fbpx
Wikipedia

In-group and out-group

In sociology and social psychology, an in-group is a social group to which a person psychologically identifies as being a member. By contrast, an out-group is a social group with which an individual does not identify. People may for example identify with their peer group, family, community, sports team, political party, gender, sexual orientation, religion, or nation. It has been found that the psychological membership of social groups and categories is associated with a wide variety of phenomena.

Multiple layers of in-groups and out-groups in an American football stadium:
  • People in this stadium form an in-group of American football fans and professionals vs. those who are not fans of the sport.
  • Fans in attendance at the stadium vs. people spectating the match via external means, e.g. television/radio coverage.
  • Fans and professionals affiliated with one team vs. those affiliated with the opposing team.
  • Professionals on the field (players, officials, coaches, mascots and cheerleaders) vs. the paying customers in the stands who are denied access to the facility's secure nucleus except by invitation from a high-status individual.
  • Ranks of the wealthy ownership and their senior executive staff, with access to private box suites vs. high-priced talent.
  • Technical staff involved in facilities maintenance and operations vs. sporting staff (referees, timekeepers, statisticians and in-game adjudicators).
  • Media with organizational endorsement and affiliation who enjoy special player access to one team vs. non-affiliated media.

The terminology was made popular by Henri Tajfel and colleagues beginning in the 1970s during his work in formulating social identity theory. The significance of in-group and out-group categorization was identified using a method called the minimal group paradigm. Tajfel and colleagues found that people can form self-preferencing in-groups within a matter of minutes and that such groups can form even on the basis of completely arbitrary and invented discriminatory characteristics, such as preferences for certain paintings.[1][2][3][4]

In neurology, there is an established literature[5] about the innate propensity of the human brain to divide the world into us and them valence categories, where the exact membership of the in-group and out-group are socially contingent (hence vulnerable to the instruments of propaganda), and the intensity exists along a spectrum from mild to complete dehumanization of the "othered" group (such as through pseudospeciation).

Associated phenomena edit

The psychological categorization of people into in-group and out-group members is associated with a variety of phenomena. The following examples have all received a great deal of academic attention.

In-group favoritism edit

This refers to the fact that under certain conditions, people will prefer and have affinity for one's in-group over the out-group, or anyone viewed as outside the in-group. This can be expressed in one's evaluation of others, linking, allocation of resources, and many other ways.[6] How we perceive the actions of others are also affected by in-group favoritism. People may perceive the same action very differently depending on whether the action was executed by a member of the same group or a member of a different group.[7] In fact, people tend to evaluate actions of their own group or team members much more favorably than those of outgroup members.[7] An illustrative example of the way this phenomenon takes place can be demonstrated just by arbitrarily assigning a person to a distinct and objectively meaningless novel group; this alone is sufficient to create intergroup biases in which members of the perceiver's own group are preferentially favored.[8] This phenomenon was demonstrated in an empirical study conducted by Molenberghs and colleagues in 2013.[7] In the study, participants were arbitrarily divided into two teams where they watched videos of individuals of competing teams and individuals from their own team perform hand actions. Participants were then asked to judge the speed of the hand movements.[7] On average participants judged members of their own teams to be faster, although the hand movements were the exact same speed across the board. Similarly, Hastorf and Cantril conducted a pioneering study in 1954, where students of both Princeton and Dartmouth viewed a contentious football game between their two teams.[9] Although they had watched the same motion picture of the game, their versions of what transpired were so starkly different it appeared as though they had watched two totally different games.[9]

Neural mechanisms of in-group favoritism and out-group bias edit

Some may wonder why in-group favoritism takes place, even in arbitrarily assigned groups where group members have nothing in common other than the group to which they were assigned. Research points to unconscious decision-making processes that takes place at the neurological level, where in-group favoritism and out-group bias occurs very early in perception. This process can begin by simply viewing a person's face.[10] Research indicates that individuals are faster and more accurate at recognizing faces of ingroup vs. outgroup members.[11] For example, researchers in a cross-race recognition study recorded blood oxygenation level-dependent signal (BOLD) activity from black and white participants while they viewed and attempted to remember pictures of unfamiliar black faces, white faces and objects.[12] They found that participants in this study exhibited greater activity in the fusiform face area (FFA), an area of the fusiform gyrus located in the inferior temporal cortex of the brain linked to object and face recognition, when viewing same race faces compared to other race faces.[12] Lower activity in the FFA reflects a failure to encode outgroup members at the individual level rather than the categorical level, which comes at the expense of encoding individuating information.[13][14][15] This suggests out-group or unfamiliar faces may not be "faces" with the same intensity as in-group faces.[16] Prior research has also shown that the devaluation and dehumanization of outgroup members is exacerbated when the initial encoding and configural processing of an outgroup face is impeded.[17] So not only does this initial encoding process dehumanize outgroup members, it also contributes to a homogeneity effect, whereby outgroup members are perceived as more similar to each other than ingroup members.[18]

Group homogeneity edit

Categorization of people into social groups increases the perception that group members are similar to one another. An outcome of this is the out-group homogeneity effect. This refers to the perception of members of an out-group as being homogenous, while members of one's in-group are perceived as being diverse, e.g. "they are alike; we are diverse".[19][20] This is especially likely to occur in regard to negative characteristics. Under certain conditions, in-group members can be perceived as being similar to one another in regard to positive characteristics. This effect is called in-group homogeneity.[21]

Out-group derogation edit

Discrimination between in-groups and out-groups is a matter of favoritism towards an in-group and the absence of equivalent favoritism towards an out-group.[22] Out-group derogation is the phenomenon in which an out-group is perceived as being threatening to the members of an in-group.[23] This phenomenon often accompanies in-group favoritism, as it requires one to have an affinity towards their in-group. Some research suggests that out-group derogation occurs when an out-group is perceived as blocking or hindering the goals of an in-group. It has also been argued that out-group derogation is a natural consequence of the categorization process.[24]

Social influence edit

People have been shown to be differentially influenced by in-group members. That is, under conditions where group categorization is psychologically salient, people will shift their beliefs in line with in-group social norms.

Group polarization edit

This generally refers to the tendency of groups to make decisions that are more extreme than the initial inclination of its members, although polarization toward the most central beliefs has also been observed. It has been shown that this effect is related to a psychologically salient in-group and outgroup categorization.

Postulated role in human evolution edit

In evolutionary psychology, in-group favoritism is seen as an evolved mechanism selected for the advantages of coalition affiliation.[25] It has been argued that characteristics such as gender and ethnicity are inflexible or even essential features of such systems.[26][27] However, there is evidence that elements of favoritism are flexible in that they can be erased by changes in social categorization.[28] One study in the field of behavioural genetics suggests that biological mechanisms may exist which favor a coexistence of both flexible and essentialist systems.[29]

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ See "Kandinsky versus Klee experiment", Tajfel et al. (1971) in Tajfel, H. (1970). Experiments in Intergroup Discrimination.
  2. ^ Taijfel, H. (1970). (PDF). Scientific American. 223 (5): 96–102. Bibcode:1970SciAm.223e..96T. doi:10.1038/scientificamerican1170-96. JSTOR 24927662. PMID 5482577. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2019-12-06.
  3. ^ Tajfel, Henri; Billig, M. G.; Bundy, R. P.; Flament, Claude (1971). "Social categorization and intergroup behaviour". European Journal of Social Psychology. 1 (2): 149–178. doi:10.1002/ejsp.2420010202.
  4. ^ Tajfel, H. (1974). "Social identity and intergroup behaviour". Social Science Information. 13 (2): 65–93. doi:10.1177/053901847401300204. S2CID 143666442.
  5. ^ Sapolsky, Robert (2017). Behave: The Biology of Humans at Our Best and Worst. Penguin. p. 38–39, 84–93, 116–117, 387–493, 570–632. ISBN 978-1-59420-507-1.
  6. ^ Aronson, E., Wilson, T. D., Akert, R. D. & Sommers, S. R. (2015). Social psychology (9th, illustrated, revised ed.). London: Pearson Education. ISBN 978-0-13393654-4.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  7. ^ a b c d Molenberghs, Pascal; Halász, Veronika; Mattingley, Jason B.; Vanman, Eric J.; Cunnington, Ross (2013). "Seeing is believing: Neural mechanisms of action–perception are biased by team membership". Human Brain Mapping. 34 (9): 2055–2068. doi:10.1002/hbm.22044. ISSN 1097-0193. PMC 6870530. PMID 22290781.
  8. ^ Tajfel, Henri; Billig, M. G.; Bundy, R. P.; Flament, Claude (1971). "Social categorization and intergroup behaviour". European Journal of Social Psychology. 1 (2): 149–178. doi:10.1002/ejsp.2420010202. ISSN 1099-0992.
  9. ^ a b Hastorf, Albert H.; Cantril, Hadley (1954). "They saw a game; a case study". The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology. 49 (1): 129–134. doi:10.1037/h0057880. ISSN 0096-851X. PMID 13128974.
  10. ^ Senholzi, Keith B.; Kubota, Jennifer T. (2016), "The Neural Mechanisms of Prejudice Intervention", Neuroimaging Personality, Social Cognition, and Character, Elsevier, pp. 337–354, doi:10.1016/b978-0-12-800935-2.00018-x, ISBN 978-0-12-800935-2, retrieved 2020-12-07
  11. ^ Corenblum, B.; Meissner, Christian A. (March 2006). "Recognition of faces of ingroup and outgroup children and adults". Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 93 (3): 187–206. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2005.09.001. PMID 16243349.
  12. ^ a b Golby, Alexandra J.; Gabrieli, John D. E.; Chiao, Joan Y.; Eberhardt, Jennifer L. (August 2001). "Differential responses in the fusiform region to same-race and other-race faces". Nature Neuroscience. 4 (8): 845–850. doi:10.1038/90565. ISSN 1097-6256. PMID 11477432. S2CID 16699355.
  13. ^ Young, Steven G.; Hugenberg, Kurt (2011-05-31). "Individuation Motivation and Face Experience Can Operate Jointly to Produce the Own-Race Bias". Social Psychological and Personality Science. 3 (1): 80–87. doi:10.1177/1948550611409759. ISSN 1948-5506. S2CID 145427030.
  14. ^ Sangrigoli, Sandy; de Schonen, Scania (October 2004). "Recognition of own-race and other-race faces by three-month-old infants". Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 45 (7): 1219–1227. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00319.x. ISSN 0021-9630. PMID 15335342.
  15. ^ Ostrom, Thomas M.; Carpenter, Sandra L.; Sedikides, Constantine; Li, Fan (1993). "Differential processing of in-group and out-group information". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 64 (1): 21–34. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.64.1.21. ISSN 1939-1315.
  16. ^ Brosch, Tobias; Bar-David, Eyal; Phelps, Elizabeth A. (2013-01-08). "Implicit Race Bias Decreases the Similarity of Neural Representations of Black and White Faces". Psychological Science. 24 (2): 160–166. doi:10.1177/0956797612451465. ISSN 0956-7976. PMC 3864653. PMID 23300228.
  17. ^ Krosch, Amy R.; Amodio, David M. (November 2019). "Scarcity disrupts the neural encoding of Black faces: A socioperceptual pathway to discrimination". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 117 (5): 859–875. doi:10.1037/pspa0000168. ISSN 1939-1315. PMID 31233317.
  18. ^ Kubota, Jennifer T; Banaji, Mahzarin R; Phelps, Elizabeth A (July 2012). "The neuroscience of race". Nature Neuroscience. 15 (7): 940–948. doi:10.1038/nn.3136. ISSN 1097-6256. PMC 3864590. PMID 22735516.
  19. ^ Leyens, Jacques-Philippe; Yzerbyt, Vincent; Schadron, Georges (1994). Stereotypes and Social Cognition. Thousand Oaks, California: SAGE Publications. pp. 104–107. ISBN 978-0-80398583-4.
  20. ^ Quattrone, George A.; Jones, Edward E. (1980). "The perception of variability within in-groups and out-groups: Implications for the law of small numbers". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 38 (1): 141–152. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.38.1.141. ISSN 0022-3514.
  21. ^ Jackson, Lynne M. (2011). The Psychology of Prejudice: From Attitudes to Social Action. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. pp. 110–112. ISBN 978-1-43380920-0.
  22. ^ Brewer, Marilynn B. (Fall 1999). "The Psychology of Prejudice: In-group Love and Out-group Hate?". Journal of Social Issues. 55 (3): 429–444. doi:10.1111/0022-4537.00126.
  23. ^ Hewstone, Miles; Rubin, Mark; Willis, Hazel (February 2002). "Intergroup Bias". Annual Review of Psychology. 53: 575–604. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135109. PMID 11752497. S2CID 11830211.
  24. ^ Zhong, Chen-Bo; Phillips, Katherine W.; Leonardelli, Geoffrey J.; Galinsky, Adam D. (2008). "Negational categorization and intergroup behavior". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 34 (6): 793–806. doi:10.1177/0146167208315457. PMID 18391025. S2CID 14610644.
  25. ^ L. Cosmides; J. Tooby; R. Kurzban (April 1, 2003). "Perceptions of race". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 7 (4): 173–179. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00057-3. PMID 12691766. S2CID 11343153.
  26. ^ L. A. Hirschfeld (1996). Race in the Making: Cognition, Culture, and the Child's Construction of Human Kinds. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Mit Press. ISBN 978-0-26208247-1.
  27. ^ F. J. Gil-White (August–October 2001). "Are Ethnic Groups Biological "Species" to the Human Brain? Essentialism in Our Cognition of Some Social Categories". Current Anthropology. University of Chicago Press. 42 (4): 515–553. doi:10.1086/321802. JSTOR 10.1086/321802. S2CID 53592867.
  28. ^ R. Kurzban; J. Tooby; L. Cosmides (December 18, 2001). "Can race be erased? Coalitional computation and social categorization". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 98 (26): 15387–15392. Bibcode:2001PNAS...9815387K. doi:10.1073/pnas.251541498. PMC 65039. PMID 11742078.
  29. ^ G. J. Lewis; T. C. Bates (November 2010). "Genetic Evidence for Multiple Biological Mechanisms Underlying In-Group Favoritism". Psychological Science. 21 (11): 1623–1628. doi:10.1177/0956797610387439. PMID 20974715. S2CID 27351021.

group, group, cladistics, ingroup, outgroup, cladistics, them, redirects, here, other, uses, them, sociology, social, psychology, group, social, group, which, person, psychologically, identifies, being, member, contrast, group, social, group, with, which, indi. For use in cladistics see Ingroup and outgroup cladistics Us and them redirects here For other uses see Us and Them In sociology and social psychology an in group is a social group to which a person psychologically identifies as being a member By contrast an out group is a social group with which an individual does not identify People may for example identify with their peer group family community sports team political party gender sexual orientation religion or nation It has been found that the psychological membership of social groups and categories is associated with a wide variety of phenomena Multiple layers of in groups and out groups in an American football stadium People in this stadium form an in group of American football fans and professionals vs those who are not fans of the sport Fans in attendance at the stadium vs people spectating the match via external means e g television radio coverage Fans and professionals affiliated with one team vs those affiliated with the opposing team Professionals on the field players officials coaches mascots and cheerleaders vs the paying customers in the stands who are denied access to the facility s secure nucleus except by invitation from a high status individual Ranks of the wealthy ownership and their senior executive staff with access to private box suites vs high priced talent Technical staff involved in facilities maintenance and operations vs sporting staff referees timekeepers statisticians and in game adjudicators Media with organizational endorsement and affiliation who enjoy special player access to one team vs non affiliated media The terminology was made popular by Henri Tajfel and colleagues beginning in the 1970s during his work in formulating social identity theory The significance of in group and out group categorization was identified using a method called the minimal group paradigm Tajfel and colleagues found that people can form self preferencing in groups within a matter of minutes and that such groups can form even on the basis of completely arbitrary and invented discriminatory characteristics such as preferences for certain paintings 1 2 3 4 In neurology there is an established literature 5 about the innate propensity of the human brain to divide the world into us and them valence categories where the exact membership of the in group and out group are socially contingent hence vulnerable to the instruments of propaganda and the intensity exists along a spectrum from mild to complete dehumanization of the othered group such as through pseudospeciation Contents 1 Associated phenomena 1 1 In group favoritism 1 2 Neural mechanisms of in group favoritism and out group bias 1 3 Group homogeneity 1 4 Out group derogation 1 5 Social influence 1 6 Group polarization 2 Postulated role in human evolution 3 See also 4 ReferencesAssociated phenomena editThe psychological categorization of people into in group and out group members is associated with a variety of phenomena The following examples have all received a great deal of academic attention In group favoritism edit See also In group favoritism Closure sociology and You are either with us or against us This refers to the fact that under certain conditions people will prefer and have affinity for one s in group over the out group or anyone viewed as outside the in group This can be expressed in one s evaluation of others linking allocation of resources and many other ways 6 How we perceive the actions of others are also affected by in group favoritism People may perceive the same action very differently depending on whether the action was executed by a member of the same group or a member of a different group 7 In fact people tend to evaluate actions of their own group or team members much more favorably than those of outgroup members 7 An illustrative example of the way this phenomenon takes place can be demonstrated just by arbitrarily assigning a person to a distinct and objectively meaningless novel group this alone is sufficient to create intergroup biases in which members of the perceiver s own group are preferentially favored 8 This phenomenon was demonstrated in an empirical study conducted by Molenberghs and colleagues in 2013 7 In the study participants were arbitrarily divided into two teams where they watched videos of individuals of competing teams and individuals from their own team perform hand actions Participants were then asked to judge the speed of the hand movements 7 On average participants judged members of their own teams to be faster although the hand movements were the exact same speed across the board Similarly Hastorf and Cantril conducted a pioneering study in 1954 where students of both Princeton and Dartmouth viewed a contentious football game between their two teams 9 Although they had watched the same motion picture of the game their versions of what transpired were so starkly different it appeared as though they had watched two totally different games 9 Neural mechanisms of in group favoritism and out group bias edit Some may wonder why in group favoritism takes place even in arbitrarily assigned groups where group members have nothing in common other than the group to which they were assigned Research points to unconscious decision making processes that takes place at the neurological level where in group favoritism and out group bias occurs very early in perception This process can begin by simply viewing a person s face 10 Research indicates that individuals are faster and more accurate at recognizing faces of ingroup vs outgroup members 11 For example researchers in a cross race recognition study recorded blood oxygenation level dependent signal BOLD activity from black and white participants while they viewed and attempted to remember pictures of unfamiliar black faces white faces and objects 12 They found that participants in this study exhibited greater activity in the fusiform face area FFA an area of the fusiform gyrus located in the inferior temporal cortex of the brain linked to object and face recognition when viewing same race faces compared to other race faces 12 Lower activity in the FFA reflects a failure to encode outgroup members at the individual level rather than the categorical level which comes at the expense of encoding individuating information 13 14 15 This suggests out group or unfamiliar faces may not be faces with the same intensity as in group faces 16 Prior research has also shown that the devaluation and dehumanization of outgroup members is exacerbated when the initial encoding and configural processing of an outgroup face is impeded 17 So not only does this initial encoding process dehumanize outgroup members it also contributes to a homogeneity effect whereby outgroup members are perceived as more similar to each other than ingroup members 18 Group homogeneity edit Main article Outgroup homogeneity Categorization of people into social groups increases the perception that group members are similar to one another An outcome of this is the out group homogeneity effect This refers to the perception of members of an out group as being homogenous while members of one s in group are perceived as being diverse e g they are alike we are diverse 19 20 This is especially likely to occur in regard to negative characteristics Under certain conditions in group members can be perceived as being similar to one another in regard to positive characteristics This effect is called in group homogeneity 21 Out group derogation edit Discrimination between in groups and out groups is a matter of favoritism towards an in group and the absence of equivalent favoritism towards an out group 22 Out group derogation is the phenomenon in which an out group is perceived as being threatening to the members of an in group 23 This phenomenon often accompanies in group favoritism as it requires one to have an affinity towards their in group Some research suggests that out group derogation occurs when an out group is perceived as blocking or hindering the goals of an in group It has also been argued that out group derogation is a natural consequence of the categorization process 24 Social influence edit Main article Social influence People have been shown to be differentially influenced by in group members That is under conditions where group categorization is psychologically salient people will shift their beliefs in line with in group social norms Group polarization edit Main article Group polarization This generally refers to the tendency of groups to make decisions that are more extreme than the initial inclination of its members although polarization toward the most central beliefs has also been observed It has been shown that this effect is related to a psychologically salient in group and outgroup categorization Postulated role in human evolution editIn evolutionary psychology in group favoritism is seen as an evolved mechanism selected for the advantages of coalition affiliation 25 It has been argued that characteristics such as gender and ethnicity are inflexible or even essential features of such systems 26 27 However there is evidence that elements of favoritism are flexible in that they can be erased by changes in social categorization 28 One study in the field of behavioural genetics suggests that biological mechanisms may exist which favor a coexistence of both flexible and essentialist systems 29 See also editAllosemitism Amity enmity complex Antilocution Ambivalent prejudice Autarky Bandwagon effect Basking in reflected glory Belongingness Benevolent prejudice Cultural identity Cronyism Collective narcissism Common ingroup identity Double standard Endogamy Elitism False consensus effect Groupthink Homophobia Hostile prejudice List of terms for ethnic out groups Microculture Nationalism Nepotism Paradox of tolerance Positional good Prejudice Racism Scapegoating Sexism Shibboleth Social class Social dominance orientation Subculture Tribalism Uchi sotoReferences edit See Kandinsky versus Klee experiment Tajfel et al 1971 in Tajfel H 1970 Experiments in Intergroup Discrimination Taijfel H 1970 Experiments in intergroup discrimination PDF Scientific American 223 5 96 102 Bibcode 1970SciAm 223e 96T doi 10 1038 scientificamerican1170 96 JSTOR 24927662 PMID 5482577 Archived from the original PDF on 2019 12 06 Tajfel Henri Billig M G Bundy R P Flament Claude 1971 Social categorization and intergroup behaviour European Journal of Social Psychology 1 2 149 178 doi 10 1002 ejsp 2420010202 Tajfel H 1974 Social identity and intergroup behaviour Social Science Information 13 2 65 93 doi 10 1177 053901847401300204 S2CID 143666442 Sapolsky Robert 2017 Behave The Biology of Humans at Our Best and Worst Penguin p 38 39 84 93 116 117 387 493 570 632 ISBN 978 1 59420 507 1 Aronson E Wilson T D Akert R D amp Sommers S R 2015 Social psychology 9th illustrated revised ed London Pearson Education ISBN 978 0 13393654 4 a href Template Cite book html title Template Cite book cite book a CS1 maint multiple names authors list link a b c d Molenberghs Pascal Halasz Veronika Mattingley Jason B Vanman Eric J Cunnington Ross 2013 Seeing is believing Neural mechanisms of action perception are biased by team membership Human Brain Mapping 34 9 2055 2068 doi 10 1002 hbm 22044 ISSN 1097 0193 PMC 6870530 PMID 22290781 Tajfel Henri Billig M G Bundy R P Flament Claude 1971 Social categorization and intergroup behaviour European Journal of Social Psychology 1 2 149 178 doi 10 1002 ejsp 2420010202 ISSN 1099 0992 a b Hastorf Albert H Cantril Hadley 1954 They saw a game a case study The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 49 1 129 134 doi 10 1037 h0057880 ISSN 0096 851X PMID 13128974 Senholzi Keith B Kubota Jennifer T 2016 The Neural Mechanisms of Prejudice Intervention Neuroimaging Personality Social Cognition and Character Elsevier pp 337 354 doi 10 1016 b978 0 12 800935 2 00018 x ISBN 978 0 12 800935 2 retrieved 2020 12 07 Corenblum B Meissner Christian A March 2006 Recognition of faces of ingroup and outgroup children and adults Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 93 3 187 206 doi 10 1016 j jecp 2005 09 001 PMID 16243349 a b Golby Alexandra J Gabrieli John D E Chiao Joan Y Eberhardt Jennifer L August 2001 Differential responses in the fusiform region to same race and other race faces Nature Neuroscience 4 8 845 850 doi 10 1038 90565 ISSN 1097 6256 PMID 11477432 S2CID 16699355 Young Steven G Hugenberg Kurt 2011 05 31 Individuation Motivation and Face Experience Can Operate Jointly to Produce the Own Race Bias Social Psychological and Personality Science 3 1 80 87 doi 10 1177 1948550611409759 ISSN 1948 5506 S2CID 145427030 Sangrigoli Sandy de Schonen Scania October 2004 Recognition of own race and other race faces by three month old infants Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 45 7 1219 1227 doi 10 1111 j 1469 7610 2004 00319 x ISSN 0021 9630 PMID 15335342 Ostrom Thomas M Carpenter Sandra L Sedikides Constantine Li Fan 1993 Differential processing of in group and out group information Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 64 1 21 34 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 64 1 21 ISSN 1939 1315 Brosch Tobias Bar David Eyal Phelps Elizabeth A 2013 01 08 Implicit Race Bias Decreases the Similarity of Neural Representations of Black and White Faces Psychological Science 24 2 160 166 doi 10 1177 0956797612451465 ISSN 0956 7976 PMC 3864653 PMID 23300228 Krosch Amy R Amodio David M November 2019 Scarcity disrupts the neural encoding of Black faces A socioperceptual pathway to discrimination Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 117 5 859 875 doi 10 1037 pspa0000168 ISSN 1939 1315 PMID 31233317 Kubota Jennifer T Banaji Mahzarin R Phelps Elizabeth A July 2012 The neuroscience of race Nature Neuroscience 15 7 940 948 doi 10 1038 nn 3136 ISSN 1097 6256 PMC 3864590 PMID 22735516 Leyens Jacques Philippe Yzerbyt Vincent Schadron Georges 1994 Stereotypes and Social Cognition Thousand Oaks California SAGE Publications pp 104 107 ISBN 978 0 80398583 4 Quattrone George A Jones Edward E 1980 The perception of variability within in groups and out groups Implications for the law of small numbers Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 38 1 141 152 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 38 1 141 ISSN 0022 3514 Jackson Lynne M 2011 The Psychology of Prejudice From Attitudes to Social Action Washington DC American Psychological Association pp 110 112 ISBN 978 1 43380920 0 Brewer Marilynn B Fall 1999 The Psychology of Prejudice In group Love and Out group Hate Journal of Social Issues 55 3 429 444 doi 10 1111 0022 4537 00126 Hewstone Miles Rubin Mark Willis Hazel February 2002 Intergroup Bias Annual Review of Psychology 53 575 604 doi 10 1146 annurev psych 53 100901 135109 PMID 11752497 S2CID 11830211 Zhong Chen Bo Phillips Katherine W Leonardelli Geoffrey J Galinsky Adam D 2008 Negational categorization and intergroup behavior Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 34 6 793 806 doi 10 1177 0146167208315457 PMID 18391025 S2CID 14610644 L Cosmides J Tooby R Kurzban April 1 2003 Perceptions of race Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7 4 173 179 doi 10 1016 S1364 6613 03 00057 3 PMID 12691766 S2CID 11343153 L A Hirschfeld 1996 Race in the Making Cognition Culture and the Child s Construction of Human Kinds Cambridge Massachusetts Mit Press ISBN 978 0 26208247 1 F J Gil White August October 2001 Are Ethnic Groups Biological Species to the Human Brain Essentialism in Our Cognition of Some Social Categories Current Anthropology University of Chicago Press 42 4 515 553 doi 10 1086 321802 JSTOR 10 1086 321802 S2CID 53592867 R Kurzban J Tooby L Cosmides December 18 2001 Can race be erased Coalitional computation and social categorization Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 98 26 15387 15392 Bibcode 2001PNAS 9815387K doi 10 1073 pnas 251541498 PMC 65039 PMID 11742078 G J Lewis T C Bates November 2010 Genetic Evidence for Multiple Biological Mechanisms Underlying In Group Favoritism Psychological Science 21 11 1623 1628 doi 10 1177 0956797610387439 PMID 20974715 S2CID 27351021 Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title In group and out group amp oldid 1205853097, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.