fbpx
Wikipedia

Jean Berko Gleason

Jean Berko Gleason (born 1931) is an American psycholinguist and professor emerita in the Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences at Boston University[1] who has made fundamental contributions to the understanding of language acquisition in children, aphasia, gender differences in language development, and parent–child interactions.[2]

Jean Berko Gleason
Gleason in 2011
Born
Jean Berko

1931 (age 92–93)
Known forResearch in language acquisition, aphasia, and language attrition
Wug Test
Spouse
(m. 1959)
Academic background
Alma mater
Doctoral advisorRoger Brown
Academic work
DisciplineLinguist
Sub-disciplinePsycholinguistics
InstitutionsBoston University
Websitejeanberkogleason.com

Gleason created the Wug Test, in which a child is shown pictures with nonsense names and then prompted to complete statements about them, and used it to demonstrate that even young children possess implicit knowledge of linguistic morphology. Menn and Ratner have written that "Perhaps no innovation other than the invention of the tape recorder has had such an indelible effect on the field of child language research", the "wug" (one of the imaginary creatures Gleason drew in creating the Wug Test) being "so basic to what [psycholinguists] know and do that increasingly it appears in the popular literature without attribution to its origins."[2]

Biography edit

 
With Andrew Gleason, 1958

Jean Berko was born to Hungarian immigrant parents in Cleveland, Ohio.[3] As a child, she has said, "I was under the impression that whatever you said meant something in some language." Her older brother's cerebral palsy made it difficult for most people to understand his speech, but

I was the person who always understood what he said. So I felt some closeness with language as well as with my brother ... I didn't start out to study psycholinguistics; I started out to study a million languages because I love them ... Norwegian, French, Russian, bits and pieces of Arabic, German, [and] enough Spanish to get dinner.[4]

After graduating from Cleveland Heights High School in 1949, Berko Gleason (then yet Berko) earned a B.A. in history and literature from Radcliffe College, then an M.A. in linguistics, and a combined Ph.D. in linguistics and psychology, at Harvard; from 1958 to 1959 she was a postdoctoral fellow at MIT.[5] In graduate school she was advised by Roger Brown, a founder in the field of child language acquisition. In January 1959 she married Harvard mathematician Andrew Gleason; they had three daughters.[6]

Most of Berko Gleason's professional career has been at Boston University, where she served as Psychology Department chair and director of the Graduate Program in Applied Linguistics; Lise Menn and Harold Goodglass were among her collaborators there.[7]

She has been a visiting scholar at Harvard University, Stanford University, and at the Linguistics Institute of the Hungarian Academy of Sciences.[8] Although officially retired and no longer teaching, she continues to be involved in research.[9]

Gleason is the author or co-author of some 125 papers on language development in children, language attrition, aphasia, and gender and cultural aspects of language acquisition and use;[10] and is editor/​coeditor of two widely used textbooks, The Development of Language (first edition 1985, ninth edition 2016) and Psycholinguistics (1993).[8] She is a Fellow of the American Association for the Advancement of Science and of the American Psychological Association, and was president of the International Association for the Study of Child Language from 1990 to 1993, and of the Gypsy Lore Society 1996 to 1999.[11] She has also served on the editorial boards of numerous academic and professional journals and was associate editor of Language from 1997 to 1999.[5]

Gleason was profiled in Beyond the Glass Ceiling: Forty Women Whose Ideas Shape the Modern World (1996).[8][8] A festschrift in her honor, Methods for Studying Language Production, was published in 2000.[2] In 2016 she received an honorary Doctor of Science degree from Washington & Jefferson College for her work as "a pioneer in the field of psycholinguistics",[12] and in 2017 the Roger Brown Award (recognizing "outstanding contribution to the international child language community")[13] from the International Association for the Study of Child Language.[14]

Since 2007 she has delivered the "Welcome, welcome" and "Goodbye, goodbye" speeches at the annual Ig Nobel Awards ceremonies.[15]

Selected research edit

Children's learning of English morphology‍—‌the Wug Test edit

 
One of Gleason's hand-drawn panels from the original Wug Test[note 1]

Gleason devised the Wug Test as part of her earliest research (1958), which used nonsense words to gauge children's acquisition of morphological rules‍—‌for example, the "default" rule that most English plurals are formed by adding an /s/, /z/, or /ɪz/ sound depending on the final consonant, e.g. hat–hats, eye–eyes, witch–witches.[17] A child is shown simple pictures of a fanciful creature or activity,[18][19] with a nonsense name, and prompted to complete a statement about it:

This is a WUG. Now there is another one. There are two of them. There are two ________.[17]

Each "target" word was a made-up (but plausible-sounding) pseudoword, so that the child cannot have heard it before. A child who knows that the plural of witch is witches may have heard and memorized that pair, but a child responding that the plural of wug (which the child presumably has never heard) is wugs (/wʌgz/, using the /z/ allomorph since "wug" ends in a voiced consonant) has apparently inferred (perhaps unconsciously) the basic rule for forming plurals.[17][note 2]

The Wug Test also includes questions involving verb conjugations, possessives, and other common derivational morphemes such as the agentive -er (e.g. "A man who 'zibs' is a ________?"), and requested explanations of common compound words e.g. "Why is a birthday called a birthday?"[note 3] Other items included:

  • This is a dog with QUIRKS on him. He is all covered in QUIRKS. What kind of a dog is he? He is a ________ dog.
  • This is a man who knows how to SPOW. He is SPOWING. He did the same thing yesterday. What did he do yesterday? Yesterday he ________.

(The expected answers were QUIRKY and SPOWED.)[17]

Gleason's major finding was that even very young children are able to connect suitable endings‍—‌to produce plurals, past tenses, possessives, and other forms‍—‌to nonsense words they have never heard before, implying that they have internalized systematic aspects of the linguistic system which no one has necessarily tried to teach them. However, she also identified an earlier stage at which children can produce such forms for real words, but not yet for nonsense words‍—‌implying that children start by memorizing singular–plural pairs they hear spoken by others, then eventually extract rules and patterns from these examples which they apply to novel words.[17]

The Wug Test was the first experimental proof that young children have extracted generalizable rules from the language around them, rather than simply memorizing words that they have heard,[20][2]: 2  and it was almost immediately adapted for children speaking languages other than English, to bilingual children, and to children (and adults) with various impairments or from a variety of cultural backgrounds.[2]: 3  Its conclusions are viewed as essential to the understanding of when and how children reach major language milestones, and its variations and progeny remain in use worldwide for studies on language acquisition.[2]: 8  It is "almost universal" for textbooks in psycholinguistics and language acquisition to include assignments calling for the student to carry out a practical variation of the Wug Test paradigm.[2]: 7  The ubiquity of discussion of the wug test has led to the wug being used as a mascot of sorts for linguists and linguistics students.[21]

The Wug Test's fundamental role in the development of psycholinguistics as a discipline has been mapped by studying references to Gleason's work in "seminal journals" in the field, many of which carried articles referencing it in their founding issues:[2]: 4 

A review of citation lists [for Gleason's paper] over the years gives an interesting mini-view of the evolution of developmental psycholinguistics... In the first 15 years following publication, the article was extensively cited by researchers attempting to validate its utility and extend its finding to nontypical populations. Over time, however... the fact that almost any human being can do that task... became much less interesting than the question of how it is accomplished.[2]: 4 

According to Ratner and Menn, "As an enduring concept in psycholinguistic research, the wug has become generic, like [kleenex] or [xerox], a concept so basic to what we know and do that increasingly it appears in the popular literature without attribution to its origins... Perhaps no innovation other than the invention of the tape recorder has had such an indelible effect on the field of child language research."[2]: 8 

It has been proposed that Wug Test–like instruments be used in the diagnosis of learning disabilities, but in practice success in this direction has been limited.[2]: 4 

Parent–child interactions edit

Another of Gleason's early papers "Fathers and Other Strangers: Men's Speech to Young Children" (1975) explored differences between mothers' and fathers' spoken interaction with their children, primarily using data produced by two female and two male daycare teachers at a large university, and by three mothers and three fathers, mostly during family dinners. Among other conclusions, this study found that:

  • mothers used less complex constructions in speaking to their children than did fathers;
  • mothers generated lengthier and more complex constructions in speaking to their eldest child than to their younger children;
  • fathers issued significantly more commands than did mothers, along with more threats and more teasing in the way of name-calling; and
  • the fathers' language also reflected traditional gender roles in the families (such as in an example in which a father, playing a game with his son, directs the son to the mother when the need for a diaper change arises).[22]

In contrast, both male and female daycare teachers used language that was similar both quantitatively and qualitatively, with both focusing on a dialogue based in the present and on the immediate needs of the children. Differences included that the male teachers tended to address the children by name more often than did the female teachers and that the male teachers issued more imperatives than did the female teachers.[23]

Acquisition of routines in children edit

Gleason's research eventually extended into the study of children's acquisition of routines‍—‌that is, standardized chunks of language (or language-plus-gesture) that the culture expects of everyone, such as greetings, farewells, and expressions of thanks. Gleason was one of the first to study the acquisition of politeness, examining English-speaking children's use of routines such as thank you, please, and I'm sorry. Researchers in this area have since studied both verbal and non-verbal routinization, and the development of politeness routines in a variety of cultures and languages.[24]

The Halloween routine edit

Gleason's 1976 paper with Weintraub, "The Acquisition of Routines in Child Language", analyzed performance on the culturally standardized Halloween trick-or-treat routine in 115 children aged two to sixteen years. Alterations in ability and the function of parental contribution were analyzed concerning cognitive and social components. They discovered that in the acquisition of routines (in contrast to the acquisition of much of the rest of language) parents' major interest is for their children to achieve accurate performance, with little stress on children's understanding of what they are expected to say. Gleason and Weintraub found that the parents rarely if ever explain to children the meaning of such routines as Bye-bye or Trick or treat‍—‌there was no concern with the child's thoughts or intentions as long as the routine was performed as expected at the appropriate times. Thus, parents' role in the acquisition of routines is very different from their role in most of the rest of language development.[25]

"Hi, Thanks, and Goodbye" edit

External videos
  Jean Berko Gleason, Hi, Thanks, and Goodbye. NOVA's Secret Life of Scientists and Engineers

Gleason and Greif analyzed children's acquisition of three ubiquitous routines in "Hi, Thanks, and Goodbye: More Routine Information" (1980). The subjects were eleven boys and eleven girls and their parents. At the conclusion of a parent-child play period, an assistant entered the playroom bearing a present, in order to evoke routines from the children. The study's purpose was to analyze how parents communicate these routines to their children; major questions proposed included whether or not some routines were more obligatory than others, and whether mothers and fathers provide different models of politeness behavior for their children. The results suggest that children's spontaneous construction of the three routines was low, with Thank you the rarest. However, parents strongly encouraged their children to generate routines and, typically, the children complied. In addition, parents were more likely to prompt the Thank you routine than the Hi and Goodbye routines. Parents practiced the routines themselves, though mothers were more likely than fathers to speak Thank you and Goodbye to the assistant.[26]

Apologies edit

Gleason and Ely made an in-depth study of apologies in children's dialogue in their paper, "I'm sorry I said that: apologies in young children's discourse" (2006),[27] which analyzed apology term usage (in parent–child dialogue) of five boys and four girls, aged one to six years. Their research suggested that apologies appear later in children than do other politeness routines, and that as the children grew older they developed a progressively refined expertise with this routine, gradually requiring fewer direct prompts and producing more elaborate apologies instead of just saying "I'm sorry". They also found that parents and other adults play an important role in fostering growth of apologetic abilities through the setting of examples, by encouraging the children to apologize, and by speaking specifically and purposefully to them about apologies.[27]

Attention to language in family discourse edit

With Ely, MacGibbon, and Zaretsky, Gleason also explored the discourse of middle-class parents and their children at the dinner table in, "Attention to Language: Lessons Learned at the Dinner Table" (2001), finding that the everyday language of these parents involves a remarkable portion of attention to language. The dinner-table conversation of twenty-two middle-class families, each with a child between two years and five and one-half years old, were recorded, then analyzed for the existence and activity of language-centered terms, including words like ask, tell, say, and speak. Mothers spoke more about language than did fathers, and fathers spoke more about it than did children: roughly eleven percent of mothers' sentences contained one or more language-centered terms, and the corresponding proportions for fathers and children were seven percent and four percent. Uses that were metalinguistic (for example, accounting for and remarking on speech) exceeded uses that were pragmatic (for example, managing how and when speech appears).[28]

The more that mothers used language-centered terms, the more the children did as well‍—‌but this was not true for fathers. The results imply that in routine family conversations, parents supply children with considerable information on the way language is used to communicate information.[28]

Foreign-language studies edit

Gleason has carried out significant research involving the learning and maintenance of second languages by sequential bilinguals. She has studied the acquisition of a second language while retaining the first (additive bilingualism),[29] examining discourse behaviors of parents who follow the one person-one language principle by using different languages with their child[30] She has also studied language attrition, the loss of a known language through lack of use,[31] and suggests that the order in which a language is learned is less important in predicting its retention than the thoroughness with which it is learned.[32]

Psychophysiological responses to taboo words edit

An unusual study carried out with Harris and Aycicegi, "Taboo words and reprimands elicit greater autonomic reactivity in a first language than in a second language" (2003), investigated the involuntary psychophysiological reactions of bilingual speakers to taboo words. Thirty-two Turkish-English bilinguals judged the pleasantness of an array of words and phrases in Turkish (their first language), and in English (their second), while their skin conductance was monitored via fingertip electrodes. Participants manifested greater autonomic arousal in response to taboo words and childhood reprimands in their first language than to those in their second language, confirming the commonplace claim that speakers of two languages are less uncomfortable speaking taboo words and phrases in their second language than in their native language.[33]

Maintenance of first and second languages edit

In "Maintaining Foreign Language Skills", which discusses "the personal, cultural, and instructional factors involved with keeping up foreign language skills" (1988), Gleason and Pan consider both humans' remarkable capacity for language acquisition and their ability to lose it. In addition to brain damage, strokes, trauma and other physical causes of language loss, individuals may lose language skills due to the absence of a linguistically supportive social environment in which to maintain such skills, such as when a speaker of a given language relocates to a place where that language is not spoken. Culture also factors in. More often than not, individuals speaking two or more languages come into contact with one another, for reasons ranging from emigration and interrelationships to alterations in political borders. The result of such contact is typically that the community of speakers undergoes a progressive shift in usage from one language to the other.[34]

Aphasia edit

Gleason has also done significant research on aphasia, a condition (usually due to brain injury) in which a person's ability to understand and/or to produce language, including their ability to find the words they need and their use of basic morphology and syntax, is impaired in a variety of ways.[35][36]

In "Some Linguistic Structures in the Speech of a Broca's Aphasic" (1972) Gleason, Goodglass, Bernholtz, and Hyde discuss an experiment carried out with a man who, after a stroke, had been left with Broca's aphasia/agrammatism, a specific form of aphasia typically impairing the production of morphology and syntax more than it impairs comprehension. This experiment employed the Story Completion Test (often used to probe a subject's capacity for producing various common grammatical forms) as well as free conversation and repetition to elicit speech from the subject; this speech was then analyzed to evaluate how well he used inflectional morphology (e.g. plural and past tense word endings) and basic syntax (the formation of, for example, simple declarative, imperative, and interrogative sentences).[37]

To do this the investigator, in a few sentences, began a simple story about a pictured situation, then asked the subject to conclude the narrative. The stories were so designed that a non–language-impaired person's response would typically employ particular structures, for example, the plural of a noun, the past tense of a verb, or a simple but complete yes-no question (e.g. "Did you take my shoes?").[37]

Gleason, Goodglass, Bernholtz, and Hyde concluded that the transition from verb to object was easier for this subject than was the transition from subject to verb and that auxiliary verbs and verb inflections were the parts of speech most likely to be omitted by the subject. There was considerable variation among consecutive repeat trials of the same test item, although responses on successive attempts usually came closer to those a normal speaker would have produced. The study concluded that the subject's speech was not the product of a stable abnormal grammar, and could not be accounted for by assuming that he was simply omitting words to minimize his effort in producing them[37]‍—‌questions of significant theoretical controversy at the time.[38]

Selected publications edit

Papers edit

  • Berko, Jean (1958). "The Child's Learning of English Morphology". WORD. 14 (2–3): 150–177. doi:10.1080/00437956.1958.11659661. hdl:11858/00-001M-0000-002A-5DDD-2.
  • Brown, Roger; Berko, Jean (1960). "Word Association and the Acquisition of Grammar". Child Development. 31 (1): 1–14. doi:10.2307/1126377. ISSN 0009-3920. JSTOR 1126377. PMID 13805002.
  • Goodglass, Harold; Berko, Jean (1960). "Agrammatism and inflectional morphology in English". Journal of Speech and Hearing Research. 3 (3): 257–267. doi:10.1044/jshr.0303.257. PMID 13851047.
  • Goodglass, Harold; Gleason, Jean Berko; Hyde, Mary R. (1970). "Some Dimensions of Auditory Language Comprehension in Aphasia". Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 13 (3): 595–606. doi:10.1044/jshr.1303.595. ISSN 1092-4388. PMID 5528304. JSLHR Editor's Award
  • Goodglass, Harold; Gleason, Jean Berko; Bernholtz, Nancy Ackerman; Hyde, Mary R. (1972). "Some Linguistic Structures in the Speech of a Broca's Aphasic". Cortex. 8 (2): 191–212. doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(72)80018-2. ISSN 0010-9452. PMID 5043793.
  • Gleason, Jean Berko (1975). "Fathers and other strangers: Men's speech to young children". In Daniel Peter Dato (ed.). Developmental psycholinguistics: theory and applications (26th Annual Roundtable). Georgetown University Press. pp. 289–297. ISBN 978-0-87840-110-9.
  • Gleason, Jean Berko; Goodglass, Harold; Green, Eugene; Ackerman, Nancy; Hyde, Mary R. (1975). "The retrieval of syntax in Broca's aphasia". Brain and Language. 2 (4): 451–471. doi:10.1016/S0093-934X(75)80083-6. ISSN 0093-934X. PMID 1218378. S2CID 569044.
  • Gleason, Jean Berko; Goodglass, Harold; Obler, Loraine; Green, Eugene; Hyde, Mary R.; Weintraub, Sandra (1980). "Narrative Strategies of Aphasic and Normal-Speaking Subjects". Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 23 (2): 370–382. doi:10.1044/jshr.2302.370. ISSN 1092-4388. PMID 7442197.
  • Bellinger, David C.; Gleason, Jean Berko (1982). "Sex differences in parental directives to young children". Sex Roles. 8 (11): 1123–1139. doi:10.1007/BF00290968. ISSN 0360-0025. S2CID 144841855.
  • Gleason, Jean Berko (2003). "Language Acquisition: Is it Like Learning to Walk, or Learning to Dance?". Contemporary Psychology. 48 (2): 172–174. doi:10.1037/000751. ISSN 1554-0138.
  • Harris, Catherine L.; Ayçiçegi, Ayse; Gleason, Jean Berko (2003). "Taboo words and reprimands elicit greater autonomic reactivity in a first language than in a second language". Applied Psycholinguistics. 24 (4): 561–579. doi:10.1017/S0142716403000286. ISSN 0142-7164. S2CID 7219049.
  • Gleason, Jean Berko; Weintraub, Sandra (2008). "The acquisition of routines in child language". Language in Society. 5 (2): 129–136. doi:10.1017/S0047404500006977. ISSN 0047-4045. S2CID 146769318.
  • Greif, Esther Blank; Gleason, Jean Berko (2008). "Hi, thanks, and goodbye: More routine information". Language in Society. 9 (2): 159–166. doi:10.1017/S0047404500008034. ISSN 0047-4045. S2CID 145771682.
  • Pan, Barbara Alexander; Gleason, Jean Berko (2008). "The study of language loss: Models and hypotheses for an emerging discipline". Applied Psycholinguistics. 7 (3): 193–206. doi:10.1017/S0142716400007530. ISSN 0142-7164. S2CID 145114776.

Book chapters edit

  • Berko, Jean; Brown, Roger (1960). "Psycholinguistic research methods". In Paul H. Mussen (ed.). Handbook of research methods in child development. Wiley. pp. 517–557.
  • Gleason, Jean Berko (1973). "Code switching in children's language". In Timothy E. Moore (ed.). Cognitive Development and Acquisition of Language. Academic Press. pp. 159–167. Reprinted as Timothy E. Moore, ed. (2014). Cognitive Development and Acquisition of Language. Elsevier. ISBN 978-1-4832-9456-8.
  • Gleason, Jean Berko (1977). "Talking to children: Some notes on feedback". In Snow, Catherine E.; Ferguson, Charles A. (eds.). Talking to Children: Language Input and Acquisition. Cambridge University Press. pp. 199–205. ISBN 978-0-521-21318-9.
  • Gleason, Jean Berko; Weintraub, Sandra (1978). "Input Language and the Acquisition of Communicative Competence". In Keith E. Nelson (ed.). Children's Language. Gardner Press. pp. 171–222. ISBN 978-0-470-99385-9.
  • Gleason, Jean Berko (1987). "Sex differences in parent-child interaction". In Susan U. Philips; Susan Steele; Christine Tanz (eds.). Language, Gender, and Sex in Comparative Perspective. Cambridge University Press. pp. 189–199. ISBN 978-0-521-33807-3.
  • Harris, Catherine L.; Gleason, Jean Berko; Ayçiçegi, Ayse (2006). "When is a first language more emotional? Psychophysiological evidence from bilingual speakers.". In Aneta Pavlenko (ed.). Bilingual Minds: Emotional Experience, Expression, and Representation. Multilingual Matters. pp. 257–283. ISBN 978-1-84769-981-7.
  • Gleason, Jean Berko (2016). "The Development of Language: An Overview and Preview". In Gleason, Jean Berko; Ratner, Nan Bernstein (eds.). The Development of Language (9th ed.). Pearson Education. pp. 1–25. ISBN 978-0-13-416114-3.

Textbooks edit

  • Gleason, Jean Berko, ed. (1985). The Development of Language. Charles E. Merrill. ISBN 978-0-675-20222-0. Latest edition: Gleason, Jean Berko; Ratner, Nan Bernstein, eds. (2023). The Development of Language (10th ed.). Plural Publishing. ISBN 978-1-63550-426-2.
  • Gleason, Jean Berko; Ratner, Nan Bernstein, eds. (1993). Psycholinguistics. Harcourt, Brace. ISBN 978-0030559648.Latest edition: Gleason, Jean Berko; Ratner, Nan Bernstein, eds. (1998). Psycholinguistics (2nd ed.). Harcourt, Brace. ISBN 978-0155041066.

Notes edit

  1. ^ The drawings used in the actual tests are colored,[clarification needed][16] though the example drawing in the paper reporting the research was printed in black and white.[17]: 153-4 
  2. ^ Very young children are baffled by the question, sometimes responding with two wug. Preschoolers ages 4 to 5 test best in dealing with /z/ after a voiced consonant, and generally say that there are two wugs, with a /z/; they do almost as well with the voiceless /s/. They do less well in dealing with /z/ in other environments such as after nasals, rhotics, and vowels. Children in the first year of primary school were almost fully competent with both /s/ and /z/. Both preschool and first-grade children dealt poorly with /ɪz/, giving the correct answer less than half the time, possibly because it occurs in the most restrictive context. Also, because the root of the test word often ended in /s/ in these cases, the children may have assumed that the word was already in its plural form. Even though the children were all able to produce the real plural "glasses" they generally responded two "tass" rather than two "tasses" when shown more than one nonsense creature called a "tass".[17]
  3. ^ [17] Preschoolers tend to form compounds rather than agentives e.g. a man whose job it is to "zib" is a zibman, and often explain compound words in terms of their cultural, rather than linguistic, features e.g. a birthday is called birthday because one receives presents.

References edit

  1. ^ "Jean Berko Gleason, PhD Professor Emerita". Retrieved January 28, 2013.
  2. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k Lise Menn; Nan Bernstein Ratner (2000). "In the Beginning Was the Wug". In Lise Menn; Nan Bernstein Ratner (eds.). Methods for Studying Language Production. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 1–26. ISBN 978-0-8058-3033-0.
  3. ^ "Jean Berko Gleason — Unfolding Language, Unfolding Life". The On Being Project. Retrieved March 8, 2021.
  4. ^ PBS Newshour. Jean Berko Gleason's Brief But Spectacular Take on language. January 2, 2020.
  5. ^ a b Jean Berko Gleason – Curriculum Vitae (PDF)
  6. ^ Bolker, Ethan D. (2009). "Andrew M. Gleason (1921–2008)" (PDF). Notices of the American Mathematical Society. 56 (10): 1236.
  7. ^ Jean Berko Gleason Boston University CV (PDF), April 2019
  8. ^ a b c d Sian Griffiths; Helena Kennedy, eds. (1996). Beyond the Glass Ceiling: Forty Women Whose Ideas Shape the Modern World. Manchester University Press. ISBN 978-0-7190-4773-2.
  9. ^ Skarabela, Barbora (July 2006). . IASCL – Child Language Bulletin. 26 (1). Archived from the original on January 3, 2019. Retrieved July 4, 2014.
  10. ^ "Jean Berko Gleason – Biographical Summary". March 2, 2014.
  11. ^ Gleason, Jean Berko; Ratner, Nan Bernstein, eds. (2009). The development of language (7th ed.). Boston: Pearson. ISBN 9780205593033. Retrieved July 1, 2014.
  12. ^ "WJ Honors Four Extraordinary Individuals During Commencement Ceremony". www.washjeff.edu. May 23, 2016. Retrieved June 2, 2016.
  13. ^ "International Association for the Study of Child Language. Roger Brown Award". Retrieved September 2, 2017.
  14. ^ Kern, Sophie (August 2017). "Report on IASCL 2017" (PDF). IASCL – Child Language Bulletin. 37 (11).
  15. ^ "Strange News: Silly Science Honored With Ig Nobel Prizes". NPR. November 26, 2010.
  16. ^ Gleason, Jean Berko (2019). The Wug Test. Larchwood Press. ISBN 978-1-73403-890-3.
  17. ^ a b c d e f g h Berko, Jean (1958). (PDF). Word. 14 (2–3): 150–177. doi:10.1080/00437956.1958.11659661. hdl:11858/00-001M-0000-002A-5DDD-2. Archived from the original (PDF) on March 7, 2016.
  18. ^ Topics in Language Acquisition
  19. ^ Balota, David A.; Marsh, Elizabeth J., eds. (2004). Cognitive psychology: key readings. Hove ; New York, N.Y.: Psychology press. p. 526. ISBN 978-1841690650.
  20. ^ Karmiloff, Kyra; Karmiloff-Smith, Annette (2001), Pathways to Language: From Fetus to Adolescent, Harvard University Press, ISBN 9780674008359
  21. ^ "This is a wug. And if there were two, there would..." Superlinguo. Retrieved August 29, 2019.
  22. ^ Gleason, Jean Berko. (1975) "Fathers and other strangers: men's speech to young children". In Daniel P. Dato, ed., Developmental Psycholinguistics: Theory and Applications. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.
  23. ^ Gleason, Jean Berko (1975). "Fathers and Other Strangers: Men's speech to Young Children". 26th Annual Roundtable: 289–297.
  24. ^ Matthews, Danielle, ed. (2014). Pragmatic development in first language acquisition. Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins Publishing Company. p. 324. ISBN 978-9027234704.
  25. ^ Gleason, Jean Berko; Weintraub, Sandra (1976). "The acquisition of routines in child language". Language in Society. 5 (2): 129–136. doi:10.1017/s0047404500006977. S2CID 146769318.
  26. ^ Greif, Esther Blank; Gleason, Jean Berko (1980). "Hi, Thanks, and Goodbye: More Routine Information". Language in Society. 9 (2): 159–166. doi:10.1017/s0047404500008034. JSTOR 4167137. S2CID 145771682.
  27. ^ a b Gleason, Jean Berko; Ely, Richard (2006). "I'm sorry I said that: apologies in young children's discourse". Journal of Child Language. 33 (3): 33, 599–620. doi:10.1017/S0305000906007446. PMID 17017280. S2CID 31823853.
  28. ^ a b Ely, Richard; Jean Berko Gleason; Ann MacGibbon; Elena Zaretsky (2001). "Attention to Language: Lessons Learned at the Dinner Table". Social Development. 10 (3): 355–373. doi:10.1111/1467-9507.00170.
  29. ^ Duarte, Joana (2010). Bilingual language proficiency. Munster: Waxmann. p. 32. ISBN 9783830923176.
  30. ^ Halliday, M.A.K., ed. (1990). Learning, Keeping and Using Language. Amsterdam: Benjamins. ISBN 9781556191046.
  31. ^ Clark, Eve V. (2003). First language acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 9780521629973.
  32. ^ Schmid, Monika S. (2002). First language attrition, use and maintenance: the case of German Jews in anglophone countries. Amsterdam: J. Benjamins. p. 13. ISBN 9789027241351.
  33. ^ Harris, Catherine; Ayşe Ayçiçeĝi; Jean Berko Gleason (2003). "Taboo Words and Reprimands Elicit Greater Autonomic Reactivity in a First than in a Second Language". Applied Psycholinguistics. 24 (4). doi:10.1017/S0142716403000286. S2CID 7219049.
  34. ^ Gleason, Jean Berko; Pan (1988). "Maintaining Foreign Language Skills". You CAN Take It with You: 1–22.
  35. ^ Menn, Lise, ed. (1990). Agrammatic aphasia a cross-language narrative sourcebook. Amsterdam: J. Benjamins. ISBN 9789027273512.
  36. ^ Rosenberg, Sheldon (1982). Handbook of applied psycholinguistics. Hillsdale, N.J.: L. Erlbaum Associates. ISBN 9780898591736.
  37. ^ a b c Goodglass Harold; Jean Berko Gleason; Nancy Ackerman Bernholtz; Mary R. Hyde (1972). "Some Linguistic Structures in the Speech of a Broca's Aphasic". Cortex. 8 (2): 191–212. doi:10.1016/s0010-9452(72)80018-2. PMID 5043793.
  38. ^ Miceli, Gabriele (1998). Gianfranco Denes; Luigi Pizzamiglio (eds.). "Grammatical deficits in aphasia". Handbook of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology.

External links edit

  • Curriculum vitae November 18, 2016, at the Wayback Machine
  • Official website
  • Departmental page
  • Nova/PBS: The Secret Life of Scientists and Engineers – Jean Berko Gleason – Psycholinguist/Speed Demon
  • Gleason discusses her education, career, and the Wug Test
  • On Being – Jean Berko Gleason – Unfolding Language, Unfolding Life
  • Brief video of Gleason administering a Wug test. With Stephen Fry.

jean, berko, gleason, born, 1931, american, psycholinguist, professor, emerita, department, psychological, brain, sciences, boston, university, made, fundamental, contributions, understanding, language, acquisition, children, aphasia, gender, differences, lang. Jean Berko Gleason born 1931 is an American psycholinguist and professor emerita in the Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences at Boston University 1 who has made fundamental contributions to the understanding of language acquisition in children aphasia gender differences in language development and parent child interactions 2 Jean Berko GleasonGleason in 2011BornJean Berko1931 age 92 93 Cleveland OhioKnown forResearch in language acquisition aphasia and language attrition Wug TestSpouseAndrew M Gleason m 1959 wbr Academic backgroundAlma materRadcliffe College A B A M Harvard University Ph D Doctoral advisorRoger BrownAcademic workDisciplineLinguistSub disciplinePsycholinguisticsInstitutionsBoston UniversityWebsitejeanberkogleason wbr com Gleason created the Wug Test in which a child is shown pictures with nonsense names and then prompted to complete statements about them and used it to demonstrate that even young children possess implicit knowledge of linguistic morphology Menn and Ratner have written that Perhaps no innovation other than the invention of the tape recorder has had such an indelible effect on the field of child language research the wug one of the imaginary creatures Gleason drew in creating the Wug Test being so basic to what psycholinguists know and do that increasingly it appears in the popular literature without attribution to its origins 2 Contents 1 Biography 2 Selected research 2 1 Children s learning of English morphology the Wug Test 2 2 Parent child interactions 2 3 Acquisition of routines in children 2 3 1 The Halloween routine 2 3 2 Hi Thanks and Goodbye 2 3 3 Apologies 2 3 4 Attention to language in family discourse 2 4 Foreign language studies 2 4 1 Psychophysiological responses to taboo words 2 4 2 Maintenance of first and second languages 2 5 Aphasia 3 Selected publications 3 1 Papers 3 2 Book chapters 3 3 Textbooks 4 Notes 5 References 6 External linksBiography edit nbsp With Andrew Gleason 1958 Jean Berko was born to Hungarian immigrant parents in Cleveland Ohio 3 As a child she has said I was under the impression that whatever you said meant something in some language Her older brother s cerebral palsy made it difficult for most people to understand his speech but I was the person who always understood what he said So I felt some closeness with language as well as with my brother I didn t start out to study psycholinguistics I started out to study a million languages because I love them Norwegian French Russian bits and pieces of Arabic German and enough Spanish to get dinner 4 After graduating from Cleveland Heights High School in 1949 Berko Gleason then yet Berko earned a B A in history and literature from Radcliffe College then an M A in linguistics and a combined Ph D in linguistics and psychology at Harvard from 1958 to 1959 she was a postdoctoral fellow at MIT 5 In graduate school she was advised by Roger Brown a founder in the field of child language acquisition In January 1959 she married Harvard mathematician Andrew Gleason they had three daughters 6 Most of Berko Gleason s professional career has been at Boston University where she served as Psychology Department chair and director of the Graduate Program in Applied Linguistics Lise Menn and Harold Goodglass were among her collaborators there 7 She has been a visiting scholar at Harvard University Stanford University and at the Linguistics Institute of the Hungarian Academy of Sciences 8 Although officially retired and no longer teaching she continues update to be involved in research 9 Gleason is the author or co author of some 125 papers on language development in children language attrition aphasia and gender and cultural aspects of language acquisition and use 10 and is editor coeditor of two widely used textbooks The Development of Language first edition 1985 ninth edition 2016 and Psycholinguistics 1993 8 She is a Fellow of the American Association for the Advancement of Science and of the American Psychological Association and was president of the International Association for the Study of Child Language from 1990 to 1993 and of the Gypsy Lore Society 1996 to 1999 11 She has also served on the editorial boards of numerous academic and professional journals and was associate editor of Language from 1997 to 1999 5 Gleason was profiled in Beyond the Glass Ceiling Forty Women Whose Ideas Shape the Modern World 1996 8 8 A festschrift in her honor Methods for Studying Language Production was published in 2000 2 In 2016 she received an honorary Doctor of Science degree from Washington amp Jefferson College for her work as a pioneer in the field of psycholinguistics 12 and in 2017 the Roger Brown Award recognizing outstanding contribution to the international child language community 13 from the International Association for the Study of Child Language 14 Since 2007 she has delivered the Welcome welcome and Goodbye goodbye speeches at the annual Ig Nobel Awards ceremonies 15 Selected research editChildren s learning of English morphology the Wug Test edit nbsp One of Gleason s hand drawn panels from the original Wug Test note 1 Gleason devised the Wug Test as part of her earliest research 1958 which used nonsense words to gauge children s acquisition of morphological rules for example the default rule that most English plurals are formed by adding an s z or ɪz sound depending on the final consonant e g hat hats eye eyes witch witches 17 A child is shown simple pictures of a fanciful creature or activity 18 19 with a nonsense name and prompted to complete a statement about it This is a WUG Now there is another one There are two of them There are two 17 Each target word was a made up but plausible sounding pseudoword so that the child cannot have heard it before A child who knows that the plural of witch is witches may have heard and memorized that pair but a child responding that the plural of wug which the child presumably has never heard is wugs wʌgz using the z allomorph since wug ends in a voiced consonant has apparently inferred perhaps unconsciously the basic rule for forming plurals 17 note 2 The Wug Test also includes questions involving verb conjugations possessives and other common derivational morphemes such as the agentive er e g A man who zibs is a and requested explanations of common compound words e g Why is a birthday called a birthday note 3 Other items included This is a dog with QUIRKS on him He is all covered in QUIRKS What kind of a dog is he He is a dog This is a man who knows how to SPOW He is SPOWING He did the same thing yesterday What did he do yesterday Yesterday he The expected answers were QUIRKY and SPOWED 17 Gleason s major finding was that even very young children are able to connect suitable endings to produce plurals past tenses possessives and other forms to nonsense words they have never heard before implying that they have internalized systematic aspects of the linguistic system which no one has necessarily tried to teach them However she also identified an earlier stage at which children can produce such forms for real words but not yet for nonsense words implying that children start by memorizing singular plural pairs they hear spoken by others then eventually extract rules and patterns from these examples which they apply to novel words 17 The Wug Test was the first experimental proof that young children have extracted generalizable rules from the language around them rather than simply memorizing words that they have heard 20 2 2 and it was almost immediately adapted for children speaking languages other than English to bilingual children and to children and adults with various impairments or from a variety of cultural backgrounds 2 3 Its conclusions are viewed as essential to the understanding of when and how children reach major language milestones and its variations and progeny remain in use worldwide for studies on language acquisition 2 8 It is almost universal for textbooks in psycholinguistics and language acquisition to include assignments calling for the student to carry out a practical variation of the Wug Test paradigm 2 7 The ubiquity of discussion of the wug test has led to the wug being used as a mascot of sorts for linguists and linguistics students 21 The Wug Test s fundamental role in the development of psycholinguistics as a discipline has been mapped by studying references to Gleason s work in seminal journals in the field many of which carried articles referencing it in their founding issues 2 4 A review of citation lists for Gleason s paper over the years gives an interesting mini view of the evolution of developmental psycholinguistics In the first 15 years following publication the article was extensively cited by researchers attempting to validate its utility and extend its finding to nontypical populations Over time however the fact that almost any human being can do that task became much less interesting than the question of how it is accomplished 2 4 According to Ratner and Menn As an enduring concept in psycholinguistic research the wug has become generic like kleenex or xerox a concept so basic to what we know and do that increasingly it appears in the popular literature without attribution to its origins Perhaps no innovation other than the invention of the tape recorder has had such an indelible effect on the field of child language research 2 8 It has been proposed that Wug Test like instruments be used in the diagnosis of learning disabilities but in practice success in this direction has been limited 2 4 Parent child interactions edit Another of Gleason s early papers Fathers and Other Strangers Men s Speech to Young Children 1975 explored differences between mothers and fathers spoken interaction with their children primarily using data produced by two female and two male daycare teachers at a large university and by three mothers and three fathers mostly during family dinners Among other conclusions this study found that mothers used less complex constructions in speaking to their children than did fathers mothers generated lengthier and more complex constructions in speaking to their eldest child than to their younger children fathers issued significantly more commands than did mothers along with more threats and more teasing in the way of name calling and the fathers language also reflected traditional gender roles in the families such as in an example in which a father playing a game with his son directs the son to the mother when the need for a diaper change arises 22 In contrast both male and female daycare teachers used language that was similar both quantitatively and qualitatively with both focusing on a dialogue based in the present and on the immediate needs of the children Differences included that the male teachers tended to address the children by name more often than did the female teachers and that the male teachers issued more imperatives than did the female teachers 23 Acquisition of routines in children edit Gleason s research eventually extended into the study of children s acquisition of routines that is standardized chunks of language or language plus gesture that the culture expects of everyone such as greetings farewells and expressions of thanks Gleason was one of the first to study the acquisition of politeness examining English speaking children s use of routines such as thank you please and I m sorry Researchers in this area have since studied both verbal and non verbal routinization and the development of politeness routines in a variety of cultures and languages 24 The Halloween routine edit Gleason s 1976 paper with Weintraub The Acquisition of Routines in Child Language analyzed performance on the culturally standardized Halloween trick or treat routine in 115 children aged two to sixteen years Alterations in ability and the function of parental contribution were analyzed concerning cognitive and social components They discovered that in the acquisition of routines in contrast to the acquisition of much of the rest of language parents major interest is for their children to achieve accurate performance with little stress on children s understanding of what they are expected to say Gleason and Weintraub found that the parents rarely if ever explain to children the meaning of such routines as Bye bye or Trick or treat there was no concern with the child s thoughts or intentions as long as the routine was performed as expected at the appropriate times Thus parents role in the acquisition of routines is very different from their role in most of the rest of language development 25 Hi Thanks and Goodbye edit External videos nbsp Jean Berko Gleason Hi Thanks and Goodbye NOVA s Secret Life of Scientists and Engineers Gleason and Greif analyzed children s acquisition of three ubiquitous routines in Hi Thanks and Goodbye More Routine Information 1980 The subjects were eleven boys and eleven girls and their parents At the conclusion of a parent child play period an assistant entered the playroom bearing a present in order to evoke routines from the children The study s purpose was to analyze how parents communicate these routines to their children major questions proposed included whether or not some routines were more obligatory than others and whether mothers and fathers provide different models of politeness behavior for their children The results suggest that children s spontaneous construction of the three routines was low with Thank you the rarest However parents strongly encouraged their children to generate routines and typically the children complied In addition parents were more likely to prompt the Thank you routine than the Hi and Goodbye routines Parents practiced the routines themselves though mothers were more likely than fathers to speak Thank you and Goodbye to the assistant 26 Apologies edit Gleason and Ely made an in depth study of apologies in children s dialogue in their paper I m sorry I said that apologies in young children s discourse 2006 27 which analyzed apology term usage in parent child dialogue of five boys and four girls aged one to six years Their research suggested that apologies appear later in children than do other politeness routines and that as the children grew older they developed a progressively refined expertise with this routine gradually requiring fewer direct prompts and producing more elaborate apologies instead of just saying I m sorry They also found that parents and other adults play an important role in fostering growth of apologetic abilities through the setting of examples by encouraging the children to apologize and by speaking specifically and purposefully to them about apologies 27 Attention to language in family discourse edit With Ely MacGibbon and Zaretsky Gleason also explored the discourse of middle class parents and their children at the dinner table in Attention to Language Lessons Learned at the Dinner Table 2001 finding that the everyday language of these parents involves a remarkable portion of attention to language The dinner table conversation of twenty two middle class families each with a child between two years and five and one half years old were recorded then analyzed for the existence and activity of language centered terms including words like ask tell say and speak Mothers spoke more about language than did fathers and fathers spoke more about it than did children roughly eleven percent of mothers sentences contained one or more language centered terms and the corresponding proportions for fathers and children were seven percent and four percent Uses that were metalinguistic for example accounting for and remarking on speech exceeded uses that were pragmatic for example managing how and when speech appears 28 The more that mothers used language centered terms the more the children did as well but this was not true for fathers The results imply that in routine family conversations parents supply children with considerable information on the way language is used to communicate information 28 Foreign language studies edit Gleason has carried out significant research involving the learning and maintenance of second languages by sequential bilinguals She has studied the acquisition of a second language while retaining the first additive bilingualism 29 examining discourse behaviors of parents who follow the one person one language principle by using different languages with their child 30 She has also studied language attrition the loss of a known language through lack of use 31 and suggests that the order in which a language is learned is less important in predicting its retention than the thoroughness with which it is learned 32 Psychophysiological responses to taboo words edit An unusual study carried out with Harris and Aycicegi Taboo words and reprimands elicit greater autonomic reactivity in a first language than in a second language 2003 investigated the involuntary psychophysiological reactions of bilingual speakers to taboo words Thirty two Turkish English bilinguals judged the pleasantness of an array of words and phrases in Turkish their first language and in English their second while their skin conductance was monitored via fingertip electrodes Participants manifested greater autonomic arousal in response to taboo words and childhood reprimands in their first language than to those in their second language confirming the commonplace claim that speakers of two languages are less uncomfortable speaking taboo words and phrases in their second language than in their native language 33 Maintenance of first and second languages edit In Maintaining Foreign Language Skills which discusses the personal cultural and instructional factors involved with keeping up foreign language skills 1988 Gleason and Pan consider both humans remarkable capacity for language acquisition and their ability to lose it In addition to brain damage strokes trauma and other physical causes of language loss individuals may lose language skills due to the absence of a linguistically supportive social environment in which to maintain such skills such as when a speaker of a given language relocates to a place where that language is not spoken Culture also factors in More often than not individuals speaking two or more languages come into contact with one another for reasons ranging from emigration and interrelationships to alterations in political borders The result of such contact is typically that the community of speakers undergoes a progressive shift in usage from one language to the other 34 Aphasia edit Gleason has also done significant research on aphasia a condition usually due to brain injury in which a person s ability to understand and or to produce language including their ability to find the words they need and their use of basic morphology and syntax is impaired in a variety of ways 35 36 In Some Linguistic Structures in the Speech of a Broca s Aphasic 1972 Gleason Goodglass Bernholtz and Hyde discuss an experiment carried out with a man who after a stroke had been left with Broca s aphasia agrammatism a specific form of aphasia typically impairing the production of morphology and syntax more than it impairs comprehension This experiment employed the Story Completion Test often used to probe a subject s capacity for producing various common grammatical forms as well as free conversation and repetition to elicit speech from the subject this speech was then analyzed to evaluate how well he used inflectional morphology e g plural and past tense word endings and basic syntax the formation of for example simple declarative imperative and interrogative sentences 37 To do this the investigator in a few sentences began a simple story about a pictured situation then asked the subject to conclude the narrative The stories were so designed that a non language impaired person s response would typically employ particular structures for example the plural of a noun the past tense of a verb or a simple but complete yes no question e g Did you take my shoes 37 Gleason Goodglass Bernholtz and Hyde concluded that the transition from verb to object was easier for this subject than was the transition from subject to verb and that auxiliary verbs and verb inflections were the parts of speech most likely to be omitted by the subject There was considerable variation among consecutive repeat trials of the same test item although responses on successive attempts usually came closer to those a normal speaker would have produced The study concluded that the subject s speech was not the product of a stable abnormal grammar and could not be accounted for by assuming that he was simply omitting words to minimize his effort in producing them 37 questions of significant theoretical controversy at the time 38 Selected publications editPapers edit Berko Jean 1958 The Child s Learning of English Morphology WORD 14 2 3 150 177 doi 10 1080 00437956 1958 11659661 hdl 11858 00 001M 0000 002A 5DDD 2 Brown Roger Berko Jean 1960 Word Association and the Acquisition of Grammar Child Development 31 1 1 14 doi 10 2307 1126377 ISSN 0009 3920 JSTOR 1126377 PMID 13805002 Goodglass Harold Berko Jean 1960 Agrammatism and inflectional morphology in English Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 3 3 257 267 doi 10 1044 jshr 0303 257 PMID 13851047 Goodglass Harold Gleason Jean Berko Hyde Mary R 1970 Some Dimensions of Auditory Language Comprehension in Aphasia Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research 13 3 595 606 doi 10 1044 jshr 1303 595 ISSN 1092 4388 PMID 5528304 JSLHR Editor s Award Goodglass Harold Gleason Jean Berko Bernholtz Nancy Ackerman Hyde Mary R 1972 Some Linguistic Structures in the Speech of a Broca s Aphasic Cortex 8 2 191 212 doi 10 1016 S0010 9452 72 80018 2 ISSN 0010 9452 PMID 5043793 Gleason Jean Berko 1975 Fathers and other strangers Men s speech to young children In Daniel Peter Dato ed Developmental psycholinguistics theory and applications 26th Annual Roundtable Georgetown University Press pp 289 297 ISBN 978 0 87840 110 9 Gleason Jean Berko Goodglass Harold Green Eugene Ackerman Nancy Hyde Mary R 1975 The retrieval of syntax in Broca s aphasia Brain and Language 2 4 451 471 doi 10 1016 S0093 934X 75 80083 6 ISSN 0093 934X PMID 1218378 S2CID 569044 Gleason Jean Berko Goodglass Harold Obler Loraine Green Eugene Hyde Mary R Weintraub Sandra 1980 Narrative Strategies of Aphasic and Normal Speaking Subjects Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research 23 2 370 382 doi 10 1044 jshr 2302 370 ISSN 1092 4388 PMID 7442197 Bellinger David C Gleason Jean Berko 1982 Sex differences in parental directives to young children Sex Roles 8 11 1123 1139 doi 10 1007 BF00290968 ISSN 0360 0025 S2CID 144841855 Gleason Jean Berko 2003 Language Acquisition Is it Like Learning to Walk or Learning to Dance Contemporary Psychology 48 2 172 174 doi 10 1037 000751 ISSN 1554 0138 Harris Catherine L Aycicegi Ayse Gleason Jean Berko 2003 Taboo words and reprimands elicit greater autonomic reactivity in a first language than in a second language Applied Psycholinguistics 24 4 561 579 doi 10 1017 S0142716403000286 ISSN 0142 7164 S2CID 7219049 Gleason Jean Berko Weintraub Sandra 2008 The acquisition of routines in child language Language in Society 5 2 129 136 doi 10 1017 S0047404500006977 ISSN 0047 4045 S2CID 146769318 Greif Esther Blank Gleason Jean Berko 2008 Hi thanks and goodbye More routine information Language in Society 9 2 159 166 doi 10 1017 S0047404500008034 ISSN 0047 4045 S2CID 145771682 Pan Barbara Alexander Gleason Jean Berko 2008 The study of language loss Models and hypotheses for an emerging discipline Applied Psycholinguistics 7 3 193 206 doi 10 1017 S0142716400007530 ISSN 0142 7164 S2CID 145114776 Book chapters edit Berko Jean Brown Roger 1960 Psycholinguistic research methods In Paul H Mussen ed Handbook of research methods in child development Wiley pp 517 557 Gleason Jean Berko 1973 Code switching in children s language In Timothy E Moore ed Cognitive Development and Acquisition of Language Academic Press pp 159 167 Reprinted as Timothy E Moore ed 2014 Cognitive Development and Acquisition of Language Elsevier ISBN 978 1 4832 9456 8 Gleason Jean Berko 1977 Talking to children Some notes on feedback In Snow Catherine E Ferguson Charles A eds Talking to Children Language Input and Acquisition Cambridge University Press pp 199 205 ISBN 978 0 521 21318 9 Gleason Jean Berko Weintraub Sandra 1978 Input Language and the Acquisition of Communicative Competence In Keith E Nelson ed Children s Language Gardner Press pp 171 222 ISBN 978 0 470 99385 9 Gleason Jean Berko 1987 Sex differences in parent child interaction In Susan U Philips Susan Steele Christine Tanz eds Language Gender and Sex in Comparative Perspective Cambridge University Press pp 189 199 ISBN 978 0 521 33807 3 Harris Catherine L Gleason Jean Berko Aycicegi Ayse 2006 When is a first language more emotional Psychophysiological evidence from bilingual speakers In Aneta Pavlenko ed Bilingual Minds Emotional Experience Expression and Representation Multilingual Matters pp 257 283 ISBN 978 1 84769 981 7 Gleason Jean Berko 2016 The Development of Language An Overview and Preview In Gleason Jean Berko Ratner Nan Bernstein eds The Development of Language 9th ed Pearson Education pp 1 25 ISBN 978 0 13 416114 3 Textbooks edit Gleason Jean Berko ed 1985 The Development of Language Charles E Merrill ISBN 978 0 675 20222 0 Latest edition Gleason Jean Berko Ratner Nan Bernstein eds 2023 The Development of Language 10th ed Plural Publishing ISBN 978 1 63550 426 2 Gleason Jean Berko Ratner Nan Bernstein eds 1993 Psycholinguistics Harcourt Brace ISBN 978 0030559648 Latest edition Gleason Jean Berko Ratner Nan Bernstein eds 1998 Psycholinguistics 2nd ed Harcourt Brace ISBN 978 0155041066 Notes edit The drawings used in the actual tests are colored clarification needed 16 though the example drawing in the paper reporting the research was printed in black and white 17 153 4 Very young children are baffled by the question sometimes responding with two wug Preschoolers ages 4 to 5 test best in dealing with z after a voiced consonant and generally say that there are two wugs with a z they do almost as well with the voiceless s They do less well in dealing with z in other environments such as after nasals rhotics and vowels Children in the first year of primary school were almost fully competent with both s and z Both preschool and first grade children dealt poorly with ɪz giving the correct answer less than half the time possibly because it occurs in the most restrictive context Also because the root of the test word often ended in s in these cases the children may have assumed that the word was already in its plural form Even though the children were all able to produce the real plural glasses they generally responded two tass rather than two tasses when shown more than one nonsense creature called a tass 17 17 Preschoolers tend to form compounds rather than agentives e g a man whose job it is to zib is a zibman and often explain compound words in terms of their cultural rather than linguistic features e g a birthday is called birthday because one receives presents References edit Jean Berko Gleason PhD Professor Emerita Retrieved January 28 2013 a b c d e f g h i j k Lise Menn Nan Bernstein Ratner 2000 In the Beginning Was the Wug In Lise Menn Nan Bernstein Ratner eds Methods for Studying Language Production Lawrence Erlbaum Associates pp 1 26 ISBN 978 0 8058 3033 0 Jean Berko Gleason Unfolding Language Unfolding Life The On Being Project Retrieved March 8 2021 PBS Newshour Jean Berko Gleason s Brief But Spectacular Take on language January 2 2020 a b Jean Berko Gleason Curriculum Vitae PDF Bolker Ethan D 2009 Andrew M Gleason 1921 2008 PDF Notices of the American Mathematical Society 56 10 1236 Jean Berko Gleason Boston University CV PDF April 2019 a b c d Sian Griffiths Helena Kennedy eds 1996 Beyond the Glass Ceiling Forty Women Whose Ideas Shape the Modern World Manchester University Press ISBN 978 0 7190 4773 2 Skarabela Barbora July 2006 An interview with Jean Berko Gleason IASCL Child Language Bulletin 26 1 Archived from the original on January 3 2019 Retrieved July 4 2014 Jean Berko Gleason Biographical Summary March 2 2014 Gleason Jean Berko Ratner Nan Bernstein eds 2009 The development of language 7th ed Boston Pearson ISBN 9780205593033 Retrieved July 1 2014 WJ Honors Four Extraordinary Individuals During Commencement Ceremony www washjeff edu May 23 2016 Retrieved June 2 2016 International Association for the Study of Child Language Roger Brown Award Retrieved September 2 2017 Kern Sophie August 2017 Report on IASCL 2017 PDF IASCL Child Language Bulletin 37 11 Strange News Silly Science Honored With Ig Nobel Prizes NPR November 26 2010 Gleason Jean Berko 2019 The Wug Test Larchwood Press ISBN 978 1 73403 890 3 a b c d e f g h Berko Jean 1958 The Child s Learning of English Morphology PDF Word 14 2 3 150 177 doi 10 1080 00437956 1958 11659661 hdl 11858 00 001M 0000 002A 5DDD 2 Archived from the original PDF on March 7 2016 Topics in Language Acquisition Balota David A Marsh Elizabeth J eds 2004 Cognitive psychology key readings Hove New York N Y Psychology press p 526 ISBN 978 1841690650 Karmiloff Kyra Karmiloff Smith Annette 2001 Pathways to Language From Fetus to Adolescent Harvard University Press ISBN 9780674008359 This is a wug And if there were two there would Superlinguo Retrieved August 29 2019 Gleason Jean Berko 1975 Fathers and other strangers men s speech to young children In Daniel P Dato ed Developmental Psycholinguistics Theory and Applications Washington DC Georgetown University Press Gleason Jean Berko 1975 Fathers and Other Strangers Men s speech to Young Children 26th Annual Roundtable 289 297 Matthews Danielle ed 2014 Pragmatic development in first language acquisition Philadelphia PA John Benjamins Publishing Company p 324 ISBN 978 9027234704 Gleason Jean Berko Weintraub Sandra 1976 The acquisition of routines in child language Language in Society 5 2 129 136 doi 10 1017 s0047404500006977 S2CID 146769318 Greif Esther Blank Gleason Jean Berko 1980 Hi Thanks and Goodbye More Routine Information Language in Society 9 2 159 166 doi 10 1017 s0047404500008034 JSTOR 4167137 S2CID 145771682 a b Gleason Jean Berko Ely Richard 2006 I m sorry I said that apologies in young children s discourse Journal of Child Language 33 3 33 599 620 doi 10 1017 S0305000906007446 PMID 17017280 S2CID 31823853 a b Ely Richard Jean Berko Gleason Ann MacGibbon Elena Zaretsky 2001 Attention to Language Lessons Learned at the Dinner Table Social Development 10 3 355 373 doi 10 1111 1467 9507 00170 Duarte Joana 2010 Bilingual language proficiency Munster Waxmann p 32 ISBN 9783830923176 Halliday M A K ed 1990 Learning Keeping and Using Language Amsterdam Benjamins ISBN 9781556191046 Clark Eve V 2003 First language acquisition Cambridge Cambridge University Press ISBN 9780521629973 Schmid Monika S 2002 First language attrition use and maintenance the case of German Jews in anglophone countries Amsterdam J Benjamins p 13 ISBN 9789027241351 Harris Catherine Ayse Ayciceĝi Jean Berko Gleason 2003 Taboo Words and Reprimands Elicit Greater Autonomic Reactivity in a First than in a Second Language Applied Psycholinguistics 24 4 doi 10 1017 S0142716403000286 S2CID 7219049 Gleason Jean Berko Pan 1988 Maintaining Foreign Language Skills You CAN Take It with You 1 22 Menn Lise ed 1990 Agrammatic aphasia a cross language narrative sourcebook Amsterdam J Benjamins ISBN 9789027273512 Rosenberg Sheldon 1982 Handbook of applied psycholinguistics Hillsdale N J L Erlbaum Associates ISBN 9780898591736 a b c Goodglass Harold Jean Berko Gleason Nancy Ackerman Bernholtz Mary R Hyde 1972 Some Linguistic Structures in the Speech of a Broca s Aphasic Cortex 8 2 191 212 doi 10 1016 s0010 9452 72 80018 2 PMID 5043793 Miceli Gabriele 1998 Gianfranco Denes Luigi Pizzamiglio eds Grammatical deficits in aphasia Handbook of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology External links editCurriculum vitae Archived November 18 2016 at the Wayback Machine Official website Departmental page Nova PBS The Secret Life of Scientists and Engineers Jean Berko Gleason Psycholinguist Speed Demon World Science Festival Spotlight The Dao of Zib Gleason discusses her education career and the Wug Test On Being Jean Berko Gleason Unfolding Language Unfolding Life Brief video of Gleason administering a Wug test With Stephen Fry Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Jean Berko Gleason amp oldid 1217504114 Children 27s learning of English morphology E2 80 8D E2 80 94 E2 80 8Cthe Wug Test, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.