fbpx
Wikipedia

Agreeableness

Agreeableness is a personality trait manifesting itself in individual behavioral characteristics that are perceived as kind, sympathetic, cooperative, warm, and considerate.[1] In contemporary personality psychology, agreeableness is one of the five major dimensions of personality structure, reflecting individual differences in cooperation and social harmony.[2]

People who score high on this dimension are empathetic and altruistic, while a low agreeableness score relates to selfish behavior (often manifesting as stinginess) and a lack of empathy.[3][4] Those who score very low on agreeableness show signs of dark triad behavior such as manipulation and competing with others rather than cooperating.[5]

Agreeableness is considered to be a superordinate trait, meaning that it is a grouping of personality sub-traits that cluster together statistically. The lower-level traits, or facets, grouped under agreeableness are: trust, straightforwardness, altruism, compliance, modesty, and tender-mindedness.[6]

History

Cattell's 16 Personality Factors

 
Agreeable Burden (Fardeau agréable) (William-Adolphe Bouguereau, 1895)

Like all Big Five personality traits, the roots of the modern concept of agreeableness can be traced to a 1936 study by Gordon Allport and Henry S. Odbert.[7] Seven years later, Raymond Cattell published a cluster analysis of the thousands of personality-related words identified by Allport and Odbert.[8] The clusters identified in this study served as a foundation for Cattell's further attempts to identify fundamental, universal, human personality factors.[9] He eventually settled on 16 personality factors through the use of factor analysis. Further factor analyses revealed five higher-order, or "global", factors to encompass these 16.[10] Although labelled "independence" by Cattell, a global factor defined by high scores on the E, H, L, and Q1 factors of the 16PF Questionnaire was an early precursor to the modern concept of agreeableness.[11]

The Big Five

Extent of agreeableness in the five factor model of personality is most commonly assessed through self-report measures, although peer-reports and third-party observation can also be used. Self-report measures are either lexical[1] or based on statements.[12] Which measure of either type is used is determined by an assessment of psychometric properties and the time and space constraints of the research being undertaken.

Lexical measures use individual adjectives that reflect agreeableness or disagreeableness traits, such as sympathetic, cooperative, warm, considerate, harsh, unkind, rude. Words representing disagreeableness are reverse coded. Goldberg (1992)[13] developed a 20-word measure as part of his 100-word Big Five markers, and Saucier (1994)[14] developed a briefer 8-word measure as part of his 40-word mini-markers. Thompson (2008)[1] systematically revised and improved these markers to develop a 40-word measure with better psychometric properties in both American and non-American populations, the International English Mini-Markers. This brief measure has good internal consistency reliabilities and other validity for assessing agreeableness and other five factor personality dimensions, both within and, especially, without American populations. Internal consistency reliability of the Agreeableness measure for native English-speakers is reported as .86, that for non-native English-speakers is .80.

Statement measures tend to comprise more words, and hence consume more research instrument space, than lexical measures. Respondents are asked the extent to which they, for example, Am on good terms with nearly everyone, Am not interested in other people's problems or Sympathize with others' feelings.[12]

Cattell's factor analytic approach, used to identify the universal personality structures, inspired countless studies in the decades following the introduction of the 16PF. Using Cattell's original clusters, the 16 Personality Factors, and original data, multiple researchers independently developed a five factor model of personality over this period. From the early 1960s on, these explorations typically included a factor called "agreeableness" or "sociability."[11][15] Despite repeated replications of five stable personality factors following Cattell's pioneering work, this framework only began to dominate personality research in the early 1980s with the work of Lewis Goldberg. Using lexical studies similar to those of Allport and Odbert, Goldberg chose the term "Big Five" to reflect the sheer number of personality-related terms encompassed by each of these five distinct factors.[11] One of these, agreeableness, was defined by a number of personality-related words similar to those present in earlier and more recent manifestations of the construct; examples include "friendly," "good-natured," "cooperative," "trustful," "nurturing," "sociable," and "considerate."[16][17]

NEO PI

Beginning in the 1970s, Paul Costa and Robert McCrae began researching the development of personality assessments based on factor models. Beginning with cluster analyses of Cattell's 16 Personality Factors, Costa and McCrae initially settled on a three-factor model of personality. These three factors were neuroticism (vs. emotional stability), extraversion (vs. introversion), and openness (vs. closedness) to experience, resulting in the acronym "NEO."[18] Due to similarities between their three-factor NEO Personality Inventory and Goldberg's Big Five, Costa and McCrae began to develop scales to assess agreeableness and conscientiousness in the early 1980s.[11] This work culminated in the 1985 publication of the first NEO PI Manual to be based on the full Five Factor Model.[19] Although this marked the introduction of agreeableness to the NEO PI, Costa and McCrae worked for an additional seven years to identify and elaborate on the facets comprising this factor in the Revised NEO Personality Inventory.[20]

NEO PI facets

In the NEO PI, each of the five factors identified by Costa and McCrae are identified with six lower-level traits. Known as facets, the lower-level traits subsumed by agreeableness were first introduced with the 1992 publication of the revised version of the NEO PI. Based on the modern NEO PI-R, the six facets of agreeableness are: Trust, straightforwardness, Altruism, Compliance, Modesty, and Tender-Mindedness.[6]

Trust

Trust is a defining feature of psychosocial development, personality theory, and folk psychological conceptions of personality.[21] Individuals who score high on this facet generally believe others' intentions to be benevolent. Those scoring low on this facet tend to be cynical and view others as suspicious, dishonest, or dangerous.

Straightforwardness

Straightforwardness is the quality of directness and honesty in communicating with others. Despite a long history in moral philosophy, straightforwardness is not as vital to personality theory as the other facets of agreeableness.[21] Those scoring high on straightforwardness tend to interact with others in a direct and honest manner. Low scorers are less direct, tend to be high in self-monitoring, and are generally deceitful or manipulative. Although the two concepts are not identical, those who score low on this facet tend to be high in Machiavellianism.[22] Straightforwardness is similar to a dimension in the Interpersonal circumplex called "Ingenuous versus calculating."[21] According to Michael C. Ashton and Kibeom Lee, straightforwardness is similar to the honesty aspect of honesty-humility in the HEXACO Model.[23]

Altruism

Similar to altruism in animals and ethical altruism, this facet is defined by measures of selflessness, self-sacrifice, generosity, and consideration, courtesy, and concern for others.[21] Altruism is similar to Alfred Adler's concept of social interest, which is a tendency to direct one's actions toward the betterment of society.[24] Individuals who score low on Altruism tend to be discourteous, selfish, or greedy, a pattern of behaviors known as "self-interest" in Adlerian psychology.

Compliance

As a facet of agreeableness, compliance is defined as an individual's typical response to conflict. Those who score high on compliance tend to be meek and mild, and to prefer cooperation or deference as a means of resolving conflict. Low scorers tend to be aggressive, antagonistic, quarrelsome, and vindictive.[21]

Modesty

While trust, straightforwardness, altruism, and compliance all refer to interpersonal or social behaviors, modesty refers to an individual's self-concept. Those who score high on modesty tend to be humble and other-focused, while low scorers tend to be arrogant and self-aggrandizing.[21] Low modesty is otherwise known as conceitedness or Narcissism and, in extreme cases, can manifest as Narcissistic personality disorder.[25] Otherwise known as "humility" in the Revised NEO Personality Inventory, modesty resembles the humility aspect of Honesty-Humility in the HEXACO Model.[23]

Tender-mindedness

Tender-mindedness is defined as the extent to which an individual's judgments and attitudes are determined by emotion. Coined by William James, this term was also prominent in early versions of the 16PF.[21] Tender-mindedness is primarily defined by sympathy[26] and corresponds to the International Personality Item Pool's "sympathy" scale.[27] In contrast, "tough minded" is a trait associated with Psychoticism on the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire.[28]

Equivalents in psychobiological models

Models based on psychobiological theories of personality have each incorporated a factor similar to agreeableness. In Cloninger's Temperament and Character Inventory the character trait known as cooperativeness is highly similar to and positively correlated with agreeableness.[29] In Zuckerman's alternative five model of personality the trait known as Aggression-hostility is inversely related to agreeableness.[30]

HEXACO Model

To address the absence of measures of Dark triad traits (i.e., narcissism, machiavellianism, and psychopathy), Michael Ashton and Kibeom Lee proposed the addition of a sixth factor to the Five Factor Model.[31] Validated with psycholexical studies similar to those used in the development of the Five Factor Model,[32] the HEXACO Model adds Honesty-Humility to five factors resembling those in the NEO PI.[33] Although Honesty-Humility does not directly correspond to any Big Five trait, it is strongly correlated with the Straightforwardness and Modesty facets of Big Five Agreeableness. As both of these facets are only weakly correlated with Big Five Agreeableness, Ashton and Lee suggest dividing NEO PI Agreeableness into two factors similar to those in the HEXACO Model: Honesty-Humility (i.e., Straightforwardness and Modesty) and a redefined Agreeableness (Trust, Altruism, Compliance, and Tender-Mindedness).[23] Reflecting this conception of Honesty-Humility and HEXACO Agreeableness as unique though similar concepts, Ashton and Lee propose that they represent different aspects of reciprocal altruism: fairness (Honesty-Humility) and tolerance (Agreeableness).[34]

Despite suggesting this reconceptualization of Agreeableness for the NEO PI, Ashton and Lee do not believe HEXACO Agreeableness is accurately captured by Trust, Altruism, Compliance, and Tender-Mindedness. In addition to accounting for these four facets of Big Five Agreeableness, the HEXACO Model's construction of Agreeableness includes content categorized under Neuroticism in the NEO PI (i.e., temperamentalness and irritability).[35] To reflect the negative emotional content at the low end of HEXACO Agreeableness, this factor is also referred to as "Agreeableness (versus Anger)."[34] The inclusion of anger in the definition of HEXACO Agreeableness further helps to differentiate this factor from Honesty-Humility. In response to offensive or transgressive actions, individuals who score low on Honesty-Humility tend not to respond immediately. Instead, they defer their response by planning their revenge and waiting for the perfect opportunity to enact it. Although those who score low on HEXACO Agreeableness also employ this premeditated strategy, they also tend to respond immediately with anger.[36]

HEXACO Agreeableness facets

To help capture the numerous distinctions between the Big Five and HEXACO models, Ashton and Lee propose four new facet labels in their conceptualization of Agreeableness: Forgiveness, Gentleness, Flexibility, and Patience.[35] In addition to these four Agreeableness-specific facets, Lee and Ashton have proposed an additional "interstitial" facet located in a space shared by Agreeableness, Honesty-Humility, and Emotionality: Altruism versus Antagonism.[37]

  • Forgiveness: A measure of an individual's response to deception or other transgressions. Individuals who score high on this facet tend to regain their trust and re-establish friendly relations by forgiving the offender, while those who score low tend to hold a grudge. Also known as "Forgivingness."[38]
  • Gentleness: A measure of how an individual typically evaluates others. Individuals who score high on this facet tend to avoid being overly judgmental, while those who score low are highly critical and judgmental.
  • Flexibility: A measure of behaviors related to compromise and cooperation. Individuals who score high on this facet prefer cooperation and compromise as means of resolving disagreement, while those who score low tend to be stubborn, argumentative, and unwilling to accommodate others.
  • Patience: A measure of one's response to anger and aggravation. Individuals who score high on this facet tend to be able to tolerate very high levels of anger and maintain their composure while expressing anger. Those who score low on Patience have difficulties remaining calm while expressing their anger and tend to have quick tempers, becoming very angry in response to comparatively little provocation.
  • Altruism versus Antagonism: Although shared between three HEXACO factors, Altruism versus Antagonism is moderately correlated with Agreeableness.[37] This interstitial facet assesses the extent to which an individual is sympathetic, soft-hearted, and helpful, with low-scoring individuals tending toward an antagonistic interpersonal style.

Interpersonal relations

Agreeableness is an asset in situations that require getting along with others. Compared to disagreeable persons, agreeable individuals display a tendency to perceive others in a more positive light.

Because agreeable children are more sensitive to the needs and perspectives of others, they are less likely to suffer from social rejection. Specifically, research indicates that children who are less disruptive, less aggressive, and more skilled at entering play groups are more likely to gain acceptance by their peers.[39]

One study found that people high in agreeableness are more emotionally responsive in social situations. This effect was measured on both self-report questionnaires and physiological measures, and offers evidence that extraversion and neuroticism are not the only Big Five personality factors that influence emotion. The effect was especially pronounced among women.[40]

Research also shows that people high in agreeableness are more likely to control negative emotions like anger in conflict situations. Those who are high in agreeableness are more likely to use conflict-avoidant tactics when in conflict with others (whereas people low in agreeableness are more likely to use coercive tactics).[41] They are also more willing to give ground to their adversary and may lose arguments with people who are less agreeable. From their perspective, they have not really lost an argument as much as maintained a congenial relationship with another person.[42]

Prosocial behaviour

A central feature of agreeableness is its positive association with altruism and helping behaviour. Across situations, people who are high in agreeableness are more likely to report an interest and involvement with helping others. Experiments have shown that most people are likely to help their own kin, and help when empathy has been aroused. Agreeable people are likely to help even when these conditions are not present.[43] In other words, agreeable people appear to be "traited for helping"[44] and do not need any other motivations.

While agreeable individuals are habitually likely to help others, disagreeable people may be more likely to cause harm. Researchers have found that low levels of agreeableness are associated with hostile thoughts and aggression in adolescents, as well as poor social adjustment.[45] People low in agreeableness are also more likely to be prejudiced against stigmatized groups such as the overweight.[46]

When mental illness is present, low agreeableness may be associated with narcissistic and anti-social tendencies.[47] In theory, individuals who are extremely high in agreeableness are at risk for problems of dependency. Empirical studies show that many more problems are associated with low agreeableness.

However, high agreeableness does not always lead to prosocial behaviour, in a Milgram experiment conscientious and agreeable people, when forced by ill-intent authority, are more willing to administer high-intensity electric shocks to a victim, because conscientious and agreeable people are less capable of resistance.[48]

From childhood to adulthood

Agreeableness is of fundamental importance to psychological well-being, predicting mental health, positive affect, and good relations with others. In both childhood and adolescence agreeableness has been tied to externalizing issues. Along with this it has also been implicated in outcomes to conflict management skills, school adjustment, peer-social status and self-esteem. Some work has been done looking into whether agreeableness levels through childhood have effects on adjustment and agreeableness into adulthood. Among young adults, individuals that have been diagnosed with either externalizing as well as internalizing disorders present lower levels of agreeableness and communion, and higher levels of negative emotionality, than those young adults without such disorders. Agreeableness also is reported to mediate links between anger and depression in young adults. Among college students, agreeableness is often associated with self-reports of emotional experience and control along with psycho-physiological responses to affectively charged stimuli. Across adulthood, low agreeableness has been found to be a health risk. High agreeableness, especially trust and honesty, has been linked to longevity.[49]

A study done by Caspi, Elder, and Bem (1987) found that explosive and ill-tempered children were found to have higher rates of divorce as adults when compared with their even-tempered peers. Further, ill-tempered men had lower educational attainment, occupational status, and work stability, and ill-tempered women married men with similar low achievement profiles.[50] A second and more recent study by Shiner (2000) found that composite variables describing middle-childhood agreeableness and friendly compliance predicted adolescent academic performance, behavioral conduct, and social competence 10 years later.[51]

Geography

United States

 
Agreeableness by state. Lighter regions have lower average agreeableness.

In the United States, people in the West, Midwest, and South tend to have higher average scores on agreeableness than people living in other regions.[52] According to researchers, the top ten most agreeable states are North Dakota, Minnesota, Mississippi, Utah, Wisconsin, Tennessee, North Carolina, Georgia, Oklahoma, and Nebraska.[53] These findings are consistent with well-known expressions in these states, such as "southern hospitality" and "Minnesota nice." Because these states are generally less urbanized than the east and west coasts, people may be more likely to live in small communities and know their neighbors. Consequently, they may be more willing to care about and help their neighbours.

In a study done by Albright et al. (1997) groups of college students from China and the United States rated strangers from both countries on the "Big Five" personality traits, external traits, and how well they were dressed. They found that both Chinese and U.S. students rated faces as showing similar levels of agreeableness and extroversion. The people who were thought to be the most agreeable wore smiles, a facial expression that is recognized around the world.[54] The findings of the research seem to suggest that the trait of agreeableness is attributed to people in a universal way.[55]

See also

References

  1. ^ a b c Thompson, E.R. (October 2008). "Development and Validation of an International English Big-Five Mini-Markers". Personality and Individual Differences. 45 (6): 542–548. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2008.06.013.
  2. ^ Graziano, W.G., & Eisenberg, N. (1997). Agreeableness; A dimension of personality. In R. Hogan, S. Briggs, & J. Johnson, (1997). Handbook of Personality Psychology. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
  3. ^ Bamford, Joshua Michael Silberstein; Davidson, Jane Whitfield (28 March 2017). "Trait Empathy associated with Agreeableness and rhythmic entrainment in a spontaneous movement to music task: Preliminary exploratory investigations". Musicae Scientiae. 23 (1): 5–24. doi:10.1177/1029864917701536. S2CID 151504600.
  4. ^ Song, Yang (10 February 2017). "Associations between empathy and big five personality traits among Chinese undergraduate medical students". PLOS ONE. 12 (2): e0171665. Bibcode:2017PLoSO..1271665S. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0171665. PMC 5302826. PMID 28187194.
  5. ^ Kaufman, Scott Barry; Yaden, David Bryce; Hyde, Elizabeth; Tsukayama, Eli (12 March 2019). "The Light vs. Dark Triad of Personality: Contrasting Two Very Different Profiles of Human Nature". Frontiers in Psychology. 10: 467. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00467. PMC 6423069. PMID 30914993.
  6. ^ a b Matsumoto, D.; Juang, L. (2012). Culture and Psychology: 5th Edition. Belmont, California: Wadsworth-Cengage Learning. p. 271. ISBN 978-1-111-34493-1.
  7. ^ Allport, G. W.; Odbert, H. S. (1936). "Trait names: A psycholexical study". Psychological Monographs. 47 (1): i–171. doi:10.1037/h0093360.
  8. ^ Cattell, R. B. (October 1943). "The description of personality: Basic traits resolved into clusters". Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology. 38 (4): 476–506. doi:10.1037/h0054116.
  9. ^ Cattell, R. B. The Description and Measurement of Personality. New York: World Book.
  10. ^ . IPAT. Archived from the original on February 29, 2012. Retrieved February 16, 2012.
  11. ^ a b c d Pervin PhD, Lawrence A.; John PhD, Oliver P. (1999). Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Press. pp. 102–138. ISBN 978-1-57230-695-0.
  12. ^ a b Goldberg, L.R.; Johnson, JA; Eber, HW; et al. (2006). "The international personality item pool and the future of public-domain personality measures". Journal of Research in Personality. 40 (1): 84–96. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2005.08.007.
  13. ^ Goldberg, L.R. (1992). "The development of markers for the Big-Five factor structure". Psychological Assessment. 4 (1): 26–42. doi:10.1037/1040-3590.4.1.26.
  14. ^ Saucier, G (1994). "Mini-Markers – a brief version of Goldberg's unipolar big-five markers". Journal of Personality Assessment. 63 (3): 506–516. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa6303_8. PMID 7844738.
  15. ^ Chamorro-Premuzic, T. (2007). Personality and Individual Differences. Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing. ISBN 978-1-4051-3008-0.
  16. ^ Goldberg, L. R. (1981). "Language and individual differences: The search for universals in personality lexicons.". In Wheeler, L. (ed.). Review of Personality and Social Psychology: Volume 2. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. pp. 141–165. ISBN 978-0-8039-1667-8.
  17. ^ Goldberg, L. R. (1981). "Developing a taxonomy of trait-descriptive terms.". In Fiske, D. W. (ed.). Problems with Language Imprecision: New Directions for Methodology of Social and Behavioral Science. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. pp. 43–65.
  18. ^ Costa, P. T.; McCrae, R. R. (1976). "Age differences in personality structure: A cluster analytic approach". Journal of Gerontology. 31 (5): 564–570. doi:10.1093/geronj/31.5.564. PMID 950450.
  19. ^ Costa, P. T. Jr.; McCrae, R. R. (1985). The NEO Personality Inventory Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.
  20. ^ Costa, P. T.; McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO PI-R) and the NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) Professional Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
  21. ^ a b c d e f g Costa, P. T.; McCrae, R. R. (1991). "Facet scales for agreeableness and conscientiousness: A revision of the NEO personality inventory". Personality and Individual Differences. 12 (9): 888. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(91)90177-D.
  22. ^ Jakobwitz S.; Egan V. (2006). "The dark triad and normal personality traits". Personality and Individual Differences. 40 (2): 331–339. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2005.07.006.
  23. ^ a b c Ashton, M. C.; Lee, K. (October 2005). (PDF). Journal of Personality. 73 (5): 1321–53. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2005.00351.x. PMID 16138875. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2012-03-17.
  24. ^ Adler, A. (1964). Superiority and Social Interest: A Collection of Later Writings. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
  25. ^ Adams, H. D.; Sutker, P. B. (2004). Comprehensive Handbook of Psychopathology: Third Edition. New York: Springer. ISBN 978-0-306-46490-4.
  26. ^ Costa, P. T.; McCrae, R. R. "NEO-PI-R • NEO Personality Inventory – Revised". Hogrefe Testsystem 4. Retrieved March 19, 2012.
  27. ^ "A Comparison between the 30 Facet Scales in Costa and McCrae's NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and the Corresponding Preliminary IPIP Scales Measuring Similar Constructs". Oregon Research Institute. Retrieved March 19, 2012.
  28. ^ Eysenck, H. J.; Eysenck, S. B. G. (1975). Manual of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire. San Diego: Educational and Industrial Testing Service.
  29. ^ De Fruyt, F.; Van De Wiele, L.; Van Heeringen, C. (2000). "Cloninger's Psychobiological Model of Temperament and Character and the Five-Factor Model of Personality". Personality and Individual Differences. 29 (3): 441–452. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(99)00204-4.
  30. ^ Aluja, Anton; García, Óscar; García, Luis F. (2002). "A comparative study of Zuckerman's three structural models for personality through the NEO-PI-R, ZKPQ-III-R, EPQ-RS and Goldberg's 50-bipolar adjectives". Personality and Individual Differences. 33 (5): 713–725. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(01)00186-6.
  31. ^ Ashton, M. C.; Lee, K.; Son, C (2000). "Honesty as the sixth factor of personality: Correlations with Machiavellianism, primary psychopathy, and social adroitness". European Journal of Personality. 14 (4): 359–368. doi:10.1002/1099-0984(200007/08)14:4<359::AID-PER382>3.0.CO;2-Y. S2CID 144035257.
  32. ^ Ashton, M. C.; Lee, K.; Perugini, M.; et al. (February 2004). "A six-factor structure of personality-descriptive adjectives: Solutions from psycholexical studies in seven languages". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 86 (2): 356–66. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.86.2.356. PMID 14769090.
  33. ^ Lee, K.; Ashton, M. C. (2004). "The HEXACO Personality Inventory: A new measure of the major dimensions of personality". Multivariate Behavioral Research. 39.
  34. ^ a b Ashton, M. C.; Lee, K. (May 2007). "Empirical, theoretical, and practical advantages of the HEXACO Model of Personality Structure". Personality and Social Psychology Review. 11 (2): 150–66. doi:10.1177/1088868306294907. PMID 18453460. S2CID 13183244.
  35. ^ a b Lee, K.; Ashton, M. C. (2004). (PDF). Multivariate Behavioral Research. 39 (2): 329–358. doi:10.1207/s15327906mbr3902_8. PMID 26804579. S2CID 27763606. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2012-03-17.
  36. ^ Lee, K.; Ashton, M. C. (April 2012). "Getting mad and getting even: Agreeableness and Honesty-Humility as predictors of revenge intentions". Personality and Individual Differences. 52 (5): 596–600. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.12.004. S2CID 14026546.
  37. ^ a b Lee, K.; Ashton, M. C. (June 2006). "Further assessment of the HEXACO Personality Inventory: Two new facet scales and an observer report form". Psychological Assessment. 18 (2): 182–91. doi:10.1037/1040-3590.18.2.182. PMID 16768594.
  38. ^ Lee, K.; Ashton, M. C. "The HEXACO Personality Inventory-Revised: Scale Descriptions". Retrieved March 20, 2012.
  39. ^ Bierman, K. L. (2003). Peer rejection: Developmental processes and intervention strategies. New York: The Guilford Press.
  40. ^ Tobin R.M.; Graziano W.G.; Vanman E.; Tassinary L. (2000). "Personality, emotional experience, and efforts to control emotions". Journal of Personality & Social Psychology. 79 (4): 656–669. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.79.4.656. PMID 11045745.
  41. ^ Jensen-Campbell L. A.; Graziano W. G. (2001). "Agreeableness as a moderator of interpersonal conflict". Journal of Personality. 69 (2): 323–361. doi:10.1111/1467-6494.00148. PMID 11339802.
  42. ^ Graziano W.G.; Jensen-Campbell L.A.; Hair E. C. (1996). "Perceiving interpersonal conflict and reacting to it: The case for agreeableness". Journal of Personality & Social Psychology. 70 (4): 820–835. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.70.4.820. PMID 8636901.
  43. ^ Graziano, W. G., Habashi, M. M., Sheese, B.E., & Tobin, R. M. (2007). Agreeableness, empathy, and helping: A person X situation perspective. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology.
  44. ^ Penner L. A.; Fritzsche B. A.; Craiger J. P.; Freifeld T. S. (1995). "Measuring the prosocial personality". Advances in Personality Assessment. 10: 147–163.
  45. ^ Gleason K.A.; Jensen-Campbell L.A.; Richardson D. (2004). "Agreeableness and aggression in adolescence". Aggressive Behavior. 30: 43–61. doi:10.1002/ab.20002.
  46. ^ Graziano W.G.; Bruce J. W.; Sheese B.E.; Tobin R.M. (2007). "Attraction, personality and prejudice: Liking none of the people most of the time". Journal of Personality & Social Psychology. 93 (4): 565–582. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.93.4.565. PMID 17892332.
  47. ^ Costa, P. T. & McCrae, R. R. (1992). NEO personality Inventory professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
  48. ^ Bègue, Laurent; Beauvois, Jean-Léon; Courbert, Didier; Oberblé, Dominique; Lepage, Johan; Duke, Aaron (2014). "Personality Predicts Obedience in a Milgram Paradigm" (PDF). Journal of Personality. 83 (3): 299–306. doi:10.1111/jopy.12104. PMID 24798990. S2CID 13868371.
  49. ^ Laursen B.; Pulkkinen L.; Adams R. (2002). "The antecedents and correlates of agreeableness in adulthood". Journal of Developmental Psychology. 38 (4): 591–603. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.38.4.591. PMC 2730208. PMID 12090488.
  50. ^ Caspi A.; Elder G. H.; Bem D. J. (1987). "Moving against the world: Life course patterns of explosive children". Journal of Developmental Psychology. 23 (2): 308–313. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.23.2.308.
  51. ^ Shiner R. L. (2000). "Linking childhood personality with adaptation: evidence for continuity and change across time into late adolescence". Journal of Personality & Social Psychology. 78 (2): 310–325. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.78.2.310. PMID 10707337.
  52. ^ . myPersonality Research. December 9, 2008. Archived from the original on March 18, 2012. Retrieved April 7, 2012.
  53. ^ Stephanie Simon (2008-09-23). "The United States of Mind. Researchers Identify Regional Personality Traits Across America". WSJ.com. Original research article: Peter J. Rentfrow; Samuel D. Gosling; Jeff Potter (2008). "A Theory of the Emergence, Persistence, and Expression of Geographic Variation in Psychological Characteristics". Perspectives on Psychological Science. 3 (5): 339–369. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00084.x. PMID 26158954. S2CID 17059908.
  54. ^ C. B. Wortman; E. F. Loftus; C. A. Weaver (1999). . The McGraw-Hill Companies. Archived from the original on 2016-03-18. Retrieved 2012-03-29.
  55. ^ Albright, Linda; Malloy, Thomas E.; Dong, Qi; Kenny, David A.; Fang, Xiaoyi; Winquist, Lynn; Yu, Da (1997). "Cross-cultural consensus in personality judgments". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 72 (3): 558–569. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.72.3.558. ISSN 1939-1315. PMID 9120784.

agreeableness, personality, trait, manifesting, itself, individual, behavioral, characteristics, that, perceived, kind, sympathetic, cooperative, warm, considerate, contemporary, personality, psychology, agreeableness, five, major, dimensions, personality, str. Agreeableness is a personality trait manifesting itself in individual behavioral characteristics that are perceived as kind sympathetic cooperative warm and considerate 1 In contemporary personality psychology agreeableness is one of the five major dimensions of personality structure reflecting individual differences in cooperation and social harmony 2 People who score high on this dimension are empathetic and altruistic while a low agreeableness score relates to selfish behavior often manifesting as stinginess and a lack of empathy 3 4 Those who score very low on agreeableness show signs of dark triad behavior such as manipulation and competing with others rather than cooperating 5 Agreeableness is considered to be a superordinate trait meaning that it is a grouping of personality sub traits that cluster together statistically The lower level traits or facets grouped under agreeableness are trust straightforwardness altruism compliance modesty and tender mindedness 6 Contents 1 History 1 1 Cattell s 16 Personality Factors 1 2 The Big Five 1 3 NEO PI 2 NEO PI facets 2 1 Trust 2 2 Straightforwardness 2 3 Altruism 2 4 Compliance 2 5 Modesty 2 6 Tender mindedness 3 Equivalents in psychobiological models 4 HEXACO Model 4 1 HEXACO Agreeableness facets 5 Interpersonal relations 6 Prosocial behaviour 7 From childhood to adulthood 8 Geography 8 1 United States 9 See also 10 ReferencesHistory EditCattell s 16 Personality Factors Edit Agreeable Burden Fardeau agreable William Adolphe Bouguereau 1895 Like all Big Five personality traits the roots of the modern concept of agreeableness can be traced to a 1936 study by Gordon Allport and Henry S Odbert 7 Seven years later Raymond Cattell published a cluster analysis of the thousands of personality related words identified by Allport and Odbert 8 The clusters identified in this study served as a foundation for Cattell s further attempts to identify fundamental universal human personality factors 9 He eventually settled on 16 personality factors through the use of factor analysis Further factor analyses revealed five higher order or global factors to encompass these 16 10 Although labelled independence by Cattell a global factor defined by high scores on the E H L and Q1 factors of the 16PF Questionnaire was an early precursor to the modern concept of agreeableness 11 The Big Five Edit Extent of agreeableness in the five factor model of personality is most commonly assessed through self report measures although peer reports and third party observation can also be used Self report measures are either lexical 1 or based on statements 12 Which measure of either type is used is determined by an assessment of psychometric properties and the time and space constraints of the research being undertaken Lexical measures use individual adjectives that reflect agreeableness or disagreeableness traits such as sympathetic cooperative warm considerate harsh unkind rude Words representing disagreeableness are reverse coded Goldberg 1992 13 developed a 20 word measure as part of his 100 word Big Five markers and Saucier 1994 14 developed a briefer 8 word measure as part of his 40 word mini markers Thompson 2008 1 systematically revised and improved these markers to develop a 40 word measure with better psychometric properties in both American and non American populations the International English Mini Markers This brief measure has good internal consistency reliabilities and other validity for assessing agreeableness and other five factor personality dimensions both within and especially without American populations Internal consistency reliability of the Agreeableness measure for native English speakers is reported as 86 that for non native English speakers is 80 Statement measures tend to comprise more words and hence consume more research instrument space than lexical measures Respondents are asked the extent to which they for example Am on good terms with nearly everyone Am not interested in other people s problems or Sympathize with others feelings 12 Cattell s factor analytic approach used to identify the universal personality structures inspired countless studies in the decades following the introduction of the 16PF Using Cattell s original clusters the 16 Personality Factors and original data multiple researchers independently developed a five factor model of personality over this period From the early 1960s on these explorations typically included a factor called agreeableness or sociability 11 15 Despite repeated replications of five stable personality factors following Cattell s pioneering work this framework only began to dominate personality research in the early 1980s with the work of Lewis Goldberg Using lexical studies similar to those of Allport and Odbert Goldberg chose the term Big Five to reflect the sheer number of personality related terms encompassed by each of these five distinct factors 11 One of these agreeableness was defined by a number of personality related words similar to those present in earlier and more recent manifestations of the construct examples include friendly good natured cooperative trustful nurturing sociable and considerate 16 17 NEO PI Edit Beginning in the 1970s Paul Costa and Robert McCrae began researching the development of personality assessments based on factor models Beginning with cluster analyses of Cattell s 16 Personality Factors Costa and McCrae initially settled on a three factor model of personality These three factors were neuroticism vs emotional stability extraversion vs introversion and openness vs closedness to experience resulting in the acronym NEO 18 Due to similarities between their three factor NEO Personality Inventory and Goldberg s Big Five Costa and McCrae began to develop scales to assess agreeableness and conscientiousness in the early 1980s 11 This work culminated in the 1985 publication of the first NEO PI Manual to be based on the full Five Factor Model 19 Although this marked the introduction of agreeableness to the NEO PI Costa and McCrae worked for an additional seven years to identify and elaborate on the facets comprising this factor in the Revised NEO Personality Inventory 20 NEO PI facets EditIn the NEO PI each of the five factors identified by Costa and McCrae are identified with six lower level traits Known as facets the lower level traits subsumed by agreeableness were first introduced with the 1992 publication of the revised version of the NEO PI Based on the modern NEO PI R the six facets of agreeableness are Trust straightforwardness Altruism Compliance Modesty and Tender Mindedness 6 Trust Edit Trust is a defining feature of psychosocial development personality theory and folk psychological conceptions of personality 21 Individuals who score high on this facet generally believe others intentions to be benevolent Those scoring low on this facet tend to be cynical and view others as suspicious dishonest or dangerous Straightforwardness Edit Straightforwardness is the quality of directness and honesty in communicating with others Despite a long history in moral philosophy straightforwardness is not as vital to personality theory as the other facets of agreeableness 21 Those scoring high on straightforwardness tend to interact with others in a direct and honest manner Low scorers are less direct tend to be high in self monitoring and are generally deceitful or manipulative Although the two concepts are not identical those who score low on this facet tend to be high in Machiavellianism 22 Straightforwardness is similar to a dimension in the Interpersonal circumplex called Ingenuous versus calculating 21 According to Michael C Ashton and Kibeom Lee straightforwardness is similar to the honesty aspect of honesty humility in the HEXACO Model 23 Altruism Edit Similar to altruism in animals and ethical altruism this facet is defined by measures of selflessness self sacrifice generosity and consideration courtesy and concern for others 21 Altruism is similar to Alfred Adler s concept of social interest which is a tendency to direct one s actions toward the betterment of society 24 Individuals who score low on Altruism tend to be discourteous selfish or greedy a pattern of behaviors known as self interest in Adlerian psychology Compliance Edit As a facet of agreeableness compliance is defined as an individual s typical response to conflict Those who score high on compliance tend to be meek and mild and to prefer cooperation or deference as a means of resolving conflict Low scorers tend to be aggressive antagonistic quarrelsome and vindictive 21 Modesty Edit While trust straightforwardness altruism and compliance all refer to interpersonal or social behaviors modesty refers to an individual s self concept Those who score high on modesty tend to be humble and other focused while low scorers tend to be arrogant and self aggrandizing 21 Low modesty is otherwise known as conceitedness or Narcissism and in extreme cases can manifest as Narcissistic personality disorder 25 Otherwise known as humility in the Revised NEO Personality Inventory modesty resembles the humility aspect of Honesty Humility in the HEXACO Model 23 Tender mindedness Edit Tender mindedness is defined as the extent to which an individual s judgments and attitudes are determined by emotion Coined by William James this term was also prominent in early versions of the 16PF 21 Tender mindedness is primarily defined by sympathy 26 and corresponds to the International Personality Item Pool s sympathy scale 27 In contrast tough minded is a trait associated with Psychoticism on the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire 28 Equivalents in psychobiological models EditModels based on psychobiological theories of personality have each incorporated a factor similar to agreeableness In Cloninger s Temperament and Character Inventory the character trait known as cooperativeness is highly similar to and positively correlated with agreeableness 29 In Zuckerman s alternative five model of personality the trait known as Aggression hostility is inversely related to agreeableness 30 HEXACO Model EditTo address the absence of measures of Dark triad traits i e narcissism machiavellianism and psychopathy Michael Ashton and Kibeom Lee proposed the addition of a sixth factor to the Five Factor Model 31 Validated with psycholexical studies similar to those used in the development of the Five Factor Model 32 the HEXACO Model adds Honesty Humility to five factors resembling those in the NEO PI 33 Although Honesty Humility does not directly correspond to any Big Five trait it is strongly correlated with the Straightforwardness and Modesty facets of Big Five Agreeableness As both of these facets are only weakly correlated with Big Five Agreeableness Ashton and Lee suggest dividing NEO PI Agreeableness into two factors similar to those in the HEXACO Model Honesty Humility i e Straightforwardness and Modesty and a redefined Agreeableness Trust Altruism Compliance and Tender Mindedness 23 Reflecting this conception of Honesty Humility and HEXACO Agreeableness as unique though similar concepts Ashton and Lee propose that they represent different aspects of reciprocal altruism fairness Honesty Humility and tolerance Agreeableness 34 Despite suggesting this reconceptualization of Agreeableness for the NEO PI Ashton and Lee do not believe HEXACO Agreeableness is accurately captured by Trust Altruism Compliance and Tender Mindedness In addition to accounting for these four facets of Big Five Agreeableness the HEXACO Model s construction of Agreeableness includes content categorized under Neuroticism in the NEO PI i e temperamentalness and irritability 35 To reflect the negative emotional content at the low end of HEXACO Agreeableness this factor is also referred to as Agreeableness versus Anger 34 The inclusion of anger in the definition of HEXACO Agreeableness further helps to differentiate this factor from Honesty Humility In response to offensive or transgressive actions individuals who score low on Honesty Humility tend not to respond immediately Instead they defer their response by planning their revenge and waiting for the perfect opportunity to enact it Although those who score low on HEXACO Agreeableness also employ this premeditated strategy they also tend to respond immediately with anger 36 HEXACO Agreeableness facets Edit To help capture the numerous distinctions between the Big Five and HEXACO models Ashton and Lee propose four new facet labels in their conceptualization of Agreeableness Forgiveness Gentleness Flexibility and Patience 35 In addition to these four Agreeableness specific facets Lee and Ashton have proposed an additional interstitial facet located in a space shared by Agreeableness Honesty Humility and Emotionality Altruism versus Antagonism 37 Forgiveness A measure of an individual s response to deception or other transgressions Individuals who score high on this facet tend to regain their trust and re establish friendly relations by forgiving the offender while those who score low tend to hold a grudge Also known as Forgivingness 38 Gentleness A measure of how an individual typically evaluates others Individuals who score high on this facet tend to avoid being overly judgmental while those who score low are highly critical and judgmental Flexibility A measure of behaviors related to compromise and cooperation Individuals who score high on this facet prefer cooperation and compromise as means of resolving disagreement while those who score low tend to be stubborn argumentative and unwilling to accommodate others Patience A measure of one s response to anger and aggravation Individuals who score high on this facet tend to be able to tolerate very high levels of anger and maintain their composure while expressing anger Those who score low on Patience have difficulties remaining calm while expressing their anger and tend to have quick tempers becoming very angry in response to comparatively little provocation Altruism versus Antagonism Although shared between three HEXACO factors Altruism versus Antagonism is moderately correlated with Agreeableness 37 This interstitial facet assesses the extent to which an individual is sympathetic soft hearted and helpful with low scoring individuals tending toward an antagonistic interpersonal style Interpersonal relations EditAgreeableness is an asset in situations that require getting along with others Compared to disagreeable persons agreeable individuals display a tendency to perceive others in a more positive light Because agreeable children are more sensitive to the needs and perspectives of others they are less likely to suffer from social rejection Specifically research indicates that children who are less disruptive less aggressive and more skilled at entering play groups are more likely to gain acceptance by their peers 39 One study found that people high in agreeableness are more emotionally responsive in social situations This effect was measured on both self report questionnaires and physiological measures and offers evidence that extraversion and neuroticism are not the only Big Five personality factors that influence emotion The effect was especially pronounced among women 40 Research also shows that people high in agreeableness are more likely to control negative emotions like anger in conflict situations Those who are high in agreeableness are more likely to use conflict avoidant tactics when in conflict with others whereas people low in agreeableness are more likely to use coercive tactics 41 They are also more willing to give ground to their adversary and may lose arguments with people who are less agreeable From their perspective they have not really lost an argument as much as maintained a congenial relationship with another person 42 Prosocial behaviour EditA central feature of agreeableness is its positive association with altruism and helping behaviour Across situations people who are high in agreeableness are more likely to report an interest and involvement with helping others Experiments have shown that most people are likely to help their own kin and help when empathy has been aroused Agreeable people are likely to help even when these conditions are not present 43 In other words agreeable people appear to be traited for helping 44 and do not need any other motivations While agreeable individuals are habitually likely to help others disagreeable people may be more likely to cause harm Researchers have found that low levels of agreeableness are associated with hostile thoughts and aggression in adolescents as well as poor social adjustment 45 People low in agreeableness are also more likely to be prejudiced against stigmatized groups such as the overweight 46 When mental illness is present low agreeableness may be associated with narcissistic and anti social tendencies 47 In theory individuals who are extremely high in agreeableness are at risk for problems of dependency Empirical studies show that many more problems are associated with low agreeableness However high agreeableness does not always lead to prosocial behaviour in a Milgram experiment conscientious and agreeable people when forced by ill intent authority are more willing to administer high intensity electric shocks to a victim because conscientious and agreeable people are less capable of resistance 48 From childhood to adulthood EditAgreeableness is of fundamental importance to psychological well being predicting mental health positive affect and good relations with others In both childhood and adolescence agreeableness has been tied to externalizing issues Along with this it has also been implicated in outcomes to conflict management skills school adjustment peer social status and self esteem Some work has been done looking into whether agreeableness levels through childhood have effects on adjustment and agreeableness into adulthood Among young adults individuals that have been diagnosed with either externalizing as well as internalizing disorders present lower levels of agreeableness and communion and higher levels of negative emotionality than those young adults without such disorders Agreeableness also is reported to mediate links between anger and depression in young adults Among college students agreeableness is often associated with self reports of emotional experience and control along with psycho physiological responses to affectively charged stimuli Across adulthood low agreeableness has been found to be a health risk High agreeableness especially trust and honesty has been linked to longevity 49 A study done by Caspi Elder and Bem 1987 found that explosive and ill tempered children were found to have higher rates of divorce as adults when compared with their even tempered peers Further ill tempered men had lower educational attainment occupational status and work stability and ill tempered women married men with similar low achievement profiles 50 A second and more recent study by Shiner 2000 found that composite variables describing middle childhood agreeableness and friendly compliance predicted adolescent academic performance behavioral conduct and social competence 10 years later 51 Geography EditUnited States Edit Agreeableness by state Lighter regions have lower average agreeableness See also List of U S states ranked per five factor model personality trait In the United States people in the West Midwest and South tend to have higher average scores on agreeableness than people living in other regions 52 According to researchers the top ten most agreeable states are North Dakota Minnesota Mississippi Utah Wisconsin Tennessee North Carolina Georgia Oklahoma and Nebraska 53 These findings are consistent with well known expressions in these states such as southern hospitality and Minnesota nice Because these states are generally less urbanized than the east and west coasts people may be more likely to live in small communities and know their neighbors Consequently they may be more willing to care about and help their neighbours In a study done by Albright et al 1997 groups of college students from China and the United States rated strangers from both countries on the Big Five personality traits external traits and how well they were dressed They found that both Chinese and U S students rated faces as showing similar levels of agreeableness and extroversion The people who were thought to be the most agreeable wore smiles a facial expression that is recognized around the world 54 The findings of the research seem to suggest that the trait of agreeableness is attributed to people in a universal way 55 See also EditTrait theory Lexical hypothesis Facet psychology Phaeton complexReferences Edit a b c Thompson E R October 2008 Development and Validation of an International English Big Five Mini Markers Personality and Individual Differences 45 6 542 548 doi 10 1016 j paid 2008 06 013 Graziano W G amp Eisenberg N 1997 Agreeableness A dimension of personality In R Hogan S Briggs amp J Johnson 1997 Handbook of Personality Psychology San Diego CA Academic Press Bamford Joshua Michael Silberstein Davidson Jane Whitfield 28 March 2017 Trait Empathy associated with Agreeableness and rhythmic entrainment in a spontaneous movement to music task Preliminary exploratory investigations Musicae Scientiae 23 1 5 24 doi 10 1177 1029864917701536 S2CID 151504600 Song Yang 10 February 2017 Associations between empathy and big five personality traits among Chinese undergraduate medical students PLOS ONE 12 2 e0171665 Bibcode 2017PLoSO 1271665S doi 10 1371 journal pone 0171665 PMC 5302826 PMID 28187194 Kaufman Scott Barry Yaden David Bryce Hyde Elizabeth Tsukayama Eli 12 March 2019 The Light vs Dark Triad of Personality Contrasting Two Very Different Profiles of Human Nature Frontiers in Psychology 10 467 doi 10 3389 fpsyg 2019 00467 PMC 6423069 PMID 30914993 a b Matsumoto D Juang L 2012 Culture and Psychology 5th Edition Belmont California Wadsworth Cengage Learning p 271 ISBN 978 1 111 34493 1 Allport G W Odbert H S 1936 Trait names A psycholexical study Psychological Monographs 47 1 i 171 doi 10 1037 h0093360 Cattell R B October 1943 The description of personality Basic traits resolved into clusters Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 38 4 476 506 doi 10 1037 h0054116 Cattell R B The Description and Measurement of Personality New York World Book The 16PF Questionnaire IPAT Archived from the original on February 29 2012 Retrieved February 16 2012 a b c d Pervin PhD Lawrence A John PhD Oliver P 1999 Handbook of Personality Theory and Research 2nd ed New York Guilford Press pp 102 138 ISBN 978 1 57230 695 0 a b Goldberg L R Johnson JA Eber HW et al 2006 The international personality item pool and the future of public domain personality measures Journal of Research in Personality 40 1 84 96 doi 10 1016 j jrp 2005 08 007 Goldberg L R 1992 The development of markers for the Big Five factor structure Psychological Assessment 4 1 26 42 doi 10 1037 1040 3590 4 1 26 Saucier G 1994 Mini Markers a brief version of Goldberg s unipolar big five markers Journal of Personality Assessment 63 3 506 516 doi 10 1207 s15327752jpa6303 8 PMID 7844738 Chamorro Premuzic T 2007 Personality and Individual Differences Malden MA Blackwell Publishing ISBN 978 1 4051 3008 0 Goldberg L R 1981 Language and individual differences The search for universals in personality lexicons In Wheeler L ed Review of Personality and Social Psychology Volume 2 Beverly Hills CA Sage pp 141 165 ISBN 978 0 8039 1667 8 Goldberg L R 1981 Developing a taxonomy of trait descriptive terms In Fiske D W ed Problems with Language Imprecision New Directions for Methodology of Social and Behavioral Science San Francisco CA Jossey Bass pp 43 65 Costa P T McCrae R R 1976 Age differences in personality structure A cluster analytic approach Journal of Gerontology 31 5 564 570 doi 10 1093 geronj 31 5 564 PMID 950450 Costa P T Jr McCrae R R 1985 The NEO Personality Inventory Manual Odessa FL Psychological Assessment Resources Inc Costa P T McCrae R R 1992 Revised NEO Personality Inventory NEO PI R and the NEO Five Factor Inventory NEO FFI Professional Manual Odessa FL Psychological Assessment Resources a b c d e f g Costa P T McCrae R R 1991 Facet scales for agreeableness and conscientiousness A revision of the NEO personality inventory Personality and Individual Differences 12 9 888 doi 10 1016 0191 8869 91 90177 D Jakobwitz S Egan V 2006 The dark triad and normal personality traits Personality and Individual Differences 40 2 331 339 doi 10 1016 j paid 2005 07 006 a b c Ashton M C Lee K October 2005 Honesty Humility the Big Five and the Five Factor Model PDF Journal of Personality 73 5 1321 53 doi 10 1111 j 1467 6494 2005 00351 x PMID 16138875 Archived from the original PDF on 2012 03 17 Adler A 1964 Superiority and Social Interest A Collection of Later Writings London Routledge amp Kegan Paul Adams H D Sutker P B 2004 Comprehensive Handbook of Psychopathology Third Edition New York Springer ISBN 978 0 306 46490 4 Costa P T McCrae R R NEO PI R NEO Personality Inventory Revised Hogrefe Testsystem 4 Retrieved March 19 2012 A Comparison between the 30 Facet Scales in Costa and McCrae s NEO Personality Inventory NEO PI R and the Corresponding Preliminary IPIP Scales Measuring Similar Constructs Oregon Research Institute Retrieved March 19 2012 Eysenck H J Eysenck S B G 1975 Manual of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire San Diego Educational and Industrial Testing Service De Fruyt F Van De Wiele L Van Heeringen C 2000 Cloninger s Psychobiological Model of Temperament and Character and the Five Factor Model of Personality Personality and Individual Differences 29 3 441 452 doi 10 1016 S0191 8869 99 00204 4 Aluja Anton Garcia oscar Garcia Luis F 2002 A comparative study of Zuckerman s three structural models for personality through the NEO PI R ZKPQ III R EPQ RS and Goldberg s 50 bipolar adjectives Personality and Individual Differences 33 5 713 725 doi 10 1016 S0191 8869 01 00186 6 Ashton M C Lee K Son C 2000 Honesty as the sixth factor of personality Correlations with Machiavellianism primary psychopathy and social adroitness European Journal of Personality 14 4 359 368 doi 10 1002 1099 0984 200007 08 14 4 lt 359 AID PER382 gt 3 0 CO 2 Y S2CID 144035257 Ashton M C Lee K Perugini M et al February 2004 A six factor structure of personality descriptive adjectives Solutions from psycholexical studies in seven languages Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 86 2 356 66 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 86 2 356 PMID 14769090 Lee K Ashton M C 2004 The HEXACO Personality Inventory A new measure of the major dimensions of personality Multivariate Behavioral Research 39 a b Ashton M C Lee K May 2007 Empirical theoretical and practical advantages of the HEXACO Model of Personality Structure Personality and Social Psychology Review 11 2 150 66 doi 10 1177 1088868306294907 PMID 18453460 S2CID 13183244 a b Lee K Ashton M C 2004 Psychometric properties of the HEXACO Personality Inventory PDF Multivariate Behavioral Research 39 2 329 358 doi 10 1207 s15327906mbr3902 8 PMID 26804579 S2CID 27763606 Archived from the original PDF on 2012 03 17 Lee K Ashton M C April 2012 Getting mad and getting even Agreeableness and Honesty Humility as predictors of revenge intentions Personality and Individual Differences 52 5 596 600 doi 10 1016 j paid 2011 12 004 S2CID 14026546 a b Lee K Ashton M C June 2006 Further assessment of the HEXACO Personality Inventory Two new facet scales and an observer report form Psychological Assessment 18 2 182 91 doi 10 1037 1040 3590 18 2 182 PMID 16768594 Lee K Ashton M C The HEXACO Personality Inventory Revised Scale Descriptions Retrieved March 20 2012 Bierman K L 2003 Peer rejection Developmental processes and intervention strategies New York The Guilford Press Tobin R M Graziano W G Vanman E Tassinary L 2000 Personality emotional experience and efforts to control emotions Journal of Personality amp Social Psychology 79 4 656 669 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 79 4 656 PMID 11045745 Jensen Campbell L A Graziano W G 2001 Agreeableness as a moderator of interpersonal conflict Journal of Personality 69 2 323 361 doi 10 1111 1467 6494 00148 PMID 11339802 Graziano W G Jensen Campbell L A Hair E C 1996 Perceiving interpersonal conflict and reacting to it The case for agreeableness Journal of Personality amp Social Psychology 70 4 820 835 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 70 4 820 PMID 8636901 Graziano W G Habashi M M Sheese B E amp Tobin R M 2007 Agreeableness empathy and helping A person X situation perspective Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Penner L A Fritzsche B A Craiger J P Freifeld T S 1995 Measuring the prosocial personality Advances in Personality Assessment 10 147 163 Gleason K A Jensen Campbell L A Richardson D 2004 Agreeableness and aggression in adolescence Aggressive Behavior 30 43 61 doi 10 1002 ab 20002 Graziano W G Bruce J W Sheese B E Tobin R M 2007 Attraction personality and prejudice Liking none of the people most of the time Journal of Personality amp Social Psychology 93 4 565 582 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 93 4 565 PMID 17892332 Costa P T amp McCrae R R 1992 NEO personality Inventory professional manual Odessa FL Psychological Assessment Resources Begue Laurent Beauvois Jean Leon Courbert Didier Oberble Dominique Lepage Johan Duke Aaron 2014 Personality Predicts Obedience in a Milgram Paradigm PDF Journal of Personality 83 3 299 306 doi 10 1111 jopy 12104 PMID 24798990 S2CID 13868371 Laursen B Pulkkinen L Adams R 2002 The antecedents and correlates of agreeableness in adulthood Journal of Developmental Psychology 38 4 591 603 doi 10 1037 0012 1649 38 4 591 PMC 2730208 PMID 12090488 Caspi A Elder G H Bem D J 1987 Moving against the world Life course patterns of explosive children Journal of Developmental Psychology 23 2 308 313 doi 10 1037 0012 1649 23 2 308 Shiner R L 2000 Linking childhood personality with adaptation evidence for continuity and change across time into late adolescence Journal of Personality amp Social Psychology 78 2 310 325 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 78 2 310 PMID 10707337 The relationship between US state and personality myPersonality Research December 9 2008 Archived from the original on March 18 2012 Retrieved April 7 2012 Stephanie Simon 2008 09 23 The United States of Mind Researchers Identify Regional Personality Traits Across America WSJ com Original research article Peter J Rentfrow Samuel D Gosling Jeff Potter 2008 A Theory of the Emergence Persistence and Expression of Geographic Variation in Psychological Characteristics Perspectives on Psychological Science 3 5 339 369 doi 10 1111 j 1745 6924 2008 00084 x PMID 26158954 S2CID 17059908 C B Wortman E F Loftus C A Weaver 1999 Psychology The McGraw Hill Companies Archived from the original on 2016 03 18 Retrieved 2012 03 29 Albright Linda Malloy Thomas E Dong Qi Kenny David A Fang Xiaoyi Winquist Lynn Yu Da 1997 Cross cultural consensus in personality judgments Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 72 3 558 569 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 72 3 558 ISSN 1939 1315 PMID 9120784 Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Agreeableness amp oldid 1129004735, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.