fbpx
Wikipedia

Trichromacy

Trichromacy or trichromatism is the possession of three independent channels for conveying color information, derived from the three different types of cone cells in the eye.[1] Organisms with trichromacy are called trichromats.

Close-up of a trichromatic in-line shadow mask CRT display, which creates most visible colors through combinations and different levels of the three primary colors: red, green and blue

The normal explanation of trichromacy is that the organism's retina contains three types of color receptors (called cone cells in vertebrates) with different absorption spectra. In actuality, the number of such receptor types may be greater than three, since different types may be active at different light intensities. In vertebrates with three types of cone cells, at low light intensities the rod cells may contribute to color vision.

Humans and other animals that are trichromats edit

Humans and some other mammals have evolved trichromacy based partly on pigments inherited from early vertebrates. In fish and birds, for example, four pigments are used for vision. These extra cone receptor visual pigments detect energy of other wavelengths, sometimes including ultraviolet. Eventually two of these pigments were lost (in placental mammals) and another was gained, resulting in trichromacy among some primates.[2] Humans and closely related primates are usually trichromats, as are some of the females of most species of New World monkeys, and both male and female howler monkeys.[3]

Recent research suggests that trichromacy may also be quite general among marsupials.[4] A study conducted regarding trichromacy in Australian marsupials suggests the medium wavelength sensitivity (MWS), cones of the honey possum (Tarsipes rostratus) and the fat-tailed dunnart (Sminthopsis crassicaudata) are features coming from the inherited reptilian retinal arrangement. The possibility of trichromacy in marsupials potentially has another evolutionary basis than that of primates. Further biological and behavioural tests may verify if trichromacy is a common characteristic of marsupials.[2]

Most other mammals are currently thought to be dichromats, with only two types of cone (though limited trichromacy is possible at low light levels where the rods and cones are both active).[5] Most studies of carnivores, as of other mammals, reveal dichromacy; examples include the domestic dog, the ferret, and the spotted hyena.[6][7] Some species of insects (such as honeybees) are also trichromats, being sensitive to ultraviolet, blue and green instead of blue, green and red.[3]

Research indicates that trichromacy allows animals to distinguish brightly colored fruit and young leaves from other vegetation that is not beneficial to their survival.[8] Another theory is that detecting skin flushing and thereby mood may have influenced the development of primate trichromate vision. The color red also has other effects on primate and human behavior as discussed in the color psychology article.[9]

Types of cones specifically found in primates edit

Primates are the only known placental mammalian trichromats.[10][failed verification] Their eyes include three different kinds of cones, each containing a different photopigment (opsin). Their peak sensitivities lie in the blue (short-wavelength S cones), green (medium-wavelength M cones) and yellow-green (long-wavelength L cones) regions of the color spectrum.[11] S cones make up 5–10% of the cones and form a regular mosaic. Special bipolar and ganglion cells pass those signals from S cones and there is evidence that they have a separate signal pathway through the thalamus to the visual cortex as well. On the other hand, the L and M cones are hard to distinguish by their shapes or other anatomical means – their opsins differ in only 15 out of 363 amino acids, so no one has yet succeeded in producing specific antibodies to them. But Mollon and Bowmaker[12] did find that L cones and M cones are randomly distributed and are in equal numbers.[13]

Mechanism of trichromatic color vision edit

 
Normalised responsivity spectra of human cone cells
 
Illustration of color metamerism:
In column 1, a ball is illuminated by monochromatic light. Multiplying the spectrum by the cones' spectral sensitivity curves gives the response for each cone type.
In column 2, metamerism is used to simulate the scene with blue, green and red LEDs, giving a similar response.

Trichromatic color vision is the ability of humans and some other animals to see different colors, mediated by interactions among three types of color-sensing cone cells. The trichromatic color theory began in the 18th century, when Thomas Young proposed that color vision was a result of three different photoreceptor cells. From the middle of the 19th century, in his Treatise on Physiological Optics,[14][15] Hermann von Helmholtz later expanded on Young's ideas using color-matching experiments which showed that people with normal vision needed three wavelengths to create the normal range of colors. Physiological evidence for trichromatic theory was later given by Gunnar Svaetichin (1956).[16]

Each of the three types of cones in the retina of the eye contains a different type of photosensitive pigment, which is composed of a transmembrane protein called opsin and a light-sensitive molecule called 11-cis retinal. Each different pigment is especially sensitive to a certain wavelength of light (that is, the pigment is most likely to produce a cellular response when it is hit by a photon with the specific wavelength to which that pigment is most sensitive). The three types of cones are L, M, and S, which have pigments that respond best to light of long (especially 560 nm), medium (530 nm), and short (420 nm) wavelengths respectively.[17][18]

Since the likelihood of response of a given cone varies not only with the wavelength of the light that hits it but also with its intensity, the brain would not be able to discriminate different colors if it had input from only one type of cone. Thus, interaction between at least two types of cone is necessary to produce the ability to perceive color. With at least two types of cones, the brain can compare the signals from each type and determine both the intensity and color of the light. For example, moderate stimulation of a medium-wavelength cone cell could mean that it is being stimulated by very bright red (long-wavelength) light, or by not very intense yellowish-green light. But very bright red light would produce a stronger response from L cones than from M cones, while not very intense yellowish light would produce a stronger response from M cones than from other cones. Thus trichromatic color vision is accomplished by using combinations of cell responses.

It is estimated that the average human can distinguish up to ten million different colors.[19]

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ . Archived from the original on 4 October 2015. Retrieved 8 November 2006.
  2. ^ a b Arrese, Catherine; Thomas, Nathan; Beazley, Lyn; Shand, Julia (2002). "Trichromacy in Australian Marsupials". Current Biology. 12 (8): 657–660. Bibcode:2002CBio...12..657A. doi:10.1016/S0960-9822(02)00772-8. PMID 11967153. S2CID 14604695.
  3. ^ a b Rowe, Michael H (2002). "Trichromatic color vision in primates". News in Physiological Sciences. 17 (3): 93–98. doi:10.1152/nips.01376.2001. PMID 12021378. S2CID 15241669.
  4. ^ Arrese, CA; Oddy, AY; Runham, PB; Hart, NS; Shand, J; Hunt, DM (2005). "Cone topography and spectral sensitivity in two potentially trichromatic marsupials, the quokka (Setonix brachyurus) and quenda (Isoodon obesulus)". Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B. 272 (1595): 791–796. doi:10.1098/rspb.2004.3009. PMC 1599861. PMID 15888411.
  5. ^ Jacobs, Gerald H.; Nathans, Jeremy (2009). "The Evolution of Primate Color Vision". Scientific American. 300 (4): 56–63. Bibcode:2009SciAm.300d..56J. doi:10.1038/scientificamerican0409-56 (inactive 2 April 2024). ISSN 0036-8733. JSTOR 26001303. PMID 19363921.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: DOI inactive as of April 2024 (link)
  6. ^ Calderone, JB; Jacobs, GH (2003). "Spectral properties and retinal distribution of ferret cones" (PDF). Visual Neuroscience. 20 (1): 11–17. doi:10.1017/s0952523803201024. PMID 12699079. S2CID 10415194.
  7. ^ Calderone, JB; Reese, BE; Jacobs, GH (2003). "Topography of photoreceptors and retinal ganglion cells in the spotted hyena (Crocuta crocuta)". Brain, Behavior and Evolution. 62 (4): 182–192. doi:10.1159/000073270. PMID 14573992. S2CID 9167855.
  8. ^ Sharpe LT, de Luca E, Hansen T, Jägle H, Gegenfurtner KR (2006). "Advantages and disadvantages of human dichromacy". Journal of Vision. 6 (3): 213–223. doi:10.1167/6.3.3. PMID 16643091.
  9. ^ Diana Widermann, Robert A. Barton, and Russel A. Hill. Evolutionary perspectives on sport and competition. In Roberts, S. C. (2011). Roberts, S. Craig (ed.). Applied Evolutionary Psychology. Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199586073.001.0001. ISBN 9780199586073.
  10. ^ Ronald G. Boothe (2002). Perception of the visual environment. Springer. p. 219. ISBN 978-0-387-98790-3.
  11. ^ Schnapf, J. L.; Kraft, T. W.; Baylor, D. A. (January 1987). "Spectral sensitivity of human cone photoreceptors". Nature. 325 (6103): 439–441. Bibcode:1987Natur.325..439S. doi:10.1038/325439a0. PMID 3808045. S2CID 11399054.
  12. ^ Mollon, J. D.; Bowmaker, J. K. (December 1992). "The spatial arrangement of cones in the primate fovea". Nature. 360 (6405): 677–679. Bibcode:1992Natur.360..677M. doi:10.1038/360677a0. PMID 1465131. S2CID 4234999.
  13. ^ Wässle, Heinz (11 February 1999). "Colour vision: A patchwork of cones". Nature. 397 (6719): 473–475. Bibcode:1999Natur.397..473W. doi:10.1038/17216. PMID 10028963. S2CID 4431471.
  14. ^ von Helmholtz, Hermann (1909). Handbuch der Physiologischen Optik (3 ed.). Hamburg ; Leipzig: Leopold Voss. Retrieved 18 February 2020.
  15. ^ von Helmholtz, Hermann (2013). Treatise on Physiological Optics. Courier Corporation. ISBN 978-0486174709. Retrieved 18 February 2020.
  16. ^ Svaetichin, G (1956). "Spectral response curves from single cones". Acta Physiologica Scandinavica. 39 (134): 17–46. PMID 13444020.
  17. ^ Kandel ER, Schwartz JH, Jessell TM (2000). Principles of Neural Science (4th ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill. pp. 182–185. ISBN 978-0-8385-7701-1.
  18. ^ Jacobs GH, Nathans J (March 2009). "Color Vision: How Our Eyes Reflect Primate Evolution". Scientific American.
  19. ^ Leong, Jennifer. "Number of Colors Distinguishable by the Human Eye". hypertextbook. Retrieved 21 February 2013.

External links edit

  • The Straight Dope: "Are cats and dogs really color-blind? How do they know?"
  • Svaetichin, Gunnar; MacNichol, Edward F. (November 1958). "Retinal mechanisms for chromatic and achromatic vision". Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 74 (2): 385–404. Bibcode:1959NYASA..74..385S. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.1958.tb39560.x. PMID 13627867. S2CID 27130943.

trichromacy, trichromat, redirects, here, chemical, species, trichromate, trichromatism, possession, three, independent, channels, conveying, color, information, derived, from, three, different, types, cone, cells, organisms, with, trichromacy, called, trichro. Trichromat redirects here For the chemical ion species see Trichromate Trichromacy or trichromatism is the possession of three independent channels for conveying color information derived from the three different types of cone cells in the eye 1 Organisms with trichromacy are called trichromats Close up of a trichromatic in line shadow mask CRT display which creates most visible colors through combinations and different levels of the three primary colors red green and blueThe normal explanation of trichromacy is that the organism s retina contains three types of color receptors called cone cells in vertebrates with different absorption spectra In actuality the number of such receptor types may be greater than three since different types may be active at different light intensities In vertebrates with three types of cone cells at low light intensities the rod cells may contribute to color vision Contents 1 Humans and other animals that are trichromats 2 Types of cones specifically found in primates 3 Mechanism of trichromatic color vision 4 See also 5 References 6 External linksHumans and other animals that are trichromats editHumans and some other mammals have evolved trichromacy based partly on pigments inherited from early vertebrates In fish and birds for example four pigments are used for vision These extra cone receptor visual pigments detect energy of other wavelengths sometimes including ultraviolet Eventually two of these pigments were lost in placental mammals and another was gained resulting in trichromacy among some primates 2 Humans and closely related primates are usually trichromats as are some of the females of most species of New World monkeys and both male and female howler monkeys 3 Recent research suggests that trichromacy may also be quite general among marsupials 4 A study conducted regarding trichromacy in Australian marsupials suggests the medium wavelength sensitivity MWS cones of the honey possum Tarsipes rostratus and the fat tailed dunnart Sminthopsis crassicaudata are features coming from the inherited reptilian retinal arrangement The possibility of trichromacy in marsupials potentially has another evolutionary basis than that of primates Further biological and behavioural tests may verify if trichromacy is a common characteristic of marsupials 2 Most other mammals are currently thought to be dichromats with only two types of cone though limited trichromacy is possible at low light levels where the rods and cones are both active 5 Most studies of carnivores as of other mammals reveal dichromacy examples include the domestic dog the ferret and the spotted hyena 6 7 Some species of insects such as honeybees are also trichromats being sensitive to ultraviolet blue and green instead of blue green and red 3 Research indicates that trichromacy allows animals to distinguish brightly colored fruit and young leaves from other vegetation that is not beneficial to their survival 8 Another theory is that detecting skin flushing and thereby mood may have influenced the development of primate trichromate vision The color red also has other effects on primate and human behavior as discussed in the color psychology article 9 Types of cones specifically found in primates editPrimates are the only known placental mammalian trichromats 10 failed verification Their eyes include three different kinds of cones each containing a different photopigment opsin Their peak sensitivities lie in the blue short wavelength S cones green medium wavelength M cones and yellow green long wavelength L cones regions of the color spectrum 11 S cones make up 5 10 of the cones and form a regular mosaic Special bipolar and ganglion cells pass those signals from S cones and there is evidence that they have a separate signal pathway through the thalamus to the visual cortex as well On the other hand the L and M cones are hard to distinguish by their shapes or other anatomical means their opsins differ in only 15 out of 363 amino acids so no one has yet succeeded in producing specific antibodies to them But Mollon and Bowmaker 12 did find that L cones and M cones are randomly distributed and are in equal numbers 13 Mechanism of trichromatic color vision edit nbsp Normalised responsivity spectra of human cone cells nbsp Illustration of color metamerism In column 1 a ball is illuminated by monochromatic light Multiplying the spectrum by the cones spectral sensitivity curves gives the response for each cone type In column 2 metamerism is used to simulate the scene with blue green and red LEDs giving a similar response Trichromatic color vision is the ability of humans and some other animals to see different colors mediated by interactions among three types of color sensing cone cells The trichromatic color theory began in the 18th century when Thomas Young proposed that color vision was a result of three different photoreceptor cells From the middle of the 19th century in his Treatise on Physiological Optics 14 15 Hermann von Helmholtz later expanded on Young s ideas using color matching experiments which showed that people with normal vision needed three wavelengths to create the normal range of colors Physiological evidence for trichromatic theory was later given by Gunnar Svaetichin 1956 16 Each of the three types of cones in the retina of the eye contains a different type of photosensitive pigment which is composed of a transmembrane protein called opsin and a light sensitive molecule called 11 cis retinal Each different pigment is especially sensitive to a certain wavelength of light that is the pigment is most likely to produce a cellular response when it is hit by a photon with the specific wavelength to which that pigment is most sensitive The three types of cones are L M and S which have pigments that respond best to light of long especially 560 nm medium 530 nm and short 420 nm wavelengths respectively 17 18 Since the likelihood of response of a given cone varies not only with the wavelength of the light that hits it but also with its intensity the brain would not be able to discriminate different colors if it had input from only one type of cone Thus interaction between at least two types of cone is necessary to produce the ability to perceive color With at least two types of cones the brain can compare the signals from each type and determine both the intensity and color of the light For example moderate stimulation of a medium wavelength cone cell could mean that it is being stimulated by very bright red long wavelength light or by not very intense yellowish green light But very bright red light would produce a stronger response from L cones than from M cones while not very intense yellowish light would produce a stronger response from M cones than from other cones Thus trichromatic color vision is accomplished by using combinations of cell responses It is estimated that the average human can distinguish up to ten million different colors 19 See also editVisual system Monochromacy Dichromacy Tetrachromacy Pentachromacy Mantis shrimp dodecachromats Evolution of color vision in primates Young Helmholtz theory LMS color spaceReferences edit Color Glossary Archived from the original on 4 October 2015 Retrieved 8 November 2006 a b Arrese Catherine Thomas Nathan Beazley Lyn Shand Julia 2002 Trichromacy in Australian Marsupials Current Biology 12 8 657 660 Bibcode 2002CBio 12 657A doi 10 1016 S0960 9822 02 00772 8 PMID 11967153 S2CID 14604695 a b Rowe Michael H 2002 Trichromatic color vision in primates News in Physiological Sciences 17 3 93 98 doi 10 1152 nips 01376 2001 PMID 12021378 S2CID 15241669 Arrese CA Oddy AY Runham PB Hart NS Shand J Hunt DM 2005 Cone topography and spectral sensitivity in two potentially trichromatic marsupials the quokka Setonix brachyurus and quenda Isoodon obesulus Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B 272 1595 791 796 doi 10 1098 rspb 2004 3009 PMC 1599861 PMID 15888411 Jacobs Gerald H Nathans Jeremy 2009 The Evolution of Primate Color Vision Scientific American 300 4 56 63 Bibcode 2009SciAm 300d 56J doi 10 1038 scientificamerican0409 56 inactive 2 April 2024 ISSN 0036 8733 JSTOR 26001303 PMID 19363921 a href Template Cite journal html title Template Cite journal cite journal a CS1 maint DOI inactive as of April 2024 link Calderone JB Jacobs GH 2003 Spectral properties and retinal distribution of ferret cones PDF Visual Neuroscience 20 1 11 17 doi 10 1017 s0952523803201024 PMID 12699079 S2CID 10415194 Calderone JB Reese BE Jacobs GH 2003 Topography of photoreceptors and retinal ganglion cells in the spotted hyena Crocuta crocuta Brain Behavior and Evolution 62 4 182 192 doi 10 1159 000073270 PMID 14573992 S2CID 9167855 Sharpe LT de Luca E Hansen T Jagle H Gegenfurtner KR 2006 Advantages and disadvantages of human dichromacy Journal of Vision 6 3 213 223 doi 10 1167 6 3 3 PMID 16643091 Diana Widermann Robert A Barton and Russel A Hill Evolutionary perspectives on sport and competition In Roberts S C 2011 Roberts S Craig ed Applied Evolutionary Psychology Oxford University Press doi 10 1093 acprof oso 9780199586073 001 0001 ISBN 9780199586073 Ronald G Boothe 2002 Perception of the visual environment Springer p 219 ISBN 978 0 387 98790 3 Schnapf J L Kraft T W Baylor D A January 1987 Spectral sensitivity of human cone photoreceptors Nature 325 6103 439 441 Bibcode 1987Natur 325 439S doi 10 1038 325439a0 PMID 3808045 S2CID 11399054 Mollon J D Bowmaker J K December 1992 The spatial arrangement of cones in the primate fovea Nature 360 6405 677 679 Bibcode 1992Natur 360 677M doi 10 1038 360677a0 PMID 1465131 S2CID 4234999 Wassle Heinz 11 February 1999 Colour vision A patchwork of cones Nature 397 6719 473 475 Bibcode 1999Natur 397 473W doi 10 1038 17216 PMID 10028963 S2CID 4431471 von Helmholtz Hermann 1909 Handbuch der Physiologischen Optik 3 ed Hamburg Leipzig Leopold Voss Retrieved 18 February 2020 von Helmholtz Hermann 2013 Treatise on Physiological Optics Courier Corporation ISBN 978 0486174709 Retrieved 18 February 2020 Svaetichin G 1956 Spectral response curves from single cones Acta Physiologica Scandinavica 39 134 17 46 PMID 13444020 Kandel ER Schwartz JH Jessell TM 2000 Principles of Neural Science 4th ed New York McGraw Hill pp 182 185 ISBN 978 0 8385 7701 1 Jacobs GH Nathans J March 2009 Color Vision How Our Eyes Reflect Primate Evolution Scientific American Leong Jennifer Number of Colors Distinguishable by the Human Eye hypertextbook Retrieved 21 February 2013 External links editThe Straight Dope Are cats and dogs really color blind How do they know Svaetichin Gunnar MacNichol Edward F November 1958 Retinal mechanisms for chromatic and achromatic vision Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 74 2 385 404 Bibcode 1959NYASA 74 385S doi 10 1111 j 1749 6632 1958 tb39560 x PMID 13627867 S2CID 27130943 Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Trichromacy amp oldid 1216841881, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.