured interview technique for children, specifically those of child sexual abuse.[60][61][63]

It uses the following techniques:[60][63]

  1. Introductory phase: This is necessary for rapport building[60]
  2. Practice in free-recall: "Tell me about your last birthday party"[60]
  3. Information about the ground rules: "It's okay to say "I don't know""[60]
  4. Open-ended questions: "Tell me why you came to talk to me"[60]

Stepwise interview utilizes open-ended questions through a "funnel-like strategy".[60] It is primarily used by legal professionals, and is most often used in North America.[60] This interview begins with open-ended questions and/or free recall and slowly incorporates more focused and detailed questions.[60]

Allegation blind interviews stress that an interviewer should refrain from gathering information prior to an interview in order to reduce suggestibility and increase interviewer patience and attentiveness.[61] This also enhances the interviewer's ability to be non-judgmental and objective.[61]

Truth-lie discussions are most useful prior to commencing abuse-related questioning. This method allows the interviewer to create a baseline with the child about what the "truth" is and what a "lie" is.[61] The interviewer is encouraged to ask questions with general examples, such as "tell me a lie about this chair".[61] This strategy has been proven to result in a less misinformed child testimony.[61]

Touch survey was developed on the basis that "touch falls on a continuum", and is beneficial to screen for child abuse.[61] It includes questions surrounding the child's experiences with touch (e.g., kissing, hugging, hitting), including where they have been touched and by whom.[61] This tool might be more useful when used in conjunction with other forensic strategies.[61]

Recommendations for forensic interviewing edit

Although there are varying suggestions for structured forensic interviewing, experts provide context into best practices that can significantly reduce suggestibility, false memories and the misinformation effect:[61]

  1. Interviews should be conducted in a safe, child-friendly environment.[61]
  2. A child's age should be considered when being interviewed, and practices should be modified accordingly.[61]
  3. Structured interview protocol should always be utilized.[61]
  4. Interviewers should engage in professional training when possible.[61]
  5. Ground rules are essential for the beginning of the interview so that the child is aware of the type of responses they can provide (e.g., "I don't know").[61]

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ a b Lindsay, D. S.; Johnson, M. K.; Kwon, P. (1991). "Developmental changes in memory source monitoring". Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 52 (3): 297–318. doi:10.1016/0022-0965(91)90065-z. PMID 1770330.
  2. ^ a b Roberts, K. P. (2002). "Children's ability to distinguish between memories from multiple sources: Implications for the quality and accuracy of eyewitness statements". Developmental Review. 22 (3): 403–435. doi:10.1016/S0273-2297(02)00005-9. S2CID 59933611.
  3. ^ Gopnik, A.; Graf, P. (1988). "Knowing how you know: Young children's ability to identify and remember the sources of their beliefs". Child Development. 59 (5): 1366–1371. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1988.tb01505.x.
  4. ^ Brainerd, C. J.; Reyna, V. F. (1990). "Gist is the grist: Fuzzy-trace theory and the new intuitionism". Developmental Review. 10 (1): 3–47. doi:10.1016/0273-2297(90)90003-m.
  5. ^ a b c Welch-Ross, M. K.; Diecidue, K.; Miller, S. A. (1997). "Young children's understanding of conflicting mental representation predicts suggestibility". Developmental Psychology. 33 (1): 43–53. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.33.1.43. PMID 9050389.
  6. ^ Farrar, M. J., & Goodman, G. S. (1990). Developmental differences in the relation between scripts and episodic memory: Do they exist? In Knowing and remembering in young children, Fivush, Robyn, Hudson, Judith A. eds., (pp. 30-64). Cambridge University Press, New York, NY
  7. ^ Ratner, H. H.; Foley, M. A. (1996). "A unifying framework for the development of children's activity memory". Advances in Child Development and Behavior. 25: 33–105. doi:10.1016/S0065-2407(08)60050-6. ISBN 9780120097258. PMID 7847174.
  8. ^ Foley, M. A.; Ratner, H. H. (1998). "Distinguishing between memories for thoughts and deeds: The role of prospective processing in children's source monitoring". British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 16 (4): 465–484. doi:10.1111/j.2044-835x.1998.tb00765.x.
  9. ^ Welch-Ross, M. (2000). A mental-state reasoning model of suggestibility and memory source monitoring. In Children's source monitoring, Roberts, Kim P., Blades, Mark, eds., (pp. 227-255). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers, Mahwah, NJ
  10. ^ Bright-Paul, A.; Jarrold, C.; Wright, D. B. (2008). "Theory of mind development influences suggestibility and source monitoring". Developmental Psychology. 44 (4): 1055–1068. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.44.4.1055. PMID 18605834.
  11. ^ a b c d e O'Neill, D. K.; Astington, J. W.; Flavell, J. H. (1992). "Young children's understanding of the role that sensory experiences play in knowledge acquisition". Child Development. 63 (2): 474–490. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1992.tb01641.x. PMID 1611948.
  12. ^ a b c d Wimmer, H.; Hogrefe; Perner, J. (1988). "Children's understanding of informational access as source of knowledge". Child Development. 59 (2): 386–396. doi:10.2307/1130318. JSTOR 1130318.
  13. ^ Pratt, C.; Bryant, P. (1990). "Young children understand that looking leads to knowing (so long as they are looking into a single barrel)". Child Development. 61 (4): 973–982. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1990.tb02835.x. PMID 2209200.
  14. ^ a b c d Waters, G. M.; Beck, S. R. (2012). "How should we question young children's understanding of aspectuality?". The British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 30 (3): 376–392. doi:10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02044.x. PMID 22882369.
  15. ^ a b c Fitneva, S. A.; Lam, N. H. L.; Dunfield, K. A. (2013). "The development of children's information gathering: To look or to ask?". Developmental Psychology. 49 (3): 533–542. doi:10.1037/a0031326. hdl:21.11116/0000-0002-5F86-D. PMID 23316769.
  16. ^ a b Robinson, E. J.; Haigh, S. N.; Pendle, J. E. C. (2008). "Children's working understanding of the knowledge gained from seeing and feeling" (PDF). Developmental Science. 11 (2): 299–305. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00676.x. PMID 18333983.
  17. ^ Doherty, M. J.; Perner, J. (1998). "Metalinguistic awareness and theory of mind: Just two words for the same thing?". Cognitive Development. 13 (3): 279–305. doi:10.1016/s0885-2014(98)90012-0.
  18. ^ Perner, J.; Rendl, B; Garnham, A. (2007). "Objects of desire, thought, and reality: Problems of anchoring discourse referents in development". Mind & Language. 22 (5): 475–513. doi:10.1111/j.1468-0017.2007.00317.x.
  19. ^ a b Nurmsoo, E.; Robinson, E. J. (2009). "Children's trust in previously inaccurate informants who were well or poorly informed: When past errors can be excused" (PDF). Child Development. 80 (1): 23–27. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01243.x. PMID 19236390.
  20. ^ Robinson, E. J.; Haigh, S. N.; Nurmsoo, E. (2008). "Children's working understanding of knowledge sources: Confidence in knowledge gained from testimony" (PDF). Cognitive Development. 23: 105–118. doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2007.05.001.
  21. ^ Jaswal, V. K.; Carrington Croft, A.; Setia, A. R.; Cole, C. A. (2010). "Young children have a specific, highly robust bias to trust testimony". Psychological Science. 21 (10): 1541–1547. doi:10.1177/0956797610383438. PMC 3507998. PMID 20855905.
  22. ^ Baier, A (1986). "Trust and anti-trust". Ethics. 96 (2): 231–260. doi:10.1086/292745. S2CID 159454549.
  23. ^ Mascaro, O.; Sperber, D. (2009). "The moral, epistemic, and mind- reading components of children's vigilance towards deception". Cognition. 112 (3): 367–380. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2009.05.012. PMID 19540473. S2CID 7087125.
  24. ^ a b Lane, J. D.; Wellman, H. M.; Gelman, S. A. (2013). "Informants' traits weigh heavily in young children's trust in testimony and in their epistemic inferences". Child Development. 84 (4): 1253–1268. doi:10.1111/cdev.12029. PMC 3601569. PMID 23240893.
  25. ^ a b Birch, S. A. J.; Vauthier, S. A.; Bloom, P. (2008). "Three- and four-year-olds spontaneously use others' past performance to guide their learning". Cognition. 107 (3): 1018–1034. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2007.12.008. PMID 18295193. S2CID 6163343.
  26. ^ Robinson, E. J.; Champion, H.; Mitchell, P. (1999). "Children's ability to infer utterance veracity from speaker informedness". Developmental Psychology. 35 (2): 535–546. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.35.2.535. PMID 10082024.
  27. ^ Sabbagh, M. A.; Baldwin, D. A. (2001). "Learning words from knowledgeable versus ignorant speakers: Links between preschoolers' theory of mind and semantic development". Child Development. 72 (4): 1054–1070. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00334. PMID 11480934.
  28. ^ a b Xu, F.; Evans, A. D.; Li, C.; Li, Q.; Heyman, G.; Lee, K. (2013). "The role of honesty and benevolence in children's judgments of trustworthiness". International Journal of Behavioral Development. 37 (3): 257–265. doi:10.1177/0165025413479861. S2CID 143930568.
  29. ^ Bacon, F. (1999). Selected philosophical works. Indianapolis, IN: Hackett Publishing Company, Inc.
  30. ^ Bok, S. (1978). Lying: Moral choice in public and private life. New York, NY: Random House.
  31. ^ Kant, I. (1949). On a supposed right to lie from altruistic motives. In L.W. Beck (Ed.), Critical of practical reason and other writings (pp.346–350). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
  32. ^ Augustine, St. (1952). Treaties on various issues. Washington, DC: Catholic University of America Press.
  33. ^ see for example, Austin, J. L. (1962). How to do things with words. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  34. ^ See; Fu, G.; Evans, A. D.; Wang, L.; Lee, K. (2008). "Lying in the name of collective good: A developmental study". Developmental Science. 11 (4): 495–503. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00695.x. PMC 2570108. PMID 18576957.
  35. ^ a b Jaswal, V. K.; Neely, L. A. (2006). "Adults don't always know best: Preschoolers use past reliability over age when learning new words". Psychological Science. 17 (9): 757–758. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01778.x. PMID 16984291. S2CID 41301338.
  36. ^ Taylor, M.; Cartwright, B.S.; Bowden, T. (1991). "Perspective taking and theory of mind: Do children predict interpretive diversity as a function of differences in observers' knowledge?". Child Development. 62 (6): 1334–1351. doi:10.2307/1130810. JSTOR 1130810. PMID 1786719.
  37. ^ Wellman, H. M.; Bartsch, K. (1988). "Young children's reasoning about beliefs". Cognition. 30 (3): 239–277. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(88)90021-2. hdl:2027.42/27034. PMID 3215003. S2CID 37767773.
  38. ^ a b c d Koenig, M. A. (2012). "Beyond semantic accuracy: Preschoolers evaluate a speaker's reasons". Child Development. 83 (3): 1051–1063. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01742.x. PMID 22417290.
  39. ^ Heyman, G. D.; Legare, C. H. (2005). "Children's evaluation of sources of information about traits". Developmental Psychology. 41 (4): 636–647. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.41.4.636. PMID 16060810.
  40. ^ Heyman, G. D.; Sweet, M. A.; Lee, K. (2009). "Children's reasoning about lie-telling and truth-telling in politeness context". Social Development. 18 (3): 728–746. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.2008.00495.x. PMC 2975356. PMID 21072132.
  41. ^ a b c d Vanderbilt, K. E.; Liu, D.; Heyman, G. D. (2011). "The development of distrust". Child Development. 82 (5): 1372–1380. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01629.x. PMC 3169730. PMID 21824130.
  42. ^ a b Schraw, G.; Dennison, R. S. (1994). "Assessing metacognitive awareness". Contemporary Educational Psychology. 19 (4): 460–475. doi:10.1006/ceps.1994.1033.
  43. ^ a b Brown, A. L. (1987). Metacognition, executive control, self-regulation, and other more mysterious mechanisms. In F. E. Weinert & R. H. Kluwe (Eds.), Metacognition, motivation, and understanding (pp. 65-116). Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  44. ^ a b c d e Beck, S. R.; Robinson, E. J. (2001). "Children's ability to make tentative interpretations of ambiguous messages". Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 79 (1): 95–114. doi:10.1006/jecp.2000.2583. PMID 11292313.
  45. ^ a b Robinson, E. J.; Robinson, W. P (1982). "Knowing when you don't know enough: Children's judgments about ambiguous information". Cognition. 12 (3): 267–280. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(82)90034-8. PMID 6891310. S2CID 35497435.
  46. ^ Robinson, E. J.; Whittaker, S. J. (1985). "Children's responses to ambiguous messages and their understanding of ambiguity". Developmental Psychology. 21 (3): 446–454. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.21.3.446.
  47. ^ a b Mitchell, P . & Robinson, E. J. (1992). Children's understanding of the evidential connotation of 'know' in relation to overestimation of their own knowledge, Journal of Child Language, 19, 167-182.
  48. ^ Moore, C. Bryant; Furrow, D. (1989). "Mental terms and the development of certainty". Child Development. 60 (1): 167–171. doi:10.2307/1131082. JSTOR 1131082.
  49. ^ a b c d e Beck, S. R.; Robinson, E. J.; Freeth, M. M. (2008). "Children can resist making interpretations when uncertain?" (PDF). Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 99 (4): 252–270. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2007.06.002. PMID 17673251.
  50. ^ Singer, J. B.; Flavell, J. H. (1981). "Development of knowledge about communication: Children's evaluation of explicitly ambiguous messages". Child Development. 52 (4): 1211–1215. doi:10.2307/1129508. JSTOR 1129508. PMID 7318521.
  51. ^ a b c d e Robinson, E. J.; Mitchell, P. (1994). "Children's judgements of ignorance on the basis of absence of experience". British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 12 (2): 113–129. doi:10.1111/j.2044-835x.1994.tb00623.x.
  52. ^ Ganea, P. A.; Saylor, M. M. (2007). "Infants' use of shared linguistic information to clarify ambiguous requests". Child Development. 78 (2): 493–502. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01011.x. PMID 17381786.
  53. ^ a b c Robinson, E. J.; Apperly, I. A. (2001). "Children's difficulties with partial representations in ambiguous messages and referentially opaque contexts". Cognitive Development. 16: 595–615. doi:10.1016/s0885-2014(00)00035-6.
  54. ^ Robinson, E. J.; Rowley, M. G.; Beck, S. R.; Carroll, D. J.; Apperly, I. A. (2006). "Children's sensitivity to their own relative ignorance: Handling of possibilities under epistemic and physical uncertainty" (PDF). Child Development. 77 (6): 1642–1655. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00964.x. PMID 17107451.
  55. ^ Beck; McColgan, K. L. T.; Robinson, E. J.; Rowley, M. G. (2011). "Imagining what might be: Why children underestimate uncertainty" (PDF). Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 110 (4): 603–610. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2011.06.010. PMID 21798553.
  56. ^ Robinson, E. J.; Pendle, J.; Rowley, M. G.; Beck, S. R.; McColgan, K. L. T. (2009). "Guessing imagined and live chance events: Adults behave like children with live events" (PDF). British Journal of Psychology. 100 (4): 645–659. doi:10.1348/000712608x386810. PMID 19079952.
  57. ^ Nelson, T.O., & Narens, L. (1994). Why investigate metacognition? In J. Metcalife &A.P. Shimamura (Eds.), Metacognition. Knowing about knowing (pp. 1–25). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  58. ^ Koriat, A (1993). "How do we know that we know? The accessibility model of the feeling of knowing". Psychological Review. 100 (4): 609–639. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.100.4.609. PMID 8255951.
  59. ^ Lockl, K.; Schneider, W. (2002). "Developmental trends in children's feeling-of-knowing judgments". International Journal of Behavioral Development. 26 (4): 327–333. doi:10.1080/01650250143000210. S2CID 143958809.
  60. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o p q Goodman, G.S.; Melinder, A. (2007). "Child witness research and forensic interviews of young children: A review". Legal and Criminological Psychology. 12: 1–19. doi:10.1348/135532506x156620.
  61. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o p q r s t u v w x y z aa ab ac ad ae Cronch, L. E.; Viljoen, J. L.; Hansen, D. J. (2006). "Forensic interviewing in child sexual abuse cases: Current techniques and future directions". Aggression and Violent Behavior. 11 (3): 195–207. doi:10.1016/j.avb.2005.07.009. S2CID 34223151.
  62. ^ Wood, J.; Garven, S. (2000). "How sexual abuse interviews go astray: Implications for prosecutors, police and child protection services". Child Maltreatment. 5 (2): 109–118. doi:10.1177/1077559500005002003. PMID 11232084. S2CID 26722045.
  63. ^ a b Lamb, M. E.; Orbach, Y.; Hershkowitz, I.; Esplin, P. W.; Horowitz, D. (2007). "A structured forensic interview protocol improves the quality and informativeness of investigative interviews with children: A review of research using the NICHD Investigative Protocol". Child Abuse & Neglect. 31 (11–12): 1201–1231. doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2007.03.021. PMC 2180422. PMID 18023872.

children, information, this, article, multiple, issues, please, help, improve, discuss, these, issues, talk, page, learn, when, remove, these, template, messages, this, article, written, like, personal, reflection, personal, essay, argumentative, essay, that, . This article has multiple issues Please help improve it or discuss these issues on the talk page Learn how and when to remove these template messages This article is written like a personal reflection personal essay or argumentative essay that states a Wikipedia editor s personal feelings or presents an original argument about a topic Please help improve it by rewriting it in an encyclopedic style March 2014 Learn how and when to remove this template message This article s lead section may be too short to adequately summarize the key points Please consider expanding the lead to provide an accessible overview of all important aspects of the article March 2014 Learn how and when to remove this template message Children s use of information is an issue in ethics and child development Information is learned from many different sources and source monitoring see also source monitoring error is important in understanding how people use information and decide which information is credible Consider the example of a parent whose child has been diagnosed with hyperactivity the parent searches the internet for information reads books participates in an online chat room with other parents in the same situation and consults various medical professionals Some of these sources will be credible contain reliable information and others will not To be well informed the parent must filter information according to the reliability of the source Children learn about the world in much the same way They are told things by numerous people e g teachers parents siblings and friends see things on the television or internet and read information in books Can children be effective consumers of information At what age are they able to do this How do they deal with ambiguous resources This page will detail answers to those questions and others by drawing on peer reviewed scientific research Contents 1 The development of source monitoring 1 1 Source monitoring theory 1 2 Fuzzy trace theory 1 3 Schema theory 1 4 Person based perspective 1 5 Mental state reasoning model 2 The origins of knowledge 2 1 Knowledge and justification 2 2 Gaining knowledge through looking touching and asking 2 2 1 Age differences 2 3 Language and question phrasing 3 Judging the credibility of sources and understanding of trust 3 1 Adults credibility judgments 3 2 Children s ability to make credibility judgments 3 2 1 Credibility judgments based on knowledge and experience 3 2 2 Credibility judgments based on traits and motivation 3 2 3 Credibility judgments based on age and reliability 3 2 4 Credibility judgments based on reasoning or support 3 3 Children s development of distrust 4 Metacognition 4 1 Knowing you don t know 4 2 Children s processing of ignorant and ambiguous statements 4 3 The role of language and prior experience 4 4 Partial information 4 5 Delaying interpretation 4 6 Referents and meaning 4 7 Physical and epistemic uncertainty 4 8 Feeling of knowing FOK 5 Forensic applications 5 1 Forensic interviews and techniques 5 1 1 Factors that can impact disclosure 5 1 2 Improper interviewing 5 1 3 Types of interviewing 5 2 Recommendations for forensic interviewing 6 See also 7 ReferencesThe development of source monitoring editYoung children have more difficulty with understanding and recalling the sources of information than adults do 1 Although episodic memory improves throughout childhood development in the area of source monitoring tends to occur between the ages of 3 and 8 years 2 At 3 years children who are able to immediately recognize the source of the information they obtain have difficulty recalling this information after a short delay 3 The development of source monitoring is gradual and children achieve and display competency in certain aspects of source monitoring before others 2 The developmental trajectory of source monitoring provides insight into what cognitive factors are necessary prerequisites While there is no generally accepted unified theory for the development of source monitoring five major theories contribute ideas about how source monitoring develops in children source monitoring theory fuzzy trace theory schema theory person based perspective and the mental state reasoning model Source monitoring theory edit According to source monitoring theory the source of information is attributed through a decision making process where source is inferred based on various characteristics inherent in the memory itself 1 This means that the sources are not directly encoded but rather reconstructed when information is recalled This decision making process can either be through automatic unconscious processing or through heightened demanding and systematic processing that may require reasoning and the retrieval of supporting memories This theory implicates the development of episodic memory and memory strategies in the development of source monitoring more generally Fuzzy trace theory edit See also Fuzzy trace theory Fuzzy trace theory hypothesizes that the source monitoring errors that children make are caused by problems with memory storage and retrieval 4 Memories are simultaneously stored in two different formats the gist level extracted from the experience and the verbatim representation information in exact detail It is proposed that the source of information is encoded in memory as a verbatim detail Memories for verbatim details decay more quickly over time than gist representations and young children demonstrate faster decay of verbatim information than older children or adults Younger children are more likely to experience memory intrusions due to weaker memory traces which leads to a susceptibility to misleading information replacing memory traces from a previous event 5 For this reason developmental changes in episodic memory performance are viewed as the driving factor in source monitoring development Schema theory edit See also Schema psychology Schema theory as a derivative of script theory states that after repeated exposure to similar events individuals form a general representation of what typically happens 6 Some details are the same at each instance of repeated events and others can vary from instance to instance In the script for what usually happens there is a slot for each variable detail and the detail for a particular time is chosen from a list of possible variations If source information is encoded as a slot errors in source monitoring can be the result of incorrect retrieval of a specific detail This framework allows for the storage of a large amount of detailed information about specific events however is very cognitively demanding for children Because of their limited cognitive resources young children require more experience with repeated events in order to generate a schema Details that vary from instance to instance can be lost and children rely instead on the generalized event representation when attempting to recall a particular instance The main assumption is that the development of source monitoring depends on an increase in cognitive processing capacity Person based perspective edit Person based perspective emphasizes prospective processes Prospective processes relate actions to one another through operations such as sequencing and planning 7 These processes are affected by the perspective taken self vs other or the goals and meaning to the individual The meaning attached to the goal of an action can interfere with source monitoring by removing attention from the source details 8 The person based perspective is social cognitive in nature more so than other theories of source monitoring and assumes that development is reliant on socialization and theory of mind development Mental state reasoning model edit The mental state reasoning model highlights possible mechanisms behind young children s suggestibility 9 Children who have difficulty with reasoning about conflicting mental representations are likely to overwrite their original memories with misinformation because they cannot reconcile two contradicting views of what actually occurred 10 Source monitoring and the understanding of knowledge states play a key role in resisting suggestions The development of source monitoring is presumed to be based on better understanding of knowledge states metacognition and theory of mind The origins of knowledge editBeing aware of how we have acquired information is particularly difficult for young children specifically 3 to 5 year olds After feeling a soft ball 3 and 4 year olds can correctly identify whether they know the ball is soft or hard but cannot always say how they know Perhaps it is because they felt the ball saw the ball or were told it was soft The ability to recognize the origin of their knowledge requires the understanding of how knowledge is acquired 11 Knowledge and justification edit When asked knowledge questions Do you know what is in the box then justification questions How do or why don t you know what is in the box children who can correctly answer are able to reflect on knowledge that they have gained from a particular source and should be able to identify the source of information 11 12 3 and 4 year olds are better at answering these questions when the questions refer to themselves in comparison to referring to another person When these children are shown a hidden object they can correctly report their perceptual access responding correctly to Did you look into the box and their knowledge access of what is in the box responding correctly to Do you know not know what is in the box 12 Despite their ability to answer correctly when referring to the self 3 and 4 year olds have difficulty responding to the other s knowledge where they deny the other has the knowledge 3 4 year olds can correctly identify that the other person has in fact looked in a box but when asked Does other know what is in the box the child will deny that the other person knows what is in the box 12 The key assumption is then that although children are aware that perceptual access is needed they are unable to acknowledge that the knowledge was gained from perceptual access 12 13 Gaining knowledge through looking touching and asking edit Gaining knowledge is knowing how to acquire new information The ability to recognize how specific knowledge can be gained by perceptual access looking feeling or smelling is the understanding of Aspectuality Aspectuality understanding is the awareness that an object is made up of many different properties colour weight odour which can be determined by a specific perceptual action looking feeling smelling 11 14 Identifying the colour of a car outside the window could involve asking someone for the information or looking out the window In most cases adults would acquire this knowledge from simply looking out the window However not all information can be gained in this manner If you were asked to find out how old the driver of the car is simply looking would not provide accurate information you would have to ask These two situations are dependent on whether the information being sought out is visible or invisible respectively 15 Knowledge development depends on children s ability to efficiently pursue their informational goal Children do not always make the most effective or efficient decision when acquiring new information 15 Age differences edit 6 year olds are able to distinguish when gaining knowledge requires looking the information is visible or when gaining knowledge requires asking the information is invisible However 4 year olds do not perform as consistently Even with an expert present 4 year olds will overestimate the knowledge they can gain through looking However when the information to be gained is regarding a group of friends and the expert is a friend of the group 4 year olds tend to overestimate knowledge acquisition through asking 15 When given access to pairs of objects which could either be identified by seeing identical objects different in colour or by touch feeling identical objects 1 soft 1 hard children perform relatively well generally recognizing when they have adequate information and when they have inadequate information i e knowing the object is blue after seeing it or knowing the object is soft after feeling it 11 16 But when young children are not given access of seeing or feeling they have difficulty predicting and identifying which mode of access would allow them to identify the object 16 In this case 3 and 4 year olds overestimate the knowledge that could be gained through feeling the object 11 3 to 4 year olds may not understand the perceptual access needed to acquire the specific knowledge Language and question phrasing edit Waters and Beck 2012 state understanding the link between perceptual access and consequent information knowledge access is a crucial component in the development of theory of mind In a typical adult population change in the phrasing of a particular question would not affect the understanding of what action needs to occur What colour is the bike that is outside or Is the bike red should not change the action of looking to gain the needed information However children s performance is susceptible to this type of question phrasing 14 17 18 There are language effects on knowledge access through 3 different question types 14 Identity Find out which one is in the tunnel Dimension Find out what colour the one in the tunnel is Aspect Find out if the one in the tunnel is the red one or the blue one 4 and 5 year olds are more likely to respond correctly to aspect and dimension questions in comparison to identity questions 14 Aspect and dimension questions are more explicit which may be why performance is better in these categories Identity questions require more cognitive effort as one has to remember how the objects were similar and how they differed Judging the credibility of sources and understanding of trust editMuch of people s knowledge about the world is not obtained from direct experience but indirectly from what other people or sources tell them With widespread use of the internet people have access to nearly unlimited sources of information Some of that information might be conflicting and different sources of information vary in their accuracy and credibility People can also deliberately deceive be misunderstood or be mistaken 19 It is important that people develop the necessary skills to assess the accuracy of what they are being told Adults credibility judgments edit Adults make credibility judgments based on two factors expertise and trustworthiness For example we might trust information from published research articles more than information from blogs because we know that anyone can write a blog but scientists experts who write peer reviewed articles are highly trained With regards to trustworthiness adults are less likely to believe someone who they think is trying to deceive them and they take into account the intentions of the information provider For example if someone s intention is to sell you something you might be more skeptical of the accuracy of the information they provide because their motives cause you to question their honesty Adults can effectively use these cues to make judgments about the credibility of different sources but whether children can also do so is an important area for research Children s ability to make credibility judgments edit Even very young children show an early sensitivity to issues of source credibility By the age of 4 children show similar patterns to adults in a preference for perception over testimony that is they would rather see something with their own eyes than be told about it Children also feel more confident in their knowledge when they have directly perceived it than if they have been told by someone else even if the speaker is well informed 20 However children are not always able to directly perceive information and they learn much of what they know from others Some people are more credible sources of information than others so children must actively evaluate information and decide whether or not to believe it There are many factors or cues that children by the age of 4 take into consideration when making judgements about whether or not to trust what a person says For instance knowledge and experience traits motivations age and reasoning or support Credibility judgments based on knowledge and experience edit Young children have a reputation for credulity or believing something without having any proof that it is true Young children often trust what adults tell them especially when they have no prior knowledge or expectations about the topic of the testimony 21 Because children tend to interact with adults who are more knowledgeable than themselves if they have no reason to believe otherwise they will trust what adults tell them In particular 3 year olds tendency to believe others is based on a selective bias to trust what people tell them This selective trust is adaptive in the early years as they are learning language and their way around the world 22 Children have difficulty disregarding information provided to them even if the source has been unreliable in the past 3 year olds will often continue to believe what a person tells them even after being repeatedly deceived by that person but 4 year olds are far better able to disregard this unreliable information 23 Three year olds are better at making trust judgments when they are able to choose between two sources of knowledge rather than deciding whether or not to believe a single person and in this situation they are often able to choose the more reliable of two speakers 24 By age 4 young children take an informant s knowledge expertise and reliability into account in order to avoid learning from unreliable or problematic sources 25 They believe statements made by knowledgeable speakers more than ignorant speakers before they can explicitly answer questions about who has access to knowledge 26 They also prefer to seek information from sources who have been knowledgeable in the past 4 year olds can spontaneously use others past performance to guide their learning 27 Credibility judgments based on traits and motivation edit Almost all human institutions e g family community business government are built on a foundation of trust 28 There are many factors that influence children s trust in people and one of the most important is honesty 29 There are various schools of philosophical thought that posits honesty to be morally right and lying to be morally wrong On one end of the continuum philosophers like Bok 30 Kant 31 and St Augustine 32 hold a deontological view that focuses on intrinsic duties relating to the treatment of others In other words telling the truth is intrinsically right and lying is intrinsically wrong On the other end of the continuum is the utilitarian view that emphasizes the greater good specifically with respect to the outcome of one s act Therefore lying and its moral implications are context dependent 33 In some situations such as when being polite to spare another person s feelings making a prosocial lie or deliberate false statement are endorsed Children consider motivation as well as traits when deciding whom to seek information from and whom to believe In both Eastern and Western cultures both adults and children adhere to the utilitarian perspective when giving moral evaluations of truths and lies in different social situations 34 In terms of people s characteristics children tend to place trust in people who are honest smart or kind over people who are dishonest not smart or not kind 24 However they also consider a person s intent or motivation From age 7 children consider both honesty and benevolence when making trust judgments about other people and older children are more likely to trust people who tell prosocial lies to avoid hurting another person s feelings or to help another person than young children 28 For younger children honesty is more important than a person s intention As children get older they increasingly attend to motivation as a key factor The relationship between telling the truth and trusting a person is stable but when it comes to lying children consider the motivation of the speaker when deciding whether or not to trust them Credibility judgments based on age and reliability edit Children evaluate the credibility of a speaker by evaluating how reliable that person has been in the past and can make predictions about a speaker based on their age 35 Children as young as 3 years old prefer to trust an unfamiliar adult rather than an unfamiliar child 36 When considering both age and reliability age is often the primary cue used to determine another s credibility For example 3 and 4 year olds found adults to be more trustworthy than peers unless the peer demonstrated greater reliability i e adult incorrectly mislabelled objects whereas peer correctly labelled them 35 Children also consider both the prior history of accuracy and the level of perceptual access the speaker has when they provide information Young children spontaneously keep track of the prior history of a person s accuracy or inaccuracy reliability and prefer to learn from someone with a good track record 25 Children commonly interpret the speaker s history of inaccuracy as a lasting trait and so the speaker is considered an unreliable informant at least within the domain they have been wrong about However under certain conditions children may excuse a person s past inaccuracy and later trust that person for information If a speaker has limited information e g lack of perceptual access in making a claim for example inaccurately identifying a toy while blindfolded then children as young as 3 years old appropriately excuse their past inaccuracy especially when they are later well informed 19 On the other hand if a speaker has full access to information while making an inaccurate claim children continue to regard him her as unreliable Credibility judgments based on reasoning or support edit Young children appreciate that people generally think and act sensibly when reasons are present and they can evaluate a claim by assessing the quality of the reasoning provided 37 Thus children create an epistemic profile of a person based on the quality of the reasons they offer when making a claim 38 As young as 3 years old children understand the difference between weak versus strong reasoning to support a statement Children are more likely to trust someone when strong support is provided through reliable testimony My teacher told me there s a book in the bag I think that there s a book looking Before I came here I looked and saw a ball in the bag I think there s a ball in there and inference It s a backpack Backpack holds books I think there s a book in there 38 On the other hand desire I like crayons I want there to be crayons in the bag I think that there are crayons in there pretense I like to pretend I m going to pretend that there s a sandwich in the bag I think there s a sandwich in there and guessing I don t know I m going to guess that there s a toy in the bag are not viewed as strong support for a claim 38 Children recognize that the mental states that they and others may hold are not always reliable means for drawing specific conclusions 3 and 4 year olds can also choose the more reasonable of two people and continue to seek as well as accept new information from the more credible person the one who had better reasons in the past 38 Children s development of distrust edit People are not always reliable in their provision of information and it is important to be able to differentiate between good sources and bad sources Assessing someone s reliability is based not only on the knowledgeability of the speaker but their motives intentions as well People may not always be motivated to tell the truth instead they may potentially lie to promote their own interest 39 or the interest of others 40 At about the age they begin preschool children become better at distinguishing between helpful and deceptive people 3 year olds are not able to identify who is trying to help or trick another person and accept advice from both helpers and trickers 41 On the other hand 4 year olds are more sceptical and could differentiate between helpers and trickers but have no preference in choosing whom to accept advice from 41 There may be a mismatch between knowledge and behaviour among 4 year olds in which they do not understand the implications of their knowledge or how to successfully apply it to their behaviour 5 year olds systematically preferred advice from helpers 41 Ultimately theory of mind or children s understanding of mental states is related to selective trust in helpers versus trickers Beginning at 5 years old children use a person s prior history of deception to make reliability judgments about that person 41 Metacognition editMetacognition is an ability that allows people to think about their own thinking process and how to use their thinking process to help themselves learn better Metacognition includes two separate abilities 1 knowledge of cognition and 2 regulation of cognition 42 43 Knowledge of one s thinking process is not enough to regulate an individual s behaviour and are required to use specific strategies to help them regulate their behaviour 42 43 Knowing you don t know edit An important skill children need to learn is being able to identify when a message is incomplete and they don t have enough information to make an accurate interpretation Being aware that an ambiguous situation has arisen is difficult for young children Children accurately know when they know but often overestimate when they don t know 44 45 Children s behaviour does not seem to match their verbal ability to acknowledge their lack of knowledge Despite incorrectly stating that they know something children are still capable of changing their response upon hearing contradicting information to an initial interpretation and or event 44 46 Language plays an important role in children s accuracy in assessing their own knowledge For children to accurately know what they know it is important for them to understand the various meanings of the word know as well as language used to describe certainty and uncertainty 47 48 Children s processing of ignorant and ambiguous statements edit Ambiguous information is a piece of information word message or view with multiple interpretations 49 Adults not only have the awareness to spot ambiguity but they also have ways to deal with it But young children cannot do the same Typically children cannot successfully deal with ambiguous statements until they are 6 or 7 years old 44 50 However it seems in certain contexts with certain tasks younger children also can somewhat deal with unclear or ambiguous statements 47 The role of language and prior experience edit Language can affect whether one can correctly deal with ambiguous statements One can know someone in many different ways for example seeing them talking to them having a prior history with them having a relationship with them etc This makes questions like Do you know very complex and hard to answer Clearer definitions of to know seem to help children to check their own knowledge better Children as young as 4 years could make far more accurate statements about their actual knowledge when a question was phrased Have you heard of rather than Do you know 51 By the age of 6 children typically could accurately check their knowledge with very little impact on their future answers regardless of the language used 4 5 year old s on the other hand were so changeable that the phrase used affected their future answers 4 5 year old s were also less likely to overestimate their knowledge of a target person if the starting question was phrased Have you heard of rather than Do you know 51 Not only can responses from children be altered by the question s phrasing but they can also change their response when the question suggests they are not familiar with the person In a study where children were asked if they really knew who a specific person was between 2 pictures of people they had never seen before they were more accurate in assessing that they didn t know when it was suggested that the target person had never been to their before 51 While experience is important children tend not to over rely on prior experiences and only use it when they ve had enough experience that can help assess their knowledge 51 Partial information edit Sometimes when confronted with ambiguous information more than one piece of information is required to make and accurate interpretation For example in a study where children had to pick one of four pictures presented to them after hearing ambiguous information they were capable of making tentative interpretations and then correctly changing their interpretations upon hearing contradicting clarifying information 44 This strategy uses multiple pieces of information and has been seen in children as young as 15 months old 52 In situations where only partial information is available young children make the best interpretation possible with the information given and go on to change this interpretation only when contradicting information emerges 45 Delaying interpretation edit Clarifying Disambiguating information is not always instantly available In these situations adults seem to delay interpretation and seek clarifying information when appropriate Children find it harder to understand what actions they need to take to clarify than to identify an ambiguous situation that pops up There are 2 different types of delay in interpreting ambiguous information one that is intentional and one that is instinctual 49 The decision to delay interpretation and seek further information is a difficult one as it involves being aware that the information given at that point are not sufficient and knowing how to ask for the ones necessary for clarification Children are typically failures in this process until the age of 7 44 However when this explicit decision to delay is simplified children aged 5 and up showed some ability in successfully choosing to delay their response 49 While the ability to intentionally delay interpretation seems to be difficult the ability to delay interpretation instinctively seems to be easier for young children In one example children were asked to stamp the correct snowman once they knew which one it was 49 Their knowledge of the correct snowman was based on a researcher slowly revealing the target snowman The children s cards contained snowmen that differed by some feature visible on the 2nd half of the snowman only and therefore the children were required to wait until the 2nd half of the snowman was revealed in order to accurately assess which snowman was the correct one In this scenario focusing on instinct children as young as 5 were able to accurately delay interpretation 49 Therefore while it is difficult for children to explicitly demonstrate their awareness of an ambiguous situation and how to resolve it they are implicitly able to handle situations in which delaying interpretation may be beneficial Referents and meaning edit It is difficult for younger children to grasp the idea that objects can be referred to in different ways and that people can have partial knowledge of the different references i e a bouncy ball might also be referred to as a rubber sphere A child might know by looking that a toy is a toy truck but they may not be aware that the toy truck is also a present Referential opacity is the concept of whether or not referring to an object changes its meaning If something is referentially transparent substitution insensitive altering the referent term does not alter the meaning and something that is referentially opaque substitution sensitive means that altering the referent term would alter the meaning An example of this is a study with a puppet named Heinz 53 There is a ball in a box and children are told that Heinz knows that there is a ball in the box but does not know that the ball is a present Children are then asked substitution insensitive questions i e Does Heinz know the ball is a present asking Does Heinz know the rubber sphere is a present does not alter the meaning of the question and substitution sensitive questions i e Does Heinz know there s a present in the box asking Does Heinz know there is a rubber sphere in the box does alter the meaning of the question Regardless of age substitution insensitive questions seem to be easier than substitution sensitive questions The ability to correctly answer substitution sensitive questions improves with age 53 The ability to answer these types of questions is closely related to effectively evaluating ambiguous messages Success on substitution insensitive questions is necessary but not sufficient for success on evaluation ambiguous messages Alternatively success on substitution sensitive questions is necessary and sufficient for success at evaluating ambiguous messages 53 Physical and epistemic uncertainty edit Children can change their interpretation of information based on the context in which the interpretation is made 54 Robinson and colleagues 2006 studied children s interpretation of information in two different physical and epistemic uncertainty Physical uncertainty occurs when an event has not yet happened and therefore the outcome of that event has not been determined i e the dice has not yet been rolled Epistemic uncertainty occurs when an event has already occurred but the child is not aware of the outcome of the event i e the dice have been rolled but the dice are hidden from the participant 4 to 8 year old children have the ability to realize multiple possibilities for an event that has not yet occurred physical uncertainty however they do not seem to acknowledge that there are exactly the same possibilities for an event that has already happened when they don t know the outcome epistemic uncertainty Under the conditions of epistemic uncertainty children simply guess one of the possibilities Beck and colleagues 2011 propose that this happens because it is much easier to imagine the outcome during epistemic uncertainty basically knowing that there is only one outcome 55 Similarly adults also prefer to make predictions or guess in epistemic uncertainty 56 Feeling of knowing FOK edit Feeling of knowing occurs when people are unable to easily recall a memory or a fact but they know that they learned it and are able to recognize it such as in a multiple choice test 57 Adults accuracy of feeling of knowing judgments is well above chance but not nearly perfect 58 It seems to be the same for children as well Lockl and Schneider 2002 did not find any developmental trends in the accuracy of feeling of knowing judgments Instead similar to adults children s accuracy of feeling of knowing judgments was low but still above chance for all age groups studied grade 1 through grade 4 59 Getting a child to attend to this feeling of knowing through language or prompting of prior experience is one way to assist them in more accurately assessing their actual knowledge allowing them to handle ambiguous situations at a much younger age 51 Forensic applications editChildren are highly susceptible to a suggestibility effect producing false memories and or incorrect post event information see misinformation effect when asked to engage in memory recall 5 This has important implications for forensic interviewing and child witness testimony 60 Consider the prominent case of Kelly Michael s see Wee Care Nursery School abuse trial where improper interviewing techniques lead to a miscarriage of justice In order to develop reliable and age appropriate instruments for interviewing children it is imperative to consider their cognitive development verbal and mental abilities 5 60 Forensic interviews and techniques edit Factors that can impact disclosure edit It is critical for a forensic interviewer to recognize their power during an interview especially with a child witness An interviewer can impact the course of a child s testimony in numerous ways including Eliciting false allegations through leading and suggestive questions 61 Determining the amount of detail a child may disclose 61 Prevent a child from disclosing 61 Interviewer s gender may also impact the quality and details of disclosure Children between the ages of 4 and 6 are more likely to disclose to an interviewer of the same gender 61 The age of a child is also an important factor during interviewing Younger children are more likely to provide shorter less detailed accounts of an event in comparison to older children 61 Preschool children are more likely to disclose information in an accidental way through triggers and obvious cues whereas older school children are more likely to make intentional disclosures based on the nature of the question they are asked 61 Improper interviewing edit There are varying techniques and extraneous factors that can influence the way a child discloses an event during child witness testimony e g experiences of abuse by a parent or caregiver There are two major types of barriers in forensic interviews 1 improper interviewing and 2 clumsy interviewing 62 61 Improper interviewing includes forensic techniques that are considered to be risky and ineffective 61 Each of the following techniques can create critically negative consequences in witness testimony and result in false allegations or the potential for a reduced conviction 61 Use of reinforcement If an interviewer utilizes rewards and punishments to coerce disclosure 61 Social influence If an interviewer informs a child of another child s disclosure or recollection 61 Asking suggestive or leading questions This can occur when an interviewer provides a child with information that has not been previously known 61 Removing the child from direct experience For example asking a child what might have happened 61 Clumsy interviewing involves interviews that fail to include best practices and recommended techniques 61 Interviewers who are not properly trained in forensic techniques can fail to follow structured interview guidelines and impact the outcome of a child s testimony and or responses to questioning This type of interviewing most often occurs when an interviewer lacks skill forgets important procedures and when there is a lack of necessary supervision 61 In order to prevent improper forensic interviewing numerous methods to reduce suggestibility and the misinformation effect have been proven effective including taping interviews recording transcripts ensuring supervision by a qualified professional experience in working with children training in forensic interviewing and maintaining a comfortable safe environment 61 Types of interviewing edit The cognitive interview utilizes cognitive theory as a focal point in its interview methodology The cognitive interview first developed in 1992 by researchers Fisher and Geiselman was originally developed for adults and later modified for children It utilizes two major perspectives from cognitive theory including the encoding specificity principle and a multi component view of memory traces 60 Specifically this method utilizes four major techniques 60 Report everything i e tell me everything you remember 60 Context reinstatement i e think back to where you were at the time 60 Reverse order i e now that you told me what happened try to remember it again but this time starting at the end and recounting it in reverse chronological order 60 Change perspective i e what would the perpetrator have seen and heard 60 The National Institute of Child Health and Human Development NICHD Protocol developed an investigative protocol in 2000 to create a structured interview technique for children specifically those of child sexual abuse 60 61 63 It uses the following techniques 60 63 Introductory phase This is necessary for rapport building 60 Practice in free recall Tell me about your last birthday party 60 Information about the ground rules It s okay to say I don t know 60 Open ended questions Tell me why you came to talk to me 60 Stepwise interview utilizes open ended questions through a funnel like strategy 60 It is primarily used by legal professionals and is most often used in North America 60 This interview begins with open ended questions and or free recall and slowly incorporates more focused and detailed questions 60 Allegation blind interviews stress that an interviewer should refrain from gathering information prior to an interview in order to reduce suggestibility and increase interviewer patience and attentiveness 61 This also enhances the interviewer s ability to be non judgmental and objective 61 Truth lie discussions are most useful prior to commencing abuse related questioning This method allows the interviewer to create a baseline with the child about what the truth is and what a lie is 61 The interviewer is encouraged to ask questions with general examples such as tell me a lie about this chair 61 This strategy has been proven to result in a less misinformed child testimony 61 Touch survey was developed on the basis that touch falls on a continuum and is beneficial to screen for child abuse 61 It includes questions surrounding the child s experiences with touch e g kissing hugging hitting including where they have been touched and by whom 61 This tool might be more useful when used in conjunction with other forensic strategies 61 Recommendations for forensic interviewing edit Although there are varying suggestions for structured forensic interviewing experts provide context into best practices that can significantly reduce suggestibility false memories and the misinformation effect 61 Interviews should be conducted in a safe child friendly environment 61 A child s age should be considered when being interviewed and practices should be modified accordingly 61 Structured interview protocol should always be utilized 61 Interviewers should engage in professional training when possible 61 Ground rules are essential for the beginning of the interview so that the child is aware of the type of responses they can provide e g I don t know 61 See also editCognitive development Developmental psychology Source monitoring errorReferences edit a b Lindsay D S Johnson M K Kwon P 1991 Developmental changes in memory source monitoring Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 52 3 297 318 doi 10 1016 0022 0965 91 90065 z PMID 1770330 a b Roberts K P 2002 Children s ability to distinguish between memories from multiple sources Implications for the quality and accuracy of eyewitness statements Developmental Review 22 3 403 435 doi 10 1016 S0273 2297 02 00005 9 S2CID 59933611 Gopnik A Graf P 1988 Knowing how you know Young children s ability to identify and remember the sources of their beliefs Child Development 59 5 1366 1371 doi 10 1111 j 1467 8624 1988 tb01505 x Brainerd C J Reyna V F 1990 Gist is the grist Fuzzy trace theory and the new intuitionism Developmental Review 10 1 3 47 doi 10 1016 0273 2297 90 90003 m a b c Welch Ross M K Diecidue K Miller S A 1997 Young children s understanding of conflicting mental representation predicts suggestibility Developmental Psychology 33 1 43 53 doi 10 1037 0012 1649 33 1 43 PMID 9050389 Farrar M J amp Goodman G S 1990 Developmental differences in the relation between scripts and episodic memory Do they exist In Knowing and remembering in young children Fivush Robyn Hudson Judith A eds pp 30 64 Cambridge University Press New York NY Ratner H H Foley M A 1996 A unifying framework for the development of children s activity memory Advances in Child Development and Behavior 25 33 105 doi 10 1016 S0065 2407 08 60050 6 ISBN 9780120097258 PMID 7847174 Foley M A Ratner H H 1998 Distinguishing between memories for thoughts and deeds The role of prospective processing in children s source monitoring British Journal of Developmental Psychology 16 4 465 484 doi 10 1111 j 2044 835x 1998 tb00765 x Welch Ross M 2000 A mental state reasoning model of suggestibility and memory source monitoring In Children s source monitoring Roberts Kim P Blades Mark eds pp 227 255 Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers Mahwah NJ Bright Paul A Jarrold C Wright D B 2008 Theory of mind development influences suggestibility and source monitoring Developmental Psychology 44 4 1055 1068 doi 10 1037 0012 1649 44 4 1055 PMID 18605834 a b c d e O Neill D K Astington J W Flavell J H 1992 Young children s understanding of the role that sensory experiences play in knowledge acquisition Child Development 63 2 474 490 doi 10 1111 j 1467 8624 1992 tb01641 x PMID 1611948 a b c d Wimmer H Hogrefe Perner J 1988 Children s understanding of informational access as source of knowledge Child Development 59 2 386 396 doi 10 2307 1130318 JSTOR 1130318 Pratt C Bryant P 1990 Young children understand that looking leads to knowing so long as they are looking into a single barrel Child Development 61 4 973 982 doi 10 1111 j 1467 8624 1990 tb02835 x PMID 2209200 a b c d Waters G M Beck S R 2012 How should we question young children s understanding of aspectuality The British Journal of Developmental Psychology 30 3 376 392 doi 10 1111 j 2044 835X 2011 02044 x PMID 22882369 a b c Fitneva S A Lam N H L Dunfield K A 2013 The development of children s information gathering To look or to ask Developmental Psychology 49 3 533 542 doi 10 1037 a0031326 hdl 21 11116 0000 0002 5F86 D PMID 23316769 a b Robinson E J Haigh S N Pendle J E C 2008 Children s working understanding of the knowledge gained from seeing and feeling PDF Developmental Science 11 2 299 305 doi 10 1111 j 1467 7687 2008 00676 x PMID 18333983 Doherty M J Perner J 1998 Metalinguistic awareness and theory of mind Just two words for the same thing Cognitive Development 13 3 279 305 doi 10 1016 s0885 2014 98 90012 0 Perner J Rendl B Garnham A 2007 Objects of desire thought and reality Problems of anchoring discourse referents in development Mind amp Language 22 5 475 513 doi 10 1111 j 1468 0017 2007 00317 x a b Nurmsoo E Robinson E J 2009 Children s trust in previously inaccurate informants who were well or poorly informed When past errors can be excused PDF Child Development 80 1 23 27 doi 10 1111 j 1467 8624 2008 01243 x PMID 19236390 Robinson E J Haigh S N Nurmsoo E 2008 Children s working understanding of knowledge sources Confidence in knowledge gained from testimony PDF Cognitive Development 23 105 118 doi 10 1016 j cogdev 2007 05 001 Jaswal V K Carrington Croft A Setia A R Cole C A 2010 Young children have a specific highly robust bias to trust testimony Psychological Science 21 10 1541 1547 doi 10 1177 0956797610383438 PMC 3507998 PMID 20855905 Baier A 1986 Trust and anti trust Ethics 96 2 231 260 doi 10 1086 292745 S2CID 159454549 Mascaro O Sperber D 2009 The moral epistemic and mind reading components of children s vigilance towards deception Cognition 112 3 367 380 doi 10 1016 j cognition 2009 05 012 PMID 19540473 S2CID 7087125 a b Lane J D Wellman H M Gelman S A 2013 Informants traits weigh heavily in young children s trust in testimony and in their epistemic inferences Child Development 84 4 1253 1268 doi 10 1111 cdev 12029 PMC 3601569 PMID 23240893 a b Birch S A J Vauthier S A Bloom P 2008 Three and four year olds spontaneously use others past performance to guide their learning Cognition 107 3 1018 1034 doi 10 1016 j cognition 2007 12 008 PMID 18295193 S2CID 6163343 Robinson E J Champion H Mitchell P 1999 Children s ability to infer utterance veracity from speaker informedness Developmental Psychology 35 2 535 546 doi 10 1037 0012 1649 35 2 535 PMID 10082024 Sabbagh M A Baldwin D A 2001 Learning words from knowledgeable versus ignorant speakers Links between preschoolers theory of mind and semantic development Child Development 72 4 1054 1070 doi 10 1111 1467 8624 00334 PMID 11480934 a b Xu F Evans A D Li C Li Q Heyman G Lee K 2013 The role of honesty and benevolence in children s judgments of trustworthiness International Journal of Behavioral Development 37 3 257 265 doi 10 1177 0165025413479861 S2CID 143930568 Bacon F 1999 Selected philosophical works Indianapolis IN Hackett Publishing Company Inc Bok S 1978 Lying Moral choice in public and private life New York NY Random House Kant I 1949 On a supposed right to lie from altruistic motives In L W Beck Ed Critical of practical reason and other writings pp 346 350 Chicago IL University of Chicago Press Augustine St 1952 Treaties on various issues Washington DC Catholic University of America Press see for example Austin J L 1962 How to do things with words Cambridge MA Harvard University Press See Fu G Evans A D Wang L Lee K 2008 Lying in the name of collective good A developmental study Developmental Science 11 4 495 503 doi 10 1111 j 1467 7687 2008 00695 x PMC 2570108 PMID 18576957 a b Jaswal V K Neely L A 2006 Adults don t always know best Preschoolers use past reliability over age when learning new words Psychological Science 17 9 757 758 doi 10 1111 j 1467 9280 2006 01778 x PMID 16984291 S2CID 41301338 Taylor M Cartwright B S Bowden T 1991 Perspective taking and theory of mind Do children predict interpretive diversity as a function of differences in observers knowledge Child Development 62 6 1334 1351 doi 10 2307 1130810 JSTOR 1130810 PMID 1786719 Wellman H M Bartsch K 1988 Young children s reasoning about beliefs Cognition 30 3 239 277 doi 10 1016 0010 0277 88 90021 2 hdl 2027 42 27034 PMID 3215003 S2CID 37767773 a b c d Koenig M A 2012 Beyond semantic accuracy Preschoolers evaluate a speaker s reasons Child Development 83 3 1051 1063 doi 10 1111 j 1467 8624 2012 01742 x PMID 22417290 Heyman G D Legare C H 2005 Children s evaluation of sources of information about traits Developmental Psychology 41 4 636 647 doi 10 1037 0012 1649 41 4 636 PMID 16060810 Heyman G D Sweet M A Lee K 2009 Children s reasoning about lie telling and truth telling in politeness context Social Development 18 3 728 746 doi 10 1111 j 1467 9507 2008 00495 x PMC 2975356 PMID 21072132 a b c d Vanderbilt K E Liu D Heyman G D 2011 The development of distrust Child Development 82 5 1372 1380 doi 10 1111 j 1467 8624 2011 01629 x PMC 3169730 PMID 21824130 a b Schraw G Dennison R S 1994 Assessing metacognitive awareness Contemporary Educational Psychology 19 4 460 475 doi 10 1006 ceps 1994 1033 a b Brown A L 1987 Metacognition executive control self regulation and other more mysterious mechanisms In F E Weinert amp R H Kluwe Eds Metacognition motivation and understanding pp 65 116 Hillsdale New Jersey Lawrence Erlbaum Associates a b c d e Beck S R Robinson E J 2001 Children s ability to make tentative interpretations of ambiguous messages Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 79 1 95 114 doi 10 1006 jecp 2000 2583 PMID 11292313 a b Robinson E J Robinson W P 1982 Knowing when you don t know enough Children s judgments about ambiguous information Cognition 12 3 267 280 doi 10 1016 0010 0277 82 90034 8 PMID 6891310 S2CID 35497435 Robinson E J Whittaker S J 1985 Children s responses to ambiguous messages and their understanding of ambiguity Developmental Psychology 21 3 446 454 doi 10 1037 0012 1649 21 3 446 a b Mitchell P amp Robinson E J 1992 Children s understanding of the evidential connotation of know in relation to overestimation of their own knowledge Journal of Child Language 19 167 182 Moore C Bryant Furrow D 1989 Mental terms and the development of certainty Child Development 60 1 167 171 doi 10 2307 1131082 JSTOR 1131082 a b c d e Beck S R Robinson E J Freeth M M 2008 Children can resist making interpretations when uncertain PDF Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 99 4 252 270 doi 10 1016 j jecp 2007 06 002 PMID 17673251 Singer J B Flavell J H 1981 Development of knowledge about communication Children s evaluation of explicitly ambiguous messages Child Development 52 4 1211 1215 doi 10 2307 1129508 JSTOR 1129508 PMID 7318521 a b c d e Robinson E J Mitchell P 1994 Children s judgements of ignorance on the basis of absence of experience British Journal of Developmental Psychology 12 2 113 129 doi 10 1111 j 2044 835x 1994 tb00623 x Ganea P A Saylor M M 2007 Infants use of shared linguistic information to clarify ambiguous requests Child Development 78 2 493 502 doi 10 1111 j 1467 8624 2007 01011 x PMID 17381786 a b c Robinson E J Apperly I A 2001 Children s difficulties with partial representations in ambiguous messages and referentially opaque contexts Cognitive Development 16 595 615 doi 10 1016 s0885 2014 00 00035 6 Robinson E J Rowley M G Beck S R Carroll D J Apperly I A 2006 Children s sensitivity to their own relative ignorance Handling of possibilities under epistemic and physical uncertainty PDF Child Development 77 6 1642 1655 doi 10 1111 j 1467 8624 2006 00964 x PMID 17107451 Beck McColgan K L T Robinson E J Rowley M G 2011 Imagining what might be Why children underestimate uncertainty PDF Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 110 4 603 610 doi 10 1016 j jecp 2011 06 010 PMID 21798553 Robinson E J Pendle J Rowley M G Beck S R McColgan K L T 2009 Guessing imagined and live chance events Adults behave like children with live events PDF British Journal of Psychology 100 4 645 659 doi 10 1348 000712608x386810 PMID 19079952 Nelson T O amp Narens L 1994 Why investigate metacognition In J Metcalife amp A P Shimamura Eds Metacognition Knowing about knowing pp 1 25 Cambridge MA MIT Press Koriat A 1993 How do we know that we know The accessibility model of the feeling of knowing Psychological Review 100 4 609 639 doi 10 1037 0033 295x 100 4 609 PMID 8255951 Lockl K Schneider W 2002 Developmental trends in children s feeling of knowing judgments International Journal of Behavioral Development 26 4 327 333 doi 10 1080 01650250143000210 S2CID 143958809 a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o p q Goodman G S Melinder A 2007 Child witness research and forensic interviews of young children A review Legal and Criminological Psychology 12 1 19 doi 10 1348 135532506x156620 a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o p q r s t u v w x y z aa ab ac ad ae Cronch L E Viljoen J L Hansen D J 2006 Forensic interviewing in child sexual abuse cases Current techniques and future directions Aggression and Violent Behavior 11 3 195 207 doi 10 1016 j avb 2005 07 009 S2CID 34223151 Wood J Garven S 2000 How sexual abuse interviews go astray Implications for prosecutors police and child protection services Child Maltreatment 5 2 109 118 doi 10 1177 1077559500005002003 PMID 11232084 S2CID 26722045 a b Lamb M E Orbach Y Hershkowitz I Esplin P W Horowitz D 2007 A structured forensic interview protocol improves the quality and informativeness of investigative interviews with children A review of research using the NICHD Investigative Protocol Child Abuse amp Neglect 31 11 12 1201 1231 doi 10 1016 j chiabu 2007 03 021 PMC 2180422 PMID 18023872 Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Children 27s use of information amp oldid 1187945462, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.