fbpx
Wikipedia

Empathic concern

Empathic concern refers to other-oriented emotions elicited by, and congruent with the perceived welfare of, someone in need.[1] These other-oriented emotions include feelings of tenderness, sympathy, compassion and soft-heartedness.

Empathic concern is often confused with empathy. To empathize is to respond to another's perceived emotional state by experiencing feeling of a similar sort. Empathic concern or sympathy includes not only empathizing, but also having a positive regard or a non-fleeting concern for the other person.[2]

Definition edit

C. Daniel Batson, a pioneer of the term, defines it as "other-oriented emotion elicited by and congruent with the perceived welfare of someone in need".[3] Batson explains this definition in the following way:

First, "congruent" here refers not to the specific content of the emotion but to the valence—positive when the perceived welfare of the other is positive, negative when the perceived welfare is negative.… Third, as defined, empathic concern is not a single, discrete emotion but includes a whole constellation. It includes feelings of sympathy, compassion, softheartedness, tenderness, sorrow, sadness, upset, distress, concern, and grief. Fourth, empathic concern is other-oriented in the sense that it involves feeling for the other—feeling sympathy for, compassion for, sorry for, distressed for, concerned for, and so on.[3]

Others use different terms for this construct or very similar constructs. Especially popular—perhaps more popular than "empathic concern"—are sympathy, compassion, or pity.[4] Other terms include the tender emotion and sympathetic distress.[5]

People are strongly motivated to be connected to others.[6] In humans and higher mammals, an impulse to care for offspring is almost certainly genetically hard-wired, although modifiable by circumstance.

Evolutionary origins edit

At the behavioral level it is evident from the descriptions by comparative psychologists and ethologists that behaviors homologous to empathic concern can be observed in other mammalian species. Notably, a variety of reports on ape empathic reactions suggest that, apart from emotional connectedness, apes have an explicit appreciation of the other's situation.[7] A good example is consolation, defined as reassurance behavior by an uninvolved bystander towards one of the combatants in a previous aggressive incident.[8]

Developmental origins edit

Empathic concern is thought to emerge later in development and to require more self-control than either emotional contagion or personal distress.[9] Developmental research indicates a broad range of social competencies children bring to their interpersonal relationships.[10] As early as two years of age, children show (a) the cognitive capacity to interpret, in simple ways, the physical and psychological states of others, (b) the emotional capacity to experience, affectively, the state of others, and (c) the behavioral repertoire that permits attempts to alleviate discomfort in others.[11]

Both temperament and social context contribute to individual differences in concern for others. Some developmental psychologists have hypothesized that empathic concern for others is an essential factor inhibiting aggression.[12]

Contribution of social psychology edit

Empathic concern may produce an altruistic motivation to help people.[13] The challenge of demonstrating the existence of altruistic motivation is to show how empathic concern leads to helping in ways that cannot be explained by prevailing theories of egoistic motivation. That is, a clear case needs to be made that it is concern about the other person's welfare, not a desire to improve one's own welfare, that primarily drives one's helping behavior in a particular situation.

Empirical studies conducted by social psychologist Daniel Batson demonstrate that one feels empathic concern when one adopts the perspective of another person in need. His work emphasizes the different emotions evoked when imagining another situation from a self-perspective or imagining from another perspective.[14] The former is often associated with personal distress (i.e., feelings of discomfort and anxiety), whereas the latter leads to empathic concern.

Social neuroscience evidence edit

Social neuroscience explores the biological underpinnings of empathic concern (and more generally, interpersonal sensitivity), using an integrative approach that bridges the biological and social levels.[15] Neural systems, including autonomic functions, that rely on brain stem neuropeptides, such as oxytocin and vasopressin, are plausible correlates for empathic concern. Alternatively, vasopressin might be implicated in situations where a more active strategy is required for an effective response.[16]

An association between executive functions, underpinned by the prefrontal cortex with reciprocal connections with the limbic system, the sense of agency, and empathic concern has been suggested based on lesion studies in neurological patients and functional neuroimaging experiments in healthy individuals.[17]

The difference between imagining from one's own perspective and imagining in the shoes or from the perspective of others is supported by a series of functional neuroimaging studies of affective processing. For instance, participants in one study reported more empathic concern when imagining the pain of others when adopting another's perspective, and more personal distress when imagining themselves to be in pain.[18]

fMRI scans revealed that imagining self in pain was associated with strong activation in brain areas involved in affective response to threat and pain, including the amygdala, insula, and anterior cingulate cortex. Imagine-other instructions produced higher activity in the right temporoparietal junction, which is associated with self-other distinctiveness and the sense of agency.

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ Batson, C.D. (1991). The altruism question: Toward a social-psychological answer. Hillsdale: Erlbaum Associates.
    • Batson, C.D. (1987). "Prosocial motivation: Is it ever truly altruistic?" In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 20, pp. 65–122). New York: Academic Press.
  2. ^ Chismar, D. (1988). "Empathy and sympathy: the important difference." The Journal of Value Inquiry, 22, pp. 257–266.
  3. ^ a b Batson, C.D. (2011). Altruism in Humans, Oxford: Oxford University Press, p. 11.
  4. ^ These are used by numerous philosophers and social scientists including Charles Darwin, Frans de Waal, Nancy Eisenberg, and Stephen Darwall, as documented in Batson's Altruism in Humans, p. 12.
  5. ^ These latter two terms are due, respectively, to William McDougall and Martin Hoffman. See Batson, C.D. (2011). Altruism in Humans, Oxford: Oxford University Press, p. 12.
  6. ^ Batson, C.D. (1990). "How social an animal? The human capacity for caring." American Psychologist 45: 336–346.
  7. ^ De Waal, F.B.M. (1996). Good Natured: The origins of right and wrong in humans and other animals. Harvard: Harvard University Press.
  8. ^ De Waal, F.B.M., & van Roosmalen, A. (1979). "Reconciliation and consolation among chimpanzees." Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 5, 55–66.
  9. ^ Eisenberg, N., & Strayer, J. (1997). Empathy and its Development. New York: Cambridge University Press.
  10. ^ Decety, J., & Meyer, M. (2008). "From emotion resonance to empathic understanding: A social developmental neuroscience account." Development and Psychopathology, 20: 1053–1080.
  11. ^ Zahn-Waxler, C, Radke-Yarrow, M., Wagner, E., & Chapman, M. (1992). "Development of concern for others." Developmental Psychology, 28: 126–136.
  12. ^ Eisenberg, N., & Eggum, N.D. (2009). "Empathic responding: sympathy and personal distress." In: J. Decety and W. Ickes, The Social Neuroscience of Empathy. Cambridge: MIT Press, pp. 71–83.
    • Hoffman, M.L. (2000). Empathy and Moral Development. New York: Cambridge University Press.
  13. ^ Batson, C.D., & Shaw, L.L. (1991). "Evidence for altruism: Toward a pluralism of prosocial motives." Psychological Inquiry, 2: 107–122.
  14. ^ Batson, C.D. (2009). "Two forms of perspective taking: imagining how another feels and imagining how you would feel." In K.D. Markman, W.M.P. Klein and J.A. Suhr (Eds.), Handbook of Imagination and Mental Simulation pp. 267–279. New York: Psychology Press.
  15. ^ Decety, J., & Keenan, J.P. (2006). "Social neuroscience: A new journal." Social Neuroscience, 1: 1–4.
  16. ^ Carter, C.S., Harris, J., & Porges, S.W. (2009). "Neural and evolutionary perspectives on empathy" In: J. Decety and W. Ickes, The Social Neuroscience of Empathy. Cambridge: MIT press, pp. 169–182.
  17. ^ Decety, J., & Jackson, P.L. (2004). "The functional architecture of human empathy." Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, 3: 71–100.
  18. ^ Lamm, C., Batson, C.D., & Decety, J. (2007). "The neural substrate of human empathy: effects of perspective-taking and cognitive appraisal." Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19: 42–58.

Further reading edit

  • Batson, C.D. (2009). "These things called empathy: eight related but distinct phenomena." In J. Decety & W. Ickes (Eds.), The Social Neuroscience of Empathy (pp. 3–15). Cambridge: MIT press.
  • Batson, C.D. (2009). "Two forms of perspective taking: imagining how another feels and imagining how you would feel." In K. D. Markman, W.M.P. Klein and J.A. Suhr (Eds.), Handbook of Imagination and Mental Simulation (pp. 267–279). New York: Psychology Press.
  • Decety, J. & Batson, C.D. (2007). "Social neuroscience approaches to interpersonal sensitivity." Social Neuroscience, 2(3-4): 151–157.
  • Decety, J. & Ickes, W. (Eds.). (2009). The Social Neuroscience of Empathy. Cambridge: MIT Press, Cambridge.
  • Thompson, E. (2001). "Empathy and consciousness." Journal of Consciousness Studies, 8: 1–32.
  • Zahn-Waxler, C., & Radke-Yarrow, M. (1990). "The origins of empathic concern." Motivation and Emotion, 14: 107–125.

empathic, concern, refers, other, oriented, emotions, elicited, congruent, with, perceived, welfare, someone, need, these, other, oriented, emotions, include, feelings, tenderness, sympathy, compassion, soft, heartedness, often, confused, with, empathy, empath. Empathic concern refers to other oriented emotions elicited by and congruent with the perceived welfare of someone in need 1 These other oriented emotions include feelings of tenderness sympathy compassion and soft heartedness Empathic concern is often confused with empathy To empathize is to respond to another s perceived emotional state by experiencing feeling of a similar sort Empathic concern or sympathy includes not only empathizing but also having a positive regard or a non fleeting concern for the other person 2 Contents 1 Definition 2 Evolutionary origins 3 Developmental origins 4 Contribution of social psychology 5 Social neuroscience evidence 6 See also 7 References 8 Further readingDefinition editC Daniel Batson a pioneer of the term defines it as other oriented emotion elicited by and congruent with the perceived welfare of someone in need 3 Batson explains this definition in the following way First congruent here refers not to the specific content of the emotion but to the valence positive when the perceived welfare of the other is positive negative when the perceived welfare is negative Third as defined empathic concern is not a single discrete emotion but includes a whole constellation It includes feelings of sympathy compassion softheartedness tenderness sorrow sadness upset distress concern and grief Fourth empathic concern is other oriented in the sense that it involves feeling for the other feeling sympathy for compassion for sorry for distressed for concerned for and so on 3 Others use different terms for this construct or very similar constructs Especially popular perhaps more popular than empathic concern are sympathy compassion or pity 4 Other terms include the tender emotion and sympathetic distress 5 People are strongly motivated to be connected to others 6 In humans and higher mammals an impulse to care for offspring is almost certainly genetically hard wired although modifiable by circumstance Evolutionary origins editAt the behavioral level it is evident from the descriptions by comparative psychologists and ethologists that behaviors homologous to empathic concern can be observed in other mammalian species Notably a variety of reports on ape empathic reactions suggest that apart from emotional connectedness apes have an explicit appreciation of the other s situation 7 A good example is consolation defined as reassurance behavior by an uninvolved bystander towards one of the combatants in a previous aggressive incident 8 Developmental origins editEmpathic concern is thought to emerge later in development and to require more self control than either emotional contagion or personal distress 9 Developmental research indicates a broad range of social competencies children bring to their interpersonal relationships 10 As early as two years of age children show a the cognitive capacity to interpret in simple ways the physical and psychological states of others b the emotional capacity to experience affectively the state of others and c the behavioral repertoire that permits attempts to alleviate discomfort in others 11 Both temperament and social context contribute to individual differences in concern for others Some developmental psychologists have hypothesized that empathic concern for others is an essential factor inhibiting aggression 12 Contribution of social psychology editEmpathic concern may produce an altruistic motivation to help people 13 The challenge of demonstrating the existence of altruistic motivation is to show how empathic concern leads to helping in ways that cannot be explained by prevailing theories of egoistic motivation That is a clear case needs to be made that it is concern about the other person s welfare not a desire to improve one s own welfare that primarily drives one s helping behavior in a particular situation Empirical studies conducted by social psychologist Daniel Batson demonstrate that one feels empathic concern when one adopts the perspective of another person in need His work emphasizes the different emotions evoked when imagining another situation from a self perspective or imagining from another perspective 14 The former is often associated with personal distress i e feelings of discomfort and anxiety whereas the latter leads to empathic concern Social neuroscience evidence editSocial neuroscience explores the biological underpinnings of empathic concern and more generally interpersonal sensitivity using an integrative approach that bridges the biological and social levels 15 Neural systems including autonomic functions that rely on brain stem neuropeptides such as oxytocin and vasopressin are plausible correlates for empathic concern Alternatively vasopressin might be implicated in situations where a more active strategy is required for an effective response 16 An association between executive functions underpinned by the prefrontal cortex with reciprocal connections with the limbic system the sense of agency and empathic concern has been suggested based on lesion studies in neurological patients and functional neuroimaging experiments in healthy individuals 17 The difference between imagining from one s own perspective and imagining in the shoes or from the perspective of others is supported by a series of functional neuroimaging studies of affective processing For instance participants in one study reported more empathic concern when imagining the pain of others when adopting another s perspective and more personal distress when imagining themselves to be in pain 18 fMRI scans revealed that imagining self in pain was associated with strong activation in brain areas involved in affective response to threat and pain including the amygdala insula and anterior cingulate cortex Imagine other instructions produced higher activity in the right temporoparietal junction which is associated with self other distinctiveness and the sense of agency See also editAffective neuroscience C Sue Carter Edward O Wilson Empathy altruism Frans de Waal Jean Decety Moral emotions Social emotions Social neuroscience Stephen Porges W D HamiltonReferences edit Batson C D 1991 The altruism question Toward a social psychological answer Hillsdale Erlbaum Associates Batson C D 1987 Prosocial motivation Is it ever truly altruistic In L Berkowitz Ed Advances in Experimental Social Psychology Vol 20 pp 65 122 New York Academic Press Chismar D 1988 Empathy and sympathy the important difference The Journal of Value Inquiry 22 pp 257 266 a b Batson C D 2011 Altruism in Humans Oxford Oxford University Press p 11 These are used by numerous philosophers and social scientists including Charles Darwin Frans de Waal Nancy Eisenberg and Stephen Darwall as documented in Batson s Altruism in Humans p 12 These latter two terms are due respectively to William McDougall and Martin Hoffman See Batson C D 2011 Altruism in Humans Oxford Oxford University Press p 12 Batson C D 1990 How social an animal The human capacity for caring American Psychologist 45 336 346 De Waal F B M 1996 Good Natured The origins of right and wrong in humans and other animals Harvard Harvard University Press De Waal F B M amp van Roosmalen A 1979 Reconciliation and consolation among chimpanzees Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 5 55 66 Eisenberg N amp Strayer J 1997 Empathy and its Development New York Cambridge University Press Decety J amp Meyer M 2008 From emotion resonance to empathic understanding A social developmental neuroscience account Development and Psychopathology 20 1053 1080 Zahn Waxler C Radke Yarrow M Wagner E amp Chapman M 1992 Development of concern for others Developmental Psychology 28 126 136 Eisenberg N amp Eggum N D 2009 Empathic responding sympathy and personal distress In J Decety and W Ickes The Social Neuroscience of Empathy Cambridge MIT Press pp 71 83 Hoffman M L 2000 Empathy and Moral Development New York Cambridge University Press Batson C D amp Shaw L L 1991 Evidence for altruism Toward a pluralism of prosocial motives Psychological Inquiry 2 107 122 Batson C D 2009 Two forms of perspective taking imagining how another feels and imagining how you would feel In K D Markman W M P Klein and J A Suhr Eds Handbook of Imagination and Mental Simulation pp 267 279 New York Psychology Press Decety J amp Keenan J P 2006 Social neuroscience A new journal Social Neuroscience 1 1 4 Carter C S Harris J amp Porges S W 2009 Neural and evolutionary perspectives on empathy In J Decety and W Ickes The Social Neuroscience of Empathy Cambridge MIT press pp 169 182 Decety J amp Jackson P L 2004 The functional architecture of human empathy Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews 3 71 100 Lamm C Batson C D amp Decety J 2007 The neural substrate of human empathy effects of perspective taking and cognitive appraisal Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 19 42 58 Further reading editBatson C D 2009 These things called empathy eight related but distinct phenomena In J Decety amp W Ickes Eds The Social Neuroscience of Empathy pp 3 15 Cambridge MIT press Batson C D 2009 Two forms of perspective taking imagining how another feels and imagining how you would feel In K D Markman W M P Klein and J A Suhr Eds Handbook of Imagination and Mental Simulation pp 267 279 New York Psychology Press Decety J amp Batson C D 2007 Social neuroscience approaches to interpersonal sensitivity Social Neuroscience 2 3 4 151 157 Decety J amp Ickes W Eds 2009 The Social Neuroscience of Empathy Cambridge MIT Press Cambridge Thompson E 2001 Empathy and consciousness Journal of Consciousness Studies 8 1 32 Zahn Waxler C amp Radke Yarrow M 1990 The origins of empathic concern Motivation and Emotion 14 107 125 Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Empathic concern amp oldid 1214457917, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.