fbpx
Wikipedia

Social experiment

A social experiment is a type of psychological or sociological research for testing people's reactions to certain situations or events. The experiment depends on a particular social approach where the main source of information is the point of view and knowledge of the participants of the experiment. To carry out a social experiment, specialists usually split participants into two groups — active participants (people who take action in particular events) and respondents (people who react to the action). Throughout the experiment, participants are monitored by specialists to identify the effects and differences as a result of the experiment. Intentional communities are generally considered as social experiments.[1]

Social psychology offers insight into how individuals act in groups and how behavior is affected by social burdens and pressures.[2] In most social experiments, the subjects are unaware that they are partaking in an experiment. Several "actors" or "plants" are used to study social behaviors. Social experiments have also been used by companies to collect data about consumers and their opinions about a product or a particular topic.[3]

History

In 1895,[4] American psychologist Norman Triplett constructed one of the earliest known social experiments, in which he found out that cyclists managed to ride a bike faster when racing against another person rather than racing against the clock. He duplicated the experiment in a laboratory using children and fishing reels and received similar results.[5] Field social experiments had proved to be efficient as it reflects real life due to its natural setting.[6]

The social experiments that are commonly referred to today were conducted decades later, in which an experiment is done in a controlled environment such as a laboratory. An example of this is Stanley Milgram's obedience experiment in 1963.[7] Social experiments began in the United States as a test of the negative income tax concept in the late 1960s and since then have been conducted on all the populated continents.[8]

During the 1970s, criticism of the ethics and accusations of gender and racial bias led to a reassessment of both the field of social psychology and the conducted experiments. While experimental methods were still employed, other methods gained popularity.[9]

Ethics

Social experimentation has raised many ethical concerns, due to its manipulation of large groups of the population, often without the consent or knowledge of the subjects.[10] In certain cases, Social experimentation has been staged unknowingly to the viewer to promote the image of the individual or for the pure purpose of generating controversy.[11]

Researchers also believed that the impact of Informal Social Experiments via social media videos may have negative consequences on formal social marketing research as well as the society in general,[12] detailing that while Informal Social Experiments addresses moral and social issues such as child safety, self-confidence, etc., producers of these social experiments might do it for their gain and benefits.[12]

Well-known social experiments

Bystander Apathy (Effect)

Based on the murder of Kitty Genovese outside her home, The New York Times stated that there were 38 witnesses that either saw or heard the fatal stabbing take place, and not a single person came to her aid. Although this number was proven to be exaggerated, this murder was coined "bystander apathy" by social psychologists Bibb Latané and John Darley in 1968.[13]

For their experiment, Latané and Darley[13] tried to replicate the Genovese slaying by having participants aware of each other but unable to communicate directly. Each participant was in a cubicle in contact with each other via a microphone; however, only one voice was allowed to speak at a time. A taped recording played of a participant having an epileptic seizure. When the participant believed themselves to be alone they invariably attempted to find help. When the participant believed others were around the speed and frequency of response declined significantly. The authors concluded that situational factors play an influential role in bystander apathy. People are less likely to help in an emergency if other people are present. Two reasons were offered by Latané and Darley: first is diffusion of responsibility, and second is pluralistic ignorance or the mentality that if nobody else is helping, then I am not needed as well.

Research on bystander apathy by psychologist Kyle Thomas et al. found that people's decisions to help are influenced by their level of knowledge. While the diffusion of responsibility and pluralistic ignorance are factors, the researchers found that bystanders also consider what they know about other bystanders and the situation before getting involved.[14][15]

HighScope

The HighScope Perry Preschool Project was evaluated in a randomized controlled trial of 123 children (58 were randomly assigned to a treatment group that received the program and a control group of 65 children that did not receive the program).[16] Prior to the program, the preschool and control groups were equivalent in measures of intellectual performance and demographic characteristics. After the program, the educational and life outcomes for the children receiving the program were much superior to outcomes for the children not receiving the program. Many of the program effects were significant or approaching significance.[17][18] At age 40, the participants were interviewed once more, and school, social services, and arrest records were pulled. The participants had higher-earning jobs, committed fewer crimes, and were more likely to hold a job than adults who did not attend preschool.

RAND Health Insurance Experiment

The RAND Health Insurance Experiment was an experimental study of health care costs, utilization, and outcomes in the United States which assigned people randomly to different kinds of plans and followed their behavior from 1974 to 1982.[19] As a result, it provided stronger evidence than studies that examine people afterward who were not randomly assigned. It concluded that cost-sharing reduced "inappropriate or unnecessary" medical care (overutilization), but also reduced "appropriate or needed" medical care. It did not have enough statistical power to tell whether people who got less appropriate or needed care were more likely to die as a result.

Oportunidades/Prospera/Progresa

Oportunidades is a government social assistance (welfare) program in Mexico founded in 2002, based on a previous program called Progresa created in 1997.[20] It is designed to target poverty by providing cash payments to families in exchange for regular school attendance, health clinic visits, and nutrition support.[21] Oportunidades is credited with decreasing poverty and improving health and educational attainment in regions where it has been deployed.[22]

Moving to Opportunity

Moving to Opportunity for Fair Housing was a randomized social experiment sponsored by the United States Department of Housing and Urban Development in the 1990s among 4600 low-income families with children living in high-poverty public housing projects.[23] The program was designed based on the assumption that households benefit from living in higher-opportunity neighborhoods. Early evaluations of the MTO program, however, showed minimal gains for participant families. One explanation for these findings is the short length of time that MTO families typically spent in lower-poverty neighborhoods; the positive effects of longer-term exposure to low-poverty neighborhoods appear more promising.[24]

Stanford prison experiment

The Stanford prison experiment was a study of the psychological effects of becoming a prisoner or prison guard. The experiment was conducted at Stanford University on August 14–20, 1971, by a team of researchers led by psychology professor Philip Zimbardo using college students.[25] It was funded by the U.S. Office of Naval Research[26] and was of interest to both the U.S. Navy and Marine Corps as an investigation into the causes of conflict between military guards and prisoners. The experiment is a study on the psychology of imprisonment[27] and is a topic covered in most introductory psychology textbooks.[28]

Experiments by Muzafer Sherif

Sherif was a founder of modern social psychology who developed several techniques for understanding social processes, particularly social norms, and social conflict. Sherif's experimental study of the autokinetic movement demonstrated how mental evaluation norms were created by human beings.[29] Sherif is equally famous for the Robbers Cave Experiments. This series of experiments, begun in Connecticut and concluded in Oklahoma, took boys from intact middle-class families, who were carefully screened to be psychologically normal, delivered them to a summer camp setting (with researchers doubling as counsellors), and created social groups that came into conflict with each other.

Bobo doll experiment

The Bobo doll experiment was a study carried out by Albert Bandura who was a professor at Stanford University. It focused on the study of aggression using three groups of preschoolers as the subjects. Bandura took inflatable plastic toys called Bobo dolls and weighted them down to always stand upright. The preschoolers were divided into three groups by gender, and then into six subgroups. One of the groups would observe an adult act aggressively towards the Bobo doll, another group would observe an adult with non-aggressive behaviours, and the last group would not be exposed to any behaviour models. The study found the preschoolers exposed to the aggressive behaviour had imitated the aggressiveness towards the doll, regardless of gender. The other two groups showed significantly less hostility towards the doll. The study had shown aggressive and non-aggressive behaviours were learned by observing others and had a significant effect on the subjects even after the study was concluded.[30]

Stanford marshmallow experiment

The Stanford marshmallow experiment was a study done by a psychologist Walter Mischel on delayed gratification in the early 1970s. During the three studies, a child was offered a choice between one small reward provided immediately or two small rewards if they waited for a short period, approximately 15 minutes, during which the tester left the room and then returned. (The reward was sometimes a marshmallow, but often a cookie or a pretzel.) In follow-up studies, the researchers found that children who were able to wait longer for the preferred rewards tended to have better life outcomes, as measured by SAT scores, educational attainment, body mass index (BMI), and other life measures.[31][32]

Asch Conformity experiment

The Asch experiment took place in the Swarthmore College in 1951. Solomon Asch conducted an experiment to investigate the extent where social pressure from a majority group could affect a person to conform.[33] Asch took 50 people from the college to participate in a vision test. They were paired up with 7 other people who they believed to be random, but instead were part of a control group who would choose the same answers. The real participant would give his or her answers last. Out of the 18 trials, the group gave the wrong answers 12 times. 75% of the participants conformed once or more, and the remaining 25% never conformed to the group's wrong answers. Participants were interviewed after the experiment, and although they knew the answers were wrong, they conformed to not be ridiculed by the group. A few individuals said they thought the group's answers were correct. Asch concluded that people conformed because they either wanted to fit in or thought the group was right.[34]

Hawthorne experiment

The Hawthorne experiment took place in 1924 in the city of Chicago. Elton Mayo is widely known as the person behind the project. However, his involvement started in 1928 after he was invited by George Pennock, the assistant works manager for the Hawthorne plant of Western Electric. During the experiment, workers were separated into two groups to study the effects of different incentives on their productivity. Different variations were tested, such as changing the light levels in the rooms. Other, more obvious incentives, such as monetary incentives and rest pauses, were also tested and seemed to show positive results.[35] Several conclusions were made, after the experiments finished:

  • When employees had more freedom to choose their own conditions and output standards, their productivity increased;
  • Social interaction played an important role in the creation of a high level of group cohesion;
  • People tend to put more effort when they feel their worth and cooperate with each other.[35]

Halo Effect

The Halo Effect was first developed and empirically examined by an American psychologist named Edward Thorndike in his 1920 piece "A Constant Error in Psychological Ratings". The halo effect, also called the halo error, refers to a type of cognitive bias in which we perceive people as better due to their other related traits. [36]

A typical halo effect example is the attractiveness stereotype, which refers to ascribing positive qualities to physically attractive people. Often, attractive individuals are believed to have lower mortality rates, better mental health, and higher intelligence. However, this is a cognitive error based on one's inclinations, beliefs, and social perception.   The halo effect is a well-known social psychology discovery. It is the concept that a person's overall evaluations (e.g., she is pleasant) bleed over into judgments regarding their characteristics (e.g. she is intelligent).[37] It's also known as the "beauty is good" notion or the "physical attractiveness" stereotype.

Informal social experiments

References

  1. ^ Pitzer, D. E. (1989). "Developmental communalism: An alternative approach to communal studies". Utopian Thought and Communal Experience: 68–76.
  2. ^ Leuba, James H.; McDougall, William (April 1909). "An Introduction to Social Psychology". The American Journal of Psychology. 20 (2): 285. doi:10.2307/1413301. JSTOR 1413301.
  3. ^ "10 marketing experiments to improve your reach on social media". Learn. 2019-02-26. Retrieved 2019-07-19.
  4. ^ "Social Facilitation and Social Inhibition". Open Textbooks for Hong Kong. 2015-07-20. Retrieved 2021-05-22.
  5. ^ "Social Facilitation | Simply Psychology". www.simplypsychology.org. Retrieved 2019-07-19.
  6. ^ "Experimental Methods in Psychology | Simply Psychology". www.simplypsychology.org. Retrieved 2019-07-19.
  7. ^ Brannigan, Augustine (2013-12-01). "Stanley Milgram's Obedience Experiments: A Report Card 50 Years Later". Society. 50 (6): 623–628. doi:10.1007/s12115-013-9724-3. ISSN 1936-4725.
  8. ^ Kershaw, David N. (1972). "A Negative-Income-Tax Experiment". Scientific American. 227 (4): 19–25. Bibcode:1972SciAm.227d..19K. doi:10.1038/scientificamerican1072-19. ISSN 0036-8733. JSTOR 24922887. S2CID 121103670.
  9. ^ T D Cook; Shadish, and W. R. (1994). "Social Experiments: Some Developments over the Past Fifteen Years". Annual Review of Psychology. 45 (1): 545–580. doi:10.1146/annurev.ps.45.020194.002553.
  10. ^ Humphreys, Macartan (2015-06-01). "Reflections on the Ethics of Social Experimentation" (PDF). Journal of Globalization and Development. 6 (1). doi:10.1515/jgd-2014-0016. ISSN 1948-1837. S2CID 155345823.
  11. ^ Chen, Ted; Klien, Asher. "Over Hoax Claims, Homeless Man and YouTuber Insist Their Viral Video Is Genuine". NBC. No. 2/01/15. Retrieved 24 November 2019.
  12. ^ a b Manna, Valerie A. (August 2017). The ethics of informal social experiments. Macromarketing Society Inc. and the Marketing Department, University of Otago. hdl:10182/9574. ISBN 978-0-473-39583-4.
  13. ^ a b Latane, Bibb; John M. Darley (June 1, 1970). The Unresponsive Bystander: Why Doesn't He Help?. Prentice Hal. ISBN 0139386130.
  14. ^ Thomas, Kyle A.; De Freitas, Julian; DeScioli, Peter; Pinker, Steven (May 2016). "Recursive mentalizing and common knowledge in the bystander effect". Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 145 (5): 621–629. doi:10.1037/xge0000153. ISSN 1939-2222. PMID 26913616.
  15. ^ Thomas, Kyle A.; De Freitas, Julian; DeScioli, Peter; Pinker, Steven (2016). "Recursive Mentalizing and Common Knowledge in the Bystander Effect". Journal of Experimental Psychology. 145 (5): 621–629. doi:10.1037/xge0000153. PMID 26913616.
  16. ^ Schweinhart, Lawrence J. (2010), Reynolds, Arthur J.; Rolnick, Arthur J.; Temple, Judy A.; Englund, Michelle M. (eds.), "The Challenge of the HighScope Perry Preschool Study", Childhood Programs and Practices in the First Decade of Life: A Human Capital Integration, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 157–167, doi:10.1017/cbo9780511762666.008, ISBN 978-0-521-19846-2, retrieved 2021-05-22
  17. ^ Significant Benefits, The High/Scope Perry Preschool Study Through Age 27[citation needed]
  18. ^ Coalition for Evidence-Based Policy: Social Programs That Work: Perry Preschool Project[citation needed]
  19. ^ Brook, Robert H.; Keeler, Emmett B.; Lohr, Kathleen N.; Newhouse, Joseph P.; Ware, John E.; Rogers, William H.; Davies, Allyson Ross; Sherbourne, Cathy D.; Goldberg, George A.; Camp, Patricia; Kamberg, Caren (2006-12-06). "The Health Insurance Experiment: A Classic RAND Study Speaks to the Current Health Care Reform Debate". {{cite journal}}: Cite journal requires |journal= (help)
  20. ^ Paying for Better Parenting, New York Times, Accessed 12/07/06
  21. ^ Mexico's Oportunidades Program Accessed 12/07/06
  22. ^ Bulletin of the World Health Organization - Reaching Mexico's poorest Accessed 12/07/06
  23. ^ "Moving To Opportunity". NBER. Retrieved 2021-05-23.
  24. ^ Blumenberg, Evelyn; Pierce, Gregory (December 2017). "Car Access and Long-term Poverty Exposure: Evidence from the Moving to Opportunity (MTO) Experiment". Journal of Transport Geography. 65: 92–100. doi:10.1016/j.jtrangeo.2017.10.009.
  25. ^ "8. Conclusion". Stanford Prison Experiment.
  26. ^ FAQ on official site Archived 2012-09-09 at archive.today
  27. ^ . Archived from the original on 2001-11-05. Retrieved 2015-04-19.
  28. ^ "Intro to psychology textbooks gloss over criticisms of Zimbardo's Stanford Prison Experiment". 7 September 2014.
  29. ^ Jaan Valsiner (2001). Comparative study of human cultural development. Fundacion Infancia y Aprendizaje. ISBN 9788495264015.
  30. ^ "Bobo doll experiment | Description, Methodology, Results, & Facts | Britannica".
  31. ^ "Delaying Gratification" (PDF). Apa.org. Retrieved 17 December 2018.
  32. ^ "Marshmallow Test Experiment | Simply Psychology". www.simplypsychology.org. Retrieved 2021-05-23.
  33. ^ McLeod, Saul. "Asch Experiment". Simply Psychology. Retrieved 16 December 2018.
  34. ^ "1951 Psychologist Solomon Asch's Famous Experiments". www.swarthmore.edu. 2016-06-15. Retrieved 2021-05-23.
  35. ^ a b "Elton Mayo: The Hawthorne Experiments Thinker". The British Library. Retrieved 2018-02-26.
  36. ^ "Halo Effect: Definition and Examples | Simply Psychology". www.simplypsychology.org. Retrieved 2021-08-10.
  37. ^ "Social Psychology Experiments: 10 Of The Most Brilliant Studies". PsyBlog. 2021-06-01. Retrieved 2021-08-10.

social, experiment, confused, with, social, psychology, methods, social, experiment, type, psychological, sociological, research, testing, people, reactions, certain, situations, events, experiment, depends, particular, social, approach, where, main, source, i. Not to be confused with Social psychology Methods A social experiment is a type of psychological or sociological research for testing people s reactions to certain situations or events The experiment depends on a particular social approach where the main source of information is the point of view and knowledge of the participants of the experiment To carry out a social experiment specialists usually split participants into two groups active participants people who take action in particular events and respondents people who react to the action Throughout the experiment participants are monitored by specialists to identify the effects and differences as a result of the experiment Intentional communities are generally considered as social experiments 1 Social psychology offers insight into how individuals act in groups and how behavior is affected by social burdens and pressures 2 In most social experiments the subjects are unaware that they are partaking in an experiment Several actors or plants are used to study social behaviors Social experiments have also been used by companies to collect data about consumers and their opinions about a product or a particular topic 3 Contents 1 History 2 Ethics 3 Well known social experiments 3 1 Bystander Apathy Effect 3 2 HighScope 3 3 RAND Health Insurance Experiment 3 4 Oportunidades Prospera Progresa 3 5 Moving to Opportunity 3 6 Stanford prison experiment 3 7 Experiments by Muzafer Sherif 3 8 Bobo doll experiment 3 9 Stanford marshmallow experiment 3 10 Asch Conformity experiment 3 11 Hawthorne experiment 3 12 Halo Effect 4 Informal social experiments 5 ReferencesHistory EditIn 1895 4 American psychologist Norman Triplett constructed one of the earliest known social experiments in which he found out that cyclists managed to ride a bike faster when racing against another person rather than racing against the clock He duplicated the experiment in a laboratory using children and fishing reels and received similar results 5 Field social experiments had proved to be efficient as it reflects real life due to its natural setting 6 The social experiments that are commonly referred to today were conducted decades later in which an experiment is done in a controlled environment such as a laboratory An example of this is Stanley Milgram s obedience experiment in 1963 7 Social experiments began in the United States as a test of the negative income tax concept in the late 1960s and since then have been conducted on all the populated continents 8 During the 1970s criticism of the ethics and accusations of gender and racial bias led to a reassessment of both the field of social psychology and the conducted experiments While experimental methods were still employed other methods gained popularity 9 Ethics EditSocial experimentation has raised many ethical concerns due to its manipulation of large groups of the population often without the consent or knowledge of the subjects 10 In certain cases Social experimentation has been staged unknowingly to the viewer to promote the image of the individual or for the pure purpose of generating controversy 11 Researchers also believed that the impact of Informal Social Experiments via social media videos may have negative consequences on formal social marketing research as well as the society in general 12 detailing that while Informal Social Experiments addresses moral and social issues such as child safety self confidence etc producers of these social experiments might do it for their gain and benefits 12 Well known social experiments EditBystander Apathy Effect Edit Main article Bystander effect Based on the murder of Kitty Genovese outside her home The New York Times stated that there were 38 witnesses that either saw or heard the fatal stabbing take place and not a single person came to her aid Although this number was proven to be exaggerated this murder was coined bystander apathy by social psychologists Bibb Latane and John Darley in 1968 13 For their experiment Latane and Darley 13 tried to replicate the Genovese slaying by having participants aware of each other but unable to communicate directly Each participant was in a cubicle in contact with each other via a microphone however only one voice was allowed to speak at a time A taped recording played of a participant having an epileptic seizure When the participant believed themselves to be alone they invariably attempted to find help When the participant believed others were around the speed and frequency of response declined significantly The authors concluded that situational factors play an influential role in bystander apathy People are less likely to help in an emergency if other people are present Two reasons were offered by Latane and Darley first is diffusion of responsibility and second is pluralistic ignorance or the mentality that if nobody else is helping then I am not needed as well Research on bystander apathy by psychologist Kyle Thomas et al found that people s decisions to help are influenced by their level of knowledge While the diffusion of responsibility and pluralistic ignorance are factors the researchers found that bystanders also consider what they know about other bystanders and the situation before getting involved 14 15 HighScope Edit Main article HighScope The HighScope Perry Preschool Project was evaluated in a randomized controlled trial of 123 children 58 were randomly assigned to a treatment group that received the program and a control group of 65 children that did not receive the program 16 Prior to the program the preschool and control groups were equivalent in measures of intellectual performance and demographic characteristics After the program the educational and life outcomes for the children receiving the program were much superior to outcomes for the children not receiving the program Many of the program effects were significant or approaching significance 17 18 At age 40 the participants were interviewed once more and school social services and arrest records were pulled The participants had higher earning jobs committed fewer crimes and were more likely to hold a job than adults who did not attend preschool RAND Health Insurance Experiment Edit Main article RAND Health Insurance Experiment The RAND Health Insurance Experiment was an experimental study of health care costs utilization and outcomes in the United States which assigned people randomly to different kinds of plans and followed their behavior from 1974 to 1982 19 As a result it provided stronger evidence than studies that examine people afterward who were not randomly assigned It concluded that cost sharing reduced inappropriate or unnecessary medical care overutilization but also reduced appropriate or needed medical care It did not have enough statistical power to tell whether people who got less appropriate or needed care were more likely to die as a result Oportunidades Prospera Progresa Edit Main article Oportunidades Oportunidades is a government social assistance welfare program in Mexico founded in 2002 based on a previous program called Progresa created in 1997 20 It is designed to target poverty by providing cash payments to families in exchange for regular school attendance health clinic visits and nutrition support 21 Oportunidades is credited with decreasing poverty and improving health and educational attainment in regions where it has been deployed 22 Moving to Opportunity Edit Main article Moving to Opportunity Moving to Opportunity for Fair Housing was a randomized social experiment sponsored by the United States Department of Housing and Urban Development in the 1990s among 4600 low income families with children living in high poverty public housing projects 23 The program was designed based on the assumption that households benefit from living in higher opportunity neighborhoods Early evaluations of the MTO program however showed minimal gains for participant families One explanation for these findings is the short length of time that MTO families typically spent in lower poverty neighborhoods the positive effects of longer term exposure to low poverty neighborhoods appear more promising 24 Stanford prison experiment Edit Main article Stanford prison experiment The Stanford prison experiment was a study of the psychological effects of becoming a prisoner or prison guard The experiment was conducted at Stanford University on August 14 20 1971 by a team of researchers led by psychology professor Philip Zimbardo using college students 25 It was funded by the U S Office of Naval Research 26 and was of interest to both the U S Navy and Marine Corps as an investigation into the causes of conflict between military guards and prisoners The experiment is a study on the psychology of imprisonment 27 and is a topic covered in most introductory psychology textbooks 28 Experiments by Muzafer Sherif Edit Main article Muzafer Sherif Sherif was a founder of modern social psychology who developed several techniques for understanding social processes particularly social norms and social conflict Sherif s experimental study of the autokinetic movement demonstrated how mental evaluation norms were created by human beings 29 Sherif is equally famous for the Robbers Cave Experiments This series of experiments begun in Connecticut and concluded in Oklahoma took boys from intact middle class families who were carefully screened to be psychologically normal delivered them to a summer camp setting with researchers doubling as counsellors and created social groups that came into conflict with each other Bobo doll experiment Edit Main article Bobo doll experiment The Bobo doll experiment was a study carried out by Albert Bandura who was a professor at Stanford University It focused on the study of aggression using three groups of preschoolers as the subjects Bandura took inflatable plastic toys called Bobo dolls and weighted them down to always stand upright The preschoolers were divided into three groups by gender and then into six subgroups One of the groups would observe an adult act aggressively towards the Bobo doll another group would observe an adult with non aggressive behaviours and the last group would not be exposed to any behaviour models The study found the preschoolers exposed to the aggressive behaviour had imitated the aggressiveness towards the doll regardless of gender The other two groups showed significantly less hostility towards the doll The study had shown aggressive and non aggressive behaviours were learned by observing others and had a significant effect on the subjects even after the study was concluded 30 Stanford marshmallow experiment Edit Main article Stanford marshmallow experiment The Stanford marshmallow experiment was a study done by a psychologist Walter Mischel on delayed gratification in the early 1970s During the three studies a child was offered a choice between one small reward provided immediately or two small rewards if they waited for a short period approximately 15 minutes during which the tester left the room and then returned The reward was sometimes a marshmallow but often a cookie or a pretzel In follow up studies the researchers found that children who were able to wait longer for the preferred rewards tended to have better life outcomes as measured by SAT scores educational attainment body mass index BMI and other life measures 31 32 Asch Conformity experiment Edit Main article Asch conformity experiment The Asch experiment took place in the Swarthmore College in 1951 Solomon Asch conducted an experiment to investigate the extent where social pressure from a majority group could affect a person to conform 33 Asch took 50 people from the college to participate in a vision test They were paired up with 7 other people who they believed to be random but instead were part of a control group who would choose the same answers The real participant would give his or her answers last Out of the 18 trials the group gave the wrong answers 12 times 75 of the participants conformed once or more and the remaining 25 never conformed to the group s wrong answers Participants were interviewed after the experiment and although they knew the answers were wrong they conformed to not be ridiculed by the group A few individuals said they thought the group s answers were correct Asch concluded that people conformed because they either wanted to fit in or thought the group was right 34 Hawthorne experiment Edit Main article Hawthorne effect The Hawthorne experiment took place in 1924 in the city of Chicago Elton Mayo is widely known as the person behind the project However his involvement started in 1928 after he was invited by George Pennock the assistant works manager for the Hawthorne plant of Western Electric During the experiment workers were separated into two groups to study the effects of different incentives on their productivity Different variations were tested such as changing the light levels in the rooms Other more obvious incentives such as monetary incentives and rest pauses were also tested and seemed to show positive results 35 Several conclusions were made after the experiments finished When employees had more freedom to choose their own conditions and output standards their productivity increased Social interaction played an important role in the creation of a high level of group cohesion People tend to put more effort when they feel their worth and cooperate with each other 35 Halo Effect Edit Main article Halo effect The Halo Effect was first developed and empirically examined by an American psychologist named Edward Thorndike in his 1920 piece A Constant Error in Psychological Ratings The halo effect also called the halo error refers to a type of cognitive bias in which we perceive people as better due to their other related traits 36 A typical halo effect example is the attractiveness stereotype which refers to ascribing positive qualities to physically attractive people Often attractive individuals are believed to have lower mortality rates better mental health and higher intelligence However this is a cognitive error based on one s inclinations beliefs and social perception The halo effect is a well known social psychology discovery It is the concept that a person s overall evaluations e g she is pleasant bleed over into judgments regarding their characteristics e g she is intelligent 37 It s also known as the beauty is good notion or the physical attractiveness stereotype Informal social experiments EditThe Button Reddit Place Reddit References Edit Pitzer D E 1989 Developmental communalism An alternative approach to communal studies Utopian Thought and Communal Experience 68 76 Leuba James H McDougall William April 1909 An Introduction to Social Psychology The American Journal of Psychology 20 2 285 doi 10 2307 1413301 JSTOR 1413301 10 marketing experiments to improve your reach on social media Learn 2019 02 26 Retrieved 2019 07 19 Social Facilitation and Social Inhibition Open Textbooks for Hong Kong 2015 07 20 Retrieved 2021 05 22 Social Facilitation Simply Psychology www simplypsychology org Retrieved 2019 07 19 Experimental Methods in Psychology Simply Psychology www simplypsychology org Retrieved 2019 07 19 Brannigan Augustine 2013 12 01 Stanley Milgram s Obedience Experiments A Report Card 50 Years Later Society 50 6 623 628 doi 10 1007 s12115 013 9724 3 ISSN 1936 4725 Kershaw David N 1972 A Negative Income Tax Experiment Scientific American 227 4 19 25 Bibcode 1972SciAm 227d 19K doi 10 1038 scientificamerican1072 19 ISSN 0036 8733 JSTOR 24922887 S2CID 121103670 T D Cook Shadish and W R 1994 Social Experiments Some Developments over the Past Fifteen Years Annual Review of Psychology 45 1 545 580 doi 10 1146 annurev ps 45 020194 002553 Humphreys Macartan 2015 06 01 Reflections on the Ethics of Social Experimentation PDF Journal of Globalization and Development 6 1 doi 10 1515 jgd 2014 0016 ISSN 1948 1837 S2CID 155345823 Chen Ted Klien Asher Over Hoax Claims Homeless Man and YouTuber Insist Their Viral Video Is Genuine NBC No 2 01 15 Retrieved 24 November 2019 a b Manna Valerie A August 2017 The ethics of informal social experiments Macromarketing Society Inc and the Marketing Department University of Otago hdl 10182 9574 ISBN 978 0 473 39583 4 a b Latane Bibb John M Darley June 1 1970 The Unresponsive Bystander Why Doesn t He Help Prentice Hal ISBN 0139386130 Thomas Kyle A De Freitas Julian DeScioli Peter Pinker Steven May 2016 Recursive mentalizing and common knowledge in the bystander effect Journal of Experimental Psychology General 145 5 621 629 doi 10 1037 xge0000153 ISSN 1939 2222 PMID 26913616 Thomas Kyle A De Freitas Julian DeScioli Peter Pinker Steven 2016 Recursive Mentalizing and Common Knowledge in the Bystander Effect Journal of Experimental Psychology 145 5 621 629 doi 10 1037 xge0000153 PMID 26913616 Schweinhart Lawrence J 2010 Reynolds Arthur J Rolnick Arthur J Temple Judy A Englund Michelle M eds The Challenge of the HighScope Perry Preschool Study Childhood Programs and Practices in the First Decade of Life A Human Capital Integration Cambridge Cambridge University Press pp 157 167 doi 10 1017 cbo9780511762666 008 ISBN 978 0 521 19846 2 retrieved 2021 05 22 Significant Benefits The High Scope Perry Preschool Study Through Age 27 citation needed Coalition for Evidence Based Policy Social Programs That Work Perry Preschool Project citation needed Brook Robert H Keeler Emmett B Lohr Kathleen N Newhouse Joseph P Ware John E Rogers William H Davies Allyson Ross Sherbourne Cathy D Goldberg George A Camp Patricia Kamberg Caren 2006 12 06 The Health Insurance Experiment A Classic RAND Study Speaks to the Current Health Care Reform Debate a href Template Cite journal html title Template Cite journal cite journal a Cite journal requires journal help Paying for Better Parenting New York Times Accessed 12 07 06 Mexico s Oportunidades Program Accessed 12 07 06 Bulletin of the World Health Organization Reaching Mexico s poorest Accessed 12 07 06 Moving To Opportunity NBER Retrieved 2021 05 23 Blumenberg Evelyn Pierce Gregory December 2017 Car Access and Long term Poverty Exposure Evidence from the Moving to Opportunity MTO Experiment Journal of Transport Geography 65 92 100 doi 10 1016 j jtrangeo 2017 10 009 8 Conclusion Stanford Prison Experiment FAQ on official site Archived 2012 09 09 at archive today Thirty Years Later Stanford Prison Experiment Lives on Archived from the original on 2001 11 05 Retrieved 2015 04 19 Intro to psychology textbooks gloss over criticisms of Zimbardo s Stanford Prison Experiment 7 September 2014 Jaan Valsiner 2001 Comparative study of human cultural development Fundacion Infancia y Aprendizaje ISBN 9788495264015 Bobo doll experiment Description Methodology Results amp Facts Britannica Delaying Gratification PDF Apa org Retrieved 17 December 2018 Marshmallow Test Experiment Simply Psychology www simplypsychology org Retrieved 2021 05 23 McLeod Saul Asch Experiment Simply Psychology Retrieved 16 December 2018 1951 Psychologist Solomon Asch s Famous Experiments www swarthmore edu 2016 06 15 Retrieved 2021 05 23 a b Elton Mayo The Hawthorne Experiments Thinker The British Library Retrieved 2018 02 26 Halo Effect Definition and Examples Simply Psychology www simplypsychology org Retrieved 2021 08 10 Social Psychology Experiments 10 Of The Most Brilliant Studies PsyBlog 2021 06 01 Retrieved 2021 08 10 Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Social experiment amp oldid 1121028893, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.