fbpx
Wikipedia

Implicit stereotype

An implicit bias or implicit stereotype is the pre-reflective attribution of particular qualities by an individual to a member of some social out group.[1] Recent studies have determined that "implicit bias" towards those of the opposite gender may be even more influential than racial implicit bias.[2]

Implicit stereotypes are thought to be shaped by experience and based on learned associations between particular qualities and social categories, including race and/or gender.[3] Individuals' perceptions and behaviors can be influenced by the implicit stereotypes they hold, even if they are sometimes unaware they hold such stereotypes.[4] Implicit bias is an aspect of implicit social cognition: the phenomenon that perceptions, attitudes, and stereotypes can operate prior to conscious intention or endorsement.[5] The existence of implicit bias is supported by a variety of scientific articles in psychological literature.[6] Implicit stereotype was first defined by psychologists Mahzarin Banaji and Anthony Greenwald in 1995.

Explicit stereotypes, by contrast, are consciously endorsed, intentional, and sometimes controllable thoughts and beliefs.[7]

Implicit biases, however, are thought to be the product of associations learned through past experiences.[8] Implicit biases can be activated by the environment and operate prior to a person's intentional, conscious endorsement.[1] Implicit bias can persist even when an individual rejects the bias explicitly.[1]

Bias, attitude, stereotype and prejudice edit

Attitudes, stereotypes, prejudices, and bias are all examples of psychological constructs. Psychological constructs are mental associations that can influence a person's behavior and feelings toward an individual or group. If the person is unaware of these mental associations the stereotypes, prejudices, or bias is said to be implicit.

Bias is defined as prejudice in favor of or against one thing, person, or group compared with another, usually in a way considered to be unfair. Bias can be seen as the overarching definition of stereotype and prejudice, because it is how we associate traits (usually negative) to a specific group of people. Our “implicit attitudes reflect constant exposure to stereotypical portrayals of members of, and items in, all kinds of different categories: racial groups, professions, women, nationalities, members of the LGBTQ community, disabilities, moral and political values, etc.”[9]

An attitude is an evaluative judgment of an object, a person, or a social group.[10] An attitude is held by or characterizes a person. Implicit attitudes are evaluations that occur without conscious awareness towards an attitude object or the self.

A stereotype is the association of a person or a social group with a consistent set of traits. This may include both positive and negative traits, such as African Americans are great at sports or African Americans are more violent than any other race in the United States. There are many types of stereotypes that exists: racial, cultural, gender, group (i.e. college students), all being very explicit in the lives of many people.

Prejudice is defined as unfair negative attitude toward a social group or a member of that group.[11] Prejudices can stem from many of the things that people observe in a different social group that include, but are not limited to, gender, sex, race/ethnicity, or religion. This is pertinent to stereotypes because a stereotype can influence the way people feel toward another group, hence prejudice.

Methods for investigation edit

There is a clear challenge in measuring the degree to which someone is biased. There are two different forms of bias: implicit and explicit. The two forms of bias are, however, connected. “Explicit bias encompasses our conscious attitudes which can be measured by self-report, but pose the potential of individuals falsely endorsing more socially desirable attitudes. Although implicit biases have been considered unconscious and involuntary attitudes which lie below the surface of consciousness, some people seem to be aware of their influence on their behavior and cognitive processes.[12] The implicit-association test (IAT) is one validated tool used to measure implicit bias. The IAT requires participants to rapidly pair two social groups with either positive or negative attributes.”[13]

Implicit-association test edit

The implicit-association test (IAT) alleges to predict prejudice an individual has toward different social groups. The test claims to do this by capturing the differences in the time it takes respondent to choose between two unassociated but related topics. Respondents are instructed to click one of two computer keys to categorize stimuli into associated categories. When the categories appear consistent to the respondent, the time taken to categorize the stimuli will be less than when the categories seem inconsistent. An implicit association is said to exist when respondents take longer to respond to a category-inconsistent pairing than a category-consistent pairing. The implicit-association test is used in psychology for a wide array of topics. These fields include gender, race, science, career, weight, sexuality, and disability.[14] While acclaimed and highly influential, the implicit-association test falls short of a strong scientific consensus. Critics of the implicit-association test cite studies that counterintuitively link biased test scores with less discriminatory behavior.[15] Studies have also asserted that the implicit-association test fails to measure unconscious thought.[4]

Go/no-go association task (GNAT) edit

The GNAT is similar to the implicit-association test. Although the IAT reveals differential associations of two target concepts (e.g. male-female and weak-strong), the GNAT reveals associations within one concept (for example, whether female is associated more strongly with weak or strong).[16]

Participants are presented with word pairs among distractors. Participants are instructed to indicate "go" if the words are target pairs, or "no-go" if they are not. For example, participants may be instructed to indicate "go" if the word pairs are female names and words that are related to strength. Then, participants are instructed to indicate "go" if the word pairs are female names and words that are related to weakness. This method relies on signal detection theory; participants' accuracy rates reveal endorsement of the implicit stereotype. For example, if participants are more accurate for female-weak pairs than for female-strong pairs, this suggests the subject more strongly associates weakness with females than strength.[17]

Semantic priming and lexical decision task edit

Semantic priming measures the association between two concepts.[18] In a lexical decision task, subjects are presented with pair of words, and asked to indicate whether the pair are words (for example, "butter") or non-words (for example, "tubter"). The theory behind semantic priming is that subjects are quicker to respond to a word if preceded by a word related to it in meaning (e.g. bread-butter vs. bread-dog).[18] In other words, the word "bread" primes other words related in meaning, including butter. Psychologists utilize semantic priming to reveal implicit associations between stereotypic-congruent words. For instance, participants may be asked to indicate whether pronouns are male or female. These pronouns are either preceded by professions that are predominantly female ("secretary, nurse"), or male ("mechanic, doctor"). Reaction times reveal strength of association between professions and gender.[19]

Sentence completion edit

In a sentence completion task, subjects may be presented with sentences that contain stereotypic black and white names (Jerome, Adam), positive and negative stereotypic black behaviors (easily made the team, blasted loud music in his car) and counter-stereotypic behaviors (got a job at Microsoft, refused to dance). Subjects are asked to add to the end of a sentence in any way that is grammatical, e.g. "Jerome got an A on his test..." could be completed with "because it was easy" (stereotypic-congruent) or "because he studied for months" (stereotypic-incongruent) or "and then he went out to celebrate" (non-explanatory). This task is used to measure stereotypic explanatory bias (SEB): participants have a larger SEB if they give more explanations for stereotype-congruent sentences than stereotype–incongruent sentences, and if they give more stereotypic-congruent explanations.[20]

Differences between measures edit

The Implicit Association Test (IAT), sequential priming, and other implicit bias tests, are mechanisms for determining how susceptible we are to stereotypes. They are widely used in Social Psychology, although measuring response time to a question as a good measure of implicit biases is still up for debate. “Some theorists do question the interpretation of the scores from tests such as the IAT, but the debate is still going on and responses to the criticisms are certainly widespread.”[9]

In qualitative market research, researchers have described a framework called bias testing to mitigate researcher bias when designing survey questions. It involves empirically testing the survey questions with real-life respondents using interviewer moderated or technology-enabled unmoderated techniques.[21]

Findings edit

Gender bias edit

Gender biases are the stereotypical attitudes or prejudices that we have towards specific genders. "The concept of gender also refers to the constantly ongoing social construction of what is considered 'feminine' and 'masculine' and is based on power and sociocultural norms about women and men."[22] Gender biases are the ways in which we judge men and women based on their hegemonically feminine and masculine assigned traits.

The category of male has been found to be associated with traits of strength and achievement. Both male and female subjects associate male category members more strongly than female category members with words like bold, mighty, and power.[23] The strength of this association is not predicted by explicit beliefs, such as responses on a gender stereotype questionnaire (for example, one question asked if subjects endorsed the word feminist).[1] In a test to reveal the false fame effect, non famous male names are more likely to be falsely identified as famous than non famous female names; this is evidence for an implicit stereotype of male achievement.[24] Females are more associated with weakness. This is true for both male and female subjects, but female subjects only show this association when the weak words are positive, such as fine, flower and gentle; female subjects do not show this pattern when the weak words are negative, such as feeble, frail, and scrawny.[23]

Particular professions are implicitly associated with genders. Elementary school teachers are implicitly stereotyped to be female, and engineers are stereotyped to be male.[25]

Gender bias in science and engineering edit

Implicit-association tests reveal an implicit association for male with science and math, and females with arts and language.[26] Girls as young as nine years old have been found to hold an implicit male-math stereotype and an implicit preference for language over math.[27] Women have stronger negative associations with math than men do, and the stronger females associate with a female gender identity, the more implicit negativity they have towards math.[26] For both men and women, the strength of these implicit stereotypes predicts both implicit and explicit math attitudes, belief in one's math ability, and SAT performance.[26] The strength of these implicit stereotypes in elementary-aged girls predicts academic self-concepts, academic achievement, and enrollment preferences, even more than do explicit measures.[27] Women with a stronger implicit gender-math stereotype were less likely to pursue a math-related career, regardless of their actual math ability or explicit gender-math stereotypes.[28] This may be because women with stronger implicit gender-math stereotypes are more at risk for stereotype threat. Thus, women with strong implicit stereotypes perform much worse on a math test when primed with gender than women who have weak implicit stereotypes.[29]

Though the number of women pursuing and earning degrees in engineering has increased in the last 20 years, women are below men at all degree levels in all fields of engineering.[30] These implicit gender stereotypes are robust; in a study of more than 500,000 respondents from 34 nations, more than 70% of individuals held this implicit stereotype.[31] The national strength of the implicit stereotype is related to national sex differences among 8th graders on the International TIMSS, a worldwide math &science standardized achievement exam. This effect is present even after statistically controlling for gender inequality in general.[31] Additionally, for women across cultures, studies have shown individual differences in strength of this implicit stereotype is associated with interest, participation and performance in sciences.[31] Extending to the professional world, implicit biases and subsequent explicit attitudes toward women can "negatively affect the education, hiring, promotion, and retention of women in STEM".[32]

The effects of such implicit biases can be seen in across multiple studies including:

  • Parents rate the math abilities of their daughters lower than parents with sons who perform identically well in school[33]
  • College faculty are less likely to respond to inquiries about research opportunities if the email appears to be from a woman as opposed to an identical email from a man[34]
  • Science faculty are less likely to hire or mentor students they believe are women as opposed to men[35]

An interagency report from the Office of Science and Technology Policy and Office of Personnel Management has investigated systemic barriers including implicit biases that have traditionally inhibited particularly women and underrepresented minorities in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM) and makes recommendations for reducing the impact of bias.[36] Research has shown that implicit bias training may improve attitudes towards women in STEM.[32]

Racial bias edit

Racial bias can be used synonymously with "stereotyping and prejudice" because "it allows for the inclusion of both positive and negative evaluations related to perceptions of race."[37] We begin to create racial biases towards other groups of people starting as young as age 3, creating an ingroup and outgroup view on members of various races, usually starting with skin color.

In lexical decision tasks, after subjects are subliminally primed with the word BLACK, they are quicker to react to words consistent with black stereotypes, such as athletic, musical, poor and promiscuous. When subjects are subliminally primed with WHITE, they are quicker to react to white stereotypes, such as intelligent, ambitious, uptight and greedy.[38] These tendencies are sometimes, but not always, associated with explicit stereotypes.[38][39]

People may also hold an implicit stereotype that associates black category members as violent. People primed with words like ghetto, slavery and jazz were more likely to interpret a character in a vignette as hostile.[40] However, this finding is controversial; because the character's race was not specified, it is suggested that the procedure primed the race-unspecified concept of hostility, and did not necessarily represent stereotypes.[38]

An implicit stereotype of violent black men may associate black men with weapons. In a video game where subjects were supposed to shoot men with weapons and not shoot men with ordinary objects, subjects were more likely to shoot a black man with an ordinary object than a white man with an ordinary object. This tendency was related to subjects' implicit attitudes toward black people. Similar results were found in a priming task; subjects who saw a black face immediately before either a weapon or an ordinary object more quickly and accurately identified the image as a weapon than when it was preceded by a white face.[41]

Implicit race stereotypes affect behaviors and perceptions. When choosing between pairs of questions to ask a black interviewee, one of which is congruent with racial stereotype, people with a high stereotypic explanatory bias (SEB) are more likely to ask the racially congruent stereotype question. In a related study, subjects with a high SEB rated a black individual more negatively in an unstructured laboratory interaction.[20]

In-group and out-group bias edit

Group prototypes define social groups through a collection of attributes that define both what representative group members have in common and what distinguishes the ingroup from relevant outgroups.[42] In-group favoritism, sometimes known as in-group–out-group bias, in-group bias, or intergroup bias, is a pattern of favoring members of one's in-group over out-group members. This can be expressed in evaluation of others, in allocation of resources, and in many other ways.[43][44] Implicit in-group preferences emerge very early in life,[45] even in children as young as six years old. In-group bias wherein people who are 'one of us' (i.e., our ingroup) are favored compared to those in the outgroup, meaning those who differ from ourselves.[46] Ingroup favoritism is associated with feelings of trust and positive regard for ingroup members and surfaces often on measures of implicit bias. This categorization (ingroup vs. outgroup) is often automatic and pre-conscious.[47]

The reasons for having in-group and out-group bias could be explained by ethnocentrism, social categorization, oxytocin, etc. A research paper done by Carsten De Dreu reviewed that oxytocin enables the development of trust, specifically towards individuals with similar characteristics—categorized as 'in-group' members—promoting cooperation with and favoritism towards such individuals.[48] People who report that they have strong needs for simplifying their environments also show more ingroup favoritism.[49] The tendency to categorize into ingroups and outgroups and resulting ingroup favoritism is likely a universal aspect of human beings.[50]

We generally tend to hold implicit biases that favor our own ingroup, though research has shown that we can still hold implicit biases against our ingroup.[46][51] The most prominent example of negative affect towards an ingroup was recorded in 1939 by Kenneth and Mamie Clark using their now famous “Dolls Test”. In this test, African American children were asked to pick their favorite doll from a choice of otherwise identical black and white dolls. A high percentage of these African American children indicated a preference for the white dolls.[52] Social identity theory and Freudian theorists explain in-group derogation as the result of a negative self-image, which they believe is then extended to the group.[53]

Other stereotypes edit

Research on implicit stereotypes primarily focuses on gender and race. However, other topics, such as age, weight, and profession, have been investigated. IATs have revealed implicit stereotypes reflecting explicit stereotypes about adolescents. The results from these tests claim that adolescents are more likely to be associated with words like trendy and defiant than adults.[54] In addition, one IAT study revealed that older adults had a higher preference for younger adults compared to older adults; and younger adults had a lower implicit preference for younger adults compared to older adults. The study also found that women and participants with more education had lower implicit preference for younger adults.[55] IATs have also revealed implicit stereotypes on the relationship between obese individuals and low work performance. Words like lazy and incompetent are more associated with images of obese individuals than images of thin ones.[56] This association is stronger for thin subjects than overweight ones.[57] Like explicit stereotypes, implicit stereotypes may contain both positive and negative traits. This can be seen in examples of occupational implicit stereotypes where people perceive preschool teachers as both warm and incompetent, while lawyers are judged as both cold and competent.[58]

Activation of implicit stereotypes edit

Implicit stereotypes are activated by environmental and situational factors. These associations develop over the course of a lifetime beginning at a very early age through exposure to direct and indirect messages. In addition to early life experiences, the media and news programming are often-cited origins of implicit associations.[59] In the laboratory, implicit stereotypes are activated by priming. When subjects are primed with dependence by unscrambling words such as dependent, cooperative, and passive, they judge a target female as more dependent. When subjects are primed with aggression with words like aggressive, confident, argumentative, they judge a target male as more aggressive.[60] The fact that females and words such as dependent, cooperative, and passive and males and words like aggressive, confident, argumentative are thought to be associated together suggest an implicit gender stereotype. Stereotypes are also activated by a subliminal prime. To exemplify, white subjects exposed to subliminal words that consist of a black stereotype (ghetto, slavery, jazz) interpret a target male as more hostile, consistent with the implicit stereotype of hostile black man.[40] However, this finding is controversial because the character's race is not specified. Instead, it is suggested that the procedure primed the race-unspecified concept of hostility, and did not necessarily represent stereotypes.[38] By getting to know people who differ from oneself on a real, personal level, one can begin to build new associations about the groups those individuals represent and break down existing implicit associations.[61]

Malleability of implicit stereotypes edit

Implicit stereotypes can, at least temporarily, be reduced or increased. Most methods have been found to reduce implicit bias temporarily, and are largely based on context.[62] Some evidence suggests that implicit bias can be reduced long-term, but it may require education and consistent effort. Some implicit bias training techniques designed to counteract implicit bias are stereotype replacement, counter-stereotypic imaging, individuation, perspective taking, and increasing opportunities for contact.[63]

Stereotype replacement is when one replaces a stereotypical response with a non-stereotypical response. Counter-stereotypic imagining is when one imagines others in a positive light and replace stereotypes with positive examples. Individuation is when one focuses on specific details of a certain member of a group to avoid over-generalizing. Perspective taking is when one takes the perspective of a member of a marginalized group. Increasing opportunities for contact is when one actively seeks out opportunities to engage in interactions with members of marginalized groups.[63]

Self and social motives

The activation of implicit stereotypes may be decreased when the individual is motivated to promote a positive self-image, either to oneself or to others in a social setting. There are two parts to this: internal and external motivation. Internal motivation is when an individual wants to be careful of what they say, and external motivation is when an individual has a desire to respond in a politically correct way.[64]

Positive feedback from a black person decreases stereotypic sentence completion, while negative feedback from a black person increases it.[65] Subjects also reveal lesser strength of race stereotypes when they feel others disagree with the stereotypes.[66] Motivated self-regulation does not immediately reduce implicit bias. It raises awareness of discrepancies when biases stand in the way of personal beliefs.[64]

Promote counterstereotypes

Implicit stereotypes can be reduced by exposure to counterstereotypes. Reading biographies of females in leadership roles (such as Meg Whitman, the CEO of eBay) increases females' associations between female names and words like leader, determined, and ambitious in a gender stereotype IAT.[67] Attending a women's college (where students are presumably more often exposed to women in leadership positions) reduces associations between leadership and males after one year of schooling.[67] Merely imagining a strong woman reduces implicit association between females and weakness, and imagining storybook princesses increases the implicit association between females and weakness.[17]

Focus of attention

Diverting a participant's focus of attention can reduce implicit stereotypes. Generally, female primes facilitate reaction time to stereotypical female traits when participants are instructed to indicate whether the prime is animate. When participants instead are instructed to indicate whether a white dot is present on the prime, this diverts their focus of attention from the primes' feminine features. This successfully weakens the strength of the prime and thus weakening the strength of gender stereotypes.[68]

Configuration of stimulus cues

Whether stereotypes are activated depends on the context. When presented with an image of a Chinese woman, Chinese stereotypes were stronger after seeing her use chopsticks, and female stereotypes were stronger after seeing her put on makeup.[69]

Characteristics of individual category members

Stereotype activation may be stronger for some category members than for others. People express weaker gender stereotypes with unfamiliar than familiar names.[70] Judgments and gut reactions that go along with implicit biases are based on how familiar something is.[71]

Criticism edit

Some social psychology research has indicated that individuating information (giving someone any information about an individual group member other than category information) may eliminate the effects of stereotype bias.[72]

Meta-analyses edit

Researchers from the University of Wisconsin–Madison, Harvard, and the University of Virginia examined 426 studies over 20 years involving 72,063 participants that used the IAT and other similar tests. They concluded two things:

  1. The correlation between implicit bias and discriminatory behavior appears weaker than previously thought.
  2. There is little evidence that changes in implicit bias correlate with changes in a person's behavior.[73]

In a 2013 meta-analysis, Hart, Blanton, et al. declared that, despite its frequent misrepresentation as a proxy for the unconscious, "the IAT provides little insight into who will discriminate against whom, and provides no more insight than explicit measures of bias."[74]

News outlets edit

Heather Mac Donald, writing in The Wall Street Journal, noted that:

Few academic ideas have been as eagerly absorbed into public discourse lately as “implicit bias.” Embraced by Barack Obama, Hillary Clinton and most of the press, implicit bias has spawned a multimillion-dollar consulting industry, along with a movement to remove the concept of individual agency from the law. Yet its scientific basis is crumbling.

Mac Donald suggests there is still a political and economic drive to use the implicit bias paradigm as a political lever and to profit off entities that want to avoid litigation.[75]

Psychometric concerns edit

Edouard Machery has argued that “the use of [indirect measures like the implicit association test] is deeply problematic” because the tests do not exhibit the psychometric properties we would expect from measures of "attitudes".[76] However, many have already admitted that these indirect tests "assess behavior" rather than attitudes.[77] This is an example of how the debate about implicit bias can involve "talking past one another" based on "different expectations of indirect measures", views of (what) implicit bias (is), assumptions about which evidence is relevant, thresholds for scientific significance, psychometric standards, and even norms of science communication.[78] So evaluating debates about tests of implicit bias requires one to pay careful attention to debators' background assumptions and whether (or how well) debators' justify those assumptions.

Statement by original authors edit

Where previously Greenwald and Banaji asserted in their book BlindSpot (2013).

Given the relatively small proportion of people who are overtly prejudiced and how clearly it is established that automatic race preference predicts discrimination, it is reasonable to conclude not only that implicit bias is a cause of Black disadvantage but also that it plausibly plays a greater role than does explicit bias.[75]

The evidence presented by their peer researchers led them to concede in correspondence that:

  1. The IAT does not predict biased behaviour(in laboratory settings)
  2. It is "problematic to use [the IAT] to classify persons as likely to engage in discrimination".

However, they also stated, "Regardless of inclusion policy, both meta-analyses estimated aggregate correlational effect sizes that were large enough to explain discriminatory impacts that are societally significant either because they can affect many people simultaneously or because they can repeatedly affect single persons."[79]

Summary edit

Implicit bias is thought to be the product of positive or negative mental associations about persons, things, or groups that are formed and activated pre-consciously or subconsciously. In 1995, researchers Banaji and Greenwald noted that someone's social learning experiences, such as observing parents, friends, or others, could create this type of association and, therefore, trigger this type of bias. Many studies have found that culture is able to stimulate biases as well, both in a negative and positive way regardless someone's personal experience with other cultures.[80] As far as many people are concerned, implicit bias knows no age restriction and it can be held by anyone regardless of their age. In fact, implicit biases can be found in a person as young as six years old.[80] Even though implicit bias may be difficult to catch, especially compared to explicit bias, it can be measured through a number of mechanisms, such as sequential priming, response competition, EDA, EMG, fMRI, ERP and ITA.[81] Thus, once a person becomes aware of their own bias, they can take action to change it, if they wish.[82]

The existence of implicitly biased behavior is supported by several articles in psychological literature. Adults- and even children- may hold implicit stereotypes of social categories, categories to which they may themselves belong to. Without intention, or even awareness, implicit stereotypes affect human behavior and judgments. This has wide-ranging implications for society, from discrimination and personal career choices to understanding others in social interactions each day.[1][27][24][40][60]

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ a b c d e Greenwald, Anthony G.; Banaji, Mahzarin R. (1995). "Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes". Psychological Review. 102 (1): 4–27. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.411.2919. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.102.1.4. PMID 7878162. S2CID 8194189.
  2. ^ Pro-female and anti-male biases are more influential than race and other factors in Implicit Association Tests Psypost.org. In Cognitive Science, Social Psychology. By Eric W. Dolan. March 26, 2023. Retrieved March 12, 2024.
  3. ^ Byrd, Nick (February 2021). "What we can (and can't) infer about implicit bias from debiasing experiments". Synthese. 198 (2): 1427–1455. doi:10.1007/s11229-019-02128-6. S2CID 60441599.
  4. ^ a b Hahn, Adam; Judd, Charles M.; Hirsh, Holen K.; Blair, Irene V. (June 2014). "Awareness of implicit attitudes". Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 143 (3): 1369–1392. doi:10.1037/a0035028. PMC 4038711. PMID 24294868.
  5. ^ Gawronski, Bertram (June 10, 2019). "Six Lessons for a Cogent Science of Implicit Bias and Its Criticism". Perspectives on Psychological Science. 14 (4): 574–595. doi:10.1177/1745691619826015. PMID 31181174.
  6. ^ Jost, John T.; Rudman, Laurie A.; Blair, Irene V.; Carney, Dana R.; Dasgupta, Nilanjana; Glaser, Jack; Hardin, Curtis D. (2009). "The existence of implicit bias is beyond reasonable doubt: A refutation of ideological and methodological objections and executive summary of ten studies that no manager should ignore". Research in Organizational Behavior. 29: 39–69. doi:10.1016/j.riob.2009.10.001.
  7. ^ Gaertner, Brown, Sam, Rupert (April 15, 2008). Blackwell Handbook of Social Psychology: Intergroup Processes. John Wiley & Sons. ISBN 9780470692707. Retrieved August 11, 2013.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  8. ^ Del Pinal, Guillermo; Spaulding, Shannon (February 2018). "Conceptual centrality and implicit bias". Mind & Language. 33 (1): 95–111. doi:10.1111/mila.12166. hdl:2027.42/142467.
  9. ^ a b Toribio, Josefa (March 1, 2018). "Implicit Bias: from social structure to representational format". Theoria. 33 (1): 41. doi:10.1387/theoria.17751. hdl:2445/145839.
  10. ^ Crano, W.D., & Prislin, R. (2008). Attitudes and attitude change. New York: CRC Press.
  11. ^ Gaertner, Samuel L.; Dovidio, John F. (1999). "Reducing Prejudice: Combating Intergroup Biases". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 8 (4): 101–105. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.00024. JSTOR 20182575. S2CID 31743564.
  12. ^ Hahn, Adam; Gawronski, Bertram (May 2019). "Facing one's implicit biases: From awareness to acknowledgment". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 116 (5): 769–794. doi:10.1037/pspi0000155. PMID 30359070. S2CID 51896790.
  13. ^ Maina, Ivy W.; Belton, Tanisha D.; Ginzberg, Sara; Singh, Ajit; Johnson, Tiffani J. (February 2018). "A decade of studying implicit racial/ethnic bias in healthcare providers using the implicit association test". Social Science & Medicine. 199: 219–229. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2017.05.009. PMID 28532892.
  14. ^ Harris, Matthew; Macinko, James; Jimenez, Geronimo; Mullachery, Pricila (December 2017). "Measuring the bias against low-income country research: an Implicit Association Test". Globalization and Health. 13 (1): 80. doi:10.1186/s12992-017-0304-y. PMC 5674740. PMID 29110668.
  15. ^ Shelton, J. N.; Richeson, J. A.; Salvatore, J.; Trawalter, S. (May 1, 2005). "Ironic Effects of Racial Bias During Interracial Interactions". Psychological Science. 16 (5): 397–402. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01547.x. PMID 15869700. S2CID 15621035.
  16. ^ Nosek, Brian A.; Banaji, Mahzarin R. (December 2001). "The Go/No-Go Association Task". Social Cognition. 19 (6): 625–666. doi:10.1521/soco.19.6.625.20886.
  17. ^ a b Blair, Irene V.; Ma, Jennifer E.; Lenton, Alison P. (2001). "Imagining stereotypes away: The moderation of implicit stereotypes through mental imagery". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 81 (5): 828–841. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.555.2509. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.5.828. PMID 11708560.
  18. ^ a b Meyer, David E.; Schvaneveldt, Roger W. (1971). "Facilitation in recognizing pairs of words: Evidence of a dependence between retrieval operations". Journal of Experimental Psychology. 90 (2): 227–234. doi:10.1037/h0031564. PMID 5134329. S2CID 36672941.
  19. ^ Banaji, Mahzarin R.; Hardin, Curtis D. (May 1996). "Automatic Stereotyping". Psychological Science. 7 (3): 136–141. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1996.tb00346.x. S2CID 207858486.
  20. ^ a b Sekaquaptewa, Denise; Espinoza, Penelope; Thompson, Mischa; Vargas, Patrick; von Hippel, William (January 2003). "Stereotypic explanatory bias: Implicit stereotyping as a predictor of discrimination". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 39 (1): 75–82. doi:10.1016/S0022-1031(02)00512-7.
  21. ^ Geisen, Emily; Sha, Mandy; Roper, Farren (2024). Bias testing in market research: A framework to enable inclusive research design (published January 3, 2024). ISBN 979-8862902785.
  22. ^ Hamberg, Katrina (May 2008). "Gender bias in medicine". Women's Health. 4 (3): 237–243. doi:10.2217/17455057.4.3.237. PMID 19072473.
  23. ^ a b Rudman, L. A.; Greenwald, A. G.; McGhee, D. E. (2001). "Implicit self-concept and evaluative implicit gender stereotypes: Self and ingroup share desirable traits". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 27 (9): 1164–1178. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.43.6589. doi:10.1177/0146167201279009. S2CID 13491811.
  24. ^ a b Banaji, M. R.; Greenwald, A. G. (1995). "Implicit gender stereotyping in judgments of fame". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 68 (2): 181–198. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.74.895. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.68.2.181. PMID 7877095.
  25. ^ White, M. J.; White, G. B. (2006). "Implicit and explicit occupational gender stereotypes". Sex Roles. 55 (3–4): 259–266. doi:10.1007/s11199-006-9078-z. S2CID 143793226.
  26. ^ a b c Nosek, B. A.; Banaji, M. R.; Greenwald, A. G. (2002). "Math = male, me = female, therefore math ≠ me". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 83 (1): 44–59. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.463.6120. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.83.1.44. PMID 12088131. S2CID 204321795.
  27. ^ a b c Steffens, M. C.; Jelenec, P.; Noack, P. (2010). "On the leaky math pipeline: Comparing implicit math-gender stereotypes and math withdrawal in female and male children and adolescents". Journal of Educational Psychology. 102 (4): 947–963. doi:10.1037/a0019920.
  28. ^ Kiefer, Amy K.; Sekaquaptewa, Denise (January 2007). "Implicit Stereotypes, Gender Identification, and Math-Related Outcomes: A Prospective Study of Female College Students". Psychological Science. 18 (1): 13–18. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01841.x. PMID 17362371. S2CID 30588471.
  29. ^ Kiefer, A. K.; Sekaquaptewa, D. (2007). "Implicit stereotypes and women's math performance: How implicit gender-math stereotypes influence women's susceptibility to stereotype threat". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 43 (5): 825–832. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2006.08.004.
  30. ^ "Women, Minorities, and Persons with Disabilities in Science and Engineering". National Science Foundation. Retrieved March 31, 2017.
  31. ^ a b c Nosek, B. A.; Smyth, F. L.; Sriram, N. N.; Lindner, N. M.; Devos, T.; Ayala, A.; Greenwald, A. G. (2009). "National differences in gender–science stereotypes predict national sex differences in science and math achievement". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 106 (26): 10593–10597. Bibcode:2009PNAS..10610593N. doi:10.1073/pnas.0809921106. PMC 2705538. PMID 19549876.
  32. ^ a b Jackson, Sarah M.; Hillard, Amy L.; Schneider, Tamera R. (September 2014). "Using implicit bias training to improve attitudes toward women in STEM". Social Psychology of Education. 17 (3): 419–438. doi:10.1007/s11218-014-9259-5. S2CID 53500821.
  33. ^ Yee, Doris K.; Eccles, Jacquelynne S. (September 1988). "Parent perceptions and attributions for children's math achievement". Sex Roles. 19 (5–6): 317–333. doi:10.1007/bf00289840. hdl:2027.42/45585. S2CID 112300.
  34. ^ Milkman, Katherine L.; Akinola, Modupe; Chugh, Dolly (2015). "What happens before? A field experiment exploring how pay and representation differentially shape bias on the pathway into organizations". Journal of Applied Psychology. 100 (6): 1678–1712. doi:10.1037/apl0000022. PMID 25867167.
  35. ^ Moss-Racusin, C. A.; Dovidio, J. F.; Brescoll, V. L.; Graham, M. J.; Handelsman, J. (October 9, 2012). "Science faculty's subtle gender biases favor male students". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 109 (41): 16474–16479. Bibcode:2012PNAS..10916474M. doi:10.1073/pnas.1211286109. PMC 3478626. PMID 22988126.
  36. ^ Handelsman, Jo; Ward, Wanda (December 12, 2016). "Increasing Diversity in the STEM Workforce by Reducing the Impact of Bias". The White House. Retrieved March 31, 2017.
  37. ^ Noles, Erica (May 1, 2014). What's age got to do with it? Examining how the age of stimulus faces affects children's implicit racial bias. UNLV Theses, Dissertations, Professional Papers, and Capstones (Thesis). University of Nevada, Las Vegas. doi:10.34917/5836145. ProQuest 1566943023.
  38. ^ a b c d Wittenbrink, B.; Judd, C. M.; Park, B. (1997). "Evidence for racial prejudice at the implicit level and its relationship with questionnaire measures". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 72 (2): 262–274. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.462.7827. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.72.2.262. PMID 9107001.
  39. ^ Gaertner, S. L.; McLaughlin, J. P. (1983). "Racial stereotypes: Associations and ascriptions of positive and negative characteristics". Social Psychology Quarterly. 46 (1): 23–30. doi:10.2307/3033657. JSTOR 3033657.
  40. ^ a b c Devine, P. G. (1989). "Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic and controlled components". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 56: 5–18. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.56.1.5. S2CID 33975027.
  41. ^ Staats, Cheryl; Patton, Charles (2013). State of the Science: Implicit Bias Review (PDF). Kirwan Institute for the Study of Race and Ethnicity at the Ohio State University. pp. 3, 37–39.
  42. ^ Hohman, Zachary P.; Gaffney, Amber M.; Hogg, Michael A. (September 2017). "Who Am I If I Am Not like My Group? Self-Uncertainty and Feeling Peripheral in a Group". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 72: 125–132. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2017.05.002.
  43. ^ Aronson, E., Wilson, T. D., & Akert, R. (2010). Social psychology. 7th ed. Upper Saddle River: Prentice Hall.[page needed]
  44. ^ Taylor, Donald M.; Doria, Janet R. (April 1981). "Self-Serving and Group-Serving Bias in Attribution". The Journal of Social Psychology. 113 (2): 201–211. doi:10.1080/00224545.1981.9924371.
  45. ^ Dunham, Y.; Baron, A. S.; Banaji, M. R. (2008). "The Development of Implicit Intergroup Cognition" (PDF). Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 12 (7): 248–253. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2008.04.006. PMID 18555736. S2CID 45599721.
  46. ^ a b Greenwald, A. G.; Krieger, L. H. (2006). "Implicit Bias: Scientific Foundations". California Law Review. 94 (4): 945–967. doi:10.2307/20439056. hdl:10125/66105. JSTOR 20439056.
  47. ^ Reskin, B (2000). "The Proximate Causes of Employment Discrimination". Contemporary Sociology. 29 (2): 319–328. doi:10.2307/2654387. JSTOR 2654387. S2CID 2003571.
  48. ^ De Dreu, Carsten K.W. (2012). "Oxytocin modulates cooperation within and competition between groups: An integrative review and research agenda". Hormones and Behavior. 61 (3): 419–428. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.12.009. PMID 22227278. S2CID 140120921.
  49. ^ Strangor, Charles; Leary, Scott P. (2006). Intergroup beliefs: Investigations from the social side. Vol. 38. pp. 243–281. doi:10.1016/S0065-2601(06)38005-7. ISBN 9780120152384. {{cite book}}: |journal= ignored (help)
  50. ^ Brewer, Marilynn B. (2002). "The psychology of prejudice: Ingroup love or outgroup hate?". Journal of Social Issues. 55 (3): 429–444. doi:10.1111/0022-4537.00126.
  51. ^ Reskin, Barbara (2005). "Unconsciousness Raising" (PDF). Regional Review, 2005. 14 (3): 32–37.
  52. ^ Clark, Kenneth B.; Clark, Mamie P. (1950). "Emotional Factors in Racial Identification and Preference in Negro Children". The Journal of Negro Education. 19 (3): 341–350. doi:10.2307/2966491. JSTOR 2966491.
  53. ^ Ma-Kellams, Christine; Spencer-Rodgers, Julie; Peng, Kaiping (January 2011). "I Am Against Us? Unpacking Cultural Differences in Ingroup Favoritism via Dialecticism". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 37 (1): 15–27. doi:10.1177/0146167210388193. PMID 21084525. S2CID 18906927.
  54. ^ Gross, E. F.; Hardin, C. D. (2007). "Implicit and explicit stereotyping of adolescents". Social Justice Research. 20 (2): 140–160. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.514.3597. doi:10.1007/s11211-007-0037-9. S2CID 46261770.
  55. ^ Chopik, William J.; Giasson, Hannah L. (August 1, 2017). "Age Differences in Explicit and Implicit Age Attitudes Across the Life Span". The Gerontologist. 57 (suppl_2): S169–S177. doi:10.1093/geront/gnx058. PMC 5881761. PMID 28854609.
  56. ^ Agerström, J.; Rooth, D. (2011). "The role of automatic obesity stereotypes in real hiring discrimination". Journal of Applied Psychology. 96 (4): 790–805. doi:10.1037/a0021594. PMID 21280934.
  57. ^ Schwartz, M. B.; Vartanian, L. R.; Nosek, B. A.; Brownell, K. D. (2006). "The Influence of One's Own Body Weight on Implicit and Explicit Anti-fat Bias". Obesity. 14 (3): 440–447. doi:10.1038/oby.2006.58. PMID 16648615.
  58. ^ Carlsson, Rickard; Björklund, Fredrick (2010). "Implicit stereotype content: mixed stereotypes can be measured with the implicit association test". Social Psychology. 41 (4): 213–222. doi:10.1027/1864-9335/a000029. S2CID 144740592.
  59. ^ "Understanding Implicit Bias". kirwaninstitute.osu.edu. Retrieved April 6, 2018.
  60. ^ a b Banaji, M. R.; Hardin, C.; Rothman, A. J. (1993). "Implicit stereotyping in person judgment". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 65 (2): 272–281. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.65.2.272. S2CID 53051217.
  61. ^ Cheryl Staats (Winter 2015–2016). "Understanding Implicit Bias" (PDF). American Educator. pp. 29–43. Retrieved December 3, 2022.
  62. ^ Calanchini, Jimmy; Lai, Calvin K.; Klauer, Karl Christoph (August 27, 2020). "Reducing implicit racial preferences: III. A process-level examination of changes in implicit preferences". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 121 (4): 796–818. doi:10.1037/pspi0000339. PMID 32852973. S2CID 221357555.
  63. ^ a b Devine, Patricia G.; Forscher, Patrick S.; Austin, Anthony J.; Cox, William T.L. (November 2012). "Long-term reduction in implicit race bias: A prejudice habit-breaking intervention". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 48 (6): 1267–1278. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2012.06.003. PMC 3603687. PMID 23524616.
  64. ^ a b Burns, Mason D.; Monteith, Margo J.; Parker, Laura R. (November 2017). "Training away bias: The differential effects of counterstereotype training and self-regulation on stereotype activation and application". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 73: 97–110. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2017.06.003.
  65. ^ Sinclair, L.; Kunda, Z. (1999). "Reactions to a Black professional: Motivated inhibition and activation of conflicting stereotypes". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 77 (5): 885–904. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.77.5.885. PMID 10573871.
  66. ^ Stangor, C.; Sechrist, G. B.; Jost, J. T. (2001). "Changing racial beliefs by providing consensus information". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 27 (4): 486–496. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.297.266. doi:10.1177/0146167201274009. S2CID 144937203.
  67. ^ a b Dasgupta, N.; Asgari, S. (2004). "Seeing is believing: Exposure to counterstereotypic women leaders and its effect on the malleability of automatic gender stereotyping". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 40 (5): 642–658. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2004.02.003.
  68. ^ Macrae, C.; Bodenhausen, G. V.; Milne, A. B.; Thorn, T. J.; Castelli, L. (1997). "On the activation of social stereotypes: The moderating role of processing objectives". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 33 (5): 471–489. doi:10.1006/jesp.1997.1328.
  69. ^ Macrae, C.; Bodenhausen, G. V.; Milne, A. B. (1995). "The dissection of selection in person perception: Inhibitory processes in social stereotyping". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 69 (3): 397–407. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.69.3.397. PMID 7562387.
  70. ^ Macrae, C.Neil; Mitchell, Jason P.; Pendry, Louise F. (March 2002). "What's in a Forename? Cue Familiarity and Stereotypical Thinking". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 38 (2): 186–193. doi:10.1006/jesp.2001.1496. S2CID 34321387.
  71. ^ Vierkant, Tillmann; Hardt, Rosa (April 2015). "Explicit Reasons, Implicit Stereotypes and the Effortful Control of the Mind" (PDF). Ethical Theory and Moral Practice. 18 (2): 251–265. doi:10.1007/s10677-015-9573-9. S2CID 144268523.
  72. ^ Rubinstein, Rachel; Jussim, Lee (March 1, 2018). "Reliance on individuating information and stereotypes in implicit and explicit person perception". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 75: 54–70. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2017.11.009. S2CID 148592498.
  73. ^ Forscher, Patrick S.; Lai, Calvin K.; Axt, Jordan R.; Ebersole, Charles R.; Herman, Michelle; Devine, Patricia G.; Nosek, Brian A. (2019). "A Meta-Analysis of Procedures to Change Implicit Measures". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 117 (3): 522–559. doi:10.1037/pspa0000160. PMC 6687518. PMID 31192631.
  74. ^ Oswald, Frederick; Mitchell, Gregory; Blanton, Hart; Jaccard, James; Tetlock, Philip (June 17, 2013). "Predicting Ethnic and Racial Discrimination: A Meta-Analysis of IAT Criterion Studies". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 105 (2): 171–192. doi:10.1037/a0032734. PMID 23773046.
  75. ^ a b Donald, Heather Mac (October 9, 2017). "The False 'Science' of Implicit Bias". The Wall Street Journal. Retrieved September 2, 2018.
  76. ^ Machery, Edouard (2021). "Anomalies in implicit attitudes research". WIREs Cognitive Science. 13 (1): e1569. doi:10.1002/wcs.1569. PMID 34130361. S2CID 235451166.
  77. ^ Brownstein, Michael; Madva, Alex; Gawronski, Bertram (2019). "What do implicit measures measure?". WIREs Cognitive Science. 10 (5): e1501. doi:10.1002/wcs.1501. PMID 31034161. S2CID 139103999.
  78. ^ Byrd, Nick; Thompson, Morgan (2022). "Testing for implicit bias: Values, psychometrics, and science communication". WIREs Cognitive Science. 13 (5): e1612. doi:10.1002/wcs.1612. PMID 35671040. S2CID 249479726.
  79. ^ Greenwald, Anthony G.; Banaji, Mahzarin R.; Nosek, Brian A. (2015). "Statistically small effects of the Implicit Association Test can have societally large effects". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 108 (4): 553–561. doi:10.1037/pspa0000016. PMID 25402677. S2CID 16000675.
  80. ^ a b Fazio; Jackson; Dunton; Williams. . Archived from the original on 2013-10-23.
  81. ^ Gawronski, Bertram; Morrison, Mike; Phills, Curtis E.; Galdi, Silvia (March 2017). "Temporal Stability of Implicit and Explicit Measures: A Longitudinal Analysis". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 43 (3): 300–312. doi:10.1177/0146167216684131. PMID 28903689. S2CID 9090341.
  82. ^ Gawronski, Bertram; Ledgerwood, Alison; Eastwick, Paul W. (October 2020). "Implicit Bias and Antidiscrimination Policy". Policy Insights from the Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 7 (2): 99–106. doi:10.1177/2372732220939128. S2CID 222111501.

External links edit

  • Project implicit Take Implicit Association Tests to reveal your own implicit attitudes and stereotypes, as well as contribute to the research as a participant.
  • Are We All Unconscious Racists? No: there's scant evidence to support the trendy implicit-bias theory. Heather Mac Donald in City Journal.
  • Li Chen, Shuyu Zeng, Rui He; Negative Emotions Can Interfere with the Inhibitory Effect of Proactive Control on Implicit Stereotypes North American Academic Research, 4(4)53-69, April 2021

implicit, stereotype, broader, coverage, this, topic, stereotype, implicit, bias, implicit, stereotype, reflective, attribution, particular, qualities, individual, member, some, social, group, recent, studies, have, determined, that, implicit, bias, towards, t. For broader coverage of this topic see Stereotype An implicit bias or implicit stereotype is the pre reflective attribution of particular qualities by an individual to a member of some social out group 1 Recent studies have determined that implicit bias towards those of the opposite gender may be even more influential than racial implicit bias 2 Implicit stereotypes are thought to be shaped by experience and based on learned associations between particular qualities and social categories including race and or gender 3 Individuals perceptions and behaviors can be influenced by the implicit stereotypes they hold even if they are sometimes unaware they hold such stereotypes 4 Implicit bias is an aspect of implicit social cognition the phenomenon that perceptions attitudes and stereotypes can operate prior to conscious intention or endorsement 5 The existence of implicit bias is supported by a variety of scientific articles in psychological literature 6 Implicit stereotype was first defined by psychologists Mahzarin Banaji and Anthony Greenwald in 1995 Explicit stereotypes by contrast are consciously endorsed intentional and sometimes controllable thoughts and beliefs 7 Implicit biases however are thought to be the product of associations learned through past experiences 8 Implicit biases can be activated by the environment and operate prior to a person s intentional conscious endorsement 1 Implicit bias can persist even when an individual rejects the bias explicitly 1 Contents 1 Bias attitude stereotype and prejudice 2 Methods for investigation 2 1 Implicit association test 2 2 Go no go association task GNAT 2 3 Semantic priming and lexical decision task 2 4 Sentence completion 3 Differences between measures 4 Findings 4 1 Gender bias 4 1 1 Gender bias in science and engineering 4 2 Racial bias 4 3 In group and out group bias 4 4 Other stereotypes 4 5 Activation of implicit stereotypes 4 6 Malleability of implicit stereotypes 5 Criticism 5 1 Meta analyses 5 2 News outlets 5 3 Psychometric concerns 5 4 Statement by original authors 6 Summary 7 See also 8 References 9 External linksBias attitude stereotype and prejudice editAttitudes stereotypes prejudices and bias are all examples of psychological constructs Psychological constructs are mental associations that can influence a person s behavior and feelings toward an individual or group If the person is unaware of these mental associations the stereotypes prejudices or bias is said to be implicit Bias is defined as prejudice in favor of or against one thing person or group compared with another usually in a way considered to be unfair Bias can be seen as the overarching definition of stereotype and prejudice because it is how we associate traits usually negative to a specific group of people Our implicit attitudes reflect constant exposure to stereotypical portrayals of members of and items in all kinds of different categories racial groups professions women nationalities members of the LGBTQ community disabilities moral and political values etc 9 An attitude is an evaluative judgment of an object a person or a social group 10 An attitude is held by or characterizes a person Implicit attitudes are evaluations that occur without conscious awareness towards an attitude object or the self A stereotype is the association of a person or a social group with a consistent set of traits This may include both positive and negative traits such as African Americans are great at sports or African Americans are more violent than any other race in the United States There are many types of stereotypes that exists racial cultural gender group i e college students all being very explicit in the lives of many people Prejudice is defined as unfair negative attitude toward a social group or a member of that group 11 Prejudices can stem from many of the things that people observe in a different social group that include but are not limited to gender sex race ethnicity or religion This is pertinent to stereotypes because a stereotype can influence the way people feel toward another group hence prejudice Methods for investigation editThere is a clear challenge in measuring the degree to which someone is biased There are two different forms of bias implicit and explicit The two forms of bias are however connected Explicit bias encompasses our conscious attitudes which can be measured by self report but pose the potential of individuals falsely endorsing more socially desirable attitudes Although implicit biases have been considered unconscious and involuntary attitudes which lie below the surface of consciousness some people seem to be aware of their influence on their behavior and cognitive processes 12 The implicit association test IAT is one validated tool used to measure implicit bias The IAT requires participants to rapidly pair two social groups with either positive or negative attributes 13 Implicit association test edit The implicit association test IAT alleges to predict prejudice an individual has toward different social groups The test claims to do this by capturing the differences in the time it takes respondent to choose between two unassociated but related topics Respondents are instructed to click one of two computer keys to categorize stimuli into associated categories When the categories appear consistent to the respondent the time taken to categorize the stimuli will be less than when the categories seem inconsistent An implicit association is said to exist when respondents take longer to respond to a category inconsistent pairing than a category consistent pairing The implicit association test is used in psychology for a wide array of topics These fields include gender race science career weight sexuality and disability 14 While acclaimed and highly influential the implicit association test falls short of a strong scientific consensus Critics of the implicit association test cite studies that counterintuitively link biased test scores with less discriminatory behavior 15 Studies have also asserted that the implicit association test fails to measure unconscious thought 4 Go no go association task GNAT edit The GNAT is similar to the implicit association test Although the IAT reveals differential associations of two target concepts e g male female and weak strong the GNAT reveals associations within one concept for example whether female is associated more strongly with weak or strong 16 Participants are presented with word pairs among distractors Participants are instructed to indicate go if the words are target pairs or no go if they are not For example participants may be instructed to indicate go if the word pairs are female names and words that are related to strength Then participants are instructed to indicate go if the word pairs are female names and words that are related to weakness This method relies on signal detection theory participants accuracy rates reveal endorsement of the implicit stereotype For example if participants are more accurate for female weak pairs than for female strong pairs this suggests the subject more strongly associates weakness with females than strength 17 Semantic priming and lexical decision task edit Semantic priming measures the association between two concepts 18 In a lexical decision task subjects are presented with pair of words and asked to indicate whether the pair are words for example butter or non words for example tubter The theory behind semantic priming is that subjects are quicker to respond to a word if preceded by a word related to it in meaning e g bread butter vs bread dog 18 In other words the word bread primes other words related in meaning including butter Psychologists utilize semantic priming to reveal implicit associations between stereotypic congruent words For instance participants may be asked to indicate whether pronouns are male or female These pronouns are either preceded by professions that are predominantly female secretary nurse or male mechanic doctor Reaction times reveal strength of association between professions and gender 19 Sentence completion edit In a sentence completion task subjects may be presented with sentences that contain stereotypic black and white names Jerome Adam positive and negative stereotypic black behaviors easily made the team blasted loud music in his car and counter stereotypic behaviors got a job at Microsoft refused to dance Subjects are asked to add to the end of a sentence in any way that is grammatical e g Jerome got an A on his test could be completed with because it was easy stereotypic congruent or because he studied for months stereotypic incongruent or and then he went out to celebrate non explanatory This task is used to measure stereotypic explanatory bias SEB participants have a larger SEB if they give more explanations for stereotype congruent sentences than stereotype incongruent sentences and if they give more stereotypic congruent explanations 20 Differences between measures editThe Implicit Association Test IAT sequential priming and other implicit bias tests are mechanisms for determining how susceptible we are to stereotypes They are widely used in Social Psychology although measuring response time to a question as a good measure of implicit biases is still up for debate Some theorists do question the interpretation of the scores from tests such as the IAT but the debate is still going on and responses to the criticisms are certainly widespread 9 In qualitative market research researchers have described a framework called bias testing to mitigate researcher bias when designing survey questions It involves empirically testing the survey questions with real life respondents using interviewer moderated or technology enabled unmoderated techniques 21 Findings editGender bias edit See also Sexism Gender stereotypes and Gender role Gender stereotypes Gender biases are the stereotypical attitudes or prejudices that we have towards specific genders The concept of gender also refers to the constantly ongoing social construction of what is considered feminine and masculine and is based on power and sociocultural norms about women and men 22 Gender biases are the ways in which we judge men and women based on their hegemonically feminine and masculine assigned traits The category of male has been found to be associated with traits of strength and achievement Both male and female subjects associate male category members more strongly than female category members with words like bold mighty and power 23 The strength of this association is not predicted by explicit beliefs such as responses on a gender stereotype questionnaire for example one question asked if subjects endorsed the word feminist 1 In a test to reveal the false fame effect non famous male names are more likely to be falsely identified as famous than non famous female names this is evidence for an implicit stereotype of male achievement 24 Females are more associated with weakness This is true for both male and female subjects but female subjects only show this association when the weak words are positive such as fine flower and gentle female subjects do not show this pattern when the weak words are negative such as feeble frail and scrawny 23 Particular professions are implicitly associated with genders Elementary school teachers are implicitly stereotyped to be female and engineers are stereotyped to be male 25 Gender bias in science and engineering edit Implicit association tests reveal an implicit association for male with science and math and females with arts and language 26 Girls as young as nine years old have been found to hold an implicit male math stereotype and an implicit preference for language over math 27 Women have stronger negative associations with math than men do and the stronger females associate with a female gender identity the more implicit negativity they have towards math 26 For both men and women the strength of these implicit stereotypes predicts both implicit and explicit math attitudes belief in one s math ability and SAT performance 26 The strength of these implicit stereotypes in elementary aged girls predicts academic self concepts academic achievement and enrollment preferences even more than do explicit measures 27 Women with a stronger implicit gender math stereotype were less likely to pursue a math related career regardless of their actual math ability or explicit gender math stereotypes 28 This may be because women with stronger implicit gender math stereotypes are more at risk for stereotype threat Thus women with strong implicit stereotypes perform much worse on a math test when primed with gender than women who have weak implicit stereotypes 29 Though the number of women pursuing and earning degrees in engineering has increased in the last 20 years women are below men at all degree levels in all fields of engineering 30 These implicit gender stereotypes are robust in a study of more than 500 000 respondents from 34 nations more than 70 of individuals held this implicit stereotype 31 The national strength of the implicit stereotype is related to national sex differences among 8th graders on the International TIMSS a worldwide math amp science standardized achievement exam This effect is present even after statistically controlling for gender inequality in general 31 Additionally for women across cultures studies have shown individual differences in strength of this implicit stereotype is associated with interest participation and performance in sciences 31 Extending to the professional world implicit biases and subsequent explicit attitudes toward women can negatively affect the education hiring promotion and retention of women in STEM 32 The effects of such implicit biases can be seen in across multiple studies including Parents rate the math abilities of their daughters lower than parents with sons who perform identically well in school 33 College faculty are less likely to respond to inquiries about research opportunities if the email appears to be from a woman as opposed to an identical email from a man 34 Science faculty are less likely to hire or mentor students they believe are women as opposed to men 35 An interagency report from the Office of Science and Technology Policy and Office of Personnel Management has investigated systemic barriers including implicit biases that have traditionally inhibited particularly women and underrepresented minorities in science technology engineering and mathematics STEM and makes recommendations for reducing the impact of bias 36 Research has shown that implicit bias training may improve attitudes towards women in STEM 32 Racial bias edit Racial bias can be used synonymously with stereotyping and prejudice because it allows for the inclusion of both positive and negative evaluations related to perceptions of race 37 We begin to create racial biases towards other groups of people starting as young as age 3 creating an ingroup and outgroup view on members of various races usually starting with skin color In lexical decision tasks after subjects are subliminally primed with the word BLACK they are quicker to react to words consistent with black stereotypes such as athletic musical poor and promiscuous When subjects are subliminally primed with WHITE they are quicker to react to white stereotypes such as intelligent ambitious uptight and greedy 38 These tendencies are sometimes but not always associated with explicit stereotypes 38 39 People may also hold an implicit stereotype that associates black category members as violent People primed with words like ghetto slavery and jazz were more likely to interpret a character in a vignette as hostile 40 However this finding is controversial because the character s race was not specified it is suggested that the procedure primed the race unspecified concept of hostility and did not necessarily represent stereotypes 38 An implicit stereotype of violent black men may associate black men with weapons In a video game where subjects were supposed to shoot men with weapons and not shoot men with ordinary objects subjects were more likely to shoot a black man with an ordinary object than a white man with an ordinary object This tendency was related to subjects implicit attitudes toward black people Similar results were found in a priming task subjects who saw a black face immediately before either a weapon or an ordinary object more quickly and accurately identified the image as a weapon than when it was preceded by a white face 41 Implicit race stereotypes affect behaviors and perceptions When choosing between pairs of questions to ask a black interviewee one of which is congruent with racial stereotype people with a high stereotypic explanatory bias SEB are more likely to ask the racially congruent stereotype question In a related study subjects with a high SEB rated a black individual more negatively in an unstructured laboratory interaction 20 In group and out group bias edit Group prototypes define social groups through a collection of attributes that define both what representative group members have in common and what distinguishes the ingroup from relevant outgroups 42 In group favoritism sometimes known as in group out group bias in group bias or intergroup bias is a pattern of favoring members of one s in group over out group members This can be expressed in evaluation of others in allocation of resources and in many other ways 43 44 Implicit in group preferences emerge very early in life 45 even in children as young as six years old In group bias wherein people who are one of us i e our ingroup are favored compared to those in the outgroup meaning those who differ from ourselves 46 Ingroup favoritism is associated with feelings of trust and positive regard for ingroup members and surfaces often on measures of implicit bias This categorization ingroup vs outgroup is often automatic and pre conscious 47 The reasons for having in group and out group bias could be explained by ethnocentrism social categorization oxytocin etc A research paper done by Carsten De Dreu reviewed that oxytocin enables the development of trust specifically towards individuals with similar characteristics categorized as in group members promoting cooperation with and favoritism towards such individuals 48 People who report that they have strong needs for simplifying their environments also show more ingroup favoritism 49 The tendency to categorize into ingroups and outgroups and resulting ingroup favoritism is likely a universal aspect of human beings 50 We generally tend to hold implicit biases that favor our own ingroup though research has shown that we can still hold implicit biases against our ingroup 46 51 The most prominent example of negative affect towards an ingroup was recorded in 1939 by Kenneth and Mamie Clark using their now famous Dolls Test In this test African American children were asked to pick their favorite doll from a choice of otherwise identical black and white dolls A high percentage of these African American children indicated a preference for the white dolls 52 Social identity theory and Freudian theorists explain in group derogation as the result of a negative self image which they believe is then extended to the group 53 Other stereotypes edit Research on implicit stereotypes primarily focuses on gender and race However other topics such as age weight and profession have been investigated IATs have revealed implicit stereotypes reflecting explicit stereotypes about adolescents The results from these tests claim that adolescents are more likely to be associated with words like trendy and defiant than adults 54 In addition one IAT study revealed that older adults had a higher preference for younger adults compared to older adults and younger adults had a lower implicit preference for younger adults compared to older adults The study also found that women and participants with more education had lower implicit preference for younger adults 55 IATs have also revealed implicit stereotypes on the relationship between obese individuals and low work performance Words like lazy and incompetent are more associated with images of obese individuals than images of thin ones 56 This association is stronger for thin subjects than overweight ones 57 Like explicit stereotypes implicit stereotypes may contain both positive and negative traits This can be seen in examples of occupational implicit stereotypes where people perceive preschool teachers as both warm and incompetent while lawyers are judged as both cold and competent 58 Activation of implicit stereotypes edit Implicit stereotypes are activated by environmental and situational factors These associations develop over the course of a lifetime beginning at a very early age through exposure to direct and indirect messages In addition to early life experiences the media and news programming are often cited origins of implicit associations 59 In the laboratory implicit stereotypes are activated by priming When subjects are primed with dependence by unscrambling words such as dependent cooperative and passive they judge a target female as more dependent When subjects are primed with aggression with words like aggressive confident argumentative they judge a target male as more aggressive 60 The fact that females and words such as dependent cooperative and passive and males and words like aggressive confident argumentative are thought to be associated together suggest an implicit gender stereotype Stereotypes are also activated by a subliminal prime To exemplify white subjects exposed to subliminal words that consist of a black stereotype ghetto slavery jazz interpret a target male as more hostile consistent with the implicit stereotype of hostile black man 40 However this finding is controversial because the character s race is not specified Instead it is suggested that the procedure primed the race unspecified concept of hostility and did not necessarily represent stereotypes 38 By getting to know people who differ from oneself on a real personal level one can begin to build new associations about the groups those individuals represent and break down existing implicit associations 61 Malleability of implicit stereotypes edit Implicit stereotypes can at least temporarily be reduced or increased Most methods have been found to reduce implicit bias temporarily and are largely based on context 62 Some evidence suggests that implicit bias can be reduced long term but it may require education and consistent effort Some implicit bias training techniques designed to counteract implicit bias are stereotype replacement counter stereotypic imaging individuation perspective taking and increasing opportunities for contact 63 Stereotype replacement is when one replaces a stereotypical response with a non stereotypical response Counter stereotypic imagining is when one imagines others in a positive light and replace stereotypes with positive examples Individuation is when one focuses on specific details of a certain member of a group to avoid over generalizing Perspective taking is when one takes the perspective of a member of a marginalized group Increasing opportunities for contact is when one actively seeks out opportunities to engage in interactions with members of marginalized groups 63 Self and social motivesThe activation of implicit stereotypes may be decreased when the individual is motivated to promote a positive self image either to oneself or to others in a social setting There are two parts to this internal and external motivation Internal motivation is when an individual wants to be careful of what they say and external motivation is when an individual has a desire to respond in a politically correct way 64 Positive feedback from a black person decreases stereotypic sentence completion while negative feedback from a black person increases it 65 Subjects also reveal lesser strength of race stereotypes when they feel others disagree with the stereotypes 66 Motivated self regulation does not immediately reduce implicit bias It raises awareness of discrepancies when biases stand in the way of personal beliefs 64 Promote counterstereotypesImplicit stereotypes can be reduced by exposure to counterstereotypes Reading biographies of females in leadership roles such as Meg Whitman the CEO of eBay increases females associations between female names and words like leader determined and ambitious in a gender stereotype IAT 67 Attending a women s college where students are presumably more often exposed to women in leadership positions reduces associations between leadership and males after one year of schooling 67 Merely imagining a strong woman reduces implicit association between females and weakness and imagining storybook princesses increases the implicit association between females and weakness 17 Focus of attentionDiverting a participant s focus of attention can reduce implicit stereotypes Generally female primes facilitate reaction time to stereotypical female traits when participants are instructed to indicate whether the prime is animate When participants instead are instructed to indicate whether a white dot is present on the prime this diverts their focus of attention from the primes feminine features This successfully weakens the strength of the prime and thus weakening the strength of gender stereotypes 68 Configuration of stimulus cuesWhether stereotypes are activated depends on the context When presented with an image of a Chinese woman Chinese stereotypes were stronger after seeing her use chopsticks and female stereotypes were stronger after seeing her put on makeup 69 Characteristics of individual category membersStereotype activation may be stronger for some category members than for others People express weaker gender stereotypes with unfamiliar than familiar names 70 Judgments and gut reactions that go along with implicit biases are based on how familiar something is 71 Criticism editSome social psychology research has indicated that individuating information giving someone any information about an individual group member other than category information may eliminate the effects of stereotype bias 72 Meta analyses edit Researchers from the University of Wisconsin Madison Harvard and the University of Virginia examined 426 studies over 20 years involving 72 063 participants that used the IAT and other similar tests They concluded two things The correlation between implicit bias and discriminatory behavior appears weaker than previously thought There is little evidence that changes in implicit bias correlate with changes in a person s behavior 73 In a 2013 meta analysis Hart Blanton et al declared that despite its frequent misrepresentation as a proxy for the unconscious the IAT provides little insight into who will discriminate against whom and provides no more insight than explicit measures of bias 74 News outlets edit Heather Mac Donald writing in The Wall Street Journal noted that Few academic ideas have been as eagerly absorbed into public discourse lately as implicit bias Embraced by Barack Obama Hillary Clinton and most of the press implicit bias has spawned a multimillion dollar consulting industry along with a movement to remove the concept of individual agency from the law Yet its scientific basis is crumbling Mac Donald suggests there is still a political and economic drive to use the implicit bias paradigm as a political lever and to profit off entities that want to avoid litigation 75 Psychometric concerns edit Edouard Machery has argued that the use of indirect measures like the implicit association test is deeply problematic because the tests do not exhibit the psychometric properties we would expect from measures of attitudes 76 However many have already admitted that these indirect tests assess behavior rather than attitudes 77 This is an example of how the debate about implicit bias can involve talking past one another based on different expectations of indirect measures views of what implicit bias is assumptions about which evidence is relevant thresholds for scientific significance psychometric standards and even norms of science communication 78 So evaluating debates about tests of implicit bias requires one to pay careful attention to debators background assumptions and whether or how well debators justify those assumptions Statement by original authors edit Where previously Greenwald and Banaji asserted in their book BlindSpot 2013 Given the relatively small proportion of people who are overtly prejudiced and how clearly it is established that automatic race preference predicts discrimination it is reasonable to conclude not only that implicit bias is a cause of Black disadvantage but also that it plausibly plays a greater role than does explicit bias 75 The evidence presented by their peer researchers led them to concede in correspondence that The IAT does not predict biased behaviour in laboratory settings It is problematic to use the IAT to classify persons as likely to engage in discrimination However they also stated Regardless of inclusion policy both meta analyses estimated aggregate correlational effect sizes that were large enough to explain discriminatory impacts that are societally significant either because they can affect many people simultaneously or because they can repeatedly affect single persons 79 Summary editThis section has multiple issues Please help improve it or discuss these issues on the talk page Learn how and when to remove these template messages This section may require cleanup to meet Wikipedia s quality standards The specific problem is the lead section should be the summary of the article this section should be merged into the rest of this article Please help improve this section if you can March 2015 Learn how and when to remove this template message This section may contain information not important or relevant to the article s subject Please help improve this section December 2019 Learn how and when to remove this template message Learn how and when to remove this template message Implicit bias is thought to be the product of positive or negative mental associations about persons things or groups that are formed and activated pre consciously or subconsciously In 1995 researchers Banaji and Greenwald noted that someone s social learning experiences such as observing parents friends or others could create this type of association and therefore trigger this type of bias Many studies have found that culture is able to stimulate biases as well both in a negative and positive way regardless someone s personal experience with other cultures 80 As far as many people are concerned implicit bias knows no age restriction and it can be held by anyone regardless of their age In fact implicit biases can be found in a person as young as six years old 80 Even though implicit bias may be difficult to catch especially compared to explicit bias it can be measured through a number of mechanisms such as sequential priming response competition EDA EMG fMRI ERP and ITA 81 Thus once a person becomes aware of their own bias they can take action to change it if they wish 82 The existence of implicitly biased behavior is supported by several articles in psychological literature Adults and even children may hold implicit stereotypes of social categories categories to which they may themselves belong to Without intention or even awareness implicit stereotypes affect human behavior and judgments This has wide ranging implications for society from discrimination and personal career choices to understanding others in social interactions each day 1 27 24 40 60 See also editStereotypes Implicit cognition Stereotype threat Implicit attitude Alief mental state Unconscious bias training Systemic bias Cognitive bias Intergroup relationsReferences edit a b c d e Greenwald Anthony G Banaji Mahzarin R 1995 Implicit social cognition Attitudes self esteem and stereotypes Psychological Review 102 1 4 27 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 411 2919 doi 10 1037 0033 295x 102 1 4 PMID 7878162 S2CID 8194189 Pro female and anti male biases are more influential than race and other factors in Implicit Association Tests Psypost org In Cognitive Science Social Psychology By Eric W Dolan March 26 2023 Retrieved March 12 2024 Byrd Nick February 2021 What we can and can t infer about implicit bias from debiasing experiments Synthese 198 2 1427 1455 doi 10 1007 s11229 019 02128 6 S2CID 60441599 a b Hahn Adam Judd Charles M Hirsh Holen K Blair Irene V June 2014 Awareness of implicit attitudes Journal of Experimental Psychology General 143 3 1369 1392 doi 10 1037 a0035028 PMC 4038711 PMID 24294868 Gawronski Bertram June 10 2019 Six Lessons for a Cogent Science of Implicit Bias and Its Criticism Perspectives on Psychological Science 14 4 574 595 doi 10 1177 1745691619826015 PMID 31181174 Jost John T Rudman Laurie A Blair Irene V Carney Dana R Dasgupta Nilanjana Glaser Jack Hardin Curtis D 2009 The existence of implicit bias is beyond reasonable doubt A refutation of ideological and methodological objections and executive summary of ten studies that no manager should ignore Research in Organizational Behavior 29 39 69 doi 10 1016 j riob 2009 10 001 Gaertner Brown Sam Rupert April 15 2008 Blackwell Handbook of Social Psychology Intergroup Processes John Wiley amp Sons ISBN 9780470692707 Retrieved August 11 2013 a href Template Cite book html title Template Cite book cite book a CS1 maint multiple names authors list link Del Pinal Guillermo Spaulding Shannon February 2018 Conceptual centrality and implicit bias Mind amp Language 33 1 95 111 doi 10 1111 mila 12166 hdl 2027 42 142467 a b Toribio Josefa March 1 2018 Implicit Bias from social structure to representational format Theoria 33 1 41 doi 10 1387 theoria 17751 hdl 2445 145839 Crano W D amp Prislin R 2008 Attitudes and attitude change New York CRC Press Gaertner Samuel L Dovidio John F 1999 Reducing Prejudice Combating Intergroup Biases Current Directions in Psychological Science 8 4 101 105 doi 10 1111 1467 8721 00024 JSTOR 20182575 S2CID 31743564 Hahn Adam Gawronski Bertram May 2019 Facing one s implicit biases From awareness to acknowledgment Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 116 5 769 794 doi 10 1037 pspi0000155 PMID 30359070 S2CID 51896790 Maina Ivy W Belton Tanisha D Ginzberg Sara Singh Ajit Johnson Tiffani J February 2018 A decade of studying implicit racial ethnic bias in healthcare providers using the implicit association test Social Science amp Medicine 199 219 229 doi 10 1016 j socscimed 2017 05 009 PMID 28532892 Harris Matthew Macinko James Jimenez Geronimo Mullachery Pricila December 2017 Measuring the bias against low income country research an Implicit Association Test Globalization and Health 13 1 80 doi 10 1186 s12992 017 0304 y PMC 5674740 PMID 29110668 Shelton J N Richeson J A Salvatore J Trawalter S May 1 2005 Ironic Effects of Racial Bias During Interracial Interactions Psychological Science 16 5 397 402 doi 10 1111 j 0956 7976 2005 01547 x PMID 15869700 S2CID 15621035 Nosek Brian A Banaji Mahzarin R December 2001 The Go No Go Association Task Social Cognition 19 6 625 666 doi 10 1521 soco 19 6 625 20886 a b Blair Irene V Ma Jennifer E Lenton Alison P 2001 Imagining stereotypes away The moderation of implicit stereotypes through mental imagery Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 81 5 828 841 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 555 2509 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 81 5 828 PMID 11708560 a b Meyer David E Schvaneveldt Roger W 1971 Facilitation in recognizing pairs of words Evidence of a dependence between retrieval operations Journal of Experimental Psychology 90 2 227 234 doi 10 1037 h0031564 PMID 5134329 S2CID 36672941 Banaji Mahzarin R Hardin Curtis D May 1996 Automatic Stereotyping Psychological Science 7 3 136 141 doi 10 1111 j 1467 9280 1996 tb00346 x S2CID 207858486 a b Sekaquaptewa Denise Espinoza Penelope Thompson Mischa Vargas Patrick von Hippel William January 2003 Stereotypic explanatory bias Implicit stereotyping as a predictor of discrimination Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 39 1 75 82 doi 10 1016 S0022 1031 02 00512 7 Geisen Emily Sha Mandy Roper Farren 2024 Bias testing in market research A framework to enable inclusive research design published January 3 2024 ISBN 979 8862902785 Hamberg Katrina May 2008 Gender bias in medicine Women s Health 4 3 237 243 doi 10 2217 17455057 4 3 237 PMID 19072473 a b Rudman L A Greenwald A G McGhee D E 2001 Implicit self concept and evaluative implicit gender stereotypes Self and ingroup share desirable traits Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 27 9 1164 1178 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 43 6589 doi 10 1177 0146167201279009 S2CID 13491811 a b Banaji M R Greenwald A G 1995 Implicit gender stereotyping in judgments of fame Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 68 2 181 198 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 74 895 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 68 2 181 PMID 7877095 White M J White G B 2006 Implicit and explicit occupational gender stereotypes Sex Roles 55 3 4 259 266 doi 10 1007 s11199 006 9078 z S2CID 143793226 a b c Nosek B A Banaji M R Greenwald A G 2002 Math male me female therefore math me Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 83 1 44 59 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 463 6120 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 83 1 44 PMID 12088131 S2CID 204321795 a b c Steffens M C Jelenec P Noack P 2010 On the leaky math pipeline Comparing implicit math gender stereotypes and math withdrawal in female and male children and adolescents Journal of Educational Psychology 102 4 947 963 doi 10 1037 a0019920 Kiefer Amy K Sekaquaptewa Denise January 2007 Implicit Stereotypes Gender Identification and Math Related Outcomes A Prospective Study of Female College Students Psychological Science 18 1 13 18 doi 10 1111 j 1467 9280 2007 01841 x PMID 17362371 S2CID 30588471 Kiefer A K Sekaquaptewa D 2007 Implicit stereotypes and women s math performance How implicit gender math stereotypes influence women s susceptibility to stereotype threat Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 43 5 825 832 doi 10 1016 j jesp 2006 08 004 Women Minorities and Persons with Disabilities in Science and Engineering National Science Foundation Retrieved March 31 2017 a b c Nosek B A Smyth F L Sriram N N Lindner N M Devos T Ayala A Greenwald A G 2009 National differences in gender science stereotypes predict national sex differences in science and math achievement Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 106 26 10593 10597 Bibcode 2009PNAS 10610593N doi 10 1073 pnas 0809921106 PMC 2705538 PMID 19549876 a b Jackson Sarah M Hillard Amy L Schneider Tamera R September 2014 Using implicit bias training to improve attitudes toward women in STEM Social Psychology of Education 17 3 419 438 doi 10 1007 s11218 014 9259 5 S2CID 53500821 Yee Doris K Eccles Jacquelynne S September 1988 Parent perceptions and attributions for children s math achievement Sex Roles 19 5 6 317 333 doi 10 1007 bf00289840 hdl 2027 42 45585 S2CID 112300 Milkman Katherine L Akinola Modupe Chugh Dolly 2015 What happens before A field experiment exploring how pay and representation differentially shape bias on the pathway into organizations Journal of Applied Psychology 100 6 1678 1712 doi 10 1037 apl0000022 PMID 25867167 Moss Racusin C A Dovidio J F Brescoll V L Graham M J Handelsman J October 9 2012 Science faculty s subtle gender biases favor male students Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 109 41 16474 16479 Bibcode 2012PNAS 10916474M doi 10 1073 pnas 1211286109 PMC 3478626 PMID 22988126 Handelsman Jo Ward Wanda December 12 2016 Increasing Diversity in the STEM Workforce by Reducing the Impact of Bias The White House Retrieved March 31 2017 Noles Erica May 1 2014 What s age got to do with it Examining how the age of stimulus faces affects children s implicit racial bias UNLV Theses Dissertations Professional Papers and Capstones Thesis University of Nevada Las Vegas doi 10 34917 5836145 ProQuest 1566943023 a b c d Wittenbrink B Judd C M Park B 1997 Evidence for racial prejudice at the implicit level and its relationship with questionnaire measures Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 72 2 262 274 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 462 7827 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 72 2 262 PMID 9107001 Gaertner S L McLaughlin J P 1983 Racial stereotypes Associations and ascriptions of positive and negative characteristics Social Psychology Quarterly 46 1 23 30 doi 10 2307 3033657 JSTOR 3033657 a b c Devine P G 1989 Stereotypes and prejudice Their automatic and controlled components Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 56 5 18 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 56 1 5 S2CID 33975027 Staats Cheryl Patton Charles 2013 State of the Science Implicit Bias Review PDF Kirwan Institute for the Study of Race and Ethnicity at the Ohio State University pp 3 37 39 Hohman Zachary P Gaffney Amber M Hogg Michael A September 2017 Who Am I If I Am Not like My Group Self Uncertainty and Feeling Peripheral in a Group Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 72 125 132 doi 10 1016 j jesp 2017 05 002 Aronson E Wilson T D amp Akert R 2010 Social psychology 7th ed Upper Saddle River Prentice Hall page needed Taylor Donald M Doria Janet R April 1981 Self Serving and Group Serving Bias in Attribution The Journal of Social Psychology 113 2 201 211 doi 10 1080 00224545 1981 9924371 Dunham Y Baron A S Banaji M R 2008 The Development of Implicit Intergroup Cognition PDF Trends in Cognitive Sciences 12 7 248 253 doi 10 1016 j tics 2008 04 006 PMID 18555736 S2CID 45599721 a b Greenwald A G Krieger L H 2006 Implicit Bias Scientific Foundations California Law Review 94 4 945 967 doi 10 2307 20439056 hdl 10125 66105 JSTOR 20439056 Reskin B 2000 The Proximate Causes of Employment Discrimination Contemporary Sociology 29 2 319 328 doi 10 2307 2654387 JSTOR 2654387 S2CID 2003571 De Dreu Carsten K W 2012 Oxytocin modulates cooperation within and competition between groups An integrative review and research agenda Hormones and Behavior 61 3 419 428 doi 10 1016 j yhbeh 2011 12 009 PMID 22227278 S2CID 140120921 Strangor Charles Leary Scott P 2006 Intergroup beliefs Investigations from the social side Vol 38 pp 243 281 doi 10 1016 S0065 2601 06 38005 7 ISBN 9780120152384 a href Template Cite book html title Template Cite book cite book a journal ignored help Brewer Marilynn B 2002 The psychology of prejudice Ingroup love or outgroup hate Journal of Social Issues 55 3 429 444 doi 10 1111 0022 4537 00126 Reskin Barbara 2005 Unconsciousness Raising PDF Regional Review 2005 14 3 32 37 Clark Kenneth B Clark Mamie P 1950 Emotional Factors in Racial Identification and Preference in Negro Children The Journal of Negro Education 19 3 341 350 doi 10 2307 2966491 JSTOR 2966491 Ma Kellams Christine Spencer Rodgers Julie Peng Kaiping January 2011 I Am Against Us Unpacking Cultural Differences in Ingroup Favoritism via Dialecticism Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 37 1 15 27 doi 10 1177 0146167210388193 PMID 21084525 S2CID 18906927 Gross E F Hardin C D 2007 Implicit and explicit stereotyping of adolescents Social Justice Research 20 2 140 160 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 514 3597 doi 10 1007 s11211 007 0037 9 S2CID 46261770 Chopik William J Giasson Hannah L August 1 2017 Age Differences in Explicit and Implicit Age Attitudes Across the Life Span The Gerontologist 57 suppl 2 S169 S177 doi 10 1093 geront gnx058 PMC 5881761 PMID 28854609 Agerstrom J Rooth D 2011 The role of automatic obesity stereotypes in real hiring discrimination Journal of Applied Psychology 96 4 790 805 doi 10 1037 a0021594 PMID 21280934 Schwartz M B Vartanian L R Nosek B A Brownell K D 2006 The Influence of One s Own Body Weight on Implicit and Explicit Anti fat Bias Obesity 14 3 440 447 doi 10 1038 oby 2006 58 PMID 16648615 Carlsson Rickard Bjorklund Fredrick 2010 Implicit stereotype content mixed stereotypes can be measured with the implicit association test Social Psychology 41 4 213 222 doi 10 1027 1864 9335 a000029 S2CID 144740592 Understanding Implicit Bias kirwaninstitute osu edu Retrieved April 6 2018 a b Banaji M R Hardin C Rothman A J 1993 Implicit stereotyping in person judgment Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 65 2 272 281 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 65 2 272 S2CID 53051217 Cheryl Staats Winter 2015 2016 Understanding Implicit Bias PDF American Educator pp 29 43 Retrieved December 3 2022 Calanchini Jimmy Lai Calvin K Klauer Karl Christoph August 27 2020 Reducing implicit racial preferences III A process level examination of changes in implicit preferences Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 121 4 796 818 doi 10 1037 pspi0000339 PMID 32852973 S2CID 221357555 a b Devine Patricia G Forscher Patrick S Austin Anthony J Cox William T L November 2012 Long term reduction in implicit race bias A prejudice habit breaking intervention Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 48 6 1267 1278 doi 10 1016 j jesp 2012 06 003 PMC 3603687 PMID 23524616 a b Burns Mason D Monteith Margo J Parker Laura R November 2017 Training away bias The differential effects of counterstereotype training and self regulation on stereotype activation and application Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 73 97 110 doi 10 1016 j jesp 2017 06 003 Sinclair L Kunda Z 1999 Reactions to a Black professional Motivated inhibition and activation of conflicting stereotypes Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 77 5 885 904 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 77 5 885 PMID 10573871 Stangor C Sechrist G B Jost J T 2001 Changing racial beliefs by providing consensus information Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 27 4 486 496 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 297 266 doi 10 1177 0146167201274009 S2CID 144937203 a b Dasgupta N Asgari S 2004 Seeing is believing Exposure to counterstereotypic women leaders and its effect on the malleability of automatic gender stereotyping Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 40 5 642 658 doi 10 1016 j jesp 2004 02 003 Macrae C Bodenhausen G V Milne A B Thorn T J Castelli L 1997 On the activation of social stereotypes The moderating role of processing objectives Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 33 5 471 489 doi 10 1006 jesp 1997 1328 Macrae C Bodenhausen G V Milne A B 1995 The dissection of selection in person perception Inhibitory processes in social stereotyping Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 69 3 397 407 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 69 3 397 PMID 7562387 Macrae C Neil Mitchell Jason P Pendry Louise F March 2002 What s in a Forename Cue Familiarity and Stereotypical Thinking Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 38 2 186 193 doi 10 1006 jesp 2001 1496 S2CID 34321387 Vierkant Tillmann Hardt Rosa April 2015 Explicit Reasons Implicit Stereotypes and the Effortful Control of the Mind PDF Ethical Theory and Moral Practice 18 2 251 265 doi 10 1007 s10677 015 9573 9 S2CID 144268523 Rubinstein Rachel Jussim Lee March 1 2018 Reliance on individuating information and stereotypes in implicit and explicit person perception Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 75 54 70 doi 10 1016 j jesp 2017 11 009 S2CID 148592498 Forscher Patrick S Lai Calvin K Axt Jordan R Ebersole Charles R Herman Michelle Devine Patricia G Nosek Brian A 2019 A Meta Analysis of Procedures to Change Implicit Measures Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 117 3 522 559 doi 10 1037 pspa0000160 PMC 6687518 PMID 31192631 Oswald Frederick Mitchell Gregory Blanton Hart Jaccard James Tetlock Philip June 17 2013 Predicting Ethnic and Racial Discrimination A Meta Analysis of IAT Criterion Studies Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 105 2 171 192 doi 10 1037 a0032734 PMID 23773046 a b Donald Heather Mac October 9 2017 The False Science of Implicit Bias The Wall Street Journal Retrieved September 2 2018 Machery Edouard 2021 Anomalies in implicit attitudes research WIREs Cognitive Science 13 1 e1569 doi 10 1002 wcs 1569 PMID 34130361 S2CID 235451166 Brownstein Michael Madva Alex Gawronski Bertram 2019 What do implicit measures measure WIREs Cognitive Science 10 5 e1501 doi 10 1002 wcs 1501 PMID 31034161 S2CID 139103999 Byrd Nick Thompson Morgan 2022 Testing for implicit bias Values psychometrics and science communication WIREs Cognitive Science 13 5 e1612 doi 10 1002 wcs 1612 PMID 35671040 S2CID 249479726 Greenwald Anthony G Banaji Mahzarin R Nosek Brian A 2015 Statistically small effects of the Implicit Association Test can have societally large effects Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 108 4 553 561 doi 10 1037 pspa0000016 PMID 25402677 S2CID 16000675 a b Fazio Jackson Dunton Williams Implicit Bias Archived from the original on 2013 10 23 Gawronski Bertram Morrison Mike Phills Curtis E Galdi Silvia March 2017 Temporal Stability of Implicit and Explicit Measures A Longitudinal Analysis Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 43 3 300 312 doi 10 1177 0146167216684131 PMID 28903689 S2CID 9090341 Gawronski Bertram Ledgerwood Alison Eastwick Paul W October 2020 Implicit Bias and Antidiscrimination Policy Policy Insights from the Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 2 99 106 doi 10 1177 2372732220939128 S2CID 222111501 External links editProject implicit Take Implicit Association Tests to reveal your own implicit attitudes and stereotypes as well as contribute to the research as a participant Are We All Unconscious Racists No there s scant evidence to support the trendy implicit bias theory Heather Mac Donald in City Journal Li Chen Shuyu Zeng Rui He Negative Emotions Can Interfere with the Inhibitory Effect of Proactive Control on Implicit Stereotypes North American Academic Research 4 4 53 69 April 2021 Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Implicit stereotype amp oldid 1215453413, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.