fbpx
Wikipedia

Implicit attitude

Implicit attitudes are evaluations that occur without conscious awareness towards an attitude object or the self. These evaluations are generally either favorable or unfavorable and come about from various influences in the individual experience.[1] The commonly used definition of implicit attitude within cognitive and social psychology comes from Anthony Greenwald and Mahzarin Banaji's template for definitions of terms related to implicit cognition:[2] "Implicit attitudes are introspectively unidentified (or inaccurately identified) traces of past experience that mediate favorable or unfavorable feeling, thought, or action toward social objects".[3] These thoughts, feelings or actions have an influence on behavior that the individual may not be aware of.[4]

An attitude is differentiated from the concept of a stereotype in that it functions as a broad favorable or unfavorable characteristic towards a social object, whereas a stereotype is a set of favorable and/or unfavorable characteristics which are applied to an individual based on social group membership.

Causes and manifestations edit

A number of different theories have been proposed relating to the formation, development, and influence of implicit attitudes.

Halo effect edit

Based on many empirical findings, Greenwald and Banaji et al. (1995) generated the fundamental idea of implicit attitude definitively for the first time, disambiguating attitude into explicit and implicit types.[3] Halo effects are an example of the empirical research used by Greenwald and Banaji in their chapter on implicit social cognition. Understanding halo effects set the foundation for understanding other theories regarding implicit attitudes. For example, it is possible to explain implicit partisanship or implicit egotism in terms of a halo effect, however these concepts will be discussed more in subsequent sections.

Pioneered by Edward Thorndike in 1920, the halo effect is the judgement of attribute "A" being influenced by a known but irrelevant attribute "B". For example, subsequent replications commonly use physical attractiveness as attribute "B" and attribute "A" being a judgement of the subject. More specifically a study Landy and Sigall et al. (1974) found that essays written by female essayists were found to be of higher quality when a photo showed the essayist as being attractive (rather than unattractive) when rated by male judges.[3]

Greenwald and Banaji et al. (1995) have suggested that attribute "B" is in fact an implicit attitude when the judge or subject cannot identify attribute "B" as the source of the judgement for attribute "A". Moreover, when attribute "B" is associated with a positive or negative attitude and additionally is unknowingly and automatically transferred onto attribute "A", that attitude of attribute "B" is known to be an implicit attitude.[3]

Experiences and socialization edit

Earlier research findings on implicit attitudes show that socialization[5] and reflections of past experiences[3] may be responsible for the development or manifestation of longer lasting implicit attitudes. As an example, Rudman and Goodwin et al. found in 2004 that individuals who were primarily raised by their mothers showed a more positive implicit attitude towards women rather than men.[6] Furthermore, Olson and Fazio et al. in 2001 and 2002 suggested that these implicit attitudes are a result of repeated pairings of positive or negative stimuli with an object; more pairings of positive stimuli would result in a more positive implicit attitude and vice versa.[7][8] This finding supports the fundamental principles of classical conditioning.[1]

Implicit attitudes are also developed by more recent experiences as well. For example, Rudmore, Ashmore, & Gary et al. in 2001 found that implicit attitude of prejudice against African Americans could be shaped through diversity training intervention using variables at an emotional level rather than increased awareness of bias which helped explicit attitude more.[9][1]

Implicit attitudes related to the self edit

Self-related objects are anything that pertains to the self; including in-groups and self-esteem (attitude towards the self).

Early research by Nuttin et al. in 1985 suggested that people generally have an implicit preference for letters in their own name, known as the Name letter effect.[10] Further replications of this same effect with varying independent variables (e.g., attractiveness to people with the same letters contained in their names) suggest that people have an implicit preference towards themselves. This manifestation of implicit attitude has come to be known as Implicit egotism. Implicit egotism additionally manifests itself in in-groups.[1]

Implicit partisanship is the heightened attractiveness and identification to a self-related group and negative or neutral attitudes towards non-self-related groups. Greenwald, Pickrell, and Farnham et al. demonstrated this effect in 2002, even when the groups were cooperative and when the members of the groups were non-human.[11] Much of the research on implicit partisanship suggests that this is an uncontrollable process, or an implicit attitude towards self-related groups.[1]

Culture and social norms edit

Generally speaking, culture and social norms have an effect on implicit attitude in the same way experiences and socialization have an effect on implicit attitude. However, culture has a very noticeable effect on implicit attitude in the way implicit attitude differs from one's explicit attitude. In 2002, Livingston et al. examined the effect of mainstream culture on one's implicit attitude towards their social group.[12] Implicitly, one will follow the cultural attitudes towards their social group that they perceive from mainstream culture in their society whether that be positive or negative. With that said, a strong cultural disadvantage (e.g., negative attitude) can effectively eliminate in-group favoritism when tested at the implicit level. However it may be important to note that at the explicit attitude level, these individuals still showed positive attitudes towards their social group. Olson and Fazio et al. suggested in 2004 that at an implicit level one's personal attitude can be influenced by the social or cultural norms that one perceives.[13] Furthermore, this may be due to a weak distinction between one's personal attitude and extrapersonal associations (e.g., one's cultural evaluations) towards an attitude object at the implicit level. Therefore, implicit attitudes are reflective of experiences but can also be shaped by the cultural context.[1]

Degree of awareness edit

Current research supports the idea that there are three different aspects of attitudes captured by current indirect measures that could be outside of conscious awareness: the source, the content, and the impact of an attitude.[14] Source awareness is described as the "awareness of the origin of a particular attitude" (emphasis added).[14] Content awareness is differentiated from source awareness by the lack of awareness about the attitude, rather than simply its origin. Finally, one may have awareness of both the attitude and its source but the attitude may still have influences on thought or behavior beyond ones awareness; this can be thought of as impact awareness.[14] Conclusions have been made that both indirectly assessed and self-reported attitudes can be characterized by lack of source awareness, there is no evidence for lack of content awareness of indirectly assessed attitudes, and there is some evidence showing that indirectly assessed, but not self-reported, attitudes can be characterized by lack of impact awareness.[14] The most compelling evidence for content awareness of implicit attitudes has showed that people are highly accurate in predicting their scores on the Implicit Association Test.[15]

Flexibility edit

Recent research indicates the possibility of the malleability of implicit attitudes based on situational context.[1] That is, implicit attitudes are not believed to be stable representations of memory, rather they are constructed based on the type of available information in a given situation. Available information can vary in context to the individual, though it is believed to serve as a prime to their behaviors. Flexibility of implicit attitudes is best demonstrated through measures that include accessibility effects. For example, it has been demonstrated that the information given to an individual prior to completing an implicit measure directly affects their response based on the information they were given.[1] Therefore, if an individual is primed with information regarding the positive, or negative, attributions of a different race and then asked to complete an implicit measures task, the participants will most likely use the information that was presented during the prime and not their own experienced information to assess the situation. This occurs because the information that was primed is most available for the participant to access without having to use conscious resources.

Effects on behavior edit

The fundamental goal of measuring implicit attitudes is to use it to predict behavior; behaviors that can't be predicted by knowledge of explicitly held attitudes. Numerous studies, such as research conducted by Chen and Bargh in 1999,[16] show that automatic evaluations triggered by various attitudes towards objects directly affected behavioral predispositions towards that object. Stimuli that elicited positive attitudes produced immediate positive behavior whilst stimuli that elicited negative attitudes triggered immediate avoidance behavior. The individuals are completely unaware of the operations that their behavioral responses because they are automatic and unconscious. In Bassenoff and Sherman et al. (2000) they found that automatic negative attitudes about overweight individuals directly predicted how far participants choose to sit from a fat woman, who they were expected to interact with. We see this phenomenon also with implicitly held racial attitudes as shown by McConnell and Leibold et al. (2001). These implicit attitudes affected how long they interacted for, how much participants smiled, how many speech errors they made and how many social comments were made. All automatic behavioral responses that measuring explicit attitudes could not predict.[1]

Types of behavior affected by implicit attitudes edit

Implicit attitudes aren't always better at predicting behavior than explicit measurements, they both play a systematic role in predicting behavior. Implicit attitudes are typically better than explicit attitudes at predicting behavior that is automatic and spontaneous. In line with Dual process theories such as Fazio's MODE model, automatic attitudes determine spontaneous actions, whereas deliberative actions reflect a contribution of multiple processes, including more controlled processes (e.g., a person's motivation to overcome prejudiced responses).[17] As demonstrated by Dasgupta an Rivera et al. (2006), individuals who endorsed traditional beliefs about gender and sexuality were friendlier towards gay confederates verbally but showed negative non-verbal behavior, this suggested that this individuals were consciously over-correcting their behavior but their prejudice leaked out through automatic responses like blinking and eye contact.[1]

Effects of motivation edit

Although, research has shown that motivation and an opportunity to react carefully can affect how much implicit attitudes influence behavioral response.[18] When individuals are highly motivated to control their responses and processing abilities are not lacking or preoccupied, behavioral responses tend to reflect intentional processes. In 2003, Towles-schwen and Fazio measured anticipated willingness and discomfort of participants to interact with a black person.[19] Individuals who were motivated to avoid interracial conflicts and where not concerned about seeming biased expressed their discomfort; whereas individuals who were concerned about not appearing biased reported less anticipated discomfort, in an attempt to hide their prejudice. Motivation to control our responses can minimize the influence of implicit attitudes on behavioral responses as shown by that example.[1]

Measurement edit

There is an assortment of different experimental tests that assess for the presence of implicit attitudes, including the implicit association test, evaluative and semantic priming tasks, the Extrinsic Affective Simon Task, Go/No-Go Association Task, and the Affect Misattribution Procedure. Though these tests vary in administration, and content, the basis of each is to "allow investigators to capture attitudes that individuals are unwilling to report."[20] Unwillingness and lack of ability are intertwined considering most individuals are unaware that these attitudes even exist. The following are brief descriptions about these measurements, which are most commonly used to assess implicit attitudes, and the empirical evidence that supports them.

Implicit Association Test edit

The Implicit Association Test is a latency-based measure of the relative associations between two concepts. In a series of tasks, participants sort words or images representing a target concept such as race (white/black) and stimuli with known positive/negative valence into two categories (usually indicated by right or left location on a computer screen). Each category of concept words or images is paired with both positive and negative stimuli. The faster the categorization occurs, the stronger the association is between words and/or images that are grouped together (ex. faster categorization of dogs when paired with positive rather than negative words), which would indicate an implicit attitude towards that object.[21] A full demonstration of the IAT procedure can be found at the Project Implicit link and the IAT Inquisit link below.

Research using the IAT measure of implicit attitudes has demonstrated consistent experimental and population-based attitudes with respect to concepts such as gender, race, and age. An analysis from the Project Implicit database found that science-gender stereotypes are predictive of differences in gender related math and science performance across countries in an international sample.[22] Research has also successfully used the IAT in consumer research.[23] Implicit attitude also directly drives the use of information systems[24] and serves as a basis upon which use habit is formed.[25]

Evaluative priming task edit

Research using the evaluative priming task has been frequently used in research on eating and attitudes towards food.[26] In clinical studies, the procedure was used to study attitudes of those diagnosed with eating disorders such as anorexia nervosa.[27] Along with many of the other methods presented here, researchers have used the procedure to measure the effects of stereotypes, including measurement of the effectiveness of stereotype reduction treatments.[28]

Semantic priming task edit

In the semantic priming task paradigm described by Wittenbrink et al. (1997), participants are shown a word prime at intervals which are too brief for reported awareness (see subliminal stimuli).[29] The word prime consists of two groups of words representing the concept in question (such as black sounding names or white sounding names). Participants were then asked to complete a lexical decision task (LDT) to identify if target stimuli are words or a non-words. The target stimuli consist of words with known positive or negative valence. When words with positive valence are categorized more quickly in the presence of one group of word primes (such as black sounding names), this indicates positive attitudes towards the group.

Extrinsic Affective Simon Task (EAST) edit

In the Extrinsic Affective Simon Task (EAST), participants categorized stimuli which consisted of words that either had positive or negative valence that were presented in either the color white or two different colors.[30] When the words are presented in white, participants categorize based words on their perceived positive or negative valence. When the words are presented in color, participants are asked to categorize based on color alone and ignore word meaning. When colored words are presented, categorization accuracy and speed are facilitated when, for words which the respondent has a positive implicit attitude, the response was the same as was expected for white words with obvious positive valence.[30] A full demonstration of the EAST procedure can be found in the external links below.

The EAST has been used in research of attitudes of those who have specific phobias and/or anxiety.[31][32] Additionally, the test has been recently used to measure implicit attitudes towards alcohol in populations who have substance abuse problems; and the test has been cited as having relatively high predictive value for problem substance use.[33]

Go/No-go Association Task (GNAT) edit

In practice, the GNAT appears similar to the Implicit Association Test in that participants are asked to categorize targets representing either a concept (such as race; ex. white or black names) or words which have obvious positive or negative valence. Participants are asked to respond ('go') or decline to respond ('no-go') during a short interval after each of the stimuli are presented. In test trials, participants are asked to respond to one of the concepts (white or black) and words with either positive or negative valence; these are then switched so that the concept is then paired with the opposite valence category. When paired with words with positive valence, faster and more accurate responding indicates greater association, and therefore positive attitude towards the target concept (either white or black race).[34] A full demonstration of the GNAT procedure can be found in the external links below.

Like the EAST, the GNAT has been used in populations who have been diagnosed with acute phobias to measure fear associations in addition to research on stereotypes and discrimination.[35]

Affect Misattribution Procedure (AMP) edit

The Affect Misattribution Procedure relies on participant ratings of neutral stimuli as an indirect measure of implicit attitudes rather than latency or accuracy measures. In the procedure, participants are first presented with a stimulus (usually an image or word), for either a brief visible period or subliminally, which is suspected to elicit a positive or negative attitude. Directly afterwards, participants are presented with a neutral stimulus (most often a Chinese pictograph) which they are asked to rate as either more or less, in this case visually, pleasing than an average stimulus. During these trials, the positive or negative affect in response to the priming image is misattributed or 'projected' onto the neutral stimulus such that it is rated as more or less pleasing than would be expected from solitary presentation.[36] Neutral stimuli which are rated as more visually pleasing indicate that the preceding concept presented in the prime stimuli are associated with positive valence. A full demonstration of the AMP procedure can be found in the external links below.

The AMP has been used to study attitudes towards political candidates and has proven useful in predicting voting behavior.[36][37] Also, the procedure is frequently used in the study of substance use; for example, attitudes towards cigarettes among smokers and non-smokers and attitudes towards alcohol among heavy drinkers.[38][39] It has also been used to measure implicit bias against minority groups.[40]

Comparison with explicit attitudes edit

Interaction of implicit and explicit attitude edit

The following are some examples of how implicit attitude and explicit attitude are moderated by each other and how they interact with each other.

Self presentation edit

Individuals will alter a response when questioned for personal or social purposes. This typically happens in situations where individuals are not willing to report or express their "affective response toward an object" because they don't want others to know how they feel about something (they don't consciously accept or endorse their evaluation). Since implicit measures are not as vulnerable to control as explicit measures are, the correlation between implicit and explicit attitudes should decrease as self presentation concerns increase. For example, in 2005 Nosek found that there was more overlap in explicit and implicit measures when people rated Pepsi vs. Coca-Cola (low self presentation concern). However, when they rated thin vs. fat people (high self presentation concern), the correlation (or overlap) of implicit and explicit measure decreased.[41][42]

Attitude strength edit

The strength of an attitude has an influence on explicit attitudes the stronger an implicit attitude the more likely it is that it will show up in an explicit attitude. Strong attitudes are stable and not easily changed due to persuasion and can therefore help predict behaviors. The more an individual expresses or acts on an attitude the stronger the attitude becomes and the more automated the attitude becomes. Attitude strength should increase the correspondence between implicit and explicit attitudes. Conscious thinking about the attitude should create more of an overlap between both implicit and explicit attitude.[42]

Early focus on explicit attitudes edit

Much of the literature within the field of social psychology has focused on explicit constructions of the attitude construct. Until more recently, examination of attitudes beyond reported awareness has lagged far behind that of explicit attitudes. This point is driven home in a review of research in the mid-1990s which found that among attitudinal research published in 1989, approximately only 1 in 9 experimental paradigms utilized an indirect measure of attitude (necessary for determining contributions of implicit attitudes) while all of the reviewed studies employed direct measures such as self report of attitudes which were explicitly aware to participants.[14]

New ideas about implicit versus explicit attitudes edit

Newer research has called into question the distinction between implicit and explicit attitudes. Fazio & Olson ask whether a person who is being primed to detect implicit attitudes is necessarily blind to their implicit beliefs.[43] In their paper they bring up the question; just because a person is primed on an unconscious level and may indeed be answering on an unconscious level, does that not mean that they could still be aware of their attitudes nonetheless. "A second troublesome aspect of the implicit-explicit distinction is that it implies preexisting dual attitudes".[43] They go on to say there is not a known test capable of measuring explicit attitudes solely without the influence of implicit attitudes as well. However, they do go on to say that context can have a significant effect on this particular line of research. People's explicitly stated and implicitly tested attitudes are more likely to be in sync for trivial matters such as preference in a presidential election than for highly charged issues such as predispositions towards a certain race. They exert that "The more sensitive the domain, the greater the likelihood that motivational factors will be evoked and exert some influence on overt responses to an explicit measure".[43] In other words, it is easier to compare explicit and implicit attitudes on safe subjects than subjects where people are likely to mask their beliefs.

A prominent dual process theory specifying the relation between implicit and explicit attitudes is Gawronski and Bodenhausen's associative-propositional evaluation (APE) model.[44] A central assumption of the APE model is that implicit and explicit evaluations are the product of two functionally distinct mental processes. Whereas implicit evaluations are assumed to be the outcome of associative processes, explicit evaluations are assumed to be the outcome of propositional processes. Associative processes are conceptualized as the activation of associations on the basis of feature similarity and spatio-temporal contiguity during learning. Propositional processes are defined as the validation of activated information on the basis of cognitive consistency. A central assumption of the APE model is that people tend to rely on their implicit evaluations when making explicit evaluative judgments to the extent that the implicit evaluative response is consistent with other momentarily considered propositional information. However, people may reject implicit evaluations for making explicit evaluative judgments when the implicit evaluative response is inconsistent with other momentarily considered propositional information. In addition to explaining the relation between implicit and explicit evaluations, the APE model accounts for diverging patterns of attitude change, including:[44]

  1. changes in implicit but not explicit evaluations
  2. changes in explicit but not implicit evaluations
  3. corresponding changes in implicit and explicit evaluations
  4. opposite changes in implicit and explicit evaluations.

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k Devos, T. (2008). "Attitudes and attitude change". Implicit attitudes 101: Theoretical and empirical Insights. New York, NY: Psychology Press. pp. 61–84.
  2. ^ See also implicit stereotype and implicit self-esteem for usage of this template
  3. ^ a b c d e Greenwald, A.G.; Banaji, M.R. (1995). "Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes". Psychological Review. 102 (1): 4–27. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.411.2919. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.102.1.4. PMID 7878162. S2CID 8194189.
  4. ^ Gawronski, B; Payne, B.K. (2010). Handbook of Implicit Social Cognition: Measurement, Theory and Application.
  5. ^ Devine, P. G (1989). "Stereotype and Prejudice: Their automatic and controlled components". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 56: 5–18. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.56.1.5. S2CID 33975027.
  6. ^ Rudman and Goodwin et al. (2004)
  7. ^ Olson and Fazio et al. (2001)
  8. ^ Olson and Fazio et al. (2002)
  9. ^ Rudmore, Ashmore, & Gary et al. (2001)
  10. ^ Nuttin et al. (1985)
  11. ^ Greenwald, Pickrell, and Farnham et al. (2002)
  12. ^ Livingston et al. (2002)
  13. ^ Olson and Fazio et al. (2004)
  14. ^ a b c d e Gawronski, B.; Hofmann, W.; Wilbur, C. (2006). "Are "implicit" attitudes unconscious?". Consciousness and Cognition. 15 (3): 485–499. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2005.11.007. PMID 16403654. S2CID 34209662.
  15. ^ Hahn, A.; Judd, C.M.; Hirsh, H.K.; Blair (2013). "Awareness of implicit attitudes". Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 143 (3): 1369–1392. doi:10.1037/a0035028. PMC 4038711. PMID 24294868.
  16. ^ Chen and Bargh (1999)
  17. ^ Fazio, R. H., & Towles-Schwen, T. (1999). The MODE model of attitude-behavior processes. In S. Chaiken Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual process theories in social psychology (pp. 97-116). New York: Guilford.
  18. ^ Fazio (2001)
  19. ^ Towles-schwen and Fazio (2003)
  20. ^ Devos, T. (2008). Implicit Attitudes 101: Theoretical and empirical insight. New York: Psychology Press. pp. 62–63.
  21. ^ Greenwald, A.G.; McGhee, D.E.; Schwartz, M. (1998). "Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The Implicit Association Test". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 74 (6): 1464–1480. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.489.4611. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.6.1464. PMID 9654756. S2CID 7840819.
  22. ^ Nosek, B.A., Smyth, F.L., Sriram, N., Lindner, N.M., Devos, T., Ayala, A., et al. (2009). National differences in gender-science stereotypes predict national sex differences in science and math achievement.
  23. ^ Martin, B.A.S.; Lee, M.S.W.; Lacey, C. (2011). "Countering negative country of origin effects using imagery processing" (PDF). Journal of Consumer Behaviour. 10 (2): 80–92. doi:10.1002/cb.351.
  24. ^ Serenko, A.; Turel, O. (2020). "Measuring implicit attitude in information systems research with the Implicit Association Test" (PDF). Communications of the Association for Information Systems. 47: 397–43. doi:10.17705/1CAIS.04719.
  25. ^ Serenko, A.; Turel, O. (2019). "A dual-attitude model of system use: The effect of explicit and implicit attitudes" (PDF). Information & Management. 56 (5): 657–668. doi:10.1016/j.im.2018.10.009. S2CID 115156846.
  26. ^ Roefs, A.; Herman, C.P.; MacLeod, C.M.; Smulders, F.T.; Jansen, A. (2005). "At first sight: How do restrained eaters evaluate high-fat palatable foods?". Appetite. 44 (1): 103–114. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.383.7839. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2004.08.001. PMID 15604037. S2CID 14993807.
  27. ^ Roefs, A.; Stapert, D.; Isabella, L.A.; Wolters, G.; Wojciechowski, F.; Jansen, A. (2005). "Early associations with food in anorexia nervosa patients and obese people assessed in the affective priming paradigm". Eating Behaviors. 6 (2): 151–163. doi:10.1016/j.eatbeh.2004.10.001. PMID 15598602.
  28. ^ Gawronski, B.; Deutsch, R.; Mbirkou, S.; Seibt, B.; Strack, F. (2008). "When "just say no" is not enough: Affirmation versus negation training and the reduction of automatic stereotype activation". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 44 (2): 370–377. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2006.12.004.
  29. ^ Wittenbrink, B.; Judd, C.M.; Park, B. (1997). "Evidence for radical prejudice at the implicit level and its relationship with questionnaire measures". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 72 (2): 262–274. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.462.7827. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.72.2.262. PMID 9107001.
  30. ^ a b De Houwer, J (2003). "The extrinsic affective Simon task". Experimental Psychology. 50 (2): 77–85. doi:10.1026//1618-3169.50.2.77. PMID 12693192.
  31. ^ Ellwart, T.; Becker, E.S.; Rinck, M. (2006). "Activation and measurement of threat associations in fear of spiders: An application of the Extrinsic Affective Simon Task". Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry. 36 (4): 281–299. doi:10.1016/j.jbtep.2004.08.008. hdl:2066/55983. PMID 16153389.
  32. ^ Vancleef, L.M.; Peters, M.L.; Gilissen, S.M.; De Jong, P.J. (2007). "Understanding the role of injury/illness sensitivity and anxiety sensitivity in (automatic) pain processing: An examination using the Extrinsic Affective Simon Task" (PDF). The Journal of Pain. 8 (7): 563–572. doi:10.1016/j.jpain.2007.02.431. PMID 17481956.
  33. ^ De Jong, P.J.; Wiers, W.; de Braak, M.; Huijding, J. (2007). "Using the Extrinsic Affective Simon Test as a measure of implicit attitudes towards alcohol: Relationship with drinking behavior and alcohol problems". Addictive Behaviors. 32 (4): 881–887. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2006.06.017. PMID 16870351.
  34. ^ Nosek, B.A.; Banaji, M.R. (2001). "The go/no-go association task". Social Cognition. 19 (6): 625–666. doi:10.1521/soco.19.6.625.20886. S2CID 6873625.
  35. ^ Teachman, B.A. (2007). "Evaluating implicit spider fear associations using the go/no-go association task". Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry. 38 (2): 156–167. doi:10.1016/j.jbtep.2006.10.006. PMID 17101115.
  36. ^ a b Payne, B.K.; Cheng, C.M.; Govorun, O.; Stewart, B.D. (2005). "An inkblot for attitudes: Affective misattribution as implicit measurement". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 89 (3): 277–293. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.392.4775. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.89.3.277. PMID 16248714.
  37. ^ Greenwald, A.G.; Smith, C.T.; Sriram, N.; Bar-Anan, Y.; Nosek, B.A. (2009). "Implicit race attitudes predicted vote in the 2008 U.S. presidential election". Analyses of Social Issues and Public Policy. 9 (1): 241–253. doi:10.1111/j.1530-2415.2009.01195.x.
  38. ^ Payne, B.K.; McClernon, F.J.; Dobbins, I.G. (2007). "Automatic affective responses to smoking cues". Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 15 (4): 400–409. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.392.1704. doi:10.1037/1064-1297.15.4.400. PMID 17696687.
  39. ^ Payne, B.K.; Govorun, O.; Arbuckle, N.L. (2008). "Automatic attitudes and alcohol: Does implicit liking predict drinking?". Cognition & Emotion. 22 (2): 238–271. doi:10.1080/02699930701357394. S2CID 145625335.
  40. ^ Cramer, Katherine (2020). "Understanding the Role of Racism in Contemporary US Public Opinion". Annual Review of Political Science. 23: 153–169. doi:10.1146/annurev-polisci-060418-042842.
  41. ^ Nosek (2005)
  42. ^ a b Devos, T (2008). Implicit Attitudes 101: Theoretical and empirical insight. New York: Psychology Press. pp. 62–63.
  43. ^ a b c Fazio, Russell; Michael Olson (6 August 2002). "Implicit Measures in Social Cognition Research: Their Meaning and Use". Annual Review of Psychology. 54: 297–327. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145225. PMID 12172003. S2CID 8797951.
  44. ^ a b Gawronski, B.; Bodenhausen, G. V. (2006). "Associative and propositional processes in evaluation: An integrative review of implicit and explicit attitude change". Psychological Bulletin. 132 (5): 692–731. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.334.2130. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.132.5.692. PMID 16910748. S2CID 9932633.

implicit, attitude, this, article, includes, inline, citations, they, properly, formatted, please, improve, this, article, correcting, them, parenthetical, citations, should, converted, footnotes, october, 2022, learn, when, remove, this, message, evaluations,. This article includes inline citations but they are not properly formatted Please improve this article by correcting them Parenthetical citations should be converted to footnotes October 2022 Learn how and when to remove this message Implicit attitudes are evaluations that occur without conscious awareness towards an attitude object or the self These evaluations are generally either favorable or unfavorable and come about from various influences in the individual experience 1 The commonly used definition of implicit attitude within cognitive and social psychology comes from Anthony Greenwald and Mahzarin Banaji s template for definitions of terms related to implicit cognition 2 Implicit attitudes are introspectively unidentified or inaccurately identified traces of past experience that mediate favorable or unfavorable feeling thought or action toward social objects 3 These thoughts feelings or actions have an influence on behavior that the individual may not be aware of 4 An attitude is differentiated from the concept of a stereotype in that it functions as a broad favorable or unfavorable characteristic towards a social object whereas a stereotype is a set of favorable and or unfavorable characteristics which are applied to an individual based on social group membership Contents 1 Causes and manifestations 1 1 Halo effect 1 2 Experiences and socialization 1 3 Implicit attitudes related to the self 1 4 Culture and social norms 2 Degree of awareness 3 Flexibility 4 Effects on behavior 4 1 Types of behavior affected by implicit attitudes 4 2 Effects of motivation 5 Measurement 5 1 Implicit Association Test 5 2 Evaluative priming task 5 3 Semantic priming task 5 4 Extrinsic Affective Simon Task EAST 5 5 Go No go Association Task GNAT 5 6 Affect Misattribution Procedure AMP 6 Comparison with explicit attitudes 6 1 Interaction of implicit and explicit attitude 6 1 1 Self presentation 6 1 2 Attitude strength 6 2 Early focus on explicit attitudes 6 3 New ideas about implicit versus explicit attitudes 7 See also 8 ReferencesCauses and manifestations editA number of different theories have been proposed relating to the formation development and influence of implicit attitudes Halo effect edit Main article Halo effect Based on many empirical findings Greenwald and Banaji et al 1995 generated the fundamental idea of implicit attitude definitively for the first time disambiguating attitude into explicit and implicit types 3 Halo effects are an example of the empirical research used by Greenwald and Banaji in their chapter on implicit social cognition Understanding halo effects set the foundation for understanding other theories regarding implicit attitudes For example it is possible to explain implicit partisanship or implicit egotism in terms of a halo effect however these concepts will be discussed more in subsequent sections Pioneered by Edward Thorndike in 1920 the halo effect is the judgement of attribute A being influenced by a known but irrelevant attribute B For example subsequent replications commonly use physical attractiveness as attribute B and attribute A being a judgement of the subject More specifically a study Landy and Sigall et al 1974 found that essays written by female essayists were found to be of higher quality when a photo showed the essayist as being attractive rather than unattractive when rated by male judges 3 Greenwald and Banaji et al 1995 have suggested that attribute B is in fact an implicit attitude when the judge or subject cannot identify attribute B as the source of the judgement for attribute A Moreover when attribute B is associated with a positive or negative attitude and additionally is unknowingly and automatically transferred onto attribute A that attitude of attribute B is known to be an implicit attitude 3 Experiences and socialization edit Earlier research findings on implicit attitudes show that socialization 5 and reflections of past experiences 3 may be responsible for the development or manifestation of longer lasting implicit attitudes As an example Rudman and Goodwin et al found in 2004 that individuals who were primarily raised by their mothers showed a more positive implicit attitude towards women rather than men 6 Furthermore Olson and Fazio et al in 2001 and 2002 suggested that these implicit attitudes are a result of repeated pairings of positive or negative stimuli with an object more pairings of positive stimuli would result in a more positive implicit attitude and vice versa 7 8 This finding supports the fundamental principles of classical conditioning 1 Implicit attitudes are also developed by more recent experiences as well For example Rudmore Ashmore amp Gary et al in 2001 found that implicit attitude of prejudice against African Americans could be shaped through diversity training intervention using variables at an emotional level rather than increased awareness of bias which helped explicit attitude more 9 1 Implicit attitudes related to the self edit Self related objects are anything that pertains to the self including in groups and self esteem attitude towards the self Early research by Nuttin et al in 1985 suggested that people generally have an implicit preference for letters in their own name known as the Name letter effect 10 Further replications of this same effect with varying independent variables e g attractiveness to people with the same letters contained in their names suggest that people have an implicit preference towards themselves This manifestation of implicit attitude has come to be known as Implicit egotism Implicit egotism additionally manifests itself in in groups 1 Implicit partisanshipis the heightened attractiveness and identification to a self related group and negative or neutral attitudes towards non self related groups Greenwald Pickrell and Farnham et al demonstrated this effect in 2002 even when the groups were cooperative and when the members of the groups were non human 11 Much of the research on implicit partisanship suggests that this is an uncontrollable process or an implicit attitude towards self related groups 1 Culture and social norms edit Generally speaking culture and social norms have an effect on implicit attitude in the same way experiences and socialization have an effect on implicit attitude However culture has a very noticeable effect on implicit attitude in the way implicit attitude differs from one s explicit attitude In 2002 Livingston et al examined the effect of mainstream culture on one s implicit attitude towards their social group 12 Implicitly one will follow the cultural attitudes towards their social group that they perceive from mainstream culture in their society whether that be positive or negative With that said a strong cultural disadvantage e g negative attitude can effectively eliminate in group favoritism when tested at the implicit level However it may be important to note that at the explicit attitude level these individuals still showed positive attitudes towards their social group Olson and Fazio et al suggested in 2004 that at an implicit level one s personal attitude can be influenced by the social or cultural norms that one perceives 13 Furthermore this may be due to a weak distinction between one s personal attitude and extrapersonal associations e g one s cultural evaluations towards an attitude object at the implicit level Therefore implicit attitudes are reflective of experiences but can also be shaped by the cultural context 1 Degree of awareness editCurrent research supports the idea that there are three different aspects of attitudes captured by current indirect measures that could be outside of conscious awareness the source the content and the impact of an attitude 14 Source awareness is described as the awareness of the origin of a particular attitude emphasis added 14 Content awareness is differentiated from source awareness by the lack of awareness about the attitude rather than simply its origin Finally one may have awareness of both the attitude and its source but the attitude may still have influences on thought or behavior beyond ones awareness this can be thought of as impact awareness 14 Conclusions have been made that both indirectly assessed and self reported attitudes can be characterized by lack of source awareness there is no evidence for lack of content awareness of indirectly assessed attitudes and there is some evidence showing that indirectly assessed but not self reported attitudes can be characterized by lack of impact awareness 14 The most compelling evidence for content awareness of implicit attitudes has showed that people are highly accurate in predicting their scores on the Implicit Association Test 15 Flexibility editRecent research indicates the possibility of the malleability of implicit attitudes based on situational context 1 That is implicit attitudes are not believed to be stable representations of memory rather they are constructed based on the type of available information in a given situation Available information can vary in context to the individual though it is believed to serve as a prime to their behaviors Flexibility of implicit attitudes is best demonstrated through measures that include accessibility effects For example it has been demonstrated that the information given to an individual prior to completing an implicit measure directly affects their response based on the information they were given 1 Therefore if an individual is primed with information regarding the positive or negative attributions of a different race and then asked to complete an implicit measures task the participants will most likely use the information that was presented during the prime and not their own experienced information to assess the situation This occurs because the information that was primed is most available for the participant to access without having to use conscious resources Effects on behavior editThe fundamental goal of measuring implicit attitudes is to use it to predict behavior behaviors that can t be predicted by knowledge of explicitly held attitudes Numerous studies such as research conducted by Chen and Bargh in 1999 16 show that automatic evaluations triggered by various attitudes towards objects directly affected behavioral predispositions towards that object Stimuli that elicited positive attitudes produced immediate positive behavior whilst stimuli that elicited negative attitudes triggered immediate avoidance behavior The individuals are completely unaware of the operations that their behavioral responses because they are automatic and unconscious In Bassenoff and Sherman et al 2000 they found that automatic negative attitudes about overweight individuals directly predicted how far participants choose to sit from a fat woman who they were expected to interact with We see this phenomenon also with implicitly held racial attitudes as shown by McConnell and Leibold et al 2001 These implicit attitudes affected how long they interacted for how much participants smiled how many speech errors they made and how many social comments were made All automatic behavioral responses that measuring explicit attitudes could not predict 1 Types of behavior affected by implicit attitudes edit Implicit attitudes aren t always better at predicting behavior than explicit measurements they both play a systematic role in predicting behavior Implicit attitudes are typically better than explicit attitudes at predicting behavior that is automatic and spontaneous In line with Dual process theories such as Fazio s MODE model automatic attitudes determine spontaneous actions whereas deliberative actions reflect a contribution of multiple processes including more controlled processes e g a person s motivation to overcome prejudiced responses 17 As demonstrated by Dasgupta an Rivera et al 2006 individuals who endorsed traditional beliefs about gender and sexuality were friendlier towards gay confederates verbally but showed negative non verbal behavior this suggested that this individuals were consciously over correcting their behavior but their prejudice leaked out through automatic responses like blinking and eye contact 1 Effects of motivation edit Although research has shown that motivation and an opportunity to react carefully can affect how much implicit attitudes influence behavioral response 18 When individuals are highly motivated to control their responses and processing abilities are not lacking or preoccupied behavioral responses tend to reflect intentional processes In 2003 Towles schwen and Fazio measured anticipated willingness and discomfort of participants to interact with a black person 19 Individuals who were motivated to avoid interracial conflicts and where not concerned about seeming biased expressed their discomfort whereas individuals who were concerned about not appearing biased reported less anticipated discomfort in an attempt to hide their prejudice Motivation to control our responses can minimize the influence of implicit attitudes on behavioral responses as shown by that example 1 Measurement editThere is an assortment of different experimental tests that assess for the presence of implicit attitudes including the implicit association test evaluative and semantic priming tasks the Extrinsic Affective Simon Task Go No Go Association Task and the Affect Misattribution Procedure Though these tests vary in administration and content the basis of each is to allow investigators to capture attitudes that individuals are unwilling to report 20 Unwillingness and lack of ability are intertwined considering most individuals are unaware that these attitudes even exist The following are brief descriptions about these measurements which are most commonly used to assess implicit attitudes and the empirical evidence that supports them Implicit Association Test edit The Implicit Association Test is a latency based measure of the relative associations between two concepts In a series of tasks participants sort words or images representing a target concept such as race white black and stimuli with known positive negative valence into two categories usually indicated by right or left location on a computer screen Each category of concept words or images is paired with both positive and negative stimuli The faster the categorization occurs the stronger the association is between words and or images that are grouped together ex faster categorization of dogs when paired with positive rather than negative words which would indicate an implicit attitude towards that object 21 A full demonstration of the IAT procedure can be found at the Project Implicit link and the IAT Inquisit link below Research using the IAT measure of implicit attitudes has demonstrated consistent experimental and population based attitudes with respect to concepts such as gender race and age An analysis from the Project Implicit database found that science gender stereotypes are predictive of differences in gender related math and science performance across countries in an international sample 22 Research has also successfully used the IAT in consumer research 23 Implicit attitude also directly drives the use of information systems 24 and serves as a basis upon which use habit is formed 25 Evaluative priming task edit Research using the evaluative priming task has been frequently used in research on eating and attitudes towards food 26 In clinical studies the procedure was used to study attitudes of those diagnosed with eating disorders such as anorexia nervosa 27 Along with many of the other methods presented here researchers have used the procedure to measure the effects of stereotypes including measurement of the effectiveness of stereotype reduction treatments 28 Semantic priming task edit In the semantic priming task paradigm described by Wittenbrink et al 1997 participants are shown a word prime at intervals which are too brief for reported awareness see subliminal stimuli 29 The word prime consists of two groups of words representing the concept in question such as black sounding names or white sounding names Participants were then asked to complete a lexical decision task LDT to identify if target stimuli are words or a non words The target stimuli consist of words with known positive or negative valence When words with positive valence are categorized more quickly in the presence of one group of word primes such as black sounding names this indicates positive attitudes towards the group Extrinsic Affective Simon Task EAST edit In the Extrinsic Affective Simon Task EAST participants categorized stimuli which consisted of words that either had positive or negative valence that were presented in either the color white or two different colors 30 When the words are presented in white participants categorize based words on their perceived positive or negative valence When the words are presented in color participants are asked to categorize based on color alone and ignore word meaning When colored words are presented categorization accuracy and speed are facilitated when for words which the respondent has a positive implicit attitude the response was the same as was expected for white words with obvious positive valence 30 A full demonstration of the EAST procedure can be found in the external links below The EAST has been used in research of attitudes of those who have specific phobias and or anxiety 31 32 Additionally the test has been recently used to measure implicit attitudes towards alcohol in populations who have substance abuse problems and the test has been cited as having relatively high predictive value for problem substance use 33 Go No go Association Task GNAT edit In practice the GNAT appears similar to the Implicit Association Test in that participants are asked to categorize targets representing either a concept such as race ex white or black names or words which have obvious positive or negative valence Participants are asked to respond go or decline to respond no go during a short interval after each of the stimuli are presented In test trials participants are asked to respond to one of the concepts white or black and words with either positive or negative valence these are then switched so that the concept is then paired with the opposite valence category When paired with words with positive valence faster and more accurate responding indicates greater association and therefore positive attitude towards the target concept either white or black race 34 A full demonstration of the GNAT procedure can be found in the external links below Like the EAST the GNAT has been used in populations who have been diagnosed with acute phobias to measure fear associations in addition to research on stereotypes and discrimination 35 Affect Misattribution Procedure AMP edit The Affect Misattribution Procedure relies on participant ratings of neutral stimuli as an indirect measure of implicit attitudes rather than latency or accuracy measures In the procedure participants are first presented with a stimulus usually an image or word for either a brief visible period or subliminally which is suspected to elicit a positive or negative attitude Directly afterwards participants are presented with a neutral stimulus most often a Chinese pictograph which they are asked to rate as either more or less in this case visually pleasing than an average stimulus During these trials the positive or negative affect in response to the priming image is misattributed or projected onto the neutral stimulus such that it is rated as more or less pleasing than would be expected from solitary presentation 36 Neutral stimuli which are rated as more visually pleasing indicate that the preceding concept presented in the prime stimuli are associated with positive valence A full demonstration of the AMP procedure can be found in the external links below The AMP has been used to study attitudes towards political candidates and has proven useful in predicting voting behavior 36 37 Also the procedure is frequently used in the study of substance use for example attitudes towards cigarettes among smokers and non smokers and attitudes towards alcohol among heavy drinkers 38 39 It has also been used to measure implicit bias against minority groups 40 Comparison with explicit attitudes editInteraction of implicit and explicit attitude edit The following are some examples of how implicit attitude and explicit attitude are moderated by each other and how they interact with each other Self presentation edit Individuals will alter a response when questioned for personal or social purposes This typically happens in situations where individuals are not willing to report or express their affective response toward an object because they don t want others to know how they feel about something they don t consciously accept or endorse their evaluation Since implicit measures are not as vulnerable to control as explicit measures are the correlation between implicit and explicit attitudes should decrease as self presentation concerns increase For example in 2005 Nosek found that there was more overlap in explicit and implicit measures when people rated Pepsi vs Coca Cola low self presentation concern However when they rated thin vs fat people high self presentation concern the correlation or overlap of implicit and explicit measure decreased 41 42 Attitude strength edit The strength of an attitude has an influence on explicit attitudes the stronger an implicit attitude the more likely it is that it will show up in an explicit attitude Strong attitudes are stable and not easily changed due to persuasion and can therefore help predict behaviors The more an individual expresses or acts on an attitude the stronger the attitude becomes and the more automated the attitude becomes Attitude strength should increase the correspondence between implicit and explicit attitudes Conscious thinking about the attitude should create more of an overlap between both implicit and explicit attitude 42 Early focus on explicit attitudes edit Much of the literature within the field of social psychology has focused on explicit constructions of the attitude construct Until more recently examination of attitudes beyond reported awareness has lagged far behind that of explicit attitudes This point is driven home in a review of research in the mid 1990s which found that among attitudinal research published in 1989 approximately only 1 in 9 experimental paradigms utilized an indirect measure of attitude necessary for determining contributions of implicit attitudes while all of the reviewed studies employed direct measures such as self report of attitudes which were explicitly aware to participants 14 New ideas about implicit versus explicit attitudes edit Newer research has called into question the distinction between implicit and explicit attitudes Fazio amp Olson ask whether a person who is being primed to detect implicit attitudes is necessarily blind to their implicit beliefs 43 In their paper they bring up the question just because a person is primed on an unconscious level and may indeed be answering on an unconscious level does that not mean that they could still be aware of their attitudes nonetheless A second troublesome aspect of the implicit explicit distinction is that it implies preexisting dual attitudes 43 They go on to say there is not a known test capable of measuring explicit attitudes solely without the influence of implicit attitudes as well However they do go on to say that context can have a significant effect on this particular line of research People s explicitly stated and implicitly tested attitudes are more likely to be in sync for trivial matters such as preference in a presidential election than for highly charged issues such as predispositions towards a certain race They exert that The more sensitive the domain the greater the likelihood that motivational factors will be evoked and exert some influence on overt responses to an explicit measure 43 In other words it is easier to compare explicit and implicit attitudes on safe subjects than subjects where people are likely to mask their beliefs A prominent dual process theory specifying the relation between implicit and explicit attitudes is Gawronski and Bodenhausen s associative propositional evaluation APE model 44 A central assumption of the APE model is that implicit and explicit evaluations are the product of two functionally distinct mental processes Whereas implicit evaluations are assumed to be the outcome of associative processes explicit evaluations are assumed to be the outcome of propositional processes Associative processes are conceptualized as the activation of associations on the basis of feature similarity and spatio temporal contiguity during learning Propositional processes are defined as the validation of activated information on the basis of cognitive consistency A central assumption of the APE model is that people tend to rely on their implicit evaluations when making explicit evaluative judgments to the extent that the implicit evaluative response is consistent with other momentarily considered propositional information However people may reject implicit evaluations for making explicit evaluative judgments when the implicit evaluative response is inconsistent with other momentarily considered propositional information In addition to explaining the relation between implicit and explicit evaluations the APE model accounts for diverging patterns of attitude change including 44 changes in implicit but not explicit evaluations changes in explicit but not implicit evaluations corresponding changes in implicit and explicit evaluations opposite changes in implicit and explicit evaluations See also editAlief mental state Attitude psychology Emotional baggage Halo Effect Implicit assumption Implicit cognition Implicit self esteem Implicit stereotypes Implicit association test Lexical decision task List of cognitive biases Mere exposure effect Priming psychology Valence psychology References edit a b c d e f g h i j k Devos T 2008 Attitudes and attitude change Implicit attitudes 101 Theoretical and empirical Insights New York NY Psychology Press pp 61 84 See also implicit stereotype and implicit self esteem for usage of this template a b c d e Greenwald A G Banaji M R 1995 Implicit social cognition Attitudes self esteem and stereotypes Psychological Review 102 1 4 27 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 411 2919 doi 10 1037 0033 295x 102 1 4 PMID 7878162 S2CID 8194189 Gawronski B Payne B K 2010 Handbook of Implicit Social Cognition Measurement Theory and Application Devine P G 1989 Stereotype and Prejudice Their automatic and controlled components Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 56 5 18 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 56 1 5 S2CID 33975027 Rudman and Goodwin et al 2004 Olson and Fazio et al 2001 Olson and Fazio et al 2002 Rudmore Ashmore amp Gary et al 2001 Nuttin et al 1985 Greenwald Pickrell and Farnham et al 2002 Livingston et al 2002 Olson and Fazio et al 2004 a b c d e Gawronski B Hofmann W Wilbur C 2006 Are implicit attitudes unconscious Consciousness and Cognition 15 3 485 499 doi 10 1016 j concog 2005 11 007 PMID 16403654 S2CID 34209662 Hahn A Judd C M Hirsh H K Blair 2013 Awareness of implicit attitudes Journal of Experimental Psychology General 143 3 1369 1392 doi 10 1037 a0035028 PMC 4038711 PMID 24294868 Chen and Bargh 1999 Fazio R H amp Towles Schwen T 1999 The MODE model of attitude behavior processes In S Chaiken Y Trope Eds Dual process theories in social psychology pp 97 116 New York Guilford Fazio 2001 Towles schwen and Fazio 2003 Devos T 2008 Implicit Attitudes 101 Theoretical and empirical insight New York Psychology Press pp 62 63 Greenwald A G McGhee D E Schwartz M 1998 Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition The Implicit Association Test Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 74 6 1464 1480 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 489 4611 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 74 6 1464 PMID 9654756 S2CID 7840819 Nosek B A Smyth F L Sriram N Lindner N M Devos T Ayala A et al 2009 National differences in gender science stereotypes predict national sex differences in science and math achievement Martin B A S Lee M S W Lacey C 2011 Countering negative country of origin effects using imagery processing PDF Journal of Consumer Behaviour 10 2 80 92 doi 10 1002 cb 351 Serenko A Turel O 2020 Measuring implicit attitude in information systems research with the Implicit Association Test PDF Communications of the Association for Information Systems 47 397 43 doi 10 17705 1CAIS 04719 Serenko A Turel O 2019 A dual attitude model of system use The effect of explicit and implicit attitudes PDF Information amp Management 56 5 657 668 doi 10 1016 j im 2018 10 009 S2CID 115156846 Roefs A Herman C P MacLeod C M Smulders F T Jansen A 2005 At first sight How do restrained eaters evaluate high fat palatable foods Appetite 44 1 103 114 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 383 7839 doi 10 1016 j appet 2004 08 001 PMID 15604037 S2CID 14993807 Roefs A Stapert D Isabella L A Wolters G Wojciechowski F Jansen A 2005 Early associations with food in anorexia nervosa patients and obese people assessed in the affective priming paradigm Eating Behaviors 6 2 151 163 doi 10 1016 j eatbeh 2004 10 001 PMID 15598602 Gawronski B Deutsch R Mbirkou S Seibt B Strack F 2008 When just say no is not enough Affirmation versus negation training and the reduction of automatic stereotype activation Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 44 2 370 377 doi 10 1016 j jesp 2006 12 004 Wittenbrink B Judd C M Park B 1997 Evidence for radical prejudice at the implicit level and its relationship with questionnaire measures Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 72 2 262 274 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 462 7827 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 72 2 262 PMID 9107001 a b De Houwer J 2003 The extrinsic affective Simon task Experimental Psychology 50 2 77 85 doi 10 1026 1618 3169 50 2 77 PMID 12693192 Ellwart T Becker E S Rinck M 2006 Activation and measurement of threat associations in fear of spiders An application of the Extrinsic Affective Simon Task Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry 36 4 281 299 doi 10 1016 j jbtep 2004 08 008 hdl 2066 55983 PMID 16153389 Vancleef L M Peters M L Gilissen S M De Jong P J 2007 Understanding the role of injury illness sensitivity and anxiety sensitivity in automatic pain processing An examination using the Extrinsic Affective Simon Task PDF The Journal of Pain 8 7 563 572 doi 10 1016 j jpain 2007 02 431 PMID 17481956 De Jong P J Wiers W de Braak M Huijding J 2007 Using the Extrinsic Affective Simon Test as a measure of implicit attitudes towards alcohol Relationship with drinking behavior and alcohol problems Addictive Behaviors 32 4 881 887 doi 10 1016 j addbeh 2006 06 017 PMID 16870351 Nosek B A Banaji M R 2001 The go no go association task Social Cognition 19 6 625 666 doi 10 1521 soco 19 6 625 20886 S2CID 6873625 Teachman B A 2007 Evaluating implicit spider fear associations using the go no go association task Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry 38 2 156 167 doi 10 1016 j jbtep 2006 10 006 PMID 17101115 a b Payne B K Cheng C M Govorun O Stewart B D 2005 An inkblot for attitudes Affective misattribution as implicit measurement Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 89 3 277 293 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 392 4775 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 89 3 277 PMID 16248714 Greenwald A G Smith C T Sriram N Bar Anan Y Nosek B A 2009 Implicit race attitudes predicted vote in the 2008 U S presidential election Analyses of Social Issues and Public Policy 9 1 241 253 doi 10 1111 j 1530 2415 2009 01195 x Payne B K McClernon F J Dobbins I G 2007 Automatic affective responses to smoking cues Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology 15 4 400 409 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 392 1704 doi 10 1037 1064 1297 15 4 400 PMID 17696687 Payne B K Govorun O Arbuckle N L 2008 Automatic attitudes and alcohol Does implicit liking predict drinking Cognition amp Emotion 22 2 238 271 doi 10 1080 02699930701357394 S2CID 145625335 Cramer Katherine 2020 Understanding the Role of Racism in Contemporary US Public Opinion Annual Review of Political Science 23 153 169 doi 10 1146 annurev polisci 060418 042842 Nosek 2005 a b Devos T 2008 Implicit Attitudes 101 Theoretical and empirical insight New York Psychology Press pp 62 63 a b c Fazio Russell Michael Olson 6 August 2002 Implicit Measures in Social Cognition Research Their Meaning and Use Annual Review of Psychology 54 297 327 doi 10 1146 annurev psych 54 101601 145225 PMID 12172003 S2CID 8797951 a b Gawronski B Bodenhausen G V 2006 Associative and propositional processes in evaluation An integrative review of implicit and explicit attitude change Psychological Bulletin 132 5 692 731 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 334 2130 doi 10 1037 0033 2909 132 5 692 PMID 16910748 S2CID 9932633 Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Implicit attitude amp oldid 1208840810, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.