fbpx
Wikipedia

Dual inheritance theory

Dual inheritance theory (DIT), also known as gene–culture coevolution or biocultural evolution,[1] was developed in the 1960s through early 1980s to explain how human behavior is a product of two different and interacting evolutionary processes: genetic evolution and cultural evolution. Genes and culture continually interact in a feedback loop:[2] changes in genes can lead to changes in culture which can then influence genetic selection, and vice versa. One of the theory's central claims is that culture evolves partly through a Darwinian selection process, which dual inheritance theorists often describe by analogy to genetic evolution.[3]

'Culture', in this context is defined as 'socially learned behavior', and 'social learning' is defined as copying behaviors observed in others or acquiring behaviors through being taught by others. Most of the modelling done in the field relies on the first dynamic (copying) though it can be extended to teaching. Social learning at its simplest involves blind copying of behaviors from a model (someone observed behaving), though it is also understood to have many potential biases, including success bias (copying from those who are perceived to be better off), status bias (copying from those with higher status), homophily (copying from those most like ourselves), conformist bias (disproportionately picking up behaviors that more people are performing), etc. Understanding social learning is a system of pattern replication, and understanding that there are different rates of survival for different socially learned cultural variants, this sets up, by definition, an evolutionary structure: cultural evolution.[4]

Because genetic evolution is relatively well understood, most of DIT examines cultural evolution and the interactions between cultural evolution and genetic evolution.

Theoretical basis edit

DIT holds that genetic and cultural evolution interacted in the evolution of Homo sapiens. DIT recognizes that the natural selection of genotypes is an important component of the evolution of human behavior and that cultural traits can be constrained by genetic imperatives. However, DIT also recognizes that genetic evolution has endowed the human species with a parallel evolutionary process of cultural evolution. DIT makes three main claims:[5]

Culture capacities are adaptations edit

The human capacity to store and transmit culture arose from genetically evolved psychological mechanisms. This implies that at some point during the evolution of the human species a type of social learning leading to cumulative cultural evolution was evolutionarily advantageous.

Culture evolves edit

Social learning processes give rise to cultural evolution. Cultural traits are transmitted differently from genetic traits and, therefore, result in different population-level effects on behavioral variation.

Genes and culture co-evolve edit

Cultural traits alter the social and physical environments under which genetic selection operates. For example, the cultural adoptions of agriculture and dairying have, in humans, caused genetic selection for the traits to digest starch and lactose, respectively.[6][7][8][9][10][11] As another example, it is likely that once culture became adaptive, genetic selection caused a refinement of the cognitive architecture that stores and transmits cultural information. This refinement may have further influenced the way culture is stored and the biases that govern its transmission.

DIT also predicts that, under certain situations, cultural evolution may select for traits that are genetically maladaptive. An example of this is the demographic transition, which describes the fall of birth rates within industrialized societies. Dual inheritance theorists hypothesize that the demographic transition may be a result of a prestige bias, where individuals that forgo reproduction to gain more influence in industrial societies are more likely to be chosen as cultural models.[12][13]

View of culture edit

People have defined the word "culture" to describe a large set of different phenomena.[14][15] A definition that sums up what is meant by "culture" in DIT is:

Culture is socially learned information stored in individuals' brains that is capable of affecting behavior.[16][17]

This view of culture emphasizes population thinking by focusing on the process by which culture is generated and maintained. It also views culture as a dynamic property of individuals, as opposed to a view of culture as a superorganic entity to which individuals must conform.[18] This view's main advantage is that it connects individual-level processes to population-level outcomes.[19]

Genetic influence on cultural evolution edit

Genes affect cultural evolution via psychological predispositions on cultural learning.[20] Genes encode much of the information needed to form the human brain. Genes constrain the brain's structure and, hence, the ability of the brain to acquire and store culture. Genes may also endow individuals with certain types of transmission bias (described below).

Cultural influences on genetic evolution edit

Culture can profoundly influence gene frequencies in a population.

Lactase persistence

One of the best known examples is the prevalence of the genotype for adult lactose absorption in human populations, such as Northern Europeans and some African societies, with a long history of raising cattle for milk. Until around 7,500 years ago,[21] lactase production stopped shortly after weaning,[22] and in societies which did not develop dairying, such as East Asians and Amerindians, this is still true today.[23][24] In areas with lactase persistence, it is believed that by domesticating animals, a source of milk became available while an adult and thus strong selection for lactase persistence could occur,[21][25] in a Scandinavian population the estimated selection coefficient was 0.09-0.19.[25] This implies that the cultural practice of raising cattle first for meat and later for milk led to selection for genetic traits for lactose digestion.[26] Recently, analysis of natural selection on the human genome suggests that civilization has accelerated genetic change in humans over the past 10,000 years.[27]

Food processing

Culture has driven changes to the human digestive systems making many digestive organs, such as teeth or stomach, smaller than expected for primates of a similar size,[28] and has been attributed to one of the reasons why humans have such large brains compared to other great apes.[29][30] This is due to food processing. Early examples of food processing include pounding, marinating and most notably cooking. Pounding meat breaks down the muscle fibres, hence taking away some of the job from the mouth, teeth and jaw.[31][32] Marinating emulates the action of the stomach with high acid levels. Cooking partially breaks down food making it more easily digestible. Food enters the body effectively partly digested, and as such food processing reduces the work that the digestive system has to do. This means that there is selection for smaller digestive organs as the tissue is energetically expensive,[28] those with smaller digestive organs can process their food but at a lower energetic cost than those with larger organs.[33] Cooking is notable because the energy available from food increases when cooked and this also means less time is spent looking for food.[29][34][35]

Humans living on cooked diets spend only a fraction of their day chewing compared to other extant primates living on raw diets. American girls and boys spent on average 7 to 8 percent of their day chewing respectively (1.68 to 1.92 hours per day), compared to chimpanzees, who spend more than 6 hours a day chewing.[36] This frees up time which can be used for hunting. A raw diet means hunting is constrained since time spent hunting is time not spent eating and chewing plant material, but cooking reduces the time required to get the day's energy requirements, allowing for more subsistence activities.[37] Digestibility of cooked carbohydrates is approximately on average 30% higher than digestibility of non-cooked carbohydrates.[34][38] This increased energy intake, more free time and savings made on tissue used in the digestive system allowed for the selection of genes for larger brain size.

Despite its benefits, brain tissue requires a large amount of calories, hence a main constraint in selection for larger brains is calorie intake. A greater calorie intake can support greater quantities of brain tissue. This is argued to explain why human brains can be much larger than other apes, since humans are the only ape to engage in food processing.[29] The cooking of food has influenced genes to the extent that, research suggests, humans cannot live without cooking.[39][29] A study on 513 individuals consuming long-term raw diets found that as the percentage of their diet which was made up of raw food and/or the length they had been on a diet of raw food increased, their BMI decreased.[39] This is despite access to many non-thermal processing, like grinding, pounding or heating to 48 °C. (118 °F).[39] With approximately 86 billion neurons in the human brain and 60–70 kg body mass, an exclusively raw diet close to that of what extant primates have would be not viable as, when modelled, it is argued that it would require an infeasible level of more than nine hours of feeding every day.[29] However, this is contested, with alternative modelling showing enough calories could be obtained within 5–6 hours per day.[40] Some scientists and anthropologists point to evidence that brain size in the Homo lineage started to increase well before the advent of cooking due to increased consumption of meat[28][40][41] and that basic food processing (slicing) accounts for the size reduction in organs related to chewing.[42] Cornélio et al. argues that improving cooperative abilities and a varying of diet to more meat and seeds improved foraging and hunting efficiency. It is this that allowed for the brain expansion, independent of cooking which they argue came much later, a consequence from the complex cognition that developed.[40] Yet this is still an example of a cultural shift in diet and the resulting genetic evolution. Further criticism comes from the controversy of the archaeological evidence available. Some claim there is a lack of evidence of fire control when brain sizes first started expanding.[40][43] Wrangham argues that anatomical evidence around the time of the origin of Homo erectus (1.8 million years ago), indicates that the control of fire and hence cooking occurred.[34] At this time, the largest reductions in tooth size in the entirety of human evolution occurred, indicating that softer foods became prevalent in the diet. Also at this time was a narrowing of the pelvis indicating a smaller gut and also there is evidence that there was a loss of the ability to climb which Wrangham argues indicates the control of fire, since sleeping on the ground needs fire to ward off predators.[44] The proposed increases in brain size from food processing will have led to a greater mental capacity for further cultural innovation in food processing which will have increased digestive efficiency further providing more energy for further gains in brain size.[45] This positive feedback loop is argued to have led to the rapid brain size increases seen in the Homo lineage.[46][40]

Mechanisms of cultural evolution edit

In DIT, the evolution and maintenance of cultures is described by five major mechanisms: natural selection of cultural variants, random variation, cultural drift, guided variation and transmission bias.

Natural selection edit

Cultural differences among individuals can lead to differential survival of individuals. The patterns of this selective process depend on transmission biases and can result in behavior that is more adaptive to a given environment.

Random variation edit

Random variation arises from errors in the learning, display or recall of cultural information, and is roughly analogous to the process of mutation in genetic evolution.

Cultural drift edit

Cultural drift is a process roughly analogous to genetic drift in evolutionary biology.[47][48][49] In cultural drift, the frequency of cultural traits in a population may be subject to random fluctuations due to chance variations in which traits are observed and transmitted (sometimes called "sampling error").[50] These fluctuations might cause cultural variants to disappear from a population. This effect should be especially strong in small populations.[51] A model by Hahn and Bentley shows that cultural drift gives a reasonably good approximation to changes in the popularity of American baby names.[50] Drift processes have also been suggested to explain changes in archaeological pottery and technology patent applications.[49] Changes in the songs of song birds are also thought to arise from drift processes, where distinct dialects in different groups occur due to errors in songbird singing and acquisition by successive generations.[52] Cultural drift is also observed in an early computer model of cultural evolution.[53]

Guided variation edit

Cultural traits may be gained in a population through the process of individual learning. Once an individual learns a novel trait, it can be transmitted to other members of the population. The process of guided variation depends on an adaptive standard that determines what cultural variants are learned.

Biased transmission edit

Understanding the different ways that culture traits can be transmitted between individuals has been an important part of DIT research since the 1970s.[54][55] Transmission biases occur when some cultural variants are favored over others during the process of cultural transmission.[56] Boyd and Richerson (1985)[56] defined and analytically modeled a number of possible transmission biases. The list of biases has been refined over the years, especially by Henrich and McElreath.[57]

Content bias edit

Content biases result from situations where some aspect of a cultural variant's content makes them more likely to be adopted.[58] Content biases can result from genetic preferences, preferences determined by existing cultural traits, or a combination of the two. For example, food preferences can result from genetic preferences for sugary or fatty foods and socially-learned eating practices and taboos.[58] Content biases are sometimes called "direct biases."[56]

Context bias edit

Context biases result from individuals using clues about the social structure of their population to determine what cultural variants to adopt. This determination is made without reference to the content of the variant. There are two major categories of context biases: model-based biases, and frequency-dependent biases.

Model-based biases edit

Model-based biases result when an individual is biased to choose a particular "cultural model" to imitate. There are four major categories of model-based biases: prestige bias, skill bias, success bias, and similarity bias.[5][59] A "prestige bias" results when individuals are more likely to imitate cultural models that are seen as having more prestige. A measure of prestige could be the amount of deference shown to a potential cultural model by other individuals. A "skill bias" results when individuals can directly observe different cultural models performing a learned skill and are more likely to imitate cultural models that perform better at the specific skill. A "success bias" results from individuals preferentially imitating cultural models that they determine are most generally successful (as opposed to successful at a specific skill as in the skill bias.) A "similarity bias" results when individuals are more likely to imitate cultural models that are perceived as being similar to the individual based on specific traits.

Frequency-dependent biases edit

Frequency-dependent biases result when an individual is biased to choose particular cultural variants based on their perceived frequency in the population. The most explored frequency-dependent bias is the "conformity bias." Conformity biases result when individuals attempt to copy the mean or the mode cultural variant in the population. Another possible frequency dependent bias is the "rarity bias." The rarity bias results when individuals preferentially choose cultural variants that are less common in the population. The rarity bias is also sometimes called a "nonconformist" or "anti-conformist" bias.

Social learning and cumulative cultural evolution edit

In DIT, the evolution of culture is dependent on the evolution of social learning. Analytic models show that social learning becomes evolutionarily beneficial when the environment changes with enough frequency that genetic inheritance can not track the changes, but not fast enough that individual learning is more efficient.[60] For environments that have very little variability, social learning is not needed since genes can adapt fast enough to the changes that occur, and innate behaviour is able to deal with the constant environment.[61] In fast changing environments cultural learning would not be useful because what the previous generation knew is now outdated and will provide no benefit in the changed environment, and hence individual learning is more beneficial. It is only in the moderately changing environment where cultural learning becomes useful since each generation shares a mostly similar environment but genes have insufficient time to change to changes in the environment.[62] While other species have social learning, and thus some level of culture, only humans, some birds and chimpanzees are known to have cumulative culture.[63] Boyd and Richerson argue that the evolution of cumulative culture depends on observational learning and is uncommon in other species because it is ineffective when it is rare in a population. They propose that the environmental changes occurring in the Pleistocene may have provided the right environmental conditions.[62] Michael Tomasello argues that cumulative cultural evolution results from a ratchet effect that began when humans developed the cognitive architecture to understand others as mental agents.[64] Furthermore, Tomasello proposed in the 80s that there are some disparities between the observational learning mechanisms found in humans and great apes - which go some way to explain the observable difference between great ape traditions and human types of culture (see Emulation (observational learning)).

Cultural group selection edit

Although group selection is commonly thought to be nonexistent or unimportant in genetic evolution,[65][66][67] DIT predicts that, due to the nature of cultural inheritance, it may be an important force in cultural evolution. Group selection occurs in cultural evolution because conformist biases make it difficult for novel cultural traits to spread through a population (see above section on transmission biases). Conformist bias also helps maintain variation between groups. These two properties, rare in genetic transmission, are necessary for group selection to operate.[68] Based on an earlier model by Cavalli-Sforza and Feldman,[69] Boyd and Richerson show that conformist biases are almost inevitable when traits spread through social learning,[70] implying that group selection is common in cultural evolution. Analysis of small groups in New Guinea imply that cultural group selection might be a good explanation for slowly changing aspects of social structure, but not for rapidly changing fads.[71] The ability of cultural evolution to maintain intergroup diversity is what allows for the study of cultural phylogenetics.[72]

Historical development edit

In 1876, Friedrich Engels wrote a manuscript titled The Part Played by Labour in the Transition from Ape to Man, accredited as a founding document of DIH;[73] “The approach to gene-culture coevolution first developed by Engels and developed later on by anthropologists…” is described by Stephen Jay Gould as “…the best nineteenth-century case for gene-culture coevolution.”[74] The idea that human cultures undergo a similar evolutionary process as genetic evolution also goes back to Darwin.[75] In the 1960s, Donald T. Campbell published some of the first theoretical work that adapted principles of evolutionary theory to the evolution of cultures.[76] In 1976, two developments in cultural evolutionary theory set the stage for DIT. In that year Richard Dawkins's The Selfish Gene introduced ideas of cultural evolution to a popular audience. Although one of the best-selling science books of all time, because of its lack of mathematical rigor, it had little effect on the development of DIT. Also in 1976, geneticists Marcus Feldman and Luigi Luca Cavalli-Sforza published the first dynamic models of gene–culture coevolution.[77] These models were to form the basis for subsequent work on DIT, heralded by the publication of three seminal books in the 1980s.

The first was Charles Lumsden and E.O. Wilson's Genes, Mind and Culture.[78] This book outlined a series of mathematical models of how genetic evolution might favor the selection of cultural traits and how cultural traits might, in turn, affect the speed of genetic evolution. While it was the first book published describing how genes and culture might coevolve, it had relatively little effect on the further development of DIT.[79] Some critics felt that their models depended too heavily on genetic mechanisms at the expense of cultural mechanisms.[80] Controversy surrounding Wilson's sociobiological theories may also have decreased the lasting effect of this book.[79]

The second 1981 book was Cavalli-Sforza and Feldman's Cultural Transmission and Evolution: A Quantitative Approach.[48] Borrowing heavily from population genetics and epidemiology, this book built a mathematical theory concerning the spread of cultural traits. It describes the evolutionary implications of vertical transmission, passing cultural traits from parents to offspring; oblique transmission, passing cultural traits from any member of an older generation to a younger generation; and horizontal transmission, passing traits between members of the same population.

The next significant DIT publication was Robert Boyd and Peter Richerson's 1985 Culture and the Evolutionary Process.[56] This book presents the now-standard mathematical models of the evolution of social learning under different environmental conditions, the population effects of social learning, various forces of selection on cultural learning rules, different forms of biased transmission and their population-level effects, and conflicts between cultural and genetic evolution. The book's conclusion also outlined areas for future research that are still relevant today.[81]

Current and future research edit

In their 1985 book, Boyd and Richerson outlined an agenda for future DIT research. This agenda, outlined below, called for the development of both theoretical models and empirical research. DIT has since built a rich tradition of theoretical models over the past two decades.[82] However, there has not been a comparable level of empirical work.

In a 2006 interview Harvard biologist E. O. Wilson expressed disappointment at the little attention afforded to DIT:

"...for some reason I haven't fully fathomed, this most promising frontier of scientific research has attracted very few people and very little effort."[83]

Kevin Laland and Gillian Ruth Brown attribute this lack of attention to DIT's heavy reliance on formal modeling.

"In many ways the most complex and potentially rewarding of all approaches, [DIT], with its multiple processes and cerebral onslaught of sigmas and deltas, may appear too abstract to all but the most enthusiastic reader. Until such a time as the theoretical hieroglyphics can be translated into a respectable empirical science most observers will remain immune to its message."[84]

Economist Herbert Gintis disagrees with this critique, citing empirical work as well as more recent work using techniques from behavioral economics.[85] These behavioral economic techniques have been adapted to test predictions of cultural evolutionary models in laboratory settings[86][87][88] as well as studying differences in cooperation in fifteen small-scale societies in the field.[89]

Since one of the goals of DIT is to explain the distribution of human cultural traits, ethnographic and ethnologic techniques may also be useful for testing hypothesis stemming from DIT. Although findings from traditional ethnologic studies have been used to buttress DIT arguments,[90][91] thus far there have been little ethnographic fieldwork designed to explicitly test these hypotheses.[71][89][92]

Herb Gintis has named DIT one of the two major conceptual theories with potential for unifying the behavioral sciences, including economics, biology, anthropology, sociology, psychology and political science. Because it addresses both the genetic and cultural components of human inheritance, Gintis sees DIT models as providing the best explanations for the ultimate cause of human behavior and the best paradigm for integrating those disciplines with evolutionary theory.[93] In a review of competing evolutionary perspectives on human behavior, Laland and Brown see DIT as the best candidate for uniting the other evolutionary perspectives under one theoretical umbrella.[94]

Relation to other fields edit

Sociology and cultural anthropology edit

Two major topics of study in both sociology and cultural anthropology are human cultures and cultural variation. However, Dual Inheritance theorists charge that both disciplines too often treat culture as a static superorganic entity that dictates human behavior.[95][96] Cultures are defined by a suite of common traits shared by a large group of people. DIT theorists argue that this doesn't sufficiently explain variation in cultural traits at the individual level. By contrast, DIT models human culture at the individual level and views culture as the result of a dynamic evolutionary process at the population level.[95][97]

Human sociobiology and evolutionary psychology edit

Evolutionary psychologists study the evolved architecture of the human mind. They see it as composed of many different programs that process information, each with assumptions and procedures that were specialized by natural selection to solve a different adaptive problem faced by our hunter-gatherer ancestors (e.g., choosing mates, hunting, avoiding predators, cooperating, using aggression).[98] These evolved programs contain content-rich assumptions about how the world and other people work. When ideas are passed from mind to mind, they are changed by these evolved inference systems (much like messages get changed in a game of telephone). But the changes are not usually random. Evolved programs add and subtract information, reshaping the ideas in ways that make them more "intuitive", more memorable, and more attention-grabbing. In other words, "memes" (ideas) are not precisely like genes. Genes are normally copied faithfully as they are replicated, but ideas normally are not. It's not just that ideas mutate every once in a while, like genes do. Ideas are transformed every time they are passed from mind to mind, because the sender's message is being interpreted by evolved inference systems in the receiver.[99][100] It is useful for some applications to note, however, that there are ways to pass ideas which are more resilient and involve substantially less mutation, such as by mass distribution of printed media.

There is no necessary contradiction between evolutionary psychology and DIT, but evolutionary psychologists argue that the psychology implicit in many DIT models is too simple; evolved programs have a rich inferential structure not captured by the idea of a "content bias". They also argue that some of the phenomena DIT models attribute to cultural evolution are cases of "evoked culture"—situations in which different evolved programs are activated in different places, in response to cues in the environment.[101]

Sociobiologists try to understand how maximizing genetic fitness, in either the modern era or past environments, can explain human behavior. When faced with a trait that seems maladaptive, some sociobiologists try to determine how the trait actually increases genetic fitness (maybe through kin selection or by speculating about early evolutionary environments). Dual inheritance theorists, in contrast, will consider a variety of genetic and cultural processes in addition to natural selection on genes.

Human behavioral ecology edit

Human behavioral ecology (HBE) and DIT have a similar relationship to what ecology and evolutionary biology have in the biological sciences. HBE is more concerned about ecological process and DIT more focused on historical process.[102] One difference is that human behavioral ecologists often assume that culture is a system that produces the most adaptive outcome in a given environment. This implies that similar behavioral traditions should be found in similar environments. However, this is not always the case. A study of African cultures showed that cultural history was a better predictor of cultural traits than local ecological conditions.[103]

Memetics edit

Memetics, which comes from the meme idea described in Dawkins's The Selfish Gene, is similar to DIT in that it treats culture as an evolutionary process that is distinct from genetic transmission. However, there are some philosophical differences between memetics and DIT.[104] One difference is that memetics' focus is on the selection potential of discrete replicators (memes), where DIT allows for transmission of both non-replicators and non-discrete cultural variants. DIT does not assume that replicators are necessary for cumulative adaptive evolution. DIT also more strongly emphasizes the role of genetic inheritance in shaping the capacity for cultural evolution. But perhaps the biggest difference is a difference in academic lineage. Memetics as a label is more influential in popular culture than in academia. Critics of memetics argue that it is lacking in empirical support or is conceptually ill-founded, and question whether there is hope for the memetic research program succeeding. Proponents point out that many cultural traits are discrete, and that many existing models of cultural inheritance assume discrete cultural units, and hence involve memes.[105]

Shortcomings and criticisms edit

Psychologist Liane Gabora has criticised DIT.[106][107][108] She argues that use of the term ‘dual inheritance’ to refer to not just traits that are transmitted by way of a self-assembly code (as in genetic evolution) but also traits that are not transmitted by way of a self-assembly code (as in cultural evolution) is misleading, because this second use does not capture the algorithmic structure that makes an inheritance system require a particular kind of mathematical framework.[109]

Other criticisms of the effort to frame culture in Darwinian terms have been leveled by Richard Lewontin,[110] Niles Eldredge,[111] and Stuart Kauffman.[112]

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ O'Neil, Dennis. "Glossary of Terms". Modern Theories of Evolution. Retrieved 28 October 2012.
  2. ^ Laland, Kevin N. (2008-11-12). "Exploring gene–culture interactions: insights from handedness, sexual selection and niche-construction case studies". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 363 (1509): 3577–3589. doi:10.1098/rstb.2008.0132. ISSN 0962-8436. PMC 2607340. PMID 18799415.
  3. ^ Richerson, Peter J.; Boyd, Robert (2005). Not By Genes Alone: How Culture Transformed Human Evolution. University of Chicago Press.
  4. ^ Campbell, D. T. (1965). "Variation and selective retention in socio-cultural evolution". Social Change in Developing Areas, A Reinterpretation of Evolutionary Theory.
  5. ^ a b McElreath, R.; Henrich, J. (2007). (PDF). In R. Dunbar and L. Barrett (ed.). Oxford Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2007-08-11.
  6. ^ Simoons, F (1969). "Primary adult lactose intolerance and the milking habit: A problem in biologic and cultural interrelations: I. Review of the medical research". The American Journal of Digestive Diseases. 14 (12): 819–836. doi:10.1007/bf02233204. PMID 4902756. S2CID 22597839.
  7. ^ Simoons, F (1970). "Primary adult lactose intolerance and the milking habit: A problem in biologic and cultural interrelations: II. A culture historical hypothesis". The American Journal of Digestive Diseases. 15 (8): 695–710. doi:10.1007/bf02235991. PMID 5468838. S2CID 2140863.
  8. ^ Cavalli-Sforza, L., P. Menozzi and A. Piazza. 1994. The history and geography of human genes Princeton: Princeton University Press
  9. ^ Holden, C.; Mace, R. (1997). "Phylogenetic analysis of the evolution of lactose digestion in adults". Human Biology. 69 (5): 605–628. PMID 9299882.
  10. ^ Durham, W. 1991. Coevolution: Genes, culture and human diversity. Stanford: Stanford University Press. Chapter 5
  11. ^ Perry, G., N. Dominy, K. Claw, A. Lee, H> Fiegler, R. Redon, J. Werner, F. Villanea, J. Mountain, R. Misra, N. Carter, C. Lee, A. Stone. Diet and the evolution of human amylase gene copy number variation. Nature Genetics 39:1256-1260.
  12. ^ Boyd, R. and P. J. Richerson. 1985. Culture and the Evolutionary Process. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. pp. 199-202.
  13. ^ Richerson, P. J. and R. Boyd. 2005. Not By Genes Alone: How Culture Transformed Human Evolution. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. pp. 169-182.
  14. ^ Kroeberm A. and C. Kluckhohn. 1952. Culture; A Critical Review of Concepts and Definitions. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University.
  15. ^ Fox, R. and B. King. 2002. Anthropology Beyond Culture Oxford: Berg.
  16. ^ Richerson, P. and R. Boyd. 2005. Not By Genes Alone: How Culture Transformed Human Evolution Chicago: University of Chicago Press. pg 6.
  17. ^ Boyd, R. and P. Richerson. 2000. Memes: Universal Acid or a Better Mouse Trap? In R. Aunger, Ed. Darwinizing Culture: The Status of Memetics as a Science. Oxford: Oxford University Press. pp. 143-162.
  18. ^ Richerson, P.J. and R. Boyd. 2001. Culture is Part of Human Biology: Why the Superorganic Concept Serves the Human Sciences Badly. In Science Studies: Probing the Dynamics of Scientific Knowledge, In S. Maasen and M. Winterhager, Ed. Bielefeld: Verlag.[1]
  19. ^ Richerson, P. and R. Boyd. 2005. Not By Genes Alone: How Culture Transformed Human Evolution Chicago: University of Chicago Press. pg 7.
  20. ^ Sasaki, Joni Y. (2013). "Promise and Challenges Surrounding Culture–Gene Coevolution and Gene–Culture Interactions". Psychological Inquiry. 24: 64–70. doi:10.1080/1047840X.2013.764814. S2CID 144039856.
  21. ^ a b Itan, Yuval; Powell, Adam; Beaumont, Mark A.; Burger, Joachim; Thomas, Mark G. (2009-08-28). "The Origins of Lactase Persistence in Europe". PLOS Computational Biology. 5 (8): e1000491. Bibcode:2009PLSCB...5E0491I. doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000491. ISSN 1553-7358. PMC 2722739. PMID 19714206.
  22. ^ Malmström, Helena; Linderholm, Anna; Lidén, Kerstin; Storå, Jan; Molnar, Petra; Holmlund, Gunilla; Jakobsson, Mattias; Götherström, Anders (2010-01-01). "High frequency of lactose intolerance in a prehistoric hunter-gatherer population in northern Europe". BMC Evolutionary Biology. 10: 89. doi:10.1186/1471-2148-10-89. ISSN 1471-2148. PMC 2862036. PMID 20353605.
  23. ^ . www.ucl.ac.uk. Archived from the original on 2017-03-28. Retrieved 2017-03-27.
  24. ^ Gerbault, Pascale; Roffet-Salque, Mélanie; Evershed, Richard P.; Thomas, Mark G. (2013-12-01). "How long have adult humans been consuming milk?". IUBMB Life. 65 (12): 983–990. doi:10.1002/iub.1227. ISSN 1521-6551. PMID 24339181. S2CID 34564411.
  25. ^ a b Bersaglieri, Todd; Sabeti, Pardis C.; Patterson, Nick; Vanderploeg, Trisha; Schaffner, Steve F.; Drake, Jared A.; Rhodes, Matthew; Reich, David E.; Hirschhorn, Joel N. (2017-03-27). "Genetic Signatures of Strong Recent Positive Selection at the Lactase Gene". American Journal of Human Genetics. 74 (6): 1111–1120. doi:10.1086/421051. ISSN 0002-9297. PMC 1182075. PMID 15114531.
  26. ^ Laland, K. N. and G. R. Brown. 2002. Sense & Nonsense: Evolutionary Perspectives on Human Behavior. Oxford: Oxford University Press. p. 260
  27. ^ Cochran, G. and H. Harpending. 2009. The 10,000 Year Explosion: How Civilization Accelerated Human Evolution. Basic Books.
  28. ^ a b c Aiello, Leslie C.; Wheeler, Peter (1995-01-01). "The Expensive-Tissue Hypothesis: The Brain and the Digestive System in Human and Primate Evolution". Current Anthropology. 36 (2): 199–221. doi:10.1086/204350. JSTOR 2744104. S2CID 144317407.
  29. ^ a b c d e Fonseca-Azevedo, Karina; Herculano-Houzel, Suzana (2012-11-06). "Metabolic constraint imposes tradeoff between body size and number of brain neurons in human evolution". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 109 (45): 18571–18576. Bibcode:2012PNAS..10918571F. doi:10.1073/pnas.1206390109. ISSN 0027-8424. PMC 3494886. PMID 23090991.
  30. ^ Gorman, Rachael Moeller (2008). "Cooking Up Bigger Brains". Scientific American. 298 (1): 102–105. Bibcode:2008SciAm.298a.102G. doi:10.1038/scientificamerican0108-102. PMID 18225702.
  31. ^ Farrell, J. H. (1956-05-01). "The effect on digestibility of methods commonly used to increase the tenderness of lean meat". British Journal of Nutrition. 10 (2): 111–115. doi:10.1079/BJN19560019. ISSN 1475-2662. PMID 13315930.
  32. ^ Henrich, Joseph (2015). The Secret of Our Success: How Culture Is Driving Human Evolution, Domesticating Our Species, and Making Us Smarter. Princeton: Princeton University Press. p. 66. ISBN 9780691166858.
  33. ^ Wrangham, Richard (2009). Catching Fire How Cooking Made Us Human. London: Profile Books. p. 40. ISBN 9781846682865.
  34. ^ a b c Carmody, Rachel N.; Wrangham, Richard W. (2009-10-01). "The energetic significance of cooking". Journal of Human Evolution. 57 (4): 379–391. doi:10.1016/j.jhevol.2009.02.011. ISSN 1095-8606. PMID 19732938. S2CID 15255649.
  35. ^ Carmody, Rachel N.; Weintraub, Gil S.; Wrangham, Richard W. (2011-11-29). "Energetic consequences of thermal and nonthermal food processing". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 108 (48): 19199–19203. Bibcode:2011PNAS..10819199C. doi:10.1073/pnas.1112128108. ISSN 1091-6490. PMC 3228431. PMID 22065771.
  36. ^ Wrangham, Richard (2010-08-06). Catching Fire: How Cooking Made Us Human (Main ed.). Profile Books. p. 140.
  37. ^ Wrangham, Richard (2010-05-27). Catching Fire: How Cooking Made Us Human (Main ed.). London: Profile Books. p. 142. ISBN 9781846682865.
  38. ^ University of California Television (UCTV) (2013-03-21), CARTA: The Evolution of Human Nutrition -- Richard Wrangham: Fire Starch Meat and Honey, archived from the original on 2021-12-19, retrieved 2017-03-27
  39. ^ a b c Koebnick, C.; Strassner, C.; Hoffmann, I.; Leitzmann, C. (1999-01-01). "Consequences of a long-term raw food diet on body weight and menstruation: results of a questionnaire survey". Annals of Nutrition & Metabolism. 43 (2): 69–79. doi:10.1159/000012770. ISSN 0250-6807. PMID 10436305. S2CID 30125503.
  40. ^ a b c d e Cornélio, Alianda M.; de Bittencourt-Navarrete, Ruben E.; de Bittencourt Brum, Ricardo; Queiroz, Claudio M.; Costa, Marcos R. (2016-04-25). "Human Brain Expansion during Evolution Is Independent of Fire Control and Cooking". Frontiers in Neuroscience. 10: 167. doi:10.3389/fnins.2016.00167. ISSN 1662-4548. PMC 4842772. PMID 27199631.
  41. ^ "06.14.99 - Meat-eating was essential for human evolution, says UC Berkeley anthropologist specializing in diet". www.berkeley.edu. Retrieved 2017-03-27.
  42. ^ Zink, Katherine D.; Lieberman, Daniel E. (2016-03-24). "Impact of meat and Lower Palaeolithic food processing techniques on chewing in humans". Nature. 531 (7595): 500–503. Bibcode:2016Natur.531..500Z. doi:10.1038/nature16990. ISSN 0028-0836. PMID 26958832. S2CID 4384345.
  43. ^ Roebroeks, Wil; Villa, Paola (2011-03-29). "On the earliest evidence for habitual use of fire in Europe". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 108 (13): 5209–5214. Bibcode:2011PNAS..108.5209R. doi:10.1073/pnas.1018116108. ISSN 0027-8424. PMC 3069174. PMID 21402905.
  44. ^ Wrangham, Richard (2010-05-27). Catching Fire: How Cooking Made Us Human (Main ed.). London: Profile Books. pp. 98–102. ISBN 9781846682865.
  45. ^ Wrangham, Richard (2010-08-06). Catching Fire: How Cooking Made Us Human (Main ed.). Profile Books. p. 127.
  46. ^ Wrangham, Richard (2010-05-27). Catching Fire: How Cooking Made Us Human (Main ed.). London: Profile Books. p. 127. ISBN 9781846682865.
  47. ^ Koerper, H.; Stickel, E. (1980). "Cultural Drift: A Primary Process of Culture Change". Journal of Anthropological Research. 36 (4): 463–469. doi:10.1086/jar.36.4.3629615. S2CID 163932368.
  48. ^ a b Cavalli-Sfornza, L. and M. Feldman. 1981. Cultural Transmission and Evolution: A Quantitative Approach. Princeton, New Jersey: Princeton University Press.
  49. ^ a b Bentley, R.A.; Hahn, M.W.; Shennan, S.J. (2004). (PDF). Proceedings of the Royal Society B. 271 (1547): 1443–1450. doi:10.1098/rspb.2004.2746. PMC 1691747. PMID 15306315. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2011-09-28. Retrieved 2008-05-05.
  50. ^ a b Hahn, M.W.; Bentley, R. A. (2003). "Drift as a mechanism for cultural change: An example from baby names" (PDF). Proceedings of the Royal Society B. 270 (Suppl 1): S120–S123. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2003.0045. PMC 1698036. PMID 12952655.
  51. ^ Boyd, R. and P. J. Richerson. 1985. Culture and the Evolutionary Process. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. pp.9, 69
  52. ^ P.J.B. Slater, V.M. Janik. Vocal Learning. In "Encyclopedia of Animal Behavior", 2010, Pages 551-557. http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/referenceworks/9780080453378
  53. ^ Gabora, L. (1995). Meme and variations: A computer model of cultural evolution 2011-08-28 at the Wayback Machine. In (L. Nadel & D. Stein, Eds.) 1993 Lectures in Complex Systems, Addison-Wesley, 471-486.
  54. ^ Feldman, M.; Cavalli-Sforza, L. (1976). "Cultural and biological evolutionary processes, selection for a trait under complex transmission". Theoretical Population Biology. 9 (2): 238–259. doi:10.1016/0040-5809(76)90047-2. PMID 1273802.
  55. ^ Feldman, M; Cavalli-Sfornza, L. (1977). "The evolution of continuous variation: II, complex transmission and assortive mating". Theoretical Population Biology. 11 (2): 161–181. doi:10.1016/0040-5809(77)90024-7. PMID 867286.
  56. ^ a b c d Boyd, R., and P. Richerson. 1985. Culture and the Evolutionary Process. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
  57. ^ Henrich, J.; McElreath, R. (2003). (PDF). Evolutionary Anthropology. 12 (3): 123–135. doi:10.1002/evan.10110. S2CID 14302229. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2005-05-28. Retrieved 2009-09-08.
  58. ^ a b Henrich, J. and R. McElreath. 2007. Dual inheritance theory: the evolution of human cultural capacities and cultural evolution. Oxford Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology, R. Dunbar and L. Barrett, eds., Ch. 38. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  59. ^ Henrich, J.; McElreath, R. (2003). (PDF). Evolutionary Anthropology. 12 (3): 123–135. doi:10.1002/evan.10110. S2CID 14302229. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2008-04-07. Retrieved 2008-03-27.
  60. ^ Richerson, P.J. and R. Boyd. 2000. Climate, culture, and the evolution of cognition. In C.M. Heyes and L. Huber, (Eds), The Evolution of Cognition. Massachusetts: MIT Press.
  61. ^ Kerr, Benjamin; Feldman, Marcus W. (2003). "Carving the Cognitive Niche: Optimal Learning Strategies in Homogeneous and Heterogeneous Environments - ScienceDirect". Journal of Theoretical Biology. 220 (2): 169–188. doi:10.1006/jtbi.2003.3146. PMID 12468290.
  62. ^ a b Richerson, P. J.; Boyd, R. (2000). "Built for Speed: Pleistocene Climate Variation and the Origin of Human Culture". Perspectives in Ethology. Vol. 13. pp. 1–45. doi:10.1007/978-1-4615-1221-9_1. ISBN 978-1-4613-5447-5.
  63. ^ Tomasello, M. 1999. The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Cambridge University Press.
  64. ^ Tomasello, M. 1999. The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  65. ^ Williams, G.C. 1972. Adaptation and Natural Selection: A Critique of Some Current Evolutionary Thought. Princeton: Princeton University Press. ISBN 0-691-02357-3
  66. ^ Williams, G.C. 1986. Evolution Through Group Selection. Blackwell. ISBN 0-632-01541-1
  67. ^ Maynard Smith, J. (1964). "Group selection and kin selection". Nature. 201 (4924): 1145–1147. Bibcode:1964Natur.201.1145S. doi:10.1038/2011145a0. S2CID 4177102.
  68. ^ Uyenoyama, M.; Feldman, M. W. (1980). "Theories of kin and group selection: a population genetics perspective". Theoretical Population Biology. 17 (3): 380–414. doi:10.1016/0040-5809(80)90033-7. PMID 7434256.
  69. ^ Cavalli-Sforza, L. L.; Feldman, M. W. (1973). "Models for cultural inheritance. I. Group mean and within group variation". Theoretical Population Biology. 4 (1): 42–44. doi:10.1016/0040-5809(73)90005-1. PMID 4726009.
  70. ^ Boyd, R. and P. J. Richerson. 1985. Culture and the Evolutionary Process. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. pg. 227–240.
  71. ^ a b Soltis, J.; Boyd, R.; Richerson, P. J. (1995). "Can group-functional behaviors evolve by cultural group selection? An empirical test" (PDF). Current Anthropology. 36 (3): 473–494. doi:10.1086/204381. S2CID 43998139.
  72. ^ Mace, R., C. Holden, and S. Shennan (Eds.) 2005. The evolution of cultural diversity: a phylogenetic approach. London:University College London Press.
  73. ^ Foster, John (2000). Marx’s Ecology: Materialism and Nature. New York: Monthly Review Press. p. 203. ISBN 9781583670125.
  74. ^ Gould, Stephen (1987). An Urchin in the Storm. New York: W.W. Norton. pp. 111–112. ISBN 0-393-02492-X.
  75. ^ Darwin, C. 1874. The descent of man and selection in relation to sex. 2nd ed. 2 vols. New York: American Home Library.
  76. ^ Campbell, D. 1965. Variation and selective retention in socio-cultural evolution. In Social change in developing areas: A reinterpretation of evolutionary theory, ed. H. Barringer, G. Blanksten, and R. Mack, 19-49. Cambridge, MA: Schenkman Publishing Company.
  77. ^ Feldman, M.; Cavalli-Sforna, L. (1976). "Cultural and biological evolutionary processes, selection for a trait under complex transmission". Theoretical Population Biology. 9 (2): 238–59. doi:10.1016/0040-5809(76)90047-2. PMID 1273802.
  78. ^ Lumsden C., and E. Wilson. 1981. Genes, Mind and Culture: The Coevolutionary Process. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  79. ^ a b Laland K. and G. Brown. 2002. Sense and Nonsense: Evolutionary Perspectives on Human Behavior. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  80. ^ Boyd, R.; Richerson, P. (1983). "The cultural transmission of acquired variation: effects on genetic fitness". Journal of Theoretical Biology. 100 (4): 567–96. Bibcode:1983JThBi.100..567B. doi:10.1016/0022-5193(83)90324-7. PMID 6876815.
  81. ^ Marwick, Ben (2005). "What Can Archaeology Do With Boyd and Richerson's Cultural Evolutionary Program?". Review of Archaeology. 26 (2): 30–40. hdl:1885/44496.
  82. ^ Boyd, R. and P. J. Richerson. 2005. The Origin and Evolution of Cultures. Oxford: Oxford University Press. pp. 294-299.
  83. ^ Haag, Allison (2006). . SEED. 2 (7): 46. Archived from the original on 2007-02-10.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: unfit URL (link)
  84. ^ Laland, K. N. and G. R. Brown. 2002. Sense & Nonsense: Evolutionary Perspectives on Human Behavior. Oxford: Oxford University Press. p. 290.
  85. ^ Herb Gintis Amazon.com review: https://www.amazon.com/review/product/0198508840/
  86. ^ McElreath, R., M. Lubell, P. J. Richerson, T. M. Waring, W. Baum, E. Edsten, C. Efferson, and B. Paciotti. 2005. Applying formal models to the laboratory study of social learning: The effect of task difficulty and environmental fluctuation. Evolution and Human Behavior 26: 483-508.
  87. ^ Efferson, C.; R. Lalive; P. J. Richerson; R. McElreath; M. Lubell (2008). "Conformists and mavericks: the empirics of frequency-dependent cultural transmission". Evolution and Human Behavior. 29 (1): 56–64. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.606.1789. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2007.08.003. S2CID 2107499.
  88. ^ Baum, W. M.; Richerson, P. J.; Efferson, C. M.; Paciotti, B. M. (2004). "Cultural evolution in laboratory micro-societies including traditions of rule-giving and rule-following" (PDF). Evolution and Human Behavior. 25 (5): 305–326. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.404.8710. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2004.05.003.
  89. ^ a b Henrich, J., R. Boyd, S. Bowles, C. Camerer, E. Fehr, H. Gintis (Eds). 2004. Foundations of Human Sociality: Economic Experiments and Ethnographic Evidence from Fifteen Small-Scale Societies Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  90. ^ Cavalli-Sfornza, L. L. and M. Feldman. 1981. Cultural Transmission and Evolution: A Quantitative Approach. Princeton, New Jersey: Princeton University Press.
  91. ^ Boyd, R. and P. J. Richerson. 1985. Culture and the Evolutionary Process. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
  92. ^ McElreath, R (2004). (PDF). American Anthropologist. 106 (2): 308–321. doi:10.1525/aa.2004.106.2.308. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2007-07-17. Retrieved 2008-03-27.
  93. ^ Gintis, H (2006). "A framework for the integration of the behavioral sciences" (PDF). Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 30 (1): 1–61. doi:10.1017/s0140525x07000581. PMID 17475022. S2CID 18887154.
  94. ^ Laland, K. N. and G. R. Brown. 2002. Sense & Nonsense: Evolutionary Perspectives on Human Behavior. Oxford: Oxford University Press. p. 287-319.
  95. ^ a b Richerson, P. and R. Boyd. 2001. Culture is part of human biology: Why the superorganic concept serves the human sciences badly. In M. Goodman and A. S. Moffat(Eds.) Probing Human Origins. Cambridge, Massachusetts: The American Academy of Arts & Sciences.
  96. ^ Gintis, H (2007). "A framework for the unification of the behavioral sciences" (PDF). Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 30 (1): 1–61. doi:10.1017/s0140525x07000581. PMID 17475022. S2CID 18887154.
  97. ^ Richerson, P. J. and R. Boyd. 2005. Not By Genes Alone: How Culture Transformed Human Evolution. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. pg. 5-8
  98. ^ Barkow, J., Cosmides, L, & Tooby, J. (1992) The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture. Oxford University Press.
  99. ^ Boyer, P. (2001) Religion Explained: The Evolutionary Origins of Religious Thought. Basic Books.
  100. ^ Sperber, D. (1996). Explaining Culture: A Naturalistic Approach. Blackwell.
  101. ^ Tooby, J. & Cosmides, L., (1992) Psychological foundations of culture. In The Adapted Mind.
  102. ^ Marwick, Ben (2008). "Three styles of Darwinian evolution in the analysis of stone artefacts: Which one to use in mainland Southeast Asia?". Australian Archaeology. 67 (1): 79–86. doi:10.1080/03122417.2008.11681880. JSTOR 40288025. S2CID 15023222.
  103. ^ Guglielmino, C. R.; Viganotti, C.; Hewlett, B.; Cavalli-Sforza, L.L. (1995). "Cultural variation in Africa: role of mechanism of transmission and adaptation". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA. 92 (16): 7585–7589. Bibcode:1995PNAS...92.7585G. doi:10.1073/pnas.92.16.7585. PMC 41384. PMID 11607569.
  104. ^ Boyd, R. and P.J. Richerson. 2000. Memes: universal acid or better mouse trap. In R. Aunger (Ed), Darwinizing Culture: The Status of Memetics as a Science. Oxford: Oxford University Press. pp. 143–162.
  105. ^ Laland, K. N. and G. R. Brown. 2002. Sense & Nonsense: Evolutionary Perspectives on Human Behavior. Oxford: Oxford University Press. pp. 289-290.
  106. ^ Gabora, L (2008). . Cognitive Systems Research. 9 (1–2): 104–113. arXiv:0803.2568. Bibcode:2008arXiv0803.2568G. doi:10.1016/j.cogsys.2007.05.004. hdl:2429/64746. S2CID 13592511. Archived from the original on 2016-03-03. Retrieved 2011-05-16.
  107. ^ Gabora, L (2011). (PDF). Journal of Cognition and Culture. 11 (1–2): 61–83. arXiv:1309.2622. doi:10.1163/156853711x568699. hdl:2429/64743. S2CID 4468795. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2016-03-03. Retrieved 2011-05-16.
  108. ^ Gabora, L. (2011). How a generation was misled about natural selection. Psychology Today, Mindbloggling.
  109. ^ Gabora, L (2013). "An evolutionary framework for culture: Selectionism versus communal exchange". Physics of Life Reviews. 10 (2): 117–145. arXiv:1206.4386. doi:10.1016/j.plrev.2013.03.006. PMID 23623043. S2CID 11208336.
  110. ^ Fracchia, J.; Lewontin, R. C. (1999). "Does culture evolve?". History and Theory. 38 (4): 52–78. doi:10.1111/0018-2656.00104.
  111. ^ Temkin, I.; Eldredge, N. (2007). "Phylogenetics and material cultural evolution". Current Anthropology. 48 (1): 146–153. doi:10.1086/510463. S2CID 53466862.
  112. ^ Kauffman, S (1999). "Darwinism, neoDarwinism, and the autocatalytic model of culture: Commentary on Origin of Culture". Psycoloquy. 10 (22): 1–4.

Further reading edit

Books edit

  • Lumsden, C. J. and E. O. Wilson. 1981. Genes, Mind, and Culture: The Coevolutionary Process. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard University Press.
  • Cavalli-Sforza, L. L. and M. Feldman. 1981. Cultural Transmission and Evolution: A Quantitative Approach. Princeton, New Jersey: Princeton University Press.
  • Boyd, R. and P. J. Richerson. 1985. Culture and the Evolutionary Process. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
  • Durham, W. H. 1991. Coevolution: Genes, Culture and Human Diversity. Stanford, California: Stanford University Press. ISBN 0-8047-1537-8
  • Tomasello, M. 1999. The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Cambridge University Press.
  • Shennan, S. J. 2002. Genes, Memes and Human History: Darwinian Archaeology and Cultural Evolution. London: Thames and Hudson.
  • Laland, K. N. and G. R. Brown. 2002. Sense & Nonsense: Evolutionary Perspectives on Human Behavior. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Boyd, R. and P. J. Richerson. 2005. The Origin and Evolution of Cultures. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Richerson, P. J. and R. Boyd. 2005. Not By Genes Alone: How Culture Transformed Human Evolution. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
  • Henrich, J. 2015. The Secret of Our Success: How Culture Is Driving Human Evolution, Domesticating Our Species, and Making Us Smarter. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
  • Laland, K.H. 2017. Darwin's Unfinished Symphony: How Culture Made the Human Mind. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

Reviews edit

  • Smith, E. A. 1999. Three styles in the evolutionary analysis of human behavior. In L. Cronk, N. Chagnon, and W. Irons, (Eds.) Adaptation and Human Behavior: An Anthropological Perspective New York: Aldine de Gruyter.
  • Henrich, J.; McElreath, R. (2003). (PDF). Evolutionary Anthropology. 12 (3): 123–135. doi:10.1002/evan.10110. S2CID 14302229. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2008-04-07. Retrieved 2008-03-27.
  • Mesoudi, A.; Whiten, A.; Laland, K. N. (2006). (PDF). Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 29 (4): 329–383. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.612.2415. doi:10.1017/s0140525x06009083. PMID 17094820. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2008-12-16. Retrieved 2008-03-27.
  • Gintis, H (2006). "A framework for the integration of the behavioral sciences" (PDF). Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 30 (1): 1–61. doi:10.1017/s0140525x07000581. PMID 17475022. S2CID 18887154.
  • Bentley, R.A., C. Lipo, H.D.G. Maschner and B. Marler 2007. Darwinian Archaeologies. In R.A. Bentley, H.D.G. Maschner & C. Chippendale (Eds.) Handbook of Archaeological Theories. Lanham (MD): AltaMira Press.
  • McElreath, R. & Henrich, J. 2007. In R. Dunbar and L. Barrett, (Eds.), Oxford Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • McElreath, R. & Henrich, J. 2007. In R. Dunbar and L. Barrett, (Eds.), Oxford Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Sterelny, Kim (2002). Review Genes, Memes and Human History (PDF). Stephen Shennan. London: Thames and Hudson. p. 304.
  • Laland, K.N.; Odling-Smee, J.; Myles, S. (2010). "How culture shaped the human genome: bringing genetics and the human sciences together". Nature Reviews Genetics. 11 (2): 137–148. doi:10.1038/nrg2734. PMID 20084086. S2CID 10287878.

Journal articles edit

  • R. Boyd; P. J. Richerson. P. Carruthers; S. Stich; S. Laurence (eds.). (PDF). The Innate Mind: Culture and Cognition. Archived from the original (PDF) on January 27, 2005.
  • P. J. Richerson; R. Boyd (2001). (PDF). History and Philosophy of the Life Sciences. 23 (23): 425–465. PMID 12472064. Archived from the original (PDF) on May 28, 2006.

External links edit

Current DIT researchers edit

  • , Department of Anthropology, UCLA
  • Marcus Feldman 2015-11-28 at the Wayback Machine, Department of Biological Sciences, Stanford
  • Joe Henrich, Departments of Psychology and Economics, University of British Columbia
  • Richard McElreath, Anthropology Department, UC Davis
  • Peter J. Richerson, Department of Environmental Science and Policy, UC Davis

Related researchers edit

  • Liane Gabora 2018-10-02 at the Wayback Machine, Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia
  • Russell Gray Max Planck Institute for the Science of Human History, Jena, Germany
  • Herb Gintis 2007-06-12 at the Wayback Machine, Emeritus Professor of Economics, University of Massachusetts & Santa Fe Institute
  • Kevin Laland 2019-01-09 at the Wayback Machine, School of Biology, University of St. Andrews
  • Ruth Mace, Department of Anthropology, University College London
  • Human Biological and Cultural Evolution Group, University of Exeter, UK
  • , Department of Developmental and Comparative Psychology, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology
  • Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, University of Connecticut
  • Mark Collard, Department of Archaeology, Simon Fraser University, and Department of Archaeology, University of Aberdeen

dual, inheritance, theory, this, article, needs, additional, citations, verification, please, help, improve, this, article, adding, citations, reliable, sources, unsourced, material, challenged, removed, find, sources, news, newspapers, books, scholar, jstor, . This article needs additional citations for verification Please help improve this article by adding citations to reliable sources Unsourced material may be challenged and removed Find sources Dual inheritance theory news newspapers books scholar JSTOR March 2023 Learn how and when to remove this template message Dual inheritance theory DIT also known as gene culture coevolution or biocultural evolution 1 was developed in the 1960s through early 1980s to explain how human behavior is a product of two different and interacting evolutionary processes genetic evolution and cultural evolution Genes and culture continually interact in a feedback loop 2 changes in genes can lead to changes in culture which can then influence genetic selection and vice versa One of the theory s central claims is that culture evolves partly through a Darwinian selection process which dual inheritance theorists often describe by analogy to genetic evolution 3 Culture in this context is defined as socially learned behavior and social learning is defined as copying behaviors observed in others or acquiring behaviors through being taught by others Most of the modelling done in the field relies on the first dynamic copying though it can be extended to teaching Social learning at its simplest involves blind copying of behaviors from a model someone observed behaving though it is also understood to have many potential biases including success bias copying from those who are perceived to be better off status bias copying from those with higher status homophily copying from those most like ourselves conformist bias disproportionately picking up behaviors that more people are performing etc Understanding social learning is a system of pattern replication and understanding that there are different rates of survival for different socially learned cultural variants this sets up by definition an evolutionary structure cultural evolution 4 Because genetic evolution is relatively well understood most of DIT examines cultural evolution and the interactions between cultural evolution and genetic evolution Contents 1 Theoretical basis 1 1 Culture capacities are adaptations 1 2 Culture evolves 1 3 Genes and culture co evolve 2 View of culture 3 Genetic influence on cultural evolution 4 Cultural influences on genetic evolution 5 Mechanisms of cultural evolution 5 1 Natural selection 5 2 Random variation 5 3 Cultural drift 5 4 Guided variation 5 5 Biased transmission 5 5 1 Content bias 5 5 2 Context bias 5 5 2 1 Model based biases 5 5 2 2 Frequency dependent biases 6 Social learning and cumulative cultural evolution 7 Cultural group selection 8 Historical development 9 Current and future research 10 Relation to other fields 10 1 Sociology and cultural anthropology 10 2 Human sociobiology and evolutionary psychology 10 3 Human behavioral ecology 10 4 Memetics 11 Shortcomings and criticisms 12 See also 13 References 14 Further reading 14 1 Books 14 2 Reviews 14 3 Journal articles 15 External links 15 1 Current DIT researchers 15 2 Related researchersTheoretical basis editDIT holds that genetic and cultural evolution interacted in the evolution of Homo sapiens DIT recognizes that the natural selection of genotypes is an important component of the evolution of human behavior and that cultural traits can be constrained by genetic imperatives However DIT also recognizes that genetic evolution has endowed the human species with a parallel evolutionary process of cultural evolution DIT makes three main claims 5 Culture capacities are adaptations edit The human capacity to store and transmit culture arose from genetically evolved psychological mechanisms This implies that at some point during the evolution of the human species a type of social learning leading to cumulative cultural evolution was evolutionarily advantageous Culture evolves edit Social learning processes give rise to cultural evolution Cultural traits are transmitted differently from genetic traits and therefore result in different population level effects on behavioral variation Genes and culture co evolve edit Cultural traits alter the social and physical environments under which genetic selection operates For example the cultural adoptions of agriculture and dairying have in humans caused genetic selection for the traits to digest starch and lactose respectively 6 7 8 9 10 11 As another example it is likely that once culture became adaptive genetic selection caused a refinement of the cognitive architecture that stores and transmits cultural information This refinement may have further influenced the way culture is stored and the biases that govern its transmission DIT also predicts that under certain situations cultural evolution may select for traits that are genetically maladaptive An example of this is the demographic transition which describes the fall of birth rates within industrialized societies Dual inheritance theorists hypothesize that the demographic transition may be a result of a prestige bias where individuals that forgo reproduction to gain more influence in industrial societies are more likely to be chosen as cultural models 12 13 View of culture editPeople have defined the word culture to describe a large set of different phenomena 14 15 A definition that sums up what is meant by culture in DIT is Culture is socially learned information stored in individuals brains that is capable of affecting behavior 16 17 This view of culture emphasizes population thinking by focusing on the process by which culture is generated and maintained It also views culture as a dynamic property of individuals as opposed to a view of culture as a superorganic entity to which individuals must conform 18 This view s main advantage is that it connects individual level processes to population level outcomes 19 Genetic influence on cultural evolution editGenes affect cultural evolution via psychological predispositions on cultural learning 20 Genes encode much of the information needed to form the human brain Genes constrain the brain s structure and hence the ability of the brain to acquire and store culture Genes may also endow individuals with certain types of transmission bias described below Cultural influences on genetic evolution editCulture can profoundly influence gene frequencies in a population Lactase persistenceOne of the best known examples is the prevalence of the genotype for adult lactose absorption in human populations such as Northern Europeans and some African societies with a long history of raising cattle for milk Until around 7 500 years ago 21 lactase production stopped shortly after weaning 22 and in societies which did not develop dairying such as East Asians and Amerindians this is still true today 23 24 In areas with lactase persistence it is believed that by domesticating animals a source of milk became available while an adult and thus strong selection for lactase persistence could occur 21 25 in a Scandinavian population the estimated selection coefficient was 0 09 0 19 25 This implies that the cultural practice of raising cattle first for meat and later for milk led to selection for genetic traits for lactose digestion 26 Recently analysis of natural selection on the human genome suggests that civilization has accelerated genetic change in humans over the past 10 000 years 27 Food processingCulture has driven changes to the human digestive systems making many digestive organs such as teeth or stomach smaller than expected for primates of a similar size 28 and has been attributed to one of the reasons why humans have such large brains compared to other great apes 29 30 This is due to food processing Early examples of food processing include pounding marinating and most notably cooking Pounding meat breaks down the muscle fibres hence taking away some of the job from the mouth teeth and jaw 31 32 Marinating emulates the action of the stomach with high acid levels Cooking partially breaks down food making it more easily digestible Food enters the body effectively partly digested and as such food processing reduces the work that the digestive system has to do This means that there is selection for smaller digestive organs as the tissue is energetically expensive 28 those with smaller digestive organs can process their food but at a lower energetic cost than those with larger organs 33 Cooking is notable because the energy available from food increases when cooked and this also means less time is spent looking for food 29 34 35 Humans living on cooked diets spend only a fraction of their day chewing compared to other extant primates living on raw diets American girls and boys spent on average 7 to 8 percent of their day chewing respectively 1 68 to 1 92 hours per day compared to chimpanzees who spend more than 6 hours a day chewing 36 This frees up time which can be used for hunting A raw diet means hunting is constrained since time spent hunting is time not spent eating and chewing plant material but cooking reduces the time required to get the day s energy requirements allowing for more subsistence activities 37 Digestibility of cooked carbohydrates is approximately on average 30 higher than digestibility of non cooked carbohydrates 34 38 This increased energy intake more free time and savings made on tissue used in the digestive system allowed for the selection of genes for larger brain size Despite its benefits brain tissue requires a large amount of calories hence a main constraint in selection for larger brains is calorie intake A greater calorie intake can support greater quantities of brain tissue This is argued to explain why human brains can be much larger than other apes since humans are the only ape to engage in food processing 29 The cooking of food has influenced genes to the extent that research suggests humans cannot live without cooking 39 29 A study on 513 individuals consuming long term raw diets found that as the percentage of their diet which was made up of raw food and or the length they had been on a diet of raw food increased their BMI decreased 39 This is despite access to many non thermal processing like grinding pounding or heating to 48 C 118 F 39 With approximately 86 billion neurons in the human brain and 60 70 kg body mass an exclusively raw diet close to that of what extant primates have would be not viable as when modelled it is argued that it would require an infeasible level of more than nine hours of feeding every day 29 However this is contested with alternative modelling showing enough calories could be obtained within 5 6 hours per day 40 Some scientists and anthropologists point to evidence that brain size in the Homo lineage started to increase well before the advent of cooking due to increased consumption of meat 28 40 41 and that basic food processing slicing accounts for the size reduction in organs related to chewing 42 Cornelio et al argues that improving cooperative abilities and a varying of diet to more meat and seeds improved foraging and hunting efficiency It is this that allowed for the brain expansion independent of cooking which they argue came much later a consequence from the complex cognition that developed 40 Yet this is still an example of a cultural shift in diet and the resulting genetic evolution Further criticism comes from the controversy of the archaeological evidence available Some claim there is a lack of evidence of fire control when brain sizes first started expanding 40 43 Wrangham argues that anatomical evidence around the time of the origin of Homo erectus 1 8 million years ago indicates that the control of fire and hence cooking occurred 34 At this time the largest reductions in tooth size in the entirety of human evolution occurred indicating that softer foods became prevalent in the diet Also at this time was a narrowing of the pelvis indicating a smaller gut and also there is evidence that there was a loss of the ability to climb which Wrangham argues indicates the control of fire since sleeping on the ground needs fire to ward off predators 44 The proposed increases in brain size from food processing will have led to a greater mental capacity for further cultural innovation in food processing which will have increased digestive efficiency further providing more energy for further gains in brain size 45 This positive feedback loop is argued to have led to the rapid brain size increases seen in the Homo lineage 46 40 Mechanisms of cultural evolution editIn DIT the evolution and maintenance of cultures is described by five major mechanisms natural selection of cultural variants random variation cultural drift guided variation and transmission bias Natural selection edit Cultural differences among individuals can lead to differential survival of individuals The patterns of this selective process depend on transmission biases and can result in behavior that is more adaptive to a given environment Random variation edit Random variation arises from errors in the learning display or recall of cultural information and is roughly analogous to the process of mutation in genetic evolution Cultural drift edit Cultural drift is a process roughly analogous to genetic drift in evolutionary biology 47 48 49 In cultural drift the frequency of cultural traits in a population may be subject to random fluctuations due to chance variations in which traits are observed and transmitted sometimes called sampling error 50 These fluctuations might cause cultural variants to disappear from a population This effect should be especially strong in small populations 51 A model by Hahn and Bentley shows that cultural drift gives a reasonably good approximation to changes in the popularity of American baby names 50 Drift processes have also been suggested to explain changes in archaeological pottery and technology patent applications 49 Changes in the songs of song birds are also thought to arise from drift processes where distinct dialects in different groups occur due to errors in songbird singing and acquisition by successive generations 52 Cultural drift is also observed in an early computer model of cultural evolution 53 Guided variation edit Cultural traits may be gained in a population through the process of individual learning Once an individual learns a novel trait it can be transmitted to other members of the population The process of guided variation depends on an adaptive standard that determines what cultural variants are learned Biased transmission edit Understanding the different ways that culture traits can be transmitted between individuals has been an important part of DIT research since the 1970s 54 55 Transmission biases occur when some cultural variants are favored over others during the process of cultural transmission 56 Boyd and Richerson 1985 56 defined and analytically modeled a number of possible transmission biases The list of biases has been refined over the years especially by Henrich and McElreath 57 Content bias edit Content biases result from situations where some aspect of a cultural variant s content makes them more likely to be adopted 58 Content biases can result from genetic preferences preferences determined by existing cultural traits or a combination of the two For example food preferences can result from genetic preferences for sugary or fatty foods and socially learned eating practices and taboos 58 Content biases are sometimes called direct biases 56 Context bias edit Context biases result from individuals using clues about the social structure of their population to determine what cultural variants to adopt This determination is made without reference to the content of the variant There are two major categories of context biases model based biases and frequency dependent biases Model based biases edit Model based biases result when an individual is biased to choose a particular cultural model to imitate There are four major categories of model based biases prestige bias skill bias success bias and similarity bias 5 59 A prestige bias results when individuals are more likely to imitate cultural models that are seen as having more prestige A measure of prestige could be the amount of deference shown to a potential cultural model by other individuals A skill bias results when individuals can directly observe different cultural models performing a learned skill and are more likely to imitate cultural models that perform better at the specific skill A success bias results from individuals preferentially imitating cultural models that they determine are most generally successful as opposed to successful at a specific skill as in the skill bias A similarity bias results when individuals are more likely to imitate cultural models that are perceived as being similar to the individual based on specific traits Frequency dependent biases edit Frequency dependent biases result when an individual is biased to choose particular cultural variants based on their perceived frequency in the population The most explored frequency dependent bias is the conformity bias Conformity biases result when individuals attempt to copy the mean or the mode cultural variant in the population Another possible frequency dependent bias is the rarity bias The rarity bias results when individuals preferentially choose cultural variants that are less common in the population The rarity bias is also sometimes called a nonconformist or anti conformist bias Social learning and cumulative cultural evolution editIn DIT the evolution of culture is dependent on the evolution of social learning Analytic models show that social learning becomes evolutionarily beneficial when the environment changes with enough frequency that genetic inheritance can not track the changes but not fast enough that individual learning is more efficient 60 For environments that have very little variability social learning is not needed since genes can adapt fast enough to the changes that occur and innate behaviour is able to deal with the constant environment 61 In fast changing environments cultural learning would not be useful because what the previous generation knew is now outdated and will provide no benefit in the changed environment and hence individual learning is more beneficial It is only in the moderately changing environment where cultural learning becomes useful since each generation shares a mostly similar environment but genes have insufficient time to change to changes in the environment 62 While other species have social learning and thus some level of culture only humans some birds and chimpanzees are known to have cumulative culture 63 Boyd and Richerson argue that the evolution of cumulative culture depends on observational learning and is uncommon in other species because it is ineffective when it is rare in a population They propose that the environmental changes occurring in the Pleistocene may have provided the right environmental conditions 62 Michael Tomasello argues that cumulative cultural evolution results from a ratchet effect that began when humans developed the cognitive architecture to understand others as mental agents 64 Furthermore Tomasello proposed in the 80s that there are some disparities between the observational learning mechanisms found in humans and great apes which go some way to explain the observable difference between great ape traditions and human types of culture see Emulation observational learning Cultural group selection editFurther information on cultural group selection Cultural group selection Although group selection is commonly thought to be nonexistent or unimportant in genetic evolution 65 66 67 DIT predicts that due to the nature of cultural inheritance it may be an important force in cultural evolution Group selection occurs in cultural evolution because conformist biases make it difficult for novel cultural traits to spread through a population see above section on transmission biases Conformist bias also helps maintain variation between groups These two properties rare in genetic transmission are necessary for group selection to operate 68 Based on an earlier model by Cavalli Sforza and Feldman 69 Boyd and Richerson show that conformist biases are almost inevitable when traits spread through social learning 70 implying that group selection is common in cultural evolution Analysis of small groups in New Guinea imply that cultural group selection might be a good explanation for slowly changing aspects of social structure but not for rapidly changing fads 71 The ability of cultural evolution to maintain intergroup diversity is what allows for the study of cultural phylogenetics 72 Historical development editIn 1876 Friedrich Engels wrote a manuscript titled The Part Played by Labour in the Transition from Ape to Man accredited as a founding document of DIH 73 The approach to gene culture coevolution first developed by Engels and developed later on by anthropologists is described by Stephen Jay Gould as the best nineteenth century case for gene culture coevolution 74 The idea that human cultures undergo a similar evolutionary process as genetic evolution also goes back to Darwin 75 In the 1960s Donald T Campbell published some of the first theoretical work that adapted principles of evolutionary theory to the evolution of cultures 76 In 1976 two developments in cultural evolutionary theory set the stage for DIT In that year Richard Dawkins s The Selfish Gene introduced ideas of cultural evolution to a popular audience Although one of the best selling science books of all time because of its lack of mathematical rigor it had little effect on the development of DIT Also in 1976 geneticists Marcus Feldman and Luigi Luca Cavalli Sforza published the first dynamic models of gene culture coevolution 77 These models were to form the basis for subsequent work on DIT heralded by the publication of three seminal books in the 1980s The first was Charles Lumsden and E O Wilson s Genes Mind and Culture 78 This book outlined a series of mathematical models of how genetic evolution might favor the selection of cultural traits and how cultural traits might in turn affect the speed of genetic evolution While it was the first book published describing how genes and culture might coevolve it had relatively little effect on the further development of DIT 79 Some critics felt that their models depended too heavily on genetic mechanisms at the expense of cultural mechanisms 80 Controversy surrounding Wilson s sociobiological theories may also have decreased the lasting effect of this book 79 The second 1981 book was Cavalli Sforza and Feldman s Cultural Transmission and Evolution A Quantitative Approach 48 Borrowing heavily from population genetics and epidemiology this book built a mathematical theory concerning the spread of cultural traits It describes the evolutionary implications of vertical transmission passing cultural traits from parents to offspring oblique transmission passing cultural traits from any member of an older generation to a younger generation and horizontal transmission passing traits between members of the same population The next significant DIT publication was Robert Boyd and Peter Richerson s 1985 Culture and the Evolutionary Process 56 This book presents the now standard mathematical models of the evolution of social learning under different environmental conditions the population effects of social learning various forces of selection on cultural learning rules different forms of biased transmission and their population level effects and conflicts between cultural and genetic evolution The book s conclusion also outlined areas for future research that are still relevant today 81 Current and future research editIn their 1985 book Boyd and Richerson outlined an agenda for future DIT research This agenda outlined below called for the development of both theoretical models and empirical research DIT has since built a rich tradition of theoretical models over the past two decades 82 However there has not been a comparable level of empirical work In a 2006 interview Harvard biologist E O Wilson expressed disappointment at the little attention afforded to DIT for some reason I haven t fully fathomed this most promising frontier of scientific research has attracted very few people and very little effort 83 Kevin Laland and Gillian Ruth Brown attribute this lack of attention to DIT s heavy reliance on formal modeling In many ways the most complex and potentially rewarding of all approaches DIT with its multiple processes and cerebral onslaught of sigmas and deltas may appear too abstract to all but the most enthusiastic reader Until such a time as the theoretical hieroglyphics can be translated into a respectable empirical science most observers will remain immune to its message 84 Economist Herbert Gintis disagrees with this critique citing empirical work as well as more recent work using techniques from behavioral economics 85 These behavioral economic techniques have been adapted to test predictions of cultural evolutionary models in laboratory settings 86 87 88 as well as studying differences in cooperation in fifteen small scale societies in the field 89 Since one of the goals of DIT is to explain the distribution of human cultural traits ethnographic and ethnologic techniques may also be useful for testing hypothesis stemming from DIT Although findings from traditional ethnologic studies have been used to buttress DIT arguments 90 91 thus far there have been little ethnographic fieldwork designed to explicitly test these hypotheses 71 89 92 Herb Gintis has named DIT one of the two major conceptual theories with potential for unifying the behavioral sciences including economics biology anthropology sociology psychology and political science Because it addresses both the genetic and cultural components of human inheritance Gintis sees DIT models as providing the best explanations for the ultimate cause of human behavior and the best paradigm for integrating those disciplines with evolutionary theory 93 In a review of competing evolutionary perspectives on human behavior Laland and Brown see DIT as the best candidate for uniting the other evolutionary perspectives under one theoretical umbrella 94 Relation to other fields editSociology and cultural anthropology edit Two major topics of study in both sociology and cultural anthropology are human cultures and cultural variation However Dual Inheritance theorists charge that both disciplines too often treat culture as a static superorganic entity that dictates human behavior 95 96 Cultures are defined by a suite of common traits shared by a large group of people DIT theorists argue that this doesn t sufficiently explain variation in cultural traits at the individual level By contrast DIT models human culture at the individual level and views culture as the result of a dynamic evolutionary process at the population level 95 97 Human sociobiology and evolutionary psychology edit Evolutionary psychologists study the evolved architecture of the human mind They see it as composed of many different programs that process information each with assumptions and procedures that were specialized by natural selection to solve a different adaptive problem faced by our hunter gatherer ancestors e g choosing mates hunting avoiding predators cooperating using aggression 98 These evolved programs contain content rich assumptions about how the world and other people work When ideas are passed from mind to mind they are changed by these evolved inference systems much like messages get changed in a game of telephone But the changes are not usually random Evolved programs add and subtract information reshaping the ideas in ways that make them more intuitive more memorable and more attention grabbing In other words memes ideas are not precisely like genes Genes are normally copied faithfully as they are replicated but ideas normally are not It s not just that ideas mutate every once in a while like genes do Ideas are transformed every time they are passed from mind to mind because the sender s message is being interpreted by evolved inference systems in the receiver 99 100 It is useful for some applications to note however that there are ways to pass ideas which are more resilient and involve substantially less mutation such as by mass distribution of printed media There is no necessary contradiction between evolutionary psychology and DIT but evolutionary psychologists argue that the psychology implicit in many DIT models is too simple evolved programs have a rich inferential structure not captured by the idea of a content bias They also argue that some of the phenomena DIT models attribute to cultural evolution are cases of evoked culture situations in which different evolved programs are activated in different places in response to cues in the environment 101 Sociobiologists try to understand how maximizing genetic fitness in either the modern era or past environments can explain human behavior When faced with a trait that seems maladaptive some sociobiologists try to determine how the trait actually increases genetic fitness maybe through kin selection or by speculating about early evolutionary environments Dual inheritance theorists in contrast will consider a variety of genetic and cultural processes in addition to natural selection on genes Human behavioral ecology edit Human behavioral ecology HBE and DIT have a similar relationship to what ecology and evolutionary biology have in the biological sciences HBE is more concerned about ecological process and DIT more focused on historical process 102 One difference is that human behavioral ecologists often assume that culture is a system that produces the most adaptive outcome in a given environment This implies that similar behavioral traditions should be found in similar environments However this is not always the case A study of African cultures showed that cultural history was a better predictor of cultural traits than local ecological conditions 103 Memetics edit Memetics which comes from the meme idea described in Dawkins s The Selfish Gene is similar to DIT in that it treats culture as an evolutionary process that is distinct from genetic transmission However there are some philosophical differences between memetics and DIT 104 One difference is that memetics focus is on the selection potential of discrete replicators memes where DIT allows for transmission of both non replicators and non discrete cultural variants DIT does not assume that replicators are necessary for cumulative adaptive evolution DIT also more strongly emphasizes the role of genetic inheritance in shaping the capacity for cultural evolution But perhaps the biggest difference is a difference in academic lineage Memetics as a label is more influential in popular culture than in academia Critics of memetics argue that it is lacking in empirical support or is conceptually ill founded and question whether there is hope for the memetic research program succeeding Proponents point out that many cultural traits are discrete and that many existing models of cultural inheritance assume discrete cultural units and hence involve memes 105 Shortcomings and criticisms editPsychologist Liane Gabora has criticised DIT 106 107 108 She argues that use of the term dual inheritance to refer to not just traits that are transmitted by way of a self assembly code as in genetic evolution but also traits that are not transmitted by way of a self assembly code as in cultural evolution is misleading because this second use does not capture the algorithmic structure that makes an inheritance system require a particular kind of mathematical framework 109 Other criticisms of the effort to frame culture in Darwinian terms have been leveled by Richard Lewontin 110 Niles Eldredge 111 and Stuart Kauffman 112 See also editNature versus nurture Debate about heredity and environment as determinants of physical or mental development Adaptive bias Theory of bias in human reasoning Cultural selection theory Study of cultural change modelled on theories of evolutionary biology Memetics Study of self replicating units of culture Sociocultural evolution Evolution of societiesReferences edit O Neil Dennis Glossary of Terms Modern Theories of Evolution Retrieved 28 October 2012 Laland Kevin N 2008 11 12 Exploring gene culture interactions insights from handedness sexual selection and niche construction case studies Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences 363 1509 3577 3589 doi 10 1098 rstb 2008 0132 ISSN 0962 8436 PMC 2607340 PMID 18799415 Richerson Peter J Boyd Robert 2005 Not By Genes Alone How Culture Transformed Human Evolution University of Chicago Press Campbell D T 1965 Variation and selective retention in socio cultural evolution Social Change in Developing Areas A Reinterpretation of Evolutionary Theory a b McElreath R Henrich J 2007 Dual inheritance theory the evolution of human cultural capacities and cultural evolution PDF In R Dunbar and L Barrett ed Oxford Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology Oxford Oxford University Press Archived from the original PDF on 2007 08 11 Simoons F 1969 Primary adult lactose intolerance and the milking habit A problem in biologic and cultural interrelations I Review of the medical research The American Journal of Digestive Diseases 14 12 819 836 doi 10 1007 bf02233204 PMID 4902756 S2CID 22597839 Simoons F 1970 Primary adult lactose intolerance and the milking habit A problem in biologic and cultural interrelations II A culture historical hypothesis The American Journal of Digestive Diseases 15 8 695 710 doi 10 1007 bf02235991 PMID 5468838 S2CID 2140863 Cavalli Sforza L P Menozzi and A Piazza 1994 The history and geography of human genes Princeton Princeton University Press Holden C Mace R 1997 Phylogenetic analysis of the evolution of lactose digestion in adults Human Biology 69 5 605 628 PMID 9299882 Durham W 1991 Coevolution Genes culture and human diversity Stanford Stanford University Press Chapter 5 Perry G N Dominy K Claw A Lee H gt Fiegler R Redon J Werner F Villanea J Mountain R Misra N Carter C Lee A Stone Diet and the evolution of human amylase gene copy number variation Nature Genetics 39 1256 1260 Boyd R and P J Richerson 1985 Culture and the Evolutionary Process Chicago University of Chicago Press pp 199 202 Richerson P J and R Boyd 2005 Not By Genes Alone How Culture Transformed Human Evolution Chicago University of Chicago Press pp 169 182 Kroeberm A and C Kluckhohn 1952 Culture A Critical Review of Concepts and Definitions Cambridge MA Harvard University Fox R and B King 2002 Anthropology Beyond Culture Oxford Berg Richerson P and R Boyd 2005 Not By Genes Alone How Culture Transformed Human Evolution Chicago University of Chicago Press pg 6 Boyd R and P Richerson 2000 Memes Universal Acid or a Better Mouse Trap In R Aunger Ed Darwinizing Culture The Status of Memetics as a Science Oxford Oxford University Press pp 143 162 Full text Richerson P J and R Boyd 2001 Culture is Part of Human Biology Why the Superorganic Concept Serves the Human Sciences Badly In Science Studies Probing the Dynamics of Scientific Knowledge In S Maasen and M Winterhager Ed Bielefeld Verlag 1 Richerson P and R Boyd 2005 Not By Genes Alone How Culture Transformed Human Evolution Chicago University of Chicago Press pg 7 Sasaki Joni Y 2013 Promise and Challenges Surrounding Culture Gene Coevolution and Gene Culture Interactions Psychological Inquiry 24 64 70 doi 10 1080 1047840X 2013 764814 S2CID 144039856 a b Itan Yuval Powell Adam Beaumont Mark A Burger Joachim Thomas Mark G 2009 08 28 The Origins of Lactase Persistence in Europe PLOS Computational Biology 5 8 e1000491 Bibcode 2009PLSCB 5E0491I doi 10 1371 journal pcbi 1000491 ISSN 1553 7358 PMC 2722739 PMID 19714206 Malmstrom Helena Linderholm Anna Liden Kerstin Stora Jan Molnar Petra Holmlund Gunilla Jakobsson Mattias Gotherstrom Anders 2010 01 01 High frequency of lactose intolerance in a prehistoric hunter gatherer population in northern Europe BMC Evolutionary Biology 10 89 doi 10 1186 1471 2148 10 89 ISSN 1471 2148 PMC 2862036 PMID 20353605 Maps www ucl ac uk Archived from the original on 2017 03 28 Retrieved 2017 03 27 Gerbault Pascale Roffet Salque Melanie Evershed Richard P Thomas Mark G 2013 12 01 How long have adult humans been consuming milk IUBMB Life 65 12 983 990 doi 10 1002 iub 1227 ISSN 1521 6551 PMID 24339181 S2CID 34564411 a b Bersaglieri Todd Sabeti Pardis C Patterson Nick Vanderploeg Trisha Schaffner Steve F Drake Jared A Rhodes Matthew Reich David E Hirschhorn Joel N 2017 03 27 Genetic Signatures of Strong Recent Positive Selection at the Lactase Gene American Journal of Human Genetics 74 6 1111 1120 doi 10 1086 421051 ISSN 0002 9297 PMC 1182075 PMID 15114531 Laland K N and G R Brown 2002 Sense amp Nonsense Evolutionary Perspectives on Human Behavior Oxford Oxford University Press p 260 Cochran G and H Harpending 2009 The 10 000 Year Explosion How Civilization Accelerated Human Evolution Basic Books a b c Aiello Leslie C Wheeler Peter 1995 01 01 The Expensive Tissue Hypothesis The Brain and the Digestive System in Human and Primate Evolution Current Anthropology 36 2 199 221 doi 10 1086 204350 JSTOR 2744104 S2CID 144317407 a b c d e Fonseca Azevedo Karina Herculano Houzel Suzana 2012 11 06 Metabolic constraint imposes tradeoff between body size and number of brain neurons in human evolution Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 109 45 18571 18576 Bibcode 2012PNAS 10918571F doi 10 1073 pnas 1206390109 ISSN 0027 8424 PMC 3494886 PMID 23090991 Gorman Rachael Moeller 2008 Cooking Up Bigger Brains Scientific American 298 1 102 105 Bibcode 2008SciAm 298a 102G doi 10 1038 scientificamerican0108 102 PMID 18225702 Farrell J H 1956 05 01 The effect on digestibility of methods commonly used to increase the tenderness of lean meat British Journal of Nutrition 10 2 111 115 doi 10 1079 BJN19560019 ISSN 1475 2662 PMID 13315930 Henrich Joseph 2015 The Secret of Our Success How Culture Is Driving Human Evolution Domesticating Our Species and Making Us Smarter Princeton Princeton University Press p 66 ISBN 9780691166858 Wrangham Richard 2009 Catching Fire How Cooking Made Us Human London Profile Books p 40 ISBN 9781846682865 a b c Carmody Rachel N Wrangham Richard W 2009 10 01 The energetic significance of cooking Journal of Human Evolution 57 4 379 391 doi 10 1016 j jhevol 2009 02 011 ISSN 1095 8606 PMID 19732938 S2CID 15255649 Carmody Rachel N Weintraub Gil S Wrangham Richard W 2011 11 29 Energetic consequences of thermal and nonthermal food processing Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 108 48 19199 19203 Bibcode 2011PNAS 10819199C doi 10 1073 pnas 1112128108 ISSN 1091 6490 PMC 3228431 PMID 22065771 Wrangham Richard 2010 08 06 Catching Fire How Cooking Made Us Human Main ed Profile Books p 140 Wrangham Richard 2010 05 27 Catching Fire How Cooking Made Us Human Main ed London Profile Books p 142 ISBN 9781846682865 University of California Television UCTV 2013 03 21 CARTA The Evolution of Human Nutrition Richard Wrangham Fire Starch Meat and Honey archived from the original on 2021 12 19 retrieved 2017 03 27 a b c Koebnick C Strassner C Hoffmann I Leitzmann C 1999 01 01 Consequences of a long term raw food diet on body weight and menstruation results of a questionnaire survey Annals of Nutrition amp Metabolism 43 2 69 79 doi 10 1159 000012770 ISSN 0250 6807 PMID 10436305 S2CID 30125503 a b c d e Cornelio Alianda M de Bittencourt Navarrete Ruben E de Bittencourt Brum Ricardo Queiroz Claudio M Costa Marcos R 2016 04 25 Human Brain Expansion during Evolution Is Independent of Fire Control and Cooking Frontiers in Neuroscience 10 167 doi 10 3389 fnins 2016 00167 ISSN 1662 4548 PMC 4842772 PMID 27199631 06 14 99 Meat eating was essential for human evolution says UC Berkeley anthropologist specializing in diet www berkeley edu Retrieved 2017 03 27 Zink Katherine D Lieberman Daniel E 2016 03 24 Impact of meat and Lower Palaeolithic food processing techniques on chewing in humans Nature 531 7595 500 503 Bibcode 2016Natur 531 500Z doi 10 1038 nature16990 ISSN 0028 0836 PMID 26958832 S2CID 4384345 Roebroeks Wil Villa Paola 2011 03 29 On the earliest evidence for habitual use of fire in Europe Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 108 13 5209 5214 Bibcode 2011PNAS 108 5209R doi 10 1073 pnas 1018116108 ISSN 0027 8424 PMC 3069174 PMID 21402905 Wrangham Richard 2010 05 27 Catching Fire How Cooking Made Us Human Main ed London Profile Books pp 98 102 ISBN 9781846682865 Wrangham Richard 2010 08 06 Catching Fire How Cooking Made Us Human Main ed Profile Books p 127 Wrangham Richard 2010 05 27 Catching Fire How Cooking Made Us Human Main ed London Profile Books p 127 ISBN 9781846682865 Koerper H Stickel E 1980 Cultural Drift A Primary Process of Culture Change Journal of Anthropological Research 36 4 463 469 doi 10 1086 jar 36 4 3629615 S2CID 163932368 a b Cavalli Sfornza L and M Feldman 1981 Cultural Transmission and Evolution A Quantitative Approach Princeton New Jersey Princeton University Press a b Bentley R A Hahn M W Shennan S J 2004 Random drift and culture change PDF Proceedings of the Royal Society B 271 1547 1443 1450 doi 10 1098 rspb 2004 2746 PMC 1691747 PMID 15306315 Archived from the original PDF on 2011 09 28 Retrieved 2008 05 05 a b Hahn M W Bentley R A 2003 Drift as a mechanism for cultural change An example from baby names PDF Proceedings of the Royal Society B 270 Suppl 1 S120 S123 doi 10 1098 rsbl 2003 0045 PMC 1698036 PMID 12952655 Boyd R and P J Richerson 1985 Culture and the Evolutionary Process Chicago University of Chicago Press pp 9 69 P J B Slater V M Janik Vocal Learning In Encyclopedia of Animal Behavior 2010 Pages 551 557 http www sciencedirect com science referenceworks 9780080453378 Gabora L 1995 Meme and variations A computer model of cultural evolution Archived 2011 08 28 at the Wayback Machine In L Nadel amp D Stein Eds 1993 Lectures in Complex Systems Addison Wesley 471 486 Feldman M Cavalli Sforza L 1976 Cultural and biological evolutionary processes selection for a trait under complex transmission Theoretical Population Biology 9 2 238 259 doi 10 1016 0040 5809 76 90047 2 PMID 1273802 Feldman M Cavalli Sfornza L 1977 The evolution of continuous variation II complex transmission and assortive mating Theoretical Population Biology 11 2 161 181 doi 10 1016 0040 5809 77 90024 7 PMID 867286 a b c d Boyd R and P Richerson 1985 Culture and the Evolutionary Process Chicago The University of Chicago Press Henrich J McElreath R 2003 The Evolution of Cultural Evolution PDF Evolutionary Anthropology 12 3 123 135 doi 10 1002 evan 10110 S2CID 14302229 Archived from the original PDF on 2005 05 28 Retrieved 2009 09 08 a b Henrich J and R McElreath 2007 Dual inheritance theory the evolution of human cultural capacities and cultural evolution Oxford Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology R Dunbar and L Barrett eds Ch 38 Oxford Oxford University Press Henrich J McElreath R 2003 The evolution of cultural evolution PDF Evolutionary Anthropology 12 3 123 135 doi 10 1002 evan 10110 S2CID 14302229 Archived from the original PDF on 2008 04 07 Retrieved 2008 03 27 Richerson P J and R Boyd 2000 Climate culture and the evolution of cognition In C M Heyes and L Huber Eds The Evolution of Cognition Massachusetts MIT Press Kerr Benjamin Feldman Marcus W 2003 Carving the Cognitive Niche Optimal Learning Strategies in Homogeneous and Heterogeneous Environments ScienceDirect Journal of Theoretical Biology 220 2 169 188 doi 10 1006 jtbi 2003 3146 PMID 12468290 a b Richerson P J Boyd R 2000 Built for Speed Pleistocene Climate Variation and the Origin of Human Culture Perspectives in Ethology Vol 13 pp 1 45 doi 10 1007 978 1 4615 1221 9 1 ISBN 978 1 4613 5447 5 Tomasello M 1999 The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition Cambridge Massachusetts Cambridge University Press Tomasello M 1999 The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition Cambridge MA Harvard University Press Williams G C 1972 Adaptation and Natural Selection A Critique of Some Current Evolutionary Thought Princeton Princeton University Press ISBN 0 691 02357 3 Williams G C 1986 Evolution Through Group Selection Blackwell ISBN 0 632 01541 1 Maynard Smith J 1964 Group selection and kin selection Nature 201 4924 1145 1147 Bibcode 1964Natur 201 1145S doi 10 1038 2011145a0 S2CID 4177102 Uyenoyama M Feldman M W 1980 Theories of kin and group selection a population genetics perspective Theoretical Population Biology 17 3 380 414 doi 10 1016 0040 5809 80 90033 7 PMID 7434256 Cavalli Sforza L L Feldman M W 1973 Models for cultural inheritance I Group mean and within group variation Theoretical Population Biology 4 1 42 44 doi 10 1016 0040 5809 73 90005 1 PMID 4726009 Boyd R and P J Richerson 1985 Culture and the Evolutionary Process Chicago University of Chicago Press pg 227 240 a b Soltis J Boyd R Richerson P J 1995 Can group functional behaviors evolve by cultural group selection An empirical test PDF Current Anthropology 36 3 473 494 doi 10 1086 204381 S2CID 43998139 Mace R C Holden and S Shennan Eds 2005 The evolution of cultural diversity a phylogenetic approach London University College London Press Foster John 2000 Marx s Ecology Materialism and Nature New York Monthly Review Press p 203 ISBN 9781583670125 Gould Stephen 1987 An Urchin in the Storm New York W W Norton pp 111 112 ISBN 0 393 02492 X Darwin C 1874 The descent of man and selection in relation to sex 2nd ed 2 vols New York American Home Library Campbell D 1965 Variation and selective retention in socio cultural evolution In Social change in developing areas A reinterpretation of evolutionary theory ed H Barringer G Blanksten and R Mack 19 49 Cambridge MA Schenkman Publishing Company Feldman M Cavalli Sforna L 1976 Cultural and biological evolutionary processes selection for a trait under complex transmission Theoretical Population Biology 9 2 238 59 doi 10 1016 0040 5809 76 90047 2 PMID 1273802 Lumsden C and E Wilson 1981 Genes Mind and Culture The Coevolutionary Process Cambridge MA Harvard University Press a b Laland K and G Brown 2002 Sense and Nonsense Evolutionary Perspectives on Human Behavior Oxford Oxford University Press Boyd R Richerson P 1983 The cultural transmission of acquired variation effects on genetic fitness Journal of Theoretical Biology 100 4 567 96 Bibcode 1983JThBi 100 567B doi 10 1016 0022 5193 83 90324 7 PMID 6876815 Marwick Ben 2005 What Can Archaeology Do With Boyd and Richerson s Cultural Evolutionary Program Review of Archaeology 26 2 30 40 hdl 1885 44496 Boyd R and P J Richerson 2005 The Origin and Evolution of Cultures Oxford Oxford University Press pp 294 299 Haag Allison 2006 The synthesizer SEED 2 7 46 Archived from the original on 2007 02 10 a href Template Cite journal html title Template Cite journal cite journal a CS1 maint unfit URL link Laland K N and G R Brown 2002 Sense amp Nonsense Evolutionary Perspectives on Human Behavior Oxford Oxford University Press p 290 Herb Gintis Amazon com review https www amazon com review product 0198508840 McElreath R M Lubell P J Richerson T M Waring W Baum E Edsten C Efferson and B Paciotti 2005 Applying formal models to the laboratory study of social learning The effect of task difficulty and environmental fluctuation Evolution and Human Behavior 26 483 508 Efferson C R Lalive P J Richerson R McElreath M Lubell 2008 Conformists and mavericks the empirics of frequency dependent cultural transmission Evolution and Human Behavior 29 1 56 64 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 606 1789 doi 10 1016 j evolhumbehav 2007 08 003 S2CID 2107499 Baum W M Richerson P J Efferson C M Paciotti B M 2004 Cultural evolution in laboratory micro societies including traditions of rule giving and rule following PDF Evolution and Human Behavior 25 5 305 326 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 404 8710 doi 10 1016 j evolhumbehav 2004 05 003 a b Henrich J R Boyd S Bowles C Camerer E Fehr H Gintis Eds 2004 Foundations of Human Sociality Economic Experiments and Ethnographic Evidence from Fifteen Small Scale Societies Oxford Oxford University Press Cavalli Sfornza L L and M Feldman 1981 Cultural Transmission and Evolution A Quantitative Approach Princeton New Jersey Princeton University Press Boyd R and P J Richerson 1985 Culture and the Evolutionary Process Chicago University of Chicago Press McElreath R 2004 Social learning and the maintenance of cultural variation An evolutionary model and data from East Africa PDF American Anthropologist 106 2 308 321 doi 10 1525 aa 2004 106 2 308 Archived from the original PDF on 2007 07 17 Retrieved 2008 03 27 Gintis H 2006 A framework for the integration of the behavioral sciences PDF Behavioral and Brain Sciences 30 1 1 61 doi 10 1017 s0140525x07000581 PMID 17475022 S2CID 18887154 Laland K N and G R Brown 2002 Sense amp Nonsense Evolutionary Perspectives on Human Behavior Oxford Oxford University Press p 287 319 a b Richerson P and R Boyd 2001 Culture is part of human biology Why the superorganic concept serves the human sciences badly In M Goodman and A S Moffat Eds Probing Human Origins Cambridge Massachusetts The American Academy of Arts amp Sciences Gintis H 2007 A framework for the unification of the behavioral sciences PDF Behavioral and Brain Sciences 30 1 1 61 doi 10 1017 s0140525x07000581 PMID 17475022 S2CID 18887154 Richerson P J and R Boyd 2005 Not By Genes Alone How Culture Transformed Human Evolution Chicago University of Chicago Press pg 5 8 Barkow J Cosmides L amp Tooby J 1992 The Adapted Mind Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture Oxford University Press Boyer P 2001 Religion Explained The Evolutionary Origins of Religious Thought Basic Books Sperber D 1996 Explaining Culture A Naturalistic Approach Blackwell Tooby J amp Cosmides L 1992 Psychological foundations of culture In The Adapted Mind Marwick Ben 2008 Three styles of Darwinian evolution in the analysis of stone artefacts Which one to use in mainland Southeast Asia Australian Archaeology 67 1 79 86 doi 10 1080 03122417 2008 11681880 JSTOR 40288025 S2CID 15023222 Guglielmino C R Viganotti C Hewlett B Cavalli Sforza L L 1995 Cultural variation in Africa role of mechanism of transmission and adaptation Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 92 16 7585 7589 Bibcode 1995PNAS 92 7585G doi 10 1073 pnas 92 16 7585 PMC 41384 PMID 11607569 Boyd R and P J Richerson 2000 Memes universal acid or better mouse trap In R Aunger Ed Darwinizing Culture The Status of Memetics as a Science Oxford Oxford University Press pp 143 162 Laland K N and G R Brown 2002 Sense amp Nonsense Evolutionary Perspectives on Human Behavior Oxford Oxford University Press pp 289 290 Gabora L 2008 The cultural evolution of socially situated cognition Cognitive Systems Research 9 1 2 104 113 arXiv 0803 2568 Bibcode 2008arXiv0803 2568G doi 10 1016 j cogsys 2007 05 004 hdl 2429 64746 S2CID 13592511 Archived from the original on 2016 03 03 Retrieved 2011 05 16 Gabora L 2011 Five clarifications about cultural evolution PDF Journal of Cognition and Culture 11 1 2 61 83 arXiv 1309 2622 doi 10 1163 156853711x568699 hdl 2429 64743 S2CID 4468795 Archived from the original PDF on 2016 03 03 Retrieved 2011 05 16 Gabora L 2011 How a generation was misled about natural selection Psychology Today Mindbloggling Gabora L 2013 An evolutionary framework for culture Selectionism versus communal exchange Physics of Life Reviews 10 2 117 145 arXiv 1206 4386 doi 10 1016 j plrev 2013 03 006 PMID 23623043 S2CID 11208336 Fracchia J Lewontin R C 1999 Does culture evolve History and Theory 38 4 52 78 doi 10 1111 0018 2656 00104 Temkin I Eldredge N 2007 Phylogenetics and material cultural evolution Current Anthropology 48 1 146 153 doi 10 1086 510463 S2CID 53466862 Kauffman S 1999 Darwinism neoDarwinism and the autocatalytic model of culture Commentary on Origin of Culture Psycoloquy 10 22 1 4 Further reading editBooks edit Lumsden C J and E O Wilson 1981 Genes Mind and Culture The Coevolutionary Process Cambridge Massachusetts Harvard University Press Cavalli Sforza L L and M Feldman 1981 Cultural Transmission and Evolution A Quantitative Approach Princeton New Jersey Princeton University Press Boyd R and P J Richerson 1985 Culture and the Evolutionary Process Chicago University of Chicago Press Durham W H 1991 Coevolution Genes Culture and Human Diversity Stanford California Stanford University Press ISBN 0 8047 1537 8 Tomasello M 1999 The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition Cambridge Massachusetts Cambridge University Press Shennan S J 2002 Genes Memes and Human History Darwinian Archaeology and Cultural Evolution London Thames and Hudson Laland K N and G R Brown 2002 Sense amp Nonsense Evolutionary Perspectives on Human Behavior Oxford Oxford University Press Boyd R and P J Richerson 2005 The Origin and Evolution of Cultures Oxford Oxford University Press Richerson P J and R Boyd 2005 Not By Genes Alone How Culture Transformed Human Evolution Chicago University of Chicago Press Henrich J 2015 The Secret of Our Success How Culture Is Driving Human Evolution Domesticating Our Species and Making Us Smarter Princeton Princeton University Press Laland K H 2017 Darwin s Unfinished Symphony How Culture Made the Human Mind Princeton Princeton University Press Reviews edit Smith E A 1999 Three styles in the evolutionary analysis of human behavior In L Cronk N Chagnon and W Irons Eds Adaptation and Human Behavior An Anthropological Perspective New York Aldine de Gruyter Henrich J McElreath R 2003 The evolution of cultural evolution PDF Evolutionary Anthropology 12 3 123 135 doi 10 1002 evan 10110 S2CID 14302229 Archived from the original PDF on 2008 04 07 Retrieved 2008 03 27 Mesoudi A Whiten A Laland K N 2006 Towards a unified science of cultural evolution PDF Behavioral and Brain Sciences 29 4 329 383 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 612 2415 doi 10 1017 s0140525x06009083 PMID 17094820 Archived from the original PDF on 2008 12 16 Retrieved 2008 03 27 Gintis H 2006 A framework for the integration of the behavioral sciences PDF Behavioral and Brain Sciences 30 1 1 61 doi 10 1017 s0140525x07000581 PMID 17475022 S2CID 18887154 Bentley R A C Lipo H D G Maschner and B Marler 2007 Darwinian Archaeologies In R A Bentley H D G Maschner amp C Chippendale Eds Handbook of Archaeological Theories Lanham MD AltaMira Press McElreath R amp Henrich J 2007 Modeling cultural evolution In R Dunbar and L Barrett Eds Oxford Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology Oxford Oxford University Press McElreath R amp Henrich J 2007 Dual inheritance theory the evolution of human cultural capacities and cultural evolution In R Dunbar and L Barrett Eds Oxford Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology Oxford Oxford University Press Sterelny Kim 2002 Review Genes Memes and Human History PDF Stephen Shennan London Thames and Hudson p 304 Laland K N Odling Smee J Myles S 2010 How culture shaped the human genome bringing genetics and the human sciences together Nature Reviews Genetics 11 2 137 148 doi 10 1038 nrg2734 PMID 20084086 S2CID 10287878 Journal articles edit R Boyd P J Richerson P Carruthers S Stich S Laurence eds Culture Adaptation and Innateness PDF The Innate Mind Culture and Cognition Archived from the original PDF on January 27 2005 P J Richerson R Boyd 2001 Built for Speed Not for Comfort Darwinian Theory and Human Culture PDF History and Philosophy of the Life Sciences 23 23 425 465 PMID 12472064 Archived from the original PDF on May 28 2006 External links editCurrent DIT researchers edit Rob Boyd Department of Anthropology UCLA Marcus Feldman Archived 2015 11 28 at the Wayback Machine Department of Biological Sciences Stanford Joe Henrich Departments of Psychology and Economics University of British Columbia Richard McElreath Anthropology Department UC Davis Peter J Richerson Department of Environmental Science and Policy UC DavisRelated researchers edit Liane Gabora Archived 2018 10 02 at the Wayback Machine Department of Psychology University of British Columbia Russell Gray Max Planck Institute for the Science of Human History Jena Germany Herb Gintis Archived 2007 06 12 at the Wayback Machine Emeritus Professor of Economics University of Massachusetts amp Santa Fe Institute Kevin Laland Archived 2019 01 09 at the Wayback Machine School of Biology University of St Andrews Ruth Mace Department of Anthropology University College London Alex Mesoudi Human Biological and Cultural Evolution Group University of Exeter UK Michael Tomasello Department of Developmental and Comparative Psychology Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology Peter Turchin Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology University of Connecticut Mark Collard Department of Archaeology Simon Fraser University and Department of Archaeology University of Aberdeen Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Dual inheritance theory amp oldid 1207617726, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.