fbpx
Wikipedia

Social-desirability bias

In social science research, social-desirability bias is a type of response bias that is the tendency of survey respondents to answer questions in a manner that will be viewed favorably by others.[1] It can take the form of over-reporting "good behavior" or under-reporting "bad", or undesirable behavior. The tendency poses a serious problem with conducting research with self-reports. This bias interferes with the interpretation of average tendencies as well as individual differences.

Topics subject to social-desirability bias edit

Topics where socially desirable responding (SDR) is of special concern are self-reports of abilities, personality, sexual behavior, and drug use. When confronted with the question "How often do you masturbate?," for example, respondents may be pressured by a social taboo against masturbation, and either under-report the frequency or avoid answering the question. Therefore, the mean rates of masturbation derived from self-report surveys are likely to be severely underestimated.

When confronted with the question, "Do you use drugs/illicit substances?" the respondent may be influenced by the fact that controlled substances, including the more commonly used marijuana, are generally illegal. Respondents may feel pressured to deny any drug use or rationalize it, e.g. "I only smoke marijuana when my friends are around." The bias can also influence reports of number of sexual partners. In fact, the bias may operate in opposite directions for different subgroups: Whereas men tend to inflate the numbers, women tend to underestimate theirs. In either case, the mean reports from both groups are likely to be distorted by social desirability bias.

Other topics that are sensitive to social-desirability bias include:

  • Self-reported personality traits will correlate strongly with social desirability bias[2]
  • Personal income and earnings, often inflated when low and deflated when high
  • Feelings of low self-worth and/or powerlessness, often denied
  • Excretory functions, often approached uncomfortably, if discussed at all
  • Compliance with medicinal-dosing schedules, often inflated
  • Family planning, including use of contraceptives and abortion[3][4]
  • Religion, often either avoided or uncomfortably approached[5]
  • Patriotism, either inflated or, if denied, done so with a fear of other party's judgment
  • Bigotry and intolerance, often denied, even if it exists within the responder
  • Intellectual achievements, often inflated
  • Physical appearance, either inflated or deflated
  • Acts of real or imagined physical violence, often denied
  • Indicators of charity or "benevolence," often inflated
  • Illegal acts, often denied
  • Voter turnout[6][7][8][9]

Individual differences in socially desirable responding edit

In 1953, Allen L. Edwards introduced the notion of social desirability to psychology, demonstrating the role of social desirability in the measurement of personality traits. He demonstrated that social desirability ratings of personality trait descriptions are very highly correlated with the probability that a subsequent group of people will endorse these trait self-descriptions. In his first demonstration of this pattern, the correlation between one group of college students’ social desirability ratings of a set of traits and the probability that college students in a second group would endorse self-descriptions describing the same traits was so high that it could distort the meaning of the personality traits. In other words, do these self-descriptions describe personality traits or social desirability?[10]

Edwards subsequently developed the first Social Desirability Scale, a set of 39, true-false questions extracted from the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI), questions that judges could, with high agreement, order according to their social desirability.[2] These items were subsequently found to be very highly correlated with a wide range of measurement scales, MMPI personality and diagnostic scales.[11] The SDS is also highly correlated with the Beck Hopelessness Inventory.[12]

The fact that people differ in their tendency to engage in socially desirable responding (SDR) is a special concern to those measuring individual differences with self-reports. Individual differences in SDR make it difficult to distinguish those people with good traits who are responding factually from those distorting their answers in a positive direction.

When SDR cannot be eliminated, researchers may resort to evaluating the tendency and then control for it. A separate SDR measure must be administered together with the primary measure (test or interview) aimed at the subject matter of the research/investigation. The key assumption is that respondents who answer in a socially desirable manner on that scale are also responding desirably to all self-reports throughout the study.

In some cases, the entire questionnaire package from high scoring respondents may simply be discarded. Alternatively, respondents' answers on the primary questionnaires may be statistically adjusted commensurate with their SDR tendencies. For example, this adjustment is performed automatically in the standard scoring of MMPI scales.

The major concern with SDR scales is that they confound style with content. After all, people actually differ in the degree to which they possess desirable traits (e.g. nuns versus criminals). Consequently, measures of social desirability confound true differences with social-desirability bias.

Standard measures of individual SDR edit

Until the 1990s, the most commonly used measure of socially desirable responding was the Marlowe–Crowne Social Desirability Scale.[13] The original version comprised 33 True-False items. A shortened version, the Strahan–Gerbasi only comprises ten items, but some have raised questions regarding the reliability of this measure.[14]

In 1991, Delroy L. Paulhus published the Balanced Inventory of Desirable Responding (BIDR): a questionnaire designed to measure two forms of SDR.[15] This forty-item instrument provides separate subscales for "impression management," the tendency to give inflated self-descriptions to an audience; and self-deceptive enhancement, the tendency to give honest but inflated self-descriptions. The commercial version of the BIDR is called the "Paulhus Deception Scales (PDS)."[16]

Scales designed to tap response styles are available in all major languages, including Italian[17][18] and German.[19]

Techniques to reduce social-desirability bias edit

Anonymity and confidentiality edit

Anonymous survey administration, compared with in-person or phone-based administration, has been shown to elicit higher reporting of items with social-desirability bias.[20] In anonymous survey settings, the subject is assured that their responses will not be linked to them, and they are not asked to divulge sensitive information directly to a surveyor. Anonymity can be established through self-administration of paper surveys returned by envelope, mail, or ballot boxes, or self-administration of electronic survey via computer, smartphone, or tablet.[1][21] Audio-assisted electronic surveys have also been established for low-literacy or non-literate study subjects.[1][22]

Confidentiality can be established in non-anonymous settings by ensuring that only study staff are present and by maintaining data confidentiality after surveys are complete. Including assurances of data confidentiality in surveys has a mixed effect on sensitive-question response; it may either increase response due to increased trust, or decrease response by increasing suspicion and concern.[1]

Specialized questioning techniques edit

Several techniques have been established to reduce bias when asking questions sensitive to social desirability.[20] Complex question techniques may reduce social-desirability bias, but may also be confusing or misunderstood by respondents.

Beyond specific techniques, social-desirability bias may be reduced by neutral question and prompt wording.[1]

Ballot Box Method edit

The Ballot Box Method (BBM) provides survey respondents anonymity by allowing them to respond in private by self-completing their responses to the sensitive survey questions on a secret ballot and submitting them to a locked box. The interviewer has no knowledge of what is recorded on the secret ballot and does not have access to the lock on the box, providing obscurity to the responses and limiting the potential for SDB. However, a unique control number on each ballot allows the answers to be reunited with a corresponding questionnaire that contains less sensitive questions.[23][24] The BBM has been used successfully to obtain estimates of sensitive sexual behaviours during an HIV prevention study,[23] as well as illegal environmental resource use.[24][25] In a validation study where observed behaviour was matched to reported behaviour using various SDB control methods, the BBM was by far the most accurate bias reduction method, performing significantly better than the Randomized Response Technique (RRT).[24]

Randomized response techniques edit

The randomized response technique asks a participant to respond with a fixed answer or to answer truthfully based on the outcome of a random act.[22] For example, respondents secretly throw a coin and respond "yes" if it comes up heads (regardless of their actual response to the question), and are instructed to respond truthfully if it comes up tails. This enables the researcher to estimate the actual prevalence of the given behavior among the study population without needing to know the true state of any one individual respondent. Research shows that the validity of the randomized response technique is limited.[26] Validation research has shown that the RRT actually performs worse than direct questioning for some sensitive behaviours and care should be taken when considering its use.[24]

Nominative and best-friend techniques edit

The nominative technique asks a participant about the behavior of their close friends, rather than about their own behavior.[27] Participants are asked how many close friends they know have done for certain a sensitive behavior and how many other people they think know about that behavior. Population estimates of behaviors can be derived from the response.

The similar best-friend methodology asks the participant about the behavior of one best friend.[28]

Unmatched-count technique edit

The unmatched-count technique asks respondents to indicate how many of a list of several items they have done or are true for them.[29] Respondents are randomized to receive either a list of non-sensitive items or that same list plus the sensitive item of interest. Differences in the total number of items between the two groups indicate how many of those in the group receiving the sensitive item said yes to it.

Grouped-answer method edit

The grouped-answer method, also known as the two-card or three-card method, combines answer choices such that the sensitive response is combined with at least one non-sensitive response option.[30]

Crosswise, triangular, and hidden-sensitivity methods edit

These methods ask participants to select one response based on two or more questions, only one of which is sensitive.[31] For example, a participant will be asked whether their birth year is even and whether they have performed an illegal activity; if yes to both or no to both, to select A, and if yes to one but no to the other, select B. By combining sensitive and non-sensitive questions, the participant's response to the sensitive item is masked. Research shows that the validity of the crosswise model is limited.[32]

Bogus pipeline edit

Bogus-pipeline techniques are those in which a participant believes that an objective test, like a lie detector, will be used along with survey response, whether or not that test or procedure is actually used.[1] Researches using this technique must convince the participants that there is a machine that can measure accurately their true attitudes and desires. While this can raise ethical questions surrounding deception in psychological research, this technique quickly became widely popular in the 1970s. However, by the 1990s the use of this technique began to wane. Interested in this change, Roese & Jamison (1993) took twenty years of research to do a meta-analysis on the effectiveness of the Bogus pipeline technique in reducing social desirability bias. They concluded that the Bogus pipeline technique while significantly effective, it perhaps became less and less used simply because it went out of fashion or became a bit cumbersome for researches to use regularly. However, they argue there are simple adjustments that can be made to this technique to make it more user-friendly for researchers. [33]

Other response styles edit

"Extreme-response style" (ERS) takes the form of exaggerated-extremity preference, e.g. for '1' or '7' on 7-point scales. Its converse, 'moderacy bias' entails a preference for middle-range (or midpoint) responses (e.g. 3–5 on 7-point scales).

"Acquiescence" (ARS) is the tendency to respond to items with agreement/affirmation independent of their content ("yea"-saying).

These kinds of response styles differ from social-desirability bias in that they are unrelated to the question's content and may be present in both socially neutral and in socially favorable or unfavorable contexts, whereas SDR is, by definition, tied to the latter.

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ a b c d e f Krumpal, Ivar (2013). "Determinants of social desirability bias in sensitive surveys: a literature review". Quality & Quantity. 47 (4): 2025–2047. doi:10.1007/s11135-011-9640-9. S2CID 143045969.
  2. ^ a b Edwards, Allen (1957). The social desirability variable in personality assessment and research. New York: The Dryden Press.
  3. ^ Stuart, Gretchen S.; Grimes, David A. (2009). "Social desirability bias in family planning studies: A neglected problem". Contraception. 80 (2): 108–112. doi:10.1016/j.contraception.2009.02.009. PMID 19631784.
  4. ^ Sedgh, Gilda; Keogh, Sarah C. (2019-04-18). "Novel approaches to estimating abortion incidence". Reproductive Health. 16 (1): 44. doi:10.1186/s12978-019-0702-0. PMC 6472065. PMID 30999917.
  5. ^ Presser, Stanley; Stinson, Linda (1998). "Data Collection Mode and Social Desirability Bias in Self-Reported Religious Attendance". American Sociological Review. 63 (1): 137–145. doi:10.2307/2657486. JSTOR 2657486.
  6. ^ Brian, Duff; Hanmer, Michael J.; Park, Won-Ho; White, Ismail K. (2007). "Good Excuses: Understanding Who Votes With An Improved Turnout Question". Public Opinion Quarterly. 71 (1): 67–90. doi:10.1093/poq/nfl045.
  7. ^ Hanmer, Michael J.; Banks, Antoine J.; White, Ismail K. (2013). "Experiments to reduce the over-reporting of voting: A pipeline to the truth". Political Analysis. 22 (1): 130–141. doi:10.1093/pan/mpt027.
  8. ^ Morin-Chassé, Alexandre; Bol, Damien; Stephenson, Laura B.; Labbé St-Vincent, Simon (2017). "How to survey about electoral turnout? The efficacy of the face-saving response items in 19 different contexts" (PDF). Political Science Research and Methods. 5 (3): 575–584. doi:10.1017/psrm.2016.31. S2CID 148277964.
  9. ^ Morin-Chassé, Alexandre (2018). "How to Survey About Electoral Turnout? Additional Evidence". Journal of Experimental Political Science. 5 (3): 230–233. doi:10.1017/XPS.2018.1. S2CID 158608425.
  10. ^ Edwards, Allen (1953). "The relationship between the judged desirability of a trait and the probability that the trait will be endorsed". Journal of Applied Psychology. 37 (2): 90–93. doi:10.1037/h0058073.
  11. ^ Fordyce, William (1956). "Social desirability in the MMPI". Journal of Consulting Psychology. 20 (3): 171–175. doi:10.1037/h0048547. PMID 13357640.
  12. ^ Linehan, Marsha (1981). "Assessment of suicide ideation and parasuicide: Hopelessness and social desirability". Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 49 (5): 773–775. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.49.5.773. PMID 7287996.
  13. ^ Crowne, Douglas P.; Marlowe, David (1960). "A new scale of social desirability independent of psychopathology". Journal of Consulting Psychology. 24 (4): 349–354. doi:10.1037/h0047358. PMID 13813058. S2CID 9781635.
  14. ^ Thompson, Edmund R.; Phua, Florence T. T. (2005). "Reliability among Senior Managers of the Marlowe–Crowne Short-Form Social Desirability Scale". Journal of Business and Psychology. 19 (4): 541–554. doi:10.1007/s10869-005-4524-4. S2CID 143818289.
  15. ^ Paulhus, D.L. (1991). Measurement and control of response biases. In J.P. Robinson et al. (Eds.), Measures of personality and social psychological attitudes. San Diego: Academic Press
  16. ^ Paulhus D.L., (1998) Paulhus Deception Scales (PDS) is published by Multi-Health Systems of Toronto.
  17. ^ Roccato M., (2003) Desiderabilità Sociale e Acquiescenza. Alcune Trappole delle Inchieste e dei Sondaggi. LED Edizioni Universitarie, Torino. ISBN 88-7916-216-0
  18. ^ Corbetta P., (2003) La ricerca sociale: metodologia e tecniche. Vol. I-IV. Il Mulino, Bologna.
  19. ^ Stöber, Joachim (2001). "The Social Desirability Scale-17 (SDS-17)" (PDF). European Journal of Psychological Assessment. 17 (3): 222–232. doi:10.1027//1015-5759.17.3.222. S2CID 51780539.
  20. ^ a b Nederhof, Anton J. (1985-07-01). "Methods of coping with social desirability bias: A review". European Journal of Social Psychology. 15 (3): 263–280. doi:10.1002/ejsp.2420150303.
  21. ^ McBurney D.H., (1994) Research Methods. Brooks/Cole, Pacific Grove, California.
  22. ^ a b Tourangeau, R.; Yan, T. (2007). "Sensitive questions in surveys". Psychological Bulletin. 133 (5): 859–83. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.563.2414. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.133.5.859. PMID 17723033. S2CID 7160451.
  23. ^ a b Lewis, James JC; Ronsmans, Carine; Ezeh, Alex; Gregson, Simon (June 2004). "The population impact of HIV on fertility in sub-Saharan Africa". AIDS. 18 (Supplement 2): S35–S43. doi:10.1097/00002030-200406002-00005. ISSN 0269-9370. PMID 15319742.
  24. ^ a b c d Bova, Christopher S.; Aswani, Shankar; Farthing, Matthew W.; Potts, Warren M. (2018-12-01). "Limitations of the random response technique and a call to implement the ballot box method for estimating recreational angler compliance using surveys". Fisheries Research. 208: 34–41. doi:10.1016/j.fishres.2018.06.017. ISSN 0165-7836. S2CID 92793552.
  25. ^ Arias, Melissa; Hinsley, Amy; Nogales-Ascarrunz, Paola; Negroes, Nuno; Glikman, Jenny Anne; Milner-Gulland, E. J. (July 2021). "Prevalence and characteristics of illegal jaguar trade in north-western Bolivia". Conservation Science and Practice. 3 (7). Bibcode:2021ConSP...3E.444A. doi:10.1111/csp2.444. ISSN 2578-4854.
  26. ^ John, Leslie K.; Loewenstein, George; Acquisti, Alessandro; Vosgerau, Joachim (September 2018). "When and why randomized response techniques (fail to) elicit the truth". Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes. 148: 101–123. doi:10.1016/j.obhdp.2018.07.004. S2CID 52263233.
  27. ^ Miller, J.D. (1985). "The nominative technique: a new method of estimating heroin prevalence" (PDF). NIDA Research Monograph. 54: 104–124. PMID 3929108.
  28. ^ Yeatman, Sara; Trinitapoli, Jenny (2011-09-01). "Best-Friend Reports: A Tool for Measuring the Prevalence of Sensitive Behaviors". American Journal of Public Health. 101 (9): 1666–1667. doi:10.2105/AJPH.2011.300194. PMC 3154247. PMID 21778489.
  29. ^ Droitcour, Judith; Caspar, Rachel A.; Hubbard, Michael L.; Parsley, Teresa L.; Visscher, Wendy; Ezzati, Trena M. (2011), "The Item Count Technique as a Method of Indirect Questioning: A Review of Its Development and a Case Study Application", Measurement Errors in Surveys, John Wiley & Sons, Ltd, pp. 185–210, doi:10.1002/9781118150382.ch11, ISBN 9781118150382
  30. ^ Droitcour, Judith A.; Larson, Eric M. (2016-07-22). "An Innovative Technique for Asking Sensitive Questions: the Three-Card Method". Bulletin of Sociological Methodology/Bulletin de Méthodologie Sociologique. 75: 5–23. doi:10.1177/075910630207500103. S2CID 73189531.
  31. ^ Yu, Jun-Wu; Tian, Guo-Liang; Tang, Man-Lai (2007-04-18). "Two new models for survey sampling with sensitive characteristic: design and analysis". Metrika. 67 (3): 251. doi:10.1007/s00184-007-0131-x. S2CID 122941401.
  32. ^ Schnapp, Patrick (2019). "Sensitive Question Techniques and Careless Responding: Adjusting the Crosswise Model for Random Answers". Methods, Data, Analyses. 13: 307–320. doi:10.12758/mda.2019.03. S2CID 88504613.
  33. ^ Roese, N. J., & Jamieson, D. W. (1993). Twenty years of bogus pipeline research: A critical review and meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 114(2), 363–375. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.114.2.363

social, desirability, bias, social, science, research, social, desirability, bias, type, response, bias, that, tendency, survey, respondents, answer, questions, manner, that, will, viewed, favorably, others, take, form, over, reporting, good, behavior, under, . In social science research social desirability bias is a type of response bias that is the tendency of survey respondents to answer questions in a manner that will be viewed favorably by others 1 It can take the form of over reporting good behavior or under reporting bad or undesirable behavior The tendency poses a serious problem with conducting research with self reports This bias interferes with the interpretation of average tendencies as well as individual differences Contents 1 Topics subject to social desirability bias 2 Individual differences in socially desirable responding 3 Standard measures of individual SDR 4 Techniques to reduce social desirability bias 4 1 Anonymity and confidentiality 4 2 Specialized questioning techniques 4 2 1 Ballot Box Method 4 2 2 Randomized response techniques 4 2 3 Nominative and best friend techniques 4 2 4 Unmatched count technique 4 2 5 Grouped answer method 4 2 6 Crosswise triangular and hidden sensitivity methods 4 2 7 Bogus pipeline 5 Other response styles 6 See also 7 ReferencesTopics subject to social desirability bias editTopics where socially desirable responding SDR is of special concern are self reports of abilities personality sexual behavior and drug use When confronted with the question How often do you masturbate for example respondents may be pressured by a social taboo against masturbation and either under report the frequency or avoid answering the question Therefore the mean rates of masturbation derived from self report surveys are likely to be severely underestimated When confronted with the question Do you use drugs illicit substances the respondent may be influenced by the fact that controlled substances including the more commonly used marijuana are generally illegal Respondents may feel pressured to deny any drug use or rationalize it e g I only smoke marijuana when my friends are around The bias can also influence reports of number of sexual partners In fact the bias may operate in opposite directions for different subgroups Whereas men tend to inflate the numbers women tend to underestimate theirs In either case the mean reports from both groups are likely to be distorted by social desirability bias Other topics that are sensitive to social desirability bias include Self reported personality traits will correlate strongly with social desirability bias 2 Personal income and earnings often inflated when low and deflated when high Feelings of low self worth and or powerlessness often denied Excretory functions often approached uncomfortably if discussed at all Compliance with medicinal dosing schedules often inflated Family planning including use of contraceptives and abortion 3 4 Religion often either avoided or uncomfortably approached 5 Patriotism either inflated or if denied done so with a fear of other party s judgment Bigotry and intolerance often denied even if it exists within the responder Intellectual achievements often inflated Physical appearance either inflated or deflated Acts of real or imagined physical violence often denied Indicators of charity or benevolence often inflated Illegal acts often denied Voter turnout 6 7 8 9 Individual differences in socially desirable responding editIn 1953 Allen L Edwards introduced the notion of social desirability to psychology demonstrating the role of social desirability in the measurement of personality traits He demonstrated that social desirability ratings of personality trait descriptions are very highly correlated with the probability that a subsequent group of people will endorse these trait self descriptions In his first demonstration of this pattern the correlation between one group of college students social desirability ratings of a set of traits and the probability that college students in a second group would endorse self descriptions describing the same traits was so high that it could distort the meaning of the personality traits In other words do these self descriptions describe personality traits or social desirability 10 Edwards subsequently developed the first Social Desirability Scale a set of 39 true false questions extracted from the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory MMPI questions that judges could with high agreement order according to their social desirability 2 These items were subsequently found to be very highly correlated with a wide range of measurement scales MMPI personality and diagnostic scales 11 The SDS is also highly correlated with the Beck Hopelessness Inventory 12 The fact that people differ in their tendency to engage in socially desirable responding SDR is a special concern to those measuring individual differences with self reports Individual differences in SDR make it difficult to distinguish those people with good traits who are responding factually from those distorting their answers in a positive direction When SDR cannot be eliminated researchers may resort to evaluating the tendency and then control for it A separate SDR measure must be administered together with the primary measure test or interview aimed at the subject matter of the research investigation The key assumption is that respondents who answer in a socially desirable manner on that scale are also responding desirably to all self reports throughout the study In some cases the entire questionnaire package from high scoring respondents may simply be discarded Alternatively respondents answers on the primary questionnaires may be statistically adjusted commensurate with their SDR tendencies For example this adjustment is performed automatically in the standard scoring of MMPI scales The major concern with SDR scales is that they confound style with content After all people actually differ in the degree to which they possess desirable traits e g nuns versus criminals Consequently measures of social desirability confound true differences with social desirability bias Standard measures of individual SDR editUntil the 1990s the most commonly used measure of socially desirable responding was the Marlowe Crowne Social Desirability Scale 13 The original version comprised 33 True False items A shortened version the Strahan Gerbasi only comprises ten items but some have raised questions regarding the reliability of this measure 14 In 1991 Delroy L Paulhus published the Balanced Inventory of Desirable Responding BIDR a questionnaire designed to measure two forms of SDR 15 This forty item instrument provides separate subscales for impression management the tendency to give inflated self descriptions to an audience and self deceptive enhancement the tendency to give honest but inflated self descriptions The commercial version of the BIDR is called the Paulhus Deception Scales PDS 16 Scales designed to tap response styles are available in all major languages including Italian 17 18 and German 19 Techniques to reduce social desirability bias editAnonymity and confidentiality edit Anonymous survey administration compared with in person or phone based administration has been shown to elicit higher reporting of items with social desirability bias 20 In anonymous survey settings the subject is assured that their responses will not be linked to them and they are not asked to divulge sensitive information directly to a surveyor Anonymity can be established through self administration of paper surveys returned by envelope mail or ballot boxes or self administration of electronic survey via computer smartphone or tablet 1 21 Audio assisted electronic surveys have also been established for low literacy or non literate study subjects 1 22 Confidentiality can be established in non anonymous settings by ensuring that only study staff are present and by maintaining data confidentiality after surveys are complete Including assurances of data confidentiality in surveys has a mixed effect on sensitive question response it may either increase response due to increased trust or decrease response by increasing suspicion and concern 1 Specialized questioning techniques edit Several techniques have been established to reduce bias when asking questions sensitive to social desirability 20 Complex question techniques may reduce social desirability bias but may also be confusing or misunderstood by respondents Beyond specific techniques social desirability bias may be reduced by neutral question and prompt wording 1 Ballot Box Method edit The Ballot Box Method BBM provides survey respondents anonymity by allowing them to respond in private by self completing their responses to the sensitive survey questions on a secret ballot and submitting them to a locked box The interviewer has no knowledge of what is recorded on the secret ballot and does not have access to the lock on the box providing obscurity to the responses and limiting the potential for SDB However a unique control number on each ballot allows the answers to be reunited with a corresponding questionnaire that contains less sensitive questions 23 24 The BBM has been used successfully to obtain estimates of sensitive sexual behaviours during an HIV prevention study 23 as well as illegal environmental resource use 24 25 In a validation study where observed behaviour was matched to reported behaviour using various SDB control methods the BBM was by far the most accurate bias reduction method performing significantly better than the Randomized Response Technique RRT 24 Randomized response techniques edit The randomized response technique asks a participant to respond with a fixed answer or to answer truthfully based on the outcome of a random act 22 For example respondents secretly throw a coin and respond yes if it comes up heads regardless of their actual response to the question and are instructed to respond truthfully if it comes up tails This enables the researcher to estimate the actual prevalence of the given behavior among the study population without needing to know the true state of any one individual respondent Research shows that the validity of the randomized response technique is limited 26 Validation research has shown that the RRT actually performs worse than direct questioning for some sensitive behaviours and care should be taken when considering its use 24 Nominative and best friend techniques edit The nominative technique asks a participant about the behavior of their close friends rather than about their own behavior 27 Participants are asked how many close friends they know have done for certain a sensitive behavior and how many other people they think know about that behavior Population estimates of behaviors can be derived from the response The similar best friend methodology asks the participant about the behavior of one best friend 28 Unmatched count technique edit The unmatched count technique asks respondents to indicate how many of a list of several items they have done or are true for them 29 Respondents are randomized to receive either a list of non sensitive items or that same list plus the sensitive item of interest Differences in the total number of items between the two groups indicate how many of those in the group receiving the sensitive item said yes to it Grouped answer method edit The grouped answer method also known as the two card or three card method combines answer choices such that the sensitive response is combined with at least one non sensitive response option 30 Crosswise triangular and hidden sensitivity methods edit These methods ask participants to select one response based on two or more questions only one of which is sensitive 31 For example a participant will be asked whether their birth year is even and whether they have performed an illegal activity if yes to both or no to both to select A and if yes to one but no to the other select B By combining sensitive and non sensitive questions the participant s response to the sensitive item is masked Research shows that the validity of the crosswise model is limited 32 Bogus pipeline edit Bogus pipeline techniques are those in which a participant believes that an objective test like a lie detector will be used along with survey response whether or not that test or procedure is actually used 1 Researches using this technique must convince the participants that there is a machine that can measure accurately their true attitudes and desires While this can raise ethical questions surrounding deception in psychological research this technique quickly became widely popular in the 1970s However by the 1990s the use of this technique began to wane Interested in this change Roese amp Jamison 1993 took twenty years of research to do a meta analysis on the effectiveness of the Bogus pipeline technique in reducing social desirability bias They concluded that the Bogus pipeline technique while significantly effective it perhaps became less and less used simply because it went out of fashion or became a bit cumbersome for researches to use regularly However they argue there are simple adjustments that can be made to this technique to make it more user friendly for researchers 33 Other response styles edit Extreme response style ERS takes the form of exaggerated extremity preference e g for 1 or 7 on 7 point scales Its converse moderacy bias entails a preference for middle range or midpoint responses e g 3 5 on 7 point scales Acquiescence ARS is the tendency to respond to items with agreement affirmation independent of their content yea saying These kinds of response styles differ from social desirability bias in that they are unrelated to the question s content and may be present in both socially neutral and in socially favorable or unfavorable contexts whereas SDR is by definition tied to the latter See also editBiased random walk on a graph Bradley effect Knowledge falsification Performative activism Preference falsification Pseudo opinion Reactivity psychology Response bias Self censorship Self report study Silent majority Social influence bias Social media bias Social research Virtue signalling Watching eye effectReferences edit a b c d e f Krumpal Ivar 2013 Determinants of social desirability bias in sensitive surveys a literature review Quality amp Quantity 47 4 2025 2047 doi 10 1007 s11135 011 9640 9 S2CID 143045969 a b Edwards Allen 1957 The social desirability variable in personality assessment and research New York The Dryden Press Stuart Gretchen S Grimes David A 2009 Social desirability bias in family planning studies A neglected problem Contraception 80 2 108 112 doi 10 1016 j contraception 2009 02 009 PMID 19631784 Sedgh Gilda Keogh Sarah C 2019 04 18 Novel approaches to estimating abortion incidence Reproductive Health 16 1 44 doi 10 1186 s12978 019 0702 0 PMC 6472065 PMID 30999917 Presser Stanley Stinson Linda 1998 Data Collection Mode and Social Desirability Bias in Self Reported Religious Attendance American Sociological Review 63 1 137 145 doi 10 2307 2657486 JSTOR 2657486 Brian Duff Hanmer Michael J Park Won Ho White Ismail K 2007 Good Excuses Understanding Who Votes With An Improved Turnout Question Public Opinion Quarterly 71 1 67 90 doi 10 1093 poq nfl045 Hanmer Michael J Banks Antoine J White Ismail K 2013 Experiments to reduce the over reporting of voting A pipeline to the truth Political Analysis 22 1 130 141 doi 10 1093 pan mpt027 Morin Chasse Alexandre Bol Damien Stephenson Laura B Labbe St Vincent Simon 2017 How to survey about electoral turnout The efficacy of the face saving response items in 19 different contexts PDF Political Science Research and Methods 5 3 575 584 doi 10 1017 psrm 2016 31 S2CID 148277964 Morin Chasse Alexandre 2018 How to Survey About Electoral Turnout Additional Evidence Journal of Experimental Political Science 5 3 230 233 doi 10 1017 XPS 2018 1 S2CID 158608425 Edwards Allen 1953 The relationship between the judged desirability of a trait and the probability that the trait will be endorsed Journal of Applied Psychology 37 2 90 93 doi 10 1037 h0058073 Fordyce William 1956 Social desirability in the MMPI Journal of Consulting Psychology 20 3 171 175 doi 10 1037 h0048547 PMID 13357640 Linehan Marsha 1981 Assessment of suicide ideation and parasuicide Hopelessness and social desirability Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 49 5 773 775 doi 10 1037 0022 006X 49 5 773 PMID 7287996 Crowne Douglas P Marlowe David 1960 A new scale of social desirability independent of psychopathology Journal of Consulting Psychology 24 4 349 354 doi 10 1037 h0047358 PMID 13813058 S2CID 9781635 Thompson Edmund R Phua Florence T T 2005 Reliability among Senior Managers of the Marlowe Crowne Short Form Social Desirability Scale Journal of Business and Psychology 19 4 541 554 doi 10 1007 s10869 005 4524 4 S2CID 143818289 Paulhus D L 1991 Measurement and control of response biases In J P Robinson et al Eds Measures of personality and social psychological attitudes San Diego Academic Press Paulhus D L 1998 Paulhus Deception Scales PDS is published by Multi Health Systems of Toronto Roccato M 2003 Desiderabilita Sociale e Acquiescenza Alcune Trappole delle Inchieste e dei Sondaggi LED Edizioni Universitarie Torino ISBN 88 7916 216 0 Corbetta P 2003 La ricerca sociale metodologia e tecniche Vol I IV Il Mulino Bologna Stober Joachim 2001 The Social Desirability Scale 17 SDS 17 PDF European Journal of Psychological Assessment 17 3 222 232 doi 10 1027 1015 5759 17 3 222 S2CID 51780539 a b Nederhof Anton J 1985 07 01 Methods of coping with social desirability bias A review European Journal of Social Psychology 15 3 263 280 doi 10 1002 ejsp 2420150303 McBurney D H 1994 Research Methods Brooks Cole Pacific Grove California a b Tourangeau R Yan T 2007 Sensitive questions in surveys Psychological Bulletin 133 5 859 83 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 563 2414 doi 10 1037 0033 2909 133 5 859 PMID 17723033 S2CID 7160451 a b Lewis James JC Ronsmans Carine Ezeh Alex Gregson Simon June 2004 The population impact of HIV on fertility in sub Saharan Africa AIDS 18 Supplement 2 S35 S43 doi 10 1097 00002030 200406002 00005 ISSN 0269 9370 PMID 15319742 a b c d Bova Christopher S Aswani Shankar Farthing Matthew W Potts Warren M 2018 12 01 Limitations of the random response technique and a call to implement the ballot box method for estimating recreational angler compliance using surveys Fisheries Research 208 34 41 doi 10 1016 j fishres 2018 06 017 ISSN 0165 7836 S2CID 92793552 Arias Melissa Hinsley Amy Nogales Ascarrunz Paola Negroes Nuno Glikman Jenny Anne Milner Gulland E J July 2021 Prevalence and characteristics of illegal jaguar trade in north western Bolivia Conservation Science and Practice 3 7 Bibcode 2021ConSP 3E 444A doi 10 1111 csp2 444 ISSN 2578 4854 John Leslie K Loewenstein George Acquisti Alessandro Vosgerau Joachim September 2018 When and why randomized response techniques fail to elicit the truth Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 148 101 123 doi 10 1016 j obhdp 2018 07 004 S2CID 52263233 Miller J D 1985 The nominative technique a new method of estimating heroin prevalence PDF NIDA Research Monograph 54 104 124 PMID 3929108 Yeatman Sara Trinitapoli Jenny 2011 09 01 Best Friend Reports A Tool for Measuring the Prevalence of Sensitive Behaviors American Journal of Public Health 101 9 1666 1667 doi 10 2105 AJPH 2011 300194 PMC 3154247 PMID 21778489 Droitcour Judith Caspar Rachel A Hubbard Michael L Parsley Teresa L Visscher Wendy Ezzati Trena M 2011 The Item Count Technique as a Method of Indirect Questioning A Review of Its Development and a Case Study Application Measurement Errors in Surveys John Wiley amp Sons Ltd pp 185 210 doi 10 1002 9781118150382 ch11 ISBN 9781118150382 Droitcour Judith A Larson Eric M 2016 07 22 An Innovative Technique for Asking Sensitive Questions the Three Card Method Bulletin of Sociological Methodology Bulletin de Methodologie Sociologique 75 5 23 doi 10 1177 075910630207500103 S2CID 73189531 Yu Jun Wu Tian Guo Liang Tang Man Lai 2007 04 18 Two new models for survey sampling with sensitive characteristic design and analysis Metrika 67 3 251 doi 10 1007 s00184 007 0131 x S2CID 122941401 Schnapp Patrick 2019 Sensitive Question Techniques and Careless Responding Adjusting the Crosswise Model for Random Answers Methods Data Analyses 13 307 320 doi 10 12758 mda 2019 03 S2CID 88504613 Roese N J amp Jamieson D W 1993 Twenty years of bogus pipeline research A critical review and meta analysis Psychological Bulletin 114 2 363 375 https doi org 10 1037 0033 2909 114 2 363 Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Social desirability bias amp oldid 1223016575, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.