fbpx
Wikipedia

Female intrasexual competition

Female intrasexual competition is competition between women over a potential mate. Such competition might include self-promotion, derogation of other women, and direct and indirect aggression toward other women. Factors that influence female intrasexual competition include the genetic quality of available mates, hormone levels, and interpersonal dynamics.

There are two modes of sexual selection: intersexual selection and intrasexual selection. Intersexual selection includes the display of desirable sexual characteristics to attract a potential mate. Intrasexual selection is competition between members of the same sex other over a potential mate.

Compared to males, females tend to prefer subtle rather than overt forms of intrasexual competition.[1][2]

Self-promotion tactics edit

Self-promotion tactics are one of the main strategies that can be used during intrasexual competition for mates.[3] It is often perceived to be the most socially desirable strategy, as it can be perceived as self-improvement, rather than an attack on competitors. Self-promotion tactics are especially useful for when women are looking for short-term mates, as such tactics will directly promote their sexual availability.[4]

Luxury consumption edit

 
A woman shopping a luxury brand: Louis Vuitton (LV)

Self-promotion tactics refers to the different strategies that women might use to make themselves look better compared to other competing women. For example, women are interested in luxury items that enhance their attractiveness.[5] Luxury items can indicate attractiveness by emphasising a higher status, which is a factor that potential mates will take into consideration.

It has been shown that when women are at their peak fertility, they will have an increased awareness and sensitivity to female intrasexual competition. This is due to the fact that when women are at their peak fertility, this is the most optimal time for them to mate and produce offspring.[6] However, this tends to be only applied in situations when women are faced with rivals who they consider to be attractive. When with an unattractive rival, women might not necessarily see them posing any threat, as they would feel more attractive in comparison.[7]

Cosmetic surgery edit

 
Facial rejuvenation (face-lift) procedure includes elevation of low eyebrows, eyelid operations and restoration of the natural fullness of the lips
 
East Asian blepharoplasty, a surgery designed to add a crease to the eyelid, has become one of the most common facial surgical procedures in Asian countries

By using plastic surgery, women can surgically change their appearances to make themselves more attractive. They can surgically alter their faces and bodies according to their wishes. They can use botulinum toxin to prevent wrinkles and get face lifts. Or they can use get liposuction to remove fat and achieve a more desirable body. Research has shown that the waist–hip ratio (WHR) of a female is a good indicator of their health, and that males tend to have a preference for females with a low WHR.[8] When comparing women's pre- and post-operative photographs, post-operative photographs where women have a lower WHR are rated as more attractive, regardless of their weight gain or their body mass index (BMI).[9] Culture plays a role in the type of plastic surgery a woman gets. The beauty standards for Westerners and Easterners are extremely different. Western models tend to be used to promote clothing and to portray seductiveness, whereas Asian models tend to be used to promote hair and skin products. Research suggest that Western models are more body-oriented.[10][11]

Regardless, by using cosmetic surgery, females can change various aspects of their body to make themselves more attractive by displaying a more desirable waist–hip ratio. This can lead to competition with other females who may be considered less attractive in comparison. When women change their appearances, such as by applying cosmetic products and wearing sexy or stylish clothes, do make a difference and has been proven to be effective.[3]

Clothing and make-up edit

Wearing form-fitting attire, such as dresses that highlight the waist and hips or pants that accentuate curves, is a common strategy to enhance the overall figure. Such outfits highlight the importance of the waist-hip ratio (WHR).[12]

Short skirts, high-heels, and attire that showcases the legs. These clothing choices subtly emphasize leg length and overall body proportions, which are characteristics often deemed attractive.

Makeup, too, plays a role in self-promotion, allowing to enhance certain facial features and draw attention to specific areas such as the eyes and lips.[13]

Competitor derogation edit

There are a number of competitive strategies that females may use in a bid to appear more attractive in comparison to other females. Whilst males may use direct forms of aggression during intrasexual competition[14][15] females typically compete for access to desired mates through the use of indirect aggression. Unlike direct aggression which involves delivering harm face to face,[16] indirect aggression describes acts that are done circuitously, where an individual aims to cause harm but attempts to appear as if they have no harmful intentions.[17] In the context of intrasexual competition, indirect aggression works to reduce the opportunities the rival may have in securing access to the desired mate to, therefore, increase one's chances of reproductive success.[18] These include behaviours such as shunning, social exclusion, getting others to dislike the individual, spreading rumours and criticizing the rival's appearance.

Female derogation edit

Female derogation is a form of indirect aggression where females attempt to reduce the perceived value of another female 'rival'. Fisher (2004)[19] studied female derogation and the effects of estrogen levels on this form of competition. Females disclosed their ovulation status and rated the attractiveness of male and female faces. Competitor derogation (giving low ratings) towards same-sex rivals occurred frequently when women were at their most fertile stages. In contrast, women gave same-sex rivals higher ratings during the least fertile stages of their ovulation. This indirect form of competition appears exclusive toward females as findings also showed that women, irrespective of ovulation status (high or low), showed no difference in the rating of male faces. Supporting research has also found that younger women who are considered as having high fertility, gossip about other women more than older women, who are no longer at their most fertile stage.[20]

Indeed, indirect aggression appears more prevalent amongst (or exclusive to) females than males who are said to engage in more direct forms of competition.[21] Research studying the relationship between indicators of attractiveness, such as physical attractiveness and indirect victimisation, showed that the likelihood of experiencing indirect victimization increased by 35% for females who perceived themselves as physically attractive.[22] In contrast, being a male who is physically attractive decreased the chances of experiencing such indirect victimization. This also highlights how the physical attractiveness of a female is a trigger for indirect aggression and forms a core part of intersexual selection between the sexes.

Female derogation is also used to enforce equality amongst females which prevents high-status ambitious females from using their status to gain resources, allies and mates at the expense of other females. Thus attempts to gain social status are punished while norms of "niceness" (which is defined as a lack of competitiveness) and equality dominates as a social norm amongst females. Equality is enforced by threat of social exclusion (which can be directed against any female but females attempting to gain status are more likely to be targets) and low thresholds for dissolving relationships when inequality arises. Within a peer group, a high-status woman who tries to interfere with another's goals risks social derision and exclusion.[2]

Slut-shaming edit

Another form of competitor derogation that is instrumental in making rivals appear less desirable is slut-shaming. In slut-shaming, females criticize and derogate same-sex rivals for engaging in sexual behaviors that are deemed "unacceptable" by society's standards, as it violates social expectations and norms with regards to their gender role. For example, an act of sexual promiscuity demonstrated by a female is often considered non-conventional and inappropriate as such behaviors are not viewed as acts that constitute femininity. Females may choose to personally confront or spread rumors and gossip about the promiscuous activity of another female. Buss and Dedden explored sex differences in competitor derogation to investigate the tactics that are commonly adopted by both sexes for intrasexual competition.[23] Researchers presented both sexes with a list of tactics that are often employed by individuals to derogate same-sex competitors in an attempt to make them look undesirable to the opposite sex. On a scale from 1 (likely) to 7 (unlikely), participants rated the likelihood that members of their own sex would perform each act. Results revealed that tactics that pointed out a competitor's promiscuity were used by females more frequently than males. These involved "calling her a tramp", "telling everyone that she sleeps around a lot" and that "she cheats on men". Indeed, accusations of promiscuity are a frequent cause of female-female violence, where females may physically retaliate in a bid to defend their sexual reputation.[24] British schoolgirls were surveyed and asked questions about their involvement in fights. In addition to 89% stating that they had actually been involved in a fight, 46% of reported fights were attacks on personal integrity related to promiscuity or gossiping.[25]

With an ultimate goal of enhancing reproductive success at the expense of others, slut-shaming effectively works to arouse suspicion and cause suitors to question the fidelity of these females. In the long term, men may have doubts regarding the paternity of any offspring produced and since humans strive for reproductive success, (which, for a man is to reproduce and to continually invest in his own children), the decision to mate with such an individual drastically reduces the chances of reproductive success. Considering this and the high-value that men attach to women who practice chastity, men are less likely to mate with a supposedly promiscuous female due to the fear of becoming a cuckold.

The effectiveness of strategies: competitor derogation vs. self-promotion tactics edit

Generally speaking, competitor derogation is often rated as less effective than self-promotion tactics. Men and women tend to judge self-promotion tactics that show resource potential and sexual availability as highly effective for short and long-term mating, respectively.[26] Women, relative to men, appear more likely to engage in self-promotion than competitor derogation tactics.[27] With females having a tendency to engage in more indirect forms of aggression/derogation such as spreading rumors and shunning (social manipulation),[17][18] studies investigate the extent to which such strategies enable females success by increasing their mating opportunities. Common indicators of reproductive success are sexual activity and dating behaviors. Research has found that the use of indirect aggression is positively correlated with increased dating behavior and early engagement in sexual activity. Arnocky and Pavilion[28] investigated whether the use of victimization or personally experiencing victimization could predict the dating behavior of adolescents over a year. In a follow-up assessment, indirect aggression (peer-nominated) was found to predict dating behavior one year after the initial assessment. Moreover, indirect aggression appeared to be a more powerful predictor of dating behavior than other factors such as initial dating status, peer-rated attractiveness, peer-perceived popularity, and age. Overall, females who used indirect aggression were more likely to be dating in comparison to victimized individuals, who were less likely to have a dating partner. The notion that peer aggression is associated with adaptive dating outcomes is further supported by studies that note that females who frequently displayed indirect aggression began dating much earlier in life than individuals who experienced female-female peer victimization, for whom dating behavior had a much later onset.[29] Dating popularity is also found to have a strong association with the use of indirect aggression.[30] With regards to sexual activity, White et al.[31] investigated the influence of peer victimization and perpetuated aggression on reproductive opportunities amongst young adults. Measures of sexual activity such as the number of previous sexual partners and the age of their first sexual intercourse were obtained alongside measures of their social experiences in middle and high school. Results found that females who experienced more peer aggression during adolescence had their first sexual intercourse at a later age. In contrast, females who perpetuated high levels of indirect peer aggression tended to have their first sexual encounter at earlier stages of adolescence.

Variables that influence female competition edit

Females often compete using low-risk strategies compared to males as females have to provide primary care and protection to their offspring.[32] Fisher (2015) suggested that attractiveness is the single route by which women compete and men have shown a preference for attractive women. [33]

Other factors that influence women's intrasexual competition are:

High genetic quality of the males edit

Females will promote themselves more often when males demonstrate various abilities to provide secure resources, protection for offspring, or when the costs of competing are inferior to the benefits gained.[34] They choose males with the highest possible qualities that can maximise reproductive success. Attractiveness and gene quality are both believed to be highly correlated.[35][36] Some research suggests that male attractiveness is biased by female's phenotypic quality, male attractiveness does not necessarily correspond to their gene quality.[37] This leads to the state-dependent choice theory which suggests females with lower qualities prefer low-quality males than high-quality males.[38][39][40] Promiscuity does not affect attractiveness rankings if physical attractiveness outweighs this variable.[41]

Ovarian hormones and hormonal variations edit

The ovarian cycle phase is an emerging concern in exploring issues related to female intrasexual competitive behaviour. It has been found that when fertility rate was maximised during the ovarian phase, women gave significantly lower ratings of attractiveness to other females. Ovarian hormones affect how females view their potential competitors and cause them to behave more competitively.[19][42]

Many studies implied that testosterone levels were one of the key factors in aggressive competitive behaviour in social situations.[43][44] When testosterone is produced in the brain and gonads in both genders, the androgen receptors in neural and peripheral tissues are being possessed and trigger behavioural and physiological responses to testosterone. The role of androgenic steroids is to activate or facilitate aggressive behaviour.[45] High levels of oestrogen are shown to have an effect on women's derogation on potential competitors (e.g. rating other female faces as less attractive) but there is no effect on ratings of male attractiveness.[19]

Interpersonal dynamics edit

Females often compete with their own sex to gain the attention of potential mates with high genetic qualities in order to induce reproductive success.[46][47] Miller et al. (2011)’s study revealed that presence of another sex individual leads to testosterone enhancement.[48]

The ratio of females to males in the course of competition might alter salivary testosterone levels in both genders which lead to competition.[46] The nonequivalent ratio of men with "good genes" to a large number of accessible females also leads to female intrasexual competition. Biosocial status hypothesis[46] indicated that to win in the female competition, it is thought to enhance in testosterone production thus facilitating violent, prevailing behaviours and exhibition of high status. Whereas, losing in female competition lowers testosterone levels which weaken the tendency of competing.[49][50] Testosterone levels correspond to various factors such as form of competition,[43] characteristics of opponent,[51] psychological state and baseline hormone levels of the person competing.

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ Benenson, Joyce F.; Abadzi, Helen (2020). "Contest versus scramble competition: Sex differences in the quest for status". Current Opinion in Psychology. 33: 62–68. doi:10.1016/j.copsyc.2019.07.013. PMID 31400660. S2CID 199342589.
  2. ^ a b Benenson, Joyce F. (2013). "The development of human female competition: allies and adversaries". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 368 (1631). 20130079. doi:10.1098/rstb.2013.0079. PMC 3826208. PMID 24167309.
  3. ^ a b Fisher, Maryanne; Cox, Anthony (2011-03-01). "Four strategies used during intrasexual competition for mates". Personal Relationships. 18 (1): 20–38. doi:10.1111/j.1475-6811.2010.01307.x. ISSN 1475-6811.
  4. ^ Schmitt, D. P.; Buss, D. M. (1996-06-01). "Strategic self-promotion and competitor derogation: sex and context effects on the perceived effectiveness of mate attraction tactics". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 70 (6): 1185–1204. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.387.5516. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.70.6.1185. ISSN 0022-3514. PMID 8667162.
  5. ^ Hudders, L.; Backer, C. De; Fisher, M.; Vyncke, P. (2014-07-01). "The Rival Wears Prada: Luxury Consumption as a Female Competition Strategy". Evolutionary Psychology. 12 (3): 570–587. doi:10.1177/147470491401200306. ISSN 1474-7049. PMID 25299993.
  6. ^ Durante, Kristina M.; Griskevicius, Vladas; Hill, Sarah E.; Perilloux, Carin; Li, Norman P. (2011-04-01). "Ovulation, Female Competition, and Product Choice: Hormonal Influences on Consumer Behavior". Journal of Consumer Research. 37 (6): 921–934. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.173.898. doi:10.1086/656575. ISSN 0093-5301. S2CID 948713.
  7. ^ Haselton, Martie G.; Mortezaie, Mina; Pillsworth, Elizabeth G.; Bleske-Rechek, April; Frederick, David A. (2007-01-01). "Ovulatory shifts in human female ornamentation: Near ovulation, women dress to impress". Hormones and Behavior. 51 (1): 40–45. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2006.07.007. PMID 17045994. S2CID 9268718.
  8. ^ Singh, Devendra; Singh, Dorian (2011-02-06). "Shape and Significance of Feminine Beauty: An Evolutionary Perspective". Sex Roles. 64 (9–10): 723–731. doi:10.1007/s11199-011-9938-z. ISSN 0360-0025. S2CID 144644793.
  9. ^ Singh, Devendra; Randall, Patrick K. (2007-07-01). "Beauty is in the eye of the plastic surgeon: Waist–hip ratio (WHR) and women's attractiveness". Personality and Individual Differences. 43 (2): 329–340. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2006.12.003.
  10. ^ Frith, Katherine Toland; Cheng, Hong; Shaw, Ping (2004-01-01). "Race and Beauty: A Comparison of Asian and Western Models in Women's Magazine Advertisements". Sex Roles. 50 (1–2): 53–61. doi:10.1023/B:SERS.0000011072.84489.e2. ISSN 0360-0025. S2CID 31114363.
  11. ^ Frith, Katherine; Shaw, Ping; Cheng, Hong (2005-03-01). "The Construction of Beauty: A Cross-Cultural Analysis of Women's Magazine Advertising". Journal of Communication. 55 (1): 56–70. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.2005.tb02658.x. ISSN 1460-2466.
  12. ^ Kościński, Krzysztof (July 2014). "Assessment of Waist-to-Hip Ratio Attractiveness in Women: An Anthropometric Analysis of Digital Silhouettes". Archives of Sexual Behavior. 43 (5): 989–997. doi:10.1007/s10508-013-0166-1. ISSN 0004-0002. PMC 4050298. PMID 23975738.
  13. ^ Hendrie, Colin; Chapman, Rhiannon; Gill, Charlotte (2020-01-01). "Women's strategic use of clothing and make-up". Human Ethology. 35 (1): 16–26. doi:10.22330/he/35/016-026.
  14. ^ Shackelford, Todd (2012). The Oxford handbook of evolutionary perspectives on violence, homicide, and war. Oxford, United Kingdom: Oxford University Press. ISBN 9780199738403.
  15. ^ Benenson, Joyce F. (2009-08-01). "Dominating versus eliminating the competition: Sex differences in human intrasexual aggression". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 32 (3–4): 268–269. doi:10.1017/S0140525X0999046X. ISSN 1469-1825.
  16. ^ Richardson, Deborah R.; Green, Laura R. (1999-01-01). "Social sanction and threat explanations of gender effects on direct and indirect aggression". Aggressive Behavior. 25 (6): 425–434. doi:10.1002/(sici)1098-2337(1999)25:6<425::aid-ab3>3.3.co;2-n. ISSN 1098-2337.
  17. ^ a b Bjorkqvist, Kaj; Österman, Karin; Lagerspetz, Kirsti (June 1, 1994). "Sex differences in covert aggression among adults". Aggressive Behavior. 20 (1): 27–33. doi:10.1002/1098-2337(1994)20:1<27::AID-AB2480200105>3.0.CO;2-Q.
  18. ^ a b Vaillancourt, Tracy (2013-12-05). "Do human females use indirect aggression as an intrasexual competition strategy?". Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B. 368 (1631): 20130080. doi:10.1098/rstb.2013.0080. ISSN 0962-8436. PMC 3826209. PMID 24167310.
  19. ^ a b c Fisher, M. L. (2004). "Female intrasexual competition decreases female facial attractiveness". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 271 (Suppl_5): S283–S285. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2004.0160. ISSN 0962-8452. PMC 1810076. PMID 15503995.
  20. ^ Massar, Karlijn; Buunk, Abraham P.; Rempt, Sanna (2012-01-01). "Age differences in women's tendency to gossip are mediated by their mate value". Personality and Individual Differences. 52 (1): 106–109. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.09.013. S2CID 145602586.
  21. ^ Campbell, A (2004). "Female competition: Causes, constraints, content, and contexts". Journal of Sex Research. 41 (1): 16–26. doi:10.1080/00224490409552210. PMID 15216421. S2CID 27896552.
  22. ^ Leenaars, Lindsey S.; Dane, Andrew V.; Marini, Zopito A. (2008-07-01). "Evolutionary perspective on indirect victimization in adolescence: the role of attractiveness, dating and sexual behavior". Aggressive Behavior. 34 (4): 404–415. doi:10.1002/ab.20252. ISSN 1098-2337. PMID 18351598.
  23. ^ Buss, David M.; Dedden, Lisa A. (1990-08-01). "Derogation of Competitors". Journal of Social and Personal Relationships. 7 (3): 395–422. doi:10.1177/0265407590073006. hdl:2027.42/69020. ISSN 0265-4075. S2CID 145522528.
  24. ^ Campell, Anne; Gibbs, John J., eds. (1986). Violent transactions: the limits of personality. New York, USA: Blackwell.
  25. ^ Campbell, Anne (1995-03-01). "A few good men: Evolutionary psychology and female adolescent aggression". Ethology and Sociobiology. 16 (2): 99–123. doi:10.1016/0162-3095(94)00072-F.
  26. ^ Schmitt, David P.; Buss, David M. (1996). "Strategic self-promotion and competitor derogation: Sex and context effects on the perceived effectiveness of mate attraction tactics". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 70 (6): 1185–1204. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.387.5516. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.70.6.1185. PMID 8667162.
  27. ^ Fisher, Maryanne; Cox, Anthony; Gordon, Fiona (2009-12-01). "Self-promotion versus competitor derogation: the influence of sex and romantic relationship status on intrasexual competition strategy selection". Journal of Evolutionary Psychology. 7 (4): 287–308. doi:10.1556/JEP.7.2009.4.6. ISSN 1789-2082.
  28. ^ Arnocky, Steven; Vaillancourt, Tracy (2012-04-01). "A Multi-Informant Longitudinal Study on the Relationship between Aggression, Peer Victimization, and Dating Status in Adolescence". Evolutionary Psychology. 10 (2): 147470491201000207. doi:10.1177/147470491201000207. ISSN 1474-7049. PMC 10426897.
  29. ^ Gallup, Andrew C.; O'Brien, Daniel T.; Wilson, David Sloan (2011-05-01). "Intrasexual peer aggression and dating behavior during adolescence: an evolutionary perspective". Aggressive Behavior. 37 (3): 258–267. doi:10.1002/ab.20384. ISSN 1098-2337. PMID 21433032.
  30. ^ Pellegrini, Anthony D.; Long, Jeffrey D. (2003-07-01). "A sexual selection theory longitudinal analysis of sexual segregation and integration in early adolescence". Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 85 (3): 257–278. doi:10.1016/s0022-0965(03)00060-2. ISSN 0022-0965. PMID 12810038.
  31. ^ White, Daniel D.; Gallup, Andrew C.; Gallup, Gordon G. (2010-01-01). "Indirect peer aggression in adolescence and reproductive behavior". Evolutionary Psychology. 8 (1): 49–65. doi:10.1177/147470491000800106. ISSN 1474-7049. PMC 10480904. PMID 22947779.
  32. ^ Fisher, M. (2015). "Women's competition for mates: Experimental findings leading to ethological studies" (PDF). Human Ethology Bulletin. 30: 53–70.
  33. ^ Buss, David M. (1989). "Sex differences in human mate preferences: Evolutionary hypotheses tested in 37 cultures". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 12 (1): 1–14. doi:10.1017/s0140525x00023992.
  34. ^ Palombit, R.A.; Cheney, D.L.; Seyfarth, R.M. (2001). "Female–female competition for male 'friends' in wild chacma baboons (Papio cynocephalus ursinus)". Animal Behaviour. 61 (6): 1159–1171. doi:10.1006/anbe.2000.1690. ISSN 0003-3472. S2CID 5713145.
  35. ^ Geary, David C. (2005). "Evolution of life-history trade-offs in mate attractiveness and health: Comment on Weeden and Sabini (2005)". Psychological Bulletin. 131 (5): 654–657. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.131.5.654. PMID 16187850.
  36. ^ Weeden, Jason; Sabini, John (2005). "Physical Attractiveness and Health in Western Societies: A Review". Psychological Bulletin. 131 (5): 635–653. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.131.5.635. PMID 16187849. S2CID 24782931.
  37. ^ Riebel K, Holveck MJ, Verhulst S, Fawcett TW (2010). "Are high-quality mates always attractive?: State-dependent mate preferences in birds and humans". Commun Integr Biol. 3 (3): 271–3. doi:10.4161/cib.3.3.11557. PMC 2918774. PMID 20714411.
  38. ^ Buston, P. M.; Emlen, S. T. (2003). "Cognitive processes underlying human mate choice: The relationship between self-perception and mate preference in Western society". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 100 (15): 8805–8810. Bibcode:2003PNAS..100.8805B. doi:10.1073/pnas.1533220100. ISSN 0027-8424. PMC 166394. PMID 12843405.
  39. ^ Todd, P. M.; Penke, L.; Fasolo, B.; Lenton, A. P. (2007). "Different cognitive processes underlie human mate choices and mate preferences". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 104 (38): 15011–15016. Bibcode:2007PNAS..10415011T. doi:10.1073/pnas.0705290104. ISSN 0027-8424. PMC 1986604. PMID 17827279.
  40. ^ Little, A. C.; Burt, D. M.; Penton-Voak, I. S.; Perrett, D. I. (2001). "Self-perceived attractiveness influences human female preferences for sexual dimorphism and symmetry in male faces". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 268 (1462): 39–44. doi:10.1098/rspb.2000.1327. ISSN 0962-8452. PMC 1087598. PMID 12123296.
  41. ^ Rucas, Stacey L.; Gurven, Michael; Kaplan, Hillard; Winking, Jeff; Gangestad, Steve; Crespo, Maria (2006-01-01). "Female intrasexual competition and reputational effects on attractiveness among the Tsimane of Bolivia". Evolution and Human Behavior. 27 (1): 40–52. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2005.07.001. ISSN 1090-5138.
  42. ^ Stockley, Paula; Bro-Jørgensen, Jakob (2011). "Female competition and its evolutionary consequences in mammals". Biological Reviews. 86 (2): 341–366. doi:10.1111/j.1469-185X.2010.00149.x. ISSN 1464-7931. PMID 20636474. S2CID 40904511.
  43. ^ a b Archer, John (2006). "Testosterone and human aggression: an evaluation of the challenge hypothesis". Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 30 (3): 319–345. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.12.007. ISSN 0149-7634. PMID 16483890. S2CID 26405251.
  44. ^ Dabbs, James M.; Hargrove, Marian F.; Heusel, Colleen (1996). "Testosterone differences among college fraternities: well-behaved vs rambunctious". Personality and Individual Differences. 20 (2): 157–161. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(95)00190-5. ISSN 0191-8869.
  45. ^ French, J. A.; Mustoe, A. C.; Cavanaugh, J.; Birnie, A. K. (2013). "The influence of androgenic steroid hormones on female aggression in 'atypical' mammals". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 368 (1631): 20130084. doi:10.1098/rstb.2013.0084. ISSN 0962-8436. PMC 3826213. PMID 24167314.
  46. ^ a b c Miller, Saul L.; Maner, Jon K.; McNulty, James K. (2012). "Adaptive attunement to the sex of individuals at a competition: the ratio of opposite- to same-sex individuals correlates with changes in competitors' testosterone levels". Evolution and Human Behavior. 33 (1): 57–63. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2011.05.006. ISSN 1090-5138.
  47. ^ Buss, David M. (1988). "The evolution of human intrasexual competition: Tactics of mate attraction" (PDF). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 54 (4): 616–628. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.54.4.616. ISSN 0022-3514. PMID 3367282.[dead link]
  48. ^ López, Hassan H.; Hay, Aleena C.; Conklin, Phoebe H. (2009). "Attractive men induce testosterone and cortisol release in women". Hormones and Behavior. 56 (1): 84–92. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2009.03.004. ISSN 0018-506X. PMID 19303881. S2CID 25498105.
  49. ^ Booth, Alan; Shelley, Greg; Mazur, Allan; Tharp, Gerry; Kittok, Roger (1989). "Testosterone, and winning and losing in human competition". Hormones and Behavior. 23 (4): 556–571. doi:10.1016/0018-506X(89)90042-1. ISSN 0018-506X. PMID 2606468. S2CID 36664488.
  50. ^ Mccaul, K (1992). "Winning, losing, mood, and testosterone". Hormones and Behavior. 26 (4): 486–504. doi:10.1016/0018-506X(92)90016-O. ISSN 0018-506X. PMID 1478633. S2CID 8027732.
  51. ^ van der Meij, Leander; Buunk, Abraham P.; Almela, Mercedes; Salvador, Alicia (2010). "Testosterone responses to competition: The opponent's psychological state makes it challenging". Biological Psychology. 84 (2): 330–335. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.03.017. ISSN 0301-0511. PMID 20359521. S2CID 7488612.
  • Huddergs, L., De Backer, C., Fisher, M., & Vyncke, P. (2014). The Rival Wears Prada: Luxury Consumption as a Female Competition Strategy. Evolutionary
  • Singh, D. (2011). "Shape and Significance of Feminine Beauty: An Evolutionary Perspective". Sex Roles. 64 (9–10): 723–731. doi:10.1007/s11199-011-9938-z. S2CID 144644793.

female, intrasexual, competition, competition, between, women, over, potential, mate, such, competition, might, include, self, promotion, derogation, other, women, direct, indirect, aggression, toward, other, women, factors, that, influence, female, intrasexua. Female intrasexual competition is competition between women over a potential mate Such competition might include self promotion derogation of other women and direct and indirect aggression toward other women Factors that influence female intrasexual competition include the genetic quality of available mates hormone levels and interpersonal dynamics There are two modes of sexual selection intersexual selection and intrasexual selection Intersexual selection includes the display of desirable sexual characteristics to attract a potential mate Intrasexual selection is competition between members of the same sex other over a potential mate Compared to males females tend to prefer subtle rather than overt forms of intrasexual competition 1 2 Contents 1 Self promotion tactics 1 1 Luxury consumption 1 2 Cosmetic surgery 1 3 Clothing and make up 2 Competitor derogation 2 1 Female derogation 2 2 Slut shaming 3 The effectiveness of strategies competitor derogation vs self promotion tactics 4 Variables that influence female competition 4 1 High genetic quality of the males 4 2 Ovarian hormones and hormonal variations 4 3 Interpersonal dynamics 5 See also 6 ReferencesSelf promotion tactics editSelf promotion tactics are one of the main strategies that can be used during intrasexual competition for mates 3 It is often perceived to be the most socially desirable strategy as it can be perceived as self improvement rather than an attack on competitors Self promotion tactics are especially useful for when women are looking for short term mates as such tactics will directly promote their sexual availability 4 Luxury consumption edit nbsp A woman shopping a luxury brand Louis Vuitton LV Self promotion tactics refers to the different strategies that women might use to make themselves look better compared to other competing women For example women are interested in luxury items that enhance their attractiveness 5 Luxury items can indicate attractiveness by emphasising a higher status which is a factor that potential mates will take into consideration It has been shown that when women are at their peak fertility they will have an increased awareness and sensitivity to female intrasexual competition This is due to the fact that when women are at their peak fertility this is the most optimal time for them to mate and produce offspring 6 However this tends to be only applied in situations when women are faced with rivals who they consider to be attractive When with an unattractive rival women might not necessarily see them posing any threat as they would feel more attractive in comparison 7 Cosmetic surgery edit nbsp Facial rejuvenation face lift procedure includes elevation of low eyebrows eyelid operations and restoration of the natural fullness of the lips nbsp East Asian blepharoplasty a surgery designed to add a crease to the eyelid has become one of the most common facial surgical procedures in Asian countries By using plastic surgery women can surgically change their appearances to make themselves more attractive They can surgically alter their faces and bodies according to their wishes They can use botulinum toxin to prevent wrinkles and get face lifts Or they can use get liposuction to remove fat and achieve a more desirable body Research has shown that the waist hip ratio WHR of a female is a good indicator of their health and that males tend to have a preference for females with a low WHR 8 When comparing women s pre and post operative photographs post operative photographs where women have a lower WHR are rated as more attractive regardless of their weight gain or their body mass index BMI 9 Culture plays a role in the type of plastic surgery a woman gets The beauty standards for Westerners and Easterners are extremely different Western models tend to be used to promote clothing and to portray seductiveness whereas Asian models tend to be used to promote hair and skin products Research suggest that Western models are more body oriented 10 11 Regardless by using cosmetic surgery females can change various aspects of their body to make themselves more attractive by displaying a more desirable waist hip ratio This can lead to competition with other females who may be considered less attractive in comparison When women change their appearances such as by applying cosmetic products and wearing sexy or stylish clothes do make a difference and has been proven to be effective 3 Clothing and make up edit Wearing form fitting attire such as dresses that highlight the waist and hips or pants that accentuate curves is a common strategy to enhance the overall figure Such outfits highlight the importance of the waist hip ratio WHR 12 Short skirts high heels and attire that showcases the legs These clothing choices subtly emphasize leg length and overall body proportions which are characteristics often deemed attractive Makeup too plays a role in self promotion allowing to enhance certain facial features and draw attention to specific areas such as the eyes and lips 13 Competitor derogation editMain article Relational aggression There are a number of competitive strategies that females may use in a bid to appear more attractive in comparison to other females Whilst males may use direct forms of aggression during intrasexual competition 14 15 females typically compete for access to desired mates through the use of indirect aggression Unlike direct aggression which involves delivering harm face to face 16 indirect aggression describes acts that are done circuitously where an individual aims to cause harm but attempts to appear as if they have no harmful intentions 17 In the context of intrasexual competition indirect aggression works to reduce the opportunities the rival may have in securing access to the desired mate to therefore increase one s chances of reproductive success 18 These include behaviours such as shunning social exclusion getting others to dislike the individual spreading rumours and criticizing the rival s appearance Female derogation edit Female derogation is a form of indirect aggression where females attempt to reduce the perceived value of another female rival Fisher 2004 19 studied female derogation and the effects of estrogen levels on this form of competition Females disclosed their ovulation status and rated the attractiveness of male and female faces Competitor derogation giving low ratings towards same sex rivals occurred frequently when women were at their most fertile stages In contrast women gave same sex rivals higher ratings during the least fertile stages of their ovulation This indirect form of competition appears exclusive toward females as findings also showed that women irrespective of ovulation status high or low showed no difference in the rating of male faces Supporting research has also found that younger women who are considered as having high fertility gossip about other women more than older women who are no longer at their most fertile stage 20 Indeed indirect aggression appears more prevalent amongst or exclusive to females than males who are said to engage in more direct forms of competition 21 Research studying the relationship between indicators of attractiveness such as physical attractiveness and indirect victimisation showed that the likelihood of experiencing indirect victimization increased by 35 for females who perceived themselves as physically attractive 22 In contrast being a male who is physically attractive decreased the chances of experiencing such indirect victimization This also highlights how the physical attractiveness of a female is a trigger for indirect aggression and forms a core part of intersexual selection between the sexes Female derogation is also used to enforce equality amongst females which prevents high status ambitious females from using their status to gain resources allies and mates at the expense of other females Thus attempts to gain social status are punished while norms of niceness which is defined as a lack of competitiveness and equality dominates as a social norm amongst females Equality is enforced by threat of social exclusion which can be directed against any female but females attempting to gain status are more likely to be targets and low thresholds for dissolving relationships when inequality arises Within a peer group a high status woman who tries to interfere with another s goals risks social derision and exclusion 2 Slut shaming edit Another form of competitor derogation that is instrumental in making rivals appear less desirable is slut shaming In slut shaming females criticize and derogate same sex rivals for engaging in sexual behaviors that are deemed unacceptable by society s standards as it violates social expectations and norms with regards to their gender role For example an act of sexual promiscuity demonstrated by a female is often considered non conventional and inappropriate as such behaviors are not viewed as acts that constitute femininity Females may choose to personally confront or spread rumors and gossip about the promiscuous activity of another female Buss and Dedden explored sex differences in competitor derogation to investigate the tactics that are commonly adopted by both sexes for intrasexual competition 23 Researchers presented both sexes with a list of tactics that are often employed by individuals to derogate same sex competitors in an attempt to make them look undesirable to the opposite sex On a scale from 1 likely to 7 unlikely participants rated the likelihood that members of their own sex would perform each act Results revealed that tactics that pointed out a competitor s promiscuity were used by females more frequently than males These involved calling her a tramp telling everyone that she sleeps around a lot and that she cheats on men Indeed accusations of promiscuity are a frequent cause of female female violence where females may physically retaliate in a bid to defend their sexual reputation 24 British schoolgirls were surveyed and asked questions about their involvement in fights In addition to 89 stating that they had actually been involved in a fight 46 of reported fights were attacks on personal integrity related to promiscuity or gossiping 25 With an ultimate goal of enhancing reproductive success at the expense of others slut shaming effectively works to arouse suspicion and cause suitors to question the fidelity of these females In the long term men may have doubts regarding the paternity of any offspring produced and since humans strive for reproductive success which for a man is to reproduce and to continually invest in his own children the decision to mate with such an individual drastically reduces the chances of reproductive success Considering this and the high value that men attach to women who practice chastity men are less likely to mate with a supposedly promiscuous female due to the fear of becoming a cuckold The effectiveness of strategies competitor derogation vs self promotion tactics editGenerally speaking competitor derogation is often rated as less effective than self promotion tactics Men and women tend to judge self promotion tactics that show resource potential and sexual availability as highly effective for short and long term mating respectively 26 Women relative to men appear more likely to engage in self promotion than competitor derogation tactics 27 With females having a tendency to engage in more indirect forms of aggression derogation such as spreading rumors and shunning social manipulation 17 18 studies investigate the extent to which such strategies enable females success by increasing their mating opportunities Common indicators of reproductive success are sexual activity and dating behaviors Research has found that the use of indirect aggression is positively correlated with increased dating behavior and early engagement in sexual activity Arnocky and Pavilion 28 investigated whether the use of victimization or personally experiencing victimization could predict the dating behavior of adolescents over a year In a follow up assessment indirect aggression peer nominated was found to predict dating behavior one year after the initial assessment Moreover indirect aggression appeared to be a more powerful predictor of dating behavior than other factors such as initial dating status peer rated attractiveness peer perceived popularity and age Overall females who used indirect aggression were more likely to be dating in comparison to victimized individuals who were less likely to have a dating partner The notion that peer aggression is associated with adaptive dating outcomes is further supported by studies that note that females who frequently displayed indirect aggression began dating much earlier in life than individuals who experienced female female peer victimization for whom dating behavior had a much later onset 29 Dating popularity is also found to have a strong association with the use of indirect aggression 30 With regards to sexual activity White et al 31 investigated the influence of peer victimization and perpetuated aggression on reproductive opportunities amongst young adults Measures of sexual activity such as the number of previous sexual partners and the age of their first sexual intercourse were obtained alongside measures of their social experiences in middle and high school Results found that females who experienced more peer aggression during adolescence had their first sexual intercourse at a later age In contrast females who perpetuated high levels of indirect peer aggression tended to have their first sexual encounter at earlier stages of adolescence Variables that influence female competition editFemales often compete using low risk strategies compared to males as females have to provide primary care and protection to their offspring 32 Fisher 2015 suggested that attractiveness is the single route by which women compete and men have shown a preference for attractive women 33 Other factors that influence women s intrasexual competition are High genetic quality of the males edit Females will promote themselves more often when males demonstrate various abilities to provide secure resources protection for offspring or when the costs of competing are inferior to the benefits gained 34 They choose males with the highest possible qualities that can maximise reproductive success Attractiveness and gene quality are both believed to be highly correlated 35 36 Some research suggests that male attractiveness is biased by female s phenotypic quality male attractiveness does not necessarily correspond to their gene quality 37 This leads to the state dependent choice theory which suggests females with lower qualities prefer low quality males than high quality males 38 39 40 Promiscuity does not affect attractiveness rankings if physical attractiveness outweighs this variable 41 Ovarian hormones and hormonal variations edit The ovarian cycle phase is an emerging concern in exploring issues related to female intrasexual competitive behaviour It has been found that when fertility rate was maximised during the ovarian phase women gave significantly lower ratings of attractiveness to other females Ovarian hormones affect how females view their potential competitors and cause them to behave more competitively 19 42 Many studies implied that testosterone levels were one of the key factors in aggressive competitive behaviour in social situations 43 44 When testosterone is produced in the brain and gonads in both genders the androgen receptors in neural and peripheral tissues are being possessed and trigger behavioural and physiological responses to testosterone The role of androgenic steroids is to activate or facilitate aggressive behaviour 45 High levels of oestrogen are shown to have an effect on women s derogation on potential competitors e g rating other female faces as less attractive but there is no effect on ratings of male attractiveness 19 Interpersonal dynamics edit Females often compete with their own sex to gain the attention of potential mates with high genetic qualities in order to induce reproductive success 46 47 Miller et al 2011 s study revealed that presence of another sex individual leads to testosterone enhancement 48 The ratio of females to males in the course of competition might alter salivary testosterone levels in both genders which lead to competition 46 The nonequivalent ratio of men with good genes to a large number of accessible females also leads to female intrasexual competition Biosocial status hypothesis 46 indicated that to win in the female competition it is thought to enhance in testosterone production thus facilitating violent prevailing behaviours and exhibition of high status Whereas losing in female competition lowers testosterone levels which weaken the tendency of competing 49 50 Testosterone levels correspond to various factors such as form of competition 43 characteristics of opponent 51 psychological state and baseline hormone levels of the person competing See also editDominance hierarchy Sexual selection Male intrasexual competition Queen Bees and WannabesReferences edit Benenson Joyce F Abadzi Helen 2020 Contest versus scramble competition Sex differences in the quest for status Current Opinion in Psychology 33 62 68 doi 10 1016 j copsyc 2019 07 013 PMID 31400660 S2CID 199342589 a b Benenson Joyce F 2013 The development of human female competition allies and adversaries Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences 368 1631 20130079 doi 10 1098 rstb 2013 0079 PMC 3826208 PMID 24167309 a b Fisher Maryanne Cox Anthony 2011 03 01 Four strategies used during intrasexual competition for mates Personal Relationships 18 1 20 38 doi 10 1111 j 1475 6811 2010 01307 x ISSN 1475 6811 Schmitt D P Buss D M 1996 06 01 Strategic self promotion and competitor derogation sex and context effects on the perceived effectiveness of mate attraction tactics Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 70 6 1185 1204 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 387 5516 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 70 6 1185 ISSN 0022 3514 PMID 8667162 Hudders L Backer C De Fisher M Vyncke P 2014 07 01 The Rival Wears Prada Luxury Consumption as a Female Competition Strategy Evolutionary Psychology 12 3 570 587 doi 10 1177 147470491401200306 ISSN 1474 7049 PMID 25299993 Durante Kristina M Griskevicius Vladas Hill Sarah E Perilloux Carin Li Norman P 2011 04 01 Ovulation Female Competition and Product Choice Hormonal Influences on Consumer Behavior Journal of Consumer Research 37 6 921 934 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 173 898 doi 10 1086 656575 ISSN 0093 5301 S2CID 948713 Haselton Martie G Mortezaie Mina Pillsworth Elizabeth G Bleske Rechek April Frederick David A 2007 01 01 Ovulatory shifts in human female ornamentation Near ovulation women dress to impress Hormones and Behavior 51 1 40 45 doi 10 1016 j yhbeh 2006 07 007 PMID 17045994 S2CID 9268718 Singh Devendra Singh Dorian 2011 02 06 Shape and Significance of Feminine Beauty An Evolutionary Perspective Sex Roles 64 9 10 723 731 doi 10 1007 s11199 011 9938 z ISSN 0360 0025 S2CID 144644793 Singh Devendra Randall Patrick K 2007 07 01 Beauty is in the eye of the plastic surgeon Waist hip ratio WHR and women s attractiveness Personality and Individual Differences 43 2 329 340 doi 10 1016 j paid 2006 12 003 Frith Katherine Toland Cheng Hong Shaw Ping 2004 01 01 Race and Beauty A Comparison of Asian and Western Models in Women s Magazine Advertisements Sex Roles 50 1 2 53 61 doi 10 1023 B SERS 0000011072 84489 e2 ISSN 0360 0025 S2CID 31114363 Frith Katherine Shaw Ping Cheng Hong 2005 03 01 The Construction of Beauty A Cross Cultural Analysis of Women s Magazine Advertising Journal of Communication 55 1 56 70 doi 10 1111 j 1460 2466 2005 tb02658 x ISSN 1460 2466 Koscinski Krzysztof July 2014 Assessment of Waist to Hip Ratio Attractiveness in Women An Anthropometric Analysis of Digital Silhouettes Archives of Sexual Behavior 43 5 989 997 doi 10 1007 s10508 013 0166 1 ISSN 0004 0002 PMC 4050298 PMID 23975738 Hendrie Colin Chapman Rhiannon Gill Charlotte 2020 01 01 Women s strategic use of clothing and make up Human Ethology 35 1 16 26 doi 10 22330 he 35 016 026 Shackelford Todd 2012 The Oxford handbook of evolutionary perspectives on violence homicide and war Oxford United Kingdom Oxford University Press ISBN 9780199738403 Benenson Joyce F 2009 08 01 Dominating versus eliminating the competition Sex differences in human intrasexual aggression Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32 3 4 268 269 doi 10 1017 S0140525X0999046X ISSN 1469 1825 Richardson Deborah R Green Laura R 1999 01 01 Social sanction and threat explanations of gender effects on direct and indirect aggression Aggressive Behavior 25 6 425 434 doi 10 1002 sici 1098 2337 1999 25 6 lt 425 aid ab3 gt 3 3 co 2 n ISSN 1098 2337 a b Bjorkqvist Kaj Osterman Karin Lagerspetz Kirsti June 1 1994 Sex differences in covert aggression among adults Aggressive Behavior 20 1 27 33 doi 10 1002 1098 2337 1994 20 1 lt 27 AID AB2480200105 gt 3 0 CO 2 Q a b Vaillancourt Tracy 2013 12 05 Do human females use indirect aggression as an intrasexual competition strategy Phil Trans R Soc B 368 1631 20130080 doi 10 1098 rstb 2013 0080 ISSN 0962 8436 PMC 3826209 PMID 24167310 a b c Fisher M L 2004 Female intrasexual competition decreases female facial attractiveness Proceedings of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences 271 Suppl 5 S283 S285 doi 10 1098 rsbl 2004 0160 ISSN 0962 8452 PMC 1810076 PMID 15503995 Massar Karlijn Buunk Abraham P Rempt Sanna 2012 01 01 Age differences in women s tendency to gossip are mediated by their mate value Personality and Individual Differences 52 1 106 109 doi 10 1016 j paid 2011 09 013 S2CID 145602586 Campbell A 2004 Female competition Causes constraints content and contexts Journal of Sex Research 41 1 16 26 doi 10 1080 00224490409552210 PMID 15216421 S2CID 27896552 Leenaars Lindsey S Dane Andrew V Marini Zopito A 2008 07 01 Evolutionary perspective on indirect victimization in adolescence the role of attractiveness dating and sexual behavior Aggressive Behavior 34 4 404 415 doi 10 1002 ab 20252 ISSN 1098 2337 PMID 18351598 Buss David M Dedden Lisa A 1990 08 01 Derogation of Competitors Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 7 3 395 422 doi 10 1177 0265407590073006 hdl 2027 42 69020 ISSN 0265 4075 S2CID 145522528 Campell Anne Gibbs John J eds 1986 Violent transactions the limits of personality New York USA Blackwell Campbell Anne 1995 03 01 A few good men Evolutionary psychology and female adolescent aggression Ethology and Sociobiology 16 2 99 123 doi 10 1016 0162 3095 94 00072 F Schmitt David P Buss David M 1996 Strategic self promotion and competitor derogation Sex and context effects on the perceived effectiveness of mate attraction tactics Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 70 6 1185 1204 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 387 5516 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 70 6 1185 PMID 8667162 Fisher Maryanne Cox Anthony Gordon Fiona 2009 12 01 Self promotion versus competitor derogation the influence of sex and romantic relationship status on intrasexual competition strategy selection Journal of Evolutionary Psychology 7 4 287 308 doi 10 1556 JEP 7 2009 4 6 ISSN 1789 2082 Arnocky Steven Vaillancourt Tracy 2012 04 01 A Multi Informant Longitudinal Study on the Relationship between Aggression Peer Victimization and Dating Status in Adolescence Evolutionary Psychology 10 2 147470491201000207 doi 10 1177 147470491201000207 ISSN 1474 7049 PMC 10426897 Gallup Andrew C O Brien Daniel T Wilson David Sloan 2011 05 01 Intrasexual peer aggression and dating behavior during adolescence an evolutionary perspective Aggressive Behavior 37 3 258 267 doi 10 1002 ab 20384 ISSN 1098 2337 PMID 21433032 Pellegrini Anthony D Long Jeffrey D 2003 07 01 A sexual selection theory longitudinal analysis of sexual segregation and integration in early adolescence Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 85 3 257 278 doi 10 1016 s0022 0965 03 00060 2 ISSN 0022 0965 PMID 12810038 White Daniel D Gallup Andrew C Gallup Gordon G 2010 01 01 Indirect peer aggression in adolescence and reproductive behavior Evolutionary Psychology 8 1 49 65 doi 10 1177 147470491000800106 ISSN 1474 7049 PMC 10480904 PMID 22947779 Fisher M 2015 Women s competition for mates Experimental findings leading to ethological studies PDF Human Ethology Bulletin 30 53 70 Buss David M 1989 Sex differences in human mate preferences Evolutionary hypotheses tested in 37 cultures Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 1 1 14 doi 10 1017 s0140525x00023992 Palombit R A Cheney D L Seyfarth R M 2001 Female female competition for male friends in wild chacma baboons Papio cynocephalus ursinus Animal Behaviour 61 6 1159 1171 doi 10 1006 anbe 2000 1690 ISSN 0003 3472 S2CID 5713145 Geary David C 2005 Evolution of life history trade offs in mate attractiveness and health Comment on Weeden and Sabini 2005 Psychological Bulletin 131 5 654 657 doi 10 1037 0033 2909 131 5 654 PMID 16187850 Weeden Jason Sabini John 2005 Physical Attractiveness and Health in Western Societies A Review Psychological Bulletin 131 5 635 653 doi 10 1037 0033 2909 131 5 635 PMID 16187849 S2CID 24782931 Riebel K Holveck MJ Verhulst S Fawcett TW 2010 Are high quality mates always attractive State dependent mate preferences in birds and humans Commun Integr Biol 3 3 271 3 doi 10 4161 cib 3 3 11557 PMC 2918774 PMID 20714411 Buston P M Emlen S T 2003 Cognitive processes underlying human mate choice The relationship between self perception and mate preference in Western society Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 100 15 8805 8810 Bibcode 2003PNAS 100 8805B doi 10 1073 pnas 1533220100 ISSN 0027 8424 PMC 166394 PMID 12843405 Todd P M Penke L Fasolo B Lenton A P 2007 Different cognitive processes underlie human mate choices and mate preferences Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 104 38 15011 15016 Bibcode 2007PNAS 10415011T doi 10 1073 pnas 0705290104 ISSN 0027 8424 PMC 1986604 PMID 17827279 Little A C Burt D M Penton Voak I S Perrett D I 2001 Self perceived attractiveness influences human female preferences for sexual dimorphism and symmetry in male faces Proceedings of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences 268 1462 39 44 doi 10 1098 rspb 2000 1327 ISSN 0962 8452 PMC 1087598 PMID 12123296 Rucas Stacey L Gurven Michael Kaplan Hillard Winking Jeff Gangestad Steve Crespo Maria 2006 01 01 Female intrasexual competition and reputational effects on attractiveness among the Tsimane of Bolivia Evolution and Human Behavior 27 1 40 52 doi 10 1016 j evolhumbehav 2005 07 001 ISSN 1090 5138 Stockley Paula Bro Jorgensen Jakob 2011 Female competition and its evolutionary consequences in mammals Biological Reviews 86 2 341 366 doi 10 1111 j 1469 185X 2010 00149 x ISSN 1464 7931 PMID 20636474 S2CID 40904511 a b Archer John 2006 Testosterone and human aggression an evaluation of the challenge hypothesis Neuroscience amp Biobehavioral Reviews 30 3 319 345 doi 10 1016 j neubiorev 2004 12 007 ISSN 0149 7634 PMID 16483890 S2CID 26405251 Dabbs James M Hargrove Marian F Heusel Colleen 1996 Testosterone differences among college fraternities well behaved vs rambunctious Personality and Individual Differences 20 2 157 161 doi 10 1016 0191 8869 95 00190 5 ISSN 0191 8869 French J A Mustoe A C Cavanaugh J Birnie A K 2013 The influence of androgenic steroid hormones on female aggression in atypical mammals Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences 368 1631 20130084 doi 10 1098 rstb 2013 0084 ISSN 0962 8436 PMC 3826213 PMID 24167314 a b c Miller Saul L Maner Jon K McNulty James K 2012 Adaptive attunement to the sex of individuals at a competition the ratio of opposite to same sex individuals correlates with changes in competitors testosterone levels Evolution and Human Behavior 33 1 57 63 doi 10 1016 j evolhumbehav 2011 05 006 ISSN 1090 5138 Buss David M 1988 The evolution of human intrasexual competition Tactics of mate attraction PDF Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 54 4 616 628 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 54 4 616 ISSN 0022 3514 PMID 3367282 dead link Lopez Hassan H Hay Aleena C Conklin Phoebe H 2009 Attractive men induce testosterone and cortisol release in women Hormones and Behavior 56 1 84 92 doi 10 1016 j yhbeh 2009 03 004 ISSN 0018 506X PMID 19303881 S2CID 25498105 Booth Alan Shelley Greg Mazur Allan Tharp Gerry Kittok Roger 1989 Testosterone and winning and losing in human competition Hormones and Behavior 23 4 556 571 doi 10 1016 0018 506X 89 90042 1 ISSN 0018 506X PMID 2606468 S2CID 36664488 Mccaul K 1992 Winning losing mood and testosterone Hormones and Behavior 26 4 486 504 doi 10 1016 0018 506X 92 90016 O ISSN 0018 506X PMID 1478633 S2CID 8027732 van der Meij Leander Buunk Abraham P Almela Mercedes Salvador Alicia 2010 Testosterone responses to competition The opponent s psychological state makes it challenging Biological Psychology 84 2 330 335 doi 10 1016 j biopsycho 2010 03 017 ISSN 0301 0511 PMID 20359521 S2CID 7488612 Huddergs L De Backer C Fisher M amp Vyncke P 2014 The Rival Wears Prada Luxury Consumption as a Female Competition Strategy Evolutionary Singh D 2011 Shape and Significance of Feminine Beauty An Evolutionary Perspective Sex Roles 64 9 10 723 731 doi 10 1007 s11199 011 9938 z S2CID 144644793 Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Female intrasexual competition amp oldid 1215374450, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.