fbpx
Wikipedia

Terror management theory

Terror management theory (TMT) is both a social and evolutionary psychology theory originally proposed by Jeff Greenberg, Sheldon Solomon, and Tom Pyszczynski[1] and codified in their book The Worm at the Core: On the Role of Death in Life (2015). It proposes that a basic psychological conflict results from having a self-preservation instinct while realizing that death is inevitable and to some extent unpredictable. This conflict produces terror, which is managed through a combination of escapism and cultural beliefs that act to counter biological reality with more significant and enduring forms of meaning and value—-basically countering the personal insignificance represented by death with the significance provided by symbolic culture.[1][2]

The most obvious examples of cultural values that assuage death anxiety are those that purport to offer literal immortality (e.g. belief in the afterlife through religion).[3] However, TMT also argues that other cultural values – including those that are seemingly unrelated to death – offer symbolic immortality. For example, values of national identity,[4] posterity,[5] cultural perspectives on sex,[6] and human superiority over animals[6] have been linked to calming death concerns. In many cases these values are thought to offer symbolic immortality, by either a) providing the sense that one is part of something greater that will ultimately outlive the individual (e.g. country, lineage, species), or b) making one's symbolic identity superior to biological nature (i.e. you are a personality, which makes you more than a glob of cells).[7] Because cultural values influence what is meaningful, they are foundational for self-esteem. TMT describes self-esteem as being the personal, subjective measure of how well an individual is living up to their cultural values.[2]

Terror management theory was developed by social psychologists Greenberg, Solomon, and Pyszczynski. However, the idea of TMT originated from anthropologist Ernest Becker's 1973 Pulitzer Prize-winning work of nonfiction The Denial of Death. Becker argues most human action is taken to ignore or avoid the inevitability of death.[8] The terror of absolute annihilation creates such a profound – albeit subconscious – anxiety in people that they spend their lives attempting to make sense of it. On large scales, societies build symbols: Laws, religious meanings, cultures, and belief systems to explain the significance of life, define what makes certain characteristics, skills, and talents extraordinary, reward others whom they find exemplify certain attributes, and punish or kill others who do not adhere to their cultural worldview. Adherence to these created "symbols" aid in relieving stresses associated with the reality of mortality.[9] On an individual level, self-esteem provides a buffer against death-related anxiety.

Background edit

The idea of death, the fear of it, haunts the human animal like nothing else; it is a mainspring of human activity designed largely to avoid the fatality of death, to overcome it by denying in some way that it is the final destiny for man.

Ernest Becker, 1973[10]

In the 1st century CE, Statius in his Thebaid suggested that "fear first made gods in the world".[11]

Cultural anthropologist Ernest Becker asserted in his 1973 book The Denial of Death that humans, as intelligent animals, are able to grasp the inevitability of death. They therefore spend their lives building and believing in cultural elements that illustrate how to make themselves stand out as individuals and to give their lives significance and meaning.[12] Death creates an anxiety in humans; it strikes at unexpected and random moments, and its nature is essentially unknowable, causing people to spend most of their time and energy to explain, forestall, and avoid it.[13]

Becker expounded upon the previous writings of Sigmund Freud, Søren Kierkegaard, Norman O. Brown, and Otto Rank. According to clinical psychiatrist Morton Levitt, Becker replaces the Freudian preoccupation with sexuality with the fear of death as the primary motivation in human behavior.[14]

People desire to think of themselves as beings of value and worth with a feeling of permanence, a concept in psychology known as self-esteem. This feeling counters the cognitive dissonance created by an individual's realization that they may be no more important than any other living thing. Becker refers to high self-esteem as heroism:

the problem of heroics is the central one of human life, that it goes deeper into human nature than anything else because it is based on organismic narcissism and on the child's need for self-esteem as the condition for his life. Society itself is a codified hero system, which means that society everywhere is a living myth of the significance of human life, a defiant creation of meaning.[15]

The rationale behind decisions regarding one's own health can be explored through a terror-management model. A 2008 research article in Psychological Review proposes a three-part model for understanding how awareness of death can ironically subvert health-promoting behaviors by redirecting one's focus towards behaviors that build self-esteem instead:

Proposition 1 suggests that conscious thoughts about death can instigate health-oriented responses aimed at removing death-related thoughts from current focal attention. Proposition 2 suggests that the unconscious resonance of death-related cognition promotes self-oriented defenses directed toward maintaining, not one's health, but a sense of meaning and self-esteem. The last proposition suggests that confrontations with the physical body may undermine symbolic defenses and thus present a previously unrecognized barrier to health promotion activities.[16]

Evolutionary backdrop edit

Terror-management theorists regard TMT as compatible with the theory of evolution:[17] Valid fears of dangerous things have an adaptive function that helped facilitate the survival of our ancestors' genes. However, generalized existential anxiety resulting from the clash between a desire for life and awareness of the inevitability of death is neither adaptive nor selected for. TMT views existential anxiety as an unfortunate byproduct of these two highly adaptive human proclivities rather than as an adaptation that the evolutionary process selected for its advantages. Just as human bipedalism confers advantages as well as disadvantages, death anxiety is an inevitable part of our intelligence and awareness of dangers.

Anxiety in response to the inevitability of death threatened to undermine adaptive functioning and therefore needed amelioration. TMT posits that humankind used the same intellectual capacities that gave rise to this problem to fashion cultural beliefs and values that provided protection against this potential anxiety. TMT considers these cultural beliefs (even unpleasant and frightening ones, such as ritual human sacrifice) when they manage potential death-anxiety in a way that promotes beliefs and behaviors which facilitated the functioning and survival of the collective.

Hunter-gatherers used their emerging cognitive abilities to facilitate solving practical problems, such as basic needs for nutrition, mating, and tool-making. As these abilities evolved, an explicit awareness of death also emerged. But once this awareness materialized, the potential for terror that it created put pressure on emerging conceptions of reality. Any conceptual formation that was to be widely accepted by the group needed to provide a means of managing this terror.

Originally, morality evolved to facilitate co-existence within groups. Together with language, morality served pragmatic functions that extended survival. The struggle to deny the finality of death co-opted and changed the function of these cultural inventions. For example, Neanderthals might have begun burying their dead as a means of avoiding unpleasant odors, disease-infested parasites, or dangerous scavengers. But during the Upper Paleolithic era, these pragmatic burial practices appear to have become imbued with layers of ritual performance and supernatural beliefs, suggested by the elaborate decoration of bodies with thousands of beads or other markers. Food and other necessities were also included within the burial chamber, indicating the potential for a belief system that included life after death. Many human cultures today treat funerals primarily as cultural events, viewed through the lens of morality and language, with little thought given to the utilitarian origins of burying the dead.

Evolutionary history also indicates that "the costs of ignoring threats have outweighed the costs of ignoring opportunities for self-development."[18] This reinforces the concept that abstract needs for individual and group self-esteem may continue to be selected for by evolution, even when they sometimes confer risks to physical health and well-being.

Self-esteem edit

Self-esteem lies at the heart of TMT and is a fundamental aspect of its core paradigms. TMT fundamentally seeks to elucidate the causes and consequences of a need for self-esteem. Theoretically, it draws heavily from Ernest Becker's conceptions of culture and self-esteem.[19][20] TMT not only attempts to explain the concept of self-esteem, it also tries to explain why we need self-esteem.[21] One explanation is that self-esteem is used as a coping mechanism for anxiety. It helps people control their sense of terror and nullify the realization that humans are just animals trying to manage the world around them. According to TMT, self-esteem is a sense of personal value that is created by beliefs in the validity of one's cultural worldview, and the belief that one is living up to the cultural standards created by that worldview.[21]

Critically, Hewstone et al. (2002) have questioned the causal direction between self-esteem and death anxiety, evaluating whether one's self-esteem comes from their desire to reduce their death anxiety, or if death anxiety arises from a lack of self-esteem.[22] In other words, an individual's suppression of death anxiety may arise from their overall need to increase their self-esteem in a positive manner.[22]

Research has demonstrated that self-esteem can play an important role in physical health. In some cases, people may be so concerned with their physical appearance and boosting their self-esteem that they ignore problems or concerns with their own physical health.[23] Arndt et al. (2009) conducted three studies to examine how peer perceptions and social acceptance of smokers contributes to their quitting, as well as if, and why these people continue smoking for outside reasons, even when faced with thoughts of death and anti-smoking prompts.[23] Tanning and exercising were also looked at in the researchers' studies. The studies found that people are influenced by the situations around them.[23] Specifically, Arndt et al. (2009) found in terms of their self-esteem and health, that participants who saw someone exercising were more likely to increase their intentions to exercise.[23] In addition, the researchers found in study two that how participants reacted to an anti-smoking commercial was affected by their motivation for smoking and the situation which they were in. For instance, people who smoked for extrinsic reasons and were previously prompted with death reminders were more likely to be compelled by the anti-smoking message.[23]

Self-esteem as anxiety buffer edit

An individual's level of self-consciousness can affect their views on life and death. To a point, increasing self-consciousness is adaptive in that it helps prevent awareness of danger. However, research has demonstrated that there may be diminishing returns from this phenomenon.[2] Individuals with higher levels of self-consciousness sometimes have increased death cognition, and a more negative outlook on life, than those with reduced self-consciousness.[24]

Conversely, self-esteem can work in the opposite manner. Research has confirmed that individuals with higher self-esteem, particularly in regard to their behavior, have a more positive attitude towards their life. Specifically, death cognition in the form of anti-smoking warnings weren't effective for smokers and in fact, increased their already positive attitudes towards the behavior.[25] The reasons behind individuals' optimistic attitudes towards smoking after mortality was made salient, indicate that people use positivity as a buffer against anxiety. Continuing to hold certain beliefs even after they are shown to be flawed creates cognitive dissonance regarding current information and past behavior, and the way to alleviate this is to simply reject new information. Therefore, anxiety buffers such as self-esteem allow individuals to cope with their fears more easily. Death cognition may in fact cause negative reinforcement that leads people to further engage in dangerous behaviors (smoking in this instance) because accepting the new information would lead to a loss of self-esteem, increasing vulnerability and awareness of mortality.[25]

Mortality salience edit

The mortality salience hypothesis (MS) states that if indeed one's cultural worldview, or one's self-esteem, serves a death-denying function, then threatening these constructs should produce defenses aimed at restoring psychological equanimity (i.e., returning the individual to a state of feeling invulnerable). In the MS paradigm, these "threats" are simply experiential reminders of one's own death. This can, and has, taken many different forms in a variety of study paradigms (e.g., asking participants to write about their own death;[1] conducting the experiment near funeral homes or cemeteries;[26] having participants watch graphic depictions of death,[27] etc.). Like the other TMT hypotheses, the literature supporting the MS hypothesis is vast and diverse. For a meta analysis of MS research, see Burke et al. (2010).[28]

Experimentally, the MS hypothesis has been tested in close to 200 empirical articles.[28] After participants in an experiment are asked to write about their own death (vs. a neutral, non-death control topic, such as dental pain), and then following a brief delay (distal, worldview/self-esteem defenses work the best after a delay; see Greenberg et al. (1994)[27] for a discussion), the participants' defenses are measured. In one early TMT study assessing the MS hypothesis, Greenberg et al. (1990)[4] had Christian participants evaluate other Christian and Jewish students that were similar demographically, but differed in their religious affiliation. After being reminded of their death (experimental MS induction), Christian participants evaluated fellow Christians more positively, and Jewish participants more negatively, relative to the control condition.[29] Conversely, bolstering self-esteem in these scenarios leads to less worldview defense and derogation of dissimilar others.[29]

Mortality salience has an influence on individuals and their decisions regarding their health. Cox et al. (2009) discuss mortality salience in terms of suntanning. Specifically, the researchers found that participants who were prompted with the idea that pale was more socially attractive along with mortality reminders, tended to lean towards decisions that resulted in more protective measures from the sun.[30] The participants were placed in two different conditions; one group of participants were given an article relating to the fear of death, while the control group received an article unrelated to death, dealing with the fear of public speaking.[30] Additionally, they gave one group an article pertaining to the message that "bronze is beautiful", one relating to the idea that "pale is pretty", and one neutral article that did not speak of tan or pale skin tones.[30] Finally, after introducing a delay activity, the researchers gave the participants a five-item questionnaire asking them about their future sun-tanning behaviors. The study illustrated that when tan skin was associated with attractiveness, mortality salience positively affected people's intentions to suntan; however, when pale skin was associated with attractiveness people's intentions to tan decreased.[30]

Mortality and self-esteem on health risks edit

Studies have shown that mortality and self-esteem are important factors of the terror management theory. Jessop et al. (2008) study this relationship within four studies that all examine how people react when they are given information on risks, specifically, in terms of the mortality related to the risks of driving.[31] More specifically, the researchers were exploring how participants acted in terms of self-esteem, and its impact on how mortality-related health-risk information would be received.[31] Overall, Jessop et al. (2008) found that even when mortality is prominent, people who engage in certain behaviors to improve their self-esteem have a greater chance of continuing with these activities.[31] Mortality and self-esteem are both factors that influence people's behaviors and decision-making regarding their health. Furthermore, individuals who are involved in behaviors and possess motivation to enhance their self-worth are less likely to be affected by the importance placed on health risks, in terms of mortality.[31]

Self-esteem is important when mortality is made salient. It can allow people a coping mechanism, one that can cushion individuals' fears; and thus, impacting one's attitudes towards a given behavior.[25] Individuals who have higher levels of self-esteem regarding their behavior(s) are less likely to have their attitudes, and thus their behaviors changed regardless of mortality salience or death messages.[25] People will use their self-esteem to hide behind their fears of dying. In terms of smoking behaviors, people with higher smoking-based self-esteem are less susceptible to anti-smoking messages that relate to death; therefore, mortality salience and death warnings afford them with an even more positive outlook on their behavior, or in this instance their smoking.[25]

In the Hansen et al. (2010) experiment the researchers manipulated mortality salience. In the experiment, Hansen et al. (2010) examined smokers' attitudes towards the behavior of smoking. Actual warning labels were utilized to create mortality salience in this specific experiment. The researchers first gave participants a questionnaire to measure their smoking-based self-esteem.[25] Following the questionnaire, participants were randomly assigned to two different conditions; the first were given anti-smoking warning labels about death and the second control group were exposed to anti-smoking warning labels not dealing with death.[25] Before the participants' attitudes towards smoking were taken the researchers introduced an unrelated question to provide a delay. Further research has demonstrated that delays allow mortality salience to emerge because thoughts of death become non-conscious.[25] Finally, participants were asked questions regarding their intended future smoking behavior.[25] However, one weakness in their conduction was that the final questionnaire addressed opinions and behavioral questions, as opposed to the participants level of persuasion regarding the different anti-smoking warning labels.

Social influences edit

Many people are more motivated by social pressures, rather than health risks. Specifically for younger people, mortality salience is stronger in eliciting changes of one's behavior when it brings awareness to the immediate loss of social status or position, rather than a loss, such as death that one can not imagine and feels far off.[32] However, there are many different factors to take into consideration, such as how strongly an individual feels toward a decision, his or her level of self-esteem, and the situation around the individual. Particularly with people's smoking behaviors, self-esteem and mortality salience have different effects on individuals' decisions. In terms of the longevity of their smoking decisions, it has been seen that individuals' smoking habits are affected, in the short-term sense, when they are exposed to mortality salience that interrelates with their own self-esteem. Moreover, people who viewed social exclusion prompts were more likely to quit smoking in the long run than those who were simply shown health-effects of smoking.[32] More specifically, it was demonstrated that when individuals had high levels of self-esteem they were more likely to quit smoking following the social pressure messages, rather than the health risk messages.[32] In this specific instance, terror management, and specifically mortality salience is showing how people are more motivated by the social pressures and consequences in their environment, rather than consequences relating to their health. This is mostly seen in young adult smokers with higher smoking-based self-esteems who are not thinking of their future health and the less-immediate effects of smoking on their health.[32]

Death thought accessibility edit

Another paradigm that TMT researchers use to get at unconscious concerns about death is what is known as the death thought accessibility (DTA) hypothesis. Essentially, the DTA hypothesis states that if individuals are motivated to avoid cognitions about death, and they avoid these cognitions by espousing a worldview or by buffering their self-esteem, then when threatened, an individual should possess more death-related cognitions (e.g., thoughts about death, and death-related stimuli) than they would when not threatened.[33]

The DTA hypothesis has its origins in work by Greenberg et al. (1994)[27] as an extension of their earlier terror management hypotheses (i.e., the anxiety buffer hypothesis and the mortality salience hypothesis). The researchers reasoned that if, as indicated by Wegner's research on thought suppression (1994; 1997), thoughts that are purposely suppressed from conscious awareness are often brought back with ease, then following a delay death-thought cognitions should be more available to consciousness than (a) those who keep the death-thoughts in their consciousness the whole time, and (b) those who suppress the death-thoughts but are not provided a delay. That is precisely what they found. However, other psychologists have failed to replicate these findings.[34]

In these initial studies (i.e., Greenberg et al. (2004); Arndt et al. (1997)[35]), and in numerous subsequent DTA studies, the main measure of DTA is a word fragment task, whereby participants can complete word fragments in distinctly death-related ways (e.g., coff_ _ as coffin, not coffee) or in non death-related ways (e.g., sk_ _l as skill, not skull).[36] If death-thoughts are indeed more available to consciousness, then it stands to reason that the word fragments should be completed in a way that is semantically related to death.

Importance of the Death Thought Accessibility hypothesis edit

The introduction of this hypothesis has refined TMT, and led to new avenues of research that formerly could not be assessed due to the lack of an empirically validated way of measuring death-related cognitions. Also, the differentiation between proximal (conscious, near, and threat-focused) and distal (unconscious, distant, symbolic) defenses that have been derived from DTA studies have been extremely important in understanding how people deal with their terror.[37]

It is important to note how the DTA paradigm subtly alters, and expands, TMT as a motivational theory. Instead of solely manipulating mortality and witnessing its effects (e.g., nationalism, increased prejudice, risky sexual behavior, etc.), the DTA paradigm allows a measure of the death-related cognitions that result from various affronts to the self. Examples include threats to self-esteem and to one's worldview; the DTA paradigm can therefore assess the role of death-thoughts in self-esteem and worldview defenses. Furthermore, the DTA hypothesis lends support to TMT in that it corroborates its central hypothesis that death is uniquely problematic for human beings, and that it is fundamentally different in its effects than meaning threats (i.e., Heine et al., 2006[38]) and that is death itself, and not uncertainty and lack of control associated with death; Fritsche et al. (2008) explore this idea.[39]

Since its inception, the DTA hypothesis had been rapidly gaining ground in TMT investigations, and as of 2009, has been employed in over 60 published papers, with a total of more than 90 empirical studies.[33]

Death anxiety on health promotion edit

How people respond to their fears and anxiety of death is investigated in TMT. Moreover, Taubman-Ben-Ari and Noy (2010) examine the idea that a person's level of self-awareness and self-consciousness should be considered in relation to their responses to their anxiety and death cognitions.[24] The more an individual is presented with their death or death cognitions in general, the more fear and anxiety one may have; therefore, to combat said anxiety one may implement anxiety buffers.[24]

Due to a change in people's lifestyles, in the direction of more unhealthy behaviors, the leading causes of death now, being cancer and heart disease, most definitely are related to individuals' unhealthy behaviors (though the statement is over-generalising and certainly cannot be applied to every case).[40] Age and death anxiety both are factors that should be considered in the terror management theory, in relation to health-promoting behaviors. Age undoubtedly plays some kind of role in people's health-promoting behaviors; however, an actual age related effect on death anxiety and health-promoting behaviors has yet to be seen. Although research has demonstrated that for young adults only, when they were prompted with death related scenarios, they yielded more health-promoting behaviors, compared to those participants in their sixties. In addition, death anxiety has been found to have an effect for young adults, on their behaviors of health promotion.[40]

Terror management health model edit

The terror management health model (TMHM) explores the role that death plays on one's health and behavior. Goldenberg and Arndt (2008) state that the TMHM proposes the idea that death, despite its threatening nature, is in fact instrumental and purposeful in the conditioning of one's behavior towards the direction of a longer life.[16]

According to Goldenberg and Arndt (2008), certain health behaviors such as breast self-exams (BSEs) can consciously activate and facilitate people to think of death, especially their own death.[16] While death can be instrumental for individuals, in some cases, when breast self-exams activate people's death thoughts an obstacle can present itself, in terms of health promotion, because of the experience of fear and threat.[16] Abel and Kruger (2009) have suggested that the stress caused by increased awareness of mortality when celebrating one's birthday might explain the birthday effect, where mortality rates seem to spike around these days.[41]

On the other hand, death and thoughts of death can serve as a way of empowering the self, not as threats. Researchers, Cooper et al. (2011) explored TMHM in terms of empowerment, specifically using BSEs under two conditions; when death thoughts were prompted, and when thoughts of death were non-conscious.[36] According to TMHM, people's health decisions, when death thoughts are not conscious, should be based on their motivations to act appropriately, in terms of the self and identity.[36] Cooper et al. (2011) found that when mortality and death thoughts were primed, women reported more empowerment feelings than those who were not prompted before performing a BSE.[36]

Additionally, TMHM suggests that mortality awareness and self-esteem are important factors in individuals' decision making and behaviors relating to their health. TMHM explores how people will engage in behaviors, whether positive or negative, even with the heightened awareness of mortality, in the attempt to conform to society's expectations and improve their self-esteem.[30] The TMHM is useful in understanding what motivates individuals regarding their health decisions and behaviors.

In terms of smoking behaviors and attitudes, the impact of warnings with death messages depends on:

  1. The individuals' level of smoking-based self-esteem
  2. The warnings' actual degree of death information[25]

Emotion edit

People with low self-esteem, but not high self-esteem, have more negative emotions when reminded of death. This is believed to be because these individuals lack the very defenses that TMT argues protect people from mortality concerns (e.g., solid worldviews). In contrast, positive mood states are not impacted by death thoughts for people of low or high self-esteem.[42]

Leadership edit

It has been suggested that culture provides meaning, organization, and a coherent world view that diminishes the psychological terror caused by the knowledge of eventual death. The terror management theory can help to explain why a leader's popularity can grow substantially during times of crisis. When a follower's mortality is made prominent they will tend to show a strong preference for iconic leaders. An example of this occurred when George W. Bush's approval rating jumped almost 50 percent following the September 11 attacks in the United States. As Forsyth (2009) posits, this tragedy made U.S. citizens aware of their mortality, and Bush provided an antidote to these existential concerns by promising to bring justice to the terrorist group responsible for the attacks.

Researchers Cohen et al. (2004), in their particular study on TMT, tested the preferences for different types of leaders, while reminding people of their mortality. Three different candidates were presented to participants. The three leaders were of three different types: task-oriented (emphasized setting goals, strategic planning, and structure), relationship-oriented (emphasized compassion, trust, and confidence in others), and charismatic. The participants were then placed in one of two conditions: mortality salient or control group. In the former condition the participants were asked to describe the emotions surrounding their own death, as well as the physical act of the death itself, whereas the control group were asked similar questions about an upcoming exam. The results of the study were that the charismatic leader was favored more, and the relationship-oriented leader was favored less, in the mortality-salient condition. Further research has shown that mortality salient individuals also prefer leaders who are members of the same group, as well as men rather than women (Hoyt et al. 2010). This has links to social role theory.

Religion edit

TMT posits that religion was created as a means for humans to cope with their own mortality. Supporting this, arguments in favor of life after death, and simply being religious, reduce the effects of mortality salience on worldview defense. Thoughts of death have also been found to increase religious beliefs. At an implicit, subconscious level, this is the case even for people who claim to be nonreligious.[43][44]

Mental health edit

Some researchers have argued that death anxiety may play a central role in numerous mental health conditions.[45] To test whether death anxiety causes a particular mental illness, TMT researchers use a mortality salience experiment, and examine whether reminding participants of death leads to increased prevalence of behaviors associated with that mental illness. Such studies have shown that reminders of death lead to increases in compulsive handwashing in obsessive-compulsive disorder,[46] avoidance in spider phobias and social anxiety,[47] and anxious behaviors in other disorders, including panic disorder and health anxiety,[48] suggesting the role of death anxiety in these conditions according to TMT researchers.

Criticisms edit

Criticisms of terror management theory have been based on several lines of arguments:[49]

  • Suppression of fear and anxiety is implausible from an evolutionary point of view.
  • The observed psychological responses to terrifying cues are better explained by coalitional psychology and theories of collective defense.
  • The responses can be explained as fear of uncertainty and the unknown.
  • The responses can be explained as search for meaning of life and mortality.
  • The experimental results are difficult to replicate.

These arguments are discussed in the following sections.

Evolutionary argument edit

Anxiety and fear are psychological responses that have evolved because they help us avoid danger. A mechanism to suppress anxiety and fear, as postulated by TMT, is unlikely to have evolved because it would reduce the chances of survival.[50][51] It is argued that TMT relies on misguided assumptions about evolved human nature originating from psychoanalytic theory.[50] Proponents of TMT argue that the cultural self-esteem that counters death anxiety is either a spandrel or exaptation created as a byproduct of the human survival instinct being impinged upon by the awareness of death brought about by increased intelligence. It is not responses to immediate danger that are suppressed, but existential reminders of mortality. They posit a "dual defense model" whereby "proximal" and "distal" defenses deal with threats differently, with the former doing so more "pragmatically" due to greater conscious awareness, and the latter more symbolically due to unconscious thought recession. [49][17] Critics argue that the observed responses are not only evoked by cues of essential mortality, but more generally by cues of danger or insecurity.[50]

Coalitional psychology and collective defense as alternative explanations edit

TMT posits that people respond to cues about mortality by strengthening shared worldviews. Critics believe that such a worldview defense is better explained by coalitional psychology. People confronted with danger tend to build shared worldviews and a pro-normative orientation in order to garner social support and to build coalitions and alliances.[50][52] Proponents of TMT argue that the coalitional psychology theory is a black box explanation that 1) cannot account for the fact that virtually all cultures have a supernatural dimension; 2) does not explain why cultural worldview defense is symbolic, involving allegiance to both specific and general systems of abstract meaning unrelated to specific threats, rather than focused on the specific adaptive threats it supposedly evolved to deal with; and, 3) dismisses TMT's dual process account of the underlying processes that generate MS effects without providing an alternative of any kind or attempting to account for the data relevant to this aspect of the TMT analysis. .[49][17] The coalitional theory is supported by a large statistical study finding that conservatism, traditionalism, and other responses represented by TMT theory are connected with collective danger, while individual danger has very different and often opposite effects. The observed connection with collective danger supports the coalitional theory, while contradicting CP's interpretation of TMT, which is understood as explicitly dealing with individual danger only.[53] TMT theorists however, have explained how CP dismisses TMT's dual process shown in lab studies whereby proximal and distal defenses deal with threats differently; with the former doing so more "pragmatically" due to greater conscious awareness, and the latter more symbolically due to unconscious thought recession. This would account for the study's distinction between individual and collective danger — with the former being more proximal and the latter more distal. Unlike TMT, CP does not view national, political and religious coalitions as imagined communities that represent primarily cultural worldviews (distal defenses). Similarly, another study has found that the response of system justification postulated by TMT theorists is increased by salience of terrorism, not by salience of individual mortality.[54] Earlier experimental findings can be explained by the fact that individual danger and collective danger are seriously confounded. The findings that the observed responses are connected with collective danger rather than individual danger was predicted by regality theory. This finding is in agreement with authoritarianism theory, realistic group conflict theory, and Ronald Inglehart's theory of modernization, but not in agreement with CP's interpretation of terror management theory, which omits its distal/proximal dual defense model.[53]

Prevalence of death edit

Since findings on mortality salience and worldview defense were first published, other researchers have claimed that the effects may have been obtained due to reasons other than death itself, such as anxiety, fear, or other aversive stimuli such as pain. The experimental manipulations in TMT research are likely to elicit a mixture of different types of negative emotions, including fear, anxiety, sadness, and anger.[55]

Other studies have found effects similar to those that mortality salience results in – for example, thinking about difficult personal choices to be made, being made to respond to open-ended questions regarding uncertainty, thinking about being robbed, thinking about being socially isolated, and being told that one's life lacks meaning.[49] While these cases exist, thoughts of death have since been compared to various aversive experimental controls, such as (but not limited to) thinking about: failure, writing a critical exam, public speaking with a considerable audience, being excluded, paralysis, dental pain, intense physical pain, etc.[49]

With regards to the studies that found similar effects, TMT theorists have argued that in the previously mentioned studies where death was not the subject thought about, the subjects would quite easily be related to death in an individual's mind due to "linguistic or experiential connection with mortality" (p. 332).[49] For example, being robbed invokes thoughts of violence and being unsafe in one's own home – many people have died trying to protect their property and family. A second possible explanation for these results involves the death-thought accessibility hypothesis: these threats somehow sabotage crucial anxiety-buffering aspects of an individual's worldview or self-esteem, which increases their death thought accessibility. For example, one study found increased death thought accessibility in response to thoughts of antagonistic relations with attachment figures.[49] However, this makes it difficult or impossible to isolate the effect of mortality salience.[55]

While many TMT theorists claim that affective responses to mortality salience are suppressed and pushed out of consciousness, later studies contradict this and show that affective responses are indeed observable.[55]

Meaning maintenance model edit

The meaning maintenance model (MMM) was initially introduced as a comprehensive motivational theory that claimed to subsume TMT, with alternative explanations for TMT findings. Essentially, it posits that people automatically give meaning to things, and when those meanings are somehow disrupted, it causes anxiety.[38] In response, people concentrate on "meaning maintenance to reestablish their sense of symbolic unity" and that such "meaning maintenance often involves the compensatory reaffirmation of alternative meaning structures".[38] These meanings, among other things, should "provide a basis for prediction and control of our...environments, help [one] to cope with tragedy and trauma...and the symbolic cheating of death via adherence to the enduring values that these cultures provide".[38]

While TMT regards the search for meaning as a defense mechanism, meaning management theory regards the quest for meaning as a primary motive because we are meaning-seeking and meaning-making creatures living in a world of meanings. When people are exposed to mortality salience, both TMT and meaning management theory would predict an increase in pro-culture and pro-esteem activities, but for very different reasons. The latter theory is replacing death denial by death acceptance.[56]

TMT theorists argue that meaning management theory cannot describe why different sets of meaning are preferred by different people, and that different types of meaning have different psychological functions.[49] TMT theorists argue that unless something is an important element of a person's anxiety-buffering worldview or self-esteem, it will not require broad meaning maintenance.[49] TMT theorists believe that meaning management theory cannot accurately claim to be an alternative to TMT because it does not seem to be able to explain the current breadth of TMT evidence.[49]

Offensive defensiveness edit

Some theorists have argued that it is not the idea of death and nonexistence that is unsettling to people, but the fact that uncertainty is involved.[57][58] For example, these researchers posited that people defend themselves by altering their fear responses from uncertainty to an enthusiasm approach.[49] Other researchers argue for distinguishing fear of death from fear of dying and, therein, posit that ultimately the fear of death has more to do with some other fear (e.g., fear of pain) or reflects uncertainty avoidance or fear of the unknown.[59]

TMT theorists agree that uncertainty can be disconcerting in some cases and it may even result in defense responses, but note that they believe the inescapability of death and the possibility of its finality regarding one's existence is most unsettling. They also note that people actually seek out some types of uncertainty, and that being uncertain is not always very unpleasant.[49] In contrast, there is substantial evidence that, all things being equal, uncertainty and the unknown represent fundamental fears and are only experienced as pleasant when there is sufficient contextual certainty.[59][60] For example, a surprise involves uncertainty, but is only perceived as pleasant if there is sufficient certainty that the surprise will be pleasant.

Though TMT theorists acknowledge that many responses to mortality salience involve greater approaches (zealousness) towards important worldviews, they also note examples of mortality salience which resulted in the opposite, which offensive defensiveness cannot account for: when negative features of a group to which participants belong were made salient, people actively distanced themselves from that group under mortality salience.[49]

Replication failure edit

In addition to the criticisms from alternative theoretical perspectives, a large-scale attempt by Many Labs 4 to replicate published findings failed to replicate the mortality salience effect on worldview defense under any condition.[61] The test is a multi-lab replication of Study 1 of Greenberg et al. (1994).[27] Psychologists in 21 labs across the U.S. re-executed the original experiment among a total of 2,200 participants. In response to the Many Labs 4 paper, Tom Pyszczynski (one of the founding psychologists of TMT), criticized the study for insufficient sample sizes, failure to follow the advice of researchers, and deviation from a preregistered protocol.[62]

Popularity edit

Psychologist Yoel Inbar summarized the popularity of the theory:

I can not explain to people who were not around during this time - which I would say was roughly 2004 to 2008 - how much everything at the time was about terror management theory. You would go to SPSP and it seemed like half of the posters were about terror management theory. It was just everywhere. There is just an explosion of terror management theory stuff. And then it sort of receded. And now you barely see it. Which is also kind of weird. We were obsessed with this for a period of 3-5 years, then we moved on to other things.[63]

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ a b c Greenberg, J.; Pyszczynski, T.; Solomon, S. (1986). "The causes and consequences of a need for self-esteem: A terror management theory". In R. F. Baumeister (ed.). Public Self and Private Self. New York: Springer-Verlag. pp. 189–212.
  2. ^ a b c Solomon, S.; Greenberg, J.; Pyszczynski, T. (1991). "A terror management theory of social behavior: The psychological functions of self-esteem and cultural worldviews". Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. 24 (93): 159.
  3. ^ Jonas, E.; Fischer, P. (2006). "Terror management and religion: evidence that intrinsic religiousness mitigates worldview defense following mortality salience". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 91 (3): 553–567. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.91.3.553. PMID 16938037. S2CID 45201747.
  4. ^ a b Greenberg, J.; Pyszczynski, T.; Solomon, S.; Rosenblatt, A.; Veeder, M.; Kirkland, S.; Lyon, D. (1990). "Evidence for terror management II: The effects of mortality salience on reactions to those who threaten or bolster the cultural worldview". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 58 (2): 308–318. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.454.2378. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.58.2.308.
  5. ^ Zhou, X.; Liu, J.; Chen, C.; Yu, Z. (2008). "Do children transcend death? An examination of the terror management function of offspring". Scandinavian Journal of Psychology. 49 (5): 413–418. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9450.2008.00665.x. PMID 18489534.
  6. ^ a b Goldenberg, J. L.; Pyszczynski, T.; Greenberg, J.; Solomon, S. (2000). "Fleeing the body: A terror management perspective on the problem of human corporeality". Personality and Social Psychology Review. 4 (3): 200–218. doi:10.1207/s15327957pspr0403_1. S2CID 31331978.
  7. ^ Solomon, S.; Pyszczynski, T.; Greenberg, J. (2015). The Worm at the Core: On the Role of Death in Life. Random House.
  8. ^ "Terror Management Theory – Ernest Becker Foundation". ernestbecker.org. Retrieved 2022-01-21.
  9. ^ Arrowood, Robert B.; Pope, J. Brian (2014). "Terror management theory: A theoretical perspective on origination, maintenance, and research".
  10. ^ Becker, p. ix.
  11. ^ Statius (2004). "Book 3, line 661". In David Roy Shackleton Bailey (ed.). Thebaid, Books 1-7. Loeb classical library, Volume 207. Translated by David Roy Shackleton Bailey. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard University Press. pp. 198–199. ISBN 9780674012080. Retrieved 29 October 2023. Primus in orbe deos fecit timor! [...] Fear first made gods in the world.
  12. ^ Landau, Mark J.; Solomon, Sheldon; Pyszczynski, Tom; Greenberg, Jeff (2007-07-01). "On the Compatibility of Terror Management Theory and Perspectives on Human Evolution". Evolutionary Psychology. 5 (3): 147470490700500. doi:10.1177/147470490700500303. ISSN 1474-7049. S2CID 2548801.
  13. ^ Becker, pp. ix–xiv.
  14. ^ Levitt, Morton (July 1974). "Reviewed work(s): The Denial of Death by Ernest Becker", Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, Vol. 414, USA-USSR: Agenda for Communication, pp. 200–201.
  15. ^ Becker, p. 7.
  16. ^ a b c d Goldenberg, J. L.; Arndt, J. (2008). "The Implications of death for health: A terror management health model for behavioral health promotion". Psychological Review. 115 (4): 1032–1053. doi:10.1037/a0013326. PMID 18954213.
  17. ^ a b c Landau, M. J.; Solomon, S.; Pyszczynski, T.; Greenberg, J. (2007). "On the compatibility of terror management theory and perspectives on human evolution" (PDF). Evolutionary Psychology. 5 (3): 476–519. doi:10.1177/147470490700500303.
  18. ^ Koole, Sander L.; van den Berg, Agnes E. (2004). "Paradise Lost and Reclaimed". In Greenberg, Jeff; Koole, Sander L.; Pyszczynski, Tom (eds.). Handbook Of Experimental Existential Psychology. New York: Guilford Press. p. 91. ISBN 978-1-59385-040-1. Retrieved 2013-08-18.
  19. ^ Becker, Ernest (1971). The birth and death of meaning (2nd ed.). New York: The Free Press.
  20. ^ Becker, Ernest (1973). The Denial of Death (1st ed.). New York: The Free Press.
  21. ^ a b Pyszczynski, T.; Greenberg, J.; Solomon, S.; Arndt, J.; Schimel, J. (2004). "Why do people need self-esteem? A theoretical and empirical review". Psychological Bulletin. 130 (130): 435–468. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.130.3.435. PMID 15122930. S2CID 1780005.
  22. ^ a b Hewstone, M.; Rubin, M.; Willis, H. (2002). "Intergroup bias". Annual Review of Psychology. 53: 575–604. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135109. PMID 11752497. S2CID 11830211.
  23. ^ a b c d e Arndt, J.; Cox, C. R.; Goldenberg, J. L.; Vess, M.; Routledge, C.; Cooper, D. P.; Cohen, F. (2009). "Blowing in the (social) wind: Implications of extrinsic esteem contingencies for terror management and health". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 96 (6): 1191–1205. doi:10.1037/a0015182. PMID 19469596.
  24. ^ a b c Taubman-Ben-Ari, O.; Noy, A. (2010). "Self-consciousness and death cognitions from a terror management perspective". Death Studies. 34 (10): 871–892. doi:10.1080/07481187.2010.496685. PMID 24482853. S2CID 24026354.
  25. ^ a b c d e f g h i j Hansen, J.; Winzeler, S.; Topolinski, S. (2010). "When the death makes you smoke: A terror management perspective on the effectiveness of cigarette on-pack warnings". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 46 (1): 226–228. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2009.09.007.
  26. ^ Pyszczynski, T.; Wicklund, R. A.; Floresku, S.; Koch, H.; Gauch, G.; Solomon, S.; Greenberg, J. (1996). "Whistling in the dark: Exaggerated consensus estimates in response to incidental reminders of mortality". Psychological Science. 7 (6): 332–336. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1996.tb00384.x. S2CID 12709488.
  27. ^ a b c d Greenberg, J.; Pyszczynski, T.; Solomon, S.; Simon, L.; Breus, M. (1994). "Role of consciousness and accessibility of death-related thoughts in mortality salience effects". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 67 (4): 627–637. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.67.4.627. PMID 7965609. S2CID 37679370.
  28. ^ a b Burke, B. L.; Martens, A.; Faucher, E. H. (2010). "Two decades of terror management theory: A meta-analysis of mortality salience research". Personality and Social Psychology Review. 14 (2): 155–195. doi:10.1177/1088868309352321. PMID 20097885. S2CID 206682555.
  29. ^ a b Harmon-Jones, E.; Simon, L.; Greenberg, J.; Pyszczynski, T.; Solomon, S.; McGregor, H. (1997). "Terror management theory and self-esteem: Evidence that increased self-esteem reduces mortality salience effects". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 72 (1): 24–36. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.72.1.24. PMID 9008372. S2CID 32261410.
  30. ^ a b c d e Cox, C. R.; Cooper, D. P.; Vess, M.; Arndt, J.; Goldenberg, J. L.; Routledge, C. (2009). "Bronze is beautiful but pale can be pretty: The effects of appearance standards and mortality salience on sun-tanning outcomes". Health Psychology. 28 (6): 746–752. doi:10.1037/a0016388. PMID 19916643.
  31. ^ a b c d Jessop, D. C.; Albery, I. P.; Rutter, J.; Garrod, H. (2008). "Understanding the impact of mortality-related health-risk information: A terror management theory perspective". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 34 (7): 951–964. doi:10.1177/0146167208316790. PMID 18453389. S2CID 10585949.
  32. ^ a b c d Martin, I. M.; Kamins, M. A. (2010). "An application of terror management theory in the design of social and health-related anti-smoking appeals". Journal of Consumer Behaviour. 9 (3): 172–190. doi:10.1002/cb.293.
  33. ^ a b Hayes, J.; Schimel, J.; Ardnt, J.; Faucher, E. (2010). "A Theoretical and Empirical Review of the Death Thought Accessibility Concept in Terror Management Research". Psychological Bulletin. 136 (5): 699–739. doi:10.1037/a0020524. PMID 20804234.
  34. ^ Trafimow, David; Jamie S. Hughes (September 2012). "Testing the Death Thought Suppression and Rebound Hypothesis". Social Psychological and Personality Science. 3 (5): 622–629. doi:10.1177/1948550611432938. S2CID 146202248.
  35. ^ Arndt, J.; Greenberg, J.; Pyszczynski, T.; Solomon, S. (1997). "Subliminal exposure to death-related stimuli increases defense of the cultural worldview". Psychological Science. 8 (5): 379–385. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1997.tb00429.x. S2CID 145101319.
  36. ^ a b c d Cooper, D. P.; Goldenberg, J. L.; Arndt, J. (2011). "Empowering the self: Using the terror management health model to promote breast self-examination". Self and Identity. 10 (3): 315–325. doi:10.1080/15298868.2010.527495. S2CID 140520175.
  37. ^ Pyszczynski, T.; Greenberg, J.; Solomon, S. (1999). "A dual-process model of defense against conscious and unconscious death-related thoughts: An extension of terror management theory". Psychological Review. 106 (4): 835–845. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.106.4.835. PMID 10560330. S2CID 2655060.
  38. ^ a b c d Heine, S. J.; Proulx, T.; Vohs, K. D. (2006). "The meaning maintenance model: On the coherence of human motivations". Personality and Social Psychology Review. 10 (2): 88–110. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.434.4237. doi:10.1207/s15327957pspr1002_1. PMID 16768649. S2CID 899167.
  39. ^ Fritsche, I.; Jonas, E.; Fankhänel, T. (2008). "The role of control motivation in mortality salience effects on ingroup support and defense". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 95 (3): 524–541. doi:10.1037/a0012666. PMID 18729692.
  40. ^ a b Bozo, Ö.; Tunca, A.; Şİmşek, Y. (2009). "The effect of death anxiety and age on health- promoting behaviors: A terror-management theory perspective". Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied. 143 (4): 377–389. doi:10.3200/JRLP.143.4.377-389. PMID 19606644. S2CID 9927722.
  41. ^ Abel, Ernest; Kruger, Michael (2009). "Mortality Salience of Birthdays on Day of Death in the Major Leagues". Death Studies. 33 (2): 175–184. doi:10.1080/07481180802138936. PMID 19143110. S2CID 8439436.
  42. ^ Routledge, C.; Ostafin, B.; Juhl, J.; Sedikides, C.; Cathey, C.; Liao, J. (2010). "The effects of self-esteem and mortality salience on well-being, growth motivation, and maladaptive behavior". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 99 (6): 897–916. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.465.8476. doi:10.1037/a0021431. PMID 21114350.
  43. ^ Heflick, Nathan; Goldenberg, J. (2012). "No atheists in foxholes: Arguments for (but not against) afterlives buffer mortality salience effects for atheists". British Journal of Social Psychology. 51 (2): 385–392. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8309.2011.02058.x. PMID 21995319.
  44. ^ Jong, Jonathan; Halberstadt, Jamin; Bluemke, Matthias (2012). "Foxhole atheism, revisited: The effects of mortality salience on explicit and implicit religious belief". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 48 (5): 983–989. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2012.03.005.
  45. ^ Iverach, Lisa; Menzies, Ross G.; Menzies, Rachel E. (2014). "Death anxiety and its role in psychopathology: reviewing the status of a transdiagnostic construct". Clinical Psychology Review. 34 (7): 580–593. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2014.09.002. ISSN 1873-7811. PMID 25306232. S2CID 11017493.
  46. ^ Menzies, R.E.; Dar-Nimrod, I. (2017). "Death anxiety and its relationship with obsessive-compulsive disorder". Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 126 (4): 367–377. doi:10.1037/abn0000263. PMID 28277734. S2CID 3823579.
  47. ^ Strachan, Eric; Schimel, Jeff; Arndt, Jamie; Williams, Todd; Solomon, Sheldon; Pyszczynski, Tom; Greenberg, Jeff (2007-08-01). "Terror Mismanagement: Evidence That Mortality Salience Exacerbates Phobic and Compulsive Behaviors". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 33 (8): 1137–1151. doi:10.1177/0146167207303018. ISSN 0146-1672. PMID 17545415. S2CID 21364203.
  48. ^ Menzies, R.E.; Sharpe, L.; Dar-Nimrod, I. (2021). "The effect of mortality salience on bodily scanning behaviors in anxiety-related disorders". Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 130 (2): 141–151. doi:10.1037/abn0000577. PMID 33301338. S2CID 228100285.
  49. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l m Pyszczynski, T.; Greenberg, J.; Solomon, S.; Maxfield, M. (2006). "On the unique psychological import of the human awareness of mortality: Theme and variations". Psychological Inquiry. 17 (4): 328–356. doi:10.1080/10478400701369542. S2CID 143508018.
  50. ^ a b c d Kirkpatrick, Lee; Carlos David Navarrete (2006). "Reports of My Death Anxiety Have Been Greatly Exaggerated: A Critique of Terror Management Theory from an Evolutionary Perspective". Psychological Inquiry. 17 (4): 288–298. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.574.4267. doi:10.1080/10478400701366969. S2CID 144262438.
  51. ^ Buss, David (1997). "Human Social Motivation in Evolutionary Perspective: Grounding Terror Management Theory". Psychological Inquiry. 8 (1): 22–26. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.387.7436. doi:10.1207/s15327965pli0801_3.
  52. ^ Navarrete, D.C.; Fessler, D.M.T. (2005). "Normative bias and adaptive challenges: A relational approach to coalitional psychology and a critique of terror management theory". Evolutionary Psychology. 3: 297–325. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.132.5201. doi:10.1177/147470490500300121. S2CID 14311881.
  53. ^ a b Fog, Agner (2023). "Psychological and cultural effects of different kinds of danger. An exploration based on survey data from 79 countries". Culture and Evolution. doi:10.1556/2055.2023.00029. S2CID 257998766.
  54. ^ Ullrich, Johannes; Cohrs, J. Christopher (2007). "Terrorism salience increases system justification: Experimental evidence". Social Justice Research. 20 (2): 117–139. doi:10.1007/s11211-007-0035-y. S2CID 145734264.
  55. ^ a b c Lambert, A. J.; et al. (2014). "Toward a greater understanding of the emotional dynamics of the mortality salience manipulation: Revisiting the "affect-free" claim of terror management research". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 106 (5): 655–678. doi:10.1037/a0036353. PMID 24749817.
  56. ^ Wong, Paul T. P.; Adrian Tomer (2011). "Beyond Terror and Denial: The Positive Psychology of Death Acceptance". Death Studies. 35 (2): 99–106. doi:10.1080/07481187.2011.535377. PMID 24501830. S2CID 1067025.
  57. ^ McGregor, I.; Zanna, M. P.; Holmes, J. G.; Spencer, S. J. (2001). "Compensatory conviction in the face of personal uncertainty: Going to extremes and being oneself". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 80 (3): 472–488. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.80.3.472. PMID 11300580. S2CID 9025151.
  58. ^ McGregor, I (2006). "Offensive defensiveness: Toward an integrative neuroscience of compensatory zeal after mortality salience, personal uncertainty, and other poignant self-threats". Psychological Inquiry. 17 (4): 299–308. doi:10.1080/10478400701366977.
  59. ^ a b Carleton, R. Nicholas (June 2016). "Fear of the unknown: One fear to rule them all?". Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 41: 5–21. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2016.03.011. PMID 27067453.
  60. ^ Carleton, R. Nicholas (April 2016). "Into the unknown: A review and synthesis of contemporary models involving uncertainty". Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 39: 30–43. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2016.02.007. PMID 26945765.
  61. ^ [1]Klein, Richard A.; Cook, Corey L.; Ebersole, Charles R.; Vitiello, Christine; Nosek, Brian A.; Ahn, Paul; Brady, Abbie J.; Chartier, Christopher R.; Christopherson, Cody D.; Clay, Samuel (2017-01-12), Many Labs 4: Replicating Mortality Salience with and without Original Author Involvement
  62. ^ Chatard, Armand; Hirschberger, Gilad; Pyszczynski, Tom (2020-02-07), A Word of Caution about Many Labs 4: If You Fail to Follow Your Preregistered Plan, You May Fail to Find a Real Effect (unpublished), Center for Open Science, doi:10.31234/osf.io/ejubn, S2CID 236806340
  63. ^ Tullett, Alexa; Inbar, Yoel (15 June 2022). "Episode 88: Many Many Labs". Two Psychologists Four Beers (Podcast). Publisher. Event occurs at 49:30 - 50:20. Retrieved 10 June 2022.

Bibliography edit

  • Becker, Ernest (1973). The Denial of Death, The Free Press. ISBN 0-02-902380-7
  • Pyszczynski, Thomas; Solomon, Sheldon; Greenberg, Jeff (2003). In the Wake of 9/11: The Psychology of Terror, American Psychological Association. ISBN 1-55798-954-0
  • Solomon, Sheldon, Greenberg, J. & Pyszczynski, T. (1991) "A terror management theory of social behavior: The psychological functions of esteem and cultural worldviews", in M. P. Zanna (Ed.) Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Volume 24, Academic Press, pp. 93–159. ISBN 0-12-015224-X

Further reading edit

  • Curtis, V.; Biran, A. (2001). "Dirt, disgust, and disease: Is hygiene in our genes?". Perspectives in Biology and Medicine. 44 (1): 17–31. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.324.760. doi:10.1353/pbm.2001.0001. PMID 11253302. S2CID 15675303.
  • Darwin, C. (1998) [1872]. The expression of the emotions in man and animals (3rd ed.). London: Harper Collins.
  • Florian, V.; Mikulincer, M. (1997). "Fear of death and the judgment of social transgressions: a multidimensional test of terror". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 73 (2): 369–80. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.73.2.369. ISSN 0022-3514. PMID 9248054.
  • Goldenberg, J.L.; Pyszczynski, T.; Greenberg, J.; Solomon, S.; Kluck, B.; Cornwell, R. (2001). "I am not an animal: Mortality salience, disgust, and the denial of human creatureliness". Journal of Experimental Psychology. 130 (3): 427–435. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.130.3.427. PMID 11561918.
  • Greenberg, J.; Pyszczynski, T.; Solomon, S.; Rosenblatt, A.; Veeder, M.; Kirkland, S. (1990). "Evidence for terror management theory. II: The effects of mortality salience on reactions to those who threaten or bolster the cultural worldview" (Fee required). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 58 (2): 308–318. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.454.2378. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.58.2.308. ISSN 0022-3514. 13817, 35400000600727.0100 (INIST-CNRS). Retrieved 2007-07-27.
  • Greenberg, J.; Solomon, S.; Pyszczynski, T. (1997). "Terror management theory of self-esteem and cultural worldviews: Empirical assessments and conceptual refinements". Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. 29 (S 61): 139. doi:10.1016/s0065-2601(08)60016-7.
  • Hansen, J; Winzeler, S; Topolinski, S (2010). "When death makes you smoke: a terror management perspective on the effectiveness of cigarette on-pack warnings". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 46: 226–228. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2009.09.007.
  • Hirschberger, G.; Florian, V.; Mikulincer, M. (2003). "Striving for romantic intimacy following partner complaint or partner criticism: A terror management perspective". Journal of Social and Personal Relationships. 20 (5): 675–687. doi:10.1177/02654075030205006. S2CID 144657212.
  • Judis, J.B. (August 27, 2007). "Death grip: How political psychology explains Bush's ghastly success". New Republic.
  • Lazarus, R.S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. New York: Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-506994-5.
  • Mikulincer, M.; Florian, V.; Hirschberger, G. (2003). "The existential function of close relationships: Introducing death into the science of love". Personality and Social Psychology Review. 7 (1): 20–40. doi:10.1207/S15327957PSPR0701_2. PMID 12584055. S2CID 11600574.
  • Pyszczynski, T.; Greenberg, J.; Solomon, S. (1997). "Why do we need what we need? A terror management perspective on the roots of human social motivation". Psychological Inquiry. 8 (1): 1–20. doi:10.1207/s15327965pli0801_1.
  • Pyszczynski, T.; Greenberg, J.; Solomon, S. (1999). "A dual process model of defense against conscious and unconscious death-related thoughts: An extension of terror management theory". Psychological Review. 106 (4): 835–845. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.106.4.835. PMID 10560330. S2CID 2655060.
  • Rosenblatt, A.; Greenberg, J.; Solomon, S.; Pyszczynski, T.; Lyon, D. (1989). "Evidence for terror management theory: I. The effects of mortality salience on reactions to those who violate or uphold cultural values". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 57 (4): 681–90. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.457.5862. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.57.4.681. ISSN 0022-3514. PMID 2795438.
  • Royzman, E.B.; Sabini, J. (2001). "Something it takes to be an emotion: The interesting case of disgust". Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour. 31 (1): 29–59. doi:10.1111/1468-5914.00145.
  • Shehryar, O.; Hunt, D.M. (2005). "A terror management perspective on the persuasiveness of fear appeals". Journal of Consumer Psychology. 15 (4): 275–287. doi:10.1207/s15327663jcp1504_2. S2CID 18866874.
  • Simon, L.; Arndt, J.; Greenberg, J.; Pyszczynski, T.; Solomon, S. (1998). "Terror management and meaning: Evidence that the opportunity to defend the worldview in response to mortality salience increases the meaningfulness of life in the mildly depressed". Journal of Personality. 66 (3359–382): 359–382. doi:10.1111/1467-6494.00016. hdl:10150/187250. PMID 9615422.
  • Simon, L.; Greenberg, J.; Harmon-Jones, E.; Solomon, S.; Pyszczynski, T.; Arndt, J.; Abend, T. (1997). "Terror management and Cognitive-Experiential Self-Theory: Evidence that terror management occurs in the experiential system". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 72 (5): 1132–1146. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.72.5.1132. PMID 9150588.
  • Greenberg, J.; Koole, S. L.; Pyszczynski, T. (2004). Handbook of experimental existential psychology. Guilford Press. ISBN 978-1-59385-040-1.
  • Cohen, Florette; Solomon, Sheldon; Maxfield, Molly; Pyszczynski, Tom; Greenberg, Jeff (2004). "Fatal Attraction". Psychological Science. 15 (12). SAGE Publications: 846–851. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00765.x. ISSN 0956-7976. PMID 15563330. S2CID 16787928.
  • Van Tilburg, W. A. P.; Igou, E. R (2011). "On the meaningfulness of existence: When life salience boosts adherence to worldviews". European Journal of Social Psychology (Submitted manuscript). 41 (6): 740–750. doi:10.1002/ejsp.819. hdl:10344/5416. S2CID 142993102.
  • Gutierrez, C. (2006). "Consumer attraction to luxury brand products: Social affiliation in terror management theory".

Discusses TMT at length

TMT and self-esteem

  • Schmeichel, B.J.; Gailliot, M.T.; Filardo, E.A.; McGregor, I.; Gitter, S.; Baumeister, R.F. (2009). "Terror management theory and self esteem revisited: The roles of implicit and explicit self-esteem in mortality salience effects". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 96 (5): 1077–1087. doi:10.1037/a0015091. PMID 19379037. S2CID 13740871.

External links edit

Listen to this article (35 minutes)
 
This audio file was created from a revision of this article dated 23 November 2019 (2019-11-23), and does not reflect subsequent edits.

terror, management, theory, this, article, needs, additional, citations, verification, please, help, improve, this, article, adding, citations, reliable, sources, unsourced, material, challenged, removed, find, sources, news, newspapers, books, scholar, jstor,. This article needs additional citations for verification Please help improve this article by adding citations to reliable sources Unsourced material may be challenged and removed Find sources Terror management theory news newspapers books scholar JSTOR March 2023 Learn how and when to remove this message Terror management theory TMT is both a social and evolutionary psychology theory originally proposed by Jeff Greenberg Sheldon Solomon and Tom Pyszczynski 1 and codified in their book The Worm at the Core On the Role of Death in Life 2015 It proposes that a basic psychological conflict results from having a self preservation instinct while realizing that death is inevitable and to some extent unpredictable This conflict produces terror which is managed through a combination of escapism and cultural beliefs that act to counter biological reality with more significant and enduring forms of meaning and value basically countering the personal insignificance represented by death with the significance provided by symbolic culture 1 2 The most obvious examples of cultural values that assuage death anxiety are those that purport to offer literal immortality e g belief in the afterlife through religion 3 However TMT also argues that other cultural values including those that are seemingly unrelated to death offer symbolic immortality For example values of national identity 4 posterity 5 cultural perspectives on sex 6 and human superiority over animals 6 have been linked to calming death concerns In many cases these values are thought to offer symbolic immortality by either a providing the sense that one is part of something greater that will ultimately outlive the individual e g country lineage species or b making one s symbolic identity superior to biological nature i e you are a personality which makes you more than a glob of cells 7 Because cultural values influence what is meaningful they are foundational for self esteem TMT describes self esteem as being the personal subjective measure of how well an individual is living up to their cultural values 2 Terror management theory was developed by social psychologists Greenberg Solomon and Pyszczynski However the idea of TMT originated from anthropologist Ernest Becker s 1973 Pulitzer Prize winning work of nonfiction The Denial of Death Becker argues most human action is taken to ignore or avoid the inevitability of death 8 The terror of absolute annihilation creates such a profound albeit subconscious anxiety in people that they spend their lives attempting to make sense of it On large scales societies build symbols Laws religious meanings cultures and belief systems to explain the significance of life define what makes certain characteristics skills and talents extraordinary reward others whom they find exemplify certain attributes and punish or kill others who do not adhere to their cultural worldview Adherence to these created symbols aid in relieving stresses associated with the reality of mortality 9 On an individual level self esteem provides a buffer against death related anxiety Contents 1 Background 1 1 Evolutionary backdrop 2 Self esteem 2 1 Self esteem as anxiety buffer 3 Mortality salience 3 1 Mortality and self esteem on health risks 3 2 Social influences 4 Death thought accessibility 4 1 Importance of the Death Thought Accessibility hypothesis 4 2 Death anxiety on health promotion 5 Terror management health model 6 Emotion 7 Leadership 8 Religion 9 Mental health 10 Criticisms 10 1 Evolutionary argument 10 2 Coalitional psychology and collective defense as alternative explanations 10 3 Prevalence of death 10 4 Meaning maintenance model 10 5 Offensive defensiveness 10 6 Replication failure 11 Popularity 12 See also 13 References 14 Bibliography 15 Further reading 16 External linksBackground editThe idea of death the fear of it haunts the human animal like nothing else it is a mainspring of human activity designed largely to avoid the fatality of death to overcome it by denying in some way that it is the final destiny for man Ernest Becker 1973 10 In the 1st century CE Statius in his Thebaid suggested that fear first made gods in the world 11 Cultural anthropologist Ernest Becker asserted in his 1973 book The Denial of Death that humans as intelligent animals are able to grasp the inevitability of death They therefore spend their lives building and believing in cultural elements that illustrate how to make themselves stand out as individuals and to give their lives significance and meaning 12 Death creates an anxiety in humans it strikes at unexpected and random moments and its nature is essentially unknowable causing people to spend most of their time and energy to explain forestall and avoid it 13 Becker expounded upon the previous writings of Sigmund Freud Soren Kierkegaard Norman O Brown and Otto Rank According to clinical psychiatrist Morton Levitt Becker replaces the Freudian preoccupation with sexuality with the fear of death as the primary motivation in human behavior 14 People desire to think of themselves as beings of value and worth with a feeling of permanence a concept in psychology known as self esteem This feeling counters the cognitive dissonance created by an individual s realization that they may be no more important than any other living thing Becker refers to high self esteem as heroism the problem of heroics is the central one of human life that it goes deeper into human nature than anything else because it is based on organismic narcissism and on the child s need for self esteem as the condition for his life Society itself is a codified hero system which means that society everywhere is a living myth of the significance of human life a defiant creation of meaning 15 The rationale behind decisions regarding one s own health can be explored through a terror management model A 2008 research article in Psychological Review proposes a three part model for understanding how awareness of death can ironically subvert health promoting behaviors by redirecting one s focus towards behaviors that build self esteem instead Proposition 1 suggests that conscious thoughts about death can instigate health oriented responses aimed at removing death related thoughts from current focal attention Proposition 2 suggests that the unconscious resonance of death related cognition promotes self oriented defenses directed toward maintaining not one s health but a sense of meaning and self esteem The last proposition suggests that confrontations with the physical body may undermine symbolic defenses and thus present a previously unrecognized barrier to health promotion activities 16 Evolutionary backdrop edit Terror management theorists regard TMT as compatible with the theory of evolution 17 Valid fears of dangerous things have an adaptive function that helped facilitate the survival of our ancestors genes However generalized existential anxiety resulting from the clash between a desire for life and awareness of the inevitability of death is neither adaptive nor selected for TMT views existential anxiety as an unfortunate byproduct of these two highly adaptive human proclivities rather than as an adaptation that the evolutionary process selected for its advantages Just as human bipedalism confers advantages as well as disadvantages death anxiety is an inevitable part of our intelligence and awareness of dangers Anxiety in response to the inevitability of death threatened to undermine adaptive functioning and therefore needed amelioration TMT posits that humankind used the same intellectual capacities that gave rise to this problem to fashion cultural beliefs and values that provided protection against this potential anxiety TMT considers these cultural beliefs even unpleasant and frightening ones such as ritual human sacrifice when they manage potential death anxiety in a way that promotes beliefs and behaviors which facilitated the functioning and survival of the collective Hunter gatherers used their emerging cognitive abilities to facilitate solving practical problems such as basic needs for nutrition mating and tool making As these abilities evolved an explicit awareness of death also emerged But once this awareness materialized the potential for terror that it created put pressure on emerging conceptions of reality Any conceptual formation that was to be widely accepted by the group needed to provide a means of managing this terror Originally morality evolved to facilitate co existence within groups Together with language morality served pragmatic functions that extended survival The struggle to deny the finality of death co opted and changed the function of these cultural inventions For example Neanderthals might have begun burying their dead as a means of avoiding unpleasant odors disease infested parasites or dangerous scavengers But during the Upper Paleolithic era these pragmatic burial practices appear to have become imbued with layers of ritual performance and supernatural beliefs suggested by the elaborate decoration of bodies with thousands of beads or other markers Food and other necessities were also included within the burial chamber indicating the potential for a belief system that included life after death Many human cultures today treat funerals primarily as cultural events viewed through the lens of morality and language with little thought given to the utilitarian origins of burying the dead Evolutionary history also indicates that the costs of ignoring threats have outweighed the costs of ignoring opportunities for self development 18 This reinforces the concept that abstract needs for individual and group self esteem may continue to be selected for by evolution even when they sometimes confer risks to physical health and well being Self esteem editSelf esteem lies at the heart of TMT and is a fundamental aspect of its core paradigms TMT fundamentally seeks to elucidate the causes and consequences of a need for self esteem Theoretically it draws heavily from Ernest Becker s conceptions of culture and self esteem 19 20 TMT not only attempts to explain the concept of self esteem it also tries to explain why we need self esteem 21 One explanation is that self esteem is used as a coping mechanism for anxiety It helps people control their sense of terror and nullify the realization that humans are just animals trying to manage the world around them According to TMT self esteem is a sense of personal value that is created by beliefs in the validity of one s cultural worldview and the belief that one is living up to the cultural standards created by that worldview 21 Critically Hewstone et al 2002 have questioned the causal direction between self esteem and death anxiety evaluating whether one s self esteem comes from their desire to reduce their death anxiety or if death anxiety arises from a lack of self esteem 22 In other words an individual s suppression of death anxiety may arise from their overall need to increase their self esteem in a positive manner 22 Research has demonstrated that self esteem can play an important role in physical health In some cases people may be so concerned with their physical appearance and boosting their self esteem that they ignore problems or concerns with their own physical health 23 Arndt et al 2009 conducted three studies to examine how peer perceptions and social acceptance of smokers contributes to their quitting as well as if and why these people continue smoking for outside reasons even when faced with thoughts of death and anti smoking prompts 23 Tanning and exercising were also looked at in the researchers studies The studies found that people are influenced by the situations around them 23 Specifically Arndt et al 2009 found in terms of their self esteem and health that participants who saw someone exercising were more likely to increase their intentions to exercise 23 In addition the researchers found in study two that how participants reacted to an anti smoking commercial was affected by their motivation for smoking and the situation which they were in For instance people who smoked for extrinsic reasons and were previously prompted with death reminders were more likely to be compelled by the anti smoking message 23 Self esteem as anxiety buffer edit An individual s level of self consciousness can affect their views on life and death To a point increasing self consciousness is adaptive in that it helps prevent awareness of danger However research has demonstrated that there may be diminishing returns from this phenomenon 2 Individuals with higher levels of self consciousness sometimes have increased death cognition and a more negative outlook on life than those with reduced self consciousness 24 Conversely self esteem can work in the opposite manner Research has confirmed that individuals with higher self esteem particularly in regard to their behavior have a more positive attitude towards their life Specifically death cognition in the form of anti smoking warnings weren t effective for smokers and in fact increased their already positive attitudes towards the behavior 25 The reasons behind individuals optimistic attitudes towards smoking after mortality was made salient indicate that people use positivity as a buffer against anxiety Continuing to hold certain beliefs even after they are shown to be flawed creates cognitive dissonance regarding current information and past behavior and the way to alleviate this is to simply reject new information Therefore anxiety buffers such as self esteem allow individuals to cope with their fears more easily Death cognition may in fact cause negative reinforcement that leads people to further engage in dangerous behaviors smoking in this instance because accepting the new information would lead to a loss of self esteem increasing vulnerability and awareness of mortality 25 Mortality salience editMain article mortality salience The mortality salience hypothesis MS states that if indeed one s cultural worldview or one s self esteem serves a death denying function then threatening these constructs should produce defenses aimed at restoring psychological equanimity i e returning the individual to a state of feeling invulnerable In the MS paradigm these threats are simply experiential reminders of one s own death This can and has taken many different forms in a variety of study paradigms e g asking participants to write about their own death 1 conducting the experiment near funeral homes or cemeteries 26 having participants watch graphic depictions of death 27 etc Like the other TMT hypotheses the literature supporting the MS hypothesis is vast and diverse For a meta analysis of MS research see Burke et al 2010 28 Experimentally the MS hypothesis has been tested in close to 200 empirical articles 28 After participants in an experiment are asked to write about their own death vs a neutral non death control topic such as dental pain and then following a brief delay distal worldview self esteem defenses work the best after a delay see Greenberg et al 1994 27 for a discussion the participants defenses are measured In one early TMT study assessing the MS hypothesis Greenberg et al 1990 4 had Christian participants evaluate other Christian and Jewish students that were similar demographically but differed in their religious affiliation After being reminded of their death experimental MS induction Christian participants evaluated fellow Christians more positively and Jewish participants more negatively relative to the control condition 29 Conversely bolstering self esteem in these scenarios leads to less worldview defense and derogation of dissimilar others 29 Mortality salience has an influence on individuals and their decisions regarding their health Cox et al 2009 discuss mortality salience in terms of suntanning Specifically the researchers found that participants who were prompted with the idea that pale was more socially attractive along with mortality reminders tended to lean towards decisions that resulted in more protective measures from the sun 30 The participants were placed in two different conditions one group of participants were given an article relating to the fear of death while the control group received an article unrelated to death dealing with the fear of public speaking 30 Additionally they gave one group an article pertaining to the message that bronze is beautiful one relating to the idea that pale is pretty and one neutral article that did not speak of tan or pale skin tones 30 Finally after introducing a delay activity the researchers gave the participants a five item questionnaire asking them about their future sun tanning behaviors The study illustrated that when tan skin was associated with attractiveness mortality salience positively affected people s intentions to suntan however when pale skin was associated with attractiveness people s intentions to tan decreased 30 Mortality and self esteem on health risks edit Studies have shown that mortality and self esteem are important factors of the terror management theory Jessop et al 2008 study this relationship within four studies that all examine how people react when they are given information on risks specifically in terms of the mortality related to the risks of driving 31 More specifically the researchers were exploring how participants acted in terms of self esteem and its impact on how mortality related health risk information would be received 31 Overall Jessop et al 2008 found that even when mortality is prominent people who engage in certain behaviors to improve their self esteem have a greater chance of continuing with these activities 31 Mortality and self esteem are both factors that influence people s behaviors and decision making regarding their health Furthermore individuals who are involved in behaviors and possess motivation to enhance their self worth are less likely to be affected by the importance placed on health risks in terms of mortality 31 Self esteem is important when mortality is made salient It can allow people a coping mechanism one that can cushion individuals fears and thus impacting one s attitudes towards a given behavior 25 Individuals who have higher levels of self esteem regarding their behavior s are less likely to have their attitudes and thus their behaviors changed regardless of mortality salience or death messages 25 People will use their self esteem to hide behind their fears of dying In terms of smoking behaviors people with higher smoking based self esteem are less susceptible to anti smoking messages that relate to death therefore mortality salience and death warnings afford them with an even more positive outlook on their behavior or in this instance their smoking 25 In the Hansen et al 2010 experiment the researchers manipulated mortality salience In the experiment Hansen et al 2010 examined smokers attitudes towards the behavior of smoking Actual warning labels were utilized to create mortality salience in this specific experiment The researchers first gave participants a questionnaire to measure their smoking based self esteem 25 Following the questionnaire participants were randomly assigned to two different conditions the first were given anti smoking warning labels about death and the second control group were exposed to anti smoking warning labels not dealing with death 25 Before the participants attitudes towards smoking were taken the researchers introduced an unrelated question to provide a delay Further research has demonstrated that delays allow mortality salience to emerge because thoughts of death become non conscious 25 Finally participants were asked questions regarding their intended future smoking behavior 25 However one weakness in their conduction was that the final questionnaire addressed opinions and behavioral questions as opposed to the participants level of persuasion regarding the different anti smoking warning labels Social influences edit Many people are more motivated by social pressures rather than health risks Specifically for younger people mortality salience is stronger in eliciting changes of one s behavior when it brings awareness to the immediate loss of social status or position rather than a loss such as death that one can not imagine and feels far off 32 However there are many different factors to take into consideration such as how strongly an individual feels toward a decision his or her level of self esteem and the situation around the individual Particularly with people s smoking behaviors self esteem and mortality salience have different effects on individuals decisions In terms of the longevity of their smoking decisions it has been seen that individuals smoking habits are affected in the short term sense when they are exposed to mortality salience that interrelates with their own self esteem Moreover people who viewed social exclusion prompts were more likely to quit smoking in the long run than those who were simply shown health effects of smoking 32 More specifically it was demonstrated that when individuals had high levels of self esteem they were more likely to quit smoking following the social pressure messages rather than the health risk messages 32 In this specific instance terror management and specifically mortality salience is showing how people are more motivated by the social pressures and consequences in their environment rather than consequences relating to their health This is mostly seen in young adult smokers with higher smoking based self esteems who are not thinking of their future health and the less immediate effects of smoking on their health 32 Death thought accessibility editAnother paradigm that TMT researchers use to get at unconscious concerns about death is what is known as the death thought accessibility DTA hypothesis Essentially the DTA hypothesis states that if individuals are motivated to avoid cognitions about death and they avoid these cognitions by espousing a worldview or by buffering their self esteem then when threatened an individual should possess more death related cognitions e g thoughts about death and death related stimuli than they would when not threatened 33 The DTA hypothesis has its origins in work by Greenberg et al 1994 27 as an extension of their earlier terror management hypotheses i e the anxiety buffer hypothesis and the mortality salience hypothesis The researchers reasoned that if as indicated by Wegner s research on thought suppression 1994 1997 thoughts that are purposely suppressed from conscious awareness are often brought back with ease then following a delay death thought cognitions should be more available to consciousness than a those who keep the death thoughts in their consciousness the whole time and b those who suppress the death thoughts but are not provided a delay That is precisely what they found However other psychologists have failed to replicate these findings 34 In these initial studies i e Greenberg et al 2004 Arndt et al 1997 35 and in numerous subsequent DTA studies the main measure of DTA is a word fragment task whereby participants can complete word fragments in distinctly death related ways e g coff as coffin not coffee or in non death related ways e g sk l as skill not skull 36 If death thoughts are indeed more available to consciousness then it stands to reason that the word fragments should be completed in a way that is semantically related to death Importance of the Death Thought Accessibility hypothesis edit The introduction of this hypothesis has refined TMT and led to new avenues of research that formerly could not be assessed due to the lack of an empirically validated way of measuring death related cognitions Also the differentiation between proximal conscious near and threat focused and distal unconscious distant symbolic defenses that have been derived from DTA studies have been extremely important in understanding how people deal with their terror 37 It is important to note how the DTA paradigm subtly alters and expands TMT as a motivational theory Instead of solely manipulating mortality and witnessing its effects e g nationalism increased prejudice risky sexual behavior etc the DTA paradigm allows a measure of the death related cognitions that result from various affronts to the self Examples include threats to self esteem and to one s worldview the DTA paradigm can therefore assess the role of death thoughts in self esteem and worldview defenses Furthermore the DTA hypothesis lends support to TMT in that it corroborates its central hypothesis that death is uniquely problematic for human beings and that it is fundamentally different in its effects than meaning threats i e Heine et al 2006 38 and that is death itself and not uncertainty and lack of control associated with death Fritsche et al 2008 explore this idea 39 Since its inception the DTA hypothesis had been rapidly gaining ground in TMT investigations and as of 2009 has been employed in over 60 published papers with a total of more than 90 empirical studies 33 Death anxiety on health promotion edit How people respond to their fears and anxiety of death is investigated in TMT Moreover Taubman Ben Ari and Noy 2010 examine the idea that a person s level of self awareness and self consciousness should be considered in relation to their responses to their anxiety and death cognitions 24 The more an individual is presented with their death or death cognitions in general the more fear and anxiety one may have therefore to combat said anxiety one may implement anxiety buffers 24 Due to a change in people s lifestyles in the direction of more unhealthy behaviors the leading causes of death now being cancer and heart disease most definitely are related to individuals unhealthy behaviors though the statement is over generalising and certainly cannot be applied to every case 40 Age and death anxiety both are factors that should be considered in the terror management theory in relation to health promoting behaviors Age undoubtedly plays some kind of role in people s health promoting behaviors however an actual age related effect on death anxiety and health promoting behaviors has yet to be seen Although research has demonstrated that for young adults only when they were prompted with death related scenarios they yielded more health promoting behaviors compared to those participants in their sixties In addition death anxiety has been found to have an effect for young adults on their behaviors of health promotion 40 Terror management health model editThe terror management health model TMHM explores the role that death plays on one s health and behavior Goldenberg and Arndt 2008 state that the TMHM proposes the idea that death despite its threatening nature is in fact instrumental and purposeful in the conditioning of one s behavior towards the direction of a longer life 16 According to Goldenberg and Arndt 2008 certain health behaviors such as breast self exams BSEs can consciously activate and facilitate people to think of death especially their own death 16 While death can be instrumental for individuals in some cases when breast self exams activate people s death thoughts an obstacle can present itself in terms of health promotion because of the experience of fear and threat 16 Abel and Kruger 2009 have suggested that the stress caused by increased awareness of mortality when celebrating one s birthday might explain the birthday effect where mortality rates seem to spike around these days 41 On the other hand death and thoughts of death can serve as a way of empowering the self not as threats Researchers Cooper et al 2011 explored TMHM in terms of empowerment specifically using BSEs under two conditions when death thoughts were prompted and when thoughts of death were non conscious 36 According to TMHM people s health decisions when death thoughts are not conscious should be based on their motivations to act appropriately in terms of the self and identity 36 Cooper et al 2011 found that when mortality and death thoughts were primed women reported more empowerment feelings than those who were not prompted before performing a BSE 36 Additionally TMHM suggests that mortality awareness and self esteem are important factors in individuals decision making and behaviors relating to their health TMHM explores how people will engage in behaviors whether positive or negative even with the heightened awareness of mortality in the attempt to conform to society s expectations and improve their self esteem 30 The TMHM is useful in understanding what motivates individuals regarding their health decisions and behaviors In terms of smoking behaviors and attitudes the impact of warnings with death messages depends on The individuals level of smoking based self esteem The warnings actual degree of death information 25 Emotion editPeople with low self esteem but not high self esteem have more negative emotions when reminded of death This is believed to be because these individuals lack the very defenses that TMT argues protect people from mortality concerns e g solid worldviews In contrast positive mood states are not impacted by death thoughts for people of low or high self esteem 42 Leadership editIt has been suggested that culture provides meaning organization and a coherent world view that diminishes the psychological terror caused by the knowledge of eventual death The terror management theory can help to explain why a leader s popularity can grow substantially during times of crisis When a follower s mortality is made prominent they will tend to show a strong preference for iconic leaders An example of this occurred when George W Bush s approval rating jumped almost 50 percent following the September 11 attacks in the United States As Forsyth 2009 posits this tragedy made U S citizens aware of their mortality and Bush provided an antidote to these existential concerns by promising to bring justice to the terrorist group responsible for the attacks Researchers Cohen et al 2004 in their particular study on TMT tested the preferences for different types of leaders while reminding people of their mortality Three different candidates were presented to participants The three leaders were of three different types task oriented emphasized setting goals strategic planning and structure relationship oriented emphasized compassion trust and confidence in others and charismatic The participants were then placed in one of two conditions mortality salient or control group In the former condition the participants were asked to describe the emotions surrounding their own death as well as the physical act of the death itself whereas the control group were asked similar questions about an upcoming exam The results of the study were that the charismatic leader was favored more and the relationship oriented leader was favored less in the mortality salient condition Further research has shown that mortality salient individuals also prefer leaders who are members of the same group as well as men rather than women Hoyt et al 2010 This has links to social role theory Religion editTMT posits that religion was created as a means for humans to cope with their own mortality Supporting this arguments in favor of life after death and simply being religious reduce the effects of mortality salience on worldview defense Thoughts of death have also been found to increase religious beliefs At an implicit subconscious level this is the case even for people who claim to be nonreligious 43 44 Mental health editSome researchers have argued that death anxiety may play a central role in numerous mental health conditions 45 To test whether death anxiety causes a particular mental illness TMT researchers use a mortality salience experiment and examine whether reminding participants of death leads to increased prevalence of behaviors associated with that mental illness Such studies have shown that reminders of death lead to increases in compulsive handwashing in obsessive compulsive disorder 46 avoidance in spider phobias and social anxiety 47 and anxious behaviors in other disorders including panic disorder and health anxiety 48 suggesting the role of death anxiety in these conditions according to TMT researchers Criticisms editCriticisms of terror management theory have been based on several lines of arguments 49 Suppression of fear and anxiety is implausible from an evolutionary point of view The observed psychological responses to terrifying cues are better explained by coalitional psychology and theories of collective defense The responses can be explained as fear of uncertainty and the unknown The responses can be explained as search for meaning of life and mortality The experimental results are difficult to replicate These arguments are discussed in the following sections Evolutionary argument edit Anxiety and fear are psychological responses that have evolved because they help us avoid danger A mechanism to suppress anxiety and fear as postulated by TMT is unlikely to have evolved because it would reduce the chances of survival 50 51 It is argued that TMT relies on misguided assumptions about evolved human nature originating from psychoanalytic theory 50 Proponents of TMT argue that the cultural self esteem that counters death anxiety is either a spandrel or exaptation created as a byproduct of the human survival instinct being impinged upon by the awareness of death brought about by increased intelligence It is not responses to immediate danger that are suppressed but existential reminders of mortality They posit a dual defense model whereby proximal and distal defenses deal with threats differently with the former doing so more pragmatically due to greater conscious awareness and the latter more symbolically due to unconscious thought recession 49 17 Critics argue that the observed responses are not only evoked by cues of essential mortality but more generally by cues of danger or insecurity 50 Coalitional psychology and collective defense as alternative explanations edit TMT posits that people respond to cues about mortality by strengthening shared worldviews Critics believe that such a worldview defense is better explained by coalitional psychology People confronted with danger tend to build shared worldviews and a pro normative orientation in order to garner social support and to build coalitions and alliances 50 52 Proponents of TMT argue that the coalitional psychology theory is a black box explanation that 1 cannot account for the fact that virtually all cultures have a supernatural dimension 2 does not explain why cultural worldview defense is symbolic involving allegiance to both specific and general systems of abstract meaning unrelated to specific threats rather than focused on the specific adaptive threats it supposedly evolved to deal with and 3 dismisses TMT s dual process account of the underlying processes that generate MS effects without providing an alternative of any kind or attempting to account for the data relevant to this aspect of the TMT analysis 49 17 The coalitional theory is supported by a large statistical study finding that conservatism traditionalism and other responses represented by TMT theory are connected with collective danger while individual danger has very different and often opposite effects The observed connection with collective danger supports the coalitional theory while contradicting CP s interpretation of TMT which is understood as explicitly dealing with individual danger only 53 TMT theorists however have explained how CP dismisses TMT s dual process shown in lab studies whereby proximal and distal defenses deal with threats differently with the former doing so more pragmatically due to greater conscious awareness and the latter more symbolically due to unconscious thought recession This would account for the study s distinction between individual and collective danger with the former being more proximal and the latter more distal Unlike TMT CP does not view national political and religious coalitions as imagined communities that represent primarily cultural worldviews distal defenses Similarly another study has found that the response of system justification postulated by TMT theorists is increased by salience of terrorism not by salience of individual mortality 54 Earlier experimental findings can be explained by the fact that individual danger and collective danger are seriously confounded The findings that the observed responses are connected with collective danger rather than individual danger was predicted by regality theory This finding is in agreement with authoritarianism theory realistic group conflict theory and Ronald Inglehart s theory of modernization but not in agreement with CP s interpretation of terror management theory which omits its distal proximal dual defense model 53 Prevalence of death edit Since findings on mortality salience and worldview defense were first published other researchers have claimed that the effects may have been obtained due to reasons other than death itself such as anxiety fear or other aversive stimuli such as pain The experimental manipulations in TMT research are likely to elicit a mixture of different types of negative emotions including fear anxiety sadness and anger 55 Other studies have found effects similar to those that mortality salience results in for example thinking about difficult personal choices to be made being made to respond to open ended questions regarding uncertainty thinking about being robbed thinking about being socially isolated and being told that one s life lacks meaning 49 While these cases exist thoughts of death have since been compared to various aversive experimental controls such as but not limited to thinking about failure writing a critical exam public speaking with a considerable audience being excluded paralysis dental pain intense physical pain etc 49 With regards to the studies that found similar effects TMT theorists have argued that in the previously mentioned studies where death was not the subject thought about the subjects would quite easily be related to death in an individual s mind due to linguistic or experiential connection with mortality p 332 49 For example being robbed invokes thoughts of violence and being unsafe in one s own home many people have died trying to protect their property and family A second possible explanation for these results involves the death thought accessibility hypothesis these threats somehow sabotage crucial anxiety buffering aspects of an individual s worldview or self esteem which increases their death thought accessibility For example one study found increased death thought accessibility in response to thoughts of antagonistic relations with attachment figures 49 However this makes it difficult or impossible to isolate the effect of mortality salience 55 While many TMT theorists claim that affective responses to mortality salience are suppressed and pushed out of consciousness later studies contradict this and show that affective responses are indeed observable 55 Meaning maintenance model edit The meaning maintenance model MMM was initially introduced as a comprehensive motivational theory that claimed to subsume TMT with alternative explanations for TMT findings Essentially it posits that people automatically give meaning to things and when those meanings are somehow disrupted it causes anxiety 38 In response people concentrate on meaning maintenance to reestablish their sense of symbolic unity and that such meaning maintenance often involves the compensatory reaffirmation of alternative meaning structures 38 These meanings among other things should provide a basis for prediction and control of our environments help one to cope with tragedy and trauma and the symbolic cheating of death via adherence to the enduring values that these cultures provide 38 While TMT regards the search for meaning as a defense mechanism meaning management theory regards the quest for meaning as a primary motive because we are meaning seeking and meaning making creatures living in a world of meanings When people are exposed to mortality salience both TMT and meaning management theory would predict an increase in pro culture and pro esteem activities but for very different reasons The latter theory is replacing death denial by death acceptance 56 TMT theorists argue that meaning management theory cannot describe why different sets of meaning are preferred by different people and that different types of meaning have different psychological functions 49 TMT theorists argue that unless something is an important element of a person s anxiety buffering worldview or self esteem it will not require broad meaning maintenance 49 TMT theorists believe that meaning management theory cannot accurately claim to be an alternative to TMT because it does not seem to be able to explain the current breadth of TMT evidence 49 Offensive defensiveness edit Some theorists have argued that it is not the idea of death and nonexistence that is unsettling to people but the fact that uncertainty is involved 57 58 For example these researchers posited that people defend themselves by altering their fear responses from uncertainty to an enthusiasm approach 49 Other researchers argue for distinguishing fear of death from fear of dying and therein posit that ultimately the fear of death has more to do with some other fear e g fear of pain or reflects uncertainty avoidance or fear of the unknown 59 TMT theorists agree that uncertainty can be disconcerting in some cases and it may even result in defense responses but note that they believe the inescapability of death and the possibility of its finality regarding one s existence is most unsettling They also note that people actually seek out some types of uncertainty and that being uncertain is not always very unpleasant 49 In contrast there is substantial evidence that all things being equal uncertainty and the unknown represent fundamental fears and are only experienced as pleasant when there is sufficient contextual certainty 59 60 For example a surprise involves uncertainty but is only perceived as pleasant if there is sufficient certainty that the surprise will be pleasant Though TMT theorists acknowledge that many responses to mortality salience involve greater approaches zealousness towards important worldviews they also note examples of mortality salience which resulted in the opposite which offensive defensiveness cannot account for when negative features of a group to which participants belong were made salient people actively distanced themselves from that group under mortality salience 49 Replication failure edit In addition to the criticisms from alternative theoretical perspectives a large scale attempt by Many Labs 4 to replicate published findings failed to replicate the mortality salience effect on worldview defense under any condition 61 The test is a multi lab replication of Study 1 of Greenberg et al 1994 27 Psychologists in 21 labs across the U S re executed the original experiment among a total of 2 200 participants In response to the Many Labs 4 paper Tom Pyszczynski one of the founding psychologists of TMT criticized the study for insufficient sample sizes failure to follow the advice of researchers and deviation from a preregistered protocol 62 Popularity editPsychologist Yoel Inbar summarized the popularity of the theory I can not explain to people who were not around during this time which I would say was roughly 2004 to 2008 how much everything at the time was about terror management theory You would go to SPSP and it seemed like half of the posters were about terror management theory It was just everywhere There is just an explosion of terror management theory stuff And then it sort of receded And now you barely see it Which is also kind of weird We were obsessed with this for a period of 3 5 years then we moved on to other things 63 See also editAnxiety buffer disruption theory Application of terror management theory Cognitive dissonance Stress from contradictory beliefs Death anxiety Anxiety caused by thoughts of death Flight from Death a documentary film based on Ernest Becker s work and terror management theory Mortality salience Awareness about death Necrophobia Fear of dead organisms Memento mori Artistic or symbolic reminder of the inevitability of death Protection motivation theoryReferences edit a b c Greenberg J Pyszczynski T Solomon S 1986 The causes and consequences of a need for self esteem A terror management theory In R F Baumeister ed Public Self and Private Self New York Springer Verlag pp 189 212 a b c Solomon S Greenberg J Pyszczynski T 1991 A terror management theory of social behavior The psychological functions of self esteem and cultural worldviews Advances in Experimental Social Psychology 24 93 159 Jonas E Fischer P 2006 Terror management and religion evidence that intrinsic religiousness mitigates worldview defense following mortality salience Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 91 3 553 567 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 91 3 553 PMID 16938037 S2CID 45201747 a b Greenberg J Pyszczynski T Solomon S Rosenblatt A Veeder M Kirkland S Lyon D 1990 Evidence for terror management II The effects of mortality salience on reactions to those who threaten or bolster the cultural worldview Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 58 2 308 318 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 454 2378 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 58 2 308 Zhou X Liu J Chen C Yu Z 2008 Do children transcend death An examination of the terror management function of offspring Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 49 5 413 418 doi 10 1111 j 1467 9450 2008 00665 x PMID 18489534 a b Goldenberg J L Pyszczynski T Greenberg J Solomon S 2000 Fleeing the body A terror management perspective on the problem of human corporeality Personality and Social Psychology Review 4 3 200 218 doi 10 1207 s15327957pspr0403 1 S2CID 31331978 Solomon S Pyszczynski T Greenberg J 2015 The Worm at the Core On the Role of Death in Life Random House Terror Management Theory Ernest Becker Foundation ernestbecker org Retrieved 2022 01 21 Arrowood Robert B Pope J Brian 2014 Terror management theory A theoretical perspective on origination maintenance and research Becker p ix Statius 2004 Book 3 line 661 In David Roy Shackleton Bailey ed Thebaid Books 1 7 Loeb classical library Volume 207 Translated by David Roy Shackleton Bailey Cambridge Massachusetts Harvard University Press pp 198 199 ISBN 9780674012080 Retrieved 29 October 2023 Primus in orbe deos fecit timor Fear first made gods in the world Landau Mark J Solomon Sheldon Pyszczynski Tom Greenberg Jeff 2007 07 01 On the Compatibility of Terror Management Theory and Perspectives on Human Evolution Evolutionary Psychology 5 3 147470490700500 doi 10 1177 147470490700500303 ISSN 1474 7049 S2CID 2548801 Becker pp ix xiv Levitt Morton July 1974 Reviewed work s The Denial of Death by Ernest Becker Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science Vol 414 USA USSR Agenda for Communication pp 200 201 Becker p 7 a b c d Goldenberg J L Arndt J 2008 The Implications of death for health A terror management health model for behavioral health promotion Psychological Review 115 4 1032 1053 doi 10 1037 a0013326 PMID 18954213 a b c Landau M J Solomon S Pyszczynski T Greenberg J 2007 On the compatibility of terror management theory and perspectives on human evolution PDF Evolutionary Psychology 5 3 476 519 doi 10 1177 147470490700500303 Koole Sander L van den Berg Agnes E 2004 Paradise Lost and Reclaimed In Greenberg Jeff Koole Sander L Pyszczynski Tom eds Handbook Of Experimental Existential Psychology New York Guilford Press p 91 ISBN 978 1 59385 040 1 Retrieved 2013 08 18 Becker Ernest 1971 The birth and death of meaning 2nd ed New York The Free Press Becker Ernest 1973 The Denial of Death 1st ed New York The Free Press a b Pyszczynski T Greenberg J Solomon S Arndt J Schimel J 2004 Why do people need self esteem A theoretical and empirical review Psychological Bulletin 130 130 435 468 doi 10 1037 0033 2909 130 3 435 PMID 15122930 S2CID 1780005 a b Hewstone M Rubin M Willis H 2002 Intergroup bias Annual Review of Psychology 53 575 604 doi 10 1146 annurev psych 53 100901 135109 PMID 11752497 S2CID 11830211 a b c d e Arndt J Cox C R Goldenberg J L Vess M Routledge C Cooper D P Cohen F 2009 Blowing in the social wind Implications of extrinsic esteem contingencies for terror management and health Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 96 6 1191 1205 doi 10 1037 a0015182 PMID 19469596 a b c Taubman Ben Ari O Noy A 2010 Self consciousness and death cognitions from a terror management perspective Death Studies 34 10 871 892 doi 10 1080 07481187 2010 496685 PMID 24482853 S2CID 24026354 a b c d e f g h i j Hansen J Winzeler S Topolinski S 2010 When the death makes you smoke A terror management perspective on the effectiveness of cigarette on pack warnings Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 46 1 226 228 doi 10 1016 j jesp 2009 09 007 Pyszczynski T Wicklund R A Floresku S Koch H Gauch G Solomon S Greenberg J 1996 Whistling in the dark Exaggerated consensus estimates in response to incidental reminders of mortality Psychological Science 7 6 332 336 doi 10 1111 j 1467 9280 1996 tb00384 x S2CID 12709488 a b c d Greenberg J Pyszczynski T Solomon S Simon L Breus M 1994 Role of consciousness and accessibility of death related thoughts in mortality salience effects Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 67 4 627 637 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 67 4 627 PMID 7965609 S2CID 37679370 a b Burke B L Martens A Faucher E H 2010 Two decades of terror management theory A meta analysis of mortality salience research Personality and Social Psychology Review 14 2 155 195 doi 10 1177 1088868309352321 PMID 20097885 S2CID 206682555 a b Harmon Jones E Simon L Greenberg J Pyszczynski T Solomon S McGregor H 1997 Terror management theory and self esteem Evidence that increased self esteem reduces mortality salience effects Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 72 1 24 36 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 72 1 24 PMID 9008372 S2CID 32261410 a b c d e Cox C R Cooper D P Vess M Arndt J Goldenberg J L Routledge C 2009 Bronze is beautiful but pale can be pretty The effects of appearance standards and mortality salience on sun tanning outcomes Health Psychology 28 6 746 752 doi 10 1037 a0016388 PMID 19916643 a b c d Jessop D C Albery I P Rutter J Garrod H 2008 Understanding the impact of mortality related health risk information A terror management theory perspective Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 34 7 951 964 doi 10 1177 0146167208316790 PMID 18453389 S2CID 10585949 a b c d Martin I M Kamins M A 2010 An application of terror management theory in the design of social and health related anti smoking appeals Journal of Consumer Behaviour 9 3 172 190 doi 10 1002 cb 293 a b Hayes J Schimel J Ardnt J Faucher E 2010 A Theoretical and Empirical Review of the Death Thought Accessibility Concept in Terror Management Research Psychological Bulletin 136 5 699 739 doi 10 1037 a0020524 PMID 20804234 Trafimow David Jamie S Hughes September 2012 Testing the Death Thought Suppression and Rebound Hypothesis Social Psychological and Personality Science 3 5 622 629 doi 10 1177 1948550611432938 S2CID 146202248 Arndt J Greenberg J Pyszczynski T Solomon S 1997 Subliminal exposure to death related stimuli increases defense of the cultural worldview Psychological Science 8 5 379 385 doi 10 1111 j 1467 9280 1997 tb00429 x S2CID 145101319 a b c d Cooper D P Goldenberg J L Arndt J 2011 Empowering the self Using the terror management health model to promote breast self examination Self and Identity 10 3 315 325 doi 10 1080 15298868 2010 527495 S2CID 140520175 Pyszczynski T Greenberg J Solomon S 1999 A dual process model of defense against conscious and unconscious death related thoughts An extension of terror management theory Psychological Review 106 4 835 845 doi 10 1037 0033 295x 106 4 835 PMID 10560330 S2CID 2655060 a b c d Heine S J Proulx T Vohs K D 2006 The meaning maintenance model On the coherence of human motivations Personality and Social Psychology Review 10 2 88 110 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 434 4237 doi 10 1207 s15327957pspr1002 1 PMID 16768649 S2CID 899167 Fritsche I Jonas E Fankhanel T 2008 The role of control motivation in mortality salience effects on ingroup support and defense Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 95 3 524 541 doi 10 1037 a0012666 PMID 18729692 a b Bozo O Tunca A SImsek Y 2009 The effect of death anxiety and age on health promoting behaviors A terror management theory perspective Journal of Psychology Interdisciplinary and Applied 143 4 377 389 doi 10 3200 JRLP 143 4 377 389 PMID 19606644 S2CID 9927722 Abel Ernest Kruger Michael 2009 Mortality Salience of Birthdays on Day of Death in the Major Leagues Death Studies 33 2 175 184 doi 10 1080 07481180802138936 PMID 19143110 S2CID 8439436 Routledge C Ostafin B Juhl J Sedikides C Cathey C Liao J 2010 The effects of self esteem and mortality salience on well being growth motivation and maladaptive behavior Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 99 6 897 916 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 465 8476 doi 10 1037 a0021431 PMID 21114350 Heflick Nathan Goldenberg J 2012 No atheists in foxholes Arguments for but not against afterlives buffer mortality salience effects for atheists British Journal of Social Psychology 51 2 385 392 doi 10 1111 j 2044 8309 2011 02058 x PMID 21995319 Jong Jonathan Halberstadt Jamin Bluemke Matthias 2012 Foxhole atheism revisited The effects of mortality salience on explicit and implicit religious belief Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 48 5 983 989 doi 10 1016 j jesp 2012 03 005 Iverach Lisa Menzies Ross G Menzies Rachel E 2014 Death anxiety and its role in psychopathology reviewing the status of a transdiagnostic construct Clinical Psychology Review 34 7 580 593 doi 10 1016 j cpr 2014 09 002 ISSN 1873 7811 PMID 25306232 S2CID 11017493 Menzies R E Dar Nimrod I 2017 Death anxiety and its relationship with obsessive compulsive disorder Journal of Abnormal Psychology 126 4 367 377 doi 10 1037 abn0000263 PMID 28277734 S2CID 3823579 Strachan Eric Schimel Jeff Arndt Jamie Williams Todd Solomon Sheldon Pyszczynski Tom Greenberg Jeff 2007 08 01 Terror Mismanagement Evidence That Mortality Salience Exacerbates Phobic and Compulsive Behaviors Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 33 8 1137 1151 doi 10 1177 0146167207303018 ISSN 0146 1672 PMID 17545415 S2CID 21364203 Menzies R E Sharpe L Dar Nimrod I 2021 The effect of mortality salience on bodily scanning behaviors in anxiety related disorders Journal of Abnormal Psychology 130 2 141 151 doi 10 1037 abn0000577 PMID 33301338 S2CID 228100285 a b c d e f g h i j k l m Pyszczynski T Greenberg J Solomon S Maxfield M 2006 On the unique psychological import of the human awareness of mortality Theme and variations Psychological Inquiry 17 4 328 356 doi 10 1080 10478400701369542 S2CID 143508018 a b c d Kirkpatrick Lee Carlos David Navarrete 2006 Reports of My Death Anxiety Have Been Greatly Exaggerated A Critique of Terror Management Theory from an Evolutionary Perspective Psychological Inquiry 17 4 288 298 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 574 4267 doi 10 1080 10478400701366969 S2CID 144262438 Buss David 1997 Human Social Motivation in Evolutionary Perspective Grounding Terror Management Theory Psychological Inquiry 8 1 22 26 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 387 7436 doi 10 1207 s15327965pli0801 3 Navarrete D C Fessler D M T 2005 Normative bias and adaptive challenges A relational approach to coalitional psychology and a critique of terror management theory Evolutionary Psychology 3 297 325 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 132 5201 doi 10 1177 147470490500300121 S2CID 14311881 a b Fog Agner 2023 Psychological and cultural effects of different kinds of danger An exploration based on survey data from 79 countries Culture and Evolution doi 10 1556 2055 2023 00029 S2CID 257998766 Ullrich Johannes Cohrs J Christopher 2007 Terrorism salience increases system justification Experimental evidence Social Justice Research 20 2 117 139 doi 10 1007 s11211 007 0035 y S2CID 145734264 a b c Lambert A J et al 2014 Toward a greater understanding of the emotional dynamics of the mortality salience manipulation Revisiting the affect free claim of terror management research Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 106 5 655 678 doi 10 1037 a0036353 PMID 24749817 Wong Paul T P Adrian Tomer 2011 Beyond Terror and Denial The Positive Psychology of Death Acceptance Death Studies 35 2 99 106 doi 10 1080 07481187 2011 535377 PMID 24501830 S2CID 1067025 McGregor I Zanna M P Holmes J G Spencer S J 2001 Compensatory conviction in the face of personal uncertainty Going to extremes and being oneself Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 80 3 472 488 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 80 3 472 PMID 11300580 S2CID 9025151 McGregor I 2006 Offensive defensiveness Toward an integrative neuroscience of compensatory zeal after mortality salience personal uncertainty and other poignant self threats Psychological Inquiry 17 4 299 308 doi 10 1080 10478400701366977 a b Carleton R Nicholas June 2016 Fear of the unknown One fear to rule them all Journal of Anxiety Disorders 41 5 21 doi 10 1016 j janxdis 2016 03 011 PMID 27067453 Carleton R Nicholas April 2016 Into the unknown A review and synthesis of contemporary models involving uncertainty Journal of Anxiety Disorders 39 30 43 doi 10 1016 j janxdis 2016 02 007 PMID 26945765 1 Klein Richard A Cook Corey L Ebersole Charles R Vitiello Christine Nosek Brian A Ahn Paul Brady Abbie J Chartier Christopher R Christopherson Cody D Clay Samuel 2017 01 12 Many Labs 4 Replicating Mortality Salience with and without Original Author Involvement Chatard Armand Hirschberger Gilad Pyszczynski Tom 2020 02 07 A Word of Caution about Many Labs 4 If You Fail to Follow Your Preregistered Plan You May Fail to Find a Real Effect unpublished Center for Open Science doi 10 31234 osf io ejubn S2CID 236806340 Tullett Alexa Inbar Yoel 15 June 2022 Episode 88 Many Many Labs Two Psychologists Four Beers Podcast Publisher Event occurs at 49 30 50 20 Retrieved 10 June 2022 Bibliography editBecker Ernest 1973 The Denial of Death The Free Press ISBN 0 02 902380 7 Pyszczynski Thomas Solomon Sheldon Greenberg Jeff 2003 In the Wake of 9 11 The Psychology of Terror American Psychological Association ISBN 1 55798 954 0 Solomon Sheldon Greenberg J amp Pyszczynski T 1991 A terror management theory of social behavior The psychological functions of esteem and cultural worldviews in M P Zanna Ed Advances in Experimental Social Psychology Volume 24 Academic Press pp 93 159 ISBN 0 12 015224 XFurther reading editCurtis V Biran A 2001 Dirt disgust and disease Is hygiene in our genes Perspectives in Biology and Medicine 44 1 17 31 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 324 760 doi 10 1353 pbm 2001 0001 PMID 11253302 S2CID 15675303 Darwin C 1998 1872 The expression of the emotions in man and animals 3rd ed London Harper Collins Florian V Mikulincer M 1997 Fear of death and the judgment of social transgressions a multidimensional test of terror Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 73 2 369 80 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 73 2 369 ISSN 0022 3514 PMID 9248054 Goldenberg J L Pyszczynski T Greenberg J Solomon S Kluck B Cornwell R 2001 I am not an animal Mortality salience disgust and the denial of human creatureliness Journal of Experimental Psychology 130 3 427 435 doi 10 1037 0096 3445 130 3 427 PMID 11561918 Greenberg J Pyszczynski T Solomon S Rosenblatt A Veeder M Kirkland S 1990 Evidence for terror management theory II The effects of mortality salience on reactions to those who threaten or bolster the cultural worldview Fee required Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 58 2 308 318 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 454 2378 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 58 2 308 ISSN 0022 3514 13817 35400000600727 0100 INIST CNRS Retrieved 2007 07 27 Greenberg J Solomon S Pyszczynski T 1997 Terror management theory of self esteem and cultural worldviews Empirical assessments and conceptual refinements Advances in Experimental Social Psychology 29 S 61 139 doi 10 1016 s0065 2601 08 60016 7 Hansen J Winzeler S Topolinski S 2010 When death makes you smoke a terror management perspective on the effectiveness of cigarette on pack warnings Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 46 226 228 doi 10 1016 j jesp 2009 09 007 Hirschberger G Florian V Mikulincer M 2003 Striving for romantic intimacy following partner complaint or partner criticism A terror management perspective Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 20 5 675 687 doi 10 1177 02654075030205006 S2CID 144657212 Judis J B August 27 2007 Death grip How political psychology explains Bush s ghastly success New Republic Lazarus R S 1991 Emotion and adaptation New York Oxford University Press ISBN 978 0 19 506994 5 Mikulincer M Florian V Hirschberger G 2003 The existential function of close relationships Introducing death into the science of love Personality and Social Psychology Review 7 1 20 40 doi 10 1207 S15327957PSPR0701 2 PMID 12584055 S2CID 11600574 Pyszczynski T Greenberg J Solomon S 1997 Why do we need what we need A terror management perspective on the roots of human social motivation Psychological Inquiry 8 1 1 20 doi 10 1207 s15327965pli0801 1 Pyszczynski T Greenberg J Solomon S 1999 A dual process model of defense against conscious and unconscious death related thoughts An extension of terror management theory Psychological Review 106 4 835 845 doi 10 1037 0033 295X 106 4 835 PMID 10560330 S2CID 2655060 Rosenblatt A Greenberg J Solomon S Pyszczynski T Lyon D 1989 Evidence for terror management theory I The effects of mortality salience on reactions to those who violate or uphold cultural values Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 57 4 681 90 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 457 5862 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 57 4 681 ISSN 0022 3514 PMID 2795438 Royzman E B Sabini J 2001 Something it takes to be an emotion The interesting case of disgust Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour 31 1 29 59 doi 10 1111 1468 5914 00145 Shehryar O Hunt D M 2005 A terror management perspective on the persuasiveness of fear appeals Journal of Consumer Psychology 15 4 275 287 doi 10 1207 s15327663jcp1504 2 S2CID 18866874 Simon L Arndt J Greenberg J Pyszczynski T Solomon S 1998 Terror management and meaning Evidence that the opportunity to defend the worldview in response to mortality salience increases the meaningfulness of life in the mildly depressed Journal of Personality 66 3359 382 359 382 doi 10 1111 1467 6494 00016 hdl 10150 187250 PMID 9615422 Simon L Greenberg J Harmon Jones E Solomon S Pyszczynski T Arndt J Abend T 1997 Terror management and Cognitive Experiential Self Theory Evidence that terror management occurs in the experiential system Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 72 5 1132 1146 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 72 5 1132 PMID 9150588 Greenberg J Koole S L Pyszczynski T 2004 Handbook of experimental existential psychology Guilford Press ISBN 978 1 59385 040 1 Cohen Florette Solomon Sheldon Maxfield Molly Pyszczynski Tom Greenberg Jeff 2004 Fatal Attraction Psychological Science 15 12 SAGE Publications 846 851 doi 10 1111 j 0956 7976 2004 00765 x ISSN 0956 7976 PMID 15563330 S2CID 16787928 Van Tilburg W A P Igou E R 2011 On the meaningfulness of existence When life salience boosts adherence to worldviews European Journal of Social Psychology Submitted manuscript 41 6 740 750 doi 10 1002 ejsp 819 hdl 10344 5416 S2CID 142993102 Gutierrez C 2006 Consumer attraction to luxury brand products Social affiliation in terror management theory Discusses TMT at length Griffin R 2007 Fascism amp Modernism New York Palgrave Macmillan ISBN 978 1 4039 8783 9 TMT and self esteem Schmeichel B J Gailliot M T Filardo E A McGregor I Gitter S Baumeister R F 2009 Terror management theory and self esteem revisited The roles of implicit and explicit self esteem in mortality salience effects Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 96 5 1077 1087 doi 10 1037 a0015091 PMID 19379037 S2CID 13740871 External links editListen to this article 35 minutes source source nbsp This audio file was created from a revision of this article dated 23 November 2019 2019 11 23 and does not reflect subsequent edits Audio help More spoken articles Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Terror management theory amp oldid 1218726074, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.