fbpx
Wikipedia

Dog behavior

Dog behavior is the internally coordinated responses of individuals or groups of domestic dogs to internal and external stimuli.[1] It has been shaped by millennia of contact with humans and their lifestyles. As a result of this physical and social evolution, dogs have acquired the ability to understand and communicate with humans.[2] Behavioral scientists have uncovered a wide range of social-cognitive abilities in domestic dogs.

A drawing by Konrad Lorenz showing facial expressions of a dog - a communication behavior. X-axis is aggression, y-axis is fear.

Co-evolution with humans Edit

The origin of the domestic dog (Canis familiaris) is not clear. Whole-genome sequencing indicates that the dog, the gray wolf and the extinct Taymyr wolf diverged around the same time 27,000–40,000 years ago.[3] How dogs became domesticated is not clear, however the two main hypotheses are self-domestication or human domestication. There exists evidence of human-canine behavioral coevolution.

Intelligence Edit

Dog intelligence is the ability of the dog to perceive information and retain it as knowledge in order to solve problems. Dogs have been shown to learn by inference. A study with Rico showed that he knew the labels of over 200 different items.[4] He inferred the names of novel items by exclusion learning and correctly retrieved those novel items immediately. He also retained this ability four weeks after the initial exposure. Dogs have advanced memory skills. A study documented the learning and memory capabilities of a border collie, "Chaser", who had learned the names and could associate by verbal command over 1,000 words. Dogs are able to read and react appropriately to human body language such as gesturing and pointing, and to understand human voice commands. After undergoing training to solve a simple manipulation task, dogs that are faced with an insolvable version of the same problem look at the human, while socialized wolves do not. Dogs demonstrate a theory of mind by engaging in deception.[5][6]

Senses Edit

The dog's senses include vision, hearing, sense of smell, taste, touch, proprioception, and sensitivity to the Earth's magnetic field.

Communication behavior Edit

Dog communication is about how dogs "speak" to each other, how they understand messages that humans send to them, and how humans can translate the ideas that dogs are trying to transmit.[7]: xii  These communication behaviors include eye gaze, facial expression, vocalization, body posture (including movements of bodies and limbs) and gustatory communication (scents, pheromones and taste). Humans communicate with dogs by using vocalization, hand signals, and body posture. Dogs can also learn to understand the communication of emotions with humans by reading human facial expressions.[8]

Social behavior Edit

Two studies have indicated that dog behavior vary based on their size, body weight, and skull size.[9][10]

Play Edit

Dog and dog Edit

Play between dogs usually involves several behaviors often seen in aggressive encounters, such as nipping, biting and growling.[11] It is therefore important for the dogs to place these behaviors in the context of the play, rather than aggression. Dogs signal their intent to play with a range of behaviors including a "play-bow", "face-paw," "open-mouthed play face" and postures inviting the other dog to chase the initiator. Similar signals are given throughout the play to maintain the context of the potentially aggressive activities.[12]

From a young age, dogs engage in play with one another. Dog play is made up primarily of mock fights. It is believed that this behavior, which is most common in puppies, is training for important behaviors later in life. Play between puppies is not necessarily a 50:50 symmetry of dominant and submissive roles between the individuals; dogs who engage in greater rates of dominant behaviors (e.g. chasing, forcing partners down) at later ages also initiate play at higher rates. This could imply that winning during play becomes more important as puppies mature.[13]

Emotional contagion is linked to facial mimicry in humans and primates. Facial mimicry is an automatic response that occurs in less than 1 second in which one person involuntarily mimics another person's facial expressions, forming empathy. It has also been found in dogs at play, and play sessions lasted longer when there were facial mimicry signals from one dog to another.[14]

Dog and human Edit

 
NASA astronaut Leland D. Melvin with his dogs Jake and Scout

The motivation for a dog to play with another dog is different from that of a dog playing with a human. Dogs walked together with opportunities to play with one another and play with their owners with the same frequency as dogs being walked alone. Dogs in households with two or more dogs play more often with their owners than dogs in households with a single dog, indicating the motivation to play with other dogs does not substitute for the motivation to play with humans.[15]

It is a common misconception that winning and losing games such as "tug-of-war" and "rough-and-tumble" can influence a dog's dominant relationship with humans. Rather, how dogs play indicates their temperament and relationship with their owner. Dogs that play rough-and-tumble are more amenable and show lower separation anxiety than dogs which play other types of games, and dogs playing tug-of-war and "fetch" are more confident. Dogs that start most games are less amenable and more likely to be aggressive.[16]

Playing with humans can affect the cortisol levels of dogs. In one study, the cortisol responses of police dogs and border guard dogs were assessed after playing with their handlers. The cortisol concentrations of the police dogs increased, whereas the border guard dogs' hormone levels decreased. The researchers noted that during the play sessions, police officers were disciplining their dogs, whereas the border guards were truly playing with them, i.e. this included bonding and affectionate behaviors. They commented that several studies have shown that behaviors associated with control, authority or aggression increase cortisol, whereas play and affiliation behavior decrease cortisol levels.[17]

Empathy Edit

In 2012, a study found that dogs oriented toward their owner or a stranger more often when the person was pretending to cry than when they were talking or humming. When the stranger pretended to cry, rather than approaching their usual source of comfort, their owner, dogs sniffed, nuzzled and licked the stranger instead. The dogs' pattern of response was behaviorally consistent with an expression of empathic concern.[18]

A study found a third of dogs suffered from anxiety when separated from others.[19]

Personalities Edit

The term personality has been applied to human research, whereas the term temperament has been mostly used for animal research.[20] However, both terms have been used interchangeably in the literature, or purely to distinguish humans from animals and avoid anthropomorphism.[21] Personality can be defined as “a set of behaviors that are consistent over context and time”.[22] Studies of dogs' personalities have tried to identify the presence of broad personality traits that are stable and consistent over time.[21][22][23][24][25]

There are different approaches to assess dog personality:

  • Ratings of individual dogs: either a caretaker or a dog expert who is familiar with the dog is asked to answer a questionnaire, for instance the Canine Behavioral Assessment and Research Questionnaire,[26] concerning how often the dog shows certain types of behavior.
  • Tests: the dog is submitted to a set of tests and its reactions are evaluated on a behavioral scale. For instance, the dog is presented to a familiar and then an unfamiliar person in order to measure sociability or aggression.[27]
  • Observational test: The dog’s behavior is evaluated in a selected but not controlled environment. An observer focuses on the dog’s reactions to naturally occurring stimuli. For example, a walk through the supermarket can allow the observer to see the dog in various types of conditions (crowded, noisy…)[28]

Several potential personality traits have been identified in dogs, for instance "Playfulness", "Curiosity/Fearlessness, "Chase-proneness", "Sociability and Aggressiveness" and "Shyness–Boldness".[29][30] A meta-analysis of 51 published peer reviewed articles identified seven dimensions of canine personality:[21]

  1. Reactivity (approach or avoidance of new objects, increased activity in novel situations)
  2. Fearfulness (shaking, avoiding novel situations)
  3. Activity
  4. Sociability (initiating friendly interactions with people and other dogs)
  5. Responsiveness to training (working with people, learning quickly)
  6. Submissiveness
  7. Aggression

With regard to the nature versus nurture debate, according to a study in April 2022 carried out by Kathleen Morrill and others who work in a laboratory that was based on genetic and survey data of nearly 2000 dogs, with the majority of them having their entire genomes sequenced, as well as survey results from 16,000 owners of dogs. The dogs included mixes and purebreds, with 128 breeds represented. The study found that the physical traits of a dog can be attributed to 80% on DNA and that retrieving and friendliness around humans were predominantly genetic. But, breed alone is only responsible for about 9% of individual personality differences, with about 25% of personality traits determined by (mainly individual) genetics in total, and the rest determined by the environment.[31]

The effects of age and sex have not been clearly determined.[22] The personality models can be used for a range of tasks, including guide and working dog selection, finding appropriate families to re-home shelter dogs, or selecting breeding stock.[32][33][34]

Leadership, dominance and social groups Edit

 
Two dogs playing follow the leader.

Dominance is a descriptive term for the relationship between pairs of individuals. Among ethologists, dominance has been defined as "an attribute of the pattern of repeated, antagonistic interactions between two individuals, characterized by a consistent outcome in favor of the same dyad member and a default yielding response of its opponent rather than escalation. The status of the consistent winner is dominant and that of the loser subordinate."[35] Another definition is that a dominant animal has "priority of access to resources".[35] Dominance is a relative attribute, not absolute; there is no reason to assume that a high-ranking individual in one group would also become high ranking if moved to another. Nor is there any good evidence that "dominance" is a lifelong character trait. Competitive behavior characterized by confident (e.g. growl, inhibited bite, stand over, stare at, chase, bark at) and submissive (e.g. crouch, avoid, displacement lick/yawn, run away) patterns exchanged.[36]

One test to ascertain in which group the dominant dog was used the following criteria: When a stranger comes to the house, which dog starts to bark first or if they start to bark together, which dog barks more or longer? Which dog licks more often the other dog's mouth? If the dogs get food at the same time and at the same spot, which dog starts to eat first or eats the other dog's food? If the dogs start to fight, which dog usually wins?[37]

Domestic dogs appear to pay little attention to relative size, despite the large weight differences between the largest and smallest individuals; for example, size was not a predictor of the outcome of encounters between dogs meeting while being exercised by their owners nor was size correlated with neutered male dogs.[38] Therefore, many dogs do not appear to pay much attention to the actual fighting ability of their opponent, presumably allowing differences in motivation (how much the dog values the resource) and perceived motivation (what the behavior of the other dog signifies about the likelihood that it will escalate) to play a much greater role.[36]

Two dogs that are contesting possession of a highly valued resource for the first time, if one is in a state of emotional arousal, in pain; if reactivity is influenced by recent endocrine changes, or motivational states such as hunger, then the outcome of the interaction may be different than if none of these factors were present. Equally, the threshold at which aggression is shown may be influenced by a range of medical factors, or, in some cases, precipitated entirely by pathological disorders. Hence, the contextual and physiological factors present when two dogs first encounter each other may profoundly influence the long-term nature of the relationship between those dogs. The complexity of the factors involved in this type of learning means that dogs may develop different "expectations" about the likely response of another individual for each resource in a range of different situations. Puppies learn early not to challenge an older dog and this respect stays with them into adulthood. When adult animals meet for the first time, they have no expectations of the behavior of the other: they will both, therefore, be initially anxious and vigilant in this encounter (characterized by the tense body posture and sudden movements typically seen when two dogs first meet), until they start to be able to predict the responses of the other individual. The outcome of these early adult–adult interactions will be influenced by the specific factors present at the time of the initial encounters. As well as contextual and physiological factors, the experiences of each member of the dyad of other dogs will also influence their behavior.[36]

Scent Edit

Dogs have an olfactory sense 40 times more sensitive than a human's and they commence their lives operating almost exclusively on smell and touch.[7]: 247  The special scents that dogs use for communication are called pheromones. Different hormones are secreted when a dog is angry, fearful or confident, and some chemical signatures identify the sex and age of the dog, and if a female is in the estrus cycle, pregnant or recently given birth. Many of the pheromone chemicals can be found dissolved in a dog's urine, and sniffing where another dog has urinated gives the dog a great deal of information about that dog.[7]: 250  Male dogs prefer to mark vertical surfaces and having the scent higher allows the air to carry it farther. The height of the marking tells other dogs about the size of the dog, as among canines size is an important factor in dominance.[7]: 251 

Dogs (and wolves) mark their territories with urine and their stools.[39] The anal gland of canines give a particular signature to fecal deposits and identifies the marker as well as the place where the dung is left. Dogs are very particular about these landmarks, and engage in what is to humans a meaningless and complex ritual before defecating. Most dogs start with a careful bout of sniffing of a location, perhaps to erect an exact line or boundary between their territory and another dog's territory. This behavior may also involve a small degree of elevation, such as a rock or fallen branch, to aid scent dispersal. Scratching the ground after defecating is a visual sign pointing to the scent marking. The freshness of the scent gives visitors some idea of the current status of a piece of territory and if it is used frequently. Regions under dispute, or used by different animals at different times, may lead to marking battles with every scent marked-over by a new competitor.[7]: 252–4 

Feral dogs Edit

Feral dogs are those dogs living in a wild state with no food and shelter intentionally provided by humans, and showing a continuous and strong avoidance of direct human contacts.[40] In the developing world pet dogs are uncommon, but feral, village or community dogs are plentiful around humans.[41] The distinction between feral, stray, and free-ranging dogs is sometimes a matter of degree, and a dog may shift its status throughout its life. In some unlikely but observed cases, a feral dog that was not born wild but living with a feral group can become behavior-modified to a domestic dog with an owner. A dog can become a stray when it escapes human control, by abandonment or being born to a stray mother. A stray dog can become feral when forced out of the human environment or when co-opted or socially accepted by a nearby feral group. Feralization occurs through the development of the human avoidance response.[40]

Feral dogs are not reproductively self-sustaining, suffer from high rates of juvenile mortality, and depend indirectly on humans for their food, their space, and the supply of co-optable individuals.[40]

See further: behavior compared to other canids.

Other behavior Edit

Dogs have a general behavioral trait of strongly preferring novelty ("neophillia") compared to familiarity.[42] The average sleep time of a dog in captivity in a 24-hour period is 10.1 hours.[43]

Reproduction behavior Edit

Estrous cycle and mating Edit

Although puppies do not have the urge to procreate, males sometimes engage in sexual play in the form of mounting.[44] In some puppies, this behavior occurs as early as 3 or 4 weeks-of-age.[45]

Dogs reach sexual maturity and can reproduce during their first year, in contrast to wolves at two years-of-age. Female dogs have their first estrus ("heat") at 6 to 12 months-of-age; smaller dogs tend to come into heat earlier whereas larger dogs take longer to mature.

Female dogs have an estrous cycle that is nonseasonal and monestrus, i.e. there is only one estrus per estrous cycle. The interval between one estrus and another is, on average, seven months, however, this may range between 4 and 12 months. This interestrous period is not influenced by the photoperiod or pregnancy. The average duration of estrus is 9 days with spontaneous ovulation usually about 3 days after the onset of estrus.[46]

For several days before estrus, a phase called proestrus, the female dog may show greater interest in male dogs and "flirt" with them (proceptive behavior). There is progressive vulval swelling and some bleeding. If males try to mount a female dog during proestrus, she may avoid mating by sitting down or turning round and growling or snapping.

Estrous behavior in the female dog is usually indicated by her standing still with the tail held up, or to the side of the perineum, when the male sniffs the vulva and attempts to mount. This tail position is sometimes called “flagging”. The female dog may also turn, presenting the vulva to the male.[46]

The male dog mounts the female and is able to achieve intromission with a non-erect penis, which contains a bone called the os penis. The dog's penis enlarges inside the vagina, thereby preventing its withdrawal; this is sometimes known as the "tie" or "copulatory lock". The male dog rapidly thrust into the female for 1–2 minutes then dismounts with the erect penis still inside the vagina, and turns to stand rear-end to rear-end with the female dog for up to 30 to 40 minutes; the penis is twisted 180 degrees in a lateral plane. During this time, prostatic fluid is ejaculated.[46]

The female dog can bear another litter within 8 months of the previous one. Dogs are polygamous in contrast to wolves that are generally monogamous. Therefore, dogs have no pair bonding and the protection of a single mate, but rather have multiple mates in a year. The consequence is that wolves put a lot of energy into producing a few pups in contrast to dogs that maximize the production of pups. This higher pup production rate enables dogs to maintain or even increase their population with a lower pup survival rate than wolves, and allows dogs a greater capacity than wolves to grow their population after a population crash or when entering a new habitat. It is proposed that these differences are an alternative breeding strategy, one adapted to a life of scavenging instead of hunting.[47]

Parenting and early life Edit

All of the wild members of the genus Canis display complex coordinated parental behaviors. Wolf pups are cared for primarily by their mother for the first 3 months of their life when she remains in the den with them while they rely on her milk for sustenance and her presence for protection. The father brings her food. Once they leave the den and can chew, the parents and pups from previous years regurgitate food for them. Wolf pups become independent by 5 to 8 months, although they often stay with their parents for years. In contrast, dog pups are cared for by the mother and rely on her for milk and protection but she gets no help from the father nor other dogs. Once pups are weaned around 10 weeks they are independent and receive no further maternal care.[47]

Behavior problems Edit

There are many different types of behavioural issues that a dog can exhibit, including growling, snapping, barking, and invading a human's personal space. A survey of 203 dog owners in Melbourne, Australia, found that the main behaviour problems reported by owners were overexcitement (63%) and jumping up on people (56%).[48] Some problems are related to attachment while others are neurological, as seen below.

Separation anxiety Edit

When dogs are separated from humans, usually the owner, they often display behaviors which can be broken into the following four categories: exploratory behaviour, object play, destructive behaviour, and vocalization, and they are related to the canine's level of arousal.[49] These behaviours may manifest as destructiveness, fecal or urinary elimination, hypersalivation or vocalization among other things. Dogs from single-owner homes are approximately 2.5 times more likely to have separation anxiety compared to dogs from multiple-owner homes. Furthermore, sexually intact dogs are only one third as likely to have separation anxiety as neutered dogs. The sex of dogs and whether there is another pet in the home do not have an effect on separation anxiety.[50] It has been estimated that at least 14% of dogs examined at typical veterinary practices in the United States have shown signs of separation anxiety. Dogs that have been diagnosed with profound separation anxiety can be left alone for no more than minutes before they begin to panic and exhibit the behaviors associated with separation anxiety. Separation problems have been found to be linked to the dog's dependency on its owner, not because of disobedience.[49] In the absence of treatment, affected dogs are often relinquished to a humane society or shelter, abandoned, or euthanized.[51]

Resource guarding Edit

Resource guarding is exhibited by many canines, and is one of the most commonly reported behaviour issues to canine professionals.[52] It is seen when a dog uses specific behaviour patterns so that they can control access to an item, and the patterns are flexible when people are around.[53] If a canine places value on some resource (i.e. food, toys, etc.) they may attempt to guard it from other animals as well as people, which leads to behavioural problems if not treated. The guarding can show in many different ways from rapid ingestion of food to using the body to shield items. It manifests as aggressive behaviour including, but not limited to, growling, barking, or snapping. Some dogs will also resource guard their owners and can become aggressive if the behaviour is allowed to continue. Owners must learn to interpret their dog's body language in order to try to judge the dog's reaction, as visual signals are used (i.e. changes in body posture, facial expression, etc.) to communicate feeling and response.[52] These behaviours are commonly seen in shelter animals, most likely due to insecurities caused by a poor environment. Resource guarding is a concern since it can lead to aggression, but research has found that aggression over guarding can be contained by teaching the dog to drop the item they are guarding.[53]

Jealousy Edit

Canines are one of a number of non-human animals that can express jealousy towards other animals or animal-like objects.[54] This emotion may feed into other behavioural problems, manifest as attention-seeking behaviour, withdrawing from social activity, or aggression towards their owner or another animal or person.

Noise anxiety Edit

Canines often fear, and exhibit stress responses to, loud noises. Noise-related anxieties in dogs may be triggered by fireworks, thunderstorms, gunshots, and even loud or sharp bird noises. Associated stimuli may also come to trigger the symptoms of the phobia or anxiety, such as a change in barometric pressure being associated with a thunderstorm, thus causing an anticipatory anxiety.

Tail chasing Edit

Tail chasing can be classified as a stereotypy. It falls under obsessive compulsive disorder, which is a neuropsychiatric disorder that can present in dogs as canine compulsive disorder.[55] In one clinical study on this potential behavioral problem, 18 tail-chasing terriers were given clomipramine orally at a dosage of 1 to 2 mg/kg (0.5 to 0.9 mg/lb) of body weight, every 12 hours. Three of the dogs required treatment at a slightly higher dosage range to control tail chasing, however, after 1 to 12 weeks of treatment, 9 of 12 dogs were reported to have a 75% or greater reduction in tail chasing.[56] Personality can also play a factor in tail chasing. Dogs who chase their tails have been found to be more shy than those who do not, and some dogs also show a lower level of response during tail chasing bouts.[55]

Behavior compared to other canids Edit

Comparisons made within the wolf-like canids allow the identification of those behaviors that may have been inherited from common ancestry and those that may have been the result of domestication or other relatively recent environmental changes.[40] Studies of free-ranging African Basenjis and New Guinea Singing Dogs indicate that their behavioral and ecological traits were the result of environmental selection pressures or selective breeding choices and not the result of artificial selection imposed by humans.[57]

Early aggression Edit

Dog pups show unrestrained fighting with their siblings from 2 weeks of age, with injury avoided only due to their undeveloped jaw muscles. This fighting gives way to play-chasing with the development of running skills at 4–5 weeks. Wolf pups possess more-developed jaw muscles from 2 weeks of age, when they first show signs of play-fighting with their siblings. Serious fighting occurs during 4–6 weeks of age.[58] Compared to wolf and dog pups, golden jackal pups develop aggression at the age of 4–6 weeks when play-fighting frequently escalates into uninhibited biting intended to harm. This aggression ceases by 10–12 weeks when a hierarchy has formed.[59]

Tameness Edit

Unlike other domestic species which were primarily selected for production-related traits, dogs were initially selected for their behaviors.[60][61] In 2016, a study found that there were only 11 fixed genes that showed variation between wolves and dogs. These gene variations were unlikely to have been the result of natural evolution, and indicate selection on both morphology and behavior during dog domestication. These genes have been shown to affect the catecholamine synthesis pathway, with the majority of the genes affecting the fight-or-flight response[61][62] (i.e. selection for tameness), and emotional processing.[61] Dogs generally show reduced fear and aggression compared to wolves.[61][63] Some of these genes have been associated with aggression in some dog breeds, indicating their importance in both the initial domestication and then later in breed formation.[61]

Social structure Edit

Among canids, packs are the social units that hunt, rear young and protect a communal territory as a stable group and their members are usually related.[64] Members of the feral dog group are usually not related. Feral dog groups are composed of a stable 2–6 members compared to the 2–15 member wolf pack whose size fluctuates with the availability of prey and reaches a maximum in winter time. The feral dog group consists of monogamous breeding pairs compared to the one breeding pair of the wolf pack. Agonistic behavior does not extend to the individual level and does not support a higher social structure compared to the ritualized agonistic behavior of the wolf pack that upholds its social structure. Feral pups have a very high mortality rate that adds little to the group size, with studies showing that adults are usually killed through accidents with humans, therefore other dogs need to be co-opted from villages to maintain stable group size.[40]

Socialization Edit

The critical period for socialization begins with walking and exploring the environment. Dog and wolf pups both develop the ability to see, hear and smell at 4 weeks of age. Dogs begin to explore the world around them at 4 weeks of age with these senses available to them, while wolves begin to explore at 2 weeks of age when they have the sense of smell but are functionally blind and deaf. The consequences of this is that more things are novel and frightening to wolf pups. The critical period for socialization closes with the avoidance of novelty, when the animal runs away from – rather than approaching and exploring – novel objects. For dogs this develops between 4 and 8 weeks of age. Wolves reach the end of the critical period after 6 weeks, after which it is not possible to socialize a wolf.[47]

Dog puppies require as little as 90 minutes of contact with humans during their critical period of socialization to form a social attachment. This will not create a highly social pet but a dog that will solicit human attention.[65] Wolves require 24 hours contact a day starting before 3 weeks of age. To create a socialized wolf the pups are removed from the den at 10 days of age, kept in constant human contact until they are 4 weeks old when they begin to bite their sleeping human companions, then spend only their waking hours in the presence of humans. This socialization process continues until age 4 months, when the pups can join other captive wolves but will require daily human contact to remain socialized. Despite this intensive socialization process, a well-socialized wolf will behave differently to a well-socialized dog and will display species-typical hunting and reproductive behaviors, only closer to humans than a wild wolf. These wolves do not generalize their socialization to all humans in the same manner as a socialized dog and they remain more fearful of novelty compared to socialized dogs.[66]

In 1982, a study to observe the differences between dogs and wolves raised in similar conditions took place. The dog puppies preferred larger amounts of sleep at the beginning of their lives, while the wolf puppies were much more active. The dog puppies also preferred the company of humans, rather than their canine foster mother, though the wolf puppies were the exact opposite, spending more time with their foster mother. The dogs also showed a greater interest in the food given to them and paid little attention to their surroundings, while the wolf puppies found their surroundings to be much more intriguing than their food or food bowl. The wolf puppies were observed taking part in antagonistic play at a younger age, while the dog puppies did not display dominant/submissive roles until they were much older. The wolf puppies were rarely seen as being aggressive to each other or towards the other canines. On the other hand, the dog puppies were much more aggressive to each other and other canines, often seen full-on attacking their foster mother or one another.[67]

A 2005 study comparing dog and wolf pups concluded that extensively socialised dogs as well as unsocialised dog pups showed greater attachment to a human owner than wolf pups did, even if the wolf was socialised. The study concluded that dogs may have evolved a capacity for attachment to humans functionally analogous to that human infants display.[68]

Cognition Edit

Despite claims that dogs show more human-like social cognition than wolves,[69][70][71] several recent studies have demonstrated that if wolves are properly socialized to humans and have the opportunity to interact with humans regularly, then they too can succeed on some human-guided cognitive tasks,[72][73][74][75][76] in some cases out-performing dogs at an individual level.[77] Similar to dogs, wolves can also follow more complex point types made with body parts other than the human arm and hand (e.g. elbow, knee, foot).[76] Both dogs and wolves have the cognitive capacity for prosocial behavior toward humans; however it is not guaranteed. For canids to perform well on traditional human-guided tasks (e.g. following the human point) both relevant lifetime experiences with humans – including socialization to humans during the critical period for social development – and opportunities to associate human body parts with certain outcomes (such as food being provided by human hands, a human throwing or kicking a ball, etc.) are required.[78]

After undergoing training to solve a simple manipulation task, dogs that are faced with an insoluble version of the same problem look at the human, while socialized wolves do not.[71]

Reproduction Edit

Dogs reach sexual maturity and can reproduce during their first year in contrast to a wolf at two years. The female dog can bear another litter within 8 months of the last one. The canid genus is influenced by the photoperiod and generally reproduces in the springtime.[40] Domestic dogs are not reliant on seasonality for reproduction in contrast to the wolf, coyote, Australian dingo and African basenji that may have only one, seasonal, estrus each year.[46] Feral dogs are influenced by the photoperiod with around half of the breeding females mating in the springtime, which is thought to indicate an ancestral reproductive trait not overcome by domestication,[40] as can be inferred from wolves[79] and Cape hunting dogs.[80]

Domestic dogs are polygamous in contrast to wolves that are generally monogamous. Therefore, domestic dogs have no pair bonding and the protection of a single mate, but rather have multiple mates in a year. There is no paternal care in dogs as opposed to wolves where all pack members assist the mother with the pups. The consequence is that wolves put a lot of energy into producing a few pups in contrast to dogs that maximize the production of pups. This higher pup production rate enables dogs to maintain or even increase their population with a lower pup survival rate than wolves, and allows dogs a greater capacity than wolves to grow their population after a population crash or when entering a new habitat. It is proposed that these differences are an alternative breeding strategy adapted to a life of scavenging instead of hunting.[47] In contrast to domestic dogs, feral dogs are monogamous. Domestic dogs tend to have a litter size of 10, wolves 3, and feral dogs 5–8. Feral pups have a very high mortality rate with only 5% surviving at the age of one year, and sometimes the pups are left unattended making them vulnerable to predators.[40] Domestic dogs stand alone among all canids for a total lack of paternal care.[81]

Dogs differ from wolves and most other large canid species as they generally do not regurgitate food for their young, nor the young of other dogs in the same territory.[82] However, this difference was not observed in all domestic dogs. Regurgitating of food by the females for the young, as well as care for the young by the males, has been observed in domestic dogs, dingos and in feral or semi-feral dogs. In one study of a group of free-ranging dogs, for the first 2 weeks immediately after parturition the lactating females were observed to be more aggressive to protect the pups. The male parents were in contact with the litters as ‘guard’ dogs for the first 6–8 weeks of the litters’ life. In absence of the mothers, they were observed to prevent the approach of strangers by vocalizations or even by physical attacks. Moreover, one male fed the litter by regurgitation showing the existence of paternal care in some free-roaming dogs.[83]

Space Edit

Space used by feral dogs is not dissimilar from most other canids in that they use defined traditional areas (home ranges) that tend to be defended against intruders, and have core areas where most of their activities are undertaken. Urban domestic dogs have a home range of 2-61 hectares, in contrast to a feral dog's home range of 58 square kilometers. Wolf home ranges vary from 78 square kilometers where prey is deer, to 2.5 square kilometers at higher latitudes where prey is moose and caribou. Wolves will defend their territory based on prey abundance and pack density, but feral dogs will defend their home ranges all year. Where wolf ranges and feral dog ranges overlap, the feral dogs will site their core areas closer to human settlement.[40]

Predation and scavenging Edit

Despite claims in the popular press, studies could not find evidence of a single predation on cattle by feral dogs.[40][84][85] However, domestic dogs were responsible for the death of 3 calves over one 5-year study.[85] Other studies in Europe and North America indicate moderate limited success in the consumption of wild boar, deer and other ungulates, however it could not be determined if this was predation or scavenging on carcasses. A new study has shown though that these were most likely due to predation. [86]

Feral dogs, like their ancestors do participate in pup rearing. Several studies show that feral dogs are not primarily scavengers, despite claims in the popular press. Studies in the modern era show that their diet is very opportunistic, ranging from garbage, carrion to live prey. The primary feature that distinguishes feral from domestic dogs is the degree of reliance or dependence on humans, and in some respect, their behavior toward people. Feral dogs survive and reproduce independently of human intervention or assistance. While it is true that some feral dogs use human garbage for food, others acquire their primary subsistence by hunting and scavenging like other wild canids. [87] Dogs may resort to hunting more than garbage consuming when their garbage food source is scarce. Even well-fed domestic dogs are prone to scavenge; gastro-intestinal veterinary visits increase during warmer weather as dogs are prone to eat decaying material.[88] Some dogs consume feces, which may contain nutrition.[89][90] On occasion well-fed dogs have been known to scavenge their owners' corpses.[91]

Dogs in human society Edit

Studies using an operant framework have indicated that humans can influence the behavior of dogs through food, petting and voice. Food and 20–30 seconds of petting maintained operant responding in dogs.[92] Some dogs will show a preference for petting once food is readily available, and dogs will remain in proximity to a person providing petting and show no satiation to that stimulus.[93] Petting alone was sufficient to maintain the operant response of military dogs to voice commands, and responses to basic obedience commands in all dogs increased when only vocal praise was provided for correct responses.[94]

A study using dogs that were trained to remain motionless while unsedated and unrestrained in an MRI scanner exhibited caudate activation to a hand signal associated with reward.[2] Further work found that the magnitude of the canine caudate response is similar to that of humans, while the between-subject variability in dogs may be less than humans.[95] In a further study, 5 scents were presented (self, familiar human, strange human, familiar dog, strange dog). While the olfactory bulb/peduncle was activated to a similar degree by all the scents, the caudate was activated maximally to the familiar human. Importantly, the scent of the familiar human was not the handler, meaning that the caudate response differentiated the scent in the absence of the person being present. The caudate activation suggested that not only did the dogs discriminate that scent from the others, they had a positive association with it. Although these signals came from two different people, the humans lived in the same household as the dog and therefore represented the dog's primary social circle. And while dogs should be highly tuned to the smell of items that are not comparable, it seems that the “reward response” is reserved for their humans.[96]

Research has shown that there are individual differences in the interactions between dogs and their human that have significant effects on dog behavior. In 1997, a study showed that the type of relationship between dog and master, characterized as either companionship or working relationship, significantly affected the dog's performance on a cognitive problem-solving task. They speculate that companion dogs have a more dependent relationship with their owners, and look to them to solve problems. In contrast, working dogs are more independent.[97]

Dogs in the family Edit

In 2013, a study produced the first evidence under controlled experimental observation for a correlation between the owner's personality and their dog's behaviour.[98]

Dogs at work Edit

Service dogs are those that are trained to help people with disabilities such as blindness, epilepsy, diabetes and autism. Detection dogs are trained to using their sense of smell to detect substances such as explosives, illegal drugs, wildlife scat, or blood. In science, dogs have helped humans understand about the conditioned reflex. Attack dogs, dogs that have been trained to attack on command, are employed in security, police, and military roles. Service dog programs have been established to help individuals suffering from Post Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) and have shown to have positive results.[99]

Attacks Edit

 
A dog's teeth can inflict serious injuries

The human-dog relationship is based on unconditional trust; however, if this trust is lost it will be difficult to reinstate.[citation needed]

In the UK between 2005 and 2013, there were 17 fatal dog attacks. In 2007–08, there were 4,611 hospital admissions due to dog attacks, which increased to 5,221 in 2008–09. It was estimated in 2013 that more than 200,000 people a year are bitten by dogs in England, with the annual cost to the National Health Service of treating injuries about £3 million.[100] A report published in 2014 stated there were 6,743 hospital admissions specifically caused by dog bites, a 5.8% increase from the 6,372 admissions in the previous 12 months.[101][needs update]

In the US between 1979 and 1996, there were more than 300 human dog bite-related fatalities.[102] In the US in 2013, there were 31 dog-bite related deaths. Each year, more than 4.5 million people in the US are bitten by dogs and almost 1 in 5 require medical attention. A dog's thick fur protects it from the bite of another dog, but humans are furless and are not so protected.[103]

Attack training is condemned by some as promoting ferocity in dogs; a 1975 American study showed that 10% of dogs that have bitten a person received attack dog training at some point.[104]

See also Edit

References Edit

  1. ^ Levitis, Daniel; William Z. Lidicker Jr, Glenn Freund; Freund, Glenn (June 2009). "Behavioural biologists do not agree on what constitutes behaviour" (PDF). Animal Behaviour. 78 (1): 103–10. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.03.018. PMC 2760923. PMID 20160973.
  2. ^ a b Berns, G. S.; Brooks, A. M.; Spivak, M. (2012). Neuhauss, Stephan C. F (ed.). "Functional MRI in Awake Unrestrained Dogs". PLOS ONE. 7 (5): e38027. Bibcode:2012PLoSO...738027B. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0038027. PMC 3350478. PMID 22606363.
  3. ^ Skoglund, P.; Ersmark, E.; Palkopoulou, E.; Dalén, L. (2015). "Ancient Wolf Genome Reveals an Early Divergence of Domestic Dog Ancestors and Admixture into High-Latitude Breeds". Current Biology. 25 (11): 1515–9. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2015.04.019. PMID 26004765.
  4. ^ "The New Science Of Understanding Dog Behavior". NPR.org. Retrieved 2022-05-28.
  5. ^ Maginnity, M.E. & Grace, R.C. (2014). "Visual perspective taking by dogs (Canis familiaris) in a Guesser–Knower task: evidence for a canine theory of mind?". Animal Cognition. 17 (6): 1375–1392. doi:10.1007/s10071-014-0773-9. PMID 24950722. S2CID 14833483.
  6. ^ Kaminski, J.; Bräuer, J.; Call, J. & Tomasello, M. (2009). "Domestic dogs are sensitive to a human's perspective" (PDF). Behaviour. 146 (7): 979–998. doi:10.1163/156853908X395530.
  7. ^ a b c d e Coren, Stanley "How To Speak Dog: Mastering the Art of Dog-Human Communication" 2000 Simon & Schuster, New York.
  8. ^ Huber, Ludwig (October 2016). "How Dogs Perceive and Understand Us". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 25 (5): 339–344. doi:10.1177/0963721416656329. ISSN 0963-7214. S2CID 151488382.
  9. ^ McGreevy, Paul D.; Georgevsky, Dana; Carrasco, Johanna; Valenzuela, Michael; Duffy, Deborah L.; Serpell, James A. (2013). "Dog Behavior Co-Varies with Height, Bodyweight and Skull Shape". PLOS ONE. 8 (12): e80529. Bibcode:2013PLoSO...880529M. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0080529. PMC 3864788. PMID 24358107.
  10. ^ Stone, Holly R.; McGreevy, Paul D.; Starling, Melissa J.; Forkman, Bjorn (2016). "Associations between Domestic-Dog Morphology and Behaviour Scores in the Dog Mentality Assessment". PLOS ONE. 11 (2): e0149403. Bibcode:2016PLoSO..1149403S. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0149403. PMC 4771026. PMID 26919495.
  11. ^ Kujala, Miiamaaria (January 23, 2017). "Human Empathy, Personality and Experience Affect the Emotion Ratings of Dog and Human Facial Expressions". PLOS ONE. 12 (1): e0170730. Bibcode:2017PLoSO..1270730K. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0170730. PMC 5257001. PMID 28114335. S2CID 1215168.
  12. ^ Horowitz, A. (2009). "Attention to attention in domestic dog Canis familiaris dyadic play". Animal Cognition. 12 (1): 107–118. doi:10.1007/s10071-008-0175-y. PMID 18679727. S2CID 207050813.
  13. ^ Ward, C., Bauer, E.B. and Smuts, B.B. (2008). "Partner preferences and asymmetries in social play among domestic dog, Canis lupus familiaris, littermates" (PDF). Animal Behaviour. 76 (4): 1187–1199. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.06.004. S2CID 1295114.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  14. ^ Palagi, Elisabetta; Nicotra, Velia; Cordoni, Giada (2015). "Rapid mimicry and emotional contagion in domestic dogs". Royal Society Open Science. 2 (12): 150505. Bibcode:2015RSOS....250505P. doi:10.1098/rsos.150505. PMC 4807458. PMID 27019737.
  15. ^ Rooney, N.J., Bradshaw, J.W.S. and Robinson, I.H. (2000). "A comparison of dog–dog and dog-human play behaviour". Applied Animal Behaviour Science. 66 (3): 235–248. doi:10.1016/S0168-1591(99)00078-7.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  16. ^ Rooney, N.J.; Bradshaw, Jv.W.S. (2003). "Links between play and dominance and attachment dimensions of dog-human relationships". Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science. 6 (2): 67–94. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.495.1702. doi:10.1207/S15327604JAWS0602_01. PMID 12909524. S2CID 2254971.
  17. ^ Horváth, Z.; Dóka, A.; Miklósi A. (2008). "Affiliative and disciplinary behavior of human handlers during play with their dog affects cortisol concentrations in opposite directions". Hormones and Behavior. 54 (1): 107–114. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2008.02.002. PMID 18353328. S2CID 16805722.
  18. ^ Custance, Deborah; Mayer, Jennifer (2012). "Empathic-like responding by domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) to distress in humans: an exploratory study". Animal Cognition. 15 (5): 851–859. doi:10.1007/s10071-012-0510-1. PMID 22644113. S2CID 15153091.
  19. ^ "Behaviour problems linked to pessimistic dogs". Sydney Morning Herald. October 12, 2010. Retrieved October 21, 2010.
  20. ^ McCrae, R. R.; Costa, P.T; Ostendorf, F.; Angleitner, A.; Hřebíčková, M.; Avia, M. D.; Saunders, P.R. (2000). "Nature over nurture: temperament, personality, and life span development". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 78 (1): 173–86. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.78.1.173. PMID 10653513.
  21. ^ a b c Jones, A. C.; Gosling, S. D. (2005). "Temperament and personality in dogs (Canis familiaris): a review and evaluation of past research". Applied Animal Behaviour Science. 95 (1): 1–53. doi:10.1016/j.applanim.2005.04.008.
  22. ^ a b c Gartner, M. C. (2015). "Pet personality: A review". Personality and Individual Differences. 75: 102–113. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2014.10.042.
  23. ^ Fratkin, J. L.; Sinn, D. L.; Patal, E. A.; Gosling, S. D. (2013). "Personality consistency in dogs: a meta-analysis". PLOS ONE. 8 (1): e54907. Bibcode:2013PLoSO...854907F. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0054907. PMC 3553070. PMID 23372787.
  24. ^ Vas, J.; Müller, C.; Győri, B.; Miklósi, Á. (2008). "Consistency of dogs' reactions to threatening cues of an unfamiliar person". Applied Animal Behaviour Science. 112 (3): 331–344. doi:10.1016/j.applanim.2007.09.002.
  25. ^ Meegahapola, L.; Constantinides, M.; Radivojevic, Z.; Li, H.; Quercia, D.; Eggleston, M. S. (2023). "Quantified Canine: Inferring Dog Personality from Wearables". Proceedings of the 2023 CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems. pp. 1–19. arXiv:2301.06964. doi:10.1145/3544548.3581088. ISBN 9781450394215. S2CID 255942162.
  26. ^ Hu, Y.; Serpell, J. A. (2003). "Development and validation of a questionnaire for measuring behavior and temperament traits in pet dogs". Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association. 223 (9): 1293–1300. doi:10.2460/javma.2003.223.1293. PMID 14621216. S2CID 9771544.
  27. ^ De Meester, R. H.; De Bacquer, D.; Peremans, K.; Vermeire, S.; Planta, D. J.; Coopman, F.; Audenaert, K. (2008). "A preliminary study on the use of the Socially Acceptable Behavior test as a test for shyness/confidence in the temperament of dogs". Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research. 3 (4): 161–170. doi:10.1016/j.jveb.2007.10.005.
  28. ^ Barnard, S.; Siracusa, C.; Reisner, I.; Valsecchi, P.; Serpell, J. A. (2012). "Validity of model devices used to assess canine temperament in behavioral tests". Applied Animal Behaviour Science. 138 (1): 79–87. doi:10.1016/j.applanim.2012.02.017.
  29. ^ Svartberga, K.; Forkman, B. (2002). "Personality traits in the domestic dog (Canis familiaris)" (PDF). Applied Animal Behaviour Science. 79 (2): 133–155. doi:10.1016/S0168-1591(02)00121-1.
  30. ^ Svartberg, K; Tapper, I; Temrin, H; Radesater, T; Thorman, S (February 2005). "Consistency of personality traits in dogs". Animal Behaviour. 69 (2): 283–291. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.04.011. S2CID 53154729.
  31. ^ David Grimm (28 Apr 2022). "Your dog's breed doesn't determine its personality, study suggests". American Association for the Advancement of Science.
  32. ^ Duffy, D. L.; Serpell, J. A. (2008). "Behavioral assessment of guide and service dogs". Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research. 3 (4): 186–188. doi:10.1016/j.jveb.2007.12.010.
  33. ^ Bollen, K. S.; Horowitz, J. (2008). "Behavioral evaluation and demographic information in the assessment of aggressiveness in shelter dogs". Applied Animal Behaviour Science. 112 (1): 120–135. doi:10.1016/j.applanim.2007.07.007.
  34. ^ Riemer, S.; Müller, C.; Virányi, Z.; Huber, L.; Range, F. (2013). "Choice of conflict resolution strategy is linked to sociability in dog puppies". Applied Animal Behaviour Science. 149 (1): 36–44. doi:10.1016/j.applanim.2013.09.006. PMC 4044588. PMID 24910487.
  35. ^ a b Drews, Carlos (1993). "The Concept and Definition of Dominance in Animal Behaviour". Behaviour. 125 (3): 283–313. doi:10.1163/156853993X00290.
  36. ^ a b c Bradshaw, John W.S.; Blackwell, Emily J.; Casey, Rachel A. (2009). "Dominance in domestic dogs—useful construct or bad habit?" (PDF). Journal of Veterinary Behavior. 4 (3): 135–144. doi:10.1016/j.jveb.2008.08.004.
  37. ^ Pongrácz, P.; et al. (2008). "How does dominance rank status affect individual and social learning performance in the dog (Canis familiaris)?". Animal Cognition. 11 (1): 75–82. doi:10.1007/s10071-007-0090-7. ISSN 1435-9456. PMID 17492317. S2CID 8930934.
  38. ^ Bradshaw, John W.S.; Lea, Amanda M. (1992). "Dyadic Interactions Between Domestic Dogs". Anthrozoös. 5 (4): 245–253. doi:10.2752/089279392787011287.
  39. ^ L. David Mech; Luigi Boitani (1 October 2010). Wolves: Behavior, Ecology, and Conservation. University of Chicago Press. pp. 84–. ISBN 978-0-226-51698-1.
  40. ^ a b c d e f g h i j Boitani, L.; Ciucci, P. (1995). "Comparative social ecology of feral dogs and wolves" (PDF). Ethology Ecology & Evolution. 7 (1): 49–72. doi:10.1080/08927014.1995.9522969.
  41. ^ Coppinger, Ray (2001). Dogs: a Startling New Understanding of Canine Origin, Behavior and Evolution. New York: Scribner. ISBN 978-0-684-85530-1.
  42. ^ Kaulfuß, P.; Mills, D.S. (2008). "Neophilia in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) and its implication for studies of dog cognition". Animal Cognition. 11 (3): 553–556. doi:10.1007/s10071-007-0128-x. PMID 18183436. S2CID 31406623.
  43. ^ "40 Winks?" Jennifer S. Holland, National Geographic Vol. 220, No. 1. July 2011.
  44. ^ Bonnie V. G. Beaver (2009). Canine Behavior: Insights and Answers. Elsevier Health Sciences. ISBN 978-1-4160-5419-1.
  45. ^ Edward C. Feldman; Richard William Nelson (2004). Canine and Feline Endocrinology and Reproduction. Elsevier Health Sciences. ISBN 978-0-7216-9315-6.
  46. ^ a b c d . Michelson Prize and Grants. Archived from the original on August 16, 2015. Retrieved April 28, 2015.
  47. ^ a b c d Lord, K. (2013). "A Comparison of the Sensory Development of Wolves (Canis lupus lupus) and Dogs (Canis lupus familiaris)". Ethology. 119 (2): 110–120. doi:10.1111/eth.12044.
  48. ^ Kobelt, A.J., Hemsworth, P.H., Barnett, J.L. and Coleman, G.J. (2003). "A survey of dog ownership in suburban Australia—conditions and behaviour problems". Applied Animal Behaviour Science. 82 (2): 137–148. doi:10.1016/S0168-1591(03)00062-5.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  49. ^ a b Lund, Jørgen Damkjer; Jørgensen, Mads Chr (1999). "Behaviour patterns and time course of activity in dogs with separation problems". Applied Animal Behaviour Science. 63 (3): 219–236. doi:10.1016/S0168-1591(99)00011-8.
  50. ^ Flannigan, G.; Dodman, N.H.A (2001). "Risk factors and behaviors associated with separation anxiety in dogs". Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association. 219 (4): 460–466. doi:10.2460/javma.2001.219.460. PMID 11518171.
  51. ^ Overall, Dunham, Frank, Karen L., Arthur E, Diane (2001). "Frequency of nonspecific clinical signs in dogs with separation anxiety, thunderstorm phobia, and noise phobia, alone or in combination" (PDF). JAVMA. 219 (4): 467–473. doi:10.2460/javma.2001.219.467. PMID 11518172 – via AVMA.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  52. ^ a b Jacobs, Jacquelyn A.; Pearl, David L.; Coe, Jason B.; Widowski, Tina M.; Niel, Lee (2017). "Ability of owners to identify resource guarding behaviour in the domestic dog". Applied Animal Behaviour Science. 188: 77–83. doi:10.1016/j.applanim.2016.12.012.
  53. ^ a b Jacobs, Jacquelyn A.; Coe, Jason B.; Pearl, David L.; Widowski, Tina M.; Niel, Lee (2017). "Factors associated with canine resource guarding behaviour in the presence of people: A cross-sectional survey of dog owners". Preventive Veterinary Medicine. 161: 143–153. doi:10.1016/j.prevetmed.2017.02.005. PMID 28268035. S2CID 207419404.
  54. ^ Harris, Christine R.; Prouvost, Caroline (2014-07-23). "Jealousy in Dogs". PLOS ONE. 9 (7): e94597. Bibcode:2014PLoSO...994597H. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0094597. ISSN 1932-6203. PMC 4108309. PMID 25054800.
  55. ^ a b Tiira, Katriina; Hakosalo, Osmo; Kareinen, Lauri; Thomas, Anne; Hielm-Bjorkman, Anna; Escriou, Catherine; Arnold, Paul; Lohi, Hannes (2012). "Environmental effects on compulsive tail chasing in dogs". PLOS ONE. 7 (7): e41684. Bibcode:2012PLoSO...741684T. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0041684. PMC 3406045. PMID 22844513.
  56. ^ Moon-Fanelli, A.A.; Dodman, N.H. (1998). "Description and development of compulsive tail chasing in terriers and response to clomipramine treatment". Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association. 212 (8): 1252–1257. PMID 9569164.
  57. ^ Brisbin, L; Risch, TS (1997). "Primitive dogs, their ecology and behavior: Unique opportunities to study the early development of the human-canine bond". Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association. 210 (8): 1122–1126. PMID 9108912.
  58. ^ Frank, Harry; Frank, Martha Gialdini (1982). "On the effects of domestication on canine social development and behavior". Applied Animal Ethology. 8 (6): 507. doi:10.1016/0304-3762(82)90215-2. hdl:2027.42/23918.
  59. ^ Feddersen-Petersen, D. (1991). "The ontogeny of social play and agonistic behaviour in selected canid species" (PDF). Bonn. Zool. Beitr. 42: 97–114.
  60. ^ Serpell J, Duffy D. Dog Breeds and Their Behavior. In: Domestic Dog Cognition and Behavior. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer; 2014
  61. ^ a b c d e Cagan, Alex; Blass, Torsten (2016). "Identification of genomic variants putatively targeted by selection during dog domestication". BMC Evolutionary Biology. 16: 10. doi:10.1186/s12862-015-0579-7. PMC 4710014. PMID 26754411.
  62. ^ Almada RC, Coimbra NC. Recruitment of striatonigral disinhibitory and nigrotectal inhibitory GABAergic pathways during the organization of defensive behavior by mice in a dangerous environment with the venomous snake Bothrops alternatus [ Reptilia , Viperidae ] Synapse 2015:n/a–n/a
  63. ^ Coppinger R, Schneider R: Evolution of working dogs. The domestic dog: Its evolution, behaviour and interactions with people. Cambridge: Cambridge University press, 1995.
  64. ^ Mech, L. D. 1970. The Wolf: The Ecology and Behavior of an Endangered Species. Natural History Press (Doubleday Publishing Co., N.Y.) 389 pp. (Reprinted in paperback by University of Minnesota Press, May 1981)
  65. ^ Freedman, Daniel G.; King, John A.; Elliot, Orville (March 1961). "Critical Period in the Social Development of Dogs". Science. 133 (3457): 1016–1017. Bibcode:1961Sci...133.1016F. doi:10.1126/science.133.3457.1016. PMID 13701603. S2CID 278125.
  66. ^ Klinghammer, E. & Goodmann, P. A. (1987). "Socialization and management of wolves in captivity". In H. Frank (ed.). Man and wolf: Advances, issues, and problems in captive wolf research. Dordrecht: W. Junk.
  67. ^ Frank H; Frank MG (1982). "On the effects of domestication on canine social development and behavior" (PDF). Applied Animal Ethology. 8 (6): 507–525. doi:10.1016/0304-3762(82)90215-2. hdl:2027.42/23918.
  68. ^ Topál, József, Márta Gácsi, Ádám Miklósi, Zsófia Virányi, Enikő Kubinyi, and Vilmos Csányi. "Attachment to humans: a comparative study on hand-reared wolves and differently socialized dog puppies." Animal behaviour 70, no. 6 (2005): 1367-1375.
  69. ^ Hare, B. (2002). "The Domestication of Social Cognition in Dogs" (PDF). Science. 298 (5598): 1634–6. Bibcode:2002Sci...298.1634H. doi:10.1126/science.1072702. PMID 12446914. S2CID 13369396.
  70. ^ Hare, Brian; Tomasello, Michael (September 2005). "Human-like social skills in dogs?". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 9 (9): 439–444. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.07.003. PMID 16061417. S2CID 9311402.
  71. ^ a b Miklósi, Adam; et al. (April 29, 2003). "A simple reason for a big difference: wolves do not look back at humans, but dogs do". Current Biology. 13 (9): 763–766. doi:10.1016/S0960-9822(03)00263-X. PMID 12725735. S2CID 10200094.
  72. ^ Gácsi, M; et al. (2009). "Explaining dog wolf differences in utilizing human pointing gestures: Selection for synergistic shifts in the development of some social skills". PLoS ONE. 4 (8): e6584. Bibcode:2009PLoSO...4.6584G. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0006584. PMC 2719091. PMID 19714197.
  73. ^ Range, F.; Virányi, Z. (2011). "Development of gaze following abilities in wolves (Canis lupus)". PLOS ONE. 6 (2): e16888. Bibcode:2011PLoSO...616888R. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0016888. PMC 3044139. PMID 21373192.
  74. ^ Udell, M. A. R, Dorey, N. R., & Wynne, C. D. L. (December 2008). "Wolves outperform dogs in following human social cues". Animal Behaviour. 76 (6): 1767–1773. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.07.028. S2CID 11226439.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: uses authors parameter (link)
  75. ^ Udell, M. A. R., Dorey, N. R., & Wynne, C. D. L. (2011). "Can your dog read your mind? Understanding the causes of canine perspective taking". Learning & Behavior. 39 (4): 289–302. doi:10.3758/s13420-011-0034-6. PMID 21643852.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: uses authors parameter (link)
  76. ^ a b Udell, M. A. R, Spencer, J. M., Dorey, N. R., & Wynne, C. D. L. (2012). "Human-Socialized Wolves Follow Diverse Human Gestures... And They May Not Be Alone". International Journal of Comparative Psychology. 25 (2): 97–117. doi:10.46867/IJCP.2012.25.02.04.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: uses authors parameter (link)
  77. ^ Udell, M. A.; Giglio, R. F.; Wynne, C. D. (2008). "Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) use human gestures but not nonhuman tokens to find hidden food". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 122 (1): 84–93. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.122.1.84. PMID 18298285.
  78. ^ Udell, M.A.R. (2014). "10. A Dog's-Eye View of Canine Cognition". In A. Horowitz (ed.). Domestic Dog Cognition and Behavior. Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg. pp. 221–240. doi:10.1007/978-3-642-53994-7_10. ISBN 978-3-642-53993-0.
  79. ^ Seal, U. S.; Mech, L. D. (July 1983). "Blood Indicators of Seasonal Metabolic Patterns in Captive Adult Gray Wolves". The Journal of Wildlife Management. 47 (3): 704–715. doi:10.2307/3808606. JSTOR 3808606.
  80. ^ Cunningham, D. J. (January 1906). "Cape Hunting Dogs (Lycaon pictus) in the Gardens of the Royal Zoological Society of Ireland". Proceedings of the Royal Society of Edinburgh. 25 (2): 843–848. doi:10.1017/S0370164600016667.
  81. ^ Kleiman, Devra G.; Malcom, James R. (1981). "The Evolution of Male Parental Investment in Mammals". In Gubernick, David J.; Klopfer, Peter H. (eds.). Parental Care in Mammals. Plenum Press. pp. 347–387. doi:10.1007/978-1-4613-3150-6_9. ISBN 9780306405334.
  82. ^ Coppinger, Ray (2001). Dogs: a Startling New Understanding of Canine Origin, Behavior and Evolution. New York: Scribner. ISBN 978-0-684-85530-1.
  83. ^ Pal, S. K. (2005). "Parental care in free-ranging dogs, Canis familiaris". Applied Animal Behaviour Science. 90: 31–47. doi:10.1016/j.applanim.2004.08.002.
  84. ^ Scott, M. Douglas; Causey, Keith (July 1973). "Ecology of Feral Dogs in Alabama". The Journal of Wildlife Management. 37 (3): 253–265. doi:10.2307/3800116. JSTOR 3800116.
  85. ^ a b Nesbitt, William H. (2009) [1975]. "Ecology of a Feral Dog Pack on a Wildlife Refuge". In Fox, Michael W. (ed.). Wild Canids: Their Systematics, Behavioral Ecology & Evolution (reprinted ed.). Dogwise Publishing. p. 391. ISBN 978-1-929242-64-1.
  86. ^ Duarte, Jesús (1 July 2016). "Depredatory impact of free-roaming domestic dogs on Mediterranean deer in southern Spain: implications for human-wolf conflict". Folia Zoologica. 65 (2): 135–141. doi:10.25225/fozo.v65.i2.a8.2016. S2CID 90407348 – via BioOne.
  87. ^ Green, Jeffrey .S. (September 1994). "The Handbook: Prevention and Control of Wildlife Damage". {{cite journal}}: Cite journal requires |journal= (help)
  88. ^ Wedderburn, Pete (19 September 2017). "Why do dogs get upset stomachs – common causes and treatments". The Telegraph. Retrieved 28 December 2017.
  89. ^ "Why Your Dog Eats Poop, and Other Odd Pet Behavior Explained". 4 November 2017. Retrieved 28 December 2017.
  90. ^ "Everyone Poops. Some Animals Eat It. Why?". Smithsonian. Retrieved 28 December 2017.
  91. ^ "Would Your Dog Eat You if You Died? Get the Facts". National Geographic. 23 June 2017. Retrieved 28 December 2017.
  92. ^ Fonberg, E.; Kostarczyk, E.; Prechtl, J. (1981). "Training of Instrumental Responses in Dogs Socially Reinforced by Humans". The Pavlovian Journal of Biological Science. 16 (4): 183–193. doi:10.1007/BF03003358. PMID 7329743. S2CID 28014282.
  93. ^ Feuerbacher, E. N.; Wynne, C. D. L. (2015). "Shut up and pet me! Domestic dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) prefer petting to vocal praise in concurrent and single-alternative choice procedures". Behavioural Processes. 110: 47–59. doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2014.08.019. PMID 25173617. S2CID 13565641.
  94. ^ McIntire, Roger W.; Colley, Thomas A. (June 1967). "Social Reinforcement In The Dog". Psychological Reports. 20 (3): 843–846. doi:10.2466/pr0.1967.20.3.843. PMID 6042498. S2CID 33962156.
  95. ^ Berns, G. S.; Brooks, A.; Spivak, M. (2013). Brass, Marcel (ed.). "Replicability and Heterogeneity of Awake Unrestrained Canine fMRI Responses". PLOS ONE. 8 (12): e81698. Bibcode:2013PLoSO...881698B. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0081698. PMC 3852264. PMID 24324719.
  96. ^ Berns, G. S.; Brooks, A. M.; Spivak, M. (2015). "Scent of the familiar: An fMRI study of canine brain responses to familiar and unfamiliar human and dog odors". Behavioural Processes. 110: 37–46. doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2014.02.011. PMID 24607363.
  97. ^ Topál, J., Miklósi, Á. and Csányi, V. (1997). "Dog-human relationship affects problem solving behavior in the dog". Anthrozoös. 10 (4): 214–224. doi:10.2752/089279397787000987.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  98. ^ Siniscalchi, Marcello; Stipo, Carlo; Quaranta, Angelo (2013). ""Like Owner, Like Dog": Correlation between the Owner's Attachment Profile and the Owner-Dog Bond". PLOS ONE. 8 (10): e78455. Bibcode:2013PLoSO...878455S. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0078455. PMC 3813613. PMID 24205235.
  99. ^ Lee, Mary R.; Olmert, Meg D.; Yount, Rick A. (2012). "Service Dog Training Program for Treatment of Posttraumatic Stress in Service Members" (PDF). U.S. Army Medical Department Journal: 63–9. PMID 22388685. Retrieved 30 September 2017.
  100. ^ Prynne, M. (November 6, 2013). "Dog attack laws and statistics". The Telegraph. Retrieved April 14, 2015.
  101. ^ "Dog bite hospitalisations highest in deprived areas". NHS Choices. 2014. Retrieved April 14, 2015.
  102. ^ Sacks, J.J.; Sinclair, L; Gilchrist, J; Golab, G. C.; Lockwood, R (2000). (PDF). JAVMA. 217 (6): 836–840. doi:10.2460/javma.2000.217.836. PMID 10997153. S2CID 10817039. Archived from the original (PDF) on April 11, 2015. Retrieved April 14, 2015.
  103. ^ Miklósi, Ádám (2007). Dog Behaviour, Evolution, and Cognition. Oxford: Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199295852.001.0001. ISBN 9780199295852.
  104. ^ United States Congress. Senate. Committee on Commerce. Subcommittee on the Environment (1976). Animal Welfare Improvement Act of 1975: Hearing Before the Subcommittee on the Environment of the Committee on Commerce. United States Government. p. 111. Nearly 10 per cent of the dogs that have bitten people have received attack dog training.

Further reading Edit

  • Donaldson, Jean (1991). The Culture Clash. James & Kenneth Publishers.
  • Hare, Brian; Woods, Venessa (2013). The Genius of Dogs. Penguin Publishing Group.
  • Jordan, Rain (2020). Such Small Hands: An Anti-Aversives Primer. Dog's Heart Press.
  • Miklosi, Adam (2007). Dog Behaviour, Evolution, and Cognition. Oxford University Press.
  • "Pet Behavior articles from the ASPCA".

External links Edit

behavior, internally, coordinated, responses, individuals, groups, domestic, dogs, internal, external, stimuli, been, shaped, millennia, contact, with, humans, their, lifestyles, result, this, physical, social, evolution, dogs, have, acquired, ability, underst. Dog behavior is the internally coordinated responses of individuals or groups of domestic dogs to internal and external stimuli 1 It has been shaped by millennia of contact with humans and their lifestyles As a result of this physical and social evolution dogs have acquired the ability to understand and communicate with humans 2 Behavioral scientists have uncovered a wide range of social cognitive abilities in domestic dogs A drawing by Konrad Lorenz showing facial expressions of a dog a communication behavior X axis is aggression y axis is fear Contents 1 Co evolution with humans 2 Intelligence 3 Senses 4 Communication behavior 5 Social behavior 5 1 Play 5 1 1 Dog and dog 5 1 2 Dog and human 5 2 Empathy 5 3 Personalities 5 4 Leadership dominance and social groups 5 5 Scent 5 6 Feral dogs 5 7 Other behavior 6 Reproduction behavior 6 1 Estrous cycle and mating 6 2 Parenting and early life 7 Behavior problems 7 1 Separation anxiety 7 2 Resource guarding 7 3 Jealousy 7 4 Noise anxiety 7 5 Tail chasing 8 Behavior compared to other canids 8 1 Early aggression 8 2 Tameness 8 3 Social structure 8 4 Socialization 8 5 Cognition 8 6 Reproduction 8 7 Space 8 8 Predation and scavenging 9 Dogs in human society 9 1 Dogs in the family 9 2 Dogs at work 10 Attacks 11 See also 12 References 13 Further reading 14 External linksCo evolution with humans EditMain article Origin of the domestic dog The origin of the domestic dog Canis familiaris is not clear Whole genome sequencing indicates that the dog the gray wolf and the extinct Taymyr wolf diverged around the same time 27 000 40 000 years ago 3 How dogs became domesticated is not clear however the two main hypotheses are self domestication or human domestication There exists evidence of human canine behavioral coevolution Intelligence EditMain article Dog intelligence Dog intelligence is the ability of the dog to perceive information and retain it as knowledge in order to solve problems Dogs have been shown to learn by inference A study with Rico showed that he knew the labels of over 200 different items 4 He inferred the names of novel items by exclusion learning and correctly retrieved those novel items immediately He also retained this ability four weeks after the initial exposure Dogs have advanced memory skills A study documented the learning and memory capabilities of a border collie Chaser who had learned the names and could associate by verbal command over 1 000 words Dogs are able to read and react appropriately to human body language such as gesturing and pointing and to understand human voice commands After undergoing training to solve a simple manipulation task dogs that are faced with an insolvable version of the same problem look at the human while socialized wolves do not Dogs demonstrate a theory of mind by engaging in deception 5 6 Senses EditMain article Dog anatomy Senses The dog s senses include vision hearing sense of smell taste touch proprioception and sensitivity to the Earth s magnetic field Communication behavior EditMain article Dog communication Dog communication is about how dogs speak to each other how they understand messages that humans send to them and how humans can translate the ideas that dogs are trying to transmit 7 xii These communication behaviors include eye gaze facial expression vocalization body posture including movements of bodies and limbs and gustatory communication scents pheromones and taste Humans communicate with dogs by using vocalization hand signals and body posture Dogs can also learn to understand the communication of emotions with humans by reading human facial expressions 8 Social behavior EditTwo studies have indicated that dog behavior vary based on their size body weight and skull size 9 10 Play Edit Dog and dog Edit Play between dogs usually involves several behaviors often seen in aggressive encounters such as nipping biting and growling 11 It is therefore important for the dogs to place these behaviors in the context of the play rather than aggression Dogs signal their intent to play with a range of behaviors including a play bow face paw open mouthed play face and postures inviting the other dog to chase the initiator Similar signals are given throughout the play to maintain the context of the potentially aggressive activities 12 From a young age dogs engage in play with one another Dog play is made up primarily of mock fights It is believed that this behavior which is most common in puppies is training for important behaviors later in life Play between puppies is not necessarily a 50 50 symmetry of dominant and submissive roles between the individuals dogs who engage in greater rates of dominant behaviors e g chasing forcing partners down at later ages also initiate play at higher rates This could imply that winning during play becomes more important as puppies mature 13 Emotional contagion is linked to facial mimicry in humans and primates Facial mimicry is an automatic response that occurs in less than 1 second in which one person involuntarily mimics another person s facial expressions forming empathy It has also been found in dogs at play and play sessions lasted longer when there were facial mimicry signals from one dog to another 14 Dog and human Edit nbsp NASA astronaut Leland D Melvin with his dogs Jake and ScoutThe motivation for a dog to play with another dog is different from that of a dog playing with a human Dogs walked together with opportunities to play with one another and play with their owners with the same frequency as dogs being walked alone Dogs in households with two or more dogs play more often with their owners than dogs in households with a single dog indicating the motivation to play with other dogs does not substitute for the motivation to play with humans 15 It is a common misconception that winning and losing games such as tug of war and rough and tumble can influence a dog s dominant relationship with humans Rather how dogs play indicates their temperament and relationship with their owner Dogs that play rough and tumble are more amenable and show lower separation anxiety than dogs which play other types of games and dogs playing tug of war and fetch are more confident Dogs that start most games are less amenable and more likely to be aggressive 16 Playing with humans can affect the cortisol levels of dogs In one study the cortisol responses of police dogs and border guard dogs were assessed after playing with their handlers The cortisol concentrations of the police dogs increased whereas the border guard dogs hormone levels decreased The researchers noted that during the play sessions police officers were disciplining their dogs whereas the border guards were truly playing with them i e this included bonding and affectionate behaviors They commented that several studies have shown that behaviors associated with control authority or aggression increase cortisol whereas play and affiliation behavior decrease cortisol levels 17 Empathy Edit In 2012 a study found that dogs oriented toward their owner or a stranger more often when the person was pretending to cry than when they were talking or humming When the stranger pretended to cry rather than approaching their usual source of comfort their owner dogs sniffed nuzzled and licked the stranger instead The dogs pattern of response was behaviorally consistent with an expression of empathic concern 18 A study found a third of dogs suffered from anxiety when separated from others 19 Personalities Edit The term personality has been applied to human research whereas the term temperament has been mostly used for animal research 20 However both terms have been used interchangeably in the literature or purely to distinguish humans from animals and avoid anthropomorphism 21 Personality can be defined as a set of behaviors that are consistent over context and time 22 Studies of dogs personalities have tried to identify the presence of broad personality traits that are stable and consistent over time 21 22 23 24 25 There are different approaches to assess dog personality Ratings of individual dogs either a caretaker or a dog expert who is familiar with the dog is asked to answer a questionnaire for instance the Canine Behavioral Assessment and Research Questionnaire 26 concerning how often the dog shows certain types of behavior Tests the dog is submitted to a set of tests and its reactions are evaluated on a behavioral scale For instance the dog is presented to a familiar and then an unfamiliar person in order to measure sociability or aggression 27 Observational test The dog s behavior is evaluated in a selected but not controlled environment An observer focuses on the dog s reactions to naturally occurring stimuli For example a walk through the supermarket can allow the observer to see the dog in various types of conditions crowded noisy 28 Several potential personality traits have been identified in dogs for instance Playfulness Curiosity Fearlessness Chase proneness Sociability and Aggressiveness and Shyness Boldness 29 30 A meta analysis of 51 published peer reviewed articles identified seven dimensions of canine personality 21 Reactivity approach or avoidance of new objects increased activity in novel situations Fearfulness shaking avoiding novel situations Activity Sociability initiating friendly interactions with people and other dogs Responsiveness to training working with people learning quickly Submissiveness AggressionWith regard to the nature versus nurture debate according to a study in April 2022 carried out by Kathleen Morrill and others who work in a laboratory that was based on genetic and survey data of nearly 2000 dogs with the majority of them having their entire genomes sequenced as well as survey results from 16 000 owners of dogs The dogs included mixes and purebreds with 128 breeds represented The study found that the physical traits of a dog can be attributed to 80 on DNA and that retrieving and friendliness around humans were predominantly genetic But breed alone is only responsible for about 9 of individual personality differences with about 25 of personality traits determined by mainly individual genetics in total and the rest determined by the environment 31 The effects of age and sex have not been clearly determined 22 The personality models can be used for a range of tasks including guide and working dog selection finding appropriate families to re home shelter dogs or selecting breeding stock 32 33 34 Leadership dominance and social groups Edit nbsp Two dogs playing follow the leader Dominance is a descriptive term for the relationship between pairs of individuals Among ethologists dominance has been defined as an attribute of the pattern of repeated antagonistic interactions between two individuals characterized by a consistent outcome in favor of the same dyad member and a default yielding response of its opponent rather than escalation The status of the consistent winner is dominant and that of the loser subordinate 35 Another definition is that a dominant animal has priority of access to resources 35 Dominance is a relative attribute not absolute there is no reason to assume that a high ranking individual in one group would also become high ranking if moved to another Nor is there any good evidence that dominance is a lifelong character trait Competitive behavior characterized by confident e g growl inhibited bite stand over stare at chase bark at and submissive e g crouch avoid displacement lick yawn run away patterns exchanged 36 One test to ascertain in which group the dominant dog was used the following criteria When a stranger comes to the house which dog starts to bark first or if they start to bark together which dog barks more or longer Which dog licks more often the other dog s mouth If the dogs get food at the same time and at the same spot which dog starts to eat first or eats the other dog s food If the dogs start to fight which dog usually wins 37 Domestic dogs appear to pay little attention to relative size despite the large weight differences between the largest and smallest individuals for example size was not a predictor of the outcome of encounters between dogs meeting while being exercised by their owners nor was size correlated with neutered male dogs 38 Therefore many dogs do not appear to pay much attention to the actual fighting ability of their opponent presumably allowing differences in motivation how much the dog values the resource and perceived motivation what the behavior of the other dog signifies about the likelihood that it will escalate to play a much greater role 36 Two dogs that are contesting possession of a highly valued resource for the first time if one is in a state of emotional arousal in pain if reactivity is influenced by recent endocrine changes or motivational states such as hunger then the outcome of the interaction may be different than if none of these factors were present Equally the threshold at which aggression is shown may be influenced by a range of medical factors or in some cases precipitated entirely by pathological disorders Hence the contextual and physiological factors present when two dogs first encounter each other may profoundly influence the long term nature of the relationship between those dogs The complexity of the factors involved in this type of learning means that dogs may develop different expectations about the likely response of another individual for each resource in a range of different situations Puppies learn early not to challenge an older dog and this respect stays with them into adulthood When adult animals meet for the first time they have no expectations of the behavior of the other they will both therefore be initially anxious and vigilant in this encounter characterized by the tense body posture and sudden movements typically seen when two dogs first meet until they start to be able to predict the responses of the other individual The outcome of these early adult adult interactions will be influenced by the specific factors present at the time of the initial encounters As well as contextual and physiological factors the experiences of each member of the dyad of other dogs will also influence their behavior 36 Scent Edit Dogs have an olfactory sense 40 times more sensitive than a human s and they commence their lives operating almost exclusively on smell and touch 7 247 The special scents that dogs use for communication are called pheromones Different hormones are secreted when a dog is angry fearful or confident and some chemical signatures identify the sex and age of the dog and if a female is in the estrus cycle pregnant or recently given birth Many of the pheromone chemicals can be found dissolved in a dog s urine and sniffing where another dog has urinated gives the dog a great deal of information about that dog 7 250 Male dogs prefer to mark vertical surfaces and having the scent higher allows the air to carry it farther The height of the marking tells other dogs about the size of the dog as among canines size is an important factor in dominance 7 251 Dogs and wolves mark their territories with urine and their stools 39 The anal gland of canines give a particular signature to fecal deposits and identifies the marker as well as the place where the dung is left Dogs are very particular about these landmarks and engage in what is to humans a meaningless and complex ritual before defecating Most dogs start with a careful bout of sniffing of a location perhaps to erect an exact line or boundary between their territory and another dog s territory This behavior may also involve a small degree of elevation such as a rock or fallen branch to aid scent dispersal Scratching the ground after defecating is a visual sign pointing to the scent marking The freshness of the scent gives visitors some idea of the current status of a piece of territory and if it is used frequently Regions under dispute or used by different animals at different times may lead to marking battles with every scent marked over by a new competitor 7 252 4 Further information Dog communication Feral dogs Edit Feral dogs are those dogs living in a wild state with no food and shelter intentionally provided by humans and showing a continuous and strong avoidance of direct human contacts 40 In the developing world pet dogs are uncommon but feral village or community dogs are plentiful around humans 41 The distinction between feral stray and free ranging dogs is sometimes a matter of degree and a dog may shift its status throughout its life In some unlikely but observed cases a feral dog that was not born wild but living with a feral group can become behavior modified to a domestic dog with an owner A dog can become a stray when it escapes human control by abandonment or being born to a stray mother A stray dog can become feral when forced out of the human environment or when co opted or socially accepted by a nearby feral group Feralization occurs through the development of the human avoidance response 40 Feral dogs are not reproductively self sustaining suffer from high rates of juvenile mortality and depend indirectly on humans for their food their space and the supply of co optable individuals 40 See further behavior compared to other canids Other behavior Edit Dogs have a general behavioral trait of strongly preferring novelty neophillia compared to familiarity 42 The average sleep time of a dog in captivity in a 24 hour period is 10 1 hours 43 Reproduction behavior EditFurther information Canine reproduction Estrous cycle and mating Edit Although puppies do not have the urge to procreate males sometimes engage in sexual play in the form of mounting 44 In some puppies this behavior occurs as early as 3 or 4 weeks of age 45 Dogs reach sexual maturity and can reproduce during their first year in contrast to wolves at two years of age Female dogs have their first estrus heat at 6 to 12 months of age smaller dogs tend to come into heat earlier whereas larger dogs take longer to mature Female dogs have an estrous cycle that is nonseasonal and monestrus i e there is only one estrus per estrous cycle The interval between one estrus and another is on average seven months however this may range between 4 and 12 months This interestrous period is not influenced by the photoperiod or pregnancy The average duration of estrus is 9 days with spontaneous ovulation usually about 3 days after the onset of estrus 46 For several days before estrus a phase called proestrus the female dog may show greater interest in male dogs and flirt with them proceptive behavior There is progressive vulval swelling and some bleeding If males try to mount a female dog during proestrus she may avoid mating by sitting down or turning round and growling or snapping Estrous behavior in the female dog is usually indicated by her standing still with the tail held up or to the side of the perineum when the male sniffs the vulva and attempts to mount This tail position is sometimes called flagging The female dog may also turn presenting the vulva to the male 46 The male dog mounts the female and is able to achieve intromission with a non erect penis which contains a bone called the os penis The dog s penis enlarges inside the vagina thereby preventing its withdrawal this is sometimes known as the tie or copulatory lock The male dog rapidly thrust into the female for 1 2 minutes then dismounts with the erect penis still inside the vagina and turns to stand rear end to rear end with the female dog for up to 30 to 40 minutes the penis is twisted 180 degrees in a lateral plane During this time prostatic fluid is ejaculated 46 The female dog can bear another litter within 8 months of the previous one Dogs are polygamous in contrast to wolves that are generally monogamous Therefore dogs have no pair bonding and the protection of a single mate but rather have multiple mates in a year The consequence is that wolves put a lot of energy into producing a few pups in contrast to dogs that maximize the production of pups This higher pup production rate enables dogs to maintain or even increase their population with a lower pup survival rate than wolves and allows dogs a greater capacity than wolves to grow their population after a population crash or when entering a new habitat It is proposed that these differences are an alternative breeding strategy one adapted to a life of scavenging instead of hunting 47 Parenting and early life Edit All of the wild members of the genus Canis display complex coordinated parental behaviors Wolf pups are cared for primarily by their mother for the first 3 months of their life when she remains in the den with them while they rely on her milk for sustenance and her presence for protection The father brings her food Once they leave the den and can chew the parents and pups from previous years regurgitate food for them Wolf pups become independent by 5 to 8 months although they often stay with their parents for years In contrast dog pups are cared for by the mother and rely on her for milk and protection but she gets no help from the father nor other dogs Once pups are weaned around 10 weeks they are independent and receive no further maternal care 47 Behavior problems EditThere are many different types of behavioural issues that a dog can exhibit including growling snapping barking and invading a human s personal space A survey of 203 dog owners in Melbourne Australia found that the main behaviour problems reported by owners were overexcitement 63 and jumping up on people 56 48 Some problems are related to attachment while others are neurological as seen below Separation anxiety Edit When dogs are separated from humans usually the owner they often display behaviors which can be broken into the following four categories exploratory behaviour object play destructive behaviour and vocalization and they are related to the canine s level of arousal 49 These behaviours may manifest as destructiveness fecal or urinary elimination hypersalivation or vocalization among other things Dogs from single owner homes are approximately 2 5 times more likely to have separation anxiety compared to dogs from multiple owner homes Furthermore sexually intact dogs are only one third as likely to have separation anxiety as neutered dogs The sex of dogs and whether there is another pet in the home do not have an effect on separation anxiety 50 It has been estimated that at least 14 of dogs examined at typical veterinary practices in the United States have shown signs of separation anxiety Dogs that have been diagnosed with profound separation anxiety can be left alone for no more than minutes before they begin to panic and exhibit the behaviors associated with separation anxiety Separation problems have been found to be linked to the dog s dependency on its owner not because of disobedience 49 In the absence of treatment affected dogs are often relinquished to a humane society or shelter abandoned or euthanized 51 Resource guarding Edit Resource guarding is exhibited by many canines and is one of the most commonly reported behaviour issues to canine professionals 52 It is seen when a dog uses specific behaviour patterns so that they can control access to an item and the patterns are flexible when people are around 53 If a canine places value on some resource i e food toys etc they may attempt to guard it from other animals as well as people which leads to behavioural problems if not treated The guarding can show in many different ways from rapid ingestion of food to using the body to shield items It manifests as aggressive behaviour including but not limited to growling barking or snapping Some dogs will also resource guard their owners and can become aggressive if the behaviour is allowed to continue Owners must learn to interpret their dog s body language in order to try to judge the dog s reaction as visual signals are used i e changes in body posture facial expression etc to communicate feeling and response 52 These behaviours are commonly seen in shelter animals most likely due to insecurities caused by a poor environment Resource guarding is a concern since it can lead to aggression but research has found that aggression over guarding can be contained by teaching the dog to drop the item they are guarding 53 Jealousy Edit Canines are one of a number of non human animals that can express jealousy towards other animals or animal like objects 54 This emotion may feed into other behavioural problems manifest as attention seeking behaviour withdrawing from social activity or aggression towards their owner or another animal or person Noise anxiety Edit Canines often fear and exhibit stress responses to loud noises Noise related anxieties in dogs may be triggered by fireworks thunderstorms gunshots and even loud or sharp bird noises Associated stimuli may also come to trigger the symptoms of the phobia or anxiety such as a change in barometric pressure being associated with a thunderstorm thus causing an anticipatory anxiety Tail chasing Edit Tail chasing can be classified as a stereotypy It falls under obsessive compulsive disorder which is a neuropsychiatric disorder that can present in dogs as canine compulsive disorder 55 In one clinical study on this potential behavioral problem 18 tail chasing terriers were given clomipramine orally at a dosage of 1 to 2 mg kg 0 5 to 0 9 mg lb of body weight every 12 hours Three of the dogs required treatment at a slightly higher dosage range to control tail chasing however after 1 to 12 weeks of treatment 9 of 12 dogs were reported to have a 75 or greater reduction in tail chasing 56 Personality can also play a factor in tail chasing Dogs who chase their tails have been found to be more shy than those who do not and some dogs also show a lower level of response during tail chasing bouts 55 Behavior compared to other canids EditComparisons made within the wolf like canids allow the identification of those behaviors that may have been inherited from common ancestry and those that may have been the result of domestication or other relatively recent environmental changes 40 Studies of free ranging African Basenjis and New Guinea Singing Dogs indicate that their behavioral and ecological traits were the result of environmental selection pressures or selective breeding choices and not the result of artificial selection imposed by humans 57 Early aggression Edit Dog pups show unrestrained fighting with their siblings from 2 weeks of age with injury avoided only due to their undeveloped jaw muscles This fighting gives way to play chasing with the development of running skills at 4 5 weeks Wolf pups possess more developed jaw muscles from 2 weeks of age when they first show signs of play fighting with their siblings Serious fighting occurs during 4 6 weeks of age 58 Compared to wolf and dog pups golden jackal pups develop aggression at the age of 4 6 weeks when play fighting frequently escalates into uninhibited biting intended to harm This aggression ceases by 10 12 weeks when a hierarchy has formed 59 Tameness Edit Unlike other domestic species which were primarily selected for production related traits dogs were initially selected for their behaviors 60 61 In 2016 a study found that there were only 11 fixed genes that showed variation between wolves and dogs These gene variations were unlikely to have been the result of natural evolution and indicate selection on both morphology and behavior during dog domestication These genes have been shown to affect the catecholamine synthesis pathway with the majority of the genes affecting the fight or flight response 61 62 i e selection for tameness and emotional processing 61 Dogs generally show reduced fear and aggression compared to wolves 61 63 Some of these genes have been associated with aggression in some dog breeds indicating their importance in both the initial domestication and then later in breed formation 61 Social structure Edit Among canids packs are the social units that hunt rear young and protect a communal territory as a stable group and their members are usually related 64 Members of the feral dog group are usually not related Feral dog groups are composed of a stable 2 6 members compared to the 2 15 member wolf pack whose size fluctuates with the availability of prey and reaches a maximum in winter time The feral dog group consists of monogamous breeding pairs compared to the one breeding pair of the wolf pack Agonistic behavior does not extend to the individual level and does not support a higher social structure compared to the ritualized agonistic behavior of the wolf pack that upholds its social structure Feral pups have a very high mortality rate that adds little to the group size with studies showing that adults are usually killed through accidents with humans therefore other dogs need to be co opted from villages to maintain stable group size 40 Socialization Edit The critical period for socialization begins with walking and exploring the environment Dog and wolf pups both develop the ability to see hear and smell at 4 weeks of age Dogs begin to explore the world around them at 4 weeks of age with these senses available to them while wolves begin to explore at 2 weeks of age when they have the sense of smell but are functionally blind and deaf The consequences of this is that more things are novel and frightening to wolf pups The critical period for socialization closes with the avoidance of novelty when the animal runs away from rather than approaching and exploring novel objects For dogs this develops between 4 and 8 weeks of age Wolves reach the end of the critical period after 6 weeks after which it is not possible to socialize a wolf 47 Dog puppies require as little as 90 minutes of contact with humans during their critical period of socialization to form a social attachment This will not create a highly social pet but a dog that will solicit human attention 65 Wolves require 24 hours contact a day starting before 3 weeks of age To create a socialized wolf the pups are removed from the den at 10 days of age kept in constant human contact until they are 4 weeks old when they begin to bite their sleeping human companions then spend only their waking hours in the presence of humans This socialization process continues until age 4 months when the pups can join other captive wolves but will require daily human contact to remain socialized Despite this intensive socialization process a well socialized wolf will behave differently to a well socialized dog and will display species typical hunting and reproductive behaviors only closer to humans than a wild wolf These wolves do not generalize their socialization to all humans in the same manner as a socialized dog and they remain more fearful of novelty compared to socialized dogs 66 In 1982 a study to observe the differences between dogs and wolves raised in similar conditions took place The dog puppies preferred larger amounts of sleep at the beginning of their lives while the wolf puppies were much more active The dog puppies also preferred the company of humans rather than their canine foster mother though the wolf puppies were the exact opposite spending more time with their foster mother The dogs also showed a greater interest in the food given to them and paid little attention to their surroundings while the wolf puppies found their surroundings to be much more intriguing than their food or food bowl The wolf puppies were observed taking part in antagonistic play at a younger age while the dog puppies did not display dominant submissive roles until they were much older The wolf puppies were rarely seen as being aggressive to each other or towards the other canines On the other hand the dog puppies were much more aggressive to each other and other canines often seen full on attacking their foster mother or one another 67 A 2005 study comparing dog and wolf pups concluded that extensively socialised dogs as well as unsocialised dog pups showed greater attachment to a human owner than wolf pups did even if the wolf was socialised The study concluded that dogs may have evolved a capacity for attachment to humans functionally analogous to that human infants display 68 Cognition Edit Despite claims that dogs show more human like social cognition than wolves 69 70 71 several recent studies have demonstrated that if wolves are properly socialized to humans and have the opportunity to interact with humans regularly then they too can succeed on some human guided cognitive tasks 72 73 74 75 76 in some cases out performing dogs at an individual level 77 Similar to dogs wolves can also follow more complex point types made with body parts other than the human arm and hand e g elbow knee foot 76 Both dogs and wolves have the cognitive capacity for prosocial behavior toward humans however it is not guaranteed For canids to perform well on traditional human guided tasks e g following the human point both relevant lifetime experiences with humans including socialization to humans during the critical period for social development and opportunities to associate human body parts with certain outcomes such as food being provided by human hands a human throwing or kicking a ball etc are required 78 After undergoing training to solve a simple manipulation task dogs that are faced with an insoluble version of the same problem look at the human while socialized wolves do not 71 Reproduction Edit Dogs reach sexual maturity and can reproduce during their first year in contrast to a wolf at two years The female dog can bear another litter within 8 months of the last one The canid genus is influenced by the photoperiod and generally reproduces in the springtime 40 Domestic dogs are not reliant on seasonality for reproduction in contrast to the wolf coyote Australian dingo and African basenji that may have only one seasonal estrus each year 46 Feral dogs are influenced by the photoperiod with around half of the breeding females mating in the springtime which is thought to indicate an ancestral reproductive trait not overcome by domestication 40 as can be inferred from wolves 79 and Cape hunting dogs 80 Domestic dogs are polygamous in contrast to wolves that are generally monogamous Therefore domestic dogs have no pair bonding and the protection of a single mate but rather have multiple mates in a year There is no paternal care in dogs as opposed to wolves where all pack members assist the mother with the pups The consequence is that wolves put a lot of energy into producing a few pups in contrast to dogs that maximize the production of pups This higher pup production rate enables dogs to maintain or even increase their population with a lower pup survival rate than wolves and allows dogs a greater capacity than wolves to grow their population after a population crash or when entering a new habitat It is proposed that these differences are an alternative breeding strategy adapted to a life of scavenging instead of hunting 47 In contrast to domestic dogs feral dogs are monogamous Domestic dogs tend to have a litter size of 10 wolves 3 and feral dogs 5 8 Feral pups have a very high mortality rate with only 5 surviving at the age of one year and sometimes the pups are left unattended making them vulnerable to predators 40 Domestic dogs stand alone among all canids for a total lack of paternal care 81 Dogs differ from wolves and most other large canid species as they generally do not regurgitate food for their young nor the young of other dogs in the same territory 82 However this difference was not observed in all domestic dogs Regurgitating of food by the females for the young as well as care for the young by the males has been observed in domestic dogs dingos and in feral or semi feral dogs In one study of a group of free ranging dogs for the first 2 weeks immediately after parturition the lactating females were observed to be more aggressive to protect the pups The male parents were in contact with the litters as guard dogs for the first 6 8 weeks of the litters life In absence of the mothers they were observed to prevent the approach of strangers by vocalizations or even by physical attacks Moreover one male fed the litter by regurgitation showing the existence of paternal care in some free roaming dogs 83 Space Edit Space used by feral dogs is not dissimilar from most other canids in that they use defined traditional areas home ranges that tend to be defended against intruders and have core areas where most of their activities are undertaken Urban domestic dogs have a home range of 2 61 hectares in contrast to a feral dog s home range of 58 square kilometers Wolf home ranges vary from 78 square kilometers where prey is deer to 2 5 square kilometers at higher latitudes where prey is moose and caribou Wolves will defend their territory based on prey abundance and pack density but feral dogs will defend their home ranges all year Where wolf ranges and feral dog ranges overlap the feral dogs will site their core areas closer to human settlement 40 Predation and scavenging Edit Despite claims in the popular press studies could not find evidence of a single predation on cattle by feral dogs 40 84 85 However domestic dogs were responsible for the death of 3 calves over one 5 year study 85 Other studies in Europe and North America indicate moderate limited success in the consumption of wild boar deer and other ungulates however it could not be determined if this was predation or scavenging on carcasses A new study has shown though that these were most likely due to predation 86 Feral dogs like their ancestors do participate in pup rearing Several studies show that feral dogs are not primarily scavengers despite claims in the popular press Studies in the modern era show that their diet is very opportunistic ranging from garbage carrion to live prey The primary feature that distinguishes feral from domestic dogs is the degree of reliance or dependence on humans and in some respect their behavior toward people Feral dogs survive and reproduce independently of human intervention or assistance While it is true that some feral dogs use human garbage for food others acquire their primary subsistence by hunting and scavenging like other wild canids 87 Dogs may resort to hunting more than garbage consuming when their garbage food source is scarce Even well fed domestic dogs are prone to scavenge gastro intestinal veterinary visits increase during warmer weather as dogs are prone to eat decaying material 88 Some dogs consume feces which may contain nutrition 89 90 On occasion well fed dogs have been known to scavenge their owners corpses 91 Dogs in human society EditFurther information Human canine bond Studies using an operant framework have indicated that humans can influence the behavior of dogs through food petting and voice Food and 20 30 seconds of petting maintained operant responding in dogs 92 Some dogs will show a preference for petting once food is readily available and dogs will remain in proximity to a person providing petting and show no satiation to that stimulus 93 Petting alone was sufficient to maintain the operant response of military dogs to voice commands and responses to basic obedience commands in all dogs increased when only vocal praise was provided for correct responses 94 A study using dogs that were trained to remain motionless while unsedated and unrestrained in an MRI scanner exhibited caudate activation to a hand signal associated with reward 2 Further work found that the magnitude of the canine caudate response is similar to that of humans while the between subject variability in dogs may be less than humans 95 In a further study 5 scents were presented self familiar human strange human familiar dog strange dog While the olfactory bulb peduncle was activated to a similar degree by all the scents the caudate was activated maximally to the familiar human Importantly the scent of the familiar human was not the handler meaning that the caudate response differentiated the scent in the absence of the person being present The caudate activation suggested that not only did the dogs discriminate that scent from the others they had a positive association with it Although these signals came from two different people the humans lived in the same household as the dog and therefore represented the dog s primary social circle And while dogs should be highly tuned to the smell of items that are not comparable it seems that the reward response is reserved for their humans 96 Research has shown that there are individual differences in the interactions between dogs and their human that have significant effects on dog behavior In 1997 a study showed that the type of relationship between dog and master characterized as either companionship or working relationship significantly affected the dog s performance on a cognitive problem solving task They speculate that companion dogs have a more dependent relationship with their owners and look to them to solve problems In contrast working dogs are more independent 97 Dogs in the family Edit In 2013 a study produced the first evidence under controlled experimental observation for a correlation between the owner s personality and their dog s behaviour 98 Further information Companion dog Dogs at work Edit Service dogs are those that are trained to help people with disabilities such as blindness epilepsy diabetes and autism Detection dogs are trained to using their sense of smell to detect substances such as explosives illegal drugs wildlife scat or blood In science dogs have helped humans understand about the conditioned reflex Attack dogs dogs that have been trained to attack on command are employed in security police and military roles Service dog programs have been established to help individuals suffering from Post Traumatic Stress Disorder PTSD and have shown to have positive results 99 Attacks EditMain article Dog attack nbsp A dog s teeth can inflict serious injuriesThe human dog relationship is based on unconditional trust however if this trust is lost it will be difficult to reinstate citation needed In the UK between 2005 and 2013 there were 17 fatal dog attacks In 2007 08 there were 4 611 hospital admissions due to dog attacks which increased to 5 221 in 2008 09 It was estimated in 2013 that more than 200 000 people a year are bitten by dogs in England with the annual cost to the National Health Service of treating injuries about 3 million 100 A report published in 2014 stated there were 6 743 hospital admissions specifically caused by dog bites a 5 8 increase from the 6 372 admissions in the previous 12 months 101 needs update In the US between 1979 and 1996 there were more than 300 human dog bite related fatalities 102 In the US in 2013 there were 31 dog bite related deaths Each year more than 4 5 million people in the US are bitten by dogs and almost 1 in 5 require medical attention A dog s thick fur protects it from the bite of another dog but humans are furless and are not so protected 103 Attack training is condemned by some as promoting ferocity in dogs a 1975 American study showed that 10 of dogs that have bitten a person received attack dog training at some point 104 See also EditGray wolf Behavior Alpha roll Dog communication Dog intelligence Calming signals Pack canine Pack hunter Separation anxiety disorder humans Temperament testReferences Edit Levitis Daniel William Z Lidicker Jr Glenn Freund Freund Glenn June 2009 Behavioural biologists do not agree on what constitutes behaviour PDF Animal Behaviour 78 1 103 10 doi 10 1016 j anbehav 2009 03 018 PMC 2760923 PMID 20160973 a b Berns G S Brooks A M Spivak M 2012 Neuhauss Stephan C F ed Functional MRI in Awake Unrestrained Dogs PLOS ONE 7 5 e38027 Bibcode 2012PLoSO 738027B doi 10 1371 journal pone 0038027 PMC 3350478 PMID 22606363 Skoglund P Ersmark E Palkopoulou E Dalen L 2015 Ancient Wolf Genome Reveals an Early Divergence of Domestic Dog Ancestors and Admixture into High Latitude Breeds Current Biology 25 11 1515 9 doi 10 1016 j cub 2015 04 019 PMID 26004765 The New Science Of Understanding Dog Behavior NPR org Retrieved 2022 05 28 Maginnity M E amp Grace R C 2014 Visual perspective taking by dogs Canis familiaris in a Guesser Knower task evidence for a canine theory of mind Animal Cognition 17 6 1375 1392 doi 10 1007 s10071 014 0773 9 PMID 24950722 S2CID 14833483 Kaminski J Brauer J Call J amp Tomasello M 2009 Domestic dogs are sensitive to a human s perspective PDF Behaviour 146 7 979 998 doi 10 1163 156853908X395530 a b c d e Coren Stanley How To Speak Dog Mastering the Art of Dog Human Communication 2000 Simon amp Schuster New York Huber Ludwig October 2016 How Dogs Perceive and Understand Us Current Directions in Psychological Science 25 5 339 344 doi 10 1177 0963721416656329 ISSN 0963 7214 S2CID 151488382 McGreevy Paul D Georgevsky Dana Carrasco Johanna Valenzuela Michael Duffy Deborah L Serpell James A 2013 Dog Behavior Co Varies with Height Bodyweight and Skull Shape PLOS ONE 8 12 e80529 Bibcode 2013PLoSO 880529M doi 10 1371 journal pone 0080529 PMC 3864788 PMID 24358107 Stone Holly R McGreevy Paul D Starling Melissa J Forkman Bjorn 2016 Associations between Domestic Dog Morphology and Behaviour Scores in the Dog Mentality Assessment PLOS ONE 11 2 e0149403 Bibcode 2016PLoSO 1149403S doi 10 1371 journal pone 0149403 PMC 4771026 PMID 26919495 Kujala Miiamaaria January 23 2017 Human Empathy Personality and Experience Affect the Emotion Ratings of Dog and Human Facial Expressions PLOS ONE 12 1 e0170730 Bibcode 2017PLoSO 1270730K doi 10 1371 journal pone 0170730 PMC 5257001 PMID 28114335 S2CID 1215168 Horowitz A 2009 Attention to attention in domestic dog Canis familiaris dyadic play Animal Cognition 12 1 107 118 doi 10 1007 s10071 008 0175 y PMID 18679727 S2CID 207050813 Ward C Bauer E B and Smuts B B 2008 Partner preferences and asymmetries in social play among domestic dog Canis lupus familiaris littermates PDF Animal Behaviour 76 4 1187 1199 doi 10 1016 j anbehav 2008 06 004 S2CID 1295114 a href Template Cite journal html title Template Cite journal cite journal a CS1 maint multiple names authors list link Palagi Elisabetta Nicotra Velia Cordoni Giada 2015 Rapid mimicry and emotional contagion in domestic dogs Royal Society Open Science 2 12 150505 Bibcode 2015RSOS 250505P doi 10 1098 rsos 150505 PMC 4807458 PMID 27019737 Rooney N J Bradshaw J W S and Robinson I H 2000 A comparison of dog dog and dog human play behaviour Applied Animal Behaviour Science 66 3 235 248 doi 10 1016 S0168 1591 99 00078 7 a href Template Cite journal html title Template Cite journal cite journal a CS1 maint multiple names authors list link Rooney N J Bradshaw Jv W S 2003 Links between play and dominance and attachment dimensions of dog human relationships Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 6 2 67 94 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 495 1702 doi 10 1207 S15327604JAWS0602 01 PMID 12909524 S2CID 2254971 Horvath Z Doka A Miklosi A 2008 Affiliative and disciplinary behavior of human handlers during play with their dog affects cortisol concentrations in opposite directions Hormones and Behavior 54 1 107 114 doi 10 1016 j yhbeh 2008 02 002 PMID 18353328 S2CID 16805722 Custance Deborah Mayer Jennifer 2012 Empathic like responding by domestic dogs Canis familiaris to distress in humans an exploratory study Animal Cognition 15 5 851 859 doi 10 1007 s10071 012 0510 1 PMID 22644113 S2CID 15153091 Behaviour problems linked to pessimistic dogs Sydney Morning Herald October 12 2010 Retrieved October 21 2010 McCrae R R Costa P T Ostendorf F Angleitner A Hrebickova M Avia M D Saunders P R 2000 Nature over nurture temperament personality and life span development Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 78 1 173 86 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 78 1 173 PMID 10653513 a b c Jones A C Gosling S D 2005 Temperament and personality in dogs Canis familiaris a review and evaluation of past research Applied Animal Behaviour Science 95 1 1 53 doi 10 1016 j applanim 2005 04 008 a b c Gartner M C 2015 Pet personality A review Personality and Individual Differences 75 102 113 doi 10 1016 j paid 2014 10 042 Fratkin J L Sinn D L Patal E A Gosling S D 2013 Personality consistency in dogs a meta analysis PLOS ONE 8 1 e54907 Bibcode 2013PLoSO 854907F doi 10 1371 journal pone 0054907 PMC 3553070 PMID 23372787 Vas J Muller C Gyori B Miklosi A 2008 Consistency of dogs reactions to threatening cues of an unfamiliar person Applied Animal Behaviour Science 112 3 331 344 doi 10 1016 j applanim 2007 09 002 Meegahapola L Constantinides M Radivojevic Z Li H Quercia D Eggleston M S 2023 Quantified Canine Inferring Dog Personality from Wearables Proceedings of the 2023 CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems pp 1 19 arXiv 2301 06964 doi 10 1145 3544548 3581088 ISBN 9781450394215 S2CID 255942162 Hu Y Serpell J A 2003 Development and validation of a questionnaire for measuring behavior and temperament traits in pet dogs Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association 223 9 1293 1300 doi 10 2460 javma 2003 223 1293 PMID 14621216 S2CID 9771544 De Meester R H De Bacquer D Peremans K Vermeire S Planta D J Coopman F Audenaert K 2008 A preliminary study on the use of the Socially Acceptable Behavior test as a test for shyness confidence in the temperament of dogs Journal of Veterinary Behavior Clinical Applications and Research 3 4 161 170 doi 10 1016 j jveb 2007 10 005 Barnard S Siracusa C Reisner I Valsecchi P Serpell J A 2012 Validity of model devices used to assess canine temperament in behavioral tests Applied Animal Behaviour Science 138 1 79 87 doi 10 1016 j applanim 2012 02 017 Svartberga K Forkman B 2002 Personality traits in the domestic dog Canis familiaris PDF Applied Animal Behaviour Science 79 2 133 155 doi 10 1016 S0168 1591 02 00121 1 Svartberg K Tapper I Temrin H Radesater T Thorman S February 2005 Consistency of personality traits in dogs Animal Behaviour 69 2 283 291 doi 10 1016 j anbehav 2004 04 011 S2CID 53154729 David Grimm 28 Apr 2022 Your dog s breed doesn t determine its personality study suggests American Association for the Advancement of Science Duffy D L Serpell J A 2008 Behavioral assessment of guide and service dogs Journal of Veterinary Behavior Clinical Applications and Research 3 4 186 188 doi 10 1016 j jveb 2007 12 010 Bollen K S Horowitz J 2008 Behavioral evaluation and demographic information in the assessment of aggressiveness in shelter dogs Applied Animal Behaviour Science 112 1 120 135 doi 10 1016 j applanim 2007 07 007 Riemer S Muller C Viranyi Z Huber L Range F 2013 Choice of conflict resolution strategy is linked to sociability in dog puppies Applied Animal Behaviour Science 149 1 36 44 doi 10 1016 j applanim 2013 09 006 PMC 4044588 PMID 24910487 a b Drews Carlos 1993 The Concept and Definition of Dominance in Animal Behaviour Behaviour 125 3 283 313 doi 10 1163 156853993X00290 a b c Bradshaw John W S Blackwell Emily J Casey Rachel A 2009 Dominance in domestic dogs useful construct or bad habit PDF Journal of Veterinary Behavior 4 3 135 144 doi 10 1016 j jveb 2008 08 004 Pongracz P et al 2008 How does dominance rank status affect individual and social learning performance in the dog Canis familiaris Animal Cognition 11 1 75 82 doi 10 1007 s10071 007 0090 7 ISSN 1435 9456 PMID 17492317 S2CID 8930934 Bradshaw John W S Lea Amanda M 1992 Dyadic Interactions Between Domestic Dogs Anthrozoos 5 4 245 253 doi 10 2752 089279392787011287 L David Mech Luigi Boitani 1 October 2010 Wolves Behavior Ecology and Conservation University of Chicago Press pp 84 ISBN 978 0 226 51698 1 a b c d e f g h i j Boitani L Ciucci P 1995 Comparative social ecology of feral dogs and wolves PDF Ethology Ecology amp Evolution 7 1 49 72 doi 10 1080 08927014 1995 9522969 Coppinger Ray 2001 Dogs a Startling New Understanding of Canine Origin Behavior and Evolution New York Scribner ISBN 978 0 684 85530 1 Kaulfuss P Mills D S 2008 Neophilia in domestic dogs Canis familiaris and its implication for studies of dog cognition Animal Cognition 11 3 553 556 doi 10 1007 s10071 007 0128 x PMID 18183436 S2CID 31406623 40 Winks Jennifer S Holland National Geographic Vol 220 No 1 July 2011 Bonnie V G Beaver 2009 Canine Behavior Insights and Answers Elsevier Health Sciences ISBN 978 1 4160 5419 1 Edward C Feldman Richard William Nelson 2004 Canine and Feline Endocrinology and Reproduction Elsevier Health Sciences ISBN 978 0 7216 9315 6 a b c d Canine and feline reproduction and contraception Michelson Prize and Grants Archived from the original on August 16 2015 Retrieved April 28 2015 a b c d Lord K 2013 A Comparison of the Sensory Development of Wolves Canis lupus lupus and Dogs Canis lupus familiaris Ethology 119 2 110 120 doi 10 1111 eth 12044 Kobelt A J Hemsworth P H Barnett J L and Coleman G J 2003 A survey of dog ownership in suburban Australia conditions and behaviour problems Applied Animal Behaviour Science 82 2 137 148 doi 10 1016 S0168 1591 03 00062 5 a href Template Cite journal html title Template Cite journal cite journal a CS1 maint multiple names authors list link a b Lund Jorgen Damkjer Jorgensen Mads Chr 1999 Behaviour patterns and time course of activity in dogs with separation problems Applied Animal Behaviour Science 63 3 219 236 doi 10 1016 S0168 1591 99 00011 8 Flannigan G Dodman N H A 2001 Risk factors and behaviors associated with separation anxiety in dogs Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association 219 4 460 466 doi 10 2460 javma 2001 219 460 PMID 11518171 Overall Dunham Frank Karen L Arthur E Diane 2001 Frequency of nonspecific clinical signs in dogs with separation anxiety thunderstorm phobia and noise phobia alone or in combination PDF JAVMA 219 4 467 473 doi 10 2460 javma 2001 219 467 PMID 11518172 via AVMA a href Template Cite journal html title Template Cite journal cite journal a CS1 maint multiple names authors list link a b Jacobs Jacquelyn A Pearl David L Coe Jason B Widowski Tina M Niel Lee 2017 Ability of owners to identify resource guarding behaviour in the domestic dog Applied Animal Behaviour Science 188 77 83 doi 10 1016 j applanim 2016 12 012 a b Jacobs Jacquelyn A Coe Jason B Pearl David L Widowski Tina M Niel Lee 2017 Factors associated with canine resource guarding behaviour in the presence of people A cross sectional survey of dog owners Preventive Veterinary Medicine 161 143 153 doi 10 1016 j prevetmed 2017 02 005 PMID 28268035 S2CID 207419404 Harris Christine R Prouvost Caroline 2014 07 23 Jealousy in Dogs PLOS ONE 9 7 e94597 Bibcode 2014PLoSO 994597H doi 10 1371 journal pone 0094597 ISSN 1932 6203 PMC 4108309 PMID 25054800 a b Tiira Katriina Hakosalo Osmo Kareinen Lauri Thomas Anne Hielm Bjorkman Anna Escriou Catherine Arnold Paul Lohi Hannes 2012 Environmental effects on compulsive tail chasing in dogs PLOS ONE 7 7 e41684 Bibcode 2012PLoSO 741684T doi 10 1371 journal pone 0041684 PMC 3406045 PMID 22844513 Moon Fanelli A A Dodman N H 1998 Description and development of compulsive tail chasing in terriers and response to clomipramine treatment Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association 212 8 1252 1257 PMID 9569164 Brisbin L Risch TS 1997 Primitive dogs their ecology and behavior Unique opportunities to study the early development of the human canine bond Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association 210 8 1122 1126 PMID 9108912 Frank Harry Frank Martha Gialdini 1982 On the effects of domestication on canine social development and behavior Applied Animal Ethology 8 6 507 doi 10 1016 0304 3762 82 90215 2 hdl 2027 42 23918 Feddersen Petersen D 1991 The ontogeny of social play and agonistic behaviour in selected canid species PDF Bonn Zool Beitr 42 97 114 Serpell J Duffy D Dog Breeds and Their Behavior In Domestic Dog Cognition and Behavior Berlin Heidelberg Springer 2014 a b c d e Cagan Alex Blass Torsten 2016 Identification of genomic variants putatively targeted by selection during dog domestication BMC Evolutionary Biology 16 10 doi 10 1186 s12862 015 0579 7 PMC 4710014 PMID 26754411 Almada RC Coimbra NC Recruitment of striatonigral disinhibitory and nigrotectal inhibitory GABAergic pathways during the organization of defensive behavior by mice in a dangerous environment with the venomous snake Bothrops alternatus Reptilia Viperidae Synapse 2015 n a n a Coppinger R Schneider R Evolution of working dogs The domestic dog Its evolution behaviour and interactions with people Cambridge Cambridge University press 1995 Mech L D 1970 The Wolf The Ecology and Behavior of an Endangered Species Natural History Press Doubleday Publishing Co N Y 389 pp Reprinted in paperback by University of Minnesota Press May 1981 Freedman Daniel G King John A Elliot Orville March 1961 Critical Period in the Social Development of Dogs Science 133 3457 1016 1017 Bibcode 1961Sci 133 1016F doi 10 1126 science 133 3457 1016 PMID 13701603 S2CID 278125 Klinghammer E amp Goodmann P A 1987 Socialization and management of wolves in captivity In H Frank ed Man and wolf Advances issues and problems in captive wolf research Dordrecht W Junk Frank H Frank MG 1982 On the effects of domestication on canine social development and behavior PDF Applied Animal Ethology 8 6 507 525 doi 10 1016 0304 3762 82 90215 2 hdl 2027 42 23918 Topal Jozsef Marta Gacsi Adam Miklosi Zsofia Viranyi Eniko Kubinyi and Vilmos Csanyi Attachment to humans a comparative study on hand reared wolves and differently socialized dog puppies Animal behaviour 70 no 6 2005 1367 1375 Hare B 2002 The Domestication of Social Cognition in Dogs PDF Science 298 5598 1634 6 Bibcode 2002Sci 298 1634H doi 10 1126 science 1072702 PMID 12446914 S2CID 13369396 Hare Brian Tomasello Michael September 2005 Human like social skills in dogs Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9 9 439 444 doi 10 1016 j tics 2005 07 003 PMID 16061417 S2CID 9311402 a b Miklosi Adam et al April 29 2003 A simple reason for a big difference wolves do not look back at humans but dogs do Current Biology 13 9 763 766 doi 10 1016 S0960 9822 03 00263 X PMID 12725735 S2CID 10200094 Gacsi M et al 2009 Explaining dog wolf differences in utilizing human pointing gestures Selection for synergistic shifts in the development of some social skills PLoS ONE 4 8 e6584 Bibcode 2009PLoSO 4 6584G doi 10 1371 journal pone 0006584 PMC 2719091 PMID 19714197 Range F Viranyi Z 2011 Development of gaze following abilities in wolves Canis lupus PLOS ONE 6 2 e16888 Bibcode 2011PLoSO 616888R doi 10 1371 journal pone 0016888 PMC 3044139 PMID 21373192 Udell M A R Dorey N R amp Wynne C D L December 2008 Wolves outperform dogs in following human social cues Animal Behaviour 76 6 1767 1773 doi 10 1016 j anbehav 2008 07 028 S2CID 11226439 a href Template Cite journal html title Template Cite journal cite journal a CS1 maint uses authors parameter link Udell M A R Dorey N R amp Wynne C D L 2011 Can your dog read your mind Understanding the causes of canine perspective taking Learning amp Behavior 39 4 289 302 doi 10 3758 s13420 011 0034 6 PMID 21643852 a href Template Cite journal html title Template Cite journal cite journal a CS1 maint uses authors parameter link a b Udell M A R Spencer J M Dorey N R amp Wynne C D L 2012 Human Socialized Wolves Follow Diverse Human Gestures And They May Not Be Alone International Journal of Comparative Psychology 25 2 97 117 doi 10 46867 IJCP 2012 25 02 04 a href Template Cite journal html title Template Cite journal cite journal a CS1 maint uses authors parameter link Udell M A Giglio R F Wynne C D 2008 Domestic dogs Canis familiaris use human gestures but not nonhuman tokens to find hidden food Journal of Comparative Psychology 122 1 84 93 doi 10 1037 0735 7036 122 1 84 PMID 18298285 Udell M A R 2014 10 A Dog s Eye View of Canine Cognition In A Horowitz ed Domestic Dog Cognition and Behavior Springer Verlag Berlin Heidelberg pp 221 240 doi 10 1007 978 3 642 53994 7 10 ISBN 978 3 642 53993 0 Seal U S Mech L D July 1983 Blood Indicators of Seasonal Metabolic Patterns in Captive Adult Gray Wolves The Journal of Wildlife Management 47 3 704 715 doi 10 2307 3808606 JSTOR 3808606 Cunningham D J January 1906 Cape Hunting Dogs Lycaon pictus in the Gardens of the Royal Zoological Society of Ireland Proceedings of the Royal Society of Edinburgh 25 2 843 848 doi 10 1017 S0370164600016667 Kleiman Devra G Malcom James R 1981 The Evolution of Male Parental Investment in Mammals In Gubernick David J Klopfer Peter H eds Parental Care in Mammals Plenum Press pp 347 387 doi 10 1007 978 1 4613 3150 6 9 ISBN 9780306405334 Coppinger Ray 2001 Dogs a Startling New Understanding of Canine Origin Behavior and Evolution New York Scribner ISBN 978 0 684 85530 1 Pal S K 2005 Parental care in free ranging dogs Canis familiaris Applied Animal Behaviour Science 90 31 47 doi 10 1016 j applanim 2004 08 002 Scott M Douglas Causey Keith July 1973 Ecology of Feral Dogs in Alabama The Journal of Wildlife Management 37 3 253 265 doi 10 2307 3800116 JSTOR 3800116 a b Nesbitt William H 2009 1975 Ecology of a Feral Dog Pack on a Wildlife Refuge In Fox Michael W ed Wild Canids Their Systematics Behavioral Ecology amp Evolution reprinted ed Dogwise Publishing p 391 ISBN 978 1 929242 64 1 Duarte Jesus 1 July 2016 Depredatory impact of free roaming domestic dogs on Mediterranean deer in southern Spain implications for human wolf conflict Folia Zoologica 65 2 135 141 doi 10 25225 fozo v65 i2 a8 2016 S2CID 90407348 via BioOne Green Jeffrey S September 1994 The Handbook Prevention and Control of Wildlife Damage a href Template Cite journal html title Template Cite journal cite journal a Cite journal requires journal help Wedderburn Pete 19 September 2017 Why do dogs get upset stomachs common causes and treatments The Telegraph Retrieved 28 December 2017 Why Your Dog Eats Poop and Other Odd Pet Behavior Explained 4 November 2017 Retrieved 28 December 2017 Everyone Poops Some Animals Eat It Why Smithsonian Retrieved 28 December 2017 Would Your Dog Eat You if You Died Get the Facts National Geographic 23 June 2017 Retrieved 28 December 2017 Fonberg E Kostarczyk E Prechtl J 1981 Training of Instrumental Responses in Dogs Socially Reinforced by Humans The Pavlovian Journal of Biological Science 16 4 183 193 doi 10 1007 BF03003358 PMID 7329743 S2CID 28014282 Feuerbacher E N Wynne C D L 2015 Shut up and pet me Domestic dogs Canis lupus familiaris prefer petting to vocal praise in concurrent and single alternative choice procedures Behavioural Processes 110 47 59 doi 10 1016 j beproc 2014 08 019 PMID 25173617 S2CID 13565641 McIntire Roger W Colley Thomas A June 1967 Social Reinforcement In The Dog Psychological Reports 20 3 843 846 doi 10 2466 pr0 1967 20 3 843 PMID 6042498 S2CID 33962156 Berns G S Brooks A Spivak M 2013 Brass Marcel ed Replicability and Heterogeneity of Awake Unrestrained Canine fMRI Responses PLOS ONE 8 12 e81698 Bibcode 2013PLoSO 881698B doi 10 1371 journal pone 0081698 PMC 3852264 PMID 24324719 Berns G S Brooks A M Spivak M 2015 Scent of the familiar An fMRI study of canine brain responses to familiar and unfamiliar human and dog odors Behavioural Processes 110 37 46 doi 10 1016 j beproc 2014 02 011 PMID 24607363 Topal J Miklosi A and Csanyi V 1997 Dog human relationship affects problem solving behavior in the dog Anthrozoos 10 4 214 224 doi 10 2752 089279397787000987 a href Template Cite journal html title Template Cite journal cite journal a CS1 maint multiple names authors list link Siniscalchi Marcello Stipo Carlo Quaranta Angelo 2013 Like Owner Like Dog Correlation between the Owner s Attachment Profile and the Owner Dog Bond PLOS ONE 8 10 e78455 Bibcode 2013PLoSO 878455S doi 10 1371 journal pone 0078455 PMC 3813613 PMID 24205235 Lee Mary R Olmert Meg D Yount Rick A 2012 Service Dog Training Program for Treatment of Posttraumatic Stress in Service Members PDF U S Army Medical Department Journal 63 9 PMID 22388685 Retrieved 30 September 2017 Prynne M November 6 2013 Dog attack laws and statistics The Telegraph Retrieved April 14 2015 Dog bite hospitalisations highest in deprived areas NHS Choices 2014 Retrieved April 14 2015 Sacks J J Sinclair L Gilchrist J Golab G C Lockwood R 2000 Breeds of dogs involved in fatal human attacks between 1979 and 1998 PDF JAVMA 217 6 836 840 doi 10 2460 javma 2000 217 836 PMID 10997153 S2CID 10817039 Archived from the original PDF on April 11 2015 Retrieved April 14 2015 Miklosi Adam 2007 Dog Behaviour Evolution and Cognition Oxford Oxford University Press doi 10 1093 acprof oso 9780199295852 001 0001 ISBN 9780199295852 United States Congress Senate Committee on Commerce Subcommittee on the Environment 1976 Animal Welfare Improvement Act of 1975 Hearing Before the Subcommittee on the Environment of the Committee on Commerce United States Government p 111 Nearly 10 per cent of the dogs that have bitten people have received attack dog training Further reading EditDonaldson Jean 1991 The Culture Clash James amp Kenneth Publishers Hare Brian Woods Venessa 2013 The Genius of Dogs Penguin Publishing Group Jordan Rain 2020 Such Small Hands An Anti Aversives Primer Dog s Heart Press Miklosi Adam 2007 Dog Behaviour Evolution and Cognition Oxford University Press Pet Behavior articles from the ASPCA External links Edit nbsp Wikimedia Commons has media related to Dog behavior Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Dog behavior amp oldid 1173150334 Reproduction behavior, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.