fbpx
Wikipedia

Paralanguage

Paralanguage, also known as vocalics, is a component of meta-communication that may modify meaning, give nuanced meaning, or convey emotion, by using techniques such as prosody, pitch, volume, intonation, etc. It is sometimes defined as relating to nonphonemic properties only. Paralanguage may be expressed consciously or unconsciously.

The study of paralanguage is known as paralinguistics and was invented by George L. Trager in the 1950s, while he was working at the Foreign Service Institute of the U.S. Department of State. His colleagues at the time included Henry Lee Smith, Charles F. Hockett (working with him on using descriptive linguistics as a model for paralanguage), Edward T. Hall developing proxemics, and Ray Birdwhistell developing kinesics.[1] Trager published his conclusions in 1958,[2] 1960[3] and 1961.[4]

His work has served as a basis for all later research, especially those investigating the relationship between paralanguage and culture (since paralanguage is learned, it differs by language and culture). A good example is the work of John J. Gumperz on language and social identity, which specifically describes paralinguistic differences between participants in intercultural interactions.[5] The film Gumperz made for BBC in 1982, Multiracial Britain: Cross talk, does a particularly good job of demonstrating cultural differences in paralanguage and their impact on relationships.

Paralinguistic information, because it is phenomenal, belongs to the external speech signal (Ferdinand de Saussure's parole) but not to the arbitrary conmodality. Even vocal language has some paralinguistic as well as linguistic properties that can be seen (lip reading, McGurk effect), and even felt, e.g. by the Tadoma method.

Aspects of the speech signal edit

Perspectival aspects edit

Speech signals arrive at a listener's ears with acoustic properties that may allow listeners to identify location of the speaker (sensing distance and direction, for example). Sound localization functions in a similar way also for non-speech sounds. The perspectival aspects of lip reading are more obvious and have more drastic effects when head turning is involved.

Organic aspects edit

The speech organs of different speakers differ in size. As children grow up, their organs of speech become larger, and there are differences between male and female adults. The differences concern not only size, but also proportions. They affect the pitch of the voice and to a substantial extent also the formant frequencies, which characterize the different speech sounds. The organic quality of speech has a communicative function in a restricted sense, since it is merely informative about the speaker. It will be expressed independently of the speaker's intention.

Expressive aspects edit

Paralinguistic cues such as loudness, rate, pitch, pitch contour, and to some extent formant frequencies of an utterance, contribute to the emotive or attitudinal quality of an utterance. Typically, attitudes are expressed intentionally and emotions without intention,[citation needed] but attempts to fake or to hide emotions are not unusual.[citation needed]

Consequently, paralinguistic cues relating to expression have a moderate effect of semantic marking. That is, a message may be made more or less coherent by adjusting its expressive presentation. For instance, upon hearing an utterance such as "I drink a glass of wine every night before I go to sleep" is coherent when made by a speaker identified as an adult, but registers a small semantic anomaly when made by a speaker identified as a child.[6] This anomaly is significant enough to be measured through electroencephalography, as an N400. Autistic individuals have a reduced sensitivity to this and similar effects.[7][failed verification]

Emotional tone of voice, itself paralinguistic information, has been shown to affect the resolution of lexical ambiguity. Some words have homophonous partners; some of these homophones appear to have an implicit emotive quality, for instance, the sad "die" contrasted with the neutral "dye"; uttering the sound /dai/ in a sad tone of voice can result in a listener writing the former word significantly more often than if the word is uttered in a neutral tone.[8]

Linguistic aspects edit

Ordinary phonetic transcriptions of utterances reflect only the linguistically informative quality. The problem of how listeners factor out the linguistically informative quality from speech signals is a topic of current research.

Some of the linguistic features of speech, in particular of its prosody, are paralinguistic or pre-linguistic in origin. A most fundamental and widespread phenomenon of this kind is described by John Ohala as the "frequency code".[9] This code works even in communication across species. It has its origin in the fact that the acoustic frequencies in the voice of small vocalizers are high, while they are low in the voice of large vocalizers. This gives rise to secondary meanings such as "harmless", "submissive", "unassertive", which are naturally associated with smallness, while meanings such as "dangerous", "dominant", and "assertive" are associated with largeness. In most languages, the frequency code also serves the purpose of distinguishing questions from statements. It is universally reflected in expressive variation, and it is reasonable to assume that it has phylogenetically given rise to the sexual dimorphism that lies behind the large difference in pitch between average female and male adults.

In text-only communication such as email, chatrooms and instant messaging, paralinguistic elements can be displayed by emoticons, font and color choices, capitalization and the use of non-alphabetic or abstract characters. Nonetheless, paralanguage in written communication is limited in comparison with face-to-face conversation, sometimes leading to misunderstandings.

Specific forms of paralinguistic respiration edit

Gasps edit

A gasp is a kind of paralinguistic respiration in the form of a sudden and sharp inhalation of air through the mouth. A gasp may indicate difficulty breathing and a panicked effort to draw air into the lungs. Gasps also occur from an emotion of surprise, shock or disgust. Like a sigh, a yawn, or a moan, a gasp is often an automatic and unintentional act.[10] Gasping is closely related to sighing, and the inhalation characterizing a gasp induced by shock or surprise may be released as a sigh if the event causing the initial emotional reaction is determined to be less shocking or surprising than the observer first believed.[11]

As a symptom of physiological problems, apneustic respirations (a.k.a. apneusis), are gasps related to the brain damage associated with a stroke or other trauma.

Sighs edit

A sigh is a kind of paralinguistic respiration in the form of a deep and especially audible, single exhalation of air out of the mouth or nose, that humans use to communicate emotion. It is a voiced pharyngeal fricative, sometimes associated with a guttural glottal breath exuded in a low tone. It often arises from a negative emotion, such as dismay, dissatisfaction, boredom, or futility.[10] A sigh can also arise from positive emotions such as relief,[12] particularly in response to some negative situation ending or being avoided. Like a gasp, a yawn, or a moan, a sigh is often an automatic and unintentional act.[10] In literature, a sigh is often used to signify that the person producing it is lovelorn.

Scientific studies show that babies sigh after 50 to 100 breaths. This serves to improve the mechanical properties of lung tissue, and it also helps babies to develop a regular breathing rhythm. Behaviors equivalent to sighing have also been observed in animals such as dogs, monkeys, and horses.

In text messages and internet chat rooms, or in comic books, a sigh is usually represented with the word itself, 'sigh', possibly within asterisks, *sigh*.

Sighing is also a reflex, governed by a few neurons.[13]

Moans and groans edit

Moaning and groaning both refer to an extended sound emanating from the throat, which is typically made by engaging in sexual activity.[14][15] Moans and groans are also noises traditionally associated with ghosts, and their supposed experience of suffering in the afterlife. They are sometimes used to indicate displeasure.

Throat clearing edit

Throat clearing is a metamessaging nonverbal form of communication[16] used in announcing one's presence upon entering the room or approaching a group. It is done by individuals who perceive themselves to be of higher rank than the group they are approaching and utilize the throat-clear as a form of communicating this perception to others.[17] It can convey nonverbalized disapproval.[18][19]

In chimpanzee social hierarchy, this utterance is a sign of rank, directed by alpha males and higher-ranking chimps to lower-ranking ones and signals a mild warning or a slight annoyance.[20]

As a form of metacommunication, the throat-clear is acceptable only to signal that a formal business meeting is about to start. It is not acceptable business etiquette to clear one's throat when approaching a group on an informal basis; the basis of one's authority has already been established and requires no further reiteration by this ancillary nonverbal communication.[21]

Mhm edit

Mhm is between a literal language and movement, by making a noise "hmm" or "mhm", to make a pause for the conversation or as a chance to stop and think.

The "mhm" utterance is often used in narrative interviews, such as an interview with a disaster survivor or sexual violence victim. In this kind of interview, it is better for the interviewers or counselors not to intervene too much when an interviewee is talking. The "mhm" assures the interviewee that they are being heard and can continue their story. Observing emotional differences and taking care of an interviewee's mental status is an important way to find slight changes during conversation.[22]

Huh? edit

"Huh?", meaning "what?" (that is, used when an utterance by another is not fully heard or requires clarification), is an essentially universal expression, but may be a normal word (learned like other words) and not paralanguage. If it is a word, it is a rare (or possibly even unique) one, being found with basically the same sound and meaning in almost all languages.[23][better source needed][24]

Physiology of paralinguistic comprehension edit

fMRI studies edit

Several studies have used the fMRI paradigm to observe brain states brought about by adjustments of paralinguistic information. One such study investigated the effect of interjections that differed along the criteria of lexical index (more or less "wordy") as well as neutral or emotional pronunciation; a higher hemodynamic response in auditory cortical gyri was found when more robust paralinguistic data was available. Some activation was found in lower brain structures such as the pons, perhaps indicating an emotional response.[25]

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ Leeds-Hurwitz, W. (1990). Notes in the history of intercultural communication: The Foreign Service Institute and the mandate for intercultural training. Quarterly Journal of Speech, 76, 262–281.
  2. ^ Trager, G. L. (1958). Paralanguage: A first approximation. Studies in Linguistics, 13, 1–12.
  3. ^ Trager, G. L. (1960). Taos III: Paralanguage. Anthropological Linguistics, 2, 24–30.
  4. ^ Trager, G. L. (1961). The typology of paralanguage. Anthropological Linguistics, 3 (1), 17–21.
  5. ^ Gumperz, J. J. (1982). Discourse strategies. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  6. ^ Van Berkum, J. J., Van den Brink, D., Tesink, C. M., Kos, M., & Hagoort, P. (2008). The neural integration of speaker and message. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 580–591.
  7. ^ Groen, W. B., Tesink, C., Petersson, K. M., Van Berkum, J., Van der Gaag, R. J., Hagoort, P. and Buitelaar, J. K. (2010). Semantic, factual, and social language comprehension in adolescents with autism: an fMRI study. Cerebral Cortex, 20(8), 1937–1945.
  8. ^ Nygaard, L. C., Lunders, E. R. (2002). Resolution of lexical ambiguity by emotional tone of voice. Memory & Cognition, 30(4), 583–593.
  9. ^ Ohala, J. J. (1984) An ethological perspective on common cross-language utilization of F0 of voice. Phonetica, 41, 1–16. doi:10.1159/000261706.
  10. ^ a b c Rachel Broncher, A labor of love: a complete guide to childbirth for the mind, body, and soul (2004), p. 145.
  11. ^ Fernando Poyatos, Paralanguage: a linguistic and interdisciplinary approach to interactive speech and sounds (1993), page 330.
  12. ^ Paul Ekman, Emotions revealed: recognizing faces and feelings to improve communication (2007), p. 193.
  13. ^ Schmidt, Elaine. "UCLA and Stanford researchers pinpoint origin of sighing reflex in the brain". UCLA Newsroom. Retrieved 2019-02-01.
  14. ^ Altman, Mara (August 21, 2018). "What Our Sex Sounds Say About Us". Time.
  15. ^ Ben-Zeév, Aaron (April 16, 2019). "Why We Moan and Scream During Sex". Psychology Today.
  16. ^ . www.nonverbal-dictionary.org. Archived from the original on 2016-09-17. Retrieved 2016-09-10.
  17. ^ Question Authority: Think for Yourself, Estren & Potter, 2012:122.
  18. ^ "ahem – Onomatopoeia List". 10 August 2013.
  19. ^ "the definition of verbal". Dictionary.com.
  20. ^ Primatology, Delmar 1984:46.
  21. ^ Book of Etiquette and Manners, Chapter 3, by Nimeran Sahukar & Prem P. Bhalla.
  22. ^ The healing effect of storytelling, Gabriele Rosenthal 2003
  23. ^ Dingemanse, Marse; Torreira, Francisco; Enfield, N. J. (November 8, 2013). "Is 'Huh?' a Universal Word? Conversational Infrastructure and the Convergent Evolution of Linguistic Items". PLOS ONE. 8 (11). PLOS. Bibcode:2013PLoSO...878273D. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0078273. PMC 3832628. PMID 24260108.
  24. ^ Olga Khazan (November 12, 2013). "Huh Means the Same Thing in Every Language". The Atlantic. Retrieved December 29, 2021.
  25. ^ Dietrich, S., Hertrich, I., Kai, A., Ischebeck, A., Ackermann, H. (2008). Understanding the emotional expression of verbal interjections: a functional MRI study. Brain Imaging, 19(18), 1751–1755.

Further reading edit

  • Cook, Guy (2001) The Discourse of Advertising. (second edition) London: Routledge. (chapter 4 on paralanguage and semiotics).
  • Robbins, S. and Langton, N. (2001) Organizational Behaviour: Concepts, Controversies, Applications (2nd Canadian ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
  • Traunmüller, H. (2005) "Paralinguale Phänomene" (Paralinguistic phenomena), chapter 76 in: SOCIOLINGUISTICS An International Handbook of the Science of Language and Society, 2nd ed., U. Ammon, N. Dittmar, K. Mattheier, P. Trudgill (eds.), Vol. 1, pp. 653–665. Walter de Gruyter, Berlin/New York.
  • Matthew McKay, Martha Davis, Patrick Fanning [1983] (1995) Messages: The Communication Skills Book, Second Edition, New Harbinger Publications, ISBN 1-57224-592-1, ISBN 978-1-57224-592-1, pp. 63–67.

paralanguage, also, known, vocalics, component, meta, communication, that, modify, meaning, give, nuanced, meaning, convey, emotion, using, techniques, such, prosody, pitch, volume, intonation, sometimes, defined, relating, nonphonemic, properties, only, expre. Paralanguage also known as vocalics is a component of meta communication that may modify meaning give nuanced meaning or convey emotion by using techniques such as prosody pitch volume intonation etc It is sometimes defined as relating to nonphonemic properties only Paralanguage may be expressed consciously or unconsciously The study of paralanguage is known as paralinguistics and was invented by George L Trager in the 1950s while he was working at the Foreign Service Institute of the U S Department of State His colleagues at the time included Henry Lee Smith Charles F Hockett working with him on using descriptive linguistics as a model for paralanguage Edward T Hall developing proxemics and Ray Birdwhistell developing kinesics 1 Trager published his conclusions in 1958 2 1960 3 and 1961 4 His work has served as a basis for all later research especially those investigating the relationship between paralanguage and culture since paralanguage is learned it differs by language and culture A good example is the work of John J Gumperz on language and social identity which specifically describes paralinguistic differences between participants in intercultural interactions 5 The film Gumperz made for BBC in 1982 Multiracial Britain Cross talk does a particularly good job of demonstrating cultural differences in paralanguage and their impact on relationships Paralinguistic information because it is phenomenal belongs to the external speech signal Ferdinand de Saussure s parole but not to the arbitrary conmodality Even vocal language has some paralinguistic as well as linguistic properties that can be seen lip reading McGurk effect and even felt e g by the Tadoma method Contents 1 Aspects of the speech signal 1 1 Perspectival aspects 1 2 Organic aspects 1 3 Expressive aspects 1 4 Linguistic aspects 2 Specific forms of paralinguistic respiration 2 1 Gasps 2 2 Sighs 2 3 Moans and groans 2 4 Throat clearing 2 5 Mhm 2 6 Huh 3 Physiology of paralinguistic comprehension 3 1 fMRI studies 4 See also 5 References 6 Further readingAspects of the speech signal editPerspectival aspects edit Speech signals arrive at a listener s ears with acoustic properties that may allow listeners to identify location of the speaker sensing distance and direction for example Sound localization functions in a similar way also for non speech sounds The perspectival aspects of lip reading are more obvious and have more drastic effects when head turning is involved Organic aspects edit The speech organs of different speakers differ in size As children grow up their organs of speech become larger and there are differences between male and female adults The differences concern not only size but also proportions They affect the pitch of the voice and to a substantial extent also the formant frequencies which characterize the different speech sounds The organic quality of speech has a communicative function in a restricted sense since it is merely informative about the speaker It will be expressed independently of the speaker s intention Expressive aspects edit Paralinguistic cues such as loudness rate pitch pitch contour and to some extent formant frequencies of an utterance contribute to the emotive or attitudinal quality of an utterance Typically attitudes are expressed intentionally and emotions without intention citation needed but attempts to fake or to hide emotions are not unusual citation needed Consequently paralinguistic cues relating to expression have a moderate effect of semantic marking That is a message may be made more or less coherent by adjusting its expressive presentation For instance upon hearing an utterance such as I drink a glass of wine every night before I go to sleep is coherent when made by a speaker identified as an adult but registers a small semantic anomaly when made by a speaker identified as a child 6 This anomaly is significant enough to be measured through electroencephalography as an N400 Autistic individuals have a reduced sensitivity to this and similar effects 7 failed verification Emotional tone of voice itself paralinguistic information has been shown to affect the resolution of lexical ambiguity Some words have homophonous partners some of these homophones appear to have an implicit emotive quality for instance the sad die contrasted with the neutral dye uttering the sound dai in a sad tone of voice can result in a listener writing the former word significantly more often than if the word is uttered in a neutral tone 8 Linguistic aspects edit Ordinary phonetic transcriptions of utterances reflect only the linguistically informative quality The problem of how listeners factor out the linguistically informative quality from speech signals is a topic of current research Some of the linguistic features of speech in particular of its prosody are paralinguistic or pre linguistic in origin A most fundamental and widespread phenomenon of this kind is described by John Ohala as the frequency code 9 This code works even in communication across species It has its origin in the fact that the acoustic frequencies in the voice of small vocalizers are high while they are low in the voice of large vocalizers This gives rise to secondary meanings such as harmless submissive unassertive which are naturally associated with smallness while meanings such as dangerous dominant and assertive are associated with largeness In most languages the frequency code also serves the purpose of distinguishing questions from statements It is universally reflected in expressive variation and it is reasonable to assume that it has phylogenetically given rise to the sexual dimorphism that lies behind the large difference in pitch between average female and male adults In text only communication such as email chatrooms and instant messaging paralinguistic elements can be displayed by emoticons font and color choices capitalization and the use of non alphabetic or abstract characters Nonetheless paralanguage in written communication is limited in comparison with face to face conversation sometimes leading to misunderstandings Specific forms of paralinguistic respiration editGasps edit Gasp redirects here For other uses see Gasp disambiguation A gasp is a kind of paralinguistic respiration in the form of a sudden and sharp inhalation of air through the mouth A gasp may indicate difficulty breathing and a panicked effort to draw air into the lungs Gasps also occur from an emotion of surprise shock or disgust Like a sigh a yawn or a moan a gasp is often an automatic and unintentional act 10 Gasping is closely related to sighing and the inhalation characterizing a gasp induced by shock or surprise may be released as a sigh if the event causing the initial emotional reaction is determined to be less shocking or surprising than the observer first believed 11 As a symptom of physiological problems apneustic respirations a k a apneusis are gasps related to the brain damage associated with a stroke or other trauma Sighs edit Sigh redirects here For other uses see Sigh disambiguation A sigh is a kind of paralinguistic respiration in the form of a deep and especially audible single exhalation of air out of the mouth or nose that humans use to communicate emotion It is a voiced pharyngeal fricative sometimes associated with a guttural glottal breath exuded in a low tone It often arises from a negative emotion such as dismay dissatisfaction boredom or futility 10 A sigh can also arise from positive emotions such as relief 12 particularly in response to some negative situation ending or being avoided Like a gasp a yawn or a moan a sigh is often an automatic and unintentional act 10 In literature a sigh is often used to signify that the person producing it is lovelorn Scientific studies show that babies sigh after 50 to 100 breaths This serves to improve the mechanical properties of lung tissue and it also helps babies to develop a regular breathing rhythm Behaviors equivalent to sighing have also been observed in animals such as dogs monkeys and horses In text messages and internet chat rooms or in comic books a sigh is usually represented with the word itself sigh possibly within asterisks sigh Sighing is also a reflex governed by a few neurons 13 Moans and groans edit Moan and Groan redirect here For other uses see Moan disambiguation and Groan disambiguation Moaning and groaning both refer to an extended sound emanating from the throat which is typically made by engaging in sexual activity 14 15 Moans and groans are also noises traditionally associated with ghosts and their supposed experience of suffering in the afterlife They are sometimes used to indicate displeasure Throat clearing edit Throat clearing is a metamessaging nonverbal form of communication 16 used in announcing one s presence upon entering the room or approaching a group It is done by individuals who perceive themselves to be of higher rank than the group they are approaching and utilize the throat clear as a form of communicating this perception to others 17 It can convey nonverbalized disapproval 18 19 In chimpanzee social hierarchy this utterance is a sign of rank directed by alpha males and higher ranking chimps to lower ranking ones and signals a mild warning or a slight annoyance 20 As a form of metacommunication the throat clear is acceptable only to signal that a formal business meeting is about to start It is not acceptable business etiquette to clear one s throat when approaching a group on an informal basis the basis of one s authority has already been established and requires no further reiteration by this ancillary nonverbal communication 21 Mhm edit Mhm is between a literal language and movement by making a noise hmm or mhm to make a pause for the conversation or as a chance to stop and think The mhm utterance is often used in narrative interviews such as an interview with a disaster survivor or sexual violence victim In this kind of interview it is better for the interviewers or counselors not to intervene too much when an interviewee is talking The mhm assures the interviewee that they are being heard and can continue their story Observing emotional differences and taking care of an interviewee s mental status is an important way to find slight changes during conversation 22 Huh edit Huh meaning what that is used when an utterance by another is not fully heard or requires clarification is an essentially universal expression but may be a normal word learned like other words and not paralanguage If it is a word it is a rare or possibly even unique one being found with basically the same sound and meaning in almost all languages 23 better source needed 24 Physiology of paralinguistic comprehension editfMRI studies edit Several studies have used the fMRI paradigm to observe brain states brought about by adjustments of paralinguistic information One such study investigated the effect of interjections that differed along the criteria of lexical index more or less wordy as well as neutral or emotional pronunciation a higher hemodynamic response in auditory cortical gyri was found when more robust paralinguistic data was available Some activation was found in lower brain structures such as the pons perhaps indicating an emotional response 25 See also editBusiness communication Intercultural competence Kinesics Meta message Meta communication Metacommunicative competence Prosody linguistics ProxemicsReferences edit Leeds Hurwitz W 1990 Notes in the history of intercultural communication The Foreign Service Institute and the mandate for intercultural training Quarterly Journal of Speech 76 262 281 Trager G L 1958 Paralanguage A first approximation Studies in Linguistics 13 1 12 Trager G L 1960 Taos III Paralanguage Anthropological Linguistics 2 24 30 Trager G L 1961 The typology of paralanguage Anthropological Linguistics 3 1 17 21 Gumperz J J 1982 Discourse strategies Cambridge Cambridge University Press Van Berkum J J Van den Brink D Tesink C M Kos M amp Hagoort P 2008 The neural integration of speaker and message Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 20 580 591 Groen W B Tesink C Petersson K M Van Berkum J Van der Gaag R J Hagoort P and Buitelaar J K 2010 Semantic factual and social language comprehension in adolescents with autism an fMRI study Cerebral Cortex 20 8 1937 1945 Nygaard L C Lunders E R 2002 Resolution of lexical ambiguity by emotional tone of voice Memory amp Cognition 30 4 583 593 Ohala J J 1984 An ethological perspective on common cross language utilization of F0 of voice Phonetica 41 1 16 doi 10 1159 000261706 a b c Rachel Broncher A labor of love a complete guide to childbirth for the mind body and soul 2004 p 145 Fernando Poyatos Paralanguage a linguistic and interdisciplinary approach to interactive speech and sounds 1993 page 330 Paul Ekman Emotions revealed recognizing faces and feelings to improve communication 2007 p 193 Schmidt Elaine UCLA and Stanford researchers pinpoint origin of sighing reflex in the brain UCLA Newsroom Retrieved 2019 02 01 Altman Mara August 21 2018 What Our Sex Sounds Say About Us Time Ben Zeev Aaron April 16 2019 Why We Moan and Scream During Sex Psychology Today Throat Clear www nonverbal dictionary org Archived from the original on 2016 09 17 Retrieved 2016 09 10 Question Authority Think for Yourself Estren amp Potter 2012 122 ahem Onomatopoeia List 10 August 2013 the definition of verbal Dictionary com Primatology Delmar 1984 46 Book of Etiquette and Manners Chapter 3 by Nimeran Sahukar amp Prem P Bhalla The healing effect of storytelling Gabriele Rosenthal 2003 Dingemanse Marse Torreira Francisco Enfield N J November 8 2013 Is Huh a Universal Word Conversational Infrastructure and the Convergent Evolution of Linguistic Items PLOS ONE 8 11 PLOS Bibcode 2013PLoSO 878273D doi 10 1371 journal pone 0078273 PMC 3832628 PMID 24260108 Olga Khazan November 12 2013 Huh Means the Same Thing in Every Language The Atlantic Retrieved December 29 2021 Dietrich S Hertrich I Kai A Ischebeck A Ackermann H 2008 Understanding the emotional expression of verbal interjections a functional MRI study Brain Imaging 19 18 1751 1755 Further reading editCook Guy 2001 The Discourse of Advertising second edition London Routledge chapter 4 on paralanguage and semiotics Robbins S and Langton N 2001 Organizational Behaviour Concepts Controversies Applications 2nd Canadian ed Upper Saddle River NJ Prentice Hall Traunmuller H 2005 Paralinguale Phanomene Paralinguistic phenomena chapter 76 in SOCIOLINGUISTICS An International Handbook of the Science of Language and Society 2nd ed U Ammon N Dittmar K Mattheier P Trudgill eds Vol 1 pp 653 665 Walter de Gruyter Berlin New York Matthew McKay Martha Davis Patrick Fanning 1983 1995 Messages The Communication Skills Book Second Edition New Harbinger Publications ISBN 1 57224 592 1 ISBN 978 1 57224 592 1 pp 63 67 Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Paralanguage amp oldid 1218523849, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.