fbpx
Wikipedia

Physical intimacy

Physical intimacy is sensual proximity or touching. It is an act or reaction, such as an expression of feelings (including close friendship, platonic love, romantic love or sexual attraction), between people. Examples of physical intimacy include being inside someone's personal space, holding hands, hugging, kissing, caressing and sexual activity.[1] Physical intimacy can often convey the real meaning or intention of an interaction in a way that accompanying speech cannot do. Physical intimacy can be exchanged between any people but as it is often used to communicate positive and intimate feelings, it most often occurs in people who have a preexisting relationship, whether familial, platonic or romantic, with romantic relationships having increased physical intimacy. Several forms of romantic touch have been noted including holding hands, hugging, kissing, cuddling, as well as caressing and massaging. Physical affection is highly correlated with overall relationship and partner satisfaction.[2]

The Proposal by William-Adolphe Bouguereau (1872)

It is possible to be physically intimate with someone without actually touching them; however, a certain proximity is necessary. For instance, a sustained eye contact is considered a form of physical intimacy, analogous to touching. When a person enters someone else's personal space for the purpose of being intimate, it is physical intimacy, regardless of the lack of actual physical contact.

Most people partake in physical intimacy, which is a natural part of interpersonal relationships and human sexuality, and research has shown it has health benefits. A hug or touch can result in the release of the hormone oxytocin and in a reduction in stress hormones.[3]

Due to the important role that language-based communication plays in humans, the role of touch is often downplayed; however, there is ample evidence that physical touch still plays an important role in everyday human relationships. While humans often communicate verbally, they also participate in close contact. Physical touch has emotional and social connotations that often far outweigh anything that can be expressed via language.[4]

Inducements towards physical intimacy can come from various sources. During colder seasons, humans as well as other animals seek physical intimacy with one another as a means to apportion thermoregulation.[5] Some forms of physical touch among monkeys and apes serve multiple functions, including cleaning, treatment of a lice influx or infection and social grooming.[6]

Some forms of physical intimacy may be received negatively. This attitude is especially marked amongst those with haphephobia.[7] One study has shown that there is generally a higher level of physical intimacy allowed between immediate family members than between second-degree relatives.[8] Intimacy norms are usually more negative near erogenous zones. Some jurisdictions may specify this as referring to the genitals, buttocks and female breasts.[9]

Development

Physical affection and intimacy appear to have a profoundly important role during infancy and childhood. The skin is the largest sensory organ and is the first to develop. Humans experience touch as early as fetal development, when the fetus begins receiving sensory information from coming in contact with the mothers’ abdominal wall. In infancy, babies receive significant amounts of touch through being held, cuddled, and breastfed. In addition to necessary functions like breastfeeding, touch is also used to soothe and calm babies or with skin-to-skin contact called "kangaroo care".[10] Vision and auditory senses are limited in infancy and babies are introduced to their world primarily through touch and are able to distinguish between temperature and texture.[11]

Decreased amounts of affectionate touch from caregivers (i.e. for infants in institutional settings or infants with depressed mothers) is related to cognitive and neurodevelopmental delays.[12] These delays appear to persist for years and sometimes whole lifetimes.[13] Studies suggest that if depressed mothers give their infants massages, it benefits both the baby and themselves, increasing growth and development for the babies and leading to increased sensitivity and responsivity of the mothers.[14] There are also biologically beneficial effects of infant massage, with premature infants displaying lower cortisol levels after being held by their mothers. During the holding period, the mothers' cortisol levels also decreased.[15]

Personal space

 
Young men engage in hugging, a form of physical intimacy

Most people value their personal space and feel discomfort, anger or anxiety when somebody encroaches on their personal space without consent.[16] Entering somebody's personal space is normally an indication of familiarity and intimacy. However, in modern society, especially in crowded urban communities, it is at times difficult to maintain personal space, for example, in a crowded train, elevator or street. Many people find the physical proximity within crowded spaces to be psychologically disturbing and uncomfortable.[16] In an impersonal crowded situation, eye contact tends to be avoided. Even in a crowded place, preserving personal space is important. Non-consensual intimate and sexual contact, such as frotteurism and groping, are unacceptable.

On the other hand, most people occasionally desire physical proximity to others, and will at times welcome a familiar and trusted person into their personal space. When a partner or friend is not available at such a time, some people satisfy this need for human contact in a crowded venue, such as a bar, nightclub, rock concert, street festival, etc.

Display of affection

People who are on a familiar basis may enter into each other's personal space to make physical contact. These can be indicators of affection and trust. The manner in which people display affection is generally different in a public context to a private one. In private, people in an intimate relationship or who are familiar with each other may be at ease with physical contact and displays of affection, which may involve:

  • cuddling,
  • caressing (e.g. head, hands, arms, back and waist),
  • tickling (e.g. back and waist),
  • massage (e.g. neck, shoulders, back, thighs), or
  • touching heads.

Bonding through intimate, non-sexual contact between platonic friends and family members includes, but is not limited to, holding hands, hugging, cuddling, and kissing on the cheeks.

In public, however, and depending on the nature of the relationship between the people, a public display of affection is generally constrained by social norms and can range from a gesture, such as a kiss or hug in greeting, to an embrace or holding hands. Maintaining eye contact can be regarded socially and psychologically as analogous to touching.

Culture

The role of touch in interpersonal relationships across development and in different cultures is understudied, however, some observational data suggests that in cultures who engage in more physical intimacy have lower rates of violence, demonstrated in adolescents and children.[17] Peoples living nearer to the equator (Mediterranean, central and South America, Islamic countries) tend to have high-contact social norms, whereas countries further from the equator tend to be lower contact (northern Europe, North America, northeast Asian). The public display of interpersonal touch and intimacy appears to vary across cultures as well.[18]

The term "skinship" (スキンシップ, sukinshippu) originated as a pseudo-English Japanese word (wasei-eigo), which was coined to describe the intimacy, or closeness, between a mother and a child.[19][20][21] Today, the word is generally used for bonding through physical contact, such as holding hands, hugging, or parents washing their child at a bath. The term has been promoted by pediatrician and developmental psychologist Nobuyoshi Hirai (平井信義), and he mentioned it was taken from a term coined by an American woman at a WHO seminar held in 1953.[22] The earliest citation of this word appears in Nihon Kokugo Daijiten in 1971.[23] According to Scott Clark, author of a study of Japanese bathing culture, the word is a portmanteau combining "skin" with the last syllable of "friendship".[24] The similarity with the English word 'kinship' suggests a further explanation.[21] Use of the word "skinship" in English publications seems to focus on the notion of sharing a bath naked, an idea known in Japanese as "naked association" (裸の付き合い, hadaka no tsukiai).[citation needed] It is not clear why the meaning shifted to the parent–child relationship when borrowed back into English. This word is also used in South Korea.[25] The term is now described in Oxford English Dictionary as a part of Korea-related update in 2021.[26]

Among non-human primates

Some animals participate in behaviors similar to physical affection in humans. Called social grooming or allo-grooming, these behaviors are less common outside of primates, while other species do perform these behaviors, primates seem to spend much more time doing this compared to other animals. Some species devote as much as 20% of their day to grooming behaviors, much of which is spent grooming others, rather than themselves.[27][28] In more social species the amount of time spent in self grooming is much less than the time spent in social grooming. While these behaviors may appear to be for the purpose of hygiene (i.e. removal of parasites, fur cleanliness, etc.), evidence suggests that grooming behaviors perform a unique social function which facilitates bonding.[27] From an evolutionary perspective, the amount of time being devoted to allo-grooming appears to exceed the amount of time in which it would be adaptive, therefore underscoring the idea that grooming must have a purpose beyond hygiene maintenance. Furthermore, there are core grooming partnerships which remain quite stable and do not change frequently, sometimes with the same partners on the timescale of years.[29]

Some argue that grooming is something which is exchanged like a service with the expectation that equal amounts of time will be spent or reciprocated by their grooming partner.[30] Primates tend to groom each other equal amounts of time or with the expectation that they will be reciprocated with defense in a dangerous situation. Primates who spend more time grooming each other are more likely to defend each other when attacked. Although it is not clear how this effect is brought about, in all likelihood it is the protective effect that known relationships have: more dominant animals are less likely to attack or harass an individual who is known to have grooming partners who might come to its aid. However, the likelihood of a female going to the aid of another female when the latter is under attack is significantly correlated with the amount of time the two of them spend grooming with each other.[31] A more plausible interpretation is that grooming provides the psychological underpinning for an individual's willingness to offer subsequent support.[32] It does this not by offering a direct exchange of benefits, but rather by creating the psychological environment that allows support to be traded mutually.

See also

References

  1. ^ Cf. [1] 2007-11-18 at the Wayback Machine
  2. ^ Guéguen, Nicolas; Fischer-Lokou, Jacques (February 2003). "Another Evaluation of Touch and Helping Behavior". Psychological Reports. 92 (1): 62–64. doi:10.2466/pr0.2003.92.1.62. ISSN 0033-2941. PMID 12674258. S2CID 30706840.
  3. ^ "Human touch may have some healing properties". USA Today. 2008-09-28. Retrieved 2011-01-03.
  4. ^ Burgoon, Judee K. (1991). "Relational message interpretations of touch, conversational distance, and posture". Journal of Nonverbal Behavior. 15 (4): 233–259. doi:10.1007/bf00986924. ISSN 0191-5886. S2CID 144507275.
  5. ^ Stabentheiner, Anton, et al. "Endothermic heat production in honeybee winter clusters." Journal of Experimental Biology 206.2 (2003): 353-358.
  6. ^ Diezinger, F. T., and J. R. Anderson. "Starting from scratch: A first look at a “displacement activity” in group‐living rhesus monkeys." American Journal of Primatology 11.2 (1986): 117-124.
  7. ^ Synnott, Anthony. "Bodily senses." The International Encyclopedia of Human Sexuality (2015).
  8. ^ "The Body Map of Acceptable Social Touching - PsyBlog". Spring.org.uk. 28 October 2015. Retrieved 18 September 2017.
  9. ^ Stein, Nan. "Sexual harassment in school: The public performance of gendered violence." Harvard educational review 65.2 (1995): 145-163.
  10. ^ Ferber, Sari Goldstein; Feldman, Ruth; Makhoul, Imad R. (June 2008). "The development of maternal touch across the first year of life". Early Human Development. 84 (6): 363–370. doi:10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2007.09.019. ISSN 0378-3782. PMID 17988808.
  11. ^ Field, Tiffany; Hernandez-Reif, Maria; Diego, Miguel (2010-05-04). "Depressed mothers' newborns are less responsive to animate and inanimate stimuli". Infant and Child Development. 20 (1): 94–105. doi:10.1002/icd.687. ISSN 1522-7227.
  12. ^ MACLEAN, KIM (2003-11-14). "The impact of institutionalization on child development". Development and Psychopathology. 15 (4): 853–884. doi:10.1017/s0954579403000415. ISSN 0954-5794. PMID 14984130. S2CID 24420625.
  13. ^ Beckett, Celia; Maughan, Barbara; Rutter, Michael; Castle, Jenny; Colvert, Emma; Groothues, Christine; Kreppner, Jana; Stevens, Suzanne; O'Connor, Thomas G. (May 2006). "Do the Effects of Early Severe Deprivation on Cognition Persist Into Early Adolescence? Findings From the English and Romanian Adoptees Study". Child Development. 77 (3): 696–711. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00898.x. ISSN 0009-3920. PMID 16686796.
  14. ^ Tiffany, Field (2006). Massage therapy research. Churchill Livingstone, Elsevier. ISBN 9780443102011. OCLC 838720638.
  15. ^ Neu, M.; Laudenslager, M. L.; Robinson, J. (2008-11-17). "Coregulation in Salivary Cortisol During Maternal Holding of Premature Infants". Biological Research for Nursing. 10 (3): 226–240. doi:10.1177/1099800408327789. ISSN 1099-8004. PMID 19028768. S2CID 25062122.
  16. ^ a b Hall, Edward T. (1966). The Hidden Dimension. Anchor Books. ISBN 978-0-385-08476-5.
  17. ^ Field, Tiffany (January 1999). "Preschoolers in America are Touched Less and are More Aggressive Than Preschoolers in France". Early Child Development and Care. 151 (1): 11–17. doi:10.1080/0300443991510102. ISSN 0300-4430.
  18. ^ Field, Tiffany (2001). Touch. Cambridge, Ma: MIT Press.
  19. ^ Ivry, Tsipy (2009). Embodying Culture: Pregnancy in Japan and Israel. Rutgers University Press. p. 162. ISBN 978-0-8135-4636-0.
  20. ^ Harkness, Sara; Super, Charles M. (1996). Parents' cultural belief systems: their origins, expressions, and consequences. Guilford Press. p. 186. ISBN 978-1-57230-031-6.
  21. ^ a b Hijirida, Kyoko; Yoshikawa, Muneo (1987). Japanese language and culture for business and travel. University of Hawaii Press. p. 218. ISBN 978-0-8248-1017-7.
  22. ^ Hirai, Nobuyoshi (1986). "スキンシップ". 大日本百科全書. Vol. 12. 小学館. ISBN 4-09-526012-2.
  23. ^ "Skinship". Word Spy. 2003-02-05. Retrieved 2007-07-03.
  24. ^ Clark, Scott. Japan, a View from the Bath. Honolulu: University of Hawaii Press, 1994, p. 73. ISBN 0-8248-1615-3, ISBN 0-8248-1657-9.
  25. ^ "스킨십". NAVER Korean Dictionary. Retrieved 2020-05-26.
  26. ^ Salazar, Danica (2021-09-06). "Daebak! The OED gets a K-update". Oxford Dictionaries. Oxford University Press. Retrieved 2021-11-17.
  27. ^ a b Dunbar, R.I.M. (1991). "Functional Significance of Social Grooming in Primates". Folia Primatologica. 57 (3): 121–131. doi:10.1159/000156574. ISSN 1421-9980.
  28. ^ Lehmann, J.; Korstjens, A.H.; Dunbar, R.I.M. (December 2007). "Group size, grooming and social cohesion in primates" (PDF). Animal Behaviour. 74 (6): 1617–1629. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.10.025. ISSN 0003-3472. S2CID 14866172.
  29. ^ Dunbar, R.I.M. (February 2010). "The social role of touch in humans and primates: Behavioural function and neurobiological mechanisms". Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 34 (2): 260–268. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.07.001. ISSN 0149-7634. PMID 18662717. S2CID 30450770.
  30. ^ Noë, Ronald (2001), "Biological markets: partner choice as the driving force behind the evolution of mutualisms", Economics in Nature, Cambridge University Press, pp. 93–118, doi:10.1017/cbo9780511752421.008, ISBN 9780511752421
  31. ^ Dunbar, R. I. M. (November 1980). "Determinants and evolutionary consequences of dominance among female gelada baboons". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 7 (4): 253–265. doi:10.1007/bf00300665. ISSN 0340-5443. S2CID 28369135.
  32. ^ Dunbar, R.I.M.; Dunbar, Patsy (August 1988). "Maternal time budgets of gelada baboons". Animal Behaviour. 36 (4): 970–980. doi:10.1016/s0003-3472(88)80055-1. ISSN 0003-3472. S2CID 53203743.

External links

  • "American men are embracing The hug" - article at The Baltimore Sun
  • NPR: Poet on Call, By Andrei Codrescu commentary on hugs on NPR

physical, intimacy, general, human, touching, behavior, haptic, communication, sense, touch, somatosensory, system, caress, redirects, here, american, brand, soap, named, caress, soap, caressing, redirects, here, racehorse, caressing, horse, sensual, proximity. For general human touching behavior see Haptic communication For the sense of touch see Somatosensory system Caress redirects here For the American brand of soap named Caress see Lux soap Caressing redirects here For the racehorse see Caressing horse Physical intimacy is sensual proximity or touching It is an act or reaction such as an expression of feelings including close friendship platonic love romantic love or sexual attraction between people Examples of physical intimacy include being inside someone s personal space holding hands hugging kissing caressing and sexual activity 1 Physical intimacy can often convey the real meaning or intention of an interaction in a way that accompanying speech cannot do Physical intimacy can be exchanged between any people but as it is often used to communicate positive and intimate feelings it most often occurs in people who have a preexisting relationship whether familial platonic or romantic with romantic relationships having increased physical intimacy Several forms of romantic touch have been noted including holding hands hugging kissing cuddling as well as caressing and massaging Physical affection is highly correlated with overall relationship and partner satisfaction 2 The Proposal by William Adolphe Bouguereau 1872 It is possible to be physically intimate with someone without actually touching them however a certain proximity is necessary For instance a sustained eye contact is considered a form of physical intimacy analogous to touching When a person enters someone else s personal space for the purpose of being intimate it is physical intimacy regardless of the lack of actual physical contact Most people partake in physical intimacy which is a natural part of interpersonal relationships and human sexuality and research has shown it has health benefits A hug or touch can result in the release of the hormone oxytocin and in a reduction in stress hormones 3 Due to the important role that language based communication plays in humans the role of touch is often downplayed however there is ample evidence that physical touch still plays an important role in everyday human relationships While humans often communicate verbally they also participate in close contact Physical touch has emotional and social connotations that often far outweigh anything that can be expressed via language 4 Inducements towards physical intimacy can come from various sources During colder seasons humans as well as other animals seek physical intimacy with one another as a means to apportion thermoregulation 5 Some forms of physical touch among monkeys and apes serve multiple functions including cleaning treatment of a lice influx or infection and social grooming 6 Some forms of physical intimacy may be received negatively This attitude is especially marked amongst those with haphephobia 7 One study has shown that there is generally a higher level of physical intimacy allowed between immediate family members than between second degree relatives 8 Intimacy norms are usually more negative near erogenous zones Some jurisdictions may specify this as referring to the genitals buttocks and female breasts 9 Contents 1 Development 2 Personal space 3 Display of affection 4 Culture 5 Among non human primates 6 See also 7 References 8 External linksDevelopment EditPhysical affection and intimacy appear to have a profoundly important role during infancy and childhood The skin is the largest sensory organ and is the first to develop Humans experience touch as early as fetal development when the fetus begins receiving sensory information from coming in contact with the mothers abdominal wall In infancy babies receive significant amounts of touch through being held cuddled and breastfed In addition to necessary functions like breastfeeding touch is also used to soothe and calm babies or with skin to skin contact called kangaroo care 10 Vision and auditory senses are limited in infancy and babies are introduced to their world primarily through touch and are able to distinguish between temperature and texture 11 Decreased amounts of affectionate touch from caregivers i e for infants in institutional settings or infants with depressed mothers is related to cognitive and neurodevelopmental delays 12 These delays appear to persist for years and sometimes whole lifetimes 13 Studies suggest that if depressed mothers give their infants massages it benefits both the baby and themselves increasing growth and development for the babies and leading to increased sensitivity and responsivity of the mothers 14 There are also biologically beneficial effects of infant massage with premature infants displaying lower cortisol levels after being held by their mothers During the holding period the mothers cortisol levels also decreased 15 Personal space Edit Young men engage in hugging a form of physical intimacy Most people value their personal space and feel discomfort anger or anxiety when somebody encroaches on their personal space without consent 16 Entering somebody s personal space is normally an indication of familiarity and intimacy However in modern society especially in crowded urban communities it is at times difficult to maintain personal space for example in a crowded train elevator or street Many people find the physical proximity within crowded spaces to be psychologically disturbing and uncomfortable 16 In an impersonal crowded situation eye contact tends to be avoided Even in a crowded place preserving personal space is important Non consensual intimate and sexual contact such as frotteurism and groping are unacceptable On the other hand most people occasionally desire physical proximity to others and will at times welcome a familiar and trusted person into their personal space When a partner or friend is not available at such a time some people satisfy this need for human contact in a crowded venue such as a bar nightclub rock concert street festival etc Display of affection EditPeople who are on a familiar basis may enter into each other s personal space to make physical contact These can be indicators of affection and trust The manner in which people display affection is generally different in a public context to a private one In private people in an intimate relationship or who are familiar with each other may be at ease with physical contact and displays of affection which may involve cuddling caressing e g head hands arms back and waist tickling e g back and waist massage e g neck shoulders back thighs or touching heads Bonding through intimate non sexual contact between platonic friends and family members includes but is not limited to holding hands hugging cuddling and kissing on the cheeks In public however and depending on the nature of the relationship between the people a public display of affection is generally constrained by social norms and can range from a gesture such as a kiss or hug in greeting to an embrace or holding hands Maintaining eye contact can be regarded socially and psychologically as analogous to touching Culture EditThe role of touch in interpersonal relationships across development and in different cultures is understudied however some observational data suggests that in cultures who engage in more physical intimacy have lower rates of violence demonstrated in adolescents and children 17 Peoples living nearer to the equator Mediterranean central and South America Islamic countries tend to have high contact social norms whereas countries further from the equator tend to be lower contact northern Europe North America northeast Asian The public display of interpersonal touch and intimacy appears to vary across cultures as well 18 The term skinship スキンシップ sukinshippu originated as a pseudo English Japanese word wasei eigo which was coined to describe the intimacy or closeness between a mother and a child 19 20 21 Today the word is generally used for bonding through physical contact such as holding hands hugging or parents washing their child at a bath The term has been promoted by pediatrician and developmental psychologist Nobuyoshi Hirai 平井信義 and he mentioned it was taken from a term coined by an American woman at a WHO seminar held in 1953 22 The earliest citation of this word appears in Nihon Kokugo Daijiten in 1971 23 According to Scott Clark author of a study of Japanese bathing culture the word is a portmanteau combining skin with the last syllable of friendship 24 The similarity with the English word kinship suggests a further explanation 21 Use of the word skinship in English publications seems to focus on the notion of sharing a bath naked an idea known in Japanese as naked association 裸の付き合い hadaka no tsukiai citation needed It is not clear why the meaning shifted to the parent child relationship when borrowed back into English This word is also used in South Korea 25 The term is now described in Oxford English Dictionary as a part of Korea related update in 2021 26 Among non human primates EditSome animals participate in behaviors similar to physical affection in humans Called social grooming or allo grooming these behaviors are less common outside of primates while other species do perform these behaviors primates seem to spend much more time doing this compared to other animals Some species devote as much as 20 of their day to grooming behaviors much of which is spent grooming others rather than themselves 27 28 In more social species the amount of time spent in self grooming is much less than the time spent in social grooming While these behaviors may appear to be for the purpose of hygiene i e removal of parasites fur cleanliness etc evidence suggests that grooming behaviors perform a unique social function which facilitates bonding 27 From an evolutionary perspective the amount of time being devoted to allo grooming appears to exceed the amount of time in which it would be adaptive therefore underscoring the idea that grooming must have a purpose beyond hygiene maintenance Furthermore there are core grooming partnerships which remain quite stable and do not change frequently sometimes with the same partners on the timescale of years 29 Some argue that grooming is something which is exchanged like a service with the expectation that equal amounts of time will be spent or reciprocated by their grooming partner 30 Primates tend to groom each other equal amounts of time or with the expectation that they will be reciprocated with defense in a dangerous situation Primates who spend more time grooming each other are more likely to defend each other when attacked Although it is not clear how this effect is brought about in all likelihood it is the protective effect that known relationships have more dominant animals are less likely to attack or harass an individual who is known to have grooming partners who might come to its aid However the likelihood of a female going to the aid of another female when the latter is under attack is significantly correlated with the amount of time the two of them spend grooming with each other 31 A more plausible interpretation is that grooming provides the psychological underpinning for an individual s willingness to offer subsequent support 32 It does this not by offering a direct exchange of benefits but rather by creating the psychological environment that allows support to be traded mutually See also EditEmotional intimacy Haptic communication ConsentReferences Edit Cf 1 Archived 2007 11 18 at the Wayback Machine Gueguen Nicolas Fischer Lokou Jacques February 2003 Another Evaluation of Touch and Helping Behavior Psychological Reports 92 1 62 64 doi 10 2466 pr0 2003 92 1 62 ISSN 0033 2941 PMID 12674258 S2CID 30706840 Human touch may have some healing properties USA Today 2008 09 28 Retrieved 2011 01 03 Burgoon Judee K 1991 Relational message interpretations of touch conversational distance and posture Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 15 4 233 259 doi 10 1007 bf00986924 ISSN 0191 5886 S2CID 144507275 Stabentheiner Anton et al Endothermic heat production in honeybee winter clusters Journal of Experimental Biology 206 2 2003 353 358 Diezinger F T and J R Anderson Starting from scratch A first look at a displacement activity in group living rhesus monkeys American Journal of Primatology 11 2 1986 117 124 Synnott Anthony Bodily senses The International Encyclopedia of Human Sexuality 2015 The Body Map of Acceptable Social Touching PsyBlog Spring org uk 28 October 2015 Retrieved 18 September 2017 Stein Nan Sexual harassment in school The public performance of gendered violence Harvard educational review 65 2 1995 145 163 Ferber Sari Goldstein Feldman Ruth Makhoul Imad R June 2008 The development of maternal touch across the first year of life Early Human Development 84 6 363 370 doi 10 1016 j earlhumdev 2007 09 019 ISSN 0378 3782 PMID 17988808 Field Tiffany Hernandez Reif Maria Diego Miguel 2010 05 04 Depressed mothers newborns are less responsive to animate and inanimate stimuli Infant and Child Development 20 1 94 105 doi 10 1002 icd 687 ISSN 1522 7227 MACLEAN KIM 2003 11 14 The impact of institutionalization on child development Development and Psychopathology 15 4 853 884 doi 10 1017 s0954579403000415 ISSN 0954 5794 PMID 14984130 S2CID 24420625 Beckett Celia Maughan Barbara Rutter Michael Castle Jenny Colvert Emma Groothues Christine Kreppner Jana Stevens Suzanne O Connor Thomas G May 2006 Do the Effects of Early Severe Deprivation on Cognition Persist Into Early Adolescence Findings From the English and Romanian Adoptees Study Child Development 77 3 696 711 doi 10 1111 j 1467 8624 2006 00898 x ISSN 0009 3920 PMID 16686796 Tiffany Field 2006 Massage therapy research Churchill Livingstone Elsevier ISBN 9780443102011 OCLC 838720638 Neu M Laudenslager M L Robinson J 2008 11 17 Coregulation in Salivary Cortisol During Maternal Holding of Premature Infants Biological Research for Nursing 10 3 226 240 doi 10 1177 1099800408327789 ISSN 1099 8004 PMID 19028768 S2CID 25062122 a b Hall Edward T 1966 The Hidden Dimension Anchor Books ISBN 978 0 385 08476 5 Field Tiffany January 1999 Preschoolers in America are Touched Less and are More Aggressive Than Preschoolers in France Early Child Development and Care 151 1 11 17 doi 10 1080 0300443991510102 ISSN 0300 4430 Field Tiffany 2001 Touch Cambridge Ma MIT Press Ivry Tsipy 2009 Embodying Culture Pregnancy in Japan and Israel Rutgers University Press p 162 ISBN 978 0 8135 4636 0 Harkness Sara Super Charles M 1996 Parents cultural belief systems their origins expressions and consequences Guilford Press p 186 ISBN 978 1 57230 031 6 a b Hijirida Kyoko Yoshikawa Muneo 1987 Japanese language and culture for business and travel University of Hawaii Press p 218 ISBN 978 0 8248 1017 7 Hirai Nobuyoshi 1986 スキンシップ 大日本百科全書 Vol 12 小学館 ISBN 4 09 526012 2 Skinship Word Spy 2003 02 05 Retrieved 2007 07 03 Clark Scott Japan a View from the Bath Honolulu University of Hawaii Press 1994 p 73 ISBN 0 8248 1615 3 ISBN 0 8248 1657 9 스킨십 NAVER Korean Dictionary Retrieved 2020 05 26 Salazar Danica 2021 09 06 Daebak The OED gets a K update Oxford Dictionaries Oxford University Press Retrieved 2021 11 17 a b Dunbar R I M 1991 Functional Significance of Social Grooming in Primates Folia Primatologica 57 3 121 131 doi 10 1159 000156574 ISSN 1421 9980 Lehmann J Korstjens A H Dunbar R I M December 2007 Group size grooming and social cohesion in primates PDF Animal Behaviour 74 6 1617 1629 doi 10 1016 j anbehav 2006 10 025 ISSN 0003 3472 S2CID 14866172 Dunbar R I M February 2010 The social role of touch in humans and primates Behavioural function and neurobiological mechanisms Neuroscience amp Biobehavioral Reviews 34 2 260 268 doi 10 1016 j neubiorev 2008 07 001 ISSN 0149 7634 PMID 18662717 S2CID 30450770 Noe Ronald 2001 Biological markets partner choice as the driving force behind the evolution of mutualisms Economics in Nature Cambridge University Press pp 93 118 doi 10 1017 cbo9780511752421 008 ISBN 9780511752421 Dunbar R I M November 1980 Determinants and evolutionary consequences of dominance among female gelada baboons Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 7 4 253 265 doi 10 1007 bf00300665 ISSN 0340 5443 S2CID 28369135 Dunbar R I M Dunbar Patsy August 1988 Maternal time budgets of gelada baboons Animal Behaviour 36 4 970 980 doi 10 1016 s0003 3472 88 80055 1 ISSN 0003 3472 S2CID 53203743 External links Edit Wikimedia Commons has media related to People touching other people American men are embracing The hug article at The Baltimore Sun NPR Poet on Call By Andrei Codrescu commentary on hugs on NPR Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Physical intimacy amp oldid 1122082960, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.