fbpx
Wikipedia

Susan Curtiss

Susan Curtiss is an American linguist. She is Professor Emerita at the University of California, Los Angeles.[1] Curtiss's main fields of research are psycholinguistics and neurolinguistics.[2] Her 1976 UCLA PhD dissertation[3] centered on the study of the grammatical development of Genie, a famous feral child.[4] Her subsequent work has been on grammatical development in children with SLI;[5] maturational constraints on first-language development ("critical period" effects);[6] hemispheric specialization for language and language acquisition;[7][8] and the cognitive modularity of grammar.[9][10]

Royce Hall of UCLA, Los Angeles where Curtiss received her PhD

Biography edit

Susan Curtiss received her undergraduate degree from the University of California, Berkeley. She attended the University of California, Los Angeles where her doctoral advisor was Victoria Fromkin.[1] There, she studied Genie, the feral child. Curtiss wrote her doctoral thesis on Genie. Curtiss's dissertation is now regarded as the most significant research that addressed Genie’s acquisition of language. After receiving her PhD, she continued to study critical periods, modularity, and grammatical development at UCLA.

She is married and has two daughters.[11]

Work with Genie edit

In November 1970, the child welfare authorities of Los Angeles discovered a young girl who had been subject to extreme isolation and abuse. At the time of her discovery, the girl, who was given the pseudonym “Genie,” was approximately 13 years and 7 months old, and it was determined that her isolation began at around 20 months of age.[12] Many people were involved in Genie’s case including social workers, psychologists, and linguists.

In May 1971, Susan Curtiss, alongside a team of researchers, began researching Genie. At the time of Genie’s discovery, there was little evidence that she had acquired any language, and so the team of researchers aimed to determine whether Genie was capable of comprehending or producing language. They were able to find that Genie could understand far more than she was able to produce, and from there, the team tried to both teach Genie facets of the English language as well as test her understanding of how to employ those features.[13] Curtiss, specifically, set out to teach Genie the difference between singular and plural count nouns. Curtiss developed a game to help Genie understand the plural construction rule—Curtiss would say a noun phrase such as “four horses,” and Genie would find the index card that had the number 4, the index card that had a picture of horses, and the index card that had the letter “s” to construct that phrase. Using this game, Genie was able to learn regular English pluralization in 3 weeks.[13]

Over the course of study, Genie’s language developed in several ways. The researchers noted, however, that there were significant differences between Genie’s language development and that of a normal child—surprisingly, considering her limited exposure to language at the time she was placed into custody, Genie’s vocabulary rapidly grew very large. For example, after 7 months in the hospital, Genie could produce about 200 words whereas a child at the equivalent linguistic stage can produce about 50 words. To contrast, her syntax was quite limited. Genie acquired few syntactic rules, and she produced few sentences longer than 3 words. Curtiss and her research team found that despite Genie’s limited acquisition of syntax, she had relatively advanced cognitive abilities. Curtiss observed isolated incidents in which she believed Genie showed evidence of using recursion, a complex feature of human language.[14]

Genie’s linguistic development was seen as an opportunity to research critical periods. Prior to the research done on Genie, it was hypothesized that the critical period to acquire a first language would end when the brain lateralized, meaning the tendency for certain functions of the brain to be localized to one hemisphere as opposed to the other.[15] Language is typically localized to the left hemisphere. It is widely held that process of lateralization of the brain ends during puberty, typically around the age of 13.[16] Curtiss and her team determined that Genie was most likely right-handed, but on dichotic listening tests they discovered that Genie, unlike most right-handed people, was right-hemisphere dominant for language; she had normal results for environmental sounds, proving that her brain was not simply reversed in dominance for language. Curtiss and her team hypothesized that the critical period may not end with the lateralization of the brain. If that were the case, Genie would have acquired no language as the left hemisphere of her brain could not be responsible for the language she had acquired thus far.[16] Curtiss's work with Genie has been frequently cited as evidence for the critical period for syntax.

In 1976, Curtiss completed her doctoral thesis and received her PhD from UCLA. Curtiss's thesis is the most significant research that has been published on Genie. In 1977, Academic Press published her thesis as a book titled Genie: A Psycholinguistic Study of A Modern-Day “Wild Child."[11]

As part of her work with Genie, Curtiss was featured in the 1994 Nova documentary Secret of the Wild Child[17] and the 2003 "Wild Child" episode of the television series Body Shock.[18] She was a script consultant for the movie Mockingbird Don't Sing (2001), and was the only person directly involved in the case to be involved in the film's making.[19]

Controversy edit

Genie’s case has been a source of controversy. During the years in which Genie was studied, Jean Butler (who later began using her married name, Ruch), Genie’s former teacher and foster parent, argued against the research. Ruch claimed that David Rigler, principal investigator of the Genie case, was putting the research ahead of Genie’s best interests and that Curtiss was a hindrance around the home that Ruch and Genie lived in. She further accused Rigler and Curtiss of disregarding data from the two months Ruch’s own home as it did not align with the goals of their research. The Riglers, who became Genie's foster parents immediately afterwards, and Curtiss said that Ruch's writing about Genie's social and linguistic development were highly inconsistent with their observations from immediately after her removal from Ruch's home.[11]

In the years following Curtiss's work with Genie, some scholars have cast doubt on her research. It has been suggested that Curtiss's findings were inconsistent, stating that there was not enough evidence to support the claim that Genie was not acquiring significant syntax, and rather that Curtiss presented examples to the contrary.[20] Others have suggested that studying Genie and administering tests was unethical. Genie's mother argued that Genie was over-tested and that the research team violated confidentiality rules. As a result, she filed a lawsuit in 1979 against those who researched Genie.[11]

Some scholars argued that the team put the research ahead of Genie’s best interests. Jay Shurley and David Elkind, who were involved in the early phases of Genie's case study, left the research team over these concerns.[11][21] Additionally, people believe that members of the research team took on too many roles in an attempt to study, befriend, and parent Genie.[20] Each of the researchers hold that they never put their work ahead of Genie’s wellbeing, and there are several scholars who support them, stating that they treated Genie as a human rather than a test subject.

Other Major Accomplishments edit

Critical Periods edit

Susan Curtiss's work with Genie lent itself greatly to her research on critical periods. As the case study of Genie demonstrated, reaching puberty without having had significant exposure to language does not mean that an individual will be unable to communicate for the remainder of their life. Rather, it means that while the individual can learn vocabulary but will be extremely unlikely to learn the syntax of the language.[13]

In 1980, Curtiss began research on a woman who was given the pseudonym “Chelsea.” Chelsea, 32, had been born deaf, but in childhood was not diagnosed as such. This misdiagnosis led to Chelsea never receiving any language input from American Sign Language (ASL) nor any auditory input.

Additionally, no system of home-sign ever arose in her home. Local social services requested an examination of Chelsea when she was 32, leading to the discovery that she was deaf. At that time, she was fitted with her first pair of hearing aids, and began working with Susan Curtiss.[22]

 
After birth, the ability to learn language quickly increases before reaching a peak. At about age 13, this window to acquire language closes

Curtiss observed that Chelsea was able to learn new vocabulary quickly, similarly to Genie. Chelsea showed strength in regard to semantics and categorizing words according to their meanings. However, she was unable to develop her syntactic abilities in any significant way. Curtiss noted that Chelsea was unable to consistently produce utterances that were well-formed. Chelsea struggled to generate sentences that followed the Subject-Verb-Object (SVO) word order of English. Curtiss notes that Chelsea was able to grasp many of the non-verbal characteristics of conversation, such as turn-taking, conversational fillers, and facial expressions.[22]

Curtiss compares Genie and Chelsea, offering both cases as supporting evidence for a critical period for syntax. Neither of these two individuals had significant exposure to language prior to reaching puberty, and despite attempts to teach them English, neither of them was able to grasp syntax. Curtiss notes that Genie was able to grasp certain facets of the English language, such as phrasal categories, whereas Chelsea was only able to enlarge her lexicon and understand discourse.[22]

Modularity edit

Curtiss has also done research on the modularity of grammar, exploring syntax and semantics are separate faculties of human language. In pursuit of this, Curtiss has conducted research on children who show evidence of dissociating different faculties of language from one another. This research has also served as evidence for the modularity of grammar. In her 1981 article titled Dissociations Between Language and Cognition: Cases and Implications, Curtiss explores the language systems of three children: Genie, Antony, and Marta.[23]

The data Curtiss collected from Genie’s case allowed her to conclude that not only were Genie’s syntactic and semantic faculties modular, but also that Genie’s cognitive abilities were not sufficient for further syntactic development. Antony, a mentally retarded 6 year old, was able to produce syntactically well-formed sentences, but they were semantically incorrect. His speech contained the very features that Genie’s speech lacked, further demonstrating the modularity of syntax and semantics as well as showing that there isn’t an innate preference towards one or the other. Marta, later referred to by her real first name, Laura, had a testable IQ between 41 and 48. From an early age she showed strength regarding the syntactic and morphological features of language, but her speech was frequently neither semantically correct nor fully intelligible.[24][23] These cases serve as evidence supporting Curtiss's commitment to the modularity of syntactic and semantic faculties.

 
A comparison of a normal brain and a brain with Alzheimer's disease

In addition to her research on children who dissociate various language faculties, Curtiss researched the language of elderly adults with Alzheimer’s disease and compared the results to that of elderly adults with unimpaired mental status. The 1987 study was titled Syntactic Preservation in Alzheimer's Disease.[25] Both the group of Alzheimer’s patients and the normal control group were recorded speaking in regular conversation; 50 utterances were transcribed from each participant to determine the number of errors made. The errors were then classified as either syntactic errors or semantic errors. She found that the patients with Alzheimer’s made significantly more semantic errors than syntactic errors. The control group made few errors, with a greater tendency towards syntactic errors.[25] Similar results were produced from a written trial, wherein the Alzheimer’s patients made more semantic errors than syntactic errors. These data supported Curtiss's belief that syntax was preserved to a greater degree than semantics in Alzheimer's patients.[26] Curtiss also determined that the number of errors made by each Alzheimer's patient correlated with the severity of the disease.[25]

Curtiss has also helped develop several key language tests for use by clinicians and researchers to diagnose language development. The CYCLE (Curtiss Yamada Comprehensive Language Evaluation) Test is widely used to diagnose language development and language impairments in children and adults.[27] Another test, the CYCLE-N (Curtiss Yamada Comprehensive Language Evaluation—Neurological), was designed specifically for adults and for mapping grammar in the brain.[28] These tests are administered to children and adults with various conditions including autism, Specific Language Impairment (SLI), and dementia to determine which language faculty has been affected so that clinicians can determine the best course of treatment.[29]

Language Impaired (LI) Children edit

 
A diagram of different types of language impairments

Susan Curtiss has performed significant research in the field of language development in children with various language impairments compared to normal children. In 1987, Curtiss provided a report to the US Congress.[30] This report offers data pertaining to the phonology, morphology, syntax, semantics, and pragmatics of both language impaired (LI) and normal English-speaking children as well as provides different subcategories of language impairment.

Curtiss notes that LI children understand phonological rules, but that their language system allows for errors in phonology that a normal child’s language system would not. Additionally, there is no data to show that LI children do not grasp morphological rules of their language. Indeed, normal children and LI children learn the same morphemes in the same order, but LI children begin to use those morphemes earlier though they take longer to master the appropriate usage. Curtiss notes that LI children often make syntactically unacceptable utterances, although they have the same syntactic categories. She suggests that these children may be creating sentences outside the rules of grammar. The semantics of LI children were the same as normal children of the same stage of language, meaning they created utterances of the same mean length (MLU). The data regarding the pragmatics of LI children were inconsistent. Although each of these areas is in need of further research, it is clear that pragmatics remains the most unclear.[30]

Curtiss suggested that not all LI children show the same performance abilities and deficits. Therefore, she classified LI children into the following four subcategories:

1. Receptively Impaired, meaning that the receptive language was in greater deficit

2. Expressively Impaired, meaning that the expressive language was in greater deficit

3. Severely Impaired, meaning that both receptive and expressive language were more than a year below the cognitive age

4. Mildly Impaired, meaning that both receptive and expressive language were more than a year below the cognitive age when averaged together[31]

Researchers observed that receptively impaired children demonstrated conversational initiative that was not reflected by any of the other subcategories of LI children or normal children. Several differences were noted in the syntactic abilities among the four subcategories of LI children at the age of 4. Expressively impaired children used the narrowest range of syntactic structures when compared with the other subcategories of LI children, and both expressively impaired and severely impaired children had difficulty mastering syntactically complex structures. Interestingly, these differences were no longer apparent in the children by age 5. Curtiss concluded from these data that the subcategories of LI children highlight “psycholinguistic and neuropsychological impairments” in the children rather than linguistic deficits.[30]

Specific Language Impairment (SLI) edit

In 1989, Curtiss published a study titled Familial Aggregation in Specific Language Impairment, in which she examined the concurrence of language impairments within family units.[32] Specific Language Impairment (SLI) is a language impairment in which a child’s language develops atypically, but their difficulties cannot be attributed to factors such as autism or slow development. Curtiss's study aimed to determine if there was a probable genetic factor involved in SLI. The researchers collected data from 76 SLI children, as well as 54 control children who were matched for age, ethnicity, and IQ. Data was also collected from the family of each child. Researchers found that the parents of SLI children were significantly more likely to report having had language impairments of their own than the parents of normal children. The siblings of impaired children were also more likely to have language impairments than the siblings of normal children. Furthermore, for families that had two language impaired parents, siblings were 52.6% more likely to have language impairments. In families where only one parent had a language impairment, the rate of sibling impairment was 31.6%. Families in which neither parent was language impaired showed a 25% rate of siblings being language impaired. Indeed, this study showed that SLI children are far more likely to have parents or siblings that are language impaired than normal children. The researchers observed that “most families reported either several [SLI] cases in the same family or no [SLI] cases at all.”[32]

Selected publications edit

Each of the following publications can be found on Curtiss's website: https://linguistics.ucla.edu/person/susan-curtiss/

Caplan, R., Curtiss, S., Chugani, H. and Vinters, H. V. 1996. Pediatric Rasmussen Encephalitis: Social communication, language, PET, and pathology before and after hemispherectomy. Brain and Cognition, 32, 45–66.

Curtiss, S. 1978. A New “Wild Child”? [Review of the book The Wild Boy of Burundi: A Study of an Outcase Child by H. Lane & R. Pillard]. Contemporary Psychology, vol. 25, 4, 337.

Curtiss, S. 1981. Dissociations between language and cognition. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, vol. 11, 1, 15–30.

Curtiss, S. 1982. Developmental dissociations of language and cognition. In L. Obler and L. Menn (Eds.) Exceptional Language and Linguistics (Perspectives in Neurolinguistics, Neuropsychology, and Psycholinguistics). New York, NY: Academic Press, 285–312.

Curtiss, S. 1982. The special talent of grammar acquisition. In L. Obler and L. Menn (Eds.) Exceptional Language and Linguistics (Perspectives in Neurolinguistics, Neuropsychology, and Psycholinguistics). New York, NY: Academic Press, 364–386.

Curtiss, S. 1983. Genie. In Reynolds, C. R. and Fletcher-Janzen, E. (Eds.) Concise Encyclopedia of Special Education: A Reference for the Education of the Handicapped and Other Exceptional Children and Adults. New York: NY, John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 709–710.

Curtiss, S. 1985. The development of human cerebral lateralization. In D. Frank Benson and E. Zaidel (Eds.) The Dual Brain. New York, NY: The Guillford Press, 97–116.

Curtiss, S. 1987. Interagency committee on learning disabilities report to the U.S. Congress: Developmental language disorders

Curtiss, S. 1988. Abnormal language acquisition. In F. J. Newmeyer (Ed.) Linguistics: The Cambridge Survey, Volume II Linguistic Theory: Extensions and Implications. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 96–116.

Curtiss, S. 1990. What happens if you are raised without language?.

Curtiss, S. 1995. Language as a cognitive system. In N. Chomsky (Ed.) Critical Assessment. Routledge, 211–255.

Curtiss, S. 2000. Acquisition of word and sentence structure. In V. Fromkin (Ed.) Linguistics: An Introduction to Linguistic Theory. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley-Blackwell Publishing.

Curtiss, S. 2013. Revisiting modularity: Using language as a window to the mind. In M. Piatelli-Palmarini and R. C. Berwick (Eds.) Rich Languages from Poor Inputs. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 68–90.

Curtiss, S. and de Bode, S. 1998. Language outcomes for hemispherectomized children. Proceedings of the 22nd Annual BUCLD, vol. 1, 121–133.

Curtiss, S. and de Bode, S. 1999. Age and etiology as predictors of language outcome following hemispherectomy. Developmental Neuroscience, vol. 21, 174-181.

Curtiss, S. and de Bode, S. 2003. How normal is grammatical development in the right hemisphere following hemispherectomy? The root infinitive stage and beyond. Brain and Language, vol. 86, 193–206.

Curtiss, S. and Schaeffer, J. 2005. Syntactic development in children with hemispherectomy: The I-, D-, and C-systems. Brain and Language, vol. 94, 2, 147–66.

Curtiss, S. Krashen, S., Fromkin, V., Rigler, D. and Rigler, M. 1973. Language acquisition after the critical period: Genie as of April, 1973. 98–103.

Curtiss, S., de Bode, S., and Mathern, G. W. 2001. Spoken language outocmes after hemispherectomy: Factoring in etiology. Brain and Language, vol. 79, 3, 379–96.

Curtiss, S., Fromkin, V., Krashen, S., Rigler, D. and Rigler, M. 1974. The linguistic development of Genie. Language, vol. 50, 3, 528–554.

Curtiss, S., Fromkin, V., Rigler, D., Riger, M. and Krashen, S. 1975. An update on the linguistic development of Genie. In Dato, D. P. (Ed.) Developmental Psycholinguistics: Theory and Applications. Washington, D.C.: Georgetown University Press, 145–153.

Curtiss, S., Fromkin, V. A. and Krashen, S. D. 1978. Language development in the mature (minor) right hemisphere. ITL: Review of Applied Linguistics 39–40, 23–37.

Curtiss, S., Jackson, C. A., Kempler, D., Hanson, W. R. and Metter, E. J. 1986. Length vs. structural complexity in sentence comprehension in aphasia. Clinical Aphasiology, vol. 16, 45–55.

Curtiss, S., Katz, W. and Tallal, P. 1992. Delay versus deviance in the language acquisition of normal and language-impaired children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, vol. 35, 2, 373–383.

Curtiss, S., MacSwan, J., Schaeffer, J., Kural, M. and Sano, T. 2004. GCS: A grammatical coding system manual.

Curtiss, S., MacSwan, J., Schaeffer, J., Kural, M. and Sano, T. 2004. GCS: A grammatical coding system for natural language data. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, vol. 3, 459–480.

Curtiss, S., Prutting, C. A. and Lowell, E. L. 1979. Pragmatic and semantic development in young children with impaired hearing. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, vol. 22, 534–552.

Curtiss, Susan. "The Case of Chelsea." UCLA Working Papers in Linguistics, vol. 18, pp. 115–46.

de Bode, S. and Curtiss, S. 1999. Neurobiological mechanisms of language acquisition in Sturge Weber Syndrome. Proceedings of the 23rd Annual BUCLD, vol. 1, 150–161.

de Bode, S. and Curtiss, S. 2000. How the brain copes with a phantom hemisphere and supports language development. In Proceedings of the 24th Annual BUCLD, vol. 1, 232–41.

de Bode, S. and Curtiss. S. 2000. Language after hemispherectomy. Brain and Cognition, vol. 43, 135–205.

Fromkin, V., Krashen, S., Curtiss, S., Rigler, D. and Rigler, M. 1974. The development of language in Genie: A case of language acquisition beyond the “Critical Period”. Brain and Language, vol. 1, 81–107.

Gilkerson, J., Hyams, N., and Curtiss, S. 2002. ‘No I Understand Negation’: A preferential looking paradigm study of early knowledge of sentential and anaphoric negation.

Gilkerson, J., Hyams, N., and Curtiss, S. 2003. On the scope of negation: More evidence for early parameter setting. To appear in Proceedings of Generative Approaches to Language Acquisition. Utrecht, The Netherlands.

Grinstead, J., MacSwan, J., Curtiss, S. and Gelman, R. 1998. The independence of language and number. Proceedings of the 22nd Annual BUCLD, vol. 1, 303–313.

Grinstead, J., MacSwan, J., Curtiss, S., and Gelman, R. 2004. The independence of language and number.

Jackson, C. A., Kempler, D., Hanson, W. R., Curtiss, S., Metter, E.J. and Van Lancker, D. 1988. Syntactic facility in fluent aphasia. In T. Prescott (Ed.), Clinical Aphasiology, 1988 (Conference on Clinical Aphasiology//Clinical Aphasiology). Cape Cod, MA: College Hill Press, 357–367.

Katz, W., Tallal, P., and Curtiss, S. 1992. Rapid automatized naming and gesture by normal and language-impaired children. Brain and Language, vol. 43. 623–641.

Kempler, D., Curtiss, S. and Jackson, C. 1987. Syntactic preservation in Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, vol. 30, 343–350.

Kempler, D., Curtiss, S., Metter, E.J., Jackson, C.A., and Hanson, W.R. 1991. Grammatical comprehension, aphasic syndromes and neuroimaging. Journal of Neurolinguistics, vol. 6, 3, 301–318.

Krashen, S. D., Curtiss, S., Fromking, V., Rigler, D. and Rigler, M. 1974. Lateralization in the case of Genie. From: BRI, CEREBRAL DOMINANCE, Conference Report #34. University of California, Los Angeles, 13–14.

Połczyńska, M., Japardi, K., Curtiss, S.,  Moody, I., Benjamin, C., Cho, A., Vigil, C., Kuhn, T., Jones, M., Bookheimer, S. 2017. Improving language mapping in clinical fMRI through assessment of grammar. Neuroimagg Clin. 15:415–427.

Połczyńska M., Kuhn T., You S. C., Walshaw P., Curtiss S., Bookheimer S. 2017b. Assessment of grammar optimizes language tasks for the intracarotid amobarbital procedure. Epilepsy & Behavior. 76:89–100.

Polczynska, M., Curtiss, S.,  Walshaw, P., Siddartha, P., Benjamin, C., Moseley, BD., Vigil, C., Jones, M., Eliashiv, D. and S, bookheimer. Grammar tests increase the ability to lateralize language function in the Wada test. Epilepsy Res. 108, (10), 1865–1873.

S. Curtiss and J. Yamada. The Curtiss-Yamada Comprehensive Language Evaluation for Neurosurgy (The CYCLE-N)

Tallal, P., Curtiss, S. and Kaplan, R. Curtiss, S. 1988. The San Diego Longitudinal Study. In S. E. Gerber & G. T. Mencher (Eds.) International Perspectives on Communication Disorders. Washington D.C.: Gallaudet University Press, 86–126.

Tallal, P., Ross, R. and Curtiss, S. 1989. Familial aggregation in Specific Language Impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, vol. 54, 167–173.

Tallal, P., Ross, R. and Curtiss, S. 1989. Unexpected sex-ratios in families with language learning-impaired children. Neuropsychologia, vol. 27, 7, 987–998.

Tallal, P., Townsend, J., Curtiss, S. and Wulfeck, B. 1991. Phenotypic profiles of language. Brain and Language, vol. 41, 81–95.

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ a b "UCLA Linguistics faculty website".
  2. ^ "Susan Curtiss'".
  3. ^ "Ph.D. Recipients". Department of Linguistics - UCLA. Retrieved 2021-03-09.
  4. ^ Curtiss, Susan (1977). Genie: A Psycholinguistic Study of a Modern-Day Wild Child. Academic Press. ISBN 9781483217611.
  5. ^ Tallal, P; Ross, R; Curtiss, S (2001). "Familial aggregation in Specific Language Impairment". Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders. 54 (2): 167–173. doi:10.1044/jshd.5402.167. PMID 2709834.
  6. ^ Curtiss, Susan; Prutting, C.A.; Lowell, E.L. (1979). "Pragmatic and semantic development in young children with impaired hearing". Journal of Speech and Hearing Research. 22 (3): 534–552. doi:10.1044/jshr.2203.534. PMID 502512.
  7. ^ Curtiss, Susan (1985). "The development of human cerebral lateralization". In Benson, D. Frank; Zaidel, E. (eds.). The Dual Brain. The Guillford Press. pp. 97–116.
  8. ^ Curtiss, Susan; Schaeffer, Jeannette (2005). "Syntactic development in children with hemispherectomy: the I-, D-, and C-systems". Brain and Language. 94 (2): 147–66. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.363.5460. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2004.12.004. PMID 15896390. S2CID 10233258.
  9. ^ Curtiss, Susan (2013). "Revisiting modularity: Using language as a window to the mind". In Piatelli-Palmarini, M.; Berwick, R.C. (eds.). Rich languages from poor inputs. Oxford University Press. pp. 68–90.
  10. ^ Curtiss, Susan. "Google Scholar". scholar.google.com. Retrieved 2021-03-09.
  11. ^ a b c d e Rymer, Russ. "A Silent Childhood." New Yorker, 20 Apr. 1992, pp. 43–77.
  12. ^ "Genie (feral child)", Wikipedia, 2021-04-29, retrieved 2021-04-29
  13. ^ a b c Curtiss, Susan, et al. "The linguistic development of Genie." Language (1974): 528-554.
  14. ^ Curtiss, S. Krashen, S., Fromkin, V., Rigler, D. and Rigler, M. 1973. Language acquisition after the critical period: Genie as of April, 1973. 98-103.
  15. ^ "Lateralization of brain function", Wikipedia, 2021-04-15, retrieved 2021-04-29
  16. ^ a b Fromkin, V., Krashen, S., Curtiss, S., Rigler, D. and Rigler, M. 1974. The development of language in Genie: A case of language acquisition beyond the “Critical Period”. Brain and Language, vol. 1, 81-107
  17. ^ "NOVA | Transcripts | Secret of the Wild Child | PBS". www.pbs.org. Retrieved 2021-03-09.
  18. ^ "Internet Movie Database, "Susan Curtiss"". IMDb.
  19. ^ "Internet Movie Database, "Susan Curtiss"". IMDb.
  20. ^ a b Jones, Peter E. "Contradictions and unanswered questions in the Genie case: A fresh look at the linguistic evidence." Language & Communication 15.3 (1995): 261–280.
  21. ^ Dutch, Jeremiah B. "“Genie” and Her Implications."
  22. ^ a b c Curtiss, Susan. "The Case of Chelsea." UCLA Working Papers in Linguistics, vol. 18, pp. 115-46.
  23. ^ a b Curtiss, Susan. "Dissociations between language and cognition: Cases and implications." Journal of autism and developmental disorders 11.1 (1981): 15-3
  24. ^ Yamada, Jeni Ellen (1990), Laura: A Case for the Modularity of Language, Issues in the Biology of Language and Cognition, Boston, MA: A Bradford Book, ISBN 978-0-26-224030-7, OCLC 21760166.
  25. ^ a b c Kempler, Daniel, Susan Curtiss, and Catherine Jackson. "Syntactic preservation in Alzheimer's disease." Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 30.3 (1987): 343–350.
  26. ^ Nebes, Robert D. "Semantic memory in Alzheimer's disease." Psychological bulletin 106.3 (1989): 377–394.
  27. ^ "CYCLE Test".
  28. ^ Połczyńska, M.; Japardi, K; Curtiss, S; Moody, T; Benjamin, Christopher; Cho, Andrew; Vigil, Celia; Kuhn, Taylor; Jones, Michael; Brookheimer, Susan (2017). "Improving language mapping in clinical fMRI through assessment of grammar". NeuroImage: Clinical. 15: 415–427. doi:10.1016/j.nicl.2017.05.021. PMC 5458087. PMID 28616382.
  29. ^ "CYCLE Test (Curtiss Yamada Comprehensive Language Evaluation)". THE CYCLE TESTS. Retrieved 2021-04-29.
  30. ^ a b c Curtiss, S. 1987. Interagency committee on learning disabilities report to the U.S. Congress: Developmental language disorders
  31. ^ "Receptive and Expressive Language". Children's Minnesota. Retrieved 2021-04-29.
  32. ^ a b Tallal, Paula, Randal Ross, and Susan Curtiss. "Familial aggregation in specific language impairment." Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 54.2 (1989): 167–173.

susan, curtiss, american, linguist, professor, emerita, university, california, angeles, curtiss, main, fields, research, psycholinguistics, neurolinguistics, 1976, ucla, dissertation, centered, study, grammatical, development, genie, famous, feral, child, sub. Susan Curtiss is an American linguist She is Professor Emerita at the University of California Los Angeles 1 Curtiss s main fields of research are psycholinguistics and neurolinguistics 2 Her 1976 UCLA PhD dissertation 3 centered on the study of the grammatical development of Genie a famous feral child 4 Her subsequent work has been on grammatical development in children with SLI 5 maturational constraints on first language development critical period effects 6 hemispheric specialization for language and language acquisition 7 8 and the cognitive modularity of grammar 9 10 Susan CurtissAlma materUniversity of California Berkeley University of California Los AngelesChildren2Scientific careerFieldsLinguistics Neurolinguistics PsycholinguisticsInstitutionsUCLAThesis 1976 Doctoral advisorVictoria FromkinRoyce Hall of UCLA Los Angeles where Curtiss received her PhD Contents 1 Biography 2 Work with Genie 2 1 Controversy 3 Other Major Accomplishments 3 1 Critical Periods 3 2 Modularity 3 3 Language Impaired LI Children 3 3 1 Specific Language Impairment SLI 4 Selected publications 5 See also 6 ReferencesBiography editSusan Curtiss received her undergraduate degree from the University of California Berkeley She attended the University of California Los Angeles where her doctoral advisor was Victoria Fromkin 1 There she studied Genie the feral child Curtiss wrote her doctoral thesis on Genie Curtiss s dissertation is now regarded as the most significant research that addressed Genie s acquisition of language After receiving her PhD she continued to study critical periods modularity and grammatical development at UCLA She is married and has two daughters 11 Work with Genie editIn November 1970 the child welfare authorities of Los Angeles discovered a young girl who had been subject to extreme isolation and abuse At the time of her discovery the girl who was given the pseudonym Genie was approximately 13 years and 7 months old and it was determined that her isolation began at around 20 months of age 12 Many people were involved in Genie s case including social workers psychologists and linguists In May 1971 Susan Curtiss alongside a team of researchers began researching Genie At the time of Genie s discovery there was little evidence that she had acquired any language and so the team of researchers aimed to determine whether Genie was capable of comprehending or producing language They were able to find that Genie could understand far more than she was able to produce and from there the team tried to both teach Genie facets of the English language as well as test her understanding of how to employ those features 13 Curtiss specifically set out to teach Genie the difference between singular and plural count nouns Curtiss developed a game to help Genie understand the plural construction rule Curtiss would say a noun phrase such as four horses and Genie would find the index card that had the number 4 the index card that had a picture of horses and the index card that had the letter s to construct that phrase Using this game Genie was able to learn regular English pluralization in 3 weeks 13 Over the course of study Genie s language developed in several ways The researchers noted however that there were significant differences between Genie s language development and that of a normal child surprisingly considering her limited exposure to language at the time she was placed into custody Genie s vocabulary rapidly grew very large For example after 7 months in the hospital Genie could produce about 200 words whereas a child at the equivalent linguistic stage can produce about 50 words To contrast her syntax was quite limited Genie acquired few syntactic rules and she produced few sentences longer than 3 words Curtiss and her research team found that despite Genie s limited acquisition of syntax she had relatively advanced cognitive abilities Curtiss observed isolated incidents in which she believed Genie showed evidence of using recursion a complex feature of human language 14 Genie s linguistic development was seen as an opportunity to research critical periods Prior to the research done on Genie it was hypothesized that the critical period to acquire a first language would end when the brain lateralized meaning the tendency for certain functions of the brain to be localized to one hemisphere as opposed to the other 15 Language is typically localized to the left hemisphere It is widely held that process of lateralization of the brain ends during puberty typically around the age of 13 16 Curtiss and her team determined that Genie was most likely right handed but on dichotic listening tests they discovered that Genie unlike most right handed people was right hemisphere dominant for language she had normal results for environmental sounds proving that her brain was not simply reversed in dominance for language Curtiss and her team hypothesized that the critical period may not end with the lateralization of the brain If that were the case Genie would have acquired no language as the left hemisphere of her brain could not be responsible for the language she had acquired thus far 16 Curtiss s work with Genie has been frequently cited as evidence for the critical period for syntax In 1976 Curtiss completed her doctoral thesis and received her PhD from UCLA Curtiss s thesis is the most significant research that has been published on Genie In 1977 Academic Press published her thesis as a book titled Genie A Psycholinguistic Study of A Modern Day Wild Child 11 As part of her work with Genie Curtiss was featured in the 1994 Nova documentary Secret of the Wild Child 17 and the 2003 Wild Child episode of the television series Body Shock 18 She was a script consultant for the movie Mockingbird Don t Sing 2001 and was the only person directly involved in the case to be involved in the film s making 19 Controversy edit Genie s case has been a source of controversy During the years in which Genie was studied Jean Butler who later began using her married name Ruch Genie s former teacher and foster parent argued against the research Ruch claimed that David Rigler principal investigator of the Genie case was putting the research ahead of Genie s best interests and that Curtiss was a hindrance around the home that Ruch and Genie lived in She further accused Rigler and Curtiss of disregarding data from the two months Ruch s own home as it did not align with the goals of their research The Riglers who became Genie s foster parents immediately afterwards and Curtiss said that Ruch s writing about Genie s social and linguistic development were highly inconsistent with their observations from immediately after her removal from Ruch s home 11 In the years following Curtiss s work with Genie some scholars have cast doubt on her research It has been suggested that Curtiss s findings were inconsistent stating that there was not enough evidence to support the claim that Genie was not acquiring significant syntax and rather that Curtiss presented examples to the contrary 20 Others have suggested that studying Genie and administering tests was unethical Genie s mother argued that Genie was over tested and that the research team violated confidentiality rules As a result she filed a lawsuit in 1979 against those who researched Genie 11 Some scholars argued that the team put the research ahead of Genie s best interests Jay Shurley and David Elkind who were involved in the early phases of Genie s case study left the research team over these concerns 11 21 Additionally people believe that members of the research team took on too many roles in an attempt to study befriend and parent Genie 20 Each of the researchers hold that they never put their work ahead of Genie s wellbeing and there are several scholars who support them stating that they treated Genie as a human rather than a test subject Other Major Accomplishments editCritical Periods edit Susan Curtiss s work with Genie lent itself greatly to her research on critical periods As the case study of Genie demonstrated reaching puberty without having had significant exposure to language does not mean that an individual will be unable to communicate for the remainder of their life Rather it means that while the individual can learn vocabulary but will be extremely unlikely to learn the syntax of the language 13 In 1980 Curtiss began research on a woman who was given the pseudonym Chelsea Chelsea 32 had been born deaf but in childhood was not diagnosed as such This misdiagnosis led to Chelsea never receiving any language input from American Sign Language ASL nor any auditory input Additionally no system of home sign ever arose in her home Local social services requested an examination of Chelsea when she was 32 leading to the discovery that she was deaf At that time she was fitted with her first pair of hearing aids and began working with Susan Curtiss 22 nbsp After birth the ability to learn language quickly increases before reaching a peak At about age 13 this window to acquire language closesCurtiss observed that Chelsea was able to learn new vocabulary quickly similarly to Genie Chelsea showed strength in regard to semantics and categorizing words according to their meanings However she was unable to develop her syntactic abilities in any significant way Curtiss noted that Chelsea was unable to consistently produce utterances that were well formed Chelsea struggled to generate sentences that followed the Subject Verb Object SVO word order of English Curtiss notes that Chelsea was able to grasp many of the non verbal characteristics of conversation such as turn taking conversational fillers and facial expressions 22 Curtiss compares Genie and Chelsea offering both cases as supporting evidence for a critical period for syntax Neither of these two individuals had significant exposure to language prior to reaching puberty and despite attempts to teach them English neither of them was able to grasp syntax Curtiss notes that Genie was able to grasp certain facets of the English language such as phrasal categories whereas Chelsea was only able to enlarge her lexicon and understand discourse 22 Modularity edit Curtiss has also done research on the modularity of grammar exploring syntax and semantics are separate faculties of human language In pursuit of this Curtiss has conducted research on children who show evidence of dissociating different faculties of language from one another This research has also served as evidence for the modularity of grammar In her 1981 article titled Dissociations Between Language and Cognition Cases and Implications Curtiss explores the language systems of three children Genie Antony and Marta 23 The data Curtiss collected from Genie s case allowed her to conclude that not only were Genie s syntactic and semantic faculties modular but also that Genie s cognitive abilities were not sufficient for further syntactic development Antony a mentally retarded 6 year old was able to produce syntactically well formed sentences but they were semantically incorrect His speech contained the very features that Genie s speech lacked further demonstrating the modularity of syntax and semantics as well as showing that there isn t an innate preference towards one or the other Marta later referred to by her real first name Laura had a testable IQ between 41 and 48 From an early age she showed strength regarding the syntactic and morphological features of language but her speech was frequently neither semantically correct nor fully intelligible 24 23 These cases serve as evidence supporting Curtiss s commitment to the modularity of syntactic and semantic faculties nbsp A comparison of a normal brain and a brain with Alzheimer s diseaseIn addition to her research on children who dissociate various language faculties Curtiss researched the language of elderly adults with Alzheimer s disease and compared the results to that of elderly adults with unimpaired mental status The 1987 study was titled Syntactic Preservation in Alzheimer s Disease 25 Both the group of Alzheimer s patients and the normal control group were recorded speaking in regular conversation 50 utterances were transcribed from each participant to determine the number of errors made The errors were then classified as either syntactic errors or semantic errors She found that the patients with Alzheimer s made significantly more semantic errors than syntactic errors The control group made few errors with a greater tendency towards syntactic errors 25 Similar results were produced from a written trial wherein the Alzheimer s patients made more semantic errors than syntactic errors These data supported Curtiss s belief that syntax was preserved to a greater degree than semantics in Alzheimer s patients 26 Curtiss also determined that the number of errors made by each Alzheimer s patient correlated with the severity of the disease 25 Curtiss has also helped develop several key language tests for use by clinicians and researchers to diagnose language development The CYCLE Curtiss Yamada Comprehensive Language Evaluation Test is widely used to diagnose language development and language impairments in children and adults 27 Another test the CYCLE N Curtiss Yamada Comprehensive Language Evaluation Neurological was designed specifically for adults and for mapping grammar in the brain 28 These tests are administered to children and adults with various conditions including autism Specific Language Impairment SLI and dementia to determine which language faculty has been affected so that clinicians can determine the best course of treatment 29 Language Impaired LI Children edit nbsp A diagram of different types of language impairmentsSusan Curtiss has performed significant research in the field of language development in children with various language impairments compared to normal children In 1987 Curtiss provided a report to the US Congress 30 This report offers data pertaining to the phonology morphology syntax semantics and pragmatics of both language impaired LI and normal English speaking children as well as provides different subcategories of language impairment Curtiss notes that LI children understand phonological rules but that their language system allows for errors in phonology that a normal child s language system would not Additionally there is no data to show that LI children do not grasp morphological rules of their language Indeed normal children and LI children learn the same morphemes in the same order but LI children begin to use those morphemes earlier though they take longer to master the appropriate usage Curtiss notes that LI children often make syntactically unacceptable utterances although they have the same syntactic categories She suggests that these children may be creating sentences outside the rules of grammar The semantics of LI children were the same as normal children of the same stage of language meaning they created utterances of the same mean length MLU The data regarding the pragmatics of LI children were inconsistent Although each of these areas is in need of further research it is clear that pragmatics remains the most unclear 30 Curtiss suggested that not all LI children show the same performance abilities and deficits Therefore she classified LI children into the following four subcategories 1 Receptively Impaired meaning that the receptive language was in greater deficit2 Expressively Impaired meaning that the expressive language was in greater deficit3 Severely Impaired meaning that both receptive and expressive language were more than a year below the cognitive age4 Mildly Impaired meaning that both receptive and expressive language were more than a year below the cognitive age when averaged together 31 Researchers observed that receptively impaired children demonstrated conversational initiative that was not reflected by any of the other subcategories of LI children or normal children Several differences were noted in the syntactic abilities among the four subcategories of LI children at the age of 4 Expressively impaired children used the narrowest range of syntactic structures when compared with the other subcategories of LI children and both expressively impaired and severely impaired children had difficulty mastering syntactically complex structures Interestingly these differences were no longer apparent in the children by age 5 Curtiss concluded from these data that the subcategories of LI children highlight psycholinguistic and neuropsychological impairments in the children rather than linguistic deficits 30 Specific Language Impairment SLI edit In 1989 Curtiss published a study titled Familial Aggregation in Specific Language Impairment in which she examined the concurrence of language impairments within family units 32 Specific Language Impairment SLI is a language impairment in which a child s language develops atypically but their difficulties cannot be attributed to factors such as autism or slow development Curtiss s study aimed to determine if there was a probable genetic factor involved in SLI The researchers collected data from 76 SLI children as well as 54 control children who were matched for age ethnicity and IQ Data was also collected from the family of each child Researchers found that the parents of SLI children were significantly more likely to report having had language impairments of their own than the parents of normal children The siblings of impaired children were also more likely to have language impairments than the siblings of normal children Furthermore for families that had two language impaired parents siblings were 52 6 more likely to have language impairments In families where only one parent had a language impairment the rate of sibling impairment was 31 6 Families in which neither parent was language impaired showed a 25 rate of siblings being language impaired Indeed this study showed that SLI children are far more likely to have parents or siblings that are language impaired than normal children The researchers observed that most families reported either several SLI cases in the same family or no SLI cases at all 32 Selected publications editEach of the following publications can be found on Curtiss s website https linguistics ucla edu person susan curtiss Caplan R Curtiss S Chugani H and Vinters H V 1996 Pediatric Rasmussen Encephalitis Social communication language PET and pathology before and after hemispherectomy Brain and Cognition 32 45 66 Curtiss S 1978 A New Wild Child Review of the book The Wild Boy of Burundi A Study of an Outcase Child by H Lane amp R Pillard Contemporary Psychology vol 25 4 337 Curtiss S 1981 Dissociations between language and cognition Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders vol 11 1 15 30 Curtiss S 1982 Developmental dissociations of language and cognition In L Obler and L Menn Eds Exceptional Language and Linguistics Perspectives in Neurolinguistics Neuropsychology and Psycholinguistics New York NY Academic Press 285 312 Curtiss S 1982 The special talent of grammar acquisition In L Obler and L Menn Eds Exceptional Language and Linguistics Perspectives in Neurolinguistics Neuropsychology and Psycholinguistics New York NY Academic Press 364 386 Curtiss S 1983 Genie In Reynolds C R and Fletcher Janzen E Eds Concise Encyclopedia of Special Education A Reference for the Education of the Handicapped and Other Exceptional Children and Adults New York NY John Wiley amp Sons Inc 709 710 Curtiss S 1985 The development of human cerebral lateralization In D Frank Benson and E Zaidel Eds The Dual Brain New York NY The Guillford Press 97 116 Curtiss S 1987 Interagency committee on learning disabilities report to the U S Congress Developmental language disordersCurtiss S 1988 Abnormal language acquisition In F J Newmeyer Ed Linguistics The Cambridge Survey Volume II Linguistic Theory Extensions and Implications Cambridge Cambridge University Press 96 116 Curtiss S 1990 What happens if you are raised without language Curtiss S 1995 Language as a cognitive system In N Chomsky Ed Critical Assessment Routledge 211 255 Curtiss S 2000 Acquisition of word and sentence structure In V Fromkin Ed Linguistics An Introduction to Linguistic Theory Hoboken NJ Wiley Blackwell Publishing Curtiss S 2013 Revisiting modularity Using language as a window to the mind In M Piatelli Palmarini and R C Berwick Eds Rich Languages from Poor Inputs Oxford Oxford University Press 68 90 Curtiss S and de Bode S 1998 Language outcomes for hemispherectomized children Proceedings of the 22nd Annual BUCLD vol 1 121 133 Curtiss S and de Bode S 1999 Age and etiology as predictors of language outcome following hemispherectomy Developmental Neuroscience vol 21 174 181 Curtiss S and de Bode S 2003 How normal is grammatical development in the right hemisphere following hemispherectomy The root infinitive stage and beyond Brain and Language vol 86 193 206 Curtiss S and Schaeffer J 2005 Syntactic development in children with hemispherectomy The I D and C systems Brain and Language vol 94 2 147 66 Curtiss S Krashen S Fromkin V Rigler D and Rigler M 1973 Language acquisition after the critical period Genie as of April 1973 98 103 Curtiss S de Bode S and Mathern G W 2001 Spoken language outocmes after hemispherectomy Factoring in etiology Brain and Language vol 79 3 379 96 Curtiss S Fromkin V Krashen S Rigler D and Rigler M 1974 The linguistic development of Genie Language vol 50 3 528 554 Curtiss S Fromkin V Rigler D Riger M and Krashen S 1975 An update on the linguistic development of Genie In Dato D P Ed Developmental Psycholinguistics Theory and Applications Washington D C Georgetown University Press 145 153 Curtiss S Fromkin V A and Krashen S D 1978 Language development in the mature minor right hemisphere ITL Review of Applied Linguistics 39 40 23 37 Curtiss S Jackson C A Kempler D Hanson W R and Metter E J 1986 Length vs structural complexity in sentence comprehension in aphasia Clinical Aphasiology vol 16 45 55 Curtiss S Katz W and Tallal P 1992 Delay versus deviance in the language acquisition of normal and language impaired children Journal of Speech and Hearing Research vol 35 2 373 383 Curtiss S MacSwan J Schaeffer J Kural M and Sano T 2004 GCS A grammatical coding system manual Curtiss S MacSwan J Schaeffer J Kural M and Sano T 2004 GCS A grammatical coding system for natural language data Behavior Research Methods Instruments amp Computers vol 3 459 480 Curtiss S Prutting C A and Lowell E L 1979 Pragmatic and semantic development in young children with impaired hearing Journal of Speech and Hearing Research vol 22 534 552 Curtiss Susan The Case of Chelsea UCLA Working Papers in Linguistics vol 18 pp 115 46 de Bode S and Curtiss S 1999 Neurobiological mechanisms of language acquisition in Sturge Weber Syndrome Proceedings of the 23rd Annual BUCLD vol 1 150 161 de Bode S and Curtiss S 2000 How the brain copes with a phantom hemisphere and supports language development In Proceedings of the 24th Annual BUCLD vol 1 232 41 de Bode S and Curtiss S 2000 Language after hemispherectomy Brain and Cognition vol 43 135 205 Fromkin V Krashen S Curtiss S Rigler D and Rigler M 1974 The development of language in Genie A case of language acquisition beyond the Critical Period Brain and Language vol 1 81 107 Gilkerson J Hyams N and Curtiss S 2002 No I Understand Negation A preferential looking paradigm study of early knowledge of sentential and anaphoric negation Gilkerson J Hyams N and Curtiss S 2003 On the scope of negation More evidence for early parameter setting To appear in Proceedings of Generative Approaches to Language Acquisition Utrecht The Netherlands Grinstead J MacSwan J Curtiss S and Gelman R 1998 The independence of language and number Proceedings of the 22nd Annual BUCLD vol 1 303 313 Grinstead J MacSwan J Curtiss S and Gelman R 2004 The independence of language and number Jackson C A Kempler D Hanson W R Curtiss S Metter E J and Van Lancker D 1988 Syntactic facility in fluent aphasia In T Prescott Ed Clinical Aphasiology 1988 Conference on Clinical Aphasiology Clinical Aphasiology Cape Cod MA College Hill Press 357 367 Katz W Tallal P and Curtiss S 1992 Rapid automatized naming and gesture by normal and language impaired children Brain and Language vol 43 623 641 Kempler D Curtiss S and Jackson C 1987 Syntactic preservation in Alzheimer s disease Journal of Speech and Hearing Research vol 30 343 350 Kempler D Curtiss S Metter E J Jackson C A and Hanson W R 1991 Grammatical comprehension aphasic syndromes and neuroimaging Journal of Neurolinguistics vol 6 3 301 318 Krashen S D Curtiss S Fromking V Rigler D and Rigler M 1974 Lateralization in the case of Genie From BRI CEREBRAL DOMINANCE Conference Report 34 University of California Los Angeles 13 14 Polczynska M Japardi K Curtiss S Moody I Benjamin C Cho A Vigil C Kuhn T Jones M Bookheimer S 2017 Improving language mapping in clinical fMRI through assessment of grammar Neuroimagg Clin 15 415 427 Polczynska M Kuhn T You S C Walshaw P Curtiss S Bookheimer S 2017b Assessment of grammar optimizes language tasks for the intracarotid amobarbital procedure Epilepsy amp Behavior 76 89 100 Polczynska M Curtiss S Walshaw P Siddartha P Benjamin C Moseley BD Vigil C Jones M Eliashiv D and S bookheimer Grammar tests increase the ability to lateralize language function in the Wada test Epilepsy Res 108 10 1865 1873 S Curtiss and J Yamada The Curtiss Yamada Comprehensive Language Evaluation for Neurosurgy The CYCLE N Tallal P Curtiss S and Kaplan R Curtiss S 1988 The San Diego Longitudinal Study In S E Gerber amp G T Mencher Eds International Perspectives on Communication Disorders Washington D C Gallaudet University Press 86 126 Tallal P Ross R and Curtiss S 1989 Familial aggregation in Specific Language Impairment Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders vol 54 167 173 Tallal P Ross R and Curtiss S 1989 Unexpected sex ratios in families with language learning impaired children Neuropsychologia vol 27 7 987 998 Tallal P Townsend J Curtiss S and Wulfeck B 1991 Phenotypic profiles of language Brain and Language vol 41 81 95 See also editLinguistic development of GenieReferences edit a b UCLA Linguistics faculty website Susan Curtiss Ph D Recipients Department of Linguistics UCLA Retrieved 2021 03 09 Curtiss Susan 1977 Genie A Psycholinguistic Study of a Modern Day Wild Child Academic Press ISBN 9781483217611 Tallal P Ross R Curtiss S 2001 Familial aggregation in Specific Language Impairment Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 54 2 167 173 doi 10 1044 jshd 5402 167 PMID 2709834 Curtiss Susan Prutting C A Lowell E L 1979 Pragmatic and semantic development in young children with impaired hearing Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 22 3 534 552 doi 10 1044 jshr 2203 534 PMID 502512 Curtiss Susan 1985 The development of human cerebral lateralization In Benson D Frank Zaidel E eds The Dual Brain The Guillford Press pp 97 116 Curtiss Susan Schaeffer Jeannette 2005 Syntactic development in children with hemispherectomy the I D and C systems Brain and Language 94 2 147 66 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 363 5460 doi 10 1016 j bandl 2004 12 004 PMID 15896390 S2CID 10233258 Curtiss Susan 2013 Revisiting modularity Using language as a window to the mind In Piatelli Palmarini M Berwick R C eds Rich languages from poor inputs Oxford University Press pp 68 90 Curtiss Susan Google Scholar scholar google com Retrieved 2021 03 09 a b c d e Rymer Russ A Silent Childhood New Yorker 20 Apr 1992 pp 43 77 Genie feral child Wikipedia 2021 04 29 retrieved 2021 04 29 a b c Curtiss Susan et al The linguistic development of Genie Language 1974 528 554 Curtiss S Krashen S Fromkin V Rigler D and Rigler M 1973 Language acquisition after the critical period Genie as of April 1973 98 103 Lateralization of brain function Wikipedia 2021 04 15 retrieved 2021 04 29 a b Fromkin V Krashen S Curtiss S Rigler D and Rigler M 1974 The development of language in Genie A case of language acquisition beyond the Critical Period Brain and Language vol 1 81 107 NOVA Transcripts Secret of the Wild Child PBS www pbs org Retrieved 2021 03 09 Internet Movie Database Susan Curtiss IMDb Internet Movie Database Susan Curtiss IMDb a b Jones Peter E Contradictions and unanswered questions in the Genie case A fresh look at the linguistic evidence Language amp Communication 15 3 1995 261 280 Dutch Jeremiah B Genie and Her Implications a b c Curtiss Susan The Case of Chelsea UCLA Working Papers in Linguistics vol 18 pp 115 46 a b Curtiss Susan Dissociations between language and cognition Cases and implications Journal of autism and developmental disorders 11 1 1981 15 3 Yamada Jeni Ellen 1990 Laura A Case for the Modularity of Language Issues in the Biology of Language and Cognition Boston MA A Bradford Book ISBN 978 0 26 224030 7 OCLC 21760166 a b c Kempler Daniel Susan Curtiss and Catherine Jackson Syntactic preservation in Alzheimer s disease Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research 30 3 1987 343 350 Nebes Robert D Semantic memory in Alzheimer s disease Psychological bulletin 106 3 1989 377 394 CYCLE Test Polczynska M Japardi K Curtiss S Moody T Benjamin Christopher Cho Andrew Vigil Celia Kuhn Taylor Jones Michael Brookheimer Susan 2017 Improving language mapping in clinical fMRI through assessment of grammar NeuroImage Clinical 15 415 427 doi 10 1016 j nicl 2017 05 021 PMC 5458087 PMID 28616382 CYCLE Test Curtiss Yamada Comprehensive Language Evaluation THE CYCLE TESTS Retrieved 2021 04 29 a b c Curtiss S 1987 Interagency committee on learning disabilities report to the U S Congress Developmental language disorders Receptive and Expressive Language Children s Minnesota Retrieved 2021 04 29 a b Tallal Paula Randal Ross and Susan Curtiss Familial aggregation in specific language impairment Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 54 2 1989 167 173 Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Susan Curtiss amp oldid 1217137316, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.