fbpx
Wikipedia

Numeracy

Numeracy is the ability to understand, reason with, and to apply simple numerical concepts.[1] The charity National Numeracy states: "Numeracy means understanding how mathematics is used in the real world and being able to apply it to make the best possible decisions...It’s as much about thinking and reasoning as about 'doing sums'". Basic numeracy skills consist of comprehending fundamental arithmetical operations like addition, subtraction, multiplication, and division. For example, if one can understand simple mathematical equations such as 2 + 2 = 4, then one would be considered to possess at least basic numeric knowledge. Substantial aspects of numeracy also include number sense, operation sense, computation, measurement, geometry, probability and statistics.[citation needed] A numerically literate person can manage and respond to the mathematical demands of life.[2][3]

Children in Laos have fun as they improve numeracy with "Number Bingo". They roll three dice, construct an equation from the numbers to produce a new number, then cover that number on the board, trying to get four in a row.
Number bingo improves math skills. LPB Laos.

By contrast, innumeracy (the lack of numeracy) can have a negative impact. Numeracy has an influence on healthy behaviors, financial literacy, and career decisions. Therefore, innumeracy may negatively affect economic choices, financial outcomes, health outcomes, and life satisfaction.[3][4][5] It also may distort risk perception in health decisions.[6] Greater numeracy has been associated with reduced susceptibility to framing effects, less influence of nonnumerical information such as mood states, and greater sensitivity to different levels of numerical risk.[7] Ellen Peters and her colleagues argue that achieving the benefits of numeric literacy, however, may depend on one's numeric self-efficacy or confidence in one's skills.[8]

Representation of numbers Edit

Humans have evolved to mentally represent numbers in two major ways from observation (not formal math).[9] These representations are often thought to be innate[10] (see Numerical cognition), to be shared across human cultures,[11] to be common to multiple species,[12] and not to be the result of individual learning or cultural transmission. They are:

  1. Approximate representation of numerical magnitude, and
  2. Precise representation of the quantity of individual items.

Approximate representations of numerical magnitude imply that one can relatively estimate and comprehend an amount if the number is large (see Approximate number system). For example, one experiment showed children and adults arrays of many dots.[11] After briefly observing them, both groups could accurately estimate the approximate number of dots. However, distinguishing differences between large numbers of dots proved to be more challenging.[11]

Precise representations of distinct items demonstrate that people are more accurate in estimating amounts and distinguishing differences when the numbers are relatively small (see Subitizing).[11] For example, in one experiment, an experimenter presented an infant with two piles of crackers, one with two crackers the other with three. The experimenter then covered each pile with a cup. When allowed to choose a cup, the infant always chose the cup with more crackers because the infant could distinguish the difference.[11]

Both systems—approximate representation of magnitude and precise representation quantity of individual items—have limited power. For example, neither allows representations of fractions or negative numbers. More complex representations require education. However, achievement in school mathematics correlates with an individual's unlearned approximate number sense.[13]

Definitions and assessment Edit

Fundamental (or rudimentary) numeracy skills include understanding of the real number line, time, measurement, and estimation.[6] Fundamental skills include basic skills (the ability to identify and understand numbers) and computational skills (the ability to perform simple arithmetical operations and compare numerical magnitudes).

More sophisticated numeracy skills include understanding of ratio concepts (notably fractions, proportions, percentages, and probabilities), and knowing when and how to perform multistep operations.[6] Two categories of skills are included at the higher levels: the analytical skills (the ability to understand numerical information, such as required to interpret graphs and charts) and the statistical skills (the ability to apply higher probabilistic and statistical computation, such as conditional probabilities).

A variety of tests have been developed for assessing numeracy and health numeracy.[6][7][14][15][16][17][18] Different tests have been developed to evaluate health numeracy. Two of these tests that have been found to be “reliable and valid” are the GHNT-21 and GHNT-6.[19]

Childhood influences Edit

The first couple of years of childhood are considered to be a vital part of life for the development of numeracy and literacy.[20] There are many components that play key roles in the development of numeracy at a young age, such as Socioeconomic Status (SES), parenting, Home Learning Environment (HLE), and age.[20]

Socioeconomic status Edit

Children who are brought up in families with high SES tend to be more engaged in developmentally enhancing activities.[20] These children are more likely to develop the necessary abilities to learn and to become more motivated to learn.[20] More specifically, a mother's education level is considered to have an effect on the child's ability to achieve in numeracy. That is, mothers with a high level of education will tend to have children who succeed more in numeracy.[20]

A number of studies have, moreover, proved that the education level of the mother is strongly correlated with the average age of getting married. More precisely, females who entered the marriage later, tend to have greater autonomy, chances for skills premium and level of education (i.e. numeracy). Hence, they were more likely to share this experience with children.[21]

Parenting Edit

Parents are advised to collaborate with their child in simple learning exercises, such as reading a book, painting, drawing, and playing with numbers. On a more expressive note[clarification needed], the act of using complex language, being more responsive towards the child, and establishing warm interactions are recommended to parents with the confirmation of positive numeracy outcomes.[20] When discussing beneficial parenting behaviors, a feedback loop is formed because pleased parents are more willing to interact with their child, which in essence promotes better development in the child.[20]

Home-learning environment Edit

Along with parenting and SES, a strong home-learning environment increases the likelihood of the child being prepared for comprehending complex mathematical schooling.[22] For example, if a child is influenced by many learning activities in the household, such as puzzles, coloring books, mazes, or books with picture riddles, then they will be more prepared to face school activities.[22]

Age Edit

Age is accounted for when discussing the development of numeracy in children.[22] Children under the age of 5 have the best opportunity to absorb basic numeracy skills.[22] After the age of seven, achievement of basic numeracy skills become less influential.[22] For example, a study was conducted to compare the reading and mathematical abilities between children of ages five and seven, each in three different mental capacity groups (underachieving, average, and overachieving). The differences in the amount of knowledge retained were greater between the three different groups aged five than between the groups aged seven. This reveals that those of younger ages have an opportunity to retain more information, like numeracy. According to Gelman and Gallistel in The Child’s Understanding of Number, ‘children as young as 2 years can accurately judge numerosity provided that the numerosity is not larger than two or three’. Children as young as three have been found to understand elementary mathematical concepts.[23] Kilpatrick and his colleagues state ‘most preschoolers show that they can understand and perform simple addition and subtraction by at least 3 years of age’.[24] Lastly, it has been observed that pre-school children benefit from their basic understanding of ‘counting, reading and writing of numbers, understanding of simple addition and subtraction, numerical reasoning, classifying of objects and shapes, estimating, measuring, [and the] reproduction of number patterns’.[25]

Literacy Edit

There seems to be a relationship between literacy and numeracy,[26][27] which can be seen in young children. Depending on the level of literacy or numeracy at a young age, one can predict the growth of literacy and/ or numeracy skills in future development.[28] There is some evidence that humans may have an inborn sense of number. In one study for example, five-month-old infants were shown two dolls, which were then hidden with a screen. The babies saw the experimenter pull one doll from behind the screen. Without the child's knowledge, a second experimenter could remove, or add dolls, unseen behind the screen. When the screen was removed, the infants showed more surprise at an unexpected number (for example, if there were still two dolls). Some researchers have concluded that the babies were able to count, although others doubt this and claim the infants noticed surface area rather than number.[29]

Employment Edit

Numeracy has a huge impact on employment.[30] In a work environment, numeracy can be a controlling factor affecting career achievements and failures.[30] Many professions require individuals to have well-developed numerical skills: for example, mathematician, physicist, accountant, actuary, Risk Analyst, financial analyst, engineer, and architect. This is why a major target of the Sustainable Development Goal 4 is to substantially increase the number of youths who have relevant skills for decent work and employment[31] because, even outside these specialized areas, the lack of numeracy skills can reduce employment opportunities and promotions, resulting in unskilled manual careers, low-paying jobs, and even unemployment.[32] For example, carpenters and interior designers need to be able to measure, use fractions, and handle budgets.[33] Another example of numeracy influencing employment was demonstrated at the Poynter Institute. The Poynter Institute has recently included numeracy as one of the skills required by competent journalists. Max Frankel, former executive editor of The New York Times, argues that "deploying numbers skillfully is as important to communication as deploying verbs". Unfortunately, it is evident that journalists often show poor numeracy skills. In a study by the Society of Professional Journalists, 58% of job applicants interviewed by broadcast news directors lacked an adequate understanding of statistical materials.[34]

To assess job applicants, psychometric numerical reasoning tests have been created by occupational psychologists, who are involved in the study of numeracy. These tests are used to assess ability to comprehend and apply numbers. They are sometimes administered with a time limit, so that the test-taker must think quickly and concisely. Research has shown that these tests are very useful in evaluating potential applicants because they do not allow the applicants to prepare for the test, unlike interview questions. This suggests that an applicant's results are reliable and accurate[citation needed]

These tests first became prevalent during the 1980s, following the pioneering work of psychologists, such as P. Kline, who published a book in 1986 entitled A handbook of test construction: Introduction to psychometric design, which explained that psychometric testing could provide reliable and objective results, which could be used to assess a candidate's numerical abilities.

Innumeracy and dyscalculia Edit

The term innumeracy is a neologism, coined by analogy with illiteracy. Innumeracy refers to a lack of ability to reason with numbers. The term was coined by cognitive scientist Douglas Hofstadter; however, it was popularized in 1989 by mathematician John Allen Paulos in his book Innumeracy: Mathematical Illiteracy and its Consequences.

Developmental dyscalculia refers to a persistent and specific impairment of basic numerical-arithmetical skills learning in the context of normal intelligence.

Patterns and differences Edit

The root causes of innumeracy vary. Innumeracy has been seen in those suffering from poor education and childhood deprivation of numeracy.[35] Innumeracy is apparent in children during the transition between numerical skills obtained before schooling and the new skills taught in the education departments because of their memory capacity to comprehend the material.[35] Patterns of innumeracy have also been observed depending on age, gender, and race.[36] Older adults have been associated with lower numeracy skills than younger adults.[36] Men have been identified to have higher numeracy skills than women.[30] Some studies seem to indicate young people of African heritage tend to have lower numeracy skills.[36] The Trends in International Mathematics and Science Study (TIMSS) in which children at fourth-grade (average 10 to 11 years) and eighth-grade (average 14 to 15 years) from 49 countries were tested on mathematical comprehension. The assessment included tests for number, algebra (also called patterns and relationships at fourth grade), measurement, geometry, and data. The latest study, in 2003, found that children from Singapore at both grade levels had the highest performance. Countries like Hong Kong SAR, Japan, and Taiwan also shared high levels of numeracy. The lowest scores were found in countries like[clarification needed] South Africa, Ghana, and Saudi Arabia. Another finding showed a noticeable difference between boys and girls, with some exceptions. For example, girls performed significantly better in Singapore, and boys performed significantly better in the United States.[11]

Theory Edit

There is a theory that innumeracy is more common than illiteracy when dividing cognitive abilities into two separate categories. David C. Geary, a notable cognitive developmental and evolutionary psychologist from the University of Missouri, created the terms "biological primary abilities" and "biological secondary abilities".[35] Biological primary abilities evolve over time and are necessary for survival. Such abilities include speaking a common language or knowledge of simple mathematics.[35] Biological secondary abilities are attained through personal experiences and cultural customs, such as reading or high level mathematics learned through schooling.[35] Literacy and numeracy are similar in the sense that they are both important skills used in life. However, they differ in the sorts of mental demands each makes. Literacy consists of acquiring vocabulary and grammatical sophistication, which seem to be more closely related to memorization, whereas numeracy involves manipulating concepts, such as in calculus or geometry, and builds from basic numeracy skills.[35] This could be a potential explanation of the challenge of being numerate.[35]

Innumeracy and risk perception in health decision-making Edit

Health numeracy has been defined as "the degree to which individuals have the capacity to access, process, interpret, communicate, and act on numerical, quantitative, graphical, biostatistical, and probabilistic health information needed to make effective health decisions".[37] The concept of health numeracy is a component of the concept of health literacy. Health numeracy and health literacy can be thought of as the combination of skills needed for understanding risk and making good choices in health-related behavior.

Health numeracy requires basic numeracy but also more advanced analytical and statistical skills. For instance, health numeracy also requires the ability to understand probabilities or relative frequencies in various numerical and graphical formats, and to engage in Bayesian inference, while avoiding errors sometimes associated with Bayesian reasoning (see Base rate fallacy, Conservatism (Bayesian)). Health numeracy also requires understanding terms with definitions that are specific to the medical context. For instance, although 'survival' and 'mortality' are complementary in common usage, these terms are not complementary in medicine (see five-year survival rate).[38][39] Innumeracy is also a very common problem when dealing with risk perception in health-related behavior; it is associated with patients, physicians, journalists and policymakers.[36][39] Those who lack or have limited health numeracy skills run the risk of making poor health-related decisions because of an inaccurate perception of information.[20][40] For example, if a patient has been diagnosed with breast cancer, being innumerate may hinder her ability to comprehend her physician's recommendations, or even the severity of the health concern or even the likelihood of treatment benefits.[41] One study found that people tended to overestimate their chances of survival or even to choose lower-quality hospitals.[30] Innumeracy also makes it difficult or impossible for some patients to read medical graphs correctly.[42] Some authors have distinguished graph literacy from numeracy.[43] Indeed, many doctors exhibit innumeracy when attempting to explain a graph or statistics to a patient. A misunderstanding between a doctor and patient, due to either the doctor, patient, or both being unable to comprehend numbers effectively, could result in serious harm to health.

Different presentation formats of numerical information, for instance natural frequency icon arrays, have been evaluated to assist both low-numeracy and high-numeracy individuals.[36][44][45][46][47] Other data formats provide more assistance to low-numeracy people.[3][48][49]

Evolution of numeracy Edit

In the field of economic history, numeracy is often used to assess human capital at times when there was no data on schooling or other educational measures. Using a method called age-heaping, researchers like Professor Jörg Baten study the development and inequalities of numeracy over time and throughout regions. For example, Baten[50] and Hippe find a numeracy gap between regions in western and central Europe and the rest of Europe for the period 1790–1880. At the same time, their data analysis reveals that these differences as well as within country inequality decreased over time. Taking a similar approach, Baten and Fourie[51] find overall high levels of numeracy for people in the Cape Colony (late 17th to early 19th century).

In contrast to these studies comparing numeracy over countries or regions, it is also possible to analyze numeracy within countries. For example, Baten, Crayen and Voth[52] look at the effects of war on numeracy in England, and Baten and Priwitzer[53] find a "military bias" in what is today western Hungary: people opting for a military career had - on average - better numeracy indicators (1 BCE to 3CE).

See also Edit

Notes Edit

  1. ^ Brooks, M.; Pui (2010). "Are individual differences in numeracy unique from general mental ability? A closer look at a common measure of numeracy". Individual Differences Research. 4. 8: 257–265.
  2. ^ Statistics Canada. (PDF). Statistics Canada. p. 209. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2011-09-27.
  3. ^ a b c Peters, Ellen (2020). Innumeracy in the wild: Misunderstanding and misusing numbers. Oxford University Press.
  4. ^ Gerardi, K.; Goette, L.; Meier, S. (2013). "Numerical ability predicts mortgage default". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 110 (28): 11267–11271. Bibcode:2013PNAS..11011267G. doi:10.1073/pnas.1220568110. PMC 3710828. PMID 23798401.
  5. ^ Banks, J.; O'Dea, C.; Oldfield, Z. (2010). "Cognitive Function, Numeracy and Retirement Saving Trajectories*". The Economic Journal. 120 (548): F381–F410. doi:10.1111/j.1468-0297.2010.02395.x. PMC 3249594. PMID 22228911.
  6. ^ a b c d Reyna, V.F.; Nelson, W.L.; Han, P.K.; Dieckmann, N.F. (2009). "How numeracy influences risk comprehension and medical decision making". Psychological Bulletin. 135 (6): 943–973. doi:10.1037/a0017327. PMC 2844786. PMID 19883143.
  7. ^ a b Weller, J.A.; Dieckmann, N.F.; Tusler, M.; Mertz, C.K.; Burns, W.J.; Peters, E. (2013). "Development and Testing of an Abbreviated Numeracy Scale: A Rasch Analysis Approach". Journal of Behavioral Decision Making. 26 (2): 198–212. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.678.6236. doi:10.1002/bdm.1751. PMC 7161838. PMID 32313367.
  8. ^ Peters, E.; Tompkins, M.K.; Knoll, M.; Ardoin, S.P.; Shoots-Reinhard, B.; Meara, A.S. (2019). "Despite high objective numeracy, lower numeric confidence relates to worse financial and medical outcomes". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 116 (39): 19386–19391. Bibcode:2019PNAS..11619386P. doi:10.1073/pnas.1903126116. PMC 6765274. PMID 31501338.
  9. ^ Feigenson, Lisa; Dehaene, Stanislas; Spelke, Elizabeth (July 2004). "Core systems of number". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 8 (7): 307–314. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2004.05.002. PMID 15242690. S2CID 17313189.
  10. ^ Izard, V.; Sann, C.; Spelke, E.S.; Streri, A. (2009). "Newborn infants perceive abstract numbers". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 106 (25): 10382–10385. Bibcode:2009PNAS..10610382I. doi:10.1073/pnas.0812142106. PMC 2700913. PMID 19520833.
  11. ^ a b c d e f Dehaene, S.; Izard, V.; Spelke, E.; Pica, P. (2008). "Log or Linear? Distinct Intuitions of the Number Scale in Western and Amazonian Indigene Cultures". Science. 320 (5880): 1217–1220. Bibcode:2008Sci...320.1217D. doi:10.1126/science.1156540. PMC 2610411. PMID 18511690.
  12. ^ Nieder, A. (2005). "Counting on neurons: The neurobiology of numerical competence". Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 6 (3): 177–190. doi:10.1038/nrn1626. PMID 15711599. S2CID 14578049.
  13. ^ Halberda, J.; Mazzocco, M.L.M.M.; Feigenson, L. (2008). "Individual differences in non-verbal number acuity correlate with maths achievement". Nature. 455 (7213): 665–668. Bibcode:2008Natur.455..665H. doi:10.1038/nature07246. PMID 18776888. S2CID 27196030.
  14. ^ Schwartz, L.M.; Woloshin, S.; Black, W.C.; Welch, H.G. (1997). "The Role of Numeracy in Understanding the Benefit of Screening Mammography". Annals of Internal Medicine. 127 (11): 966–972. doi:10.7326/0003-4819-127-11-199712010-00003. PMID 9412301. S2CID 19412405.
  15. ^ Lipkus, I.M.; Samsa, G.; Rimer, B.K. (2001). "General Performance on a Numeracy Scale among Highly Educated Samples". Medical Decision Making. 21 (1): 37–44. doi:10.1177/0272989X0102100105. PMID 11206945. S2CID 25249366.
  16. ^ Cokely, E.T.; Galesic, M.; Schulz, E.; Ghazal, S.; Garcia-Retamero, R. (2012). "Measuring risk literacy: The Berlin Numeracy Test" (PDF). Judgment and Decision Making. 7 (1): 25–47. doi:10.1017/S1930297500001819. S2CID 11617465.
  17. ^ Schapira, M.M.; Walker, C.M.; Cappaert, K.J.; Ganschow, P.S.; Fletcher, K.E.; McGinley, E.L.; Del Pozo, S.; Schauer, C.; Tarima, S.; Jacobs, E.A. (2012). "The Numeracy Understanding in Medicine Instrument: A Measure of Health Numeracy Developed Using Item Response Theory". Medical Decision Making. 32 (6): 851–865. doi:10.1177/0272989X12447239. PMC 4162626. PMID 22635285.
  18. ^ Fagerlin, A.; Zikmund-Fisher, B.J.; Ubel, P.A.; Jankovic, A.; Derry, H.A.; Smith, D.M. (2007). "Measuring Numeracy without a Math Test: Development of the Subjective Numeracy Scale". Medical Decision Making. 27 (5): 672–680. doi:10.1177/0272989X07304449. PMID 17641137. S2CID 30150256.
  19. ^ Osborn, Chandra Y.; Wallston, Kenneth A.; Shpigel, Adam; Cavanaugh, Kerri; Kripalani, Sunil; Rothman, Russell L. (June 2013). "Development and Validation of the General Health Numeracy Test (GHNT)". Patient Education and Counseling. 91 (3): 350–356. doi:10.1016/j.pec.2013.01.001. ISSN 0738-3991. PMC 3644342. PMID 23433635.
  20. ^ a b c d e f g h Ciampa, Philip J.; Osborn, Chandra Y.; Peterson, Neeraja B.; Rothman, Russell L. (13 December 2010). "Patient Numeracy, Perceptions of Provider Communication, and Colorectal Cancer Screening Utilization". Journal of Health Communication. 15 (sup3): 157–168. doi:10.1080/10810730.2010.522699. PMC 3075203. PMID 21154091.
  21. ^ Joerg, Baten; Mikołaj, Szołtysek; Monica, Campestrini (14 December 2016). (PDF). European Review of Economic History. doi:10.1093/ereh/hew017. S2CID 51963985. Archived from the original (PDF) on 28 February 2019.
  22. ^ a b c d e Melhuish, Edward C.; Phan, Mai B.; Sylva, Kathy; Sammons, Pam; Siraj-Blatchford, Iram; Taggart, Brenda (March 2008). "Effects of the Home Learning Environment and Preschool Center Experience upon Literacy and Numeracy Development in Early Primary School". Journal of Social Issues. 64 (1): 95–114. doi:10.1111/j.1540-4560.2008.00550.x. S2CID 18460752.
  23. ^ Hughes, Martin (1986). Children and Number. Wiley-Blackwell. ISBN 9780631135814.
  24. ^ Jane., Findell, Bradford. Kilpatrick, Jeremy. Swafford (2001). Adding + it up : helping children learn mathematics. National Academy Press. ISBN 0-309-06995-5. OCLC 248386156.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  25. ^ Ewers‐Rogers, Jennifer; Cowan, Richard (January 1996). "Children as Apprentices to Number". Early Child Development and Care. 125 (1): 15–25. doi:10.1080/0300443961250102. ISSN 0300-4430.
  26. ^ Bullock, James O. (October 1994), "Literacy in the Language of Mathematics", The American Mathematical Monthly, 101 (8): 735–743, doi:10.2307/2974528, JSTOR 2974528
  27. ^ Steen, Lynn Arthur (2001), "Mathematics and Numeracy: Two Literacies, One Language", The Mathematics Educator (Journal of the Singapore Association of Mathematics Educators), 6 (1): 10–16
  28. ^ Purpura, David; Hume, L.; Sims, D.; Lonigan, C. (2011). "Early literacy and early numeracy: The value of including early literacy skills in the prediction of numeracy". Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 110 (4): 647–658. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2011.07.004. PMID 21831396.
  29. ^ Numbers in Mind
  30. ^ a b c d Brooks, M.; Pui, S. (2010). "Are individual differences in numeracy unique from general mental ability? A closer look at a common measure of numeracy". Individual Differences Research. 4. 8: 257–265.
  31. ^ "SDG4's 10 targets". Global Campaign For Education. Retrieved 2020-09-22.
  32. ^ Ciampa, Philip J.; Osborn, Chandra Y.; Peterson, Neeraja B.; Rothman, Russell L. (2010). "Patient Numeracy, Perceptions of Provider Communication, and Colorectal Cancer Screening Utilization". Journal of Health Communication. 15 (Suppl 3): 157–168. doi:10.1080/10810730.2010.522699. PMC 3075203. PMID 21154091.
  33. ^ Melhuish, Edward C.; Phan, Mai B.; Sylva, Kathy; Sammons, Pam; Siraj-Blatchford, Iram; Taggart, Brenda (2008). "Effects of the Home Learning Environment and Preschool Center Experience upon Literacy and Numeracy Development in Early Primary School". Journal of Social Issues. 64 (1): 95–114. doi:10.1111/j.1540-4560.2008.00550.x. S2CID 18460752.
  34. ^ Scanlan, Chip (2004). "Why Math Matters 2009-01-14 at the Wayback Machine" Poynter Online, September 8, 2004.
  35. ^ a b c d e f g Lefevre, Jo-Anne (2000). "Research on the development of academic skills: Introduction to the special issue on early literacy and early numeracy". Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology. 54 (2): 57–60. doi:10.1037/h0088185. PMID 10881390.
  36. ^ a b c d e Donelle, L.; Hoffman-Goetz, L.; Arocha, J.F. (2007). "Assessing health numeracy among community-dwelling older adults". Journal of Health Communication. 7. 12 (7): 651–665. doi:10.1080/10810730701619919. PMID 17934942. S2CID 20421979.
  37. ^ Golbeck, AL; Ahlers-Schmidt, CR; Paschal, AM; Dismuke, SE (2005). "A definition and operational framework for health numeracy". American Journal of Preventive Medicine. 29 (4): 375–376. doi:10.1016/j.amepre.2005.06.012. PMID 16242604.
  38. ^ Welch, H.G.; Schwartz, L.M.; Woloshin, S. (2000). "Are Increasing 5-Year Survival Rates Evidence of Success Against Cancer?". JAMA. 283 (22): 2975–2978. doi:10.1001/jama.283.22.2975. PMID 10865276.
  39. ^ a b Gigerenzer, G.; Gaissmaier, W.; Kurz-Milcke, E.; Schwartz, L.M.; Woloshin, S. (2007). "Helping Doctors and Patients Make Sense of Health Statistics". Psychological Science in the Public Interest. 8 (2): 53–96. doi:10.1111/j.1539-6053.2008.00033.x. PMID 26161749.
  40. ^ Låg, T.; Bauger, L.; Lindberg, M.; Friborg, O. (2014). "The role of numeracy and intelligence in health-risk estimation and medical data interpretation". Behavioral Decision Making. 30 (2): 95–108. doi:10.1002/bdm.1788.
  41. ^ Lipkus, I.M.; Peters, E.; Kimmick, G.; Liotcheva, V.; Marcom, P. (2010). "Breast cancer patients' treatment expectations after exposure to the decision aid program, Adjuvant Online: The influence of numeracy". Medical Decision Making. 30 (4): 464–473. doi:10.1177/0272989X09360371. PMC 3616375. PMID 20160070.
  42. ^ Hess, R.; Visschers, V.H.M.; Siegrist, M.; Keller, C. (2011). "How do people perceive graphical risk communication? The role of subjective numeracy". Journal of Risk Research. 14: 47–61. doi:10.1080/13669877.2010.488745. S2CID 146594087.
  43. ^ Galesic, M.; Garcia-Retamero, R. (2010). "Graph Literacy: A Cross-Cultural Comparison". Medical Decision Making. 31 (3): 444–457. doi:10.1177/0272989X10373805. PMID 20671213. S2CID 32662502.
  44. ^ Ancker, J.S.; Senathirajah, Y.; Kukafka, R.; Starren, J.B. (2006). "Design Features of Graphs in Health Risk Communication: A Systematic Review". Journal of the American Medical Informatics Association. 13 (6): 608–618. doi:10.1197/jamia.M2115. PMC 1656964. PMID 16929039.
  45. ^ Garcia-Retamero, R.; Okan, Y.; Cokely, E.T. (2012). "Using Visual Aids to Improve Communication of Risks about Health: A Review". The Scientific World Journal. 2012: 1–10. doi:10.1100/2012/562637. PMC 3354448. PMID 22629146.
  46. ^ Hoffrage, U.; Lindsey, S.; Hertwig, R.; Gigerenzer, G. (2000). "Medicine: Communicating Statistical Information". Science. 290 (5500): 2261–2262. doi:10.1126/science.290.5500.2261. hdl:11858/00-001M-0000-0025-9B18-3. PMID 11188724. S2CID 33050943.
  47. ^ Galesic, M.; Garcia-Retamero, R.; Gigerenzer, G. (2009). "Using icon arrays to communicate medical risks: Overcoming low numeracy". Health Psychology. 28 (2): 210–216. doi:10.1037/a0014474. hdl:11858/00-001M-0000-0024-F6BE-3. PMID 19290713. S2CID 10545246.
  48. ^ Peters, E.; Dieckmann, N.; Dixon, A.; Hibbard, J.H.; Mertz, C.K. (2007). "Less is more in presenting quality information to consumers". Medical Care Research & Review. 64 (2): 169–190. doi:10.1177/10775587070640020301. PMID 17406019. S2CID 1001326.
  49. ^ Peters, E.; Dieckmann, N.; Våstfjåll, D.; Mertz, C.K.; Slovic, P.; Hibbard, J.H. (2009). "Bringing meaning to numbers: The impact of evaluative categories on decisions". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied. 15 (3): 213–227. doi:10.1037/a0016978. PMID 19751072. S2CID 11035873.
  50. ^ Baten, Jörg; Hippe, Ralph (2012). "The early regional development of human capital in Europe, 1790–1880" (PDF). Scandinavian Economic History Review. 60 (3): 254–289. doi:10.1080/03585522.2012.727763. S2CID 154669586.
  51. ^ Baten, Jörg; Fourie, Johan (2015). "Numeracy of Africans, Asians, and Europeans during the early modern period: new evidence from Cape Colony court registers". The Economic History Review. 68 (2): 632–656. doi:10.1111/1468-0289.12064. hdl:10.1111/1468-0289.12064. S2CID 51961313.
  52. ^ Baten, Jörg; Crayen, Dorothee; Voth, Hans-Joachim (2014). "Numeracy and the impact of high food prices in industrializing Britain, 1780–1850" (PDF). Review of Economics and Statistics. 96 (3): 418–430. doi:10.1162/REST_a_00403. S2CID 3518364.
  53. ^ Baten, Jörg; Priwitzer, Stefan (2015). "Social and intertemporal differences of basic numeracy in Pannonia (first century BCE to third century CE)". Scandinavian Economic History Review. 63 (2): 110–134. doi:10.1080/03585522.2015.1032339. S2CID 51962193.

External links Edit

  • The Berlin Numeracy Test
  • CDC Health Literacy Resources
  • Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality Health Literacy Measurement tools
  • Australian blog post reviewing the increasing importance of teaching numeracy skills

numeracy, this, article, about, ability, apply, numerical, concepts, academic, journal, journal, innumeracy, redirects, here, book, innumeracy, book, ability, understand, reason, with, apply, simple, numerical, concepts, charity, national, states, means, under. This article is about the ability to apply numerical concepts For the academic journal see Numeracy journal Innumeracy redirects here For the book see Innumeracy book Numeracy is the ability to understand reason with and to apply simple numerical concepts 1 The charity National Numeracy states Numeracy means understanding how mathematics is used in the real world and being able to apply it to make the best possible decisions It s as much about thinking and reasoning as about doing sums Basic numeracy skills consist of comprehending fundamental arithmetical operations like addition subtraction multiplication and division For example if one can understand simple mathematical equations such as 2 2 4 then one would be considered to possess at least basic numeric knowledge Substantial aspects of numeracy also include number sense operation sense computation measurement geometry probability and statistics citation needed A numerically literate person can manage and respond to the mathematical demands of life 2 3 Children in Laos have fun as they improve numeracy with Number Bingo They roll three dice construct an equation from the numbers to produce a new number then cover that number on the board trying to get four in a row Number bingo improves math skills LPB Laos By contrast innumeracy the lack of numeracy can have a negative impact Numeracy has an influence on healthy behaviors financial literacy and career decisions Therefore innumeracy may negatively affect economic choices financial outcomes health outcomes and life satisfaction 3 4 5 It also may distort risk perception in health decisions 6 Greater numeracy has been associated with reduced susceptibility to framing effects less influence of nonnumerical information such as mood states and greater sensitivity to different levels of numerical risk 7 Ellen Peters and her colleagues argue that achieving the benefits of numeric literacy however may depend on one s numeric self efficacy or confidence in one s skills 8 Contents 1 Representation of numbers 2 Definitions and assessment 3 Childhood influences 4 Socioeconomic status 5 Parenting 6 Home learning environment 7 Age 8 Literacy 9 Employment 10 Innumeracy and dyscalculia 11 Patterns and differences 12 Theory 13 Innumeracy and risk perception in health decision making 14 Evolution of numeracy 15 See also 16 Notes 17 External linksRepresentation of numbers EditHumans have evolved to mentally represent numbers in two major ways from observation not formal math 9 These representations are often thought to be innate 10 see Numerical cognition to be shared across human cultures 11 to be common to multiple species 12 and not to be the result of individual learning or cultural transmission They are Approximate representation of numerical magnitude and Precise representation of the quantity of individual items Approximate representations of numerical magnitude imply that one can relatively estimate and comprehend an amount if the number is large see Approximate number system For example one experiment showed children and adults arrays of many dots 11 After briefly observing them both groups could accurately estimate the approximate number of dots However distinguishing differences between large numbers of dots proved to be more challenging 11 Precise representations of distinct items demonstrate that people are more accurate in estimating amounts and distinguishing differences when the numbers are relatively small see Subitizing 11 For example in one experiment an experimenter presented an infant with two piles of crackers one with two crackers the other with three The experimenter then covered each pile with a cup When allowed to choose a cup the infant always chose the cup with more crackers because the infant could distinguish the difference 11 Both systems approximate representation of magnitude and precise representation quantity of individual items have limited power For example neither allows representations of fractions or negative numbers More complex representations require education However achievement in school mathematics correlates with an individual s unlearned approximate number sense 13 Definitions and assessment EditFundamental or rudimentary numeracy skills include understanding of the real number line time measurement and estimation 6 Fundamental skills include basic skills the ability to identify and understand numbers and computational skills the ability to perform simple arithmetical operations and compare numerical magnitudes More sophisticated numeracy skills include understanding of ratio concepts notably fractions proportions percentages and probabilities and knowing when and how to perform multistep operations 6 Two categories of skills are included at the higher levels the analytical skills the ability to understand numerical information such as required to interpret graphs and charts and the statistical skills the ability to apply higher probabilistic and statistical computation such as conditional probabilities A variety of tests have been developed for assessing numeracy and health numeracy 6 7 14 15 16 17 18 Different tests have been developed to evaluate health numeracy Two of these tests that have been found to be reliable and valid are the GHNT 21 and GHNT 6 19 Childhood influences EditThe first couple of years of childhood are considered to be a vital part of life for the development of numeracy and literacy 20 There are many components that play key roles in the development of numeracy at a young age such as Socioeconomic Status SES parenting Home Learning Environment HLE and age 20 Socioeconomic status EditChildren who are brought up in families with high SES tend to be more engaged in developmentally enhancing activities 20 These children are more likely to develop the necessary abilities to learn and to become more motivated to learn 20 More specifically a mother s education level is considered to have an effect on the child s ability to achieve in numeracy That is mothers with a high level of education will tend to have children who succeed more in numeracy 20 A number of studies have moreover proved that the education level of the mother is strongly correlated with the average age of getting married More precisely females who entered the marriage later tend to have greater autonomy chances for skills premium and level of education i e numeracy Hence they were more likely to share this experience with children 21 Parenting EditParents are advised to collaborate with their child in simple learning exercises such as reading a book painting drawing and playing with numbers On a more expressive note clarification needed the act of using complex language being more responsive towards the child and establishing warm interactions are recommended to parents with the confirmation of positive numeracy outcomes 20 When discussing beneficial parenting behaviors a feedback loop is formed because pleased parents are more willing to interact with their child which in essence promotes better development in the child 20 Home learning environment EditAlong with parenting and SES a strong home learning environment increases the likelihood of the child being prepared for comprehending complex mathematical schooling 22 For example if a child is influenced by many learning activities in the household such as puzzles coloring books mazes or books with picture riddles then they will be more prepared to face school activities 22 Age EditAge is accounted for when discussing the development of numeracy in children 22 Children under the age of 5 have the best opportunity to absorb basic numeracy skills 22 After the age of seven achievement of basic numeracy skills become less influential 22 For example a study was conducted to compare the reading and mathematical abilities between children of ages five and seven each in three different mental capacity groups underachieving average and overachieving The differences in the amount of knowledge retained were greater between the three different groups aged five than between the groups aged seven This reveals that those of younger ages have an opportunity to retain more information like numeracy According to Gelman and Gallistel in The Child s Understanding of Number children as young as 2 years can accurately judge numerosity provided that the numerosity is not larger than two or three Children as young as three have been found to understand elementary mathematical concepts 23 Kilpatrick and his colleagues state most preschoolers show that they can understand and perform simple addition and subtraction by at least 3 years of age 24 Lastly it has been observed that pre school children benefit from their basic understanding of counting reading and writing of numbers understanding of simple addition and subtraction numerical reasoning classifying of objects and shapes estimating measuring and the reproduction of number patterns 25 Literacy EditThere seems to be a relationship between literacy and numeracy 26 27 which can be seen in young children Depending on the level of literacy or numeracy at a young age one can predict the growth of literacy and or numeracy skills in future development 28 There is some evidence that humans may have an inborn sense of number In one study for example five month old infants were shown two dolls which were then hidden with a screen The babies saw the experimenter pull one doll from behind the screen Without the child s knowledge a second experimenter could remove or add dolls unseen behind the screen When the screen was removed the infants showed more surprise at an unexpected number for example if there were still two dolls Some researchers have concluded that the babies were able to count although others doubt this and claim the infants noticed surface area rather than number 29 Employment EditNumeracy has a huge impact on employment 30 In a work environment numeracy can be a controlling factor affecting career achievements and failures 30 Many professions require individuals to have well developed numerical skills for example mathematician physicist accountant actuary Risk Analyst financial analyst engineer and architect This is why a major target of the Sustainable Development Goal 4 is to substantially increase the number of youths who have relevant skills for decent work and employment 31 because even outside these specialized areas the lack of numeracy skills can reduce employment opportunities and promotions resulting in unskilled manual careers low paying jobs and even unemployment 32 For example carpenters and interior designers need to be able to measure use fractions and handle budgets 33 Another example of numeracy influencing employment was demonstrated at the Poynter Institute The Poynter Institute has recently included numeracy as one of the skills required by competent journalists Max Frankel former executive editor of The New York Times argues that deploying numbers skillfully is as important to communication as deploying verbs Unfortunately it is evident that journalists often show poor numeracy skills In a study by the Society of Professional Journalists 58 of job applicants interviewed by broadcast news directors lacked an adequate understanding of statistical materials 34 To assess job applicants psychometric numerical reasoning tests have been created by occupational psychologists who are involved in the study of numeracy These tests are used to assess ability to comprehend and apply numbers They are sometimes administered with a time limit so that the test taker must think quickly and concisely Research has shown that these tests are very useful in evaluating potential applicants because they do not allow the applicants to prepare for the test unlike interview questions This suggests that an applicant s results are reliable and accurate citation needed These tests first became prevalent during the 1980s following the pioneering work of psychologists such as P Kline who published a book in 1986 entitled A handbook of test construction Introduction to psychometric design which explained that psychometric testing could provide reliable and objective results which could be used to assess a candidate s numerical abilities Innumeracy and dyscalculia EditThe term innumeracy is a neologism coined by analogy with illiteracy Innumeracy refers to a lack of ability to reason with numbers The term was coined by cognitive scientist Douglas Hofstadter however it was popularized in 1989 by mathematician John Allen Paulos in his book Innumeracy Mathematical Illiteracy and its Consequences Developmental dyscalculia refers to a persistent and specific impairment of basic numerical arithmetical skills learning in the context of normal intelligence Patterns and differences EditThe root causes of innumeracy vary Innumeracy has been seen in those suffering from poor education and childhood deprivation of numeracy 35 Innumeracy is apparent in children during the transition between numerical skills obtained before schooling and the new skills taught in the education departments because of their memory capacity to comprehend the material 35 Patterns of innumeracy have also been observed depending on age gender and race 36 Older adults have been associated with lower numeracy skills than younger adults 36 Men have been identified to have higher numeracy skills than women 30 Some studies seem to indicate young people of African heritage tend to have lower numeracy skills 36 The Trends in International Mathematics and Science Study TIMSS in which children at fourth grade average 10 to 11 years and eighth grade average 14 to 15 years from 49 countries were tested on mathematical comprehension The assessment included tests for number algebra also called patterns and relationships at fourth grade measurement geometry and data The latest study in 2003 found that children from Singapore at both grade levels had the highest performance Countries like Hong Kong SAR Japan and Taiwan also shared high levels of numeracy The lowest scores were found in countries like clarification needed South Africa Ghana and Saudi Arabia Another finding showed a noticeable difference between boys and girls with some exceptions For example girls performed significantly better in Singapore and boys performed significantly better in the United States 11 Theory EditThere is a theory that innumeracy is more common than illiteracy when dividing cognitive abilities into two separate categories David C Geary a notable cognitive developmental and evolutionary psychologist from the University of Missouri created the terms biological primary abilities and biological secondary abilities 35 Biological primary abilities evolve over time and are necessary for survival Such abilities include speaking a common language or knowledge of simple mathematics 35 Biological secondary abilities are attained through personal experiences and cultural customs such as reading or high level mathematics learned through schooling 35 Literacy and numeracy are similar in the sense that they are both important skills used in life However they differ in the sorts of mental demands each makes Literacy consists of acquiring vocabulary and grammatical sophistication which seem to be more closely related to memorization whereas numeracy involves manipulating concepts such as in calculus or geometry and builds from basic numeracy skills 35 This could be a potential explanation of the challenge of being numerate 35 Innumeracy and risk perception in health decision making EditHealth numeracy has been defined as the degree to which individuals have the capacity to access process interpret communicate and act on numerical quantitative graphical biostatistical and probabilistic health information needed to make effective health decisions 37 The concept of health numeracy is a component of the concept of health literacy Health numeracy and health literacy can be thought of as the combination of skills needed for understanding risk and making good choices in health related behavior Health numeracy requires basic numeracy but also more advanced analytical and statistical skills For instance health numeracy also requires the ability to understand probabilities or relative frequencies in various numerical and graphical formats and to engage in Bayesian inference while avoiding errors sometimes associated with Bayesian reasoning see Base rate fallacy Conservatism Bayesian Health numeracy also requires understanding terms with definitions that are specific to the medical context For instance although survival and mortality are complementary in common usage these terms are not complementary in medicine see five year survival rate 38 39 Innumeracy is also a very common problem when dealing with risk perception in health related behavior it is associated with patients physicians journalists and policymakers 36 39 Those who lack or have limited health numeracy skills run the risk of making poor health related decisions because of an inaccurate perception of information 20 40 For example if a patient has been diagnosed with breast cancer being innumerate may hinder her ability to comprehend her physician s recommendations or even the severity of the health concern or even the likelihood of treatment benefits 41 One study found that people tended to overestimate their chances of survival or even to choose lower quality hospitals 30 Innumeracy also makes it difficult or impossible for some patients to read medical graphs correctly 42 Some authors have distinguished graph literacy from numeracy 43 Indeed many doctors exhibit innumeracy when attempting to explain a graph or statistics to a patient A misunderstanding between a doctor and patient due to either the doctor patient or both being unable to comprehend numbers effectively could result in serious harm to health Different presentation formats of numerical information for instance natural frequency icon arrays have been evaluated to assist both low numeracy and high numeracy individuals 36 44 45 46 47 Other data formats provide more assistance to low numeracy people 3 48 49 Evolution of numeracy EditIn the field of economic history numeracy is often used to assess human capital at times when there was no data on schooling or other educational measures Using a method called age heaping researchers like Professor Jorg Baten study the development and inequalities of numeracy over time and throughout regions For example Baten 50 and Hippe find a numeracy gap between regions in western and central Europe and the rest of Europe for the period 1790 1880 At the same time their data analysis reveals that these differences as well as within country inequality decreased over time Taking a similar approach Baten and Fourie 51 find overall high levels of numeracy for people in the Cape Colony late 17th to early 19th century In contrast to these studies comparing numeracy over countries or regions it is also possible to analyze numeracy within countries For example Baten Crayen and Voth 52 look at the effects of war on numeracy in England and Baten and Priwitzer 53 find a military bias in what is today western Hungary people opting for a military career had on average better numeracy indicators 1 BCE to 3CE See also EditAcalculia Approximate number system Bayesian inference Dyscalculia Graphicacy Health literacy Literacy National Numeracy Network Number sense Numeracy bias Numerical cognition Numerosity adaptation effect Oracy QuickSmart SubitizingNotes Edit Brooks M Pui 2010 Are individual differences in numeracy unique from general mental ability A closer look at a common measure of numeracy Individual Differences Research 4 8 257 265 Statistics Canada Building on our Competencies Canadian Results of the International Adult Literacy and Skills Survey PDF Statistics Canada p 209 Archived from the original PDF on 2011 09 27 a b c Peters Ellen 2020 Innumeracy in the wild Misunderstanding and misusing numbers Oxford University Press Gerardi K Goette L Meier S 2013 Numerical ability predicts mortgage default Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 110 28 11267 11271 Bibcode 2013PNAS 11011267G doi 10 1073 pnas 1220568110 PMC 3710828 PMID 23798401 Banks J O Dea C Oldfield Z 2010 Cognitive Function Numeracy and Retirement Saving Trajectories The Economic Journal 120 548 F381 F410 doi 10 1111 j 1468 0297 2010 02395 x PMC 3249594 PMID 22228911 a b c d Reyna V F Nelson W L Han P K Dieckmann N F 2009 How numeracy influences risk comprehension and medical decision making Psychological Bulletin 135 6 943 973 doi 10 1037 a0017327 PMC 2844786 PMID 19883143 a b Weller J A Dieckmann N F Tusler M Mertz C K Burns W J Peters E 2013 Development and Testing of an Abbreviated Numeracy Scale A Rasch Analysis Approach Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 26 2 198 212 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 678 6236 doi 10 1002 bdm 1751 PMC 7161838 PMID 32313367 Peters E Tompkins M K Knoll M Ardoin S P Shoots Reinhard B Meara A S 2019 Despite high objective numeracy lower numeric confidence relates to worse financial and medical outcomes Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 116 39 19386 19391 Bibcode 2019PNAS 11619386P doi 10 1073 pnas 1903126116 PMC 6765274 PMID 31501338 Feigenson Lisa Dehaene Stanislas Spelke Elizabeth July 2004 Core systems of number Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8 7 307 314 doi 10 1016 j tics 2004 05 002 PMID 15242690 S2CID 17313189 Izard V Sann C Spelke E S Streri A 2009 Newborn infants perceive abstract numbers Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 106 25 10382 10385 Bibcode 2009PNAS 10610382I doi 10 1073 pnas 0812142106 PMC 2700913 PMID 19520833 a b c d e f Dehaene S Izard V Spelke E Pica P 2008 Log or Linear Distinct Intuitions of the Number Scale in Western and Amazonian Indigene Cultures Science 320 5880 1217 1220 Bibcode 2008Sci 320 1217D doi 10 1126 science 1156540 PMC 2610411 PMID 18511690 Nieder A 2005 Counting on neurons The neurobiology of numerical competence Nature Reviews Neuroscience 6 3 177 190 doi 10 1038 nrn1626 PMID 15711599 S2CID 14578049 Halberda J Mazzocco M L M M Feigenson L 2008 Individual differences in non verbal number acuity correlate with maths achievement Nature 455 7213 665 668 Bibcode 2008Natur 455 665H doi 10 1038 nature07246 PMID 18776888 S2CID 27196030 Schwartz L M Woloshin S Black W C Welch H G 1997 The Role of Numeracy in Understanding the Benefit of Screening Mammography Annals of Internal Medicine 127 11 966 972 doi 10 7326 0003 4819 127 11 199712010 00003 PMID 9412301 S2CID 19412405 Lipkus I M Samsa G Rimer B K 2001 General Performance on a Numeracy Scale among Highly Educated Samples Medical Decision Making 21 1 37 44 doi 10 1177 0272989X0102100105 PMID 11206945 S2CID 25249366 Cokely E T Galesic M Schulz E Ghazal S Garcia Retamero R 2012 Measuring risk literacy The Berlin Numeracy Test PDF Judgment and Decision Making 7 1 25 47 doi 10 1017 S1930297500001819 S2CID 11617465 Schapira M M Walker C M Cappaert K J Ganschow P S Fletcher K E McGinley E L Del Pozo S Schauer C Tarima S Jacobs E A 2012 The Numeracy Understanding in Medicine Instrument A Measure of Health Numeracy Developed Using Item Response Theory Medical Decision Making 32 6 851 865 doi 10 1177 0272989X12447239 PMC 4162626 PMID 22635285 Fagerlin A Zikmund Fisher B J Ubel P A Jankovic A Derry H A Smith D M 2007 Measuring Numeracy without a Math Test Development of the Subjective Numeracy Scale Medical Decision Making 27 5 672 680 doi 10 1177 0272989X07304449 PMID 17641137 S2CID 30150256 Osborn Chandra Y Wallston Kenneth A Shpigel Adam Cavanaugh Kerri Kripalani Sunil Rothman Russell L June 2013 Development and Validation of the General Health Numeracy Test GHNT Patient Education and Counseling 91 3 350 356 doi 10 1016 j pec 2013 01 001 ISSN 0738 3991 PMC 3644342 PMID 23433635 a b c d e f g h Ciampa Philip J Osborn Chandra Y Peterson Neeraja B Rothman Russell L 13 December 2010 Patient Numeracy Perceptions of Provider Communication and Colorectal Cancer Screening Utilization Journal of Health Communication 15 sup3 157 168 doi 10 1080 10810730 2010 522699 PMC 3075203 PMID 21154091 Joerg Baten Mikolaj Szoltysek Monica Campestrini 14 December 2016 Girl Power in Eastern Europe The human capital development of Central Eastern and Eastern Europe in the seventeenth to nineteenth centuries and its determinants PDF European Review of Economic History doi 10 1093 ereh hew017 S2CID 51963985 Archived from the original PDF on 28 February 2019 a b c d e Melhuish Edward C Phan Mai B Sylva Kathy Sammons Pam Siraj Blatchford Iram Taggart Brenda March 2008 Effects of the Home Learning Environment and Preschool Center Experience upon Literacy and Numeracy Development in Early Primary School Journal of Social Issues 64 1 95 114 doi 10 1111 j 1540 4560 2008 00550 x S2CID 18460752 Hughes Martin 1986 Children and Number Wiley Blackwell ISBN 9780631135814 Jane Findell Bradford Kilpatrick Jeremy Swafford 2001 Adding it up helping children learn mathematics National Academy Press ISBN 0 309 06995 5 OCLC 248386156 a href Template Cite book html title Template Cite book cite book a CS1 maint multiple names authors list link Ewers Rogers Jennifer Cowan Richard January 1996 Children as Apprentices to Number Early Child Development and Care 125 1 15 25 doi 10 1080 0300443961250102 ISSN 0300 4430 Bullock James O October 1994 Literacy in the Language of Mathematics The American Mathematical Monthly 101 8 735 743 doi 10 2307 2974528 JSTOR 2974528 Steen Lynn Arthur 2001 Mathematics and Numeracy Two Literacies One Language The Mathematics Educator Journal of the Singapore Association of Mathematics Educators 6 1 10 16 Purpura David Hume L Sims D Lonigan C 2011 Early literacy and early numeracy The value of including early literacy skills in the prediction of numeracy Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 110 4 647 658 doi 10 1016 j jecp 2011 07 004 PMID 21831396 Numbers in Mind a b c d Brooks M Pui S 2010 Are individual differences in numeracy unique from general mental ability A closer look at a common measure of numeracy Individual Differences Research 4 8 257 265 SDG4 s 10 targets Global Campaign For Education Retrieved 2020 09 22 Ciampa Philip J Osborn Chandra Y Peterson Neeraja B Rothman Russell L 2010 Patient Numeracy Perceptions of Provider Communication and Colorectal Cancer Screening Utilization Journal of Health Communication 15 Suppl 3 157 168 doi 10 1080 10810730 2010 522699 PMC 3075203 PMID 21154091 Melhuish Edward C Phan Mai B Sylva Kathy Sammons Pam Siraj Blatchford Iram Taggart Brenda 2008 Effects of the Home Learning Environment and Preschool Center Experience upon Literacy and Numeracy Development in Early Primary School Journal of Social Issues 64 1 95 114 doi 10 1111 j 1540 4560 2008 00550 x S2CID 18460752 Scanlan Chip 2004 Why Math Matters Archived 2009 01 14 at the Wayback Machine Poynter Online September 8 2004 a b c d e f g Lefevre Jo Anne 2000 Research on the development of academic skills Introduction to the special issue on early literacy and early numeracy Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology 54 2 57 60 doi 10 1037 h0088185 PMID 10881390 a b c d e Donelle L Hoffman Goetz L Arocha J F 2007 Assessing health numeracy among community dwelling older adults Journal of Health Communication 7 12 7 651 665 doi 10 1080 10810730701619919 PMID 17934942 S2CID 20421979 Golbeck AL Ahlers Schmidt CR Paschal AM Dismuke SE 2005 A definition and operational framework for health numeracy American Journal of Preventive Medicine 29 4 375 376 doi 10 1016 j amepre 2005 06 012 PMID 16242604 Welch H G Schwartz L M Woloshin S 2000 Are Increasing 5 Year Survival Rates Evidence of Success Against Cancer JAMA 283 22 2975 2978 doi 10 1001 jama 283 22 2975 PMID 10865276 a b Gigerenzer G Gaissmaier W Kurz Milcke E Schwartz L M Woloshin S 2007 Helping Doctors and Patients Make Sense of Health Statistics Psychological Science in the Public Interest 8 2 53 96 doi 10 1111 j 1539 6053 2008 00033 x PMID 26161749 Lag T Bauger L Lindberg M Friborg O 2014 The role of numeracy and intelligence in health risk estimation and medical data interpretation Behavioral Decision Making 30 2 95 108 doi 10 1002 bdm 1788 Lipkus I M Peters E Kimmick G Liotcheva V Marcom P 2010 Breast cancer patients treatment expectations after exposure to the decision aid program Adjuvant Online The influence of numeracy Medical Decision Making 30 4 464 473 doi 10 1177 0272989X09360371 PMC 3616375 PMID 20160070 Hess R Visschers V H M Siegrist M Keller C 2011 How do people perceive graphical risk communication The role of subjective numeracy Journal of Risk Research 14 47 61 doi 10 1080 13669877 2010 488745 S2CID 146594087 Galesic M Garcia Retamero R 2010 Graph Literacy A Cross Cultural Comparison Medical Decision Making 31 3 444 457 doi 10 1177 0272989X10373805 PMID 20671213 S2CID 32662502 Ancker J S Senathirajah Y Kukafka R Starren J B 2006 Design Features of Graphs in Health Risk Communication A Systematic Review Journal of the American Medical Informatics Association 13 6 608 618 doi 10 1197 jamia M2115 PMC 1656964 PMID 16929039 Garcia Retamero R Okan Y Cokely E T 2012 Using Visual Aids to Improve Communication of Risks about Health A Review The Scientific World Journal 2012 1 10 doi 10 1100 2012 562637 PMC 3354448 PMID 22629146 Hoffrage U Lindsey S Hertwig R Gigerenzer G 2000 Medicine Communicating Statistical Information Science 290 5500 2261 2262 doi 10 1126 science 290 5500 2261 hdl 11858 00 001M 0000 0025 9B18 3 PMID 11188724 S2CID 33050943 Galesic M Garcia Retamero R Gigerenzer G 2009 Using icon arrays to communicate medical risks Overcoming low numeracy Health Psychology 28 2 210 216 doi 10 1037 a0014474 hdl 11858 00 001M 0000 0024 F6BE 3 PMID 19290713 S2CID 10545246 Peters E Dieckmann N Dixon A Hibbard J H Mertz C K 2007 Less is more in presenting quality information to consumers Medical Care Research amp Review 64 2 169 190 doi 10 1177 10775587070640020301 PMID 17406019 S2CID 1001326 Peters E Dieckmann N Vastfjall D Mertz C K Slovic P Hibbard J H 2009 Bringing meaning to numbers The impact of evaluative categories on decisions Journal of Experimental Psychology Applied 15 3 213 227 doi 10 1037 a0016978 PMID 19751072 S2CID 11035873 Baten Jorg Hippe Ralph 2012 The early regional development of human capital in Europe 1790 1880 PDF Scandinavian Economic History Review 60 3 254 289 doi 10 1080 03585522 2012 727763 S2CID 154669586 Baten Jorg Fourie Johan 2015 Numeracy of Africans Asians and Europeans during the early modern period new evidence from Cape Colony court registers The Economic History Review 68 2 632 656 doi 10 1111 1468 0289 12064 hdl 10 1111 1468 0289 12064 S2CID 51961313 Baten Jorg Crayen Dorothee Voth Hans Joachim 2014 Numeracy and the impact of high food prices in industrializing Britain 1780 1850 PDF Review of Economics and Statistics 96 3 418 430 doi 10 1162 REST a 00403 S2CID 3518364 Baten Jorg Priwitzer Stefan 2015 Social and intertemporal differences of basic numeracy in Pannonia first century BCE to third century CE Scandinavian Economic History Review 63 2 110 134 doi 10 1080 03585522 2015 1032339 S2CID 51962193 External links EditThe Berlin Numeracy Test CDC Health Literacy Resources Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality Health Literacy Measurement tools Australian blog post reviewing the increasing importance of teaching numeracy skills Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Numeracy amp oldid 1171900095, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.