fbpx
Wikipedia

Impact of health on intelligence

Health can affect intelligence in various ways. Conversely, intelligence can affect health. Health effects on intelligence have been described as being among the most important factors in the origins of human group differences in IQ test scores and other measures of cognitive ability.[1] Several factors can lead to significant cognitive impairment, particularly if they occur during pregnancy and childhood when the brain is growing and the blood–brain barrier of the child is less effective. Such impairment may sometimes be permanent, sometimes be partially or wholly compensated for by later growth.

Developed nations have implemented several health policies regarding nutrients and toxins known to influence cognitive function. These include laws requiring fortification of certain food products and laws establishing safe levels of pollutants (e.g. lead, mercury, and organochlorides). Comprehensive policy recommendations targeting reduction of cognitive impairment in children have been proposed.[2][3]

Improvements in nutrition (often involving specific micronutrients) due to public policy changes have been implicated in IQ increases in many nations (as part of the overall Flynn effect), such as efforts fighting iodine deficiency in the U.S.[4]

Nutrition

Malnutrition may occur during several periods of growth, such as pregnancy, during breastfeeding, infancy, or childhood. It may also happen due to deficiencies of different nutrients, such as micronutrients, protein or energy. This may cause different effects.

Timing

Some observers have argued that malnutrition during the first six months of life harms cognitive development much more than malnutrition later in life. However, a study from the Philippines argues that malnutrition in the second year of life may have a larger negative impact than malnutrition in the first year of life. While it is debatable whether or not as an infant or after two years is the worst time for malnourishment, the bottom line in these studies is that not having enough nutrients at a young age negatively effects learning. [5]

Intrauterine growth retardation

Undernutrition during pregnancy, and other factors, may cause intrauterine growth retardation (IUGR), which is one cause of low birth weight. However, it has been suggested that in IUGR the brain may be selectively spared. Brain growth is usually less affected than whole body weight or length. Several studies from developed nations have found that with the exception of extreme intrauterine growth retardation also affecting brain growth, and hypoxic injury, IUGR seems to have little or no measurable effect on mental performance and behavior in adolescence or adulthood. For example, acute undernutrition for a few months during the Dutch famine of 1944 caused a decrease in mean birthweight in certain areas. This was later associated with a change in performance on IQ tests for 18–19 years old Dutch males draftees from these areas compared to control areas. The subjects were exposed to famine prenatally but not after birth. During the famine, births decreased more among those with lower socioeconomic status (SES), whereas after the famine, there was a compensatory increase in births among those with lower SES. Since SES correlates with IQ, this may have hidden an effect caused by the undernutrition.[6]

Breastfeeding

Studies often find higher IQ in children and adults who were breastfed.[3][7] It has been proposed that omega-3 fatty acids in breast milk, known to be essential constituents of brain tissues, could at least partially account for an increase in IQ.

Recently, however, the longstanding belief that breastfeeding causes an increase in the IQ of offspring was challenged in a 2006 paper published in the British Medical Journal. The results indicated that mother's IQ, not breastfeeding, explained the differences in the IQ scores of offspring measured between ages 5 and 14. The results of this meta-analysis argued that prior studies had not controlled for the mother's IQ. Since mother's IQ was predictive of whether a child was breastfed, the study concluded that "breast feeding [itself] has little or no effect on intelligence in children." Instead, it was the mother's IQ that had a significant correlation with the IQ of her offspring, whether the offspring was breastfed or was not breastfed.[8]

One study found that breastfeeding was linked to raised IQ (as much as 7 points when not controlling for maternal IQ) if the infants had an SNP coding for a "C" rather than G base within the FADS2 gene. Those with the "G" version showed no IQ advantage, suggesting a biochemical interaction of child's genes on the effect of breastfeeding.[9][10] Other studies have failed to replicate any correlation between the FADS2 gene,[11] breastfeeding and IQ, while others show a negative effect on IQ when combining bottled feeding, and the "G" version of FADS2.[12]

Infancy

Two studies in Chile on 18-year-old high-school graduates found that nutritional status during the first year of life affected IQ, scholastic achievement, and brain volume.[13][14]

Micronutrients and vitamin deficiencies

Micronutrient deficiencies (e.g. in iodine and iron) influence the development of intelligence and remain a problem in the developing world. For example, iodine deficiency causes a fall, on average, of 12 IQ points. These deficiencies could technically show up in medical scans at various ages. [15]

Policy recommendations to increase availability of micronutrient supplements have been made and justified in part by the potential to counteract intelligence-related developmental problems. For example, the Copenhagen consensus, states that lack of both iodine and iron has been implicated in impaired brain development, and this can affect enormous numbers of people: it is estimated that 2 billion people (one-third of the total global population) are affected by iodine deficiency, including 285 million 6- to 12-year-old children. In developing countries, it is estimated that 40% of children aged four and under have anaemia because of insufficient iron in their diets.[16]

A joint statement on vitamin and mineral deficiencies says that the severity of such deficiencies "means the impairment of hundreds of millions of growing minds and the lowering of national IQs." Since the brain is not fully developed until age 25, this can be effecting people through their late teens.[17]

Overall, studies investigating whether cognitive function in already iron-deficient children can be improved with iron supplements have produced mixed results, possibly because deficiency in critical growth periods may cause irreversible damage. However, several studies with better design have shown substantial benefits. One way to prevent iron deficiency is to give specific supplementation to children, for example as tablets. However, this is costly, distribution mechanisms are often ineffective, and compliance is low. Fortification of staple foods (cereals, flour, sugar, salt) to deliver micronutrients to children on a large scale is probably the most sustainable and affordable option, even though commitment from governments and the food industry is needed.[18] Developed nations fortify several foods with various micronutrients.[19]

Additional vitamin-mineral supplementation may have an effect also in the developed world. A study giving such supplementation to "working class," primarily Hispanic, 6–12-year-old children in the United States for 3 months found an average increase of 2 to 3 IQ points. Most of this can be explained by the very large increase of a subgroup of the children, presumably because these were not adequately nourished unlike the majority. The study suggests that parents of schoolchildren whose academic performance is substandard would be well advised to seek a nutritionally oriented physician for assessment of their children's nutritional status as a possible etiology.[20]

More speculatively, other nutrients may prove important in the future. Vitamin B12 and folate may be important for cognitive function in old age.[21] Fish oil supplement to pregnant and lactating mothers has been linked to increased cognitive ability in one study.[22]

Another study found that pregnant women who consumed 340 grams of low-mercury containing fish with fatty acids per week have benefits that outweigh the risks for mercury poisoning. They were less likely to have children with low verbal IQ, motor coordination and behavioral problems. However, foods containing high amounts of mercury, such as shark, swordfish, king mackerel and tilefish, might cause mental retardation.[23][24][25][26][27][28]

Protein and energy malnutrition

One study from a developing country, Guatemala, found that poor growth during infancy, rather than low birth weight, was negatively related to adolescent performance on cognitive and achievement tests.[29] A later related very long-term study looked at the effect of giving 6–24-month-old children in Guatemala a high protein-energy drink as a dietary supplement. A significantly positive and fairly substantial effects was found on increasing the probability of attending school and of passing the first grade, increasing the grade attained by age 13, increasing completed schooling attainment, and for adults aged 25–40 increasing IQ test scores.[30]

Stunting

31% of children under the age of 5 in the developing world are moderately (height-for-age is below minus 2 standard deviations) or severely stunted (below minus 3 standard deviations).[31] The prevalence was even higher previously since the worldwide prevalence of stunting is declining by about half of a percentage point each year.[32] A study on stunted children aged 9–24 months in Jamaica found that when aged 17–18 years they had significantly poorer scores than a non-stunted group on cognitive and educational tests and psychosocial functioning. Giving a nutritional supplementation (1 kg milk based formula each week) to these already stunted children had no significant effect on later scores, but psychosocial stimulation (weekly play sessions with mother and child) had a positive effect.[33][34]

Toxins

Industrial chemicals

Certain toxins, such as lead, mercury, toluene, and PCB are well-known causes of neuro-developmental disorders. Recognition of these risks has led to evidence-based programmes of prevention, such as elimination of lead additives in petrol. Although these prevention campaigns are highly successful, most were initiated only after substantial delays.[35]

Policies to manage lead differ between nations, particularly between the developed and developing world. Use of leaded gasoline has been reduced or eliminated in most developed nations, and lead levels in US children have been substantially reduced by policies relating to lead reduction.[36] Even slightly elevated lead levels around the age of 24 months are associated with intellectual and academic performance deficits at age 10 years.[37]

Certain, at least previously, widely used organochlorides, such as dioxins, DDT, and PCB, have been associated with cognitive deficits.[38]

A Lancet review identified 201 chemicals with the ability to cause clinical neurotoxic effects in human adults, as described in the peer-reviewed scientific literature. Most of them are commonly used. Many additional chemicals have been shown to be neurotoxic in laboratory models. The article notes that children are more vulnerable and argues that new, precautionary approaches that recognise the unique vulnerability of the developing brain are needed for testing and control of chemicals in order to avoid the previous substantial before starting restrictions on usage.[39] An appendix listed further industrial chemicals considered to be neurotoxic.[40]

Alcohol and drugs

Fetal alcohol exposure, causing Fetal alcohol syndrome, is one of the leading known causes of intellectual disability in the Western world.[41]

Current cannabis use was found to be significantly correlated in a dose-dependent manner with a decline in IQ scores, during the effect of the use. However, no such decline was seen in subjects who had formerly been heavy cannabis users and had stopped taking the drug. The authors concluded that cannabis does not have a long-term effect on intelligence. However this is contradicted by the long-term longitudinal study, carried out by Otago and Duke universities, which found that regular use of marijuana in teenage years affects IQ in adulthood even when the use stops. The drop in IQ was 8 points. Adults smoking marijuana had no lasting effect on IQ.[42] Effects on fetal development are minimal when compared with the well-documented adverse effects of tobacco or alcohol use.[43]

Maternal tobacco smoking during pregnancy is associated with increased activity, decreased attention, and diminished intellectual abilities.[44] However, a recent study finds that maternal tobacco smoking has no direct causal effect on the child's IQ. Adjusting for maternal cognitive ability as measured by IQ and education eliminated the association between lower IQ and tobacco smoking.[45] But another study instead looking at the relationship between environmental tobacco smoke exposure, measured with a blood biomarker, and cognitive abilities among U.S. children and adolescents 6–16 years of age, found an inverse association between exposure and cognitive ability among children even at extremely low levels of exposure. The study controlled for sex, race, region, poverty, parent education and marital status, ferritin, and blood lead concentration.[46]

Healthcare during pregnancy and childbirth

Healthcare during pregnancy and childbirth, access to which is often governed by policy, also influences cognitive development. Preventable causes of low intelligence in children include infectious diseases such as meningitis, parasites, and cerebral malaria, prenatal drug and alcohol exposure, newborn asphyxia, low birth weight, head injuries, and endocrine disorders. A direct policy focus on determinants of childhood cognitive ability has been urged.[2]

Stress

A recent theory suggests that early childhood stress may affect the developing brain and cause negative effects.[47] Exposure to violence in childhood has been associated with lower school grades[48] and lower IQ in children of all races.[49] A group of largely African American urban first-grade children and their caregivers were evaluated using self-report, interview, and standardized tests, including IQ tests. The study reported that exposure to violence and trauma-related distress in young children were associated with substantial decrements in IQ and reading achievement. Exposure to Violence or Trauma lead to a 7.5-point (SD, 0.5) decrement in IQ and a 9.8-point (SD, 0.66) decrement in reading achievement.[48]

Violence may have a negative impact on IQ, or IQ may be protective against violence.[49] The causal mechanism and direction of causation is unknown.[48] Neighborhood risk has been related to lower school grades for African-American adolescents in another study from 2006.[50]

Infectious diseases

A 2010 study by Eppig, Fincher and Thornhill found a close correlation between the infectious disease burden in a country and the average IQ of its population. The researchers found that when disease was controlled for, IQ showed no correlation with other variables such as educational and nutritional levels. Since brain development requires a very high proportion of all the body's energy in newborns and children, the researchers argue that fighting infection reduces children's IQ potential. The Eppig research may help to explain the Flynn effect, the rise in intelligence noted in rich countries.[51] They also tested other hypotheses as well, including genetic explanations, concluding that infectious disease was "the best predictor".[52] Christopher Hassall and Thomas Sherratt repeated the analysis, and concluded "that infectious disease may be the only really important predictor of average national IQ".[52]

In order to mitigate the effects of education on IQ, Eppig, Fincher & Thornhill (2010) repeated their analysis across the United States where standardized and compulsory education exists.[52] The correlation between infectious disease and average IQ was confirmed, and they concluded that the "evidence suggests that infectious disease is a primary cause of the global variation in human intelligence".[52]

Tropical infectious diseases

Malaria affects 300–500 million persons each year, mostly children under age five in Africa, causing widespread anemia during a period of rapid brain development and also direct brain damage from cerebral malaria to which children are more vulnerable.[53] A 2006 systematic review found that Plasmodium falciparum infection causes cognitive deficits in both the short- and long-term.[54] Policies aimed at malaria reduction may have cognitive benefits. It has been suggested that the future economic and educational development of Africa critically depends on the eradication of malaria.

Roundworms infect hundreds of millions of people. There is evidence that high intensities of worms in the intestines can affect mental performance,[55] but a systematic review in 2000 and a 2009 update found that there was insufficient evidence to show that deworming treatments improve cognitive performance or school performance in children.[56][57]

HIV infection in children in sub-Saharan Africa affects their motor development, but there is insufficient evidence to show a slowing of language development.[58]

Effects of other diseases

There are numerous diseases affecting the central nervous system which can cause cognitive impairment. Many of these are associated with aging. Some common examples include Alzheimer's disease and Multi-infarct dementia. Many diseases may be neurological or psychiatric and may primarily affect the brain. Others may affect many other organs, like HIV, Hashimoto's thyroiditis causing hypothyroidism, or cancer. According to a 2015 report in The American Scholar, an assortment of neglected tropical diseases as well as some recently identified pathogens such as Pseudo-nitzschia have also been found to erode human intelligence.[59]

Major depression, affecting about 16% of the population on at least one occasion in their lives and the leading cause of disability in North America, may give symptoms similar to dementia. Patients treated for depression score higher on IQ tests than before treatment.[60][61]

Myopia and hyperopia

A 2008 literature review writes that studies in several nations have found a relationship between myopia and higher IQ and between myopia and school achievement. Several, but not all, studies have found hyperopia to be associated with lower IQ and school achievements. A common explanation for myopia is near-work. Regarding the relationship to IQ, several explanations have been proposed. One is that the myopic child is better adapted at reading, and reads and studies more, which increases intelligence. The reverse explanation is that the intelligent and studious child reads more which causes myopia. Another is that the myopic child has an advantage at IQ testing which is near work because of less eye strain. Still another explanation is that pleiotropic gene(s) affect the size of both brain and eyes simultaneously.[62] A study of Chinese schoolchildren found that after controlling for age, gender, school, parental myopia, father's education, and books read per week, myopia was still associated with high nonverbal IQ. Nonverbal IQ was a more important explanation than books read per week.[63]

Other associations

Long working hours (55 vs. 40) was associated with decreased scores on cognitive tests in a 5-year study on midlife British civil servants.[64]

See also

References

  1. ^ I. J. Deary (2008). "Why do intelligent people live longer?". Nature. 456 (7219): 175–6. Bibcode:2008Natur.456..175D. doi:10.1038/456175a. PMID 19005537. S2CID 205042144.
  2. ^ a b Olness K (April 2003). "Effects on brain development leading to cognitive impairment: a worldwide epidemic". J Dev Behav Pediatr. 24 (2): 120–30. doi:10.1097/00004703-200304000-00009. PMID 12692458. S2CID 31999992.
  3. ^ a b Perlmutter, David; Carol Colman (2006). Raise a Smarter Child By Kindergarten: Raise Iq Points By Up to 30 Points and Turn on Your Child's Smart Genes Points. Morgan Road Books. ISBN 978-0-7679-2301-9.
  4. ^ Nisen, Max (July 22, 2013). "How Adding Iodine To Salt Resulted In A Decade's Worth Of IQ Gains For The United States". Business Insider. Retrieved June 25, 2016.
  5. ^ Glewwe P, King EM (2001). "The Impact of Early Childhood Nutritional Status on Cognitive Development: Does the Timing of Malnutrition Matter?". World Bank Economic Review. 15 (1): 81–113. doi:10.1093/wber/15.1.81. hdl:10986/17213.
  6. ^ . Eur J Clin Nutr. 52 (Suppl 1): S1–103. January 1998. PMID 9547065. Archived from the original on January 29, 2007. Neisser; et al. (August 7, 1995). "Intelligence: Knowns and Unknowns". Board of Scientific Affairs of the American Psychological Association. Archived from the original on June 1, 2012. Retrieved August 6, 2006.
  7. ^ Masters R (1997). "Brain biochemistry and social status: The neurotoxicity hypothesis". In White, Elliott (ed.). Intelligence, political inequality, and public policy. New York, N.Y: Prager. pp. 141–183. ISBN 0-275-95655-5.
  8. ^ Der G, Batty GD, Deary IJ (November 2006). "Effect of breast feeding on intelligence in children: prospective study, sibling pairs analysis, and meta-analysis". BMJ. 333 (7575): 945. doi:10.1136/bmj.38978.699583.55. PMC 1633819. PMID 17020911.
  9. ^ . Archived from the original on 2010-07-26. Retrieved 2007-11-20.
  10. ^ Caspi A, Williams B, Kim-Cohen J, et al. (2007). "Moderation of breastfeeding effects on the IQ by genetic variation in fatty acid metabolism". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 104 (47): 18860–5. Bibcode:2007PNAS..10418860C. doi:10.1073/pnas.0704292104. PMC 2141867. PMID 17984066.
  11. ^ N. W. Martin; B. Benyamin; N. K. Hansell; G. W. Montgomery; N. G. Martin; M. J. Wright & T. C. Bates (2011). "Cognitive function in adolescence: testing for interactions between breast-feeding and FADS2 polymorphisms". Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 50 (1): 55–62 e4. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2010.10.010. PMID 21156270.
  12. ^ Steer CD, Davey Smith G, Emmett PM, Hibbeln JR, Golding J (2010). Penha-Goncalves C (ed.). "FADS2 polymorphisms modify the effect of breastfeeding on child IQ". PLOS ONE. 5 (7): e11570. Bibcode:2010PLoSO...511570S. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0011570. PMC 2903485. PMID 20644632.
  13. ^ Ivanovic DM, Leiva BP, Pérez HT, et al. (January 2002). "Nutritional status, brain development and scholastic achievement of Chilean high-school graduates from high and low intellectual quotient and socio-economic status". Br. J. Nutr. 87 (1): 81–92. doi:10.1079/BJN2001485. PMID 11895316.
  14. ^ Ivanovic DM, Leiva BP, Pérez HT, et al. (2004). "Head size and intelligence, learning, nutritional status and brain development. Head, IQ, learning, nutrition and brain". Neuropsychologia. 42 (8): 1118–31. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.11.022. PMID 15093150. S2CID 2114185.
  15. ^ Qian M, Wang D, Watkins WE, et al. (2005). "The effects of iodine on intelligence in children: a meta-analysis of studies conducted in China". Asia Pacific Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 14 (1): 32–42. PMID 15734706.
  16. ^ Behrman, J.R., Alderman, H., and Hoddinott, J., "Hunger and Malnutrition 2012-07-17 at the Wayback Machine," Copenhagen Consensus 2004.
  17. ^ UNICEF and The Micronutrient Initiative, "Vitamin & Mineral Deficiency: A Global Progress Report 2008-09-24 at the Wayback Machine," March 2004.
  18. ^ Saloojee H, Pettifor JM (December 2001). "Iron deficiency and impaired child development". BMJ. 323 (7326): 1377–8. doi:10.1136/bmj.323.7326.1377. PMC 1121846. PMID 11744547.
  19. ^ FOOD FORTIFICATION TECHNOLOGY Food Fortification: Technology and Quality Control. (FAO Food And Nutrition Paper - 60)
  20. ^ Schoenthaler SJ, Bier ID, Young K, Nichols D, Jansenns S (February 2000). "The effect of vitamin-mineral supplementation on the intelligence of American schoolchildren: a randomized, double-blind placebo-controlled trial". J Altern Complement Med. 6 (1): 19–29. doi:10.1089/acm.2000.6.19. PMID 10706232.
  21. ^ Duthie SJ, Whalley LJ, Collins AR, Leaper S, Berger K, Deary IJ (May 2002). "Homocysteine, B vitamin status, and cognitive function in the elderly". Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 75 (5): 908–13. doi:10.1093/ajcn/75.5.908. PMID 11976166. Erratum in: Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 77 (2): 523.
  22. ^ Helland IB, Smith L, Saarem K, Saugstad OD, Drevon CA (January 2003). "Maternal supplementation with very-long-chain n-3 fatty acids during pregnancy and lactation augments children's IQ at 4 years of age". Pediatrics. 111 (1): e39–44. doi:10.1542/peds.111.1.e39. PMID 12509593.
  23. ^ Lyketsos CG (May 2003). "Should pregnant women avoid eating fish? Lessons from the Seychelles". Lancet. 361 (9370): 1667–8. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(03)13379-X. PMID 12767728. S2CID 35728830.
  24. ^
  25. ^ Fish Diet in Pregnancy May Hone Kids' IQ
  26. ^ "The omega point". The Economist. January 19, 2006.
  27. ^ Medical News: Eating Fish During Pregnancy Provides 'Brain Food' for Child - in OB/GYN, Pregnancy from MedPage Today
  28. ^ Pregnant? Omega-3 Essential for Baby's Brain
  29. ^ Pollitt E, Gorman KS, Engle PL, Martorell R, Rivera J (1993). "Early supplementary feeding and cognition: effects over two decades". Monogr Soc Res Child Dev. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, Vol. 58, No. 7. 58 (7): 1–99, discussion 111–8. doi:10.2307/1166162. JSTOR 1166162. PMID 8272081.
  30. ^ Stein AD, Behrman JR, DiGirolamo A, et al. (June 2005). "Schooling, educational achievement, and cognitive functioning among young Guatemalan adults". Food Nutr Bull. 26 (2 Suppl 1): S46–54. doi:10.1177/15648265050262S105. PMID 16060211.
  31. ^ . World Resources Institute. Archived from the original on February 5, 2012.
  32. ^ Susan S. Lang (May 29, 1998). . Cornell News. Archived from the original on June 17, 2012.
  33. ^ Walker, S. P.; Chang, S. M.; Powell, C. A.; Grantham-McGregor, S. M (2005). "Effects of early childhood psychosocial stimulation and nutritional supplementation on cognition and education in growth-stunted Jamaican children: prospective cohort study". Lancet (British ed.). 366 (9499): 1804–1807. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(05)67574-5. PMID 16298218. S2CID 25797832.
  34. ^ Walker SP, Chang SM, Powell CA, Simonoff E, Grantham-McGregor SM (September 2006). "Effects of psychosocial stimulation and dietary supplementation in early childhood on psychosocial functioning in late adolescence: follow-up of randomised controlled trial". BMJ. 333 (7566): 472. doi:10.1136/bmj.38897.555208.2F. PMC 1557928. PMID 16877454.
  35. ^ Grandjean P, Landrigan PJ (December 2006). "Developmental neurotoxicity of industrial chemicals". Lancet. 368 (9553): 2167–78. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(06)69665-7. PMID 17174709. S2CID 12795774.
  36. ^ Meyer PA, McGeehin MA, Falk H (August 2003). "A global approach to childhood lead poisoning prevention". Int J Hyg Environ Health. 206 (4–5): 363–9. doi:10.1078/1438-4639-00232. PMID 12971691.
  37. ^ Bellinger DC, Stiles KM, Needleman HL (December 1992). "Low-level lead exposure, intelligence and academic achievement: a long-term follow-up study". Pediatrics. 90 (6): 855–61. doi:10.1542/peds.90.6.855. PMID 1437425. S2CID 25746146.
  38. ^ Ribas-Fitó N, Torrent M, Carrizo D, et al. (November 2006). "In utero exposure to background concentrations of DDT and cognitive functioning among preschoolers". Am. J. Epidemiol. 164 (10): 955–62. doi:10.1093/aje/kwj299. PMID 16968864.
  39. ^ Grandjean P, Landrigan PJ (December 2006). "Developmental neurotoxicity of industrial chemicals". Lancet. 368 (9553): 2167–78. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(06)69665-7. PMID 17174709. S2CID 12795774.
  40. ^ Potentials for exposure to industrial chemicals suspected of causing developmental neurotoxicity Philippe Grandjean, MD, PhD, Adjunct Professor Marian Perez, MPH, Project Coordinator Department of Environmental Health, Harvard School of Public Health, Boston, MA, USA
  41. ^ Abel EL, Sokol RJ (January 1987). "Incidence of fetal alcohol syndrome and economic impact of FAS-related anomalies". Drug Alcohol Depend. 19 (1): 51–70. doi:10.1016/0376-8716(87)90087-1. PMID 3545731.
  42. ^ Nature Journal. August 2012 and 30mt Radio discussion on Radio NZ with authors
  43. ^ Iversen L (February 2005). "Long-term effects of exposure to cannabis". Curr Opin Pharmacol. 5 (1): 69–72. doi:10.1016/j.coph.2004.08.010. PMID 15661628.
  44. ^ Weitzman M, Byrd RS, Aligne CA, Moss M (2002). "The effects of tobacco exposure on children's behavioral and cognitive functioning: implications for clinical and public health policy and future research". Neurotoxicol Teratol. 24 (3): 397–406. doi:10.1016/S0892-0362(02)00201-5. PMID 12009494.
  45. ^ Breslau N, Paneth N, Lucia VC, Paneth-Pollak R (October 2005). "Maternal smoking during pregnancy and offspring IQ". Int J Epidemiol. 34 (5): 1047–53. doi:10.1093/ije/dyi163. PMID 16085682.
  46. ^ Yolton K, Dietrich K, Auinger P, Lanphear BP, Hornung R (January 2005). "Exposure to environmental tobacco smoke and cognitive abilities among U.S. children and adolescents". Environ. Health Perspect. 113 (1): 98–103. doi:10.1289/ehp.7210. PMC 1253717. PMID 15626655. Archived from the original on 2012-07-12. Retrieved 2009-06-14.
  47. ^ Blair C (April 2006). "How similar are fluid cognition and general intelligence? A developmental neuroscience perspective on fluid cognition as an aspect of human cognitive ability". Behav Brain Sci. 29 (2): 109–25, discussion 125–60. doi:10.1017/S0140525X06009034. PMID 16606477. S2CID 8508500. Multiple comments can be seen on Google Scholar.
  48. ^ a b c Delaney-Black V, Covington C, Ondersma SJ, et al. (March 2002). "Violence exposure, trauma, and IQ and/or reading deficits among urban children". Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 156 (3): 280–5. doi:10.1001/archpedi.156.3.280. PMID 11876674.
  49. ^ a b Saltzman KM, Weems CF, Carrion VG (2006). "IQ and posttraumatic stress symptoms in children exposed to interpersonal violence". Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 36 (3): 261–72. doi:10.1007/s10578-005-0002-5. PMID 16362242. S2CID 14002367.
  50. ^ Gonzales NA, Cauce AM, Friedman RJ, Mason CA (June 1996). "Family, peer, and neighborhood influences on academic achievement among African-American adolescents: one-year prospective effects". Am J Community Psychol. 24 (3): 365–87. doi:10.1007/BF02512027. PMID 8864209. S2CID 37387862.
  51. ^ MacKenzie, Debora. "Link found between infectious disease and IQ". New Scientist. Retrieved 4 July 2010.
  52. ^ a b c d Eppig, Christopher (2011). "Why Is Average IQ Higher in Some Places?". Scientific American.
  53. ^ Boivin MJ (October 2002). "Effects of early cerebral malaria on cognitive ability in Senegalese children". J Dev Behav Pediatr. 23 (5): 353–64. doi:10.1097/00004703-200210000-00010. PMID 12394524. S2CID 26453764.
  54. ^ Kihara, Michael; Carter, J. A.; Newton, C. R. J. C. (2006). "The effect of Plasmodium falciparum on cognition: a systematic review". Tropical Medicine & International Health. 11 (4): 386–97. doi:10.1111/j.1365-3156.2006.01579.x. PMID 16553922.
  55. ^ Watkins WE, Pollitt E (March 1997). ""Stupidity or worms": do intestinal worms impair mental performance?". Psychol Bull. 121 (2): 171–91. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.121.2.171. PMID 9100486.
  56. ^ Dickson, Rumona; Shally Awasthi; Paula Williamson; Colin Demellweek; Paul Garner (2000). "Effects of treatment for intestinal helminth infection on growth and cognitive performance in children: systematic review of randomised trials". BMJ. 320 (7251): 1697–1701. doi:10.1136/bmj.320.7251.1697. PMC 27412. PMID 10864543.
  57. ^ Taylor-Robinson, David; Ashley Jones; Paul Garner (2009). Yamey, Gavin (ed.). "Does Deworming Improve Growth and School Performance in Children?". PLOS Neglected Tropical Diseases. 3 (1): e358. doi:10.1371/journal.pntd.0000358. PMC 2627941. PMID 19172183.
  58. ^ Abubakar, Amina; Anneloes Van Baar; Fons J. R. Van de Vijver; Penny Holding; Charles R. J. C. Newton (2008). "Paediatric HIV and neurodevelopment in sub-Saharan Africa: a systematic review". Tropical Medicine & International Health. 13 (7): 880–7. doi:10.1111/j.1365-3156.2008.02079.x. PMID 18384479.
  59. ^ Washington, Harriet A. (2015). "The Well Curve". The American Scholar (Autumn): 12.
  60. ^ Sackeim HA, Freeman J, McElhiney M, Coleman E, Prudic J, Devanand DP; Freeman; McElhiney; Coleman; Prudic; Devanand (March 1992). "Effects of major depression on estimates of intelligence". J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 14 (2): 268–88. doi:10.1080/01688639208402828. PMID 1572949.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  61. ^ Mandelli L, Serretti A, Colombo C, et al. (October 2006). "Improvement of cognitive functioning in mood disorder patients with depressive symptomatic recovery during treatment: an exploratory analysis". Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 60 (5): 598–604. doi:10.1111/j.1440-1819.2006.01564.x. PMID 16958944. S2CID 32869139.
  62. ^ Czepita, D.; Lodygowska, E.; Czepita, M. (2008). "Are children with myopia more intelligent? A literature review". Annales Academiae Medicae Stetinensis. 54 (1): 13–16, discussion 16. PMID 19127804.
  63. ^ Saw, S. -M.; Tan, S. -B.; Fung, D.; Chia, K. -S.; Koh, D.; Tan, D. T. H.; Stone, R. A. (2004). "IQ and the Association with Myopia in Children". Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science. 45 (9): 2943–2948. doi:10.1167/iovs.03-1296. PMID 15326105.
  64. ^ Virtanen M.; A. Singh-Manoux; J.E. Ferrie; D. Gimeno; M.G. Marmot; M. Elovainio; M. Jokela; J. Vahtera; M. Kivimäki (2009). "Long Working Hours and Cognitive Function: The Whitehall II Study". American Journal of Epidemiology. 169 (5): 596–605. doi:10.1093/aje/kwn382. PMC 2727184. PMID 19126590.

impact, health, intelligence, this, article, written, like, personal, reflection, personal, essay, argumentative, essay, that, states, wikipedia, editor, personal, feelings, presents, original, argument, about, topic, please, help, improve, rewriting, encyclop. This article is written like a personal reflection personal essay or argumentative essay that states a Wikipedia editor s personal feelings or presents an original argument about a topic Please help improve it by rewriting it in an encyclopedic style December 2021 Learn how and when to remove this template message Health can affect intelligence in various ways Conversely intelligence can affect health Health effects on intelligence have been described as being among the most important factors in the origins of human group differences in IQ test scores and other measures of cognitive ability 1 Several factors can lead to significant cognitive impairment particularly if they occur during pregnancy and childhood when the brain is growing and the blood brain barrier of the child is less effective Such impairment may sometimes be permanent sometimes be partially or wholly compensated for by later growth Developed nations have implemented several health policies regarding nutrients and toxins known to influence cognitive function These include laws requiring fortification of certain food products and laws establishing safe levels of pollutants e g lead mercury and organochlorides Comprehensive policy recommendations targeting reduction of cognitive impairment in children have been proposed 2 3 Improvements in nutrition often involving specific micronutrients due to public policy changes have been implicated in IQ increases in many nations as part of the overall Flynn effect such as efforts fighting iodine deficiency in the U S 4 Contents 1 Nutrition 1 1 Timing 1 2 Intrauterine growth retardation 1 3 Breastfeeding 1 4 Infancy 1 5 Micronutrients and vitamin deficiencies 1 6 Protein and energy malnutrition 1 7 Stunting 2 Toxins 2 1 Industrial chemicals 2 2 Alcohol and drugs 3 Healthcare during pregnancy and childbirth 4 Stress 5 Infectious diseases 5 1 Tropical infectious diseases 6 Effects of other diseases 6 1 Myopia and hyperopia 7 Other associations 8 See also 9 ReferencesNutrition EditMalnutrition may occur during several periods of growth such as pregnancy during breastfeeding infancy or childhood It may also happen due to deficiencies of different nutrients such as micronutrients protein or energy This may cause different effects Timing Edit Some observers have argued that malnutrition during the first six months of life harms cognitive development much more than malnutrition later in life However a study from the Philippines argues that malnutrition in the second year of life may have a larger negative impact than malnutrition in the first year of life While it is debatable whether or not as an infant or after two years is the worst time for malnourishment the bottom line in these studies is that not having enough nutrients at a young age negatively effects learning 5 Intrauterine growth retardation Edit Undernutrition during pregnancy and other factors may cause intrauterine growth retardation IUGR which is one cause of low birth weight However it has been suggested that in IUGR the brain may be selectively spared Brain growth is usually less affected than whole body weight or length Several studies from developed nations have found that with the exception of extreme intrauterine growth retardation also affecting brain growth and hypoxic injury IUGR seems to have little or no measurable effect on mental performance and behavior in adolescence or adulthood For example acute undernutrition for a few months during the Dutch famine of 1944 caused a decrease in mean birthweight in certain areas This was later associated with a change in performance on IQ tests for 18 19 years old Dutch males draftees from these areas compared to control areas The subjects were exposed to famine prenatally but not after birth During the famine births decreased more among those with lower socioeconomic status SES whereas after the famine there was a compensatory increase in births among those with lower SES Since SES correlates with IQ this may have hidden an effect caused by the undernutrition 6 Breastfeeding Edit Studies often find higher IQ in children and adults who were breastfed 3 7 It has been proposed that omega 3 fatty acids in breast milk known to be essential constituents of brain tissues could at least partially account for an increase in IQ Recently however the longstanding belief that breastfeeding causes an increase in the IQ of offspring was challenged in a 2006 paper published in the British Medical Journal The results indicated that mother s IQ not breastfeeding explained the differences in the IQ scores of offspring measured between ages 5 and 14 The results of this meta analysis argued that prior studies had not controlled for the mother s IQ Since mother s IQ was predictive of whether a child was breastfed the study concluded that breast feeding itself has little or no effect on intelligence in children Instead it was the mother s IQ that had a significant correlation with the IQ of her offspring whether the offspring was breastfed or was not breastfed 8 One study found that breastfeeding was linked to raised IQ as much as 7 points when not controlling for maternal IQ if the infants had an SNP coding for a C rather than G base within the FADS2 gene Those with the G version showed no IQ advantage suggesting a biochemical interaction of child s genes on the effect of breastfeeding 9 10 Other studies have failed to replicate any correlation between the FADS2 gene 11 breastfeeding and IQ while others show a negative effect on IQ when combining bottled feeding and the G version of FADS2 12 Infancy Edit Two studies in Chile on 18 year old high school graduates found that nutritional status during the first year of life affected IQ scholastic achievement and brain volume 13 14 Micronutrients and vitamin deficiencies Edit Micronutrient deficiencies e g in iodine and iron influence the development of intelligence and remain a problem in the developing world For example iodine deficiency causes a fall on average of 12 IQ points These deficiencies could technically show up in medical scans at various ages 15 Policy recommendations to increase availability of micronutrient supplements have been made and justified in part by the potential to counteract intelligence related developmental problems For example the Copenhagen consensus states that lack of both iodine and iron has been implicated in impaired brain development and this can affect enormous numbers of people it is estimated that 2 billion people one third of the total global population are affected by iodine deficiency including 285 million 6 to 12 year old children In developing countries it is estimated that 40 of children aged four and under have anaemia because of insufficient iron in their diets 16 A joint statement on vitamin and mineral deficiencies says that the severity of such deficiencies means the impairment of hundreds of millions of growing minds and the lowering of national IQs Since the brain is not fully developed until age 25 this can be effecting people through their late teens 17 Overall studies investigating whether cognitive function in already iron deficient children can be improved with iron supplements have produced mixed results possibly because deficiency in critical growth periods may cause irreversible damage However several studies with better design have shown substantial benefits One way to prevent iron deficiency is to give specific supplementation to children for example as tablets However this is costly distribution mechanisms are often ineffective and compliance is low Fortification of staple foods cereals flour sugar salt to deliver micronutrients to children on a large scale is probably the most sustainable and affordable option even though commitment from governments and the food industry is needed 18 Developed nations fortify several foods with various micronutrients 19 Additional vitamin mineral supplementation may have an effect also in the developed world A study giving such supplementation to working class primarily Hispanic 6 12 year old children in the United States for 3 months found an average increase of 2 to 3 IQ points Most of this can be explained by the very large increase of a subgroup of the children presumably because these were not adequately nourished unlike the majority The study suggests that parents of schoolchildren whose academic performance is substandard would be well advised to seek a nutritionally oriented physician for assessment of their children s nutritional status as a possible etiology 20 More speculatively other nutrients may prove important in the future Vitamin B12 and folate may be important for cognitive function in old age 21 Fish oil supplement to pregnant and lactating mothers has been linked to increased cognitive ability in one study 22 Another study found that pregnant women who consumed 340 grams of low mercury containing fish with fatty acids per week have benefits that outweigh the risks for mercury poisoning They were less likely to have children with low verbal IQ motor coordination and behavioral problems However foods containing high amounts of mercury such as shark swordfish king mackerel and tilefish might cause mental retardation 23 24 25 26 27 28 Protein and energy malnutrition Edit One study from a developing country Guatemala found that poor growth during infancy rather than low birth weight was negatively related to adolescent performance on cognitive and achievement tests 29 A later related very long term study looked at the effect of giving 6 24 month old children in Guatemala a high protein energy drink as a dietary supplement A significantly positive and fairly substantial effects was found on increasing the probability of attending school and of passing the first grade increasing the grade attained by age 13 increasing completed schooling attainment and for adults aged 25 40 increasing IQ test scores 30 Stunting Edit 31 of children under the age of 5 in the developing world are moderately height for age is below minus 2 standard deviations or severely stunted below minus 3 standard deviations 31 The prevalence was even higher previously since the worldwide prevalence of stunting is declining by about half of a percentage point each year 32 A study on stunted children aged 9 24 months in Jamaica found that when aged 17 18 years they had significantly poorer scores than a non stunted group on cognitive and educational tests and psychosocial functioning Giving a nutritional supplementation 1 kg milk based formula each week to these already stunted children had no significant effect on later scores but psychosocial stimulation weekly play sessions with mother and child had a positive effect 33 34 Toxins EditIndustrial chemicals Edit Certain toxins such as lead mercury toluene and PCB are well known causes of neuro developmental disorders Recognition of these risks has led to evidence based programmes of prevention such as elimination of lead additives in petrol Although these prevention campaigns are highly successful most were initiated only after substantial delays 35 Policies to manage lead differ between nations particularly between the developed and developing world Use of leaded gasoline has been reduced or eliminated in most developed nations and lead levels in US children have been substantially reduced by policies relating to lead reduction 36 Even slightly elevated lead levels around the age of 24 months are associated with intellectual and academic performance deficits at age 10 years 37 Certain at least previously widely used organochlorides such as dioxins DDT and PCB have been associated with cognitive deficits 38 A Lancet review identified 201 chemicals with the ability to cause clinical neurotoxic effects in human adults as described in the peer reviewed scientific literature Most of them are commonly used Many additional chemicals have been shown to be neurotoxic in laboratory models The article notes that children are more vulnerable and argues that new precautionary approaches that recognise the unique vulnerability of the developing brain are needed for testing and control of chemicals in order to avoid the previous substantial before starting restrictions on usage 39 An appendix listed further industrial chemicals considered to be neurotoxic 40 Alcohol and drugs Edit Fetal alcohol exposure causing Fetal alcohol syndrome is one of the leading known causes of intellectual disability in the Western world 41 Current cannabis use was found to be significantly correlated in a dose dependent manner with a decline in IQ scores during the effect of the use However no such decline was seen in subjects who had formerly been heavy cannabis users and had stopped taking the drug The authors concluded that cannabis does not have a long term effect on intelligence However this is contradicted by the long term longitudinal study carried out by Otago and Duke universities which found that regular use of marijuana in teenage years affects IQ in adulthood even when the use stops The drop in IQ was 8 points Adults smoking marijuana had no lasting effect on IQ 42 Effects on fetal development are minimal when compared with the well documented adverse effects of tobacco or alcohol use 43 Maternal tobacco smoking during pregnancy is associated with increased activity decreased attention and diminished intellectual abilities 44 However a recent study finds that maternal tobacco smoking has no direct causal effect on the child s IQ Adjusting for maternal cognitive ability as measured by IQ and education eliminated the association between lower IQ and tobacco smoking 45 But another study instead looking at the relationship between environmental tobacco smoke exposure measured with a blood biomarker and cognitive abilities among U S children and adolescents 6 16 years of age found an inverse association between exposure and cognitive ability among children even at extremely low levels of exposure The study controlled for sex race region poverty parent education and marital status ferritin and blood lead concentration 46 Healthcare during pregnancy and childbirth EditHealthcare during pregnancy and childbirth access to which is often governed by policy also influences cognitive development Preventable causes of low intelligence in children include infectious diseases such as meningitis parasites and cerebral malaria prenatal drug and alcohol exposure newborn asphyxia low birth weight head injuries and endocrine disorders A direct policy focus on determinants of childhood cognitive ability has been urged 2 Stress EditFurther information Psychological trauma Historical trauma and Neuroplasticity A recent theory suggests that early childhood stress may affect the developing brain and cause negative effects 47 Exposure to violence in childhood has been associated with lower school grades 48 and lower IQ in children of all races 49 A group of largely African American urban first grade children and their caregivers were evaluated using self report interview and standardized tests including IQ tests The study reported that exposure to violence and trauma related distress in young children were associated with substantial decrements in IQ and reading achievement Exposure to Violence or Trauma lead to a 7 5 point SD 0 5 decrement in IQ and a 9 8 point SD 0 66 decrement in reading achievement 48 Violence may have a negative impact on IQ or IQ may be protective against violence 49 The causal mechanism and direction of causation is unknown 48 Neighborhood risk has been related to lower school grades for African American adolescents in another study from 2006 50 Infectious diseases EditA 2010 study by Eppig Fincher and Thornhill found a close correlation between the infectious disease burden in a country and the average IQ of its population The researchers found that when disease was controlled for IQ showed no correlation with other variables such as educational and nutritional levels Since brain development requires a very high proportion of all the body s energy in newborns and children the researchers argue that fighting infection reduces children s IQ potential The Eppig research may help to explain the Flynn effect the rise in intelligence noted in rich countries 51 They also tested other hypotheses as well including genetic explanations concluding that infectious disease was the best predictor 52 Christopher Hassall and Thomas Sherratt repeated the analysis and concluded that infectious disease may be the only really important predictor of average national IQ 52 In order to mitigate the effects of education on IQ Eppig Fincher amp Thornhill 2010 harvtxt error no target CITEREFEppigFincherThornhill2010 help repeated their analysis across the United States where standardized and compulsory education exists 52 The correlation between infectious disease and average IQ was confirmed and they concluded that the evidence suggests that infectious disease is a primary cause of the global variation in human intelligence 52 Tropical infectious diseases Edit Malaria affects 300 500 million persons each year mostly children under age five in Africa causing widespread anemia during a period of rapid brain development and also direct brain damage from cerebral malaria to which children are more vulnerable 53 A 2006 systematic review found that Plasmodium falciparum infection causes cognitive deficits in both the short and long term 54 Policies aimed at malaria reduction may have cognitive benefits It has been suggested that the future economic and educational development of Africa critically depends on the eradication of malaria Roundworms infect hundreds of millions of people There is evidence that high intensities of worms in the intestines can affect mental performance 55 but a systematic review in 2000 and a 2009 update found that there was insufficient evidence to show that deworming treatments improve cognitive performance or school performance in children 56 57 HIV infection in children in sub Saharan Africa affects their motor development but there is insufficient evidence to show a slowing of language development 58 Effects of other diseases EditThere are numerous diseases affecting the central nervous system which can cause cognitive impairment Many of these are associated with aging Some common examples include Alzheimer s disease and Multi infarct dementia Many diseases may be neurological or psychiatric and may primarily affect the brain Others may affect many other organs like HIV Hashimoto s thyroiditis causing hypothyroidism or cancer According to a 2015 report in The American Scholar an assortment of neglected tropical diseases as well as some recently identified pathogens such as Pseudo nitzschia have also been found to erode human intelligence 59 Major depression affecting about 16 of the population on at least one occasion in their lives and the leading cause of disability in North America may give symptoms similar to dementia Patients treated for depression score higher on IQ tests than before treatment 60 61 Myopia and hyperopia Edit A 2008 literature review writes that studies in several nations have found a relationship between myopia and higher IQ and between myopia and school achievement Several but not all studies have found hyperopia to be associated with lower IQ and school achievements A common explanation for myopia is near work Regarding the relationship to IQ several explanations have been proposed One is that the myopic child is better adapted at reading and reads and studies more which increases intelligence The reverse explanation is that the intelligent and studious child reads more which causes myopia Another is that the myopic child has an advantage at IQ testing which is near work because of less eye strain Still another explanation is that pleiotropic gene s affect the size of both brain and eyes simultaneously 62 A study of Chinese schoolchildren found that after controlling for age gender school parental myopia father s education and books read per week myopia was still associated with high nonverbal IQ Nonverbal IQ was a more important explanation than books read per week 63 Other associations EditLong working hours 55 vs 40 was associated with decreased scores on cognitive tests in a 5 year study on midlife British civil servants 64 See also Edit Biology portalCognitive epidemiology Field of research Flynn effect 20th century rise in overall human intelligence g factor psychometrics Psychometric factor also known as general intelligence Health and race Race and heightReferences Edit I J Deary 2008 Why do intelligent people live longer Nature 456 7219 175 6 Bibcode 2008Natur 456 175D doi 10 1038 456175a PMID 19005537 S2CID 205042144 a b Olness K April 2003 Effects on brain development leading to cognitive impairment a worldwide epidemic J Dev Behav Pediatr 24 2 120 30 doi 10 1097 00004703 200304000 00009 PMID 12692458 S2CID 31999992 a b Perlmutter David Carol Colman 2006 Raise a Smarter Child By Kindergarten Raise Iq Points By Up to 30 Points and Turn on Your Child s Smart Genes Points Morgan Road Books ISBN 978 0 7679 2301 9 Nisen Max July 22 2013 How Adding Iodine To Salt Resulted In A Decade s Worth Of IQ Gains For The United States Business Insider Retrieved June 25 2016 Glewwe P King EM 2001 The Impact of Early Childhood Nutritional Status on Cognitive Development Does the Timing of Malnutrition Matter World Bank Economic Review 15 1 81 113 doi 10 1093 wber 15 1 81 hdl 10986 17213 Causes and consequences of intrauterine growth retardation Proceedings of an IDECG Workshop Baton Rouge Louisiana USA November 11 15 1996 Eur J Clin Nutr 52 Suppl 1 S1 103 January 1998 PMID 9547065 Archived from the original on January 29 2007 Neisser et al August 7 1995 Intelligence Knowns and Unknowns Board of Scientific Affairs of the American Psychological Association Archived from the original on June 1 2012 Retrieved August 6 2006 Masters R 1997 Brain biochemistry and social status The neurotoxicity hypothesis In White Elliott ed Intelligence political inequality and public policy New York N Y Prager pp 141 183 ISBN 0 275 95655 5 Der G Batty GD Deary IJ November 2006 Effect of breast feeding on intelligence in children prospective study sibling pairs analysis and meta analysis BMJ 333 7575 945 doi 10 1136 bmj 38978 699583 55 PMC 1633819 PMID 17020911 Baby s IQ Raised by Breastmilk and Genes Archived from the original on 2010 07 26 Retrieved 2007 11 20 Caspi A Williams B Kim Cohen J et al 2007 Moderation of breastfeeding effects on the IQ by genetic variation in fatty acid metabolism Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 104 47 18860 5 Bibcode 2007PNAS 10418860C doi 10 1073 pnas 0704292104 PMC 2141867 PMID 17984066 N W Martin B Benyamin N K Hansell G W Montgomery N G Martin M J Wright amp T C Bates 2011 Cognitive function in adolescence testing for interactions between breast feeding and FADS2 polymorphisms Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 50 1 55 62 e4 doi 10 1016 j jaac 2010 10 010 PMID 21156270 Steer CD Davey Smith G Emmett PM Hibbeln JR Golding J 2010 Penha Goncalves C ed FADS2 polymorphisms modify the effect of breastfeeding on child IQ PLOS ONE 5 7 e11570 Bibcode 2010PLoSO 511570S doi 10 1371 journal pone 0011570 PMC 2903485 PMID 20644632 Ivanovic DM Leiva BP Perez HT et al January 2002 Nutritional status brain development and scholastic achievement of Chilean high school graduates from high and low intellectual quotient and socio economic status Br J Nutr 87 1 81 92 doi 10 1079 BJN2001485 PMID 11895316 Ivanovic DM Leiva BP Perez HT et al 2004 Head size and intelligence learning nutritional status and brain development Head IQ learning nutrition and brain Neuropsychologia 42 8 1118 31 doi 10 1016 j neuropsychologia 2003 11 022 PMID 15093150 S2CID 2114185 Qian M Wang D Watkins WE et al 2005 The effects of iodine on intelligence in children a meta analysis of studies conducted in China Asia Pacific Journal of Clinical Nutrition 14 1 32 42 PMID 15734706 Behrman J R Alderman H and Hoddinott J Hunger and Malnutrition Archived 2012 07 17 at the Wayback Machine Copenhagen Consensus 2004 UNICEF and The Micronutrient Initiative Vitamin amp Mineral Deficiency A Global Progress Report Archived 2008 09 24 at the Wayback Machine March 2004 Saloojee H Pettifor JM December 2001 Iron deficiency and impaired child development BMJ 323 7326 1377 8 doi 10 1136 bmj 323 7326 1377 PMC 1121846 PMID 11744547 FOOD FORTIFICATION TECHNOLOGY Food Fortification Technology and Quality Control FAO Food And Nutrition Paper 60 Schoenthaler SJ Bier ID Young K Nichols D Jansenns S February 2000 The effect of vitamin mineral supplementation on the intelligence of American schoolchildren a randomized double blind placebo controlled trial J Altern Complement Med 6 1 19 29 doi 10 1089 acm 2000 6 19 PMID 10706232 Duthie SJ Whalley LJ Collins AR Leaper S Berger K Deary IJ May 2002 Homocysteine B vitamin status and cognitive function in the elderly Am J Clin Nutr 75 5 908 13 doi 10 1093 ajcn 75 5 908 PMID 11976166 Erratum in Am J Clin Nutr 77 2 523 Helland IB Smith L Saarem K Saugstad OD Drevon CA January 2003 Maternal supplementation with very long chain n 3 fatty acids during pregnancy and lactation augments children s IQ at 4 years of age Pediatrics 111 1 e39 44 doi 10 1542 peds 111 1 e39 PMID 12509593 Lyketsos CG May 2003 Should pregnant women avoid eating fish Lessons from the Seychelles Lancet 361 9370 1667 8 doi 10 1016 S0140 6736 03 13379 X PMID 12767728 S2CID 35728830 Pregnant Women Eat More Fish or Not To Your Health MSNBC com Fish Diet in Pregnancy May Hone Kids IQ The omega point The Economist January 19 2006 Medical News Eating Fish During Pregnancy Provides Brain Food for Child in OB GYN Pregnancy from MedPage Today Pregnant Omega 3 Essential for Baby s Brain Pollitt E Gorman KS Engle PL Martorell R Rivera J 1993 Early supplementary feeding and cognition effects over two decades Monogr Soc Res Child Dev Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development Vol 58 No 7 58 7 1 99 discussion 111 8 doi 10 2307 1166162 JSTOR 1166162 PMID 8272081 Stein AD Behrman JR DiGirolamo A et al June 2005 Schooling educational achievement and cognitive functioning among young Guatemalan adults Food Nutr Bull 26 2 Suppl 1 S46 54 doi 10 1177 15648265050262S105 PMID 16060211 Children s Health Stunting in children under 5 moderate and severe World Resources Institute Archived from the original on February 5 2012 Susan S Lang May 29 1998 Stunted growth affects almost 40 percent of the developing world s infants Cornell study reports Cornell News Archived from the original on June 17 2012 Walker S P Chang S M Powell C A Grantham McGregor S M 2005 Effects of early childhood psychosocial stimulation and nutritional supplementation on cognition and education in growth stunted Jamaican children prospective cohort study Lancet British ed 366 9499 1804 1807 doi 10 1016 S0140 6736 05 67574 5 PMID 16298218 S2CID 25797832 Walker SP Chang SM Powell CA Simonoff E Grantham McGregor SM September 2006 Effects of psychosocial stimulation and dietary supplementation in early childhood on psychosocial functioning in late adolescence follow up of randomised controlled trial BMJ 333 7566 472 doi 10 1136 bmj 38897 555208 2F PMC 1557928 PMID 16877454 Grandjean P Landrigan PJ December 2006 Developmental neurotoxicity of industrial chemicals Lancet 368 9553 2167 78 doi 10 1016 S0140 6736 06 69665 7 PMID 17174709 S2CID 12795774 Meyer PA McGeehin MA Falk H August 2003 A global approach to childhood lead poisoning prevention Int J Hyg Environ Health 206 4 5 363 9 doi 10 1078 1438 4639 00232 PMID 12971691 Bellinger DC Stiles KM Needleman HL December 1992 Low level lead exposure intelligence and academic achievement a long term follow up study Pediatrics 90 6 855 61 doi 10 1542 peds 90 6 855 PMID 1437425 S2CID 25746146 Ribas Fito N Torrent M Carrizo D et al November 2006 In utero exposure to background concentrations of DDT and cognitive functioning among preschoolers Am J Epidemiol 164 10 955 62 doi 10 1093 aje kwj299 PMID 16968864 Grandjean P Landrigan PJ December 2006 Developmental neurotoxicity of industrial chemicals Lancet 368 9553 2167 78 doi 10 1016 S0140 6736 06 69665 7 PMID 17174709 S2CID 12795774 Potentials for exposure to industrial chemicals suspected of causing developmental neurotoxicity Philippe Grandjean MD PhD Adjunct Professor Marian Perez MPH Project Coordinator Department of Environmental Health Harvard School of Public Health Boston MA USA Abel EL Sokol RJ January 1987 Incidence of fetal alcohol syndrome and economic impact of FAS related anomalies Drug Alcohol Depend 19 1 51 70 doi 10 1016 0376 8716 87 90087 1 PMID 3545731 Nature Journal August 2012 and 30mt Radio discussion on Radio NZ with authors Iversen L February 2005 Long term effects of exposure to cannabis Curr Opin Pharmacol 5 1 69 72 doi 10 1016 j coph 2004 08 010 PMID 15661628 Weitzman M Byrd RS Aligne CA Moss M 2002 The effects of tobacco exposure on children s behavioral and cognitive functioning implications for clinical and public health policy and future research Neurotoxicol Teratol 24 3 397 406 doi 10 1016 S0892 0362 02 00201 5 PMID 12009494 Breslau N Paneth N Lucia VC Paneth Pollak R October 2005 Maternal smoking during pregnancy and offspring IQ Int J Epidemiol 34 5 1047 53 doi 10 1093 ije dyi163 PMID 16085682 Yolton K Dietrich K Auinger P Lanphear BP Hornung R January 2005 Exposure to environmental tobacco smoke and cognitive abilities among U S children and adolescents Environ Health Perspect 113 1 98 103 doi 10 1289 ehp 7210 PMC 1253717 PMID 15626655 Archived from the original on 2012 07 12 Retrieved 2009 06 14 Blair C April 2006 How similar are fluid cognition and general intelligence A developmental neuroscience perspective on fluid cognition as an aspect of human cognitive ability Behav Brain Sci 29 2 109 25 discussion 125 60 doi 10 1017 S0140525X06009034 PMID 16606477 S2CID 8508500 Multiple comments can be seen on Google Scholar a b c Delaney Black V Covington C Ondersma SJ et al March 2002 Violence exposure trauma and IQ and or reading deficits among urban children Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 156 3 280 5 doi 10 1001 archpedi 156 3 280 PMID 11876674 a b Saltzman KM Weems CF Carrion VG 2006 IQ and posttraumatic stress symptoms in children exposed to interpersonal violence Child Psychiatry Hum Dev 36 3 261 72 doi 10 1007 s10578 005 0002 5 PMID 16362242 S2CID 14002367 Gonzales NA Cauce AM Friedman RJ Mason CA June 1996 Family peer and neighborhood influences on academic achievement among African American adolescents one year prospective effects Am J Community Psychol 24 3 365 87 doi 10 1007 BF02512027 PMID 8864209 S2CID 37387862 MacKenzie Debora Link found between infectious disease and IQ New Scientist Retrieved 4 July 2010 a b c d Eppig Christopher 2011 Why Is Average IQ Higher in Some Places Scientific American Boivin MJ October 2002 Effects of early cerebral malaria on cognitive ability in Senegalese children J Dev Behav Pediatr 23 5 353 64 doi 10 1097 00004703 200210000 00010 PMID 12394524 S2CID 26453764 Kihara Michael Carter J A Newton C R J C 2006 The effect of Plasmodium falciparum on cognition a systematic review Tropical Medicine amp International Health 11 4 386 97 doi 10 1111 j 1365 3156 2006 01579 x PMID 16553922 Watkins WE Pollitt E March 1997 Stupidity or worms do intestinal worms impair mental performance Psychol Bull 121 2 171 91 doi 10 1037 0033 2909 121 2 171 PMID 9100486 Dickson Rumona Shally Awasthi Paula Williamson Colin Demellweek Paul Garner 2000 Effects of treatment for intestinal helminth infection on growth and cognitive performance in children systematic review of randomised trials BMJ 320 7251 1697 1701 doi 10 1136 bmj 320 7251 1697 PMC 27412 PMID 10864543 Taylor Robinson David Ashley Jones Paul Garner 2009 Yamey Gavin ed Does Deworming Improve Growth and School Performance in Children PLOS Neglected Tropical Diseases 3 1 e358 doi 10 1371 journal pntd 0000358 PMC 2627941 PMID 19172183 Abubakar Amina Anneloes Van Baar Fons J R Van de Vijver Penny Holding Charles R J C Newton 2008 Paediatric HIV and neurodevelopment in sub Saharan Africa a systematic review Tropical Medicine amp International Health 13 7 880 7 doi 10 1111 j 1365 3156 2008 02079 x PMID 18384479 Washington Harriet A 2015 The Well Curve The American Scholar Autumn 12 Sackeim HA Freeman J McElhiney M Coleman E Prudic J Devanand DP Freeman McElhiney Coleman Prudic Devanand March 1992 Effects of major depression on estimates of intelligence J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 14 2 268 88 doi 10 1080 01688639208402828 PMID 1572949 a href Template Cite journal html title Template Cite journal cite journal a CS1 maint multiple names authors list link Mandelli L Serretti A Colombo C et al October 2006 Improvement of cognitive functioning in mood disorder patients with depressive symptomatic recovery during treatment an exploratory analysis Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 60 5 598 604 doi 10 1111 j 1440 1819 2006 01564 x PMID 16958944 S2CID 32869139 Czepita D Lodygowska E Czepita M 2008 Are children with myopia more intelligent A literature review Annales Academiae Medicae Stetinensis 54 1 13 16 discussion 16 PMID 19127804 Saw S M Tan S B Fung D Chia K S Koh D Tan D T H Stone R A 2004 IQ and the Association with Myopia in Children Investigative Ophthalmology amp Visual Science 45 9 2943 2948 doi 10 1167 iovs 03 1296 PMID 15326105 Virtanen M A Singh Manoux J E Ferrie D Gimeno M G Marmot M Elovainio M Jokela J Vahtera M Kivimaki 2009 Long Working Hours and Cognitive Function The Whitehall II Study American Journal of Epidemiology 169 5 596 605 doi 10 1093 aje kwn382 PMC 2727184 PMID 19126590 Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Impact of health on intelligence amp oldid 1111003848, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.