fbpx
Wikipedia

Mechanism of autism

Autism's symptoms result from maturation-related changes in various systems of the brain. Although it is not well understood how autism occurs, there have been attempts to describe the mechanisms involved.[1] Conceptually, one can divide its development into two areas: the pathophysiology of brain structures and processes associated with autism, and the neuropsychological linkages between brain structures and behaviors.[2] The behaviors appear to have multiple pathophysiologies.[3][4]

There is evidence that gut–brain axis abnormalities may be involved.[5][6][7] A 2015 review proposed that immune dysregulation, gastrointestinal inflammation, malfunction of the autonomic nervous system, gut flora alterations, and food metabolites may cause brain neuroinflammation and dysfunction.[5] A 2016 review concludes that enteric nervous system abnormalities might play a role in neurological disorders such as autism. Neural connections and the immune system are a pathway that may allow diseases originated in the intestine to spread to the brain.[6]

Several lines of evidence point to synaptic dysfunction as a cause of autism.[8] Some rare mutations may lead to autism by disrupting some synaptic pathways, such as those involved with cell adhesion.[9] Gene replacement studies in mice suggest that autistic symptoms are closely related to later developmental steps that depend on activity in synapses and on activity-dependent changes.[10] All known teratogens (agents that cause birth defects) related to the risk of autism appear to act during the first eight weeks from conception, and though this does not exclude the possibility that autism can be initiated or affected later, there is strong evidence that autism arises very early in development.[11]

Pathophysiology

 
Autism affects the amygdala, cerebellum, and many other parts of the brain.[12]

Unlike many other brain disorders, such as Parkinson's, autism does not have a clear unifying mechanism at either the molecular, cellular, or systems level; it is not known whether autism is a few disorders caused by mutations converging on a few common molecular pathways, or is (like intellectual disability) a large set of disorders with diverse mechanisms.[13] Autism appears to result from developmental factors that affect many or all functional brain systems,[14] and to disturb the timing of brain development more than the final product.[12] Neuroanatomical studies and the associations with teratogens strongly suggest that autism's mechanism includes alteration of brain development soon after conception.[11] This anomaly appears to start a cascade of pathological events in the brain that are significantly influenced by environmental factors.[15] Just after birth, the brains of children with autism tend to grow faster than usual, followed by normal or relatively slower growth in childhood. It is not known whether early overgrowth occurs in all children with autism. It seems to be most prominent in brain areas underlying the development of higher cognitive specialization.[16] Hypotheses for the cellular and molecular bases of pathological early overgrowth include the following:

The immune system is thought to play an important role in autism. Children with autism have been found by researchers to have inflammation of both the peripheral and central immune systems as indicated by increased levels of pro-inflammatory cytokines and significant activation of microglia.[24][25][26] Biomarkers of abnormal immune function have also been associated with increased impairments in behaviors that are characteristic of the core features of autism such as, deficits in social interactions and communication.[25] Interactions between the immune system and the nervous system begin early during the embryonic stage of life, and successful neurodevelopment depends on a balanced immune response. It is thought that activation of a pregnant mother's immune system such as from environmental toxicants or infection can contribute to causing autism through causing a disruption of brain development.[27][28][29] This is supported by recent studies that have found that infection during pregnancy is associated with an increased risk of autism.[30][31]

The relationship of neurochemicals to autism is not well understood; several have been investigated, with the most evidence for the role of serotonin and of genetic differences in its transport.[8] The role of group I metabotropic glutamate receptors (mGluR) in the pathogenesis of fragile X syndrome, the most common identified genetic cause of autism, has led to interest in the possible implications for future autism research into this pathway.[32] Some data suggests neuronal overgrowth potentially related to an increase in several growth hormones[33] or to impaired regulation of growth factor receptors. Also, some inborn errors of metabolism are associated with autism, but probably account for less than 5% of cases.[34]

The mirror neuron system (MNS) theory of autism hypothesizes that distortion in the development of the MNS interferes with imitation and leads to autism's core features of social impairment and communication difficulties. The MNS operates when an animal performs an action or observes another animal perform the same action. The MNS may contribute to an individual's understanding of other people by enabling the modeling of their behavior via embodied simulation of their actions, intentions, and emotions.[35] Several studies have tested this hypothesis by demonstrating structural abnormalities in MNS regions of individuals with ASD, delay in the activation in the core circuit for imitation in individuals with ASD, and a correlation between reduced MNS activity and severity of the syndrome in children with ASD.[36] However, individuals with autism also have abnormal brain activation in many circuits outside the MNS[37] and the MNS theory does not explain the normal performance of children with autism on imitation tasks that involve a goal or object.[38]

 
Autistic individuals tend to use different areas of the brain (yellow) for a movement task compared to a control group (blue).[39]

ASD-related patterns of low function and aberrant activation in the brain differ depending on whether the brain is doing social or nonsocial tasks.[40] In autism there is evidence for reduced functional connectivity of the default network (a large-scale brain network involved in social and emotional processing), with intact connectivity of the task-positive network (used in sustained attention and goal-directed thinking). In people with autism the two networks are not negatively correlated in time, suggesting an imbalance in toggling between the two networks, possibly reflecting a disturbance of self-referential thought.[41]

The underconnectivity theory of autism hypothesizes that autism is marked by underfunctioning high-level neural connections and synchronization, along with an excess of low-level processes.[42] Evidence for this theory has been found in functional neuroimaging studies on autistic individuals[43] and by a brainwave study that suggested that adults with ASD have local overconnectivity in the cortex and weak functional connections between the frontal lobe and the rest of the cortex.[44] Other evidence suggests the underconnectivity is mainly within each hemisphere of the cortex and that autism is a disorder of the association cortex.[45]

From studies based on event-related potentials, transient changes to the brain's electrical activity in response to stimuli, there is considerable evidence for differences in autistic individuals with respect to attention, orientation to auditory and visual stimuli, novelty detection, language and face processing, and information storage; several studies have found a preference for nonsocial stimuli.[46] For example, magnetoencephalography studies have found evidence in children with autism of delayed responses in the brain's processing of auditory signals.[47]

In the genetic area, relations have been found between autism and schizophrenia based on duplications and deletions of chromosomes; research showed that schizophrenia and autism are significantly more common in combination with 1q21.1 deletion syndrome. Research on autism/schizophrenia relations for chromosome 15 (15q13.3), chromosome 16 (16p13.1) and chromosome 17 (17p12) are inconclusive.[48]

Functional connectivity studies have found both hypo- and hyper-connectivity in brains of people with autism. Hypo-connectivity seems to dominate, especially for interhemispheric and cortico-cortical functional connectivity.[49]

Neuropsychology

Two major categories of cognitive theories have been proposed about the links between autistic brains and behavior.

The first category focuses on deficits in social cognition. Simon Baron-Cohen's empathizing–systemizing theory postulates that autistic individuals can systemize—that is, they can develop internal rules of operation to handle events inside the brain—but are less effective at empathizing by handling events generated by other agents. An extension, the extreme male brain theory, hypothesizes that autism is an extreme case of the male brain, defined psychometrically as individuals in whom systemizing is better than empathizing.[50] These theories are somewhat related to Baron-Cohen's earlier theory of mind approach, which hypothesizes that autistic behavior arises from an inability to ascribe mental states to oneself and others. The theory of mind hypothesis is supported by the atypical responses of children with autism to the Sally–Anne test for reasoning about others' motivations,[50] and the mirror neuron system theory of autism described in Pathophysiology maps well to the hypothesis.[36] However, most studies have found no evidence of impairment in autistic individuals' ability to understand other people's basic intentions or goals; instead, data suggests that impairments are found in understanding more complex social emotions or in considering others' viewpoints.[51]

The second category focuses on nonsocial or general processing: the executive functions such as working memory, planning, inhibition. In his review, Kenworthy states that "the claim of executive dysfunction as a causal factor in autism is controversial", however, "it is clear that executive dysfunction plays a role in the social and cognitive deficits observed in individuals with autism".[52] Tests of core executive processes such as eye movement tasks indicate improvement from late childhood to adolescence, but performance never reaches typical adult levels.[53] A strength of the theory is predicting stereotyped behavior and narrow interests;[54] two weaknesses are that executive function is hard to measure[52] and that executive function deficits have not been found in young children with autism.[55]

Weak central coherence theory hypothesizes that a limited ability to see the big picture underlies the central disturbance in autism. One strength of this theory is predicting special talents and peaks in performance in autistic people.[56] A related theory—enhanced perceptual functioning—focuses more on the superiority of locally oriented and perceptual operations in autistic individuals.[57] Yet another, monotropism, posits that autism stems from a different cognitive style, tending to focus attention (or processing resources) intensely, to the exclusion of other stimuli.[58] These theories map well from the underconnectivity theory of autism.

Neither category is satisfactory on its own; social cognition theories poorly address autism's rigid and repetitive behaviors, while most of the nonsocial theories have difficulty explaining social impairment and communication difficulties.[59] A combined theory based on multiple deficits may prove to be more useful.[60]

References

  1. ^ Sarovic D (November 2021). "A Unifying Theory for Autism: The Pathogenetic Triad as a Theoretical Framework". Frontiers in Psychiatry (Review). 12: 767075. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2021.767075. PMC 8637925. PMID 34867553. S2CID 244119594.
  2. ^ Penn HE (2006). "Neurobiological correlates of autism: a review of recent research". Child Neuropsychol. 12 (1): 57–79. doi:10.1080/09297040500253546. PMID 16484102. S2CID 46119993.
  3. ^ London E (2007). "The role of the neurobiologist in redefining the diagnosis of autism". Brain Pathol. 17 (4): 408–11. doi:10.1111/j.1750-3639.2007.00103.x. PMC 8095627. PMID 17919126. S2CID 24860348.
  4. ^ Baird G, Cass H, Slonims V (2003). "Diagnosis of autism". BMJ. 327 (7413): 488–93. doi:10.1136/bmj.327.7413.488. PMC 188387. PMID 12946972.
  5. ^ a b Wasilewska J, Klukowski M (2015). "Gastrointestinal symptoms and autism spectrum disorder: links and risks - a possible new overlap syndrome". Pediatric Health Med Ther (Review). 6: 153–166. doi:10.2147/PHMT.S85717. PMC 5683266. PMID 29388597.
  6. ^ a b Rao M, Gershon MD (September 2016). "The bowel and beyond: the enteric nervous system in neurological disorders". Nat Rev Gastroenterol Hepatol (Review). 13 (9): 517–28. doi:10.1038/nrgastro.2016.107. PMC 5005185. PMID 27435372.
  7. ^ Israelyan N, Margolis KG (2018). "Serotonin as a link between the gut-brain-microbiome axis in autism spectrum disorders". Pharmacol Res (Review). 132: 1–6. doi:10.1016/j.phrs.2018.03.020. PMC 6368356. PMID 29614380.
  8. ^ a b Levy SE, Mandell DS, Schultz RT (2009). "Autism". Lancet. 374 (9701): 1627–38. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(09)61376-3. PMC 2863325. PMID 19819542.
  9. ^ Betancur C, Sakurai T, Buxbaum JD (2009). "The emerging role of synaptic cell-adhesion pathways in the pathogenesis of autism spectrum disorders". Trends Neurosci. 32 (7): 402–12. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2009.04.003. PMID 19541375. S2CID 8644511.
  10. ^ Walsh CA, Morrow EM, Rubenstein JL (2008). "Autism and brain development". Cell. 135 (3): 396–400. doi:10.1016/j.cell.2008.10.015. PMC 2701104. PMID 18984148.
  11. ^ a b Arndt TL, Stodgell CJ, Rodier PM (2005). "The teratology of autism". Int J Dev Neurosci. 23 (2–3): 189–99. doi:10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2004.11.001. PMID 15749245. S2CID 17797266.
  12. ^ a b Amaral DG, Schumann CM, Nordahl CW (2008). "Neuroanatomy of autism". Trends Neurosci. 31 (3): 137–45. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2007.12.005. PMID 18258309. S2CID 18648870.
  13. ^ Geschwind DH (2008). "Autism: many genes, common pathways?". Cell. 135 (3): 391–95. doi:10.1016/j.cell.2008.10.016. PMC 2756410. PMID 18984147.
  14. ^ Müller RA (2007). "The study of autism as a distributed disorder". Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev. 13 (1): 85–95. doi:10.1002/mrdd.20141. PMC 3315379. PMID 17326118.
  15. ^ Casanova MF (2007). "The neuropathology of autism". Brain Pathol. 17 (4): 422–33. doi:10.1111/j.1750-3639.2007.00100.x. PMC 8095561. PMID 17919128. S2CID 6959302.
  16. ^ Geschwind DH (2009). "Advances in autism". Annu Rev Med. 60: 367–80. doi:10.1146/annurev.med.60.053107.121225. PMC 3645857. PMID 19630577.
  17. ^ Courchesne E, Pierce K, Schumann CM, Redcay E, Buckwalter JA, Kennedy DP, Morgan J (2007). "Mapping early brain development in autism". Neuron. 56 (2): 399–413. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2007.10.016. PMID 17964254. S2CID 10662307.
  18. ^ Schmitz C, Rezaie P (2008). "The neuropathology of autism: where do we stand?". Neuropathol Appl Neurobiol. 34 (1): 4–11. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2990.2007.00872.x. PMID 17971078. S2CID 23551620.
  19. ^ a b c Persico AM, Bourgeron T (2006). "Searching for ways out of the autism maze: genetic, epigenetic and environmental clues". Trends Neurosci. 29 (7): 349–58. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2006.05.010. PMID 16808981. S2CID 26722022.
  20. ^ Südhof TC (2008). "Neuroligins and neurexins link synaptic function to cognitive disease". Nature. 455 (7215): 903–11. Bibcode:2008Natur.455..903S. doi:10.1038/nature07456. PMC 2673233. PMID 18923512.
  21. ^ Kelleher RJ, Bear MF (2008). "The autistic neuron: troubled translation?". Cell. 135 (3): 401–06. doi:10.1016/j.cell.2008.10.017. PMID 18984149. S2CID 619383.
  22. ^ Bear MF, Dölen G, Osterweil E, Nagarajan N (2008). "Fragile X: translation in action". Neuropsychopharmacology. 33 (1): 84–7. doi:10.1038/sj.npp.1301610. PMC 4327813. PMID 17940551.
  23. ^ Tuchman R, Moshé SL, Rapin I (2009). "Convulsing toward the pathophysiology of autism". Brain Dev. 31 (2): 95–103. doi:10.1016/j.braindev.2008.09.009. PMC 2734903. PMID 19006654.
  24. ^ Hsiao EY (2013). "Immune Dysregulation in Autism Spectrum Disorder". Neurobiology of Autism. International Review of Neurobiology. Vol. 113. pp. 269–302. doi:10.1016/B978-0-12-418700-9.00009-5. ISBN 9780124187009. PMID 24290389.
  25. ^ a b Onore C, Careaga M, Ashwood P (August 2011). "The role of immune dysfunction in the pathophysiology of autism". Brain, Behavior, and Immunity. 26 (3): 383–92. doi:10.1016/j.bbi.2011.08.007. PMC 3418145. PMID 21906670.
  26. ^ Rossignol DA, Frye RE (2014). "Evidence linking oxidative stress, mitochondrial dysfunction, and inflammation in the brain of individuals with autism". Frontiers in Physiology. 5: 150. doi:10.3389/fphys.2014.00150. PMC 4001006. PMID 24795645.
  27. ^ Patterson PH (July 2011). "Maternal infection and immune involvement in autism". Trends in Molecular Medicine. 17 (7): 389–94. doi:10.1016/j.molmed.2011.03.001. PMC 3135697. PMID 21482187.
  28. ^ Chaste P, Leboyer M (2012). "Autism risk factors: genes, environment, and gene-environment interactions". Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 14 (3): 281–92. doi:10.31887/DCNS.2012.14.3/pchaste. PMC 3513682. PMID 23226953.
  29. ^ Ashwood P, Wills S, Van de Water J (2006). . J Leukoc Biol. 80 (1): 1–15. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.329.777. doi:10.1189/jlb.1205707. PMID 16698940. S2CID 17531542. Archived from the original on 5 October 2006.
  30. ^ Lee BK, Magnusson C, Gardner RM, Blomström S, Newschaffer CJ, Burstyn I, Karlsson H, Dalman C (September 2014). "Maternal hospitalization with infection during pregnancy and risk of autism spectrum disorders". Brain, Behavior, and Immunity. 44: 100–105. doi:10.1016/j.bbi.2014.09.001. PMC 4418173. PMID 25218900.
  31. ^ Atladóttir HO, Thorsen P, Østergaard L, Schendel DE, Lemcke S, Abdallah M, Parner ET (December 2010). "Maternal infection requiring hospitalization during pregnancy and autism spectrum disorders". Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 40 (12): 1423–30. doi:10.1007/s10803-010-1006-y. PMID 20414802. S2CID 23471371.
  32. ^ Dölen G, Osterweil E, Rao BS, Smith GB, Auerbach BD, Chattarji S, Bear MF (2007). "Correction of fragile X syndrome in mice". Neuron. 56 (6): 955–62. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2007.12.001. PMC 2199268. PMID 18093519.
  33. ^ Hughes JR (2009). "Update on autism: A review of 1300 reports published in 2008". Epilepsy Behav. 16 (4): 569–89. doi:10.1016/j.yebeh.2009.09.023. PMID 19896907. S2CID 8013774.
  34. ^ Manzi B, Loizzo AL, Giana G, Curatolo P (2008). "Autism and metabolic diseases". J Child Neurol. 23 (3): 307–14. doi:10.1177/0883073807308698. PMID 18079313. S2CID 30809774.
  35. ^ MNS and autism:
    • Williams JH (2008). "Self–other relations in social development and autism: multiple roles for mirror neurons and other brain bases". Autism Res. 1 (2): 73–90. doi:10.1002/aur.15. PMID 19360654. S2CID 15269399.
    • Dinstein I, Thomas C, Behrmann M, Heeger DJ (2008). "A mirror up to nature". Curr Biol. 18 (1): R13–18. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2007.11.004. PMC 2517574. PMID 18177704.
  36. ^ a b Iacoboni M, Dapretto M (2006). "The mirror neuron system and the consequences of its dysfunction". Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 7 (12): 942–51. doi:10.1038/nrn2024. PMID 17115076. S2CID 9463011.
  37. ^ Frith U, Frith CD (2003). "Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B. 358 (1431): 459–73. doi:10.1098/rstb.2002.1218. PMC 1693139. PMID 12689373.
  38. ^ Hamilton AF (2008). "Emulation and mimicry for social interaction: a theoretical approach to imitation in autism". Q J Exp Psychol. 61 (1): 101–15. doi:10.1080/17470210701508798. PMID 18038342. S2CID 14569936.
  39. ^ Powell K (2004). "Opening a window to the autistic brain". PLOS Biol. 2 (8): E267. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0020267. PMC 509312. PMID 15314667.
  40. ^ Di Martino A, Ross K, Uddin LQ, Sklar AB, Castellanos FX, Milham MP (2009). "Functional brain correlates of social and nonsocial processes in autism spectrum disorders: an activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis". Biol Psychiatry. 65 (1): 63–74. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.09.022. PMC 2993772. PMID 18996505.
  41. ^ Broyd SJ, Demanuele C, Debener S, Helps SK, James CJ, Sonuga-Barke EJ (2009). "Default-mode brain dysfunction in mental disorders: a systematic review". Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 33 (3): 279–96. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.09.002. PMID 18824195. S2CID 7175805.
  42. ^ Just MA, Cherkassky VL, Keller TA, Kana RK, Minshew NJ (2007). "Functional and anatomical cortical underconnectivity in autism: evidence from an FMRI study of an executive function task and corpus callosum morphometry". Cereb Cortex. 17 (4): 951–61. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhl006. PMC 4500121. PMID 16772313.
  43. ^ Williams DL, Goldstein G, Minshew NJ (2006). "Neuropsychologic functioning in children with autism: further evidence for disordered complex information-processing". Child Neuropsychol. 12 (4–5): 279–98. doi:10.1080/09297040600681190. PMC 1803025. PMID 16911973.
  44. ^ Murias M, Webb SJ, Greenson J, Dawson G (2007). "Resting state cortical connectivity reflected in EEG coherence in individuals with autism". Biol Psychiatry. 62 (3): 270–73. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.11.012. PMC 2001237. PMID 17336944.
  45. ^ Minshew NJ, Williams DL (2007). "The new neurobiology of autism: cortex, connectivity, and neuronal organization". Arch Neurol. 64 (7): 945–50. doi:10.1001/archneur.64.7.945. PMC 2597785. PMID 17620483.
  46. ^ Jeste SS, Nelson CA (2009). "Event related potentials in the understanding of autism spectrum disorders: an analytical review". J Autism Dev Disord. 39 (3): 495–510. doi:10.1007/s10803-008-0652-9. PMC 4422389. PMID 18850262.
  47. ^ Roberts TP, Schmidt GL, Egeth M, Blaskey L, Rey MM, Edgar JC, Levy SE (2008). "Electrophysiological signatures: magnetoencephalographic studies of the neural correlates of language impairment in autism spectrum disorders". Int J Psychophysiol. 68 (2): 149–60. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.01.012. PMC 2397446. PMID 18336941.
  48. ^ Crespi B, Stead P, Elliot M (2010). "Evolution in health and medicine Sackler colloquium: Comparative genomics of autism and schizophrenia". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 107 (Suppl 1): 1736–41. Bibcode:2010PNAS..107.1736C. doi:10.1073/pnas.0906080106. PMC 2868282. PMID 19955444.
  49. ^ Ha S, Sohn IJ, Kim N, Sim HJ, Cheon KA (December 2015). "Characteristics of Brains in Autism Spectrum Disorder: Structure, Function and Connectivity across the Lifespan". Exp Neurobiol (Review). 24 (4): 273–84. doi:10.5607/en.2015.24.4.273. PMC 4688328. PMID 26713076.
  50. ^ a b Baron-Cohen S (2009). "Autism: the empathizing–systemizing (E-S) theory" (PDF). Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1156 (1): 68–80. Bibcode:2009NYASA1156...68B. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04467.x. PMID 19338503. S2CID 1440395.
  51. ^ Hamilton AF (2009). "Goals, intentions and mental states: challenges for theories of autism". J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 50 (8): 881–92. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.621.6275. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02098.x. PMID 19508497.
  52. ^ a b Kenworthy L, Yerys BE, Anthony LG, Wallace GL (2008). "Understanding executive control in autism spectrum disorders in the lab and in the real world". Neuropsychol Rev. 18 (4): 320–38. doi:10.1007/s11065-008-9077-7. PMC 2856078. PMID 18956239.
  53. ^ O'Hearn K, Asato M, Ordaz S, Luna B (2008). "Neurodevelopment and executive function in autism". Dev Psychopathol. 20 (4): 1103–32. doi:10.1017/S0954579408000527. PMID 18838033. S2CID 33559397.
  54. ^ Hill EL (2004). "Executive dysfunction in autism". Trends Cogn Sci. 8 (1): 26–32. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2004.01.001. PMID 14697400.
  55. ^ Sigman M, Spence SJ, Wang AT (2006). "Autism from developmental and neuropsychological perspectives". Annual Review of Clinical Psychology. 2: 327–55. doi:10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.2.022305.095210. PMID 17716073.
  56. ^ Happé F, Frith U (January 2006). "The weak coherence account: detail-focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders". Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 36 (1): 5–25. doi:10.1007/s10803-005-0039-0. PMID 16450045. S2CID 14999943.
  57. ^ Mottron L, Dawson M, Soulières I, Hubert B, Burack J (January 2006). "Enhanced perceptual functioning in autism: an update, and eight principles of autistic perception". Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 36 (1): 27–43. doi:10.1007/s10803-005-0040-7. PMID 16453071. S2CID 327253.
  58. ^ Murray D, Lesser M, Lawson W (May 2005). (PDF). Autism. 9 (2): 139–56. doi:10.1177/1362361305051398. PMID 15857859. S2CID 6476917. Archived from the original (PDF) on 19 May 2018. Retrieved 18 March 2018.
  59. ^ Happé F, Ronald A, Plomin R (2006). "Time to give up on a single explanation for autism". Nature Neuroscience. 9 (10): 1218–20. doi:10.1038/nn1770. PMID 17001340.
  60. ^ Rajendran G, Mitchell P (2007). "Cognitive theories of autism" (PDF). Dev Rev. 27 (2): 224–60. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2007.02.001.

mechanism, autism, autism, symptoms, result, from, maturation, related, changes, various, systems, brain, although, well, understood, autism, occurs, there, have, been, attempts, describe, mechanisms, involved, conceptually, divide, development, into, areas, p. Autism s symptoms result from maturation related changes in various systems of the brain Although it is not well understood how autism occurs there have been attempts to describe the mechanisms involved 1 Conceptually one can divide its development into two areas the pathophysiology of brain structures and processes associated with autism and the neuropsychological linkages between brain structures and behaviors 2 The behaviors appear to have multiple pathophysiologies 3 4 There is evidence that gut brain axis abnormalities may be involved 5 6 7 A 2015 review proposed that immune dysregulation gastrointestinal inflammation malfunction of the autonomic nervous system gut flora alterations and food metabolites may cause brain neuroinflammation and dysfunction 5 A 2016 review concludes that enteric nervous system abnormalities might play a role in neurological disorders such as autism Neural connections and the immune system are a pathway that may allow diseases originated in the intestine to spread to the brain 6 Several lines of evidence point to synaptic dysfunction as a cause of autism 8 Some rare mutations may lead to autism by disrupting some synaptic pathways such as those involved with cell adhesion 9 Gene replacement studies in mice suggest that autistic symptoms are closely related to later developmental steps that depend on activity in synapses and on activity dependent changes 10 All known teratogens agents that cause birth defects related to the risk of autism appear to act during the first eight weeks from conception and though this does not exclude the possibility that autism can be initiated or affected later there is strong evidence that autism arises very early in development 11 Pathophysiology Edit Autism affects the amygdala cerebellum and many other parts of the brain 12 Unlike many other brain disorders such as Parkinson s autism does not have a clear unifying mechanism at either the molecular cellular or systems level it is not known whether autism is a few disorders caused by mutations converging on a few common molecular pathways or is like intellectual disability a large set of disorders with diverse mechanisms 13 Autism appears to result from developmental factors that affect many or all functional brain systems 14 and to disturb the timing of brain development more than the final product 12 Neuroanatomical studies and the associations with teratogens strongly suggest that autism s mechanism includes alteration of brain development soon after conception 11 This anomaly appears to start a cascade of pathological events in the brain that are significantly influenced by environmental factors 15 Just after birth the brains of children with autism tend to grow faster than usual followed by normal or relatively slower growth in childhood It is not known whether early overgrowth occurs in all children with autism It seems to be most prominent in brain areas underlying the development of higher cognitive specialization 16 Hypotheses for the cellular and molecular bases of pathological early overgrowth include the following An excess of neurons that causes local overconnectivity in key brain regions 17 Disturbed neuronal migration during early gestation 18 19 Unbalanced excitatory inhibitory networks 19 Abnormal formation of synapses and dendritic spines 19 for example by modulation of the neurexin neuroligin cell adhesion system 20 or by poorly regulated synthesis of synaptic proteins 21 22 Disrupted synaptic development may also contribute to epilepsy which may explain why the two conditions are associated 23 The immune system is thought to play an important role in autism Children with autism have been found by researchers to have inflammation of both the peripheral and central immune systems as indicated by increased levels of pro inflammatory cytokines and significant activation of microglia 24 25 26 Biomarkers of abnormal immune function have also been associated with increased impairments in behaviors that are characteristic of the core features of autism such as deficits in social interactions and communication 25 Interactions between the immune system and the nervous system begin early during the embryonic stage of life and successful neurodevelopment depends on a balanced immune response It is thought that activation of a pregnant mother s immune system such as from environmental toxicants or infection can contribute to causing autism through causing a disruption of brain development 27 28 29 This is supported by recent studies that have found that infection during pregnancy is associated with an increased risk of autism 30 31 The relationship of neurochemicals to autism is not well understood several have been investigated with the most evidence for the role of serotonin and of genetic differences in its transport 8 The role of group I metabotropic glutamate receptors mGluR in the pathogenesis of fragile X syndrome the most common identified genetic cause of autism has led to interest in the possible implications for future autism research into this pathway 32 Some data suggests neuronal overgrowth potentially related to an increase in several growth hormones 33 or to impaired regulation of growth factor receptors Also some inborn errors of metabolism are associated with autism but probably account for less than 5 of cases 34 The mirror neuron system MNS theory of autism hypothesizes that distortion in the development of the MNS interferes with imitation and leads to autism s core features of social impairment and communication difficulties The MNS operates when an animal performs an action or observes another animal perform the same action The MNS may contribute to an individual s understanding of other people by enabling the modeling of their behavior via embodied simulation of their actions intentions and emotions 35 Several studies have tested this hypothesis by demonstrating structural abnormalities in MNS regions of individuals with ASD delay in the activation in the core circuit for imitation in individuals with ASD and a correlation between reduced MNS activity and severity of the syndrome in children with ASD 36 However individuals with autism also have abnormal brain activation in many circuits outside the MNS 37 and the MNS theory does not explain the normal performance of children with autism on imitation tasks that involve a goal or object 38 Autistic individuals tend to use different areas of the brain yellow for a movement task compared to a control group blue 39 ASD related patterns of low function and aberrant activation in the brain differ depending on whether the brain is doing social or nonsocial tasks 40 In autism there is evidence for reduced functional connectivity of the default network a large scale brain network involved in social and emotional processing with intact connectivity of the task positive network used in sustained attention and goal directed thinking In people with autism the two networks are not negatively correlated in time suggesting an imbalance in toggling between the two networks possibly reflecting a disturbance of self referential thought 41 The underconnectivity theory of autism hypothesizes that autism is marked by underfunctioning high level neural connections and synchronization along with an excess of low level processes 42 Evidence for this theory has been found in functional neuroimaging studies on autistic individuals 43 and by a brainwave study that suggested that adults with ASD have local overconnectivity in the cortex and weak functional connections between the frontal lobe and the rest of the cortex 44 Other evidence suggests the underconnectivity is mainly within each hemisphere of the cortex and that autism is a disorder of the association cortex 45 From studies based on event related potentials transient changes to the brain s electrical activity in response to stimuli there is considerable evidence for differences in autistic individuals with respect to attention orientation to auditory and visual stimuli novelty detection language and face processing and information storage several studies have found a preference for nonsocial stimuli 46 For example magnetoencephalography studies have found evidence in children with autism of delayed responses in the brain s processing of auditory signals 47 In the genetic area relations have been found between autism and schizophrenia based on duplications and deletions of chromosomes research showed that schizophrenia and autism are significantly more common in combination with 1q21 1 deletion syndrome Research on autism schizophrenia relations for chromosome 15 15q13 3 chromosome 16 16p13 1 and chromosome 17 17p12 are inconclusive 48 Functional connectivity studies have found both hypo and hyper connectivity in brains of people with autism Hypo connectivity seems to dominate especially for interhemispheric and cortico cortical functional connectivity 49 Neuropsychology EditTwo major categories of cognitive theories have been proposed about the links between autistic brains and behavior The first category focuses on deficits in social cognition Simon Baron Cohen s empathizing systemizing theory postulates that autistic individuals can systemize that is they can develop internal rules of operation to handle events inside the brain but are less effective at empathizing by handling events generated by other agents An extension the extreme male brain theory hypothesizes that autism is an extreme case of the male brain defined psychometrically as individuals in whom systemizing is better than empathizing 50 These theories are somewhat related to Baron Cohen s earlier theory of mind approach which hypothesizes that autistic behavior arises from an inability to ascribe mental states to oneself and others The theory of mind hypothesis is supported by the atypical responses of children with autism to the Sally Anne test for reasoning about others motivations 50 and the mirror neuron system theory of autism described in Pathophysiology maps well to the hypothesis 36 However most studies have found no evidence of impairment in autistic individuals ability to understand other people s basic intentions or goals instead data suggests that impairments are found in understanding more complex social emotions or in considering others viewpoints 51 The second category focuses on nonsocial or general processing the executive functions such as working memory planning inhibition In his review Kenworthy states that the claim of executive dysfunction as a causal factor in autism is controversial however it is clear that executive dysfunction plays a role in the social and cognitive deficits observed in individuals with autism 52 Tests of core executive processes such as eye movement tasks indicate improvement from late childhood to adolescence but performance never reaches typical adult levels 53 A strength of the theory is predicting stereotyped behavior and narrow interests 54 two weaknesses are that executive function is hard to measure 52 and that executive function deficits have not been found in young children with autism 55 Weak central coherence theory hypothesizes that a limited ability to see the big picture underlies the central disturbance in autism One strength of this theory is predicting special talents and peaks in performance in autistic people 56 A related theory enhanced perceptual functioning focuses more on the superiority of locally oriented and perceptual operations in autistic individuals 57 Yet another monotropism posits that autism stems from a different cognitive style tending to focus attention or processing resources intensely to the exclusion of other stimuli 58 These theories map well from the underconnectivity theory of autism Neither category is satisfactory on its own social cognition theories poorly address autism s rigid and repetitive behaviors while most of the nonsocial theories have difficulty explaining social impairment and communication difficulties 59 A combined theory based on multiple deficits may prove to be more useful 60 References Edit Sarovic D November 2021 A Unifying Theory for Autism The Pathogenetic Triad as a Theoretical Framework Frontiers in Psychiatry Review 12 767075 doi 10 3389 fpsyt 2021 767075 PMC 8637925 PMID 34867553 S2CID 244119594 Penn HE 2006 Neurobiological correlates of autism a review of recent research Child Neuropsychol 12 1 57 79 doi 10 1080 09297040500253546 PMID 16484102 S2CID 46119993 London E 2007 The role of the neurobiologist in redefining the diagnosis of autism Brain Pathol 17 4 408 11 doi 10 1111 j 1750 3639 2007 00103 x PMC 8095627 PMID 17919126 S2CID 24860348 Baird G Cass H Slonims V 2003 Diagnosis of autism BMJ 327 7413 488 93 doi 10 1136 bmj 327 7413 488 PMC 188387 PMID 12946972 a b Wasilewska J Klukowski M 2015 Gastrointestinal symptoms and autism spectrum disorder links and risks a possible new overlap syndrome Pediatric Health Med Ther Review 6 153 166 doi 10 2147 PHMT S85717 PMC 5683266 PMID 29388597 a b Rao M Gershon MD September 2016 The bowel and beyond the enteric nervous system in neurological disorders Nat Rev Gastroenterol Hepatol Review 13 9 517 28 doi 10 1038 nrgastro 2016 107 PMC 5005185 PMID 27435372 Israelyan N Margolis KG 2018 Serotonin as a link between the gut brain microbiome axis in autism spectrum disorders Pharmacol Res Review 132 1 6 doi 10 1016 j phrs 2018 03 020 PMC 6368356 PMID 29614380 a b Levy SE Mandell DS Schultz RT 2009 Autism Lancet 374 9701 1627 38 doi 10 1016 S0140 6736 09 61376 3 PMC 2863325 PMID 19819542 Betancur C Sakurai T Buxbaum JD 2009 The emerging role of synaptic cell adhesion pathways in the pathogenesis of autism spectrum disorders Trends Neurosci 32 7 402 12 doi 10 1016 j tins 2009 04 003 PMID 19541375 S2CID 8644511 Walsh CA Morrow EM Rubenstein JL 2008 Autism and brain development Cell 135 3 396 400 doi 10 1016 j cell 2008 10 015 PMC 2701104 PMID 18984148 a b Arndt TL Stodgell CJ Rodier PM 2005 The teratology of autism Int J Dev Neurosci 23 2 3 189 99 doi 10 1016 j ijdevneu 2004 11 001 PMID 15749245 S2CID 17797266 a b Amaral DG Schumann CM Nordahl CW 2008 Neuroanatomy of autism Trends Neurosci 31 3 137 45 doi 10 1016 j tins 2007 12 005 PMID 18258309 S2CID 18648870 Geschwind DH 2008 Autism many genes common pathways Cell 135 3 391 95 doi 10 1016 j cell 2008 10 016 PMC 2756410 PMID 18984147 Muller RA 2007 The study of autism as a distributed disorder Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev 13 1 85 95 doi 10 1002 mrdd 20141 PMC 3315379 PMID 17326118 Casanova MF 2007 The neuropathology of autism Brain Pathol 17 4 422 33 doi 10 1111 j 1750 3639 2007 00100 x PMC 8095561 PMID 17919128 S2CID 6959302 Geschwind DH 2009 Advances in autism Annu Rev Med 60 367 80 doi 10 1146 annurev med 60 053107 121225 PMC 3645857 PMID 19630577 Courchesne E Pierce K Schumann CM Redcay E Buckwalter JA Kennedy DP Morgan J 2007 Mapping early brain development in autism Neuron 56 2 399 413 doi 10 1016 j neuron 2007 10 016 PMID 17964254 S2CID 10662307 Schmitz C Rezaie P 2008 The neuropathology of autism where do we stand Neuropathol Appl Neurobiol 34 1 4 11 doi 10 1111 j 1365 2990 2007 00872 x PMID 17971078 S2CID 23551620 a b c Persico AM Bourgeron T 2006 Searching for ways out of the autism maze genetic epigenetic and environmental clues Trends Neurosci 29 7 349 58 doi 10 1016 j tins 2006 05 010 PMID 16808981 S2CID 26722022 Sudhof TC 2008 Neuroligins and neurexins link synaptic function to cognitive disease Nature 455 7215 903 11 Bibcode 2008Natur 455 903S doi 10 1038 nature07456 PMC 2673233 PMID 18923512 Kelleher RJ Bear MF 2008 The autistic neuron troubled translation Cell 135 3 401 06 doi 10 1016 j cell 2008 10 017 PMID 18984149 S2CID 619383 Bear MF Dolen G Osterweil E Nagarajan N 2008 Fragile X translation in action Neuropsychopharmacology 33 1 84 7 doi 10 1038 sj npp 1301610 PMC 4327813 PMID 17940551 Tuchman R Moshe SL Rapin I 2009 Convulsing toward the pathophysiology of autism Brain Dev 31 2 95 103 doi 10 1016 j braindev 2008 09 009 PMC 2734903 PMID 19006654 Hsiao EY 2013 Immune Dysregulation in Autism Spectrum Disorder Neurobiology of Autism International Review of Neurobiology Vol 113 pp 269 302 doi 10 1016 B978 0 12 418700 9 00009 5 ISBN 9780124187009 PMID 24290389 a b Onore C Careaga M Ashwood P August 2011 The role of immune dysfunction in the pathophysiology of autism Brain Behavior and Immunity 26 3 383 92 doi 10 1016 j bbi 2011 08 007 PMC 3418145 PMID 21906670 Rossignol DA Frye RE 2014 Evidence linking oxidative stress mitochondrial dysfunction and inflammation in the brain of individuals with autism Frontiers in Physiology 5 150 doi 10 3389 fphys 2014 00150 PMC 4001006 PMID 24795645 Patterson PH July 2011 Maternal infection and immune involvement in autism Trends in Molecular Medicine 17 7 389 94 doi 10 1016 j molmed 2011 03 001 PMC 3135697 PMID 21482187 Chaste P Leboyer M 2012 Autism risk factors genes environment and gene environment interactions Dialogues Clin Neurosci 14 3 281 92 doi 10 31887 DCNS 2012 14 3 pchaste PMC 3513682 PMID 23226953 Ashwood P Wills S Van de Water J 2006 The immune response in autism a new frontier for autism research J Leukoc Biol 80 1 1 15 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 329 777 doi 10 1189 jlb 1205707 PMID 16698940 S2CID 17531542 Archived from the original on 5 October 2006 Lee BK Magnusson C Gardner RM Blomstrom S Newschaffer CJ Burstyn I Karlsson H Dalman C September 2014 Maternal hospitalization with infection during pregnancy and risk of autism spectrum disorders Brain Behavior and Immunity 44 100 105 doi 10 1016 j bbi 2014 09 001 PMC 4418173 PMID 25218900 Atladottir HO Thorsen P Ostergaard L Schendel DE Lemcke S Abdallah M Parner ET December 2010 Maternal infection requiring hospitalization during pregnancy and autism spectrum disorders Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 40 12 1423 30 doi 10 1007 s10803 010 1006 y PMID 20414802 S2CID 23471371 Dolen G Osterweil E Rao BS Smith GB Auerbach BD Chattarji S Bear MF 2007 Correction of fragile X syndrome in mice Neuron 56 6 955 62 doi 10 1016 j neuron 2007 12 001 PMC 2199268 PMID 18093519 Hughes JR 2009 Update on autism A review of 1300 reports published in 2008 Epilepsy Behav 16 4 569 89 doi 10 1016 j yebeh 2009 09 023 PMID 19896907 S2CID 8013774 Manzi B Loizzo AL Giana G Curatolo P 2008 Autism and metabolic diseases J Child Neurol 23 3 307 14 doi 10 1177 0883073807308698 PMID 18079313 S2CID 30809774 MNS and autism Williams JH 2008 Self other relations in social development and autism multiple roles for mirror neurons and other brain bases Autism Res 1 2 73 90 doi 10 1002 aur 15 PMID 19360654 S2CID 15269399 Dinstein I Thomas C Behrmann M Heeger DJ 2008 A mirror up to nature Curr Biol 18 1 R13 18 doi 10 1016 j cub 2007 11 004 PMC 2517574 PMID 18177704 a b Iacoboni M Dapretto M 2006 The mirror neuron system and the consequences of its dysfunction Nature Reviews Neuroscience 7 12 942 51 doi 10 1038 nrn2024 PMID 17115076 S2CID 9463011 Frith U Frith CD 2003 Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 358 1431 459 73 doi 10 1098 rstb 2002 1218 PMC 1693139 PMID 12689373 Hamilton AF 2008 Emulation and mimicry for social interaction a theoretical approach to imitation in autism Q J Exp Psychol 61 1 101 15 doi 10 1080 17470210701508798 PMID 18038342 S2CID 14569936 Powell K 2004 Opening a window to the autistic brain PLOS Biol 2 8 E267 doi 10 1371 journal pbio 0020267 PMC 509312 PMID 15314667 Di Martino A Ross K Uddin LQ Sklar AB Castellanos FX Milham MP 2009 Functional brain correlates of social and nonsocial processes in autism spectrum disorders an activation likelihood estimation meta analysis Biol Psychiatry 65 1 63 74 doi 10 1016 j biopsych 2008 09 022 PMC 2993772 PMID 18996505 Broyd SJ Demanuele C Debener S Helps SK James CJ Sonuga Barke EJ 2009 Default mode brain dysfunction in mental disorders a systematic review Neurosci Biobehav Rev 33 3 279 96 doi 10 1016 j neubiorev 2008 09 002 PMID 18824195 S2CID 7175805 Just MA Cherkassky VL Keller TA Kana RK Minshew NJ 2007 Functional and anatomical cortical underconnectivity in autism evidence from an FMRI study of an executive function task and corpus callosum morphometry Cereb Cortex 17 4 951 61 doi 10 1093 cercor bhl006 PMC 4500121 PMID 16772313 Williams DL Goldstein G Minshew NJ 2006 Neuropsychologic functioning in children with autism further evidence for disordered complex information processing Child Neuropsychol 12 4 5 279 98 doi 10 1080 09297040600681190 PMC 1803025 PMID 16911973 Murias M Webb SJ Greenson J Dawson G 2007 Resting state cortical connectivity reflected in EEG coherence in individuals with autism Biol Psychiatry 62 3 270 73 doi 10 1016 j biopsych 2006 11 012 PMC 2001237 PMID 17336944 Minshew NJ Williams DL 2007 The new neurobiology of autism cortex connectivity and neuronal organization Arch Neurol 64 7 945 50 doi 10 1001 archneur 64 7 945 PMC 2597785 PMID 17620483 Jeste SS Nelson CA 2009 Event related potentials in the understanding of autism spectrum disorders an analytical review J Autism Dev Disord 39 3 495 510 doi 10 1007 s10803 008 0652 9 PMC 4422389 PMID 18850262 Roberts TP Schmidt GL Egeth M Blaskey L Rey MM Edgar JC Levy SE 2008 Electrophysiological signatures magnetoencephalographic studies of the neural correlates of language impairment in autism spectrum disorders Int J Psychophysiol 68 2 149 60 doi 10 1016 j ijpsycho 2008 01 012 PMC 2397446 PMID 18336941 Crespi B Stead P Elliot M 2010 Evolution in health and medicine Sackler colloquium Comparative genomics of autism and schizophrenia Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 107 Suppl 1 1736 41 Bibcode 2010PNAS 107 1736C doi 10 1073 pnas 0906080106 PMC 2868282 PMID 19955444 Ha S Sohn IJ Kim N Sim HJ Cheon KA December 2015 Characteristics of Brains in Autism Spectrum Disorder Structure Function and Connectivity across the Lifespan Exp Neurobiol Review 24 4 273 84 doi 10 5607 en 2015 24 4 273 PMC 4688328 PMID 26713076 a b Baron Cohen S 2009 Autism the empathizing systemizing E S theory PDF Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1156 1 68 80 Bibcode 2009NYASA1156 68B doi 10 1111 j 1749 6632 2009 04467 x PMID 19338503 S2CID 1440395 Hamilton AF 2009 Goals intentions and mental states challenges for theories of autism J Child Psychol Psychiatry 50 8 881 92 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 621 6275 doi 10 1111 j 1469 7610 2009 02098 x PMID 19508497 a b Kenworthy L Yerys BE Anthony LG Wallace GL 2008 Understanding executive control in autism spectrum disorders in the lab and in the real world Neuropsychol Rev 18 4 320 38 doi 10 1007 s11065 008 9077 7 PMC 2856078 PMID 18956239 O Hearn K Asato M Ordaz S Luna B 2008 Neurodevelopment and executive function in autism Dev Psychopathol 20 4 1103 32 doi 10 1017 S0954579408000527 PMID 18838033 S2CID 33559397 Hill EL 2004 Executive dysfunction in autism Trends Cogn Sci 8 1 26 32 doi 10 1016 j dr 2004 01 001 PMID 14697400 Sigman M Spence SJ Wang AT 2006 Autism from developmental and neuropsychological perspectives Annual Review of Clinical Psychology 2 327 55 doi 10 1146 annurev clinpsy 2 022305 095210 PMID 17716073 Happe F Frith U January 2006 The weak coherence account detail focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 36 1 5 25 doi 10 1007 s10803 005 0039 0 PMID 16450045 S2CID 14999943 Mottron L Dawson M Soulieres I Hubert B Burack J January 2006 Enhanced perceptual functioning in autism an update and eight principles of autistic perception Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 36 1 27 43 doi 10 1007 s10803 005 0040 7 PMID 16453071 S2CID 327253 Murray D Lesser M Lawson W May 2005 Attention monotropism and the diagnostic criteria for autism PDF Autism 9 2 139 56 doi 10 1177 1362361305051398 PMID 15857859 S2CID 6476917 Archived from the original PDF on 19 May 2018 Retrieved 18 March 2018 Happe F Ronald A Plomin R 2006 Time to give up on a single explanation for autism Nature Neuroscience 9 10 1218 20 doi 10 1038 nn1770 PMID 17001340 Rajendran G Mitchell P 2007 Cognitive theories of autism PDF Dev Rev 27 2 224 60 doi 10 1016 j dr 2007 02 001 Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Mechanism of autism amp oldid 1113519691, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.