fbpx
Wikipedia

Hard and soft science

Hard science and soft science are colloquial terms used to compare scientific fields on the basis of perceived methodological rigor, exactitude, and objectivity.[1][2][3] In general, the formal sciences and natural sciences are considered hard science, whereas the social sciences and other sciences are described as soft science.[4]

Precise definitions vary,[5] but features often cited as characteristic of hard science include producing testable predictions, performing controlled experiments, relying on quantifiable data and mathematical models, a high degree of accuracy and objectivity, higher levels of consensus, faster progression of the field, greater explanatory success, cumulativeness, replicability, and generally applying a purer form of the scientific method.[2][6][7][8][9][10][11][12] A closely related idea (originating in the nineteenth century with Auguste Comte) is that scientific disciplines can be arranged into a hierarchy of hard to soft on the basis of factors such as rigor, "development", and whether they are basic or applied.[5][13]

Philosophers and historians of science have questioned the relationship between these characteristics and perceived hardness or softness. The more "developed" hard sciences do not necessarily have a greater degree of consensus or selectivity in accepting new results.[6] Commonly cited methodological differences are also not a reliable indicator. For example, social sciences such as psychology and sociology use mathematical models extensively, but are usually considered soft sciences.[1][2] However, there are some measurable differences between hard and soft sciences. For example, hard sciences make more extensive use of graphs,[5][14] and soft sciences are more prone to a rapid turnover of buzzwords.[15]

The metaphor has been criticised for unduly stigmatizing soft sciences, creating an unwarranted imbalance in the public perception, funding, and recognition of different fields.[2][3][16]

History of the terms edit

The origin of the terms "hard science" and "soft science" is obscure. The earliest attested use of "hard science" is found in an 1858 issue of the Journal of the Society of Arts,[17][18] but the idea of a hierarchy of the sciences can be found earlier, in the work of the French philosopher Auguste Comte (1798‒1857). He identified astronomy as the most general science,[note 1] followed by physics, chemistry, biology, then sociology. This view was highly influential, and was intended to classify fields based on their degree of intellectual development and the complexity of their subject matter.[6]

The modern distinction between hard and soft science is often attributed to a 1964 article published in Science by John R. Platt. He explored why he considered some scientific fields to be more productive than others, though he did not actually use the terms themselves.[19][20] In 1967, sociologist of science Norman W. Storer specifically distinguished between the natural sciences as hard and the social sciences as soft. He defined hardness in terms of the degree to which a field uses mathematics and described a trend of scientific fields increasing in hardness over time, identifying features of increased hardness as including better integration and organization of knowledge, an improved ability to detect errors, and an increase in the difficulty of learning the subject.[6][21]

Empirical support edit

In the 1970s sociologist Stephen Cole conducted a number of empirical studies attempting to find evidence for a hierarchy of scientific disciplines, and was unable to find significant differences in terms of core of knowledge, degree of codification, or research material. Differences that he did find evidence for included a tendency for textbooks in soft sciences to rely on more recent work, while the material in textbooks from the hard sciences was more consistent over time.[6] After he published in 1983, it has been suggested that Cole might have missed some relationships in the data because he studied individual measurements, without accounting for the way multiple measurements could trend in the same direction, and because not all the criteria that could indicate a discipline's scientific status were analysed.[22]

In 1984, Cleveland performed a survey of 57 journals and found that natural science journals used many more graphs than journals in mathematics or social science, and that social science journals often presented large amounts of observational data in the absence of graphs. The amount of page area used for graphs ranged from 0% to 31%, and the variation was primarily due to the number of graphs included rather than their sizes.[23] Further analyses by Smith in 2000,[5] based on samples of graphs from journals in seven major scientific disciplines, found that the amount of graph usage correlated "almost perfectly" with hardness (r=0.97). They also suggested that the hierarchy applies to individual fields, and demonstrated the same result using ten subfields of psychology (r=0.93).[5]

In a 2010 article, Fanelli proposed that we expect more positive outcomes in "softer" sciences because there are fewer constraints on researcher bias. They found that among research papers that tested a hypothesis, the frequency of positive results was predicted by the perceived hardness of the field. For example, the social sciences as a whole had a 2.3-fold increased odds of positive results compared to the physical sciences, with the biological sciences in between. They added that this supported the idea that the social sciences and natural sciences differ only in degree, as long as the social sciences follow the scientific approach.[7]

In 2013, Fanelli tested whether the ability of researchers in a field to "achieve consensus and accumulate knowledge" increases with the hardness of the science, and sampled 29,000 papers from 12 disciplines using measurements that indicate the degree of scholarly consensus. Out of the three possibilities (hierarchy, hard/soft distinction, or no ordering), the results supported a hierarchy, with physical sciences performing the best followed by biological sciences and then social sciences. The results also held within disciplines, as well as when mathematics and the humanities were included.[24]

Criticism edit

Critics of the concept argue that soft sciences are implicitly considered to be less "legitimate" scientific fields,[2] or simply not scientific at all.[25] An editorial in Nature stated that social science findings are more likely to intersect with everyday experience and may be dismissed as "obvious or insignificant" as a result.[16] Being labelled a soft science can affect the perceived value of a discipline to society and the amount of funding available to it.[3] In the 1980s, mathematician Serge Lang successfully blocked influential political scientist Samuel P. Huntington's admission to the US National Academy of Sciences, describing Huntington's use of mathematics to quantify the relationship between factors such as "social frustration" (Lang asked Huntington if he possessed a "social-frustration meter") as "pseudoscience".[11][26][27] During the late 2000s recessions, social science was disproportionately targeted for funding cuts compared to mathematics and natural science.[28][29] Proposals were made for the United States' National Science Foundation to cease funding disciplines such as political science altogether.[16][30] Both of these incidents prompted critical discussion of the distinction between hard and soft sciences.[11][16]

The perception of hard vs soft science is influenced by gender bias with a higher proportion of women in a given field leading to a "soft" perception even within STEM fields. This perception of softness is accompanied by a devaluation of the field's worth.[31]

See also edit

Notes edit

  1. ^ Comte viewed astronomy as studying the physics of the entire cosmos, calling it "celestial physics". He classified the rest of physics (under the modern definition) as "terrestrial physics", which was therefore less general.

References edit

  1. ^ a b "In praise of soft science". Nature. 435 (7045): 1003–2005. 2005. doi:10.1038/4351003a. PMID 15973363.
  2. ^ a b c d e Wilson, Timothy D. (12 July 2012). "'Soft' sciences don't deserve the snobbery". Los Angeles Times. Retrieved 19 December 2012.
  3. ^ a b c Frost, Pamela. "Soft science and hard news". Columbia University. Metanews. Retrieved 10 August 2009.
  4. ^ Helmenstine, Anne Marie (29 November 2019). "What Is the Difference Between Hard and Soft Science?". ThoughtCo.
  5. ^ a b c d e Smith LD, Best LA, Stubbs A, Johnston J, Archibald AB (2000). "Scientific Graphs and the Hierarchy of the Sciences". Social Studies of Science. 30 (1): 73–94. doi:10.1177/030631200030001003. S2CID 145685575.
  6. ^ a b c d e Cole, Stephen (1983). "The Hierarchy of the Sciences?". American Journal of Sociology. 89 (1): 111–139. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.1033.9702. doi:10.1086/227835. JSTOR 2779049. S2CID 144920176.
  7. ^ a b Fanelli D (2010). ""Positive" results increase down the Hierarchy of the Sciences". PLOS ONE. 5 (4): e10068. Bibcode:2010PLoSO...510068F. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0010068. PMC 2850928. PMID 20383332.
  8. ^ Lemons, John (1996). Scientific Uncertainty and Environmental Problem Solving. Blackwell. p. 99. ISBN 978-0865424760.
  9. ^ Rose, Steven (1997). "Chapter One". Lifelines: Biology Beyond Determinism. Oxford: Oxford University Press. ISBN 9780195120356.
  10. ^ Gutting, Gary (17 May 2012). "How Reliable Are the Social Sciences?". The New York Times. Retrieved 19 December 2012.
  11. ^ a b c Diamond, Jared (August 1987). . Discover. Archived from the original on 13 December 2012. Retrieved 19 December 2012.
  12. ^ Hedges, Larry (1 May 1987). "How hard is hard science, how soft is soft science? The empirical cumulativeness of research". American Psychologist. 42 (5): 443–455. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.408.2317. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.42.5.443.
  13. ^ Lodahl, Janice Beyer; Gordon, Gerald (1972). "The Structure of Scientific Fields and the Functioning of University Graduate Departments". American Sociological Review. 37 (1): 57–72. doi:10.2307/2093493. JSTOR 2093493.
  14. ^ Latour, B. (1990). "Drawing things together". In M. Lynch; S. Woolgar (eds.). Representation in scientific practice. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. pp. 19–68.
  15. ^ Bentley, R. A. (2008). Allen, Colin (ed.). "Random Drift versus Selection in Academic Vocabulary: An Evolutionary Analysis of Published Keywords". PLOS ONE. 3 (8): e3057. arXiv:0807.1182. Bibcode:2008PLoSO...3.3057B. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0003057. PMC 2518107. PMID 18728786.
  16. ^ a b c d "A different agenda". Nature. 487 (7407): 271. 2012. Bibcode:2012Natur.487Q.271.. doi:10.1038/487271a. PMID 22810654.
  17. ^ Winkworth, Thos. (29 October 1858). "Journal of the Society of Arts, Vol. 6, no. 310]". The Journal of the Society of Arts. 6 (310): 697–706. JSTOR 41323682.
  18. ^ "hard, adj. and n.". Oxford English Dictionary (3rd ed.). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. June 2015. Retrieved 10 August 2018.
  19. ^ Platt, J. R. (16 October 1964). "Strong Inference: Certain systematic methods of scientific thinking may produce much more rapid progress than others". Science. 146 (3642): 347–353. doi:10.1126/science.146.3642.347. ISSN 0036-8075. PMID 17739513.
  20. ^ VanLandingham, Mark (2014). "On the Hard and Soft Sciences in Public Health". Public Health Reports. 129 (2): 124–126. doi:10.1177/003335491412900204. ISSN 0033-3549. PMC 3904890. PMID 24587545.
  21. ^ Storer, N. W. (1967). "The hard sciences and the soft: some sociological observations". Bull Med Libr Assoc. 55 (1): 75–84. PMC 198502. PMID 6016373.
  22. ^ Simonton DK (2004). "Psychology's Status as a Scientific Discipline: Its Empirical Placement Within an Implicit Hierarchy of the Sciences". Review of General Psychology. 8 (1): 59–67. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.8.1.59. S2CID 145134072.
  23. ^ Cleveland WS (1984). "Graphs in Scientific Publications". The American Statistician. 38 (4): 261–269. doi:10.2307/2683400. JSTOR 2683400.
  24. ^ Fanelli D, Glänzel W (2013). "Bibliometric Evidence for a Hierarchy of the Sciences". PLOS ONE. 8 (6): e66938. Bibcode:2013PLoSO...866938F. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0066938. PMC 3694152. PMID 23840557.
  25. ^ Berezow, Alex B. (13 July 2012). "Why psychology isn't science". Los Angeles Times. Retrieved 19 December 2012.
  26. ^ Johnson, George; Laura Mansnerus (3 May 1987). "Science Academy Rejects Harvard Political Scientist". The New York Times. Retrieved 19 December 2012.
  27. ^ Change, Kenneth; Warren Leary (25 September 2005). "Serge Lang, 78, a Gadfly and Mathematical Theorist, Dies". The New York Times. Retrieved 19 December 2012.
  28. ^ Richardson, Hannah (26 October 2010). "Humanities to lose English universities teaching grant". BBC News. Retrieved 19 December 2012.
  29. ^ Jump, Paul (20 January 2011). "Social science emulates scientific method to escape retrenchment". Times Higher Education. Retrieved 19 December 2012.
  30. ^ Lane, Charles (4 June 2012). . The Washington Post. Archived from the original on 29 October 2013. Retrieved 19 December 2012.
  31. ^ Light, Alysson. "More women in a STEM field leads people to label it as a 'soft science,' according to new research". theconversation.com. The Conversation. Retrieved 25 January 2022.

hard, soft, science, hard, science, soft, science, colloquial, terms, used, compare, scientific, fields, basis, perceived, methodological, rigor, exactitude, objectivity, general, formal, sciences, natural, sciences, considered, hard, science, whereas, social,. Hard science and soft science are colloquial terms used to compare scientific fields on the basis of perceived methodological rigor exactitude and objectivity 1 2 3 In general the formal sciences and natural sciences are considered hard science whereas the social sciences and other sciences are described as soft science 4 Precise definitions vary 5 but features often cited as characteristic of hard science include producing testable predictions performing controlled experiments relying on quantifiable data and mathematical models a high degree of accuracy and objectivity higher levels of consensus faster progression of the field greater explanatory success cumulativeness replicability and generally applying a purer form of the scientific method 2 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 A closely related idea originating in the nineteenth century with Auguste Comte is that scientific disciplines can be arranged into a hierarchy of hard to soft on the basis of factors such as rigor development and whether they are basic or applied 5 13 Philosophers and historians of science have questioned the relationship between these characteristics and perceived hardness or softness The more developed hard sciences do not necessarily have a greater degree of consensus or selectivity in accepting new results 6 Commonly cited methodological differences are also not a reliable indicator For example social sciences such as psychology and sociology use mathematical models extensively but are usually considered soft sciences 1 2 However there are some measurable differences between hard and soft sciences For example hard sciences make more extensive use of graphs 5 14 and soft sciences are more prone to a rapid turnover of buzzwords 15 The metaphor has been criticised for unduly stigmatizing soft sciences creating an unwarranted imbalance in the public perception funding and recognition of different fields 2 3 16 Contents 1 History of the terms 2 Empirical support 3 Criticism 4 See also 5 Notes 6 ReferencesHistory of the terms editThe origin of the terms hard science and soft science is obscure The earliest attested use of hard science is found in an 1858 issue of the Journal of the Society of Arts 17 18 but the idea of a hierarchy of the sciences can be found earlier in the work of the French philosopher Auguste Comte 1798 1857 He identified astronomy as the most general science note 1 followed by physics chemistry biology then sociology This view was highly influential and was intended to classify fields based on their degree of intellectual development and the complexity of their subject matter 6 The modern distinction between hard and soft science is often attributed to a 1964 article published in Science by John R Platt He explored why he considered some scientific fields to be more productive than others though he did not actually use the terms themselves 19 20 In 1967 sociologist of science Norman W Storer specifically distinguished between the natural sciences as hard and the social sciences as soft He defined hardness in terms of the degree to which a field uses mathematics and described a trend of scientific fields increasing in hardness over time identifying features of increased hardness as including better integration and organization of knowledge an improved ability to detect errors and an increase in the difficulty of learning the subject 6 21 Empirical support editIn the 1970s sociologist Stephen Cole conducted a number of empirical studies attempting to find evidence for a hierarchy of scientific disciplines and was unable to find significant differences in terms of core of knowledge degree of codification or research material Differences that he did find evidence for included a tendency for textbooks in soft sciences to rely on more recent work while the material in textbooks from the hard sciences was more consistent over time 6 After he published in 1983 it has been suggested that Cole might have missed some relationships in the data because he studied individual measurements without accounting for the way multiple measurements could trend in the same direction and because not all the criteria that could indicate a discipline s scientific status were analysed 22 In 1984 Cleveland performed a survey of 57 journals and found that natural science journals used many more graphs than journals in mathematics or social science and that social science journals often presented large amounts of observational data in the absence of graphs The amount of page area used for graphs ranged from 0 to 31 and the variation was primarily due to the number of graphs included rather than their sizes 23 Further analyses by Smith in 2000 5 based on samples of graphs from journals in seven major scientific disciplines found that the amount of graph usage correlated almost perfectly with hardness r 0 97 They also suggested that the hierarchy applies to individual fields and demonstrated the same result using ten subfields of psychology r 0 93 5 In a 2010 article Fanelli proposed that we expect more positive outcomes in softer sciences because there are fewer constraints on researcher bias They found that among research papers that tested a hypothesis the frequency of positive results was predicted by the perceived hardness of the field For example the social sciences as a whole had a 2 3 fold increased odds of positive results compared to the physical sciences with the biological sciences in between They added that this supported the idea that the social sciences and natural sciences differ only in degree as long as the social sciences follow the scientific approach 7 In 2013 Fanelli tested whether the ability of researchers in a field to achieve consensus and accumulate knowledge increases with the hardness of the science and sampled 29 000 papers from 12 disciplines using measurements that indicate the degree of scholarly consensus Out of the three possibilities hierarchy hard soft distinction or no ordering the results supported a hierarchy with physical sciences performing the best followed by biological sciences and then social sciences The results also held within disciplines as well as when mathematics and the humanities were included 24 Criticism editCritics of the concept argue that soft sciences are implicitly considered to be less legitimate scientific fields 2 or simply not scientific at all 25 An editorial in Nature stated that social science findings are more likely to intersect with everyday experience and may be dismissed as obvious or insignificant as a result 16 Being labelled a soft science can affect the perceived value of a discipline to society and the amount of funding available to it 3 In the 1980s mathematician Serge Lang successfully blocked influential political scientist Samuel P Huntington s admission to the US National Academy of Sciences describing Huntington s use of mathematics to quantify the relationship between factors such as social frustration Lang asked Huntington if he possessed a social frustration meter as pseudoscience 11 26 27 During the late 2000s recessions social science was disproportionately targeted for funding cuts compared to mathematics and natural science 28 29 Proposals were made for the United States National Science Foundation to cease funding disciplines such as political science altogether 16 30 Both of these incidents prompted critical discussion of the distinction between hard and soft sciences 11 16 The perception of hard vs soft science is influenced by gender bias with a higher proportion of women in a given field leading to a soft perception even within STEM fields This perception of softness is accompanied by a devaluation of the field s worth 31 See also editDemarcation problem Exact sciences Hard and soft science fiction History of science Methodological dualism Non science Philosophy of social science Positivism dispute STEM fieldsNotes edit Comte viewed astronomy as studying the physics of the entire cosmos calling it celestial physics He classified the rest of physics under the modern definition as terrestrial physics which was therefore less general References edit a b In praise of soft science Nature 435 7045 1003 2005 2005 doi 10 1038 4351003a PMID 15973363 a b c d e Wilson Timothy D 12 July 2012 Soft sciences don t deserve the snobbery Los Angeles Times Retrieved 19 December 2012 a b c Frost Pamela Soft science and hard news Columbia University Metanews Retrieved 10 August 2009 Helmenstine Anne Marie 29 November 2019 What Is the Difference Between Hard and Soft Science ThoughtCo a b c d e Smith LD Best LA Stubbs A Johnston J Archibald AB 2000 Scientific Graphs and the Hierarchy of the Sciences Social Studies of Science 30 1 73 94 doi 10 1177 030631200030001003 S2CID 145685575 a b c d e Cole Stephen 1983 The Hierarchy of the Sciences American Journal of Sociology 89 1 111 139 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 1033 9702 doi 10 1086 227835 JSTOR 2779049 S2CID 144920176 a b Fanelli D 2010 Positive results increase down the Hierarchy of the Sciences PLOS ONE 5 4 e10068 Bibcode 2010PLoSO 510068F doi 10 1371 journal pone 0010068 PMC 2850928 PMID 20383332 Lemons John 1996 Scientific Uncertainty and Environmental Problem Solving Blackwell p 99 ISBN 978 0865424760 Rose Steven 1997 Chapter One Lifelines Biology Beyond Determinism Oxford Oxford University Press ISBN 9780195120356 Gutting Gary 17 May 2012 How Reliable Are the Social Sciences The New York Times Retrieved 19 December 2012 a b c Diamond Jared August 1987 Soft sciences are often harder than hard sciences Discover Archived from the original on 13 December 2012 Retrieved 19 December 2012 Hedges Larry 1 May 1987 How hard is hard science how soft is soft science The empirical cumulativeness of research American Psychologist 42 5 443 455 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 408 2317 doi 10 1037 0003 066X 42 5 443 Lodahl Janice Beyer Gordon Gerald 1972 The Structure of Scientific Fields and the Functioning of University Graduate Departments American Sociological Review 37 1 57 72 doi 10 2307 2093493 JSTOR 2093493 Latour B 1990 Drawing things together In M Lynch S Woolgar eds Representation in scientific practice Cambridge MA MIT Press pp 19 68 Bentley R A 2008 Allen Colin ed Random Drift versus Selection in Academic Vocabulary An Evolutionary Analysis of Published Keywords PLOS ONE 3 8 e3057 arXiv 0807 1182 Bibcode 2008PLoSO 3 3057B doi 10 1371 journal pone 0003057 PMC 2518107 PMID 18728786 a b c d A different agenda Nature 487 7407 271 2012 Bibcode 2012Natur 487Q 271 doi 10 1038 487271a PMID 22810654 Winkworth Thos 29 October 1858 Journal of the Society of Arts Vol 6 no 310 The Journal of the Society of Arts 6 310 697 706 JSTOR 41323682 hard adj and n Oxford English Dictionary 3rd ed Oxford England Oxford University Press June 2015 Retrieved 10 August 2018 Platt J R 16 October 1964 Strong Inference Certain systematic methods of scientific thinking may produce much more rapid progress than others Science 146 3642 347 353 doi 10 1126 science 146 3642 347 ISSN 0036 8075 PMID 17739513 VanLandingham Mark 2014 On the Hard and Soft Sciences in Public Health Public Health Reports 129 2 124 126 doi 10 1177 003335491412900204 ISSN 0033 3549 PMC 3904890 PMID 24587545 Storer N W 1967 The hard sciences and the soft some sociological observations Bull Med Libr Assoc 55 1 75 84 PMC 198502 PMID 6016373 Simonton DK 2004 Psychology s Status as a Scientific Discipline Its Empirical Placement Within an Implicit Hierarchy of the Sciences Review of General Psychology 8 1 59 67 doi 10 1037 1089 2680 8 1 59 S2CID 145134072 Cleveland WS 1984 Graphs in Scientific Publications The American Statistician 38 4 261 269 doi 10 2307 2683400 JSTOR 2683400 Fanelli D Glanzel W 2013 Bibliometric Evidence for a Hierarchy of the Sciences PLOS ONE 8 6 e66938 Bibcode 2013PLoSO 866938F doi 10 1371 journal pone 0066938 PMC 3694152 PMID 23840557 Berezow Alex B 13 July 2012 Why psychology isn t science Los Angeles Times Retrieved 19 December 2012 Johnson George Laura Mansnerus 3 May 1987 Science Academy Rejects Harvard Political Scientist The New York Times Retrieved 19 December 2012 Change Kenneth Warren Leary 25 September 2005 Serge Lang 78 a Gadfly and Mathematical Theorist Dies The New York Times Retrieved 19 December 2012 Richardson Hannah 26 October 2010 Humanities to lose English universities teaching grant BBC News Retrieved 19 December 2012 Jump Paul 20 January 2011 Social science emulates scientific method to escape retrenchment Times Higher Education Retrieved 19 December 2012 Lane Charles 4 June 2012 Congress should cut funding for political science research The Washington Post Archived from the original on 29 October 2013 Retrieved 19 December 2012 Light Alysson More women in a STEM field leads people to label it as a soft science according to new research theconversation com The Conversation Retrieved 25 January 2022 Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Hard and soft science amp oldid 1199653278, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.