fbpx
Wikipedia

Math–verbal achievement gap

The math–verbal achievement gap is a phenomenon first observed by Richard Rothstein in a brief 2002 article written in The New York Times.[1] This achievement gap reveals a growing disparity in the United States between the rising national average on the math portions of the college entry SAT and ACT exams, as opposed to the flat-lining verbal portions on the same tests.

Graph showing the Math–Verbal Achievement Gap in the United States

Historical trends edit

Around 1990 the national average on the math portion of the SAT began its slow but steady ascension over the national average for the verbal portion. It took only one decade for the math average to eclipse the verbal average, continuing to widen since that point. The difference was sizable and significant: in 1992, the average math score on the SAT was 501, similar to the average verbal score of 500. In the decade that followed, the math score increased by fifteen points while the verbal score increased by only four. However, this was not always the case. In fact, in the 1970s the relationship was precisely the opposite when national verbal scores routinely trumped the national math average by similar margins.[1] Since then, the point gap between the math and evidence-based reading and writing (EBRW) tests has closed significantly: the class of 2019 had an average math test score of 528 and an average EBRW score of 531. Because 50 more points are required to meet the math benchmark than the EBRW benchmark, only 48% of the class of 2019 met the math benchmark while 68% met the EBRW benchmark.[2]

The first academic analysis of this gap was conducted by James Lech Ed.D., M.T.S.[3] Lech posited that this gap could be "collateral damage" from the well-documented academic and social "erosion" known as dumbing-down.[4] In other words, the SAT math questions progressively eased in difficulty over time while the verbal difficulty levels 1) remained constant, or 2) dumbed-down at much slower rates.

Testing the "dumbing down" hypothesis edit

To test that hypothesis Lech constructed scores of survey instruments compiled from the math questions derived from publicly released SATs from the early 1980s through 2005. The survey instruments were intentionally created to conceal the source of the questions in order to reduce reader-bias towards standardized testing. They were then disseminated to more than 1,500 qualified high school math teachers[5] that were randomly selected from a pool of every high school (both public and private) in the United States. Their responses were tabulated. Lech found that the SAT math questions were not getting easier at all in the eyes of these math teachers; in fact they were getting slightly harder.

Alternative hypotheses edit

The research hypothesis Lech tested was rejected: the questions on the math portions of the SAT are not getting easier over time. As noted, they are getting a little harder. This confounds the math-verbal achievement gap in general. There were several proposals put forth to explain rising national averages, year-after-year, on the difficult math portions of the SAT, as opposed to stagnating national verbal averages on the same test, during the same three decades.

Rothstein's theories edit

Rothstein, in the 2002 New York Times article, admitted that "Nobody really knows why we seem to make more progress in math than reading. But one likely cause is that students learn math mostly in school, while literacy also comes from habits at home. Even if reading instruction improves, scores would suffer if students did less out-of-school reading or had a less literate home environment."

Rothstein also suggested that the hugely successful SAT and ACT test preparation courses are unwittingly emphasizing mathematical prowess over verbal acuity. He rationalized that successful test-taking techniques taught in these courses to boost test scores work very well for multiple-choice math questions.[6]

Rothstein also highlighted that English-language-learners (hereinafter ELL) in American schools were on the increase, and certainly that could have a downward pull on the national verbal average on these standardized tests. It would seem likely that ELL status would adversely affect a student's verbal score to a greater extent as opposed to the mathematics portion on the same standardized test.

Finally, Rothstein indicated that studies revealed then, as they still do now, that student reading was on the decline while television-watching by American youth was on the increase. These dual social trends, when combined, could negatively impact student verbal scores at higher rates than their math scores.[6] These behaviors also lower math scores—a point that Rothstein could have overlooked.[7][8] What Rothstein could not anticipate in 2002 is the exponential proliferation of text messages teenagers send and receive, and the potential for negative ramifications of that activity when it comes to progressing with their verbal skills. The New York Times notes that this "phenomenon is beginning to worry physicians and psychologists, who say it is leading to anxiety, distraction in school, falling grades, repetitive stress injury and sleep deprivation." [9] There may indeed be cause for concern as Neilsen's reported in 2008 that "American teenagers sent and received an average of 2,272 text messages per month in the fourth quarter of 2008 ... almost 80 messages a day, more than double the average of a year earlier (Ibid.). Neilsen's 2010 report indicates a 47 percent increase in teen texting to 3,339 texts per month (4,050 for female teens).[10]

Lech's theories edit

Lech's alternative suggestions for further research in that 2007 dissertation indicated that it is quite likely the math–verbal achievement gap could be partially explained by the rapid proliferation of math and science Advanced Placement courses in American high schools. This wave of science and math AP courses, over that of humanities AP courses, is rising concurrently with higher math scores over that of verbal scores on these standardized tests.[11] He added that this effect could be multiplied if large numbers of savvy students are front-loading the math and science AP courses in high school to intentionally "bone-up" for corresponding sections on the SAT/ACT. Indeed there does seem to be a solid preference for better math and science marks by college admission personnel. This would simultaneously explain both better scores on the math portions of the SAT/ACT and stagnating verbal scores on those tests. After all, AP English in 12th grade will not provide praxis for college-entry tests taken in 11th grade.

Lech also contended that migration from South Asia, West Asia, and East Asia during these same decades could also be positively influencing these rising SAT math scores (while simultaneously suppressing the verbal scores as many of those students may also be ELL students as well). That's because he noted the number of Asian students taking the SAT are disproportionately overrepresented during the very years when this math–verbal gap really takes form. Of the students taking the SAT, the proportion or percentage who are Asian is more than double the Asian proportion [or percentage] in the U.S. population.[12] That is significant because, as Lech pointed out, Asian students themselves averaged 580 (out of a possible 800) on the math portion of the SAT in 2005, but the national math average that same year (which mathematically includes those high-scoring Asian students) was only 520.[13]

Lastly, Lech agreed with Rothstein that another influence on the math–verbal achievement gap could be the quality of the math teachers themselves. Lech referenced literature indicating that U.S. math teachers tend to be much better organized than their humanities counterparts across the hall.[14] Rothstein held that American math teachers tend to be better trained because they have more resources available, such as the extremely large $1 billion cash grant from the National Science Foundation for math and science instruction in high school, which English teachers cannot reap.

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ a b Rothstein, Richard (August 28, 2002). "LESSONS; Sums vs. Summarizing: SAT's Math–Verbal Gap". New York Times. Retrieved June 8, 2010.
  2. ^ "SAT Results – 2019 SAT Suite of Assessments Program Results – The College Board". College Board Program Results. 2019-09-10. Retrieved 2020-08-30.
  3. ^ The math–verbal achievement gap: Are we getting better at math, or are the SAT questions getting easier? (Thesis).- Total Dissertation Pages: 34; United States Copyright Office Registration Number: TX 6-534-442; AAT 3263829
  4. ^ Washburn, Katharine (1997). Dumbing down: essays on the strip mining of American culture (1997 ed.). W.W. Norton. ISBN 978-0-393-31723-7.
  5. ^ A qualified High School Math teacher was considered a person with a B.S. in mathematics and a handful of years of instruction, or a teacher without a math degree that taught Algebra II or higher for a significant number of years.
  6. ^ a b Rich, Motoko (November 19, 2007). "Study Links Drop in Test Scores to a Decline in Time Spent Reading". New York Times. Retrieved June 8, 2010.
  7. ^ Huang, Fali; Myoung-Jae Lee (2010). "Dynamic Treatment of Effect Analysis of TV Effects" (PDF). Journal of Applied Econometrics. 25 (3): 392–419. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.523.6941. doi:10.1002/jae.1165. S2CID 2728836. Retrieved 16 May 2011.
  8. ^ Borzekowski, Dina; Thomas N. Robinson (2005). "The Remote, the Mouse, and the No. 2 Pencil: The Household Media Environment and Academic Achievement Among Third Grade Students". Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine. 159 (7): 607–613. doi:10.1001/archpedi.159.7.607. PMID 15996991.
  9. ^ Hafner, Katie (May 29, 2009). "Texting May Be Taking a Toll". New York Times. Retrieved 16 May 2011.
  10. ^ "U.S. Teen Mobile Report: Calling Yesterday, Texting Today, Using Apps Tomorrow". Neilsen Wire. Retrieved 16 May 2011.
  11. ^ "The College Board's Archived AP research and reports". professionals.collegeboard.com. 2010. Retrieved June 8, 2010.
  12. ^ "The Asian Population: 2000" (PDF). Retrieved 2010-06-02.
  13. ^ "Archived SAT Data & Reports". professionals.collegeboard.com. 2010. Retrieved June 8, 2010.
  14. ^ Ravitch, Diane (2001). Left back: a century of failed school reforms (2001 ed.). Simon & Schuster. p. 440. ISBN 978-0-7432-0326-5.

External links edit

  • National Council of the Teachers of Mathematics

math, verbal, achievement, this, article, multiple, issues, please, help, improve, discuss, these, issues, talk, page, learn, when, remove, these, template, messages, major, contributor, this, article, appears, have, close, connection, with, subject, require, . This article has multiple issues Please help improve it or discuss these issues on the talk page Learn how and when to remove these template messages A major contributor to this article appears to have a close connection with its subject It may require cleanup to comply with Wikipedia s content policies particularly neutral point of view Please discuss further on the talk page October 2013 Learn how and when to remove this template message This article is written like a personal reflection personal essay or argumentative essay that states a Wikipedia editor s personal feelings or presents an original argument about a topic Please help improve it by rewriting it in an encyclopedic style October 2013 Learn how and when to remove this template message Learn how and when to remove this template message The math verbal achievement gap is a phenomenon first observed by Richard Rothstein in a brief 2002 article written in The New York Times 1 This achievement gap reveals a growing disparity in the United States between the rising national average on the math portions of the college entry SAT and ACT exams as opposed to the flat lining verbal portions on the same tests Graph showing the Math Verbal Achievement Gap in the United States Contents 1 Historical trends 2 Testing the dumbing down hypothesis 3 Alternative hypotheses 3 1 Rothstein s theories 3 2 Lech s theories 4 See also 5 References 6 External linksHistorical trends editAround 1990 the national average on the math portion of the SAT began its slow but steady ascension over the national average for the verbal portion It took only one decade for the math average to eclipse the verbal average continuing to widen since that point The difference was sizable and significant in 1992 the average math score on the SAT was 501 similar to the average verbal score of 500 In the decade that followed the math score increased by fifteen points while the verbal score increased by only four However this was not always the case In fact in the 1970s the relationship was precisely the opposite when national verbal scores routinely trumped the national math average by similar margins 1 Since then the point gap between the math and evidence based reading and writing EBRW tests has closed significantly the class of 2019 had an average math test score of 528 and an average EBRW score of 531 Because 50 more points are required to meet the math benchmark than the EBRW benchmark only 48 of the class of 2019 met the math benchmark while 68 met the EBRW benchmark 2 The first academic analysis of this gap was conducted by James Lech Ed D M T S 3 Lech posited that this gap could be collateral damage from the well documented academic and social erosion known as dumbing down 4 In other words the SAT math questions progressively eased in difficulty over time while the verbal difficulty levels 1 remained constant or 2 dumbed down at much slower rates Testing the dumbing down hypothesis editTo test that hypothesis Lech constructed scores of survey instruments compiled from the math questions derived from publicly released SATs from the early 1980s through 2005 The survey instruments were intentionally created to conceal the source of the questions in order to reduce reader bias towards standardized testing They were then disseminated to more than 1 500 qualified high school math teachers 5 that were randomly selected from a pool of every high school both public and private in the United States Their responses were tabulated Lech found that the SAT math questions were not getting easier at all in the eyes of these math teachers in fact they were getting slightly harder Alternative hypotheses editThe research hypothesis Lech tested was rejected the questions on the math portions of the SAT are not getting easier over time As noted they are getting a little harder This confounds the math verbal achievement gap in general There were several proposals put forth to explain rising national averages year after year on the difficult math portions of the SAT as opposed to stagnating national verbal averages on the same test during the same three decades Rothstein s theories edit Rothstein in the 2002 New York Times article admitted that Nobody really knows why we seem to make more progress in math than reading But one likely cause is that students learn math mostly in school while literacy also comes from habits at home Even if reading instruction improves scores would suffer if students did less out of school reading or had a less literate home environment Rothstein also suggested that the hugely successful SAT and ACT test preparation courses are unwittingly emphasizing mathematical prowess over verbal acuity He rationalized that successful test taking techniques taught in these courses to boost test scores work very well for multiple choice math questions 6 Rothstein also highlighted that English language learners hereinafter ELL in American schools were on the increase and certainly that could have a downward pull on the national verbal average on these standardized tests It would seem likely that ELL status would adversely affect a student s verbal score to a greater extent as opposed to the mathematics portion on the same standardized test Finally Rothstein indicated that studies revealed then as they still do now that student reading was on the decline while television watching by American youth was on the increase These dual social trends when combined could negatively impact student verbal scores at higher rates than their math scores 6 These behaviors also lower math scores a point that Rothstein could have overlooked 7 8 What Rothstein could not anticipate in 2002 is the exponential proliferation of text messages teenagers send and receive and the potential for negative ramifications of that activity when it comes to progressing with their verbal skills The New York Times notes that this phenomenon is beginning to worry physicians and psychologists who say it is leading to anxiety distraction in school falling grades repetitive stress injury and sleep deprivation 9 There may indeed be cause for concern as Neilsen s reported in 2008 that American teenagers sent and received an average of 2 272 text messages per month in the fourth quarter of 2008 almost 80 messages a day more than double the average of a year earlier Ibid Neilsen s 2010 report indicates a 47 percent increase in teen texting to 3 339 texts per month 4 050 for female teens 10 Lech s theories edit Lech s alternative suggestions for further research in that 2007 dissertation indicated that it is quite likely the math verbal achievement gap could be partially explained by the rapid proliferation of math and science Advanced Placement courses in American high schools This wave of science and math AP courses over that of humanities AP courses is rising concurrently with higher math scores over that of verbal scores on these standardized tests 11 He added that this effect could be multiplied if large numbers of savvy students are front loading the math and science AP courses in high school to intentionally bone up for corresponding sections on the SAT ACT Indeed there does seem to be a solid preference for better math and science marks by college admission personnel This would simultaneously explain both better scores on the math portions of the SAT ACT and stagnating verbal scores on those tests After all AP English in 12th grade will not provide praxis for college entry tests taken in 11th grade Lech also contended that migration from South Asia West Asia and East Asia during these same decades could also be positively influencing these rising SAT math scores while simultaneously suppressing the verbal scores as many of those students may also be ELL students as well That s because he noted the number of Asian students taking the SAT are disproportionately overrepresented during the very years when this math verbal gap really takes form Of the students taking the SAT the proportion or percentage who are Asian is more than double the Asian proportion or percentage in the U S population 12 That is significant because as Lech pointed out Asian students themselves averaged 580 out of a possible 800 on the math portion of the SAT in 2005 but the national math average that same year which mathematically includes those high scoring Asian students was only 520 13 Lastly Lech agreed with Rothstein that another influence on the math verbal achievement gap could be the quality of the math teachers themselves Lech referenced literature indicating that U S math teachers tend to be much better organized than their humanities counterparts across the hall 14 Rothstein held that American math teachers tend to be better trained because they have more resources available such as the extremely large 1 billion cash grant from the National Science Foundation for math and science instruction in high school which English teachers cannot reap See also editAchievement gap in the United StatesReferences edit a b Rothstein Richard August 28 2002 LESSONS Sums vs Summarizing SAT s Math Verbal Gap New York Times Retrieved June 8 2010 SAT Results 2019 SAT Suite of Assessments Program Results The College Board College Board Program Results 2019 09 10 Retrieved 2020 08 30 The math verbal achievement gap Are we getting better at math or are the SAT questions getting easier Thesis Total Dissertation Pages 34 United States Copyright Office Registration Number TX 6 534 442 AAT 3263829 Washburn Katharine 1997 Dumbing down essays on the strip mining of American culture 1997 ed W W Norton ISBN 978 0 393 31723 7 A qualified High School Math teacher was considered a person with a B S in mathematics and a handful of years of instruction or a teacher without a math degree that taught Algebra II or higher for a significant number of years a b Rich Motoko November 19 2007 Study Links Drop in Test Scores to a Decline in Time Spent Reading New York Times Retrieved June 8 2010 Huang Fali Myoung Jae Lee 2010 Dynamic Treatment of Effect Analysis of TV Effects PDF Journal of Applied Econometrics 25 3 392 419 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 523 6941 doi 10 1002 jae 1165 S2CID 2728836 Retrieved 16 May 2011 Borzekowski Dina Thomas N Robinson 2005 The Remote the Mouse and the No 2 Pencil The Household Media Environment and Academic Achievement Among Third Grade Students Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine 159 7 607 613 doi 10 1001 archpedi 159 7 607 PMID 15996991 Hafner Katie May 29 2009 Texting May Be Taking a Toll New York Times Retrieved 16 May 2011 U S Teen Mobile Report Calling Yesterday Texting Today Using Apps Tomorrow Neilsen Wire Retrieved 16 May 2011 The College Board s Archived AP research and reports professionals collegeboard com 2010 Retrieved June 8 2010 The Asian Population 2000 PDF Retrieved 2010 06 02 Archived SAT Data amp Reports professionals collegeboard com 2010 Retrieved June 8 2010 Ravitch Diane 2001 Left back a century of failed school reforms 2001 ed Simon amp Schuster p 440 ISBN 978 0 7432 0326 5 External links editNational Council of the Teachers of Mathematics Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Math verbal achievement gap amp oldid 1153066822, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.