fbpx
Wikipedia

Music and emotion

Research into music and emotion seeks to understand the psychological relationship between human affect and music. The field, a branch of music psychology, covers numerous areas of study, including the nature of emotional reactions to music, how characteristics of the listener may determine which emotions are felt, and which components of a musical composition or performance may elicit certain reactions.

Simon Vouet, Saint Cecilia, c. 1626

The research draws upon, and has significant implications for, such areas as philosophy, musicology, music therapy, music theory, and aesthetics, as well as the acts of musical composition and of musical performance like a concert.

Philosophical approaches edit

Appearance emotionalism edit

Two of the most influential philosophers in the aesthetics of music are Stephen Davies and Jerrold Levinson.[1][2] Davies calls his view of the expressiveness of emotions in music "appearance emotionalism", which holds that music expresses emotion without feeling it. Objects can convey emotion because their structures can contain certain characteristics that resemble emotional expression. He says, "The resemblance that counts most for music's expressiveness ... is between music's temporally unfolding dynamic structure and configurations of human behaviour associated with the expression of emotion."[3] The observer can note emotions from the listener's posture, gait, gestures, attitude, and comportment.[4]

Associations between musical features and emotion differ among individuals. Appearance emotionalism claims many listeners' perceiving associations constitutes the expressiveness of music. Which musical features are more commonly associated with which emotions is part of music psychology. Davies says that expressiveness is an objective property of music and not subjective in the sense of being projected into the music by the listener. Music's expressiveness is certainly response-dependent, i.e. it is realized in the listener's judgement. Skilled listeners very similarly attribute emotional expressiveness to a certain piece of music, thereby indicating according to Davies that the expressiveness of music is somewhat objective because if the music lacked expressiveness, then no expression could be projected into it as a reaction to the music.[5]

Process theory edit

The philosopher Jennifer Robinson assumes the existence of a mutual dependence between cognition and elicitation in her description of "emotions as process, music as process" theory, or process theory. Robinson argues that the process of emotional elicitation begins with an "automatic, immediate response that initiates motor and autonomic activity and prepares us for possible action" causing a process of cognition that may enable listeners to name the felt emotion. This series of events continually exchanges with new, incoming information. Robinson argues that emotions may transform into one another, causing blends, conflicts, and ambiguities that make impede describing with one word the emotional state that one experiences at any given moment; instead, inner feelings are better thought of as the products of multiple emotional streams. Robinson argues that music is a series of simultaneous processes, and that it therefore is an ideal medium for mirroring such more cognitive aspects of emotion as musical themes' desiring resolution or leitmotif's mirrors memory processes. These simultaneous musical processes can reinforce or conflict with each other and thus also express the way one emotion "morphs into another over time".[6][page needed]

Conveying emotion through music edit

The ability to perceive emotion in music is said[weasel words] to develop early in childhood, and improve significantly throughout development.[7] The capacity to perceive emotion in music is also subject to cultural influences, and both similarities and differences in emotion perception have been observed in cross-cultural studies.[8][9] Empirical research has looked at which emotions can be conveyed as well as what structural factors in music help contribute to the perceived emotional expression. There are two schools of thought on how we interpret emotion in music. The cognitivists' approach argues that music simply displays an emotion, but does not allow for the personal experience of emotion in the listener. Emotivists argue that music elicits real emotional responses in the listener.[10][11]

It has been argued that the emotion experienced from a piece of music is a multiplicative function of structural features, performance features, listener features, contextual features and extra-musical features of the piece, shown as:

Experienced Emotion = Structural features × Performance features × Listener features × Contextual features × Extra-Musical features

where:

Structural features = Segmental features × Suprasegmental features
Performance features = Performer skill × Performer state
Listener features = Musical expertise × Stable disposition × Current motivation
Contextual features = Location × Event[10]
Extra-musical features = Non-auditory features × Expertise[12]

Structural features edit

Structural features are divided into two parts, segmental features and suprasegmental features. Segmental features are the individual sounds or tones that make up the music; this includes acoustic structures such as duration, amplitude, and pitch. Suprasegmental features are the foundational structures of a piece, such as melody, tempo and rhythm.[10] There are a number of specific musical features that are highly associated with particular emotions.[13] Within the factors affecting emotional expression in music, tempo is typically regarded as the most important, but a number of other factors, such as mode, loudness, and melody, also influence the emotional valence of the piece.[13]

Structural Feature Definition Associated Emotions
Tempo The speed or pace of a musical piece Fast tempo: excitement, anger. Slow tempo: sadness, serenity.
Mode The type of scale Major tonality: happiness, joy. Minor tonality: sadness.
Loudness The physical strength and amplitude of a sound Intensity, power, or anger
Melody The linear succession of musical tones that the listener perceives as a single entity Complementing harmonies: happiness, relaxation, serenity. Clashing harmonies: excitement, anger, unpleasantness.
Rhythm The regularly recurring pattern or beat of a song Smooth/consistent rhythm: happiness, peace. Rough/irregular rhythm: amusement, uneasiness. Varied rhythm: joy.

Some studies find that perception of basic emotional features are a cultural universal, though people can more easily perceive emotion, and perceive more nuanced emotion, in music from their own culture.[14][15][16][17] Music without lyrics is unlikely to elicit social emotions like anger, shame, and jealousy; it typically only elicits basic emotions, like happiness and sadness.[18]

Music has a direct connection to emotional states present in human beings. Different musical structures have been found to have a relationship with physiological responses. Research has shown that suprasegmental structures such as tonal space, specifically dissonance, create unpleasant negative emotions in participants. The emotional responses were measured with physiological assessments, such as skin conductance and electromyographic signals (EMG), while participants listened to musical excerpts.[19] Further research on psychophysiological measures pertaining to music were conducted and found similar results; musical structures of rhythmic articulation, accentuation, and tempo were found to correlate strongly with physiological measures, the measured used here included heart rate and respiratory monitors that correlated with self-report questionnaires.[20]

These associations can be innate, learned, or both. Studies on young children and isolated cultures show innate associations for features are similar to a human voice (e.g. low and slow is sad, faster and high is happy). Cross-cultural studies show that associations between major mode vs. minor mode and consonance vs. dissonance are probably learned.[21][22]

Music also affects socially-relevant memories, specifically memories produced by nostalgic musical excerpts (e.g., music from a significant time period in one’s life, like music listened to on road trips). Musical structures are more strongly interpreted in certain areas of the brain when the music evokes nostalgia. The interior frontal gyrus, substantia nigra, cerebellum, and insula were all identified to have a stronger correlation with nostalgic music than not.[23] Brain activity is a very individualized concept with many of the musical excerpts having certain effects based on individuals’ past life experiences, thus this caveat should be kept in mind when generalizing findings across individuals.

Performance features edit

Performance features refer to the manner in which a piece of music is executed by the performer(s). These are broken into two categories: performer skills, and performer state. Performer skills are the compound ability and appearance of the performer; including physical appearance, reputation, and technical skills. The performer state is the interpretation, motivation, and stage presence of the performer.[10]

Listener features edit

Listener features refer to the individual and social identity of the listener(s). This includes their personality, age, knowledge of music, and motivation to listen to the music.[10]

Contextual features edit

Contextual features are aspects of the performance such as the location and the particular occasion for the performance (i.e., funeral, wedding, dance).[10]

Extra-musical features edit

Extra-musical features refer to extra-musical information detached from auditory music signals, such as the genre or style of music. [12]

These different factors influence expressed emotion at different magnitudes, and their effects are compounded by one another. Thus, experienced emotion is felt to a stronger degree if more factors are present. The order the factors are listed within the model denotes how much weight in the equation they carry. For this reason, the bulk of research has been done in structural features and listener features.[10]

Conflicting cues edit

Which emotion is perceived is dependent on the context of the piece of music. Past research has argued that opposing emotions like happiness and sadness fall on a bipolar scale, where both cannot be felt at the same time.[24] More recent research has suggested that happiness and sadness are experienced separately, which implies that they can be felt concurrently.[24] One study investigated the latter possibility by having participants listen to computer-manipulated musical excerpts that have mixed cues between tempo and mode.[24] Examples of mix-cue music include a piece with major key and slow tempo, and a minor-chord piece with a fast tempo. Participants then rated the extent to which the piece conveyed happiness or sadness. The results indicated that mixed-cue music conveys both happiness and sadness; however, it remained unclear whether participants perceived happiness and sadness simultaneously or vacillated between these two emotions.[24] A follow-up study was done to examine these possibilities. While listening to mixed or consistent cue music, participants pressed one button when the music conveyed happiness, and another button when it conveyed sadness.[25] The results revealed that subjects pressed both buttons simultaneously during songs with conflicting cues.[25] These findings indicate that listeners can perceive both happiness and sadness concurrently. This has significant implications for how the structural features influence emotion, because when a mix of structural cues is used, a number of emotions may be conveyed.[25]

Specific listener features edit

Development edit

Studies indicate that the ability to understand emotional messages in music starts early, and improves throughout child development.[7][13][26] Studies investigating music and emotion in children primarily play a musical excerpt for children and have them look at pictorial expressions of faces. These facial expressions display different emotions and children are asked to select the face that best matches the music's emotional tone.[27][28][29] Studies have shown that children are able to assign specific emotions to pieces of music; however, there is debate regarding the age at which this ability begins.[7][13][26]

Infants edit

An infant is often exposed to a mother's speech that is musical in nature. It is possible that the motherly singing allows the mother to relay emotional messages to the infant.[30] Infants also tend to prefer positive speech to neutral speech as well as happy music to negative music.[27][30] It has also been posited that listening to their mother's singing may play a role in identity formation.[30] This hypothesis is supported by a study that interviewed adults and asked them to describe musical experiences from their childhood. Findings showed that music was good for developing knowledge of emotions during childhood.[31]

Pre-school children edit

These studies have shown that children at the age of 4 are able to begin to distinguish between emotions found in musical excerpts in ways that are similar to adults.[27][28] The ability to distinguish these musical emotions seems to increase with age until adulthood.[29] However, children at the age of 3 were unable to make the distinction between emotions expressed in music through matching a facial expression with the type of emotion found in the music.[28] Some emotions, such as anger and fear, were also found to be harder to distinguish within music.[29][32]

Elementary-age children edit

In studies with four-year-olds and five-year-olds, they are asked to label musical excerpts with the affective labels "happy", "sad", "angry", and "afraid".[7] Results in one study showed that four-year-olds did not perform above chance with the labels "sad" and "angry", and the five-year-olds did not perform above chance with the label "afraid".[7] A follow-up study found conflicting results, where five-year-olds performed much like adults. However, all ages confused categorizing "angry" and "afraid".[7] Pre-school and elementary-age children listened to twelve short melodies, each in either major or minor mode, and were instructed to choose between four pictures of faces: happy, contented, sad, and angry.[13] All the children, even as young as three years old, performed above chance in assigning positive faces with major mode and negative faces with minor mode.[13]

Personality effects edit

Different people perceive events differently based upon their individual characteristics. Similarly, the emotions elicited by listening to different types of music seem to be affected by factors such as personality and previous musical training.[33][34][35] People with the personality type of agreeableness have been found to have higher emotional responses to music in general. Stronger sad feelings have also been associated with people with personality types of agreeableness and neuroticism. While some studies have shown that musical training can be correlated with music that evoked mixed feelings[33] as well as higher IQ and test of emotional comprehension scores,[34] other studies refute the claim that musical training affects perception of emotion in music.[32][36] It is also worth noting that previous exposure to music can affect later behavioral choices, schoolwork, and social interactions.[37] Therefore, previous music exposure does seem to have an effect on the personality and emotions of a child later in their life, and would subsequently affect their ability to perceive as well as express emotions during exposure to music. Gender, however, has not been shown to lead to a difference in perception of emotions found in music.[32][36] Further research into which factors affect an individual's perception of emotion in music and the ability of the individual to have music-induced emotions are needed.

Eliciting emotion through music edit

Along with the research that music conveys an emotion to its listener(s), it has also been shown that music can produce emotion in the listener(s).[38] This view often causes debate because the emotion is produced within the listener, and is consequently hard to measure. In spite of controversy, studies have shown observable responses to elicited emotions, which reinforces the Emotivists' view that music does elicit real emotional responses.[7][11]

Responses to elicited emotion edit

The structural features of music not only help convey an emotional message to the listener, but also may create emotion in the listener.[10] These emotions can be completely new feelings or may be an extension of previous emotional events. Empirical research has shown how listeners can absorb the piece's expression as their own emotion, as well as invoke a unique response based on their personal experiences.[26]

Basic emotions edit

In research on eliciting emotion, participants report personally feeling a certain emotion in response to hearing a musical piece.[38] Researchers have investigated whether the same structures that conveyed a particular emotion could elicit it as well. The researchers presented excerpts of fast tempo, major mode music and slow tempo, minor tone music to participants; these musical structures were chosen because they are known to convey happiness and sadness respectively.[24] Participants rated their own emotions with elevated levels of happiness after listening to music with structures that convey happiness and elevated sadness after music with structures that convey sadness.[24]

This evidence suggests that the same structures that convey emotions in music can also elicit those same emotions in the listener. In light of this finding, there has been particular controversy about music eliciting negative emotions. Cognitivists argue that choosing to listen to music that elicits negative emotions like sadness would be paradoxical, as listeners would not willingly strive to induce sadness,[11] whereas emotivists purport that music can elicit negative emotions, and listeners knowingly choose to listen in order to feel sadness in an impersonal way, similar to a viewer's desire to watch a tragic film.[11][38] The reasons why people sometimes listen to sad music when feeling sad has been explored by means of interviewing people about their motivations for doing so. As a result of this research, it has been found that people sometimes listen to sad music when feeling sad to intensify feelings of sadness. Other reasons for listening to sad music when feeling sad were in order to retrieve memories, to feel closer to other people, for cognitive reappraisal, to feel befriended by the music, to distract oneself, and for mood enhancement.[39]

Researchers have also found an effect between one's familiarity with a piece of music and the emotions it elicits.[40] One study suggested that familiarity with a piece of music increases the emotions experienced by the listener; half of participants were played twelve random musical excerpts one time, and rated their emotions after each piece. The other half of the participants listened to twelve random excerpts five times, and started their ratings on the third repetition. Findings showed that participants who listened to the excerpts five times rated their emotions with higher intensity than the participants who listened to them only once.[40]

Emotional memories and actions edit

Music may not only elicit new emotions, but connect listeners with other emotional sources.[10] Music serves as a powerful cue to recall emotional memories back into awareness.[41] Because music is such a pervasive part of social life, present in weddings, funerals and religious ceremonies, it brings back emotional memories that are often already associated with it.[10][26] Music is also processed by the lower, sensory levels of the brain, making it impervious to later memory distortions. Therefore creating a strong connection between emotion and music within memory makes it easier to recall one when prompted by the other.[10] Music can also tap into empathy, inducing emotions that are assumed to be felt by the performer or composer. Listeners can become sad because they recognize that those emotions must have been felt by the composer,[42][43] much as the viewer of a play can empathize for the actors.

Listeners may also respond to emotional music through action.[10] Throughout history music was composed to inspire people into specific action - to march, dance, sing or fight. Consequently, heightening the emotions in all these events. In fact, many people report being unable to sit still when certain rhythms are played, in some cases even engaging in subliminal actions when physical manifestations should be suppressed.[26] Examples of this can be seen in young children's spontaneous outbursts into motion upon hearing music, or exuberant expressions shown at concerts.[26]

Juslin and Västfjäll's BRECVEM model edit

Juslin and Västfjäll developed a model of seven ways in which music can elicit emotion, called the BRECVEM model.[44][45]

  • Brain stem reflex: "This refers to a process whereby an emotion is induced by music because one or more fundamental acoustical characteristics of the music are taken by the brain stem to signal a potentially important and urgent event. All other things being equal, sounds that are sudden, loud, dissonant, or feature fast temporal patterns induce arousal or feelings of unpleasantness in listeners...Such responses reflect the impact of auditory sensations – music as sound in the most basic sense."
  • Rhythmic entrainment: "This refers to a process whereby an emotion is evoked by a piece of music because a powerful, external rhythm in the music influences some internal bodily rhythm of the listener (e.g. heart rate), such that the latter rhythm adjusts toward and eventually 'locks in' to a common periodicity. The adjusted heart rate can then spread to other components of emotion such as feeling, through proprioceptive feedback. This may produce an increased level of arousal in the listener."[46]
  • Evaluative conditioning: "This refers to a process whereby an emotion is induced by a piece of music simply because this stimulus has been paired repeatedly with other positive or negative stimuli. Thus, for instance, a particular piece of music may have occurred repeatedly together in time with a specific event that always made you happy (e.g., meeting your best friend). Over time, through repeated pairings, the music will eventually come to evoke happiness even in the absence of the friendly interaction."
  • Emotional contagion: "This refers to a process whereby an emotion is induced by a piece of music because the listener perceives the emotional expression of the music, and then 'mimics' this expression internally, which by means of either peripheral feedback from muscles, or a more direct activation of the relevant emotional representations in the brain, leads to an induction of the same emotion."
  • Visual imagery: "This refers to a process whereby an emotion is induced in a listener because he or she conjures up visual images (e.g., of a beautiful landscape) while listening to the music."
  • Episodic memory: "This refers to a process whereby an emotion is induced in a listener because the music evokes a memory of a particular event in the listener's life. This is sometimes referred to as the 'Darling, they are playing our tune' phenomenon."[47]
  • Musical expectancy: "This refers to a process whereby an emotion is induced in a listener because a specific feature of the music violates, delays, or confirms the listener's expectations about the continuation of the music."

Musical expectancy edit

With regards to violations of expectation in music several interesting results have been found. It has for example been found that listening to unconventional music may sometimes cause a meaning threat and result in compensatory behaviour in order to restore meaning.[48] Musical expectancy is defined as a process whereby an emotion is aroused in a listener because a specific feature of the music violates, delays, or confirms the listener's expectations about the continuation of the music. Every time the listener hears a piece of music, he or she has such expectations, based on music he or she has heard before. For example, the sequential progression of E-F# may set up the expectation that the music will continue with G#. In other words, some notes seem to imply other notes; and if these musical implications are not realized — if the listener's expectations are thwarted — an affective response might be induced.[49]

Aesthetic judgement and BRECVEMA edit

In 2013, Juslin created an additional aspect to the BRECVEM model called aesthetic judgement.[50] This is the criteria which each individual has as a metric for music's aesthetic value. This can involve a number of varying personal preferences, such as the message conveyed, skill presented or novelty of style or idea.

Comparison of conveyed and elicited emotions edit

Evidence for emotion in music edit

There has been a bulk of evidence that listeners can identify specific emotions with certain types of music, but there has been less concrete evidence that music may elicit emotions.[10] This is due to the fact that elicited emotion is subjective; and thus, it is difficult to find a valid criterion to study it.[10] Elicited and conveyed emotion in music is usually understood from three types of evidence: self-report, physiological responses, and expressive behavior. Researchers use one or a combination of these methods to investigate emotional reactions to music.[10]

Self-report edit

The self-report method is a verbal report by the listener regarding what they are experiencing. This is the most widely used method for studying emotion and has shown that people identify emotions and personally experience emotions while listening to music.[10] Research in the area has shown that listeners' emotional responses are highly consistent. In fact, a meta-analysis of 41 studies on music performance found that happiness, sadness, tenderness, threat, and anger were identified above chance by listeners.[51] Another study compared untrained listeners to musically trained listeners.[51] Both groups were required to categorize musical excerpts that conveyed similar emotions. The findings showed that the categorizations were not different between the trained and untrained; thus demonstrating that the untrained listeners are highly accurate in perceiving emotion.[51] It is more difficult to find evidence for elicited emotion, as it depends solely on the subjective response of the listener. This leaves reporting vulnerable to self-report biases such as participants responding according to social prescriptions or responding as they think the experimenter wants them to.[10] As a result, the validity of the self-report method is often questioned, and consequently researchers are reluctant to draw definitive conclusions solely from these reports.[10]

Physiological responses edit

Emotions are known to create physiological, or bodily, changes in a person, which can be tested experimentally. Some evidence shows one of these changes is within the nervous system.[10] Arousing music is related to increased heart rate and muscle tension; calming music is connected to decreased heart rate and muscle tension, and increased skin temperature.[10] Other research identifies outward physical responses such as shivering or goose bumps to be caused by changes in harmony, while tears and a lump-in-the-throat sensation are provoked by changes in melody.[52] Researchers test these responses through the use of instruments for physiological measurement, such as recording pulse rate.[10]

Expressive behavior edit

People are also known to show outward manifestations of their emotional states while listening to music. Studies using facial electromyography (EMG) have found that people react with subliminal facial expressions when listening to expressive music.[26] In addition, music provides a stimulus for expressive behavior in many social contexts, such as concerts, dances, and ceremonies.[10][26] Although these expressive behaviors can be measured experimentally, there have been very few controlled studies observing this behavior.[10]

Strength of effects edit

Within the comparison between elicited and conveyed emotions, researchers have examined the relationship between these two types of responses to music. In general, research agrees that feeling and perception ratings are highly correlated, but not identical.[24] More specifically, studies are inconclusive as to whether one response has a stronger effect than the other, and in what ways these two responses relate.[24][40][53]

Conveyed more than elicited edit

In one study, participants heard a random selection of 24 excerpts, displaying six types of emotions, five times in a row.[40] Half the participants described the emotions the music conveyed, and the other half responded with how the music made them feel. The results found that emotions conveyed by music were more intense than the emotions elicited by the same piece of music.[40] Another study investigated under what specific conditions strong emotions were conveyed. Findings showed that ratings for conveyed emotions were higher in happy responses to music with consistent cues for happiness (i.e., fast tempo and major mode), for sad responses to music with consistent cues for sadness (i.e., slow tempo and minor mode,) and for sad responses in general.[24] These studies suggest that people can recognize the emotion displayed in music more readily than feeling it personally.

Sometimes conveyed, sometimes elicited edit

Another study that had 32 participants listen to twelve musical pieces and found that the strength of perceived and elicited emotions were dependent on the structures of the piece of music.[53] Perceived emotions were stronger than felt emotions when listeners rated for arousal and positive and negative activation. On the other hand, elicited emotions were stronger than perceived emotions when rating for pleasantness.[53]

Elicited more than conveyed edit

In another study analysis revealed that emotional responses were stronger than the listeners' perceptions of emotions.[53] This study used a between-subjects design, where 20 listeners judged to what extent they perceived four emotions: happy, sad, peaceful, and scared. A separate 19 listeners rated to what extent they experienced each of these emotions. The findings showed that all music stimuli elicited specific emotions for the group of participants rating elicited emotion, while music stimuli only occasionally conveyed emotion to the participants in the group identifying which emotions the music conveyed.[53] Based on these inconsistent findings, there is much research left to be done in order to determine how conveyed and elicited emotions are similar and different. There is disagreement about whether music induces 'true' emotions or if the emotions reported as felt in studies are instead just participants stating the emotions found in the music they are listening to.[54][55]

Music as a therapeutic tool edit

Music therapy as a therapeutic tool has been shown to be an effective treatment for various ailments. Therapeutic techniques involve eliciting emotions by listening to music, composing music or lyrics and performing music.[56]

Music therapy sessions may have the ability to help drug users who are attempting to break a drug habit, with users reporting feeling better able to feel emotions without the aid of drug use.[57] Music therapy may also be a viable option for people experiencing extended stays in a hospital due to illness. In one study, music therapy provided child oncology patients with enhanced environmental support elements and elicited more engaging behaviors from the child.[58] When treating troubled teenagers, a study by Keen revealed that music therapy has allowed therapists to interact with teenagers with less resistance, thus facilitating self-expression in the teenager.[citation needed]

Music therapy has also shown great promise in individuals with autism, serving as an emotional outlet for these patients. While other avenues of emotional expression and understanding may be difficult for people with autism, music may provide those with limited understanding of socio-emotional cues a way of accessing emotion.[59]

References edit

  1. ^ Cf. http://plato.stanford.edu/entries/music/
  2. ^ Davies, S. (2005). "Artistic Expression and the Hard Case of Pure Music", in: Kieran, M. (Ed.), Contemporary Debates in Aesthetics and the Philosophy of Art: 179-91.
  3. ^ Davies 2006, p. 181.
  4. ^ Davies 2006, p. 182.
  5. ^ Davies 2006
  6. ^ Jenefer Robinson, Deeper than Reason: Emotion and its Role in Literature, Music, and Art, Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2005; pp. 310-13
  7. ^ a b c d e f g Dowling, W.J. (2002). "The development of music perception and cognition". Foundations of Cognitive Psychology: Core Reading: 481–502.
  8. ^ Susino, M.; Schubert, S. (2017). "Cross-cultural anger communication in music: Towards a stereotype theory of emotion in music". Musicae Scientiae. 21: 60–74. doi:10.1177/1029864916637641. S2CID 148139308.
  9. ^ Thompson, William Forde & Balkwill, Laura-Lee (2010). "Chapter 27: Cross-cultural similarities and differences" (PDF). In Juslin, Patrik & Sloboda, John (eds.). Handbook of Music and Emotion: Theory, Research, Applications. Oxford University Press. pp. 755–788. ISBN 978-0-19-960496-8.
  10. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o p q r s t u v w Scherer, K. R.; Zentner, M. R. (2001). "Emotional effects of music: production rules". Music and Emotion: Theory and Research: 361–387.
  11. ^ a b c d Radford, C. (1989). "Emotions and music: A reply to the cognitivists". The Journal of Aesthetics and Art Criticism. 47 (1): 69–76. doi:10.2307/431994. JSTOR 431994.
  12. ^ a b Susino, M.; Schubert, E. (2020). "Musical emotions in the absence of music: A cross-cultural investigation of emotion communication in music by extra-musical cues". PLOS ONE. 15 (11): e0241196. Bibcode:2020PLoSO..1541196S. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0241196. PMC 7673536. PMID 33206664.
  13. ^ a b c d e f Gabrielle, A.; Stromboli, E. (2001). "The influence of musical structure on emotional expression". Music and Emotion: Theory and Research: 223–243.
  14. ^ Is Music a Universal Language?
  15. ^ Could Singing Help Your Mental Health?
  16. ^ Egermann, H; Fernando, N; Chuen, L; McAdams, S (2014). "Music induces universal emotion-related psychophysiological responses: comparing Canadian listeners to Congolese Pygmies". Front Psychol. 5: 1341. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01341. PMC 4286616. PMID 25620935.
  17. ^ Perception of basic emotions in music: Culture-specific or multicultural?
  18. ^ How Art Works (podcast, 10 min 18 sec)
  19. ^ Dellacherie, D., Roy, M., Hugueville, L., Peretz, I., & Samson, S. (2011). The effect of musical experience on emotional self-reports and psychophysiological responses to dissonance. Psychophysiology, 48(3), 337-349. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01075.x
  20. ^ Gomez, P.; Danuser, B. (2007). "Relationships between musical structure and psychophysiological measures of emotion". Emotion. 7 (2): 377–387. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.463.5753. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.7.2.377. PMID 17516815.
  21. ^ Ellen Winner (2018). How Art Works: A Psychological Exploration. Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0190863357.
  22. ^ How Art Works (podcast, 6 min 15 sec)
  23. ^ Barrett, F. S.; Janata, P. (2016). "Neural responses to nostalgia-evoking music modeled by elements of dynamic musical structure and individual differences in affective traits". Neuropsychologia. 91: 234–246. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.08.012. PMID 27526666. S2CID 32435787.
  24. ^ a b c d e f g h i Hunter, P. G.; Schellenburg, E. G.; Schimmack, U. (2010). "Feelings and perceptions of happiness and sadness induced by music: Similarities, differences, and mixed emotions". Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts. 4: 47–56. doi:10.1037/a0016873. S2CID 49568657.
  25. ^ a b c Larsen, J. T.; Stastny, B. J. (2011). "It's a bittersweet symphony: Simultaneously mixed emotional responses to music with conflicting cues". Emotion. 11 (6): 1469–1473. doi:10.1037/a0024081. PMID 21707144.
  26. ^ a b c d e f g h Sloboda, J. A.; Juslin, P. N. (2001). "Psychological perspectives on music and emotion". Music and Emotion: Theory and Research: 79–96.
  27. ^ a b c Nawrot, E. S. (1 January 2003). "The Perception of Emotional Expression in Music: Evidence from Infants, Children and Adults". Psychology of Music. 31 (1): 75–92. doi:10.1177/0305735603031001325. S2CID 144089631.
  28. ^ a b c Mote, Jasmine (1 January 2011). "The effects of tempo and familiarity on children's affective interpretation of music". Emotion. 11 (3): 618–622. doi:10.1037/a0022573. PMID 21668112.
  29. ^ a b c TERWOGT, MARK MEERUM; VAN GRINSVEN, FLORA (1 December 1988). "Recognition of Emotions in Music by Children and Adults". Perceptual and Motor Skills. 67 (3): 697–698. doi:10.2466/pms.1988.67.3.697. PMID 3226819. S2CID 36329792.
  30. ^ a b c Trehub, Sandra E.; Nakata, Takayuki (2001). "Emotion and music in infancy". Musicae Scientiae. SPEC ISSUE, 2001–2002: 37–61. doi:10.1177/10298649020050S103. S2CID 147382224.
  31. ^ Vist, Torill (16 November 2011). "Music Experience in Early Childhood: Potential for Emotion Knowledge?". International Journal of Early Childhood. 43 (3): 277–290. doi:10.1007/s13158-011-0045-7. S2CID 144761804.
  32. ^ a b c ROBAZZA, CLAUDIO; MACALUSO, CRISTINA; D'URSO, VALENTINA (1 October 1994). "Emotional Reactions to Music by Gender, Age, and Expertise". Perceptual and Motor Skills. 79 (2): 939–944. doi:10.2466/pms.1994.79.2.939. PMID 7870518. S2CID 22959117.
  33. ^ a b Ladinig, Olivia; Schellenberg, E. Glenn (1 January 2012). "Liking unfamiliar music: Effects of felt emotion and individual differences". Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts. 6 (2): 146–154. doi:10.1037/a0024671.
  34. ^ a b Schellenberg, E. Glenn; Mankarious, Monika (1 January 2012). "Music training and emotion comprehension in childhood". Emotion. 12 (5): 887–891. doi:10.1037/a0027971. PMID 22642351.
  35. ^ Vuoskoski, J. K.; Eerola, T. (13 July 2011). "Measuring music-induced emotion: A comparison of emotion models, personality biases, and intensity of experiences". Musicae Scientiae. 15 (2): 159–173. doi:10.1177/1029864911403367. S2CID 144079608.
  36. ^ a b Kratus, J. (1 January 1993). "A Developmental Study of Children's Interpretation of Emotion in Music". Psychology of Music. 21 (1): 3–19. doi:10.1177/030573569302100101. S2CID 145078460.
  37. ^ Council on Communications Media (19 October 2009). "Impact of Music, Music Lyrics, and Music Videos on Children and Youth". Pediatrics. 124 (5): 1488–1494. doi:10.1542/peds.2009-2145. PMID 19841124.
  38. ^ a b c Garrido, S.; E. Shubert (2011). "Individual differences in the enjoyment of negative emotion in music: a literature review and experiment". Music Perception. 28 (3): 279–295. doi:10.1525/MP.2011.28.3.279.
  39. ^ Van den TOl, A.J.M.; Edwards, J. (2013). "Exploring a rationale for choosing to listen to sad music when feeling sad". Psychology of Music.
  40. ^ a b c d e Ali, S. O.; Peynircioglu, Z. F. (2010). "Intensity of emotions conveyed and elicited by familiar and unfamiliar music". Music Perception. 27 (3): 177–182. doi:10.1525/MP.2010.27.3.177. JSTOR 10.1525/mp.2010.27.3.177.
  41. ^ Van den Tol, A. J. M., & Ritchie, T. D.(book-chapter in press). Emotion memory and music: A critical review and recommendations for future research.Music, Memory and Autobiography. (Eds: Professor Strollo Maria Rosaria and Dr. Romano Alessandra).
  42. ^ Van; den Tol, A. J. M.; Edwards, J. (2013). "Exploring a rationale for choosing to listen to sad music when feeling sad". Psychology of Music. 41 (4): 440–465. doi:10.1177/0305735611430433. S2CID 145108524.
  43. ^ Van; den Tol, A. J. M.; Edwards, J. (2014). "Listening to sad music in adverse situations: Music selection strategies, self-regulatory goals, listening effect, and mood-enhancement" (PDF). Psychology of Music. 43 (4): 473–494. doi:10.1177/0305735613517410. S2CID 145208440.
  44. ^ Patrik Juslin & Daniel V ̈astfj ̈all, 'Emotional responses to music: The need to consider underlying mechanisms, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31, 2008; 559-621.
  45. ^ Juslin, Liljeström, Västfjäll, & Lundqvist. (2010). How does music evoke emotions? Exploring the underlying mechanisms. In P.N. Juslin & J. Sloboda (Eds.), Handbook of Music and Emotion: Theory, Research, and Applications (pp. 605-642). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  46. ^ Juslin, P. (2011). Music and Emotion: seven questions, seven answers. In I. Deliège, J. Davidson, J. A. Sloboda (Eds.), Music and the Mind: Essays in Honour of John Sloboda (pp.113-138). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  47. ^ Davies, J. B. (1978). The psychology of music. Hutchinson.
  48. ^ Maher, P; Van Tilburg, W. A. P.; Van; den Tol, A. J. M. (2013). "Meaning in music: Deviations from expectation in music polarize in-group and out-group attitudes". European Journal of Social Psychology. 34 (2): 155–172. doi:10.1002/ejsp.196.
  49. ^ Juslin, Patrick (2019). What Comes Next? Musical Expectancy', Musical Emotions Explained: Unlocking the Secrets of Musical Affect (online ed.). Oxford Academic. pp. Pages 343–C24. ISBN 9780198753421.
  50. ^ Juslin, P. N. (2013). "From everyday emotions to aesthetic emotions: towards a unified theory of musical emotions". Physics of Life Reviews. 10 (3): 235–266. Bibcode:2013PhLRv..10..235J. doi:10.1016/j.plrev.2013.05.008. PMID 23769678.
  51. ^ a b c Vieillard, S.; Peretz, I.; Gosselin, N.; Khalfa, S. (2008). "Happy, sad, scary, and peaceful musical excerpts for research on emotions". Cognition and Emotion. 22 (4): 720–752. doi:10.1080/02699930701503567. S2CID 15310907.
  52. ^ Gabrielsson, A. (2001). "Emotion in strong experiences with music". Music and Emotion: Theory and Research: 431–449.
  53. ^ a b c d e Kallenin, K; Ravaja, N. (2006). "Emotion perceived and emotion felt: Same and different". Musicae Scientiae. 10 (2): 191–213. doi:10.1177/102986490601000203. S2CID 143503605.
  54. ^ Schubert, Emery (2007). "Locus of emotion: The effect of task order and age on emotion perceived and emotion felt in response to music". Journal of Music Therapy. 44 (4): 344–368. doi:10.1093/jmt/44.4.344. PMID 17997625.
  55. ^ Zentner, Marcel; Grandjean, Didier; Scherer, Klaus R. (1 January 2008). "Emotions evoked by the sound of music: Characterization, classification, and measurement". Emotion. 8 (4): 494–521. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.459.9866. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.8.4.494. PMID 18729581.
  56. ^ Keen MSocSc, Alexander W. (2005). "Using Music as a Therapy Tool to Motivate Troubled Adolescents". Social Work in Health Care. 39 (3–4): 361–373. doi:10.1300/J010v39n03_09. PMID 15774401. S2CID 25035875.
  57. ^ Baker, Felicity A.; Gleadhill, Libby M.; Dingle, Genevieve A. (2007). "Music therapy and emotional exploration: Exposing substance abuse clients to the experiences of non-drug-induced emotions". The Arts in Psychotherapy. 34 (4): 321–330. doi:10.1016/j.aip.2007.04.005.
  58. ^ Robb, Sheri L. (2000). "The effect of therapeutic music interventions on the behavior of hospitalized children in isolation: Developing a contextual support model of music therapy". Journal of Music Therapy. 37 (2): 118–146. doi:10.1093/jmt/37.2.118. PMID 10932125.
  59. ^ Heaton, Pam (2009). "Music - shelter for the frazzled mind?"". The Psychologist. 22 (12): 1018–1020.

Further reading edit

  • Gabrielsson, Alf (2011). Strong experiences with music: Music is much more than just music. Oxford University Press.
  • Juslin, Patrik N., and John Sloboda, eds (2011). Handbook of music and emotion: Theory, research, applications. Oxford University Press.
  • Willimek, Daniela; Willimek, Bernd (2013). "Abstract: 'Music and Emotions -- Research on the Theory of Musical Equilibration (die Strebetendenz-Theorie)'". EUNOMIOS.
  • Willimek, Daniela; Willimek, Bernd (2013). Music and Emotions -- Research on the Theory of Musical Equilibration (die Strebetendenz-Theorie) (PDF). Wilimekmusic.de.

This article incorporates material from the Citizendium article "Music and emotion", which is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike 3.0 Unported License but not under the GFDL.

music, emotion, this, article, need, rewritten, comply, with, wikipedia, quality, standards, help, talk, page, contain, suggestions, june, 2023, research, into, music, emotion, seeks, understand, psychological, relationship, between, human, affect, music, fiel. This article may need to be rewritten to comply with Wikipedia s quality standards You can help The talk page may contain suggestions June 2023 Research into music and emotion seeks to understand the psychological relationship between human affect and music The field a branch of music psychology covers numerous areas of study including the nature of emotional reactions to music how characteristics of the listener may determine which emotions are felt and which components of a musical composition or performance may elicit certain reactions Simon Vouet Saint Cecilia c 1626The research draws upon and has significant implications for such areas as philosophy musicology music therapy music theory and aesthetics as well as the acts of musical composition and of musical performance like a concert Contents 1 Philosophical approaches 1 1 Appearance emotionalism 1 2 Process theory 2 Conveying emotion through music 2 1 Structural features 2 2 Performance features 2 3 Listener features 2 4 Contextual features 2 5 Extra musical features 2 5 1 Conflicting cues 2 6 Specific listener features 2 6 1 Development 2 6 1 1 Infants 2 6 1 2 Pre school children 2 6 1 3 Elementary age children 2 6 2 Personality effects 3 Eliciting emotion through music 3 1 Responses to elicited emotion 3 1 1 Basic emotions 3 1 2 Emotional memories and actions 3 1 3 Juslin and Vastfjall s BRECVEM model 3 1 4 Musical expectancy 3 1 5 Aesthetic judgement and BRECVEMA 4 Comparison of conveyed and elicited emotions 4 1 Evidence for emotion in music 4 1 1 Self report 4 1 2 Physiological responses 4 1 3 Expressive behavior 4 2 Strength of effects 4 2 1 Conveyed more than elicited 4 2 2 Sometimes conveyed sometimes elicited 4 2 3 Elicited more than conveyed 5 Music as a therapeutic tool 6 References 7 Further readingPhilosophical approaches editAppearance emotionalism edit Two of the most influential philosophers in the aesthetics of music are Stephen Davies and Jerrold Levinson 1 2 Davies calls his view of the expressiveness of emotions in music appearance emotionalism which holds that music expresses emotion without feeling it Objects can convey emotion because their structures can contain certain characteristics that resemble emotional expression He says The resemblance that counts most for music s expressiveness is between music s temporally unfolding dynamic structure and configurations of human behaviour associated with the expression of emotion 3 The observer can note emotions from the listener s posture gait gestures attitude and comportment 4 Associations between musical features and emotion differ among individuals Appearance emotionalism claims many listeners perceiving associations constitutes the expressiveness of music Which musical features are more commonly associated with which emotions is part of music psychology Davies says that expressiveness is an objective property of music and not subjective in the sense of being projected into the music by the listener Music s expressiveness is certainly response dependent i e it is realized in the listener s judgement Skilled listeners very similarly attribute emotional expressiveness to a certain piece of music thereby indicating according to Davies that the expressiveness of music is somewhat objective because if the music lacked expressiveness then no expression could be projected into it as a reaction to the music 5 Process theory edit The philosopher Jennifer Robinson assumes the existence of a mutual dependence between cognition and elicitation in her description of emotions as process music as process theory or process theory Robinson argues that the process of emotional elicitation begins with an automatic immediate response that initiates motor and autonomic activity and prepares us for possible action causing a process of cognition that may enable listeners to name the felt emotion This series of events continually exchanges with new incoming information Robinson argues that emotions may transform into one another causing blends conflicts and ambiguities that make impede describing with one word the emotional state that one experiences at any given moment instead inner feelings are better thought of as the products of multiple emotional streams Robinson argues that music is a series of simultaneous processes and that it therefore is an ideal medium for mirroring such more cognitive aspects of emotion as musical themes desiring resolution or leitmotif s mirrors memory processes These simultaneous musical processes can reinforce or conflict with each other and thus also express the way one emotion morphs into another over time 6 page needed Conveying emotion through music editThe ability to perceive emotion in music is said weasel words to develop early in childhood and improve significantly throughout development 7 The capacity to perceive emotion in music is also subject to cultural influences and both similarities and differences in emotion perception have been observed in cross cultural studies 8 9 Empirical research has looked at which emotions can be conveyed as well as what structural factors in music help contribute to the perceived emotional expression There are two schools of thought on how we interpret emotion in music The cognitivists approach argues that music simply displays an emotion but does not allow for the personal experience of emotion in the listener Emotivists argue that music elicits real emotional responses in the listener 10 11 It has been argued that the emotion experienced from a piece of music is a multiplicative function of structural features performance features listener features contextual features and extra musical features of the piece shown as Experienced Emotion Structural features Performance features Listener features Contextual features Extra Musical featureswhere Structural features Segmental features Suprasegmental features Performance features Performer skill Performer state Listener features Musical expertise Stable disposition Current motivation Contextual features Location Event 10 Extra musical features Non auditory features Expertise 12 Structural features edit Structural features are divided into two parts segmental features and suprasegmental features Segmental features are the individual sounds or tones that make up the music this includes acoustic structures such as duration amplitude and pitch Suprasegmental features are the foundational structures of a piece such as melody tempo and rhythm 10 There are a number of specific musical features that are highly associated with particular emotions 13 Within the factors affecting emotional expression in music tempo is typically regarded as the most important but a number of other factors such as mode loudness and melody also influence the emotional valence of the piece 13 Structural Feature Definition Associated EmotionsTempo The speed or pace of a musical piece Fast tempo excitement anger Slow tempo sadness serenity Mode The type of scale Major tonality happiness joy Minor tonality sadness Loudness The physical strength and amplitude of a sound Intensity power or angerMelody The linear succession of musical tones that the listener perceives as a single entity Complementing harmonies happiness relaxation serenity Clashing harmonies excitement anger unpleasantness Rhythm The regularly recurring pattern or beat of a song Smooth consistent rhythm happiness peace Rough irregular rhythm amusement uneasiness Varied rhythm joy Some studies find that perception of basic emotional features are a cultural universal though people can more easily perceive emotion and perceive more nuanced emotion in music from their own culture 14 15 16 17 Music without lyrics is unlikely to elicit social emotions like anger shame and jealousy it typically only elicits basic emotions like happiness and sadness 18 Music has a direct connection to emotional states present in human beings Different musical structures have been found to have a relationship with physiological responses Research has shown that suprasegmental structures such as tonal space specifically dissonance create unpleasant negative emotions in participants The emotional responses were measured with physiological assessments such as skin conductance and electromyographic signals EMG while participants listened to musical excerpts 19 Further research on psychophysiological measures pertaining to music were conducted and found similar results musical structures of rhythmic articulation accentuation and tempo were found to correlate strongly with physiological measures the measured used here included heart rate and respiratory monitors that correlated with self report questionnaires 20 These associations can be innate learned or both Studies on young children and isolated cultures show innate associations for features are similar to a human voice e g low and slow is sad faster and high is happy Cross cultural studies show that associations between major mode vs minor mode and consonance vs dissonance are probably learned 21 22 Music also affects socially relevant memories specifically memories produced by nostalgic musical excerpts e g music from a significant time period in one s life like music listened to on road trips Musical structures are more strongly interpreted in certain areas of the brain when the music evokes nostalgia The interior frontal gyrus substantia nigra cerebellum and insula were all identified to have a stronger correlation with nostalgic music than not 23 Brain activity is a very individualized concept with many of the musical excerpts having certain effects based on individuals past life experiences thus this caveat should be kept in mind when generalizing findings across individuals Performance features edit Performance features refer to the manner in which a piece of music is executed by the performer s These are broken into two categories performer skills and performer state Performer skills are the compound ability and appearance of the performer including physical appearance reputation and technical skills The performer state is the interpretation motivation and stage presence of the performer 10 Listener features edit Listener features refer to the individual and social identity of the listener s This includes their personality age knowledge of music and motivation to listen to the music 10 Contextual features edit Contextual features are aspects of the performance such as the location and the particular occasion for the performance i e funeral wedding dance 10 Extra musical features edit Extra musical features refer to extra musical information detached from auditory music signals such as the genre or style of music 12 These different factors influence expressed emotion at different magnitudes and their effects are compounded by one another Thus experienced emotion is felt to a stronger degree if more factors are present The order the factors are listed within the model denotes how much weight in the equation they carry For this reason the bulk of research has been done in structural features and listener features 10 Conflicting cues edit Which emotion is perceived is dependent on the context of the piece of music Past research has argued that opposing emotions like happiness and sadness fall on a bipolar scale where both cannot be felt at the same time 24 More recent research has suggested that happiness and sadness are experienced separately which implies that they can be felt concurrently 24 One study investigated the latter possibility by having participants listen to computer manipulated musical excerpts that have mixed cues between tempo and mode 24 Examples of mix cue music include a piece with major key and slow tempo and a minor chord piece with a fast tempo Participants then rated the extent to which the piece conveyed happiness or sadness The results indicated that mixed cue music conveys both happiness and sadness however it remained unclear whether participants perceived happiness and sadness simultaneously or vacillated between these two emotions 24 A follow up study was done to examine these possibilities While listening to mixed or consistent cue music participants pressed one button when the music conveyed happiness and another button when it conveyed sadness 25 The results revealed that subjects pressed both buttons simultaneously during songs with conflicting cues 25 These findings indicate that listeners can perceive both happiness and sadness concurrently This has significant implications for how the structural features influence emotion because when a mix of structural cues is used a number of emotions may be conveyed 25 Specific listener features edit Development edit Studies indicate that the ability to understand emotional messages in music starts early and improves throughout child development 7 13 26 Studies investigating music and emotion in children primarily play a musical excerpt for children and have them look at pictorial expressions of faces These facial expressions display different emotions and children are asked to select the face that best matches the music s emotional tone 27 28 29 Studies have shown that children are able to assign specific emotions to pieces of music however there is debate regarding the age at which this ability begins 7 13 26 Infants edit An infant is often exposed to a mother s speech that is musical in nature It is possible that the motherly singing allows the mother to relay emotional messages to the infant 30 Infants also tend to prefer positive speech to neutral speech as well as happy music to negative music 27 30 It has also been posited that listening to their mother s singing may play a role in identity formation 30 This hypothesis is supported by a study that interviewed adults and asked them to describe musical experiences from their childhood Findings showed that music was good for developing knowledge of emotions during childhood 31 Pre school children edit These studies have shown that children at the age of 4 are able to begin to distinguish between emotions found in musical excerpts in ways that are similar to adults 27 28 The ability to distinguish these musical emotions seems to increase with age until adulthood 29 However children at the age of 3 were unable to make the distinction between emotions expressed in music through matching a facial expression with the type of emotion found in the music 28 Some emotions such as anger and fear were also found to be harder to distinguish within music 29 32 Elementary age children edit In studies with four year olds and five year olds they are asked to label musical excerpts with the affective labels happy sad angry and afraid 7 Results in one study showed that four year olds did not perform above chance with the labels sad and angry and the five year olds did not perform above chance with the label afraid 7 A follow up study found conflicting results where five year olds performed much like adults However all ages confused categorizing angry and afraid 7 Pre school and elementary age children listened to twelve short melodies each in either major or minor mode and were instructed to choose between four pictures of faces happy contented sad and angry 13 All the children even as young as three years old performed above chance in assigning positive faces with major mode and negative faces with minor mode 13 Personality effects edit Different people perceive events differently based upon their individual characteristics Similarly the emotions elicited by listening to different types of music seem to be affected by factors such as personality and previous musical training 33 34 35 People with the personality type of agreeableness have been found to have higher emotional responses to music in general Stronger sad feelings have also been associated with people with personality types of agreeableness and neuroticism While some studies have shown that musical training can be correlated with music that evoked mixed feelings 33 as well as higher IQ and test of emotional comprehension scores 34 other studies refute the claim that musical training affects perception of emotion in music 32 36 It is also worth noting that previous exposure to music can affect later behavioral choices schoolwork and social interactions 37 Therefore previous music exposure does seem to have an effect on the personality and emotions of a child later in their life and would subsequently affect their ability to perceive as well as express emotions during exposure to music Gender however has not been shown to lead to a difference in perception of emotions found in music 32 36 Further research into which factors affect an individual s perception of emotion in music and the ability of the individual to have music induced emotions are needed Eliciting emotion through music editAlong with the research that music conveys an emotion to its listener s it has also been shown that music can produce emotion in the listener s 38 This view often causes debate because the emotion is produced within the listener and is consequently hard to measure In spite of controversy studies have shown observable responses to elicited emotions which reinforces the Emotivists view that music does elicit real emotional responses 7 11 Responses to elicited emotion edit The structural features of music not only help convey an emotional message to the listener but also may create emotion in the listener 10 These emotions can be completely new feelings or may be an extension of previous emotional events Empirical research has shown how listeners can absorb the piece s expression as their own emotion as well as invoke a unique response based on their personal experiences 26 Basic emotions edit In research on eliciting emotion participants report personally feeling a certain emotion in response to hearing a musical piece 38 Researchers have investigated whether the same structures that conveyed a particular emotion could elicit it as well The researchers presented excerpts of fast tempo major mode music and slow tempo minor tone music to participants these musical structures were chosen because they are known to convey happiness and sadness respectively 24 Participants rated their own emotions with elevated levels of happiness after listening to music with structures that convey happiness and elevated sadness after music with structures that convey sadness 24 This evidence suggests that the same structures that convey emotions in music can also elicit those same emotions in the listener In light of this finding there has been particular controversy about music eliciting negative emotions Cognitivists argue that choosing to listen to music that elicits negative emotions like sadness would be paradoxical as listeners would not willingly strive to induce sadness 11 whereas emotivists purport that music can elicit negative emotions and listeners knowingly choose to listen in order to feel sadness in an impersonal way similar to a viewer s desire to watch a tragic film 11 38 The reasons why people sometimes listen to sad music when feeling sad has been explored by means of interviewing people about their motivations for doing so As a result of this research it has been found that people sometimes listen to sad music when feeling sad to intensify feelings of sadness Other reasons for listening to sad music when feeling sad were in order to retrieve memories to feel closer to other people for cognitive reappraisal to feel befriended by the music to distract oneself and for mood enhancement 39 Researchers have also found an effect between one s familiarity with a piece of music and the emotions it elicits 40 One study suggested that familiarity with a piece of music increases the emotions experienced by the listener half of participants were played twelve random musical excerpts one time and rated their emotions after each piece The other half of the participants listened to twelve random excerpts five times and started their ratings on the third repetition Findings showed that participants who listened to the excerpts five times rated their emotions with higher intensity than the participants who listened to them only once 40 Emotional memories and actions edit Music may not only elicit new emotions but connect listeners with other emotional sources 10 Music serves as a powerful cue to recall emotional memories back into awareness 41 Because music is such a pervasive part of social life present in weddings funerals and religious ceremonies it brings back emotional memories that are often already associated with it 10 26 Music is also processed by the lower sensory levels of the brain making it impervious to later memory distortions Therefore creating a strong connection between emotion and music within memory makes it easier to recall one when prompted by the other 10 Music can also tap into empathy inducing emotions that are assumed to be felt by the performer or composer Listeners can become sad because they recognize that those emotions must have been felt by the composer 42 43 much as the viewer of a play can empathize for the actors Listeners may also respond to emotional music through action 10 Throughout history music was composed to inspire people into specific action to march dance sing or fight Consequently heightening the emotions in all these events In fact many people report being unable to sit still when certain rhythms are played in some cases even engaging in subliminal actions when physical manifestations should be suppressed 26 Examples of this can be seen in young children s spontaneous outbursts into motion upon hearing music or exuberant expressions shown at concerts 26 Juslin and Vastfjall s BRECVEM model edit Juslin and Vastfjall developed a model of seven ways in which music can elicit emotion called the BRECVEM model 44 45 Brain stem reflex This refers to a process whereby an emotion is induced by music because one or more fundamental acoustical characteristics of the music are taken by the brain stem to signal a potentially important and urgent event All other things being equal sounds that are sudden loud dissonant or feature fast temporal patterns induce arousal or feelings of unpleasantness in listeners Such responses reflect the impact of auditory sensations music as sound in the most basic sense Rhythmic entrainment This refers to a process whereby an emotion is evoked by a piece of music because a powerful external rhythm in the music influences some internal bodily rhythm of the listener e g heart rate such that the latter rhythm adjusts toward and eventually locks in to a common periodicity The adjusted heart rate can then spread to other components of emotion such as feeling through proprioceptive feedback This may produce an increased level of arousal in the listener 46 Evaluative conditioning This refers to a process whereby an emotion is induced by a piece of music simply because this stimulus has been paired repeatedly with other positive or negative stimuli Thus for instance a particular piece of music may have occurred repeatedly together in time with a specific event that always made you happy e g meeting your best friend Over time through repeated pairings the music will eventually come to evoke happiness even in the absence of the friendly interaction Emotional contagion This refers to a process whereby an emotion is induced by a piece of music because the listener perceives the emotional expression of the music and then mimics this expression internally which by means of either peripheral feedback from muscles or a more direct activation of the relevant emotional representations in the brain leads to an induction of the same emotion Visual imagery This refers to a process whereby an emotion is induced in a listener because he or she conjures up visual images e g of a beautiful landscape while listening to the music Episodic memory This refers to a process whereby an emotion is induced in a listener because the music evokes a memory of a particular event in the listener s life This is sometimes referred to as the Darling they are playing our tune phenomenon 47 Musical expectancy This refers to a process whereby an emotion is induced in a listener because a specific feature of the music violates delays or confirms the listener s expectations about the continuation of the music Musical expectancy edit With regards to violations of expectation in music several interesting results have been found It has for example been found that listening to unconventional music may sometimes cause a meaning threat and result in compensatory behaviour in order to restore meaning 48 Musical expectancy is defined as a process whereby an emotion is aroused in a listener because a specific feature of the music violates delays or confirms the listener s expectations about the continuation of the music Every time the listener hears a piece of music he or she has such expectations based on music he or she has heard before For example the sequential progression of E F may set up the expectation that the music will continue with G In other words some notes seem to imply other notes and if these musical implications are not realized if the listener s expectations are thwarted an affective response might be induced 49 Aesthetic judgement and BRECVEMA edit In 2013 Juslin created an additional aspect to the BRECVEM model called aesthetic judgement 50 This is the criteria which each individual has as a metric for music s aesthetic value This can involve a number of varying personal preferences such as the message conveyed skill presented or novelty of style or idea Comparison of conveyed and elicited emotions editEvidence for emotion in music edit There has been a bulk of evidence that listeners can identify specific emotions with certain types of music but there has been less concrete evidence that music may elicit emotions 10 This is due to the fact that elicited emotion is subjective and thus it is difficult to find a valid criterion to study it 10 Elicited and conveyed emotion in music is usually understood from three types of evidence self report physiological responses and expressive behavior Researchers use one or a combination of these methods to investigate emotional reactions to music 10 Self report edit The self report method is a verbal report by the listener regarding what they are experiencing This is the most widely used method for studying emotion and has shown that people identify emotions and personally experience emotions while listening to music 10 Research in the area has shown that listeners emotional responses are highly consistent In fact a meta analysis of 41 studies on music performance found that happiness sadness tenderness threat and anger were identified above chance by listeners 51 Another study compared untrained listeners to musically trained listeners 51 Both groups were required to categorize musical excerpts that conveyed similar emotions The findings showed that the categorizations were not different between the trained and untrained thus demonstrating that the untrained listeners are highly accurate in perceiving emotion 51 It is more difficult to find evidence for elicited emotion as it depends solely on the subjective response of the listener This leaves reporting vulnerable to self report biases such as participants responding according to social prescriptions or responding as they think the experimenter wants them to 10 As a result the validity of the self report method is often questioned and consequently researchers are reluctant to draw definitive conclusions solely from these reports 10 Physiological responses edit Emotions are known to create physiological or bodily changes in a person which can be tested experimentally Some evidence shows one of these changes is within the nervous system 10 Arousing music is related to increased heart rate and muscle tension calming music is connected to decreased heart rate and muscle tension and increased skin temperature 10 Other research identifies outward physical responses such as shivering or goose bumps to be caused by changes in harmony while tears and a lump in the throat sensation are provoked by changes in melody 52 Researchers test these responses through the use of instruments for physiological measurement such as recording pulse rate 10 Expressive behavior edit People are also known to show outward manifestations of their emotional states while listening to music Studies using facial electromyography EMG have found that people react with subliminal facial expressions when listening to expressive music 26 In addition music provides a stimulus for expressive behavior in many social contexts such as concerts dances and ceremonies 10 26 Although these expressive behaviors can be measured experimentally there have been very few controlled studies observing this behavior 10 Strength of effects edit Within the comparison between elicited and conveyed emotions researchers have examined the relationship between these two types of responses to music In general research agrees that feeling and perception ratings are highly correlated but not identical 24 More specifically studies are inconclusive as to whether one response has a stronger effect than the other and in what ways these two responses relate 24 40 53 Conveyed more than elicited edit In one study participants heard a random selection of 24 excerpts displaying six types of emotions five times in a row 40 Half the participants described the emotions the music conveyed and the other half responded with how the music made them feel The results found that emotions conveyed by music were more intense than the emotions elicited by the same piece of music 40 Another study investigated under what specific conditions strong emotions were conveyed Findings showed that ratings for conveyed emotions were higher in happy responses to music with consistent cues for happiness i e fast tempo and major mode for sad responses to music with consistent cues for sadness i e slow tempo and minor mode and for sad responses in general 24 These studies suggest that people can recognize the emotion displayed in music more readily than feeling it personally Sometimes conveyed sometimes elicited edit Another study that had 32 participants listen to twelve musical pieces and found that the strength of perceived and elicited emotions were dependent on the structures of the piece of music 53 Perceived emotions were stronger than felt emotions when listeners rated for arousal and positive and negative activation On the other hand elicited emotions were stronger than perceived emotions when rating for pleasantness 53 Elicited more than conveyed edit In another study analysis revealed that emotional responses were stronger than the listeners perceptions of emotions 53 This study used a between subjects design where 20 listeners judged to what extent they perceived four emotions happy sad peaceful and scared A separate 19 listeners rated to what extent they experienced each of these emotions The findings showed that all music stimuli elicited specific emotions for the group of participants rating elicited emotion while music stimuli only occasionally conveyed emotion to the participants in the group identifying which emotions the music conveyed 53 Based on these inconsistent findings there is much research left to be done in order to determine how conveyed and elicited emotions are similar and different There is disagreement about whether music induces true emotions or if the emotions reported as felt in studies are instead just participants stating the emotions found in the music they are listening to 54 55 Music as a therapeutic tool editMain article Music therapy Music therapy as a therapeutic tool has been shown to be an effective treatment for various ailments Therapeutic techniques involve eliciting emotions by listening to music composing music or lyrics and performing music 56 Music therapy sessions may have the ability to help drug users who are attempting to break a drug habit with users reporting feeling better able to feel emotions without the aid of drug use 57 Music therapy may also be a viable option for people experiencing extended stays in a hospital due to illness In one study music therapy provided child oncology patients with enhanced environmental support elements and elicited more engaging behaviors from the child 58 When treating troubled teenagers a study by Keen revealed that music therapy has allowed therapists to interact with teenagers with less resistance thus facilitating self expression in the teenager citation needed Music therapy has also shown great promise in individuals with autism serving as an emotional outlet for these patients While other avenues of emotional expression and understanding may be difficult for people with autism music may provide those with limited understanding of socio emotional cues a way of accessing emotion 59 References edit Cf http plato stanford edu entries music Davies S 2005 Artistic Expression and the Hard Case of Pure Music in Kieran M Ed Contemporary Debates in Aesthetics and the Philosophy of Art 179 91 Davies 2006 p 181 Davies 2006 p 182 Davies 2006 Jenefer Robinson Deeper than Reason Emotion and its Role in Literature Music and Art Oxford Oxford University Press 2005 pp 310 13 a b c d e f g Dowling W J 2002 The development of music perception and cognition Foundations of Cognitive Psychology Core Reading 481 502 Susino M Schubert S 2017 Cross cultural anger communication in music Towards a stereotype theory of emotion in music Musicae Scientiae 21 60 74 doi 10 1177 1029864916637641 S2CID 148139308 Thompson William Forde amp Balkwill Laura Lee 2010 Chapter 27 Cross cultural similarities and differences PDF In Juslin Patrik amp Sloboda John eds Handbook of Music and Emotion Theory Research Applications Oxford University Press pp 755 788 ISBN 978 0 19 960496 8 a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o p q r s t u v w Scherer K R Zentner M R 2001 Emotional effects of music production rules Music and Emotion Theory and Research 361 387 a b c d Radford C 1989 Emotions and music A reply to the cognitivists The Journal of Aesthetics and Art Criticism 47 1 69 76 doi 10 2307 431994 JSTOR 431994 a b Susino M Schubert E 2020 Musical emotions in the absence of music A cross cultural investigation of emotion communication in music by extra musical cues PLOS ONE 15 11 e0241196 Bibcode 2020PLoSO 1541196S doi 10 1371 journal pone 0241196 PMC 7673536 PMID 33206664 a b c d e f Gabrielle A Stromboli E 2001 The influence of musical structure on emotional expression Music and Emotion Theory and Research 223 243 Is Music a Universal Language Could Singing Help Your Mental Health Egermann H Fernando N Chuen L McAdams S 2014 Music induces universal emotion related psychophysiological responses comparing Canadian listeners to Congolese Pygmies Front Psychol 5 1341 doi 10 3389 fpsyg 2014 01341 PMC 4286616 PMID 25620935 Perception of basic emotions in music Culture specific or multicultural How Art Works podcast 10 min 18 sec Dellacherie D Roy M Hugueville L Peretz I amp Samson S 2011 The effect of musical experience on emotional self reports and psychophysiological responses to dissonance Psychophysiology 48 3 337 349 https doi org 10 1111 j 1469 8986 2010 01075 x Gomez P Danuser B 2007 Relationships between musical structure and psychophysiological measures of emotion Emotion 7 2 377 387 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 463 5753 doi 10 1037 1528 3542 7 2 377 PMID 17516815 Ellen Winner 2018 How Art Works A Psychological Exploration Oxford University Press ISBN 978 0190863357 How Art Works podcast 6 min 15 sec Barrett F S Janata P 2016 Neural responses to nostalgia evoking music modeled by elements of dynamic musical structure and individual differences in affective traits Neuropsychologia 91 234 246 doi 10 1016 j neuropsychologia 2016 08 012 PMID 27526666 S2CID 32435787 a b c d e f g h i Hunter P G Schellenburg E G Schimmack U 2010 Feelings and perceptions of happiness and sadness induced by music Similarities differences and mixed emotions Psychology of Aesthetics Creativity and the Arts 4 47 56 doi 10 1037 a0016873 S2CID 49568657 a b c Larsen J T Stastny B J 2011 It s a bittersweet symphony Simultaneously mixed emotional responses to music with conflicting cues Emotion 11 6 1469 1473 doi 10 1037 a0024081 PMID 21707144 a b c d e f g h Sloboda J A Juslin P N 2001 Psychological perspectives on music and emotion Music and Emotion Theory and Research 79 96 a b c Nawrot E S 1 January 2003 The Perception of Emotional Expression in Music Evidence from Infants Children and Adults Psychology of Music 31 1 75 92 doi 10 1177 0305735603031001325 S2CID 144089631 a b c Mote Jasmine 1 January 2011 The effects of tempo and familiarity on children s affective interpretation of music Emotion 11 3 618 622 doi 10 1037 a0022573 PMID 21668112 a b c TERWOGT MARK MEERUM VAN GRINSVEN FLORA 1 December 1988 Recognition of Emotions in Music by Children and Adults Perceptual and Motor Skills 67 3 697 698 doi 10 2466 pms 1988 67 3 697 PMID 3226819 S2CID 36329792 a b c Trehub Sandra E Nakata Takayuki 2001 Emotion and music in infancy Musicae Scientiae SPEC ISSUE 2001 2002 37 61 doi 10 1177 10298649020050S103 S2CID 147382224 Vist Torill 16 November 2011 Music Experience in Early Childhood Potential for Emotion Knowledge International Journal of Early Childhood 43 3 277 290 doi 10 1007 s13158 011 0045 7 S2CID 144761804 a b c ROBAZZA CLAUDIO MACALUSO CRISTINA D URSO VALENTINA 1 October 1994 Emotional Reactions to Music by Gender Age and Expertise Perceptual and Motor Skills 79 2 939 944 doi 10 2466 pms 1994 79 2 939 PMID 7870518 S2CID 22959117 a b Ladinig Olivia Schellenberg E Glenn 1 January 2012 Liking unfamiliar music Effects of felt emotion and individual differences Psychology of Aesthetics Creativity and the Arts 6 2 146 154 doi 10 1037 a0024671 a b Schellenberg E Glenn Mankarious Monika 1 January 2012 Music training and emotion comprehension in childhood Emotion 12 5 887 891 doi 10 1037 a0027971 PMID 22642351 Vuoskoski J K Eerola T 13 July 2011 Measuring music induced emotion A comparison of emotion models personality biases and intensity of experiences Musicae Scientiae 15 2 159 173 doi 10 1177 1029864911403367 S2CID 144079608 a b Kratus J 1 January 1993 A Developmental Study of Children s Interpretation of Emotion in Music Psychology of Music 21 1 3 19 doi 10 1177 030573569302100101 S2CID 145078460 Council on Communications Media 19 October 2009 Impact of Music Music Lyrics and Music Videos on Children and Youth Pediatrics 124 5 1488 1494 doi 10 1542 peds 2009 2145 PMID 19841124 a b c Garrido S E Shubert 2011 Individual differences in the enjoyment of negative emotion in music a literature review and experiment Music Perception 28 3 279 295 doi 10 1525 MP 2011 28 3 279 Van den TOl A J M Edwards J 2013 Exploring a rationale for choosing to listen to sad music when feeling sad Psychology of Music a b c d e Ali S O Peynircioglu Z F 2010 Intensity of emotions conveyed and elicited by familiar and unfamiliar music Music Perception 27 3 177 182 doi 10 1525 MP 2010 27 3 177 JSTOR 10 1525 mp 2010 27 3 177 Van den Tol A J M amp Ritchie T D book chapter in press Emotion memory and music A critical review and recommendations for future research Music Memory and Autobiography Eds Professor Strollo Maria Rosaria and Dr Romano Alessandra Van den Tol A J M Edwards J 2013 Exploring a rationale for choosing to listen to sad music when feeling sad Psychology of Music 41 4 440 465 doi 10 1177 0305735611430433 S2CID 145108524 Van den Tol A J M Edwards J 2014 Listening to sad music in adverse situations Music selection strategies self regulatory goals listening effect and mood enhancement PDF Psychology of Music 43 4 473 494 doi 10 1177 0305735613517410 S2CID 145208440 Patrik Juslin amp Daniel V astfj all Emotional responses to music The need to consider underlying mechanisms Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31 2008 559 621 Juslin Liljestrom Vastfjall amp Lundqvist 2010 How does music evoke emotions Exploring the underlying mechanisms In P N Juslin amp J Sloboda Eds Handbook of Music and Emotion Theory Research and Applications pp 605 642 Oxford Oxford University Press Juslin P 2011 Music and Emotion seven questions seven answers In I Deliege J Davidson J A Sloboda Eds Music and the Mind Essays in Honour of John Sloboda pp 113 138 Oxford Oxford University Press Davies J B 1978 The psychology of music Hutchinson Maher P Van Tilburg W A P Van den Tol A J M 2013 Meaning in music Deviations from expectation in music polarize in group and out group attitudes European Journal of Social Psychology 34 2 155 172 doi 10 1002 ejsp 196 Juslin Patrick 2019 What Comes Next Musical Expectancy Musical Emotions Explained Unlocking the Secrets of Musical Affect online ed Oxford Academic pp Pages 343 C24 ISBN 9780198753421 Juslin P N 2013 From everyday emotions to aesthetic emotions towards a unified theory of musical emotions Physics of Life Reviews 10 3 235 266 Bibcode 2013PhLRv 10 235J doi 10 1016 j plrev 2013 05 008 PMID 23769678 a b c Vieillard S Peretz I Gosselin N Khalfa S 2008 Happy sad scary and peaceful musical excerpts for research on emotions Cognition and Emotion 22 4 720 752 doi 10 1080 02699930701503567 S2CID 15310907 Gabrielsson A 2001 Emotion in strong experiences with music Music and Emotion Theory and Research 431 449 a b c d e Kallenin K Ravaja N 2006 Emotion perceived and emotion felt Same and different Musicae Scientiae 10 2 191 213 doi 10 1177 102986490601000203 S2CID 143503605 Schubert Emery 2007 Locus of emotion The effect of task order and age on emotion perceived and emotion felt in response to music Journal of Music Therapy 44 4 344 368 doi 10 1093 jmt 44 4 344 PMID 17997625 Zentner Marcel Grandjean Didier Scherer Klaus R 1 January 2008 Emotions evoked by the sound of music Characterization classification and measurement Emotion 8 4 494 521 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 459 9866 doi 10 1037 1528 3542 8 4 494 PMID 18729581 Keen MSocSc Alexander W 2005 Using Music as a Therapy Tool to Motivate Troubled Adolescents Social Work in Health Care 39 3 4 361 373 doi 10 1300 J010v39n03 09 PMID 15774401 S2CID 25035875 Baker Felicity A Gleadhill Libby M Dingle Genevieve A 2007 Music therapy and emotional exploration Exposing substance abuse clients to the experiences of non drug induced emotions The Arts in Psychotherapy 34 4 321 330 doi 10 1016 j aip 2007 04 005 Robb Sheri L 2000 The effect of therapeutic music interventions on the behavior of hospitalized children in isolation Developing a contextual support model of music therapy Journal of Music Therapy 37 2 118 146 doi 10 1093 jmt 37 2 118 PMID 10932125 Heaton Pam 2009 Music shelter for the frazzled mind The Psychologist 22 12 1018 1020 Further reading editGabrielsson Alf 2011 Strong experiences with music Music is much more than just music Oxford University Press Juslin Patrik N and John Sloboda eds 2011 Handbook of music and emotion Theory research applications Oxford University Press Willimek Daniela Willimek Bernd 2013 Abstract Music and Emotions Research on the Theory of Musical Equilibration die Strebetendenz Theorie EUNOMIOS Willimek Daniela Willimek Bernd 2013 Music and Emotions Research on the Theory of Musical Equilibration die Strebetendenz Theorie PDF Wilimekmusic de This article incorporates material from the Citizendium article Music and emotion which is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution ShareAlike 3 0 Unported License but not under the GFDL Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Music and emotion amp oldid 1202879112, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.